You are on page 1of 9

International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

International Journal of Educational Research


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijedures

‘University?. . . hell no!’: Stammering through education


Clare Butler
Newcastle University Business School, Newcastle University, Barrack Road, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 4SE, UK

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Little research has addressed the effect of having a stammer on academic achievement,
Received 26 July 2012 specifically progression into higher education. This study spans six decades of educational
Received in revised form 10 February 2013 practice and shows few differences in participants’ experiences. They describe their
Accepted 6 March 2013 education as occasions of scant interaction, spatial segregation and limited encourage-
Available online 28 March 2013 ment. This alongside an abundance of ‘tension’, ‘humiliation’ and a host of unasked
questions, restricts their educational achievement and aspirations. Findings also offer
Keywords: insights into the possible long-term impact for students with other impairments or
Disability
disabilities. These include their not being embedded in the educational networks, which
Social exclusion
may restrict their ability to develop the vital life skills of teamwork, persuasion and
Higher education
Impairment
negotiation. The implications are considered using a frame of social exclusion.
Stammer ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Widening access

1. Introduction

School days are reportedly the best days of your life: carefree and full of promise. The skills developed during this time are
vital for an individual’s personal, social and economic wellbeing. As the OECD Secretary-General declared: ‘Children who get
a good start thanks to high quality education in early childhood are more likely to succeed in later stages of education and in
the labour market’ (Gurrı́a, 2011). Yet, it is reported that for many children their school days are far from carefree. For those
children who have a disability or impairment (hereafter disability) the learning environment can present barriers which
hinder their capacity to engage with their learning. These barriers have a detrimental effect on their level of educational
attainment and progression into higher education (Gregory, Shanahan, & Walberg, 1989; Holt & Allen, 1989; Mooney &
Smith, 1995). Alongside, being ‘different’ is socially marginalising and having a speech impediment, such as a stammer or
stutter (hereafter stammer), is one such marginalising difference.
It is reported that 1% of the worldwide population has a stammer with the instances being higher among men than
women: with the ratio of 4:1 being reported in the literature (Lees, 1999). Stammering is a communication disorder with the
most obvious feature being a disruption in the smooth flow of speech: often shown via the repetition of parts of words,
hesitations, prolongations or blocks where there is a complete cessation of speech for an extended period of time (Bloodstein
& Bernstein Ratner, 2008). The wide range of speech patterns demonstrated by people who stammer is matched only by the
number of theories which surround the reasons for, and ‘correct’ treatment of, speech dysfluency (Yaruss, 2010). Yet studies
have repeatedly found that people who stammer suffer from social stigma because of their manner of speech and negative
stereotypes are rife (Crichton-Smith, 2002). People who stammer are often categorised as being timid, emotionally
imbalanced, shy and introverted (Ham, 1990; Smith, 2004; Von Tiling, 2011).

E-mail address: clare.butler@newcastle.ac.uk.

0883-0355/$ – see front matter ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2013.03.002
58 C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65

Stammering is also said to have a detrimental effect on the parents of children who stammer, they report frustration; guilt
that they may have somehow caused the stammer; and fear for their children’s educational prospects (Langevin, Packman, &
Onslow, 2010). But is this fear well founded? In this article I explore this question. I relay the findings of a research project in
which people who stammer are asked to share ‘the story of their stammer’ with this article focusing specifically on their
experiences of the educational environment. There is an excellent body of research which examines students’ experiences of
disability in school (Gregory et al., 1989; Holt & Allen, 1989) and higher education (Fuller, Healey, Bradley, & Hall, 2004;
Goode, 2006; Richardson, 2009; Tinklin & Hall, 1999; Tinklin, Riddell, & Wilson, 2004; Vickerman & Blundell, 2010). Those
examining higher education typically focus on students who are in higher education and engaging with their studies. This
present research differs from prior studies of disability and higher education by considering another group – those disabled
students who never arrived. Besides, there is much work in the speech and language literature which considers the
experiences of children who stammer while they are still at school. Scholars have reflected on children’s day-to-day
experiences (Williams, 1982); examined speech dysfluency detection rates by teachers (Lees, Stark, Baird, & Birse, 2000); and
explored the impact of differing speech therapies on classroom engagement (Finn, 2003). Yet, in-depth empirical research
that draws on the recollection of adults of their school experiences and then, by virtue of this retrospective view, their
educational experiences beyond school is to my knowledge missing in the literature. This research seeks to fill this gap and in
doing so aims to support the agenda of widening access to higher education.

2. The study

Participant sampling was achieved via requests during ‘open microphone’ sessions at stammering support events and by
contacting, and subsequently taking part in, stammering self-help groups in the north of England, UK. Participants
volunteered to take part either verbally during the events or via email and were therefore self-selecting. Participants totalled
thirty-eight people who stammer: seventeen of whom took part in a semi-structured interview with the remainder engaging
in focus groups. They ranged from nineteen to eighty-nine years of age with them typically experiencing speech dysfluency
from five years old. The sample was just over 80% male, being representative of the prevalence of stammering in men
compared to women. Those with employment experience are working, or have worked, in the civil service, construction,
education, health and social care, retailing, law, along with a number who ran their own businesses.
The aim of the research, described broadly as ‘the story of your stammer’, was discussed with each of the participants
prior to their involvement. This discussion was integral to the ethical research process and ensured they were aware of
the focus of the study and the processes which would surround their involvement. All were given the opportunity to ask
questions, verbally or textually, and were expressly assured that their identifying details would be removed from the
data. This negotiated process of anonymising the data led to participants being classified by age group and gender only.
In presenting the data they are numbered within these categories for purposes of differentiation. Semi-structured
interviews were chosen because they offer a guiding framework while also appreciating that each individual will have
their own story to tell (Patton, 2002). However, due to the nature of the research and its participants the ability to
engage in a structured conversation was sometimes not easy, and participants’ needs were paramount and considered
throughout. In flexing to circumstance, for example, two participants asked to be told the questions in advance so that
they could prepare. Another requested that he be allowed to speak without being asked any questions as he found
uninterrupted talk more comfortable. He was then asked supplementary questions at the end of his initial verbal
presentation.
The interviews averaged 1 h in length and took place by telephone or face-to-face. Handwritten notes were taken during
the telephone interviews and were transferred to a typed format immediately after the interview. Face-to-face interviews
were digitally recorded with the permission of the participant and were transcribed verbatim. The focus groups lasted an
average of 75 min and took the form of a ‘speaking circle’ where participants were asked to discuss their stammer and how
being a person who stammers had impacted on their lives. Notes were taken throughout these sessions. Alongside, they were
digitally recorded with the permission of attendees and then all data transcribed.
Data analysis centred on the perceived impact of being a person who stammers and how participants spoke of their
stammer in terms of its effect on their behaviour, feelings and activities in a range of contexts. The data were analysed using a
grounded approach that underscored the emergent themes. Each transcript was first analysed individually, this involved
fragmenting and coding the data and then reviewing and working with the emergent codes to create abstract categories that
fit the data (Charmaz, 2011). This individual analysis was part of an iterative process which involved returning to previously
analysed transcripts, making cross-transcript comparisons, and, if necessary, revising the codes to reflect the data (Miles &
Huberman, 1994). This reciprocal process elucidated the primary themes and led to the final categorisation. The
commentary below focuses on participants’ experiences of their time in education and is presented under the categories
which led from the above analysis.

3. Stammering through education

This section presents findings drawn from the conversations detailed above; specifically, it describes the way in which
education was experienced as a person who stammers. Responses are reported within the sub-themes of friends and family;
school and teachers; other advisors; and higher education. There are a number of extended abstracts used in presenting the
C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65 59

findings, they remain complete as they offer rich data and inform our understanding. Also, this presentation offers
participants an opportunity to be ‘heard’ in ways they are not typically afforded.
Participants’ stories are underpinned by one consistent message: ‘school was a sodding nightmare, an absolute sodding
nightmare’ (M1, 30–39). Other participants commented in much the same vein: ‘I wouldn’t wish it on my worst enemy, what
I went through’ (M1, 50–59); ‘it was tough, yeah it was. Couldn’t wait to leave, got out as soon as I could’ (F1, 20–29); and ‘I
left last year it was like a weight was lifted’ (M1, under 20). Many of the contexts which led to these feelings will be described
below but the emotion summoned in recalling their school days was audible and visible: invoking distress as well as anger.

3.1. Friends and family

A number of participants reported difficulty in fitting in with the other children at school. Male respondents particularly
described ‘not being part of the gang’ (M1, 40–49) or ‘the one that was picked for the team’ (M2, 30–39). They also spoke of
not having close friendships: ‘it was isolating I used to sit and read or just hang about’ (M1, 20–29). This isolation has been
reflected in prior research where children who stammer were found to be rejected more often by their peers (Davis, Howell,
& Cooke, 2002). However, this was not always the case and was different for the female participants who all reported having
‘strong and close friendships with one or two girls at school’ (F2, 50–59). The male participants noted that friendships were
typically made outside of the school environment, and most commonly at sports clubs. Having a sibling at the same school
brought mixed comments, some reported their help in developing relationships while others found ‘he just used to pretend
he didn’t know me, you know he was popular at school, outgoing like and so I guess now I can understand it he didn’t want
the same treatment as me and he was just a kid too wasn’t he?’ (M3, 30–39).
Moving to the wider family, participants spoke of their support as being largely ineffective. This comment was often
quickly followed by a note of their being ill-equipped: ‘my mother didn’t have a clue how to handle it at all . . . although I
don’t know what she could have done or my Dad. I mean what can anyone do?’ (M2, 40–49). The lack of control and an
inability to manage or otherwise limit the negative impact of their stammer was a concern at home, and yet was consistently
not talked about within the family where the ‘tactic was ignore it, it may just go away’ (M5, 30–39). In sum, the effect of
having a stammer on relationships during their schooldays received mixed comments. However, there was a consistency in
that even for those participants who reported good relationships with family or friends their stammer was not discussed.
This silence resulted in them experiencing an underlying feeling of isolation as their stammer was ‘a no go area for
conversation not at home, with friends or at school’ (M4, 40–49). This led to little or, more commonly, no discussion of the
impact of their stammer on their educational experience.

3.2. School and teachers

Participants moved onto describe the events or circumstances which made their school days most difficult: ‘teachers’ was
the resounding response (M, under 20; M, 30–39 * 3; M, 40–49; M, 60–69; M, 80–89; F, 20–29 and F, 30–39). There followed a
series of stories which had a recurring theme, spanning six decades of educational practice. The theme which dominated
their accounts related to the refusal of teachers to make allowances for participants’ stammers. For example, one participant
noted ‘my English teacher still asked me to read out, even though I asked her not to’ (M4, 30–39). Another participant
recalled the actions of one teacher: ‘he made me take part in the school play, I was so nervous I threw up and he still didn’t let
me leave’ (M6, 40–49). One interviewee struggled to hide his anger and upset when he described an incident at school when
he was around eleven years of age:
‘I know teachers have a lot of their plate and we were a class of thirty odd but to be put at the back, to be told to try and
keep up as if you were thick and to be left to it was so. . . well I don’t know what the word is I just felt invisible. But there
again there seemed to be no happy medium because others [teachers] would ask you to read out [loud] in class and
just allow you to struggle through each effing word. I remember when I was in a real block, when I was part way
through reading, the teacher told me to come to the front and to start again. The class was laughing at me and when I
turned to look so was he’ (M1, 30–39).
All participants stated that their stammer had impacted on their ability to engage with their learning, they reported this
occurred in a number of ways. First, and by far the most common theme, participants talked of a constant anxiety: ‘it wasn’t
so much being asked to do it [read out loud or speak], it was about spending every bloody day worrying that you would be
asked’ (M6, 30–39). The impact of the fear of speaking meant that many participants excluded themselves physically,
emotionally or cognitively. The effect on interviewees is highlighted in a number of their comments: ‘I spent the whole time
in school trying to avoid speaking, meant I learnt nothing’ (M9, 40–49); ‘I was so tense I think I must have had a headache
every day’ (F1, 50–59); ‘I just felt sick all the time’ (M1, 20–29); ‘I sat at the back and kept my head down’ (M1, 60–69); ‘I sat
there thinking all the time don’t ask me, don’t ask me’ (M1, 80–89).
What is striking is that all participants said that they asked their teachers to be excused from reading aloud or being asked
questions in class and yet this was rarely done. Only one participant said that their school supported their request. At a focus
group there was a discussion of the refusal of educators to offer what one teacher had termed ‘special treatment’ (M6, 40–
49). Participants reported that their teachers appeared to not believe them and ‘saw it as another ploy’ (M8, 40–49), but as
one remarked ‘surely their ears would have told them we weren’t lying’ (M3, 30–39). One group member commented that a
60 C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65

relative had recently visited his son’s school for this same reason and was told that ‘no concessions would be made because
we treat all the children the same’ (M7, 40–49).
Second, participants spoke of how their educational experience was hampered by their inability to ask questions in class,
and with no other mechanism being offered other than verbal communication. As one participant stated, ‘if I didn’t know
what was going on, I couldn’t ask a question so my schools days were wasted really’ (M2, 40–49). In describing this context
they referred to their inability to assume the normal role of student. They considered that this inhibited the teachers in
assuming their normal role as teacher: this contextual alignment between student and teacher has been termed classroom
identity formation (Castanheira, Green, Dixon, & Yeagerb, 2007). Participants often spoke of their misalignment in terms of
rules of the classroom where their enforced spatial positioning, such as their physical exclusion to the back of the class, upset
the ‘natural’ hierarchy and that this differed from their peers who chose their seats and thus their place:
I don’t know why I was told to sit at the back because the mouthy kids sat at the back, the ones who messed about. The
clever ones sat at the front and so I didn’t have a chance at the back. I was with a different set. Also because I was put at
the back and the teacher always stood at the front if I had’ve plucked up courage to ask something everyone would’ve
heard (M10, 40–49).
Third, alongside the formal learning experiences, participants spoke of their exclusion from some extra-curricular
activities including plays, events and school clubs. This exclusion many felt restricted their ability to learn about teamwork
and interpersonal interaction, sometimes labelled the class community of practice (Brown, 2007). They repeatedly described
being in a ‘backstage’ role (M9, 40–49) or ‘not in the picture’ (F1, 30–39). This was sometimes their choice but equally often
was on the instruction of the teacher. Yet, in this regard one participant described the teacher as ‘being between a rock and a
hard place, I mean what should she have done made me leading man’ (M2, 50–59). However, this exclusion from communal
activities was a regular occurrence and was often self-perpetuating as it only served to heighten their isolation from the
decision-making processes that were part of class events. One participant reflected on the impact of this exclusion, he noted
his lack of understanding of the ‘political and give and take, the negotiation of working together . . . I never got a chance to
learn that and it’s so important’ (M9, 40–49). Others also described missing out on opportunities to develop skills in
persuading and negotiating with others. Alongside it was noted that because friendships were made in these settings they
were again ‘set apart’ (M3, 50–59) from their peers.
For many participants it was activities outside of the normal classroom environment which gave them their strongest
sense of belonging and raised their self-esteem. A number reported that taking part in practical subjects allowed them to
‘talk in other ways, in art I could talk in pictures’ (M3, 30–39). As another participant described: ‘[during] woodwork it was
like look see what I can do. I could be good. I was allowed to be good at something. I could help the others, they came to me. It
was the best hour of the week’ (M11, 40–49). The positive impact of taking part in sport was also a recurrent theme: ‘football
saved my life I think at primary school. Out on the field I was free from it, I was the top goal scorer and that gave me the
respect of the other boys. I can’t tell you how important it was to be able to be good at something’ (M2, 30–39). Participants
typically described the impact of teachers in negative terms; however, this was not always the case as the extended extract
below highlights:
‘We had a new science teacher part way through comp [comprehensive school]. The other one retired or something
thank God, he was hard as hell. A stereotypical school master type would point at you and say ‘‘You boy what’s the
answer to. . .’’ and when I started stammering it out he’d say ‘‘We haven’t got all day boy’’ or something like that.
Humiliation constantly, I hated him but I loved science. The new teacher was amazing, the best ever. He knew I had
a stammer and called me back at the end of one of his first lessons, he asked me what help I was getting I said
nothing. He asked me what I needed him to do for me. I mean I was so shocked I don’t think I said anything in
particular. But then because I didn’t answer he asked me to think about it, come back the next day and to make a list
so we could discuss it. It was amazing. . . it um. . .. it makes me quite. . . um. . . emotional to think about it now. I was
what fourteen and he was the first teacher to talk to me about my stammer. Saved my life I think cos the rest of my
time in school was hell, you can’t imagine. I went onto do ‘A’ levels in the sciences. He told me to make sure I got my
message across in whatever way I needed to. I remember him saying that I had as much right to an education as
anyone else but that I had to make sure I got it. I guess he moved half way, made me do my bit too . . .an amazing
guy. . . saved my life’ (M8, 40-49).
In sum, when participants describe their experiences of school the words ‘tension’ and ‘worry’ frequently appear. This is
often related directly with their concern of being called upon to speak and this anxiety overrode their ability to engage with
their learning. The refusal of teachers to respond to their requests for support was reported by all but one participant. This
action resulted in anger and/or upset. Another theme which emerged related to their inability to engage in the question and
answer that is typical to the student–educator dynamic: limiting their ability to clarify areas of their studies where they were
unclear. A number of participants described being moved in the classroom, often to the back, where they felt they were
outside of their ‘natural’ place in the class hierarchy. They also described not being included within collective events which
hampered their ability to develop team working, interpersonal and political skills. However, sport and other activities in
which speech was not an integral part offered participants the opportunity to demonstrate their abilities and gain self-
esteem. Together with parents and educators, there were two other groups that participants referred to as part of their
educational experiences: speech therapists and career advisors.
C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65 61

3.3. Other advisors

Most participants attended speech therapy during their school days. Responses to the sessions were mixed with some
declaring them ‘useless’ (M1, 20–29; M2, 40–49 and M4, 50–59) but others stating they ‘couldn’t have got through without
them’ (M3, 30–39) and speech therapists were regarded as a ‘life line’ (M5, 30–39). As they moved towards the end of their
general school education most participants talked of feeling an increased pressure to be fluent in their speech, combined
with a fear of the future and of ‘not knowing what I could possibly do’ (M3, 40–49). The majority of participants reported that
they had a poor level of academic achievement. In considering their future and the careers advice they received, one
participant relayed an experience that was typical:
‘Careers advice was interesting, I was told to develop a practical skill and that it was my only chance of getting a job. I
was in the room five minutes, if that. She had never met me before. She asked my name and when I stammered well
that was it she didn’t ask me what I was good at or what I wanted to do’ (M1, 50–59).
Being labelled in a negative and stereotypical manner, considered as lacking in intelligence or academic ability while at
school was common as another recalls:

‘I remember when the careers teacher came to the school my form teacher asked me if I particularly wanted to see
them as I’d probably end up doing something in a factory or manual work or something like that. There’s little wonder
that I came out of school feeling useless and hopeless’ (M2, 30–39).

This feeling of hopelessness at the end of their school days was widespread (also Fisher & Harnisch, 1989) and for many
had removed any notion of career or educational aspiration.

3.4. Higher education

Their limited qualifications combined with, and arguably related to, their negative experiences at school reduced the
likelihood of them being able (or wanting) to enter higher education. In responding to the question of whether they moved
onto further education, ‘University? . . .hell no! I shook all the way through school [with fear of speaking] I wasn’t going to
volunteer for more of that’ (M4, 30–39) sums up the most frequent response. Examples of other replies, which are
representative, include: ‘No way’ (M3, 40–49); ‘No, absolutely not’ (F2, 20–29); ‘School was bad enough’ (M2, 20–29); ‘I just
couldn’t have thought of it. No. No. . . I just couldn’t have’ (F2, 30–39). The negative impact of their school experiences often
precluded any consideration of continuing their studies beyond this time.
However this was not the case for all, those who attended university (five out of the thirty-eight participants) spoke of
their time at university in positive terms. Yet, they typically described themselves within an academic frame, as students
who ‘worked hard’ (M8, 30–39) and were ‘always reading or in the library’ (M5, 40–49). There were no accounts of the wider
student experience with stories of isolated activities being more common. Of those participants who attended higher
education, not one reported their stammer on their admission form or to anyone within the university: thus, stammering
remains for many an unseen disability (as in the wider context of education as discussed by Richardson, 2009). They reported
this omission was either due to their fear that they would not receive a place (also Vickerman & Blundell, 2010) or because
they did not consider their stammer a ‘real disability’ (M6, 30–39). In addition, as another participant noted: ‘what could they
have done anyway? With dyslexia someone can take notes or you can get extra time in the exams but with a stammer – I’m
not saying it’s worse than other things but – I just don’t see what the university could have done. I just kept my head down,
got by, got a degree, and got out’ (M7, 30–39).
Hence, having a stammer impacted on participants’ ability to receive an education at university in much the same way as
school. This included an inability to ask questions or clarify learning points, but the setting differed in that they were often
part of a larger cohort. Participants spoke of how they enjoyed the relative anonymity afforded to them at university where
the fear of speaking was reduced and they were ‘better able to hide’ (M5, 40–49) from the pressure to interact that they had
experienced in the smaller classes at school. However, there were other aspects of university life, specifically seminar
discussions and oral presentations, where they did experience difficulties but in the context of higher education they ‘simply
didn’t turn up to those classes and got marked down’ (M4, 40–49). They reported that they either worked harder on other
areas to compensate or achieved a lower grade degree. When subjected to further exploration then, the positive university
experience that participants initially described is probably better regarded as an ‘easier exercise in avoidance’ (M5, 40–49).
Other ways in which they managed higher education as a person who stammers was to choose courses that they
considered would require less interaction, as one participant noted: ‘I looked and thought right where will I have to talk the
least’ (M4, 40–49) (also Fuller et al., 2004). Irrespective of their chosen degree, group work was often integral to their course.
Participants stated that being effective in collaborative tasks was increasingly important, both at university and the
workplace. One participant who reported that his stammer had little impact on his time at university or at school spoke of his
experience of group work:

I can’t remember much about my time at school, it was largely uneventful I was quiet obviously but no my stammer
didn’t really have much of an effect. I wasn’t bullied or anything so school was fine. Yeah I went onto university, it was
okay too no real problems there either. Actually the only thing I struggled with, nothing to do with my speech though,
62 C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65

but I struggled with this one course which was based on a joint project. I found it really difficult to find my feet in that
course. I remember finding that hard, working on a team project as I say nothing to do with my speech sorry but I never
seemed to be able to sort out where I fitted. Don’t know why I thought of that then (M8, 30–39).

In referring to himself as ‘quiet obviously’ he demonstrates that he avoided interaction, as many participants, but he
spoke of this as if natural for a person with a stammer. He also comments that his stammer resulted in no negative effects.
However, in referring to his struggle with collaborative work he went onto describe how he continues to find this difficult in
the workplace. Despite his statement that this is ‘nothing’ to do with his speech when considered alongside other
participants’ comments of their being excluded from collective activities at school, and how they felt that this has impaired
their ability to develop the political and negotiating skills required for effective collaboration, then this may shed some light
on his difficulty.

4. Discussion and conclusions

There is recognition of the rights of students with disabilities to enjoy equality of access to, and experience of, an
education. This starts with an effective general education and moves through to widening participation in higher education.
This article adds to the body of work in the education field by considering disabled students who entered the higher
education system but also takes an unconventional approach in considering another group – those disabled students who
never arrived. This article is grounded in a qualitative research project and draws on data from semi-structured interviews
and focus groups which explored the experiences of thirty-eight people who stammer from the time they started
stammering through to their time in the workplace. Given the biographic approach participants were called to describe their
educational experiences but this was not the sole research focus. Yet, the emergent data and emotive responses aired during
the discussions identified that these experiences have for many had a lasting, significant and often detrimental impact. This
includes a participant of nearly ninety years of age who remains angry and upset when discussing his time in education, he
stated: ‘I just hope to God it’s different now’.
Participants consistently reported that school was an extremely difficult time. However, differing from much prior
research published in the speech and language literature, peers were not the main cause of their difficulty. The widespread
declaration that education was a ‘nightmare’ due to the actions of the teachers was both a surprise and a concern given its
prevalence. Participants reported that the behaviour of teachers varied between those who would purposefully taunt them,
ignore a request for differing treatment, through to those who physically excluded them to the back of the class. These were
not tales from the distant past, participants under the age of thirty reported very similar treatment to those over sixty years
of age. These experiences had a direct and detrimental impact on participants’ learning experience; educational
achievement; and their subsequent ‘choice’ of whether to move onto higher education.

4.1. Educational and social exclusion

Whether stammering is a disability or an impairment caused division among participants, but with a majority supporting
the social model of disability (Hughes & Paterson, 1997) which proposes that disabled people are people with impairments
who are disabled by their environment. Related to the earlier discussion, this study suggests that the disabling effect of the
learning environment for many people who stammer can turn their speech impairment into a chronic state of social
exclusion. The European Commission defines social exclusion as follows:

Social exclusion refers to the multiple and changing factors resulting in people being excluded from the normal
exchanges, practices and rights of modern society. Poverty is one of the most obvious factors, but social exclusion also
refers to inadequate rights in housing, education, health and access to services. It affects individuals and groups [. . .]
who are in some way subject to discrimination or segregation; and it emphasises the weaknesses in the social
infrastructure and the risk of allowing a two-tier society to become established by default (Commission of the
European Communities, 1993, p. 1).
The present research highlights the potential for social exclusion for people who have a stammer, specifically their right
to an integrative and inclusive education. Findings report the importance of friendships, which might have been anticipated,
but participants also described the negative impact of not being embedded in the network of the educational environment.
Their displacement emerged as a significant theme where vital life lessons were missed (also see Gibson, 2012), and has
resulted in a number of participants experiencing difficulties in engaging in group work (also Healey, Bradley, Fuller, & Hall,
2006). These skills are much needed in everyday civic life but also in the workplace (Gillies, 2003). In summarising the main
findings from this research, Reimer’s (2004) dimensions of social exclusion are used as a supporting framework. Table 1
details the factors which this research suggests may result in educational and social exclusion for people who stammer.
The implications of stammering on educational and social exclusion as suggested by this research are shown in Table 1
over four dimensions. The characteristics of each dimension are now briefly reviewed: (a) market – exclusion from the
educational marketplace limits students’ ability to gain knowledge plus it impairs their capacity to hone skills of bargaining
and negotiation; (b) bureaucratic – roles exist in the classroom community of practice: between educator and student and
between students. Classroom practices are interrupted when communication channels are weakened or differ from the
C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65 63

Table 1
Implications of stammering on educational and social exclusion.

Dimension of social Present research suggests Possible implications for educational and social exclusion
exclusion

Market Reduced interaction between Limits knowledge exchange


(terms of exchange) educator and student who - educational underachievement
stammers Low transfer of life and workplace skills
Restricted communication - negative impact on social wellbeing
with peers Reduced opportunity for progression into further or higher education
- limiting opportunities for the labour market and economic wellbeing

Bureaucratic Inability to assume student role Outside of hierarchical structures developed with educators and peer group
(roles and rules) as generally perceived and to - group displacement
align to the ‘typical’ rules of - reduced opportunity to develop political and negotiation skills
classroom conduct
Spatial segregation

Associative Low involvement in structured Limited interaction with peer group


(shared interests) extra-curricular events and - loss of shared identity
activities Restricting opportunity to develop ability for teamwork, vital life skills
- negative impact on social wellbeing
Reduced development of close friendships

Communal Subject to negative stereotyping Detrimental impact on mental and physical wellbeing
(grouping/classification) Minimal encouragement Reduced self-esteem and confidence
towards higher education Reduced aspiration for higher education or future career
Low level career goals
transmitted via educators

norm. The hierarchical roles among the peer group are also disrupted when students’ physical location in the classroom is
imposed externally; (c) associative –detachment from peer groups as they pursue joint goals prevents the development of a
shared identity. Exclusion from associates in their mutual endeavours also limits students’ ability to become skilled at
teamwork; and (d) communal – linking with the last – segregation encourages negative stereotyping with this having a
detrimental impact on self-esteem, wellbeing and aspiration.
All higher education institutions in the UK state that admissions are considered on academic merit. For many students
this statement, with its air of equality, fails to reflect the barriers they encounter: the story of their passage through the
education system is far more complex. This research suggests that the challenge for widening access to higher education for
those who have a stammer, however, is not to be underestimated. A number of the interconnected issues set here within the
context of stammering may apply equally to other student groups. First, some, although of course not all, will not have
achieved their academic potential and may not have the qualifications for entrance to higher education (or reduced grades as
per those with dyslexia see Richardson, 2009). Second, yet equally as challenging, based on the data from this study many
people who stammer will not want to continue their time in the educational environment and enter university. Third,
opinion is divided on whether stammering is a ‘real disability’ (participant)- also found among those with other hidden
disabilities (Olney & Kim, 2001)- and so people who stammer may not declare their stammer on the university admission
form. Fourth, relating to the last, some people who stammer fear that declaring their stammer may prevent their gaining a
place at university. Fifth, and finally, many people who stammer go to great lengths to appear fluent in speech (termed covert
or interiorised stammerers), encouraging them to highlight their stammer will be difficult.
Given this series of not insignificant challenges, and the placement of higher education at the ‘mercy’ of the general
education system, are there any policy moves that could be made. The answer to this question is – a diplomatic – yes. There is
an opportunity for higher education to affect a pull policy change rather than reacting to a general school system push. In the
UK at the end of 2011, the British Stammering Association (BSA) launched a campaign in which they called for children who
stammer to produce a speech passport which details their specific communication abilities, challenges and needs. This
passport follows the child through school, being adapted as necessary, and helps with the transition between classes and
particularly between schools, often a challenging time (Anderson, Jacobs, Schramm, & Splittgerber, 2000) and especially
challenging for disabled children (Tinklin & Hall, 1999). This policy has yet to be adopted, but if progressed then a summary
of this passport could be considered alongside other documentation for entrance into higher education. The nature of this
document if developed with the input of people who stammer, involving future and current students (Redpath, Kearney,
Nicholl, Mulvenna, Wallace, & Martin, 2012), and the support of the BSA and other worldwide stammering associations,
could put higher education at the leading edge of policy change for widening access for people who stammer rather than as a
reactor.
Beyond the specifics of people who stammer, the findings and recommendations following this research align with prior
studies regarding the needs of disabled students. To be clear, their needs are not about ‘special treatment’ (comment made to
participant); rather, what is required is a sense of inclusivity in the educational environment. This inclusivity ‘is not only
about meeting the requirements of disability discrimination law. It should be about enhancing the student learning
64 C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65

experience, cancelling out distinctions, removing ghettoising ‘barriers’’ (Madriaga, Hanson, Heaton, Kay, Newitt, & Walker,
2010, p. 656). There needs to be flexibility in the delivery and nature of teaching approaches but also in assessment: one size
does not fit all whether disabled or non-disabled (Fuller et al., 2004). The findings of this research also highlight that the
beliefs and behaviours of educators has a significant role in creating this inclusive environment (Tinklin et al., 2004). As
noted by one participant, a single positive encounter between student and teacher can be transformative.
Finally, in seeking to answer my question: should parents of children who stammer fear for their educational prospects,
specifically their ability to progress into higher education? The findings of this research highlight that they should be aware
of the risks of a ‘tactic of silence’ and maintain a dialogue with the school, the speech therapist and the child. There is a clear
and present danger of children who stammer being subject to a series of contextual influences within the school
environment which can impair their educational chances and progression onto higher education. Considering these
influences in terms of both educational and social exclusion, as shown in Table 1, highlights the possible long-term
detrimental implications of inaction.
In order to offer practical lessons as a result of this research I asked participants to suggest if they were an educator what
they would do to best ensure that their students’ experiences of education were positive. They provided a number of
suggestions, as follows: (a) invite students to discuss any personal challenges they may encounter in the classroom; (b) be
open to this and other conversations via verbal or textual means; (c) if requested by a student to make reasonable
adjustments to the classroom interaction, such as being called upon to avoid asking a student to speak in class, every effort
should be made to do so. Participants consistently reported that the fear of being asked to speak at school was extremely
debilitating and significantly hampered their ability to engage with their learning; and (d) seek out to offer support to those
students who have a stammer (much in the same way that one participant described); that is, do not ignore the stammer,
avoid silence, and ask the student what support they need. Additionally, a number of participants referred to the need for
people who stammer to fight for their right to an education. They acknowledged, however, that this may prove difficult given
the power differential between student and educator. Even so, this research finds that increasing the level of communication
between all parties, despite the apprehension or discomfort is the only way to: improve the educational achievement of
people who stammer; make a truly inclusive and responsive learning environment; and offer people who stammer the
chance to engage in the exchanges and practices that are vital for personal, social and economic wellbeing.

References

Anderson, L. W., Jacobs, J., Schramm, S., & Splittgerber, F. (2000). School transitions: Beginning of the end or a new beginning? International Journal of Educational
Research, 33(4), 325–339.
Bloodstein, O., & Bernstein Ratner, N. (2008). A handbook on stuttering. Clifton Park, NY: Thomson/Delmar Learning.
Brown, R. (2007). Exploring the social positions that students construct within a classroom community of practice. International Journal of Educational Research,
46(3–4), 116–128.
Castanheira, M. L., Green, J., Dixon, C., & Yeagerb, B. (2007). (Re)formulating identities in the face of fluid modernity: An interactional ethnographic approach.
International Journal of Educational Research, 46(3-4), 172–189.
Charmaz, K. (2011). Grounded theory methods in social justice research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (pp. 359–
380). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Commission of the European Communities. (1993). Background report: Social exclusion–poverty and other social problems in the European community. ISEC/
B11/93. Luxembourg.
Crichton-Smith, I. (2002). Communicating in the real world: Accounts from people who stammer. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 27, 333–352.
Davis, S., Howell, P., & Cooke, F. (2002). Sociodynamic relationships between children who stutter and their non-stuttering classmates. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 43(7), 939–947.
Finn, P. (2003). Addressing generalization and maintenance of stuttering treatment in the schools: A critical look. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 153–164.
Fisher, A. T., & Harnisch, D. L. (1989). Career expectations and aspirations of youth with and without handicaps. International Journal of Educational Research, 13(5),
515–531.
Fuller, M., Healey, M., Bradley, A., & Hall, T. (2004). Barriers to learning: A systematic study of the experience of disabled students in one university. Studies in
Higher Education, 29(3), 303–318.
Gibson, S. (2012). Narrative accounts of university education: Socio-cultural perspectives of students with disabilities. Disability and Society, 27(3), 353–369.
Gillies, R. M. (2003). Structuring cooperative group work in classrooms. International Journal of Educational Research, 39(1-2), 35–49.
Goode, J. (2006). ‘Managing’ disability: Early experiences of university students with disabilities. Disability and Society, 22(1), 35–48.
Gregory, J. F., Shanahan, T., & Walberg, H. (1989). High school seniors with special needs: An analysis of characteristics pertinent to the school-to-work transition.
International Journal of Educational Research, 13(5), 489–499.
Gurrı́a, A. (2011). Skills for the 21st century: From lifetime employment to lifetime employability. Retrieved 15 May 2012. from http://www.oecd.org/document/32/
0,3746,en_21571361_44315115_48374880_1_1_1_1,00.html.
Ham, R. E. (1990). What is stuttering: Variations and stereotypes. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 15(5-6), 259–273.
Healey, M., Bradley, A., Fuller, M., & Hall, T. (2006). Listening to students: The experiences of disabled students of learning at university. In M. Adams & S. Brown
(Eds.), Towards inclusive learning in higher education: Developing curricula for disabled students. London: Routledge.
Holt, J. A., & Allen, T. E. (1989). The effects of schools and their curricula on the reading and mathematics achievement of hearing impaired students. International
Journal of Educational Research, 13(5), 547–562.
Hughes, B., & Paterson, K. (1997). The social model of disability and the disappearing body: Towards a sociology of impairment. Disability and Society, 12(3), 325–
340.
Langevin, M., Packman, A., & Onslow, M. (2010). Parent perceptions of the impact of stuttering on their preschoolers and themselves. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 43(5), 407–423.
Lees, R., Stark, C., Baird, J., & Birse, S. (2000). Primary care professionals’ knowledge and attitudes on speech disfluency in pre-school children. Child Language
Teaching and Therapy, 16(3), 241–254.
Lees, R. M. (1999). Stammering in school children. Support for Learning, 14(1), 22–26.
Madriaga, M., Hanson, K., Heaton, C., Kay, H., Newitt, S., & Walker, A. (2010). Confronting similar challenges? Disabled and non-disabled students’ learning and
assessment experiences. Studies in Higher Education, 35(6), 647–658.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Mooney, S., & Smith, P. K. (1995). Bullying and the child who stammers. British Journal of Special Education, 22(1), 24–27.
C. Butler / International Journal of Educational Research 59 (2013) 57–65 65

Olney, M. F., & Kim, A. (2001). Beyond adjustment: Integration of cognitive disability into identity. Disability and Society, 16(4), 563–583.
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Redpath, J., Kearney, P., Nicholl, P., Mulvenna, M., Wallace, J., & Martin, S. (2012). A qualitative study of the lived experiences of disabled post-transition students in
higher education institutions in Northern Ireland. Studies in Higher Education, 1–17.
Reimer, B. (2004). Social exclusion in a comparative context. Sociologia Ruralis, 44(1), 76–94.
Richardson, J. T. E. (2009). The academic attainment of students with disabilities in UK higher education. Studies in Higher Education, 34(2), 123–137.
Smith, L. (2004). Lewis Carroll: Stammering, photography and the voice of infancy. Journal of Visual Culture, 3(1), 95–105.
Tinklin, T., & Hall, J. (1999). Getting round obstacles: Disabled students’ experiences in higher education in scotland. Studies in Higher Education, 24(2), 183–194.
Tinklin, T., Riddell, S., & Wilson, A. (2004). Policy and provision for disabled students in higher education in Scotland and England: The current state of play. Studies
in Higher Education, 29(5), 637–657.
Vickerman, P., & Blundell, M. (2010). Hearing the voices of disabled students in higher education. Disability and Society, 25(1), 21–32.
Von Tiling, J. (2011). Listeners perceptions of stuttering, prolonged speech, and verbal avoidance behaviors. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(2), 161–172.
Williams, D. E. (1982). Coping with attitudes and beliefs about stuttering. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 6(1), 60–66.
Yaruss, J. S. (2010). Assessing quality of life in stuttering treatment outcomes research. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 35(3), 190–202.

You might also like