You are on page 1of 21

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/317160011

Music education, academic achievement, and executive functions

Article  in  Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts · May 2017


DOI: 10.1037/aca0000112

CITATIONS READS
81 7,616

8 authors, including:

Steven J. Holochwost Dennis Wolf


City University of New York City - Lehman College WolfBrown
65 PUBLICATIONS   696 CITATIONS    58 PUBLICATIONS   944 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Kelly R. Fisher Jacek Kolacz


AnLar The Ohio State University
15 PUBLICATIONS   618 CITATIONS    45 PUBLICATIONS   586 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Arts Corps: Creative Schools Initiative View project

REACH Lab View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Steven J. Holochwost on 31 May 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2017 American Psychological Association
2017, Vol. 11, No. 2, 147–166 1931-3896/17/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000112

Music Education, Academic Achievement, and Executive Functions

Steven J. Holochwost Cathi B. Propper


WolfBrown, Cambridge, Massachusetts University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Dennie Palmer Wolf Michael T. Willoughby


WolfBrown, Cambridge, Massachusetts RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Kelly R. Fisher Jacek Kolacz and Vanessa V. Volpe


Johns Hopkins University University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

Sara R. Jaffee
University of Pennsylvania

This study examined whether music education was associated with improved performance on
measures of academic achievement and executive functions. Participants were 265 school-age
children (Grades 1 through 8, 58% female, and 86% African American) who were selected by lottery
to participate in an out-of-school program offering individual- and large-ensemble training on
orchestral instruments. Measures of academic achievement (standardized test scores and grades in
English language arts and math) were taken from participants’ academic records, whereas executive
functions (EFs) were assessed through students’ performance on a computerized battery of common
EF tasks. Results indicated that, relative to controls, students in the music education program scored
higher on standardized tests, t(217) ⫽ 2.74, p ⫽ .007; earned better grades in English language arts,
t(163) ⫽ 3.58, p ⬍ .001, and math, t(163) ⫽ 2.56, p ⫽ .011; and exhibited superior performance on
select tasks of EFs and short-term memory. Further analyses revealed that although the largest
differences in performance were observed between students in the control group and those who had
received the music program for 2 to 3 years, conditional effects were also observed on 3 EF tasks
for students who had been in the program for 1 year. These findings are discussed in light of current
educational policy, with a particular emphasis on the implications for future research designed to
understand the pathways connecting music education and EFs.

Keywords: music education, achievement gap, standardized tests, executive functions

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000112.supp

From antiquity to the present day, proponents of music edu- risk—those from households with relatively few material re-
cation have argued that instruction in music yields ancillary sources—are less likely to have access to private music instruc-
benefits to children’s development (cf., Pont, 2004). These tion (Duncan & Murnane, 2015; Southgate & Roscigno, 2009)
arguments have recently become more urgent, as practitioners and are more likely to attend schools where music teachers’
and policymakers have seized upon music education (and arts positions have been eliminated (Parsad & Spiegelman, 2012).
education more generally) to foster a broad array of essential The question of who has access to music education has thus
skills in children, and in particular, children at risk. Children at assumed a moral dimension germane to the current debate

Steven J. Holochwost, WolfBrown, Cambridge, Massachusetts; Cathi B. Jacek Kolacz is now at the Traumatic Stress Research Center, Kinsey
Propper, Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina Institute, Indiana University. Vanessa V. Volpe is now at Department of
at Chapel Hill; Dennie Palmer Wolf, WolfBrown; Michael T. Willoughby, Psychology, Ursinus College.
RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina; Kelly R. We would like to thank the children who participated in this study and
Fisher, Science of Learning Institute, Johns Hopkins University; Jacek the teachers and leadership of the program.
Kolacz and Vanessa V. Volpe, Department of Psychology, University of Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Steven J.
North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Sara R. Jaffee, Department of Psychology, Holochwost, WolfBrown, 8A Francis Avenue, Cambridge, MA 02138.
University of Pennsylvania. E-mail: steven@wolfbrown.com

147
148 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

regarding the inequality of educational opportunity in the Music Education and Academic Achievement
United States (Putnam, 2015).
The issue of access has also gained relevance as the policy- Correlational Research
pendulum has, at least in some quarters, begun to swing toward
a more holistic perspective on education after 15 years of more Numerous studies have demonstrated an association between
narrow focus on standardized metrics of academic achievement music education and higher levels of academic achievement. For
(cf. Robinson & Aronica, 2015). Aspects of children’s broader example, meta-analyses of data from the College Board revealed
development, such as prosocial behavior (Kirschner & Toma- significantly higher SAT math (Vaughn & Winner, 2000) and
sello, 2010; Schellenberg, Corrigall, Dys, & Malti, 2015) and verbal (Butzlaff, 2000) scores among students who took at least
nonacademic skills like persistence (Scott, 1992) have been one music course in high school. The correlation between music
linked to instruction in music, and thus proponents of music education and academic achievement has been found to extend to
education have found themselves well-positioned to advocate course grades in some cases, but not others. Although participation
for music instruction on the grounds that it benefits the whole in music education has been linked to higher academic grades
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

child. Two aspects of development that have received particular among Canadian high-school students (Cabanac, Perlovsky,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bonniot-Cabanac, & Cabanac, 2013; Gouzouasis, Guhn, &


attention are executive functions (EFs)—the foundational cog-
Kishor, 2007), studies with both German (Bastian, 2000) and
nitive capacities that allow individuals to set and pursue
Swiss (Weber, Spychiger, & Patry, 1993) elementary schoolchil-
goals—and the closely related construct of short-term memory
dren (ages 9 to 12) found no differences in course grades between
(STM). Research indicating that the “specific features” of mu-
students enrolled in music classes and their peers.
sic education (Schellenberg, 2006, p. 466) are associated with
These inconsistent results may be explained, in part, by the fact
improved capabilities in these areas has readily been embraced that these correlational studies did not control for relevant cova-
by those advocating for music education. riates. This is problematic given the known associations between
However, as we will review below, the body of evidence linking factors such as gender and ethnicity and academic achievement
music education to improved academic achievement, EFs, and (Kinney, 2008). As others have noted (Fitzpatrick, 2006; Schel-
memory is large and complex. Much of the evidence is correla- lenberg & Weiss, 2013), socioeconomic status is a particularly
tional in nature and controls for alternative explanations of the salient potential confounding variable in the relationship between
relation between music and these domains to varying degrees. This music education and academic achievement. It is related to both
raises concerns about internal validity, and in particular, if the academic achievement and access to music education, leading
experience of music education causes ancillary benefits or merely some to speculate that the observed association between music
co-occurs with them (Schellenberg & Weiss, 2013). Although a education and academic achievement may be an epiphenonmenon
relatively small number of experimental studies do allow for explained entirely by the relationship between affluence and music
causal inference, they are often subject to concerns about external education (Winner & Cooper, 2000).
validity: For example, assigning children to brief, standardized
programs of music instruction does not resemble how music is Quasi-Experimental Research
typically taught in the field, in which instruction is offered for
longer but variable periods of time. Moreover, for reasons of The authors of a number of quasi-experimental studies have
student attendance, parent permissions, and staff turnover these sought to address this issue by controlling for socioeconomic
more controlled studies are typically most feasible in schools that status in their analyses. Work with large, nationally representative
serve students who are more ethnically homogenous and affluent data sets of students in the United States has revealed a relation-
than those at the center of the debate around musical education and ship between in-school music instruction and higher standardized
inequality of opportunity, raising additional questions about the test scores among students in elementary (Southgate & Roscigno,
2009) and high school (Miksza, 2010) that is robust to controls for
generalizability of results.
socioeconomic status and ethnicity. Studies conducted with
In the current study, we sought to address these limitations
smaller samples of Canadian (Schellenberg, 2006), Portugese
through an experimental evaluation of the effects of a music
(Santos-Luiz, Mónico, Almeida, & Coimbra, 2015), and Swiss
education program offered to a diverse sample of children
(Wetter, Koerner, & Schwaninger, 2009) elementary schoolchil-
attending a school in a high-risk area of a large U.S. city.
dren yielded similar results: Music instruction was associated with
Although children were assigned by lottery to one of a limited higher course grades regardless of whether that instruction had
number of spots in the program, the number of years in which been offered in (e.g., Santos-Luiz et al., 2015) or out of school
they participated varied. While random assignment allowed us (e.g., Wetter et al., 2009), and this association held after including
to evaluate whether participation in the program caused differ- socioeconomic status as a covariate.
ences in students’ academic achievement, EFs, and STM, the These studies cannot, however, rule out the possibility that selec-
natural variability of the program’s dosage allowed us investi- tion bias accounts for the association between music education and
gate whether there was heterochronicity in these effects without academic achievement. As Schellenberg noted, “high-functioning
interfering with program implementation. Moreover, the fact children are more likely than other children to take music lessons, and
that there were students new to the program in its most recent to perform well on virtually any test they take” (Schellenberg, 2011,
year allowed us to examine whether program effects were p. 285), and indeed there is evidence to support this account. In two
contingent upon students’ academic achievement, EFs, and different samples of students attending elementary school in an urban
STM prior to program entry. school district in the United States, both Fitzpatrick (2006) and
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 149

Kinney (2008) found that students enrolled in music classes exhibited that 5- to 7-year-old children who received a year of musical or
higher standardized test scores not only while enrolled in those visual arts instruction earned higher standardized test scores in
classes, but also in years prior to enrollment. Elpus (2013) reported math than their peers (Gardiner, Fox, Knowles, & Jeffrey, 1996),
similar findings for SAT scores of high-school students in a large, whereas Schellenberg (2004) found that 6-year-old children ran-
nationally representative sample. domly assigned to a year of an out-of-school musical exhibited
higher scores on a standardized test of academic achievement.
Music Education and IQ However, Costa-Giomi (2004) found no differences in test scores
or grades among 12-year-old children after three years of piano
Further support for Schellenberg’s argument may be provided instruction. Clearly there is a need for additional experimental
by studies reporting a positive correlation between music educa- research that uses random assignment to investigate if music
tion and overall cognitive function as indexed by IQ. Studies with education results in higher levels of academic achievement, and, if
school-aged Canadian (Corrigall, Schellenberg, & Misura, 2013; so, whether these effects vary as a function of the duration of
Schellenberg, 2011; Schellenberg & Mankarious, 2012) and Ger- instruction.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

man (Hille, Gust, Bitz, & Kammer, 2011; Roden, Grube, Bongard,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

& Kreutz, 2014) children ranging in age from 7 to 12 years have


consistently reported an association between music education Music Education and EFs
and IQ, and each of these studies save Hille et al. (2011)
controlled for families’ income. If this association can be ex- Like measures of IQ, measures of EFs seek to assess a broad
plained by Schellenberg’s (2011) argument (i.e., higher- array of cognitive functions that underlie performance in multiple
functioning children choose to pursue musical instruction), then so domains, including academic achievement. EFs refer to a set of
too might the observed relationship between music education and cognitive processes that are essential to both setting goals and
academic achievement. organizing behavior in the pursuit of those goals (Diamond, 2013).
However—and contrary to this argument—it is also possible Although the specific processes said to comprise EFs varies (cf.
that musical training caused these students to become more intel- Willoughby, Holochwost, Blanton, & Blair, 2014), included in
ligent. Consistent with this account, a small number of experimen- most definitions are working memory, inhibitory control, and the
tal studies have indicated that musical training results in modest ability to flexibly shift attention (also referred to as set shifting;
increases in cognitive function as indexed by IQ. Studies with Miyake et al., 2000; Ursache, Blair, & Raver, 2012). Working
Canadian (Schellenberg, 2004), Iranian (Kaviani, Mirbaha, Pour- memory includes a central executive that actively manipulates
naseh, & Sagan, 2014), and Israeli (Portowitz, Lichtenstein, Ego- information, and two short-term storage buffers where this
rova, & Brand, 2009) children ranging in age from 5 to 9 years information is held (the phonological loop and visuospatial
have demonstrated that students randomly assigned to in- (Kaviani sketchpad; Baddeley, Logie, Bressi, Della Sala, & Spinnler,
et al., 2014; Portowitz et al., 2009) or out-of-school (Schellenberg, 1986). Inhibitory control is defined as the ability to inhibit a
2004) musical instruction ranging from 12 weeks to 2 years yield prepotent or dominant behavioral response, while attentional
increases in IQ corresponding to effect sizes of 0.35 (Schellenberg, shifting refers to the ability to willfully orient attention to a new
2004) to 0.40 (Kaviani et al., 2014). Despite these results, it would stimulus (Diamond, 2013).
be premature to conclude that music education causes improve- As others have noted, the “specific features” of music education
ment in cognitive function, as other studies have produced null may foster EFs more readily than other activities (Schellenberg,
findings. For example, Moreno and colleagues found that 8-year- 2006, p. 466). Practicing music draws heavily on inhibitory and
old Portugese children did not exhibit higher IQ following a attentional resources (Jäncke, 2009). This is certainly the case
6-month course of instruction (Moreno et al., 2009). And although
when aspects of the music being played change (e.g., tempi, key;
10-year-old French-Canadian children did exhibit significantly
Zuk et al., 2014), but even when these are stable, playing music
higher IQ scores after 1 and 2 years of music lessons, these
(particularly in an ensemble setting) requires moment-to-moment
differences were not observed after 3 years (Costa-Giomi, 1999).
self-monitoring (e.g., correcting intonation), inhibitory control (re-
sisting the urge to play over others; Jentzsch, Mkrtchian, & Kansal,
Experimental Research 2014), and shifts in attentional focus (listening to oneself and to
In summary, although there is ample correlational evidence of a one’s stand partner, reading the music, glancing at the conductor,
relationship between music education and academic achievement, and so forth; Loehr, Kourtis, Vesper, Sebanz, & Knoblich, 2013).
this evidence is subject to limitations imposed by third-factor As Moreno et al. (2011) noted, the intense training of these
explanations (socioeconomic status chief among them) and selec- abilities through music may translate to improved abilities outside
tion bias. And though there are findings from some experimental of that context due to the frequent and prolonged recruitment of
studies indicating that music education leads to gains in cognitive brain areas that underlie essential cognitive processes across con-
function (as indexed by IQ), other studies have not provided texts (citing Jung & Haier, 2007). These areas include the prefron-
evidence for this effect. In any event, even evidence for a causal tal cortex, which both functional imaging (Zuk et al., 2014) and
link between music education and cognitive function could not electrophysiological (Fujioka et al., 2006; Moreno et al., 2011)
speak directly to whether music education would yield similar methods have revealed to be more active during tasks of EFs
benefits for academic achievement. To date, only three experimen- among musically trained children than their peers, and which has
tal studies of which we are aware addressed this question, and they been shown to develop more rapidly among musically trained
produced contradictory results. Gardner and his colleagues found children (Hudziak et al., 2014).
150 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

Correlational Research Experimental Research


Despite this compelling conceptual account, much of the evi- Short of recruiting a prohibitively large sample that could permit
dence linking music education to enhanced EFs is correlational in controlling for all potentially relevant covariates, demonstrating a
nature and drawn from research conducted with adults. Studies causal relationship between music education and improved EFs
conducted with older adults have demonstrated that musicians requires that participants be assigned at random to receive musical
outperform their nonmusician peers on tasks of both auditory and training. Two studies of which we are aware have used this
visual inhibitory control and attention (Amer, Kalender, Hasher, approach. Bugos and her colleagues found that older adults (ages
Trehub, & Wong, 2013) and planning (Hanna-Pladdy & Gajewski, 60 to 85) who were randomly assigned to receive 6 months of
2012). Research with younger adults has produced similar results. piano instruction exhibited significantly faster performance on
For example, younger adult musicians outperformed their peers on interference trials for the trial-making task, though no improve-
ment in difference scores was observed (difference scores were
a Stroop test of auditory inhibitory control (Bialystok & DePape,
calculated as reaction time [RT] for interference trials minus RT
2009), whereas Slevc and colleagues found that musical ability
for noninterference trials; Bugos, Perlstein, McCrae, Brophy, &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

was associated with performance on tests of auditory and visual


Bedenbaugh, 2007). Moreno and colleagues (2011) assigned pre-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

working memory (Slevc, Davey, Buschkuehl, & Jaeggi, 2016).


school children to a 4-week course of instruction in either music or
It would be premature, however, to conclude that these results
visual arts. Although there were no differences in performance on
offer unequivocal correlational evidence of an association between a task of inhibitory control (a go/no-go task) prior to the course of
music education and EFs. In each study cited above there were instruction, children assigned to music instruction were signifi-
other tasks on which musicians did not outperform their peers, and cantly more accurate in their responses following the course.
in many cases differences in performance that were observed on a
task in one study were not observed for that same task in another
study. This uneven pattern of results by task, both within and Music Education and STM
between studies, is evident in research investigating the correlation As noted above, the correlational evidence linking music edu-
between music education and EFs in children as well. cation to working memory is mixed, with some studies reporting
In one such study, Zuk and colleagues (2014) found that school- an association between musical instruction and performance on
age children (ages 9 to 12 years) in the United States who had backward-span tasks (e.g., Roden et al., 2014) and others reporting
received at least two years of music training outperformed a group no such relation (e.g., Zuk et al., 2014). A more modest proposi-
of musically untrained children on tests of processing speed, verbal tion is that music education may enhance the capacity to encode
fluency, and set-shifting (a trail-making task), but not on measures and retrieve information held in STM. This may be due, in part, to
of working memory (backward digit span) or inhibitory control the memorization of musical passages (Schellenberg, 2004), which
(color-word Stroop). In contrast, studies of German students found involves the processing of information across multiple sensory
that musical training was associated with better performance on a modalities (Williamon & Valentine, 2002).
measure of working memory (backward digit span) among chil- Given the fundamentally aural nature of music, the link between
dren ages 7 to 8 (Roden et al., 2014), and that the number of musical instruction and aspects of auditory memory have been the
months for which children had received music lessons predicted focus of numerous studies. In correlational studies with adults
performance on a series of Stroop tasks assessing selective audi- (Chan, Ho, & Cheung, 1998; Jakobson, Cuddy, & Kilgour, 2003;
tory attention and inhibitory control in a sample of 9- to 12-year- Jakobson, Lewycky, Kilgour, & Stoesz, 2008) and children (Ho,
old children (Degé, Kubicek, & Schwarzer, 2011). However, a Cheung, & Chan, 2003), musicians were found to exhibit better
third study by Schellenberg (2011) found that although Canadian performance on delayed-recall assessments of verbal memory than
children (again, ages 9 to 12) who received music lessons per- nonmusicians after controlling for relevant confounds. Studies that
formed better than their musically untrained peers on tasks assess- have focused more explicitly on verbal STM have yielded similar
ing working memory, similar results were not observed for mea- results. Among older adults (Fauvel et al., 2014), younger adults
(Franklin et al., 2008; Hansen, Wallentin, & Vuust, 2013; Lee, Lu,
sures of phonological fluency, mental flexibility, inhibitory
& Ko, 2007), and children (Lee et al., 2007; Roden et al., 2014),
control, or planning.
musically trained individuals have been found to outperform their
As Degé and colleagues noted, these discrepancies may be
peers on auditory forward digit-span tasks.
attributable to methodological differences among these studies,
However, the argument has also been made that students of
including the measures used, the length and intensity of musical
musical traditions using notation may realize benefits to the more
training children received, and the way musical training was coded remote domain of visual memory. Reading notated music from the
in the analyses (e.g., continuously, as in Degé et al., 2011, or page requires the recognition and encoding of simple visual stimuli
dichotomously, as in the case of Schellenberg, 2011). Moreover, in a predetermined order (i.e., left to right; Jakobson et al., 2008).
many of the third-factor explanations at work in the relationship Correlational studies with older adults (Amer et al., 2013) and
between music education and academic achievement apply to the school-age children (Lee et al., 2007) have supported this account,
correlation between musical training and EFs. These factors in- reporting associations between musical training and better perfor-
clude students’ socioeconomic status and the varying extent to mance on forward spatial-span tasks of visual STM. In a quasi-
which studies controlled for socioeconomic status by matching experimental study that controlled for parental education and in-
participants (as in Zuk et al., 2014) or through statistical means come, Bilhartz and her colleagues observed significantly larger
(Schellenberg, 2011). rates of positive change in visual STM among preschoolers who
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 151

had received 30 weeks of musical training relative to their peers end measures of academic achievement, EFs, and STM contingent
(Bilhartz, Bruhn, & Olson, 1999). upon levels of performance on these measures prior to program
Although some of these studies controlled for third-factor ex- entry? We made the hypothesis that these conditional effects
planations (e.g., Bilhartz et al., 1999; Lee et al., 2007), experimen- would be observed, but made no statement about the precise nature
tal evidence establishing a causal link between music education of these effects.
and either verbal or visual STM is scarce. To date, only the study
conducted by Bugos and her colleagues has suggested such a link: Method
older adults who were randomly assigned to receive piano instruc-
tion exhibited improvements in verbal STM (digit forward span)
that approached significance (Bugos et al., 2007).
Participants
The sample comprised 265 children attending a single parochial
Current Study school located in an economically disadvantaged neighborhood of
a large Northeastern city. Students were enrolled in Grades 1
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

As noted earlier in the introduction, the literature regarding the through 8, with a mean age of 10.2 years (SD ⫽ 2.15 years) when
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ancillary benefits of music education to academic achievement, the first round of tests of EFs and STM were administered in the
EFs, and STM is complex and leaves gaps in our understanding. fall of 2012. According to school administrative records, none of
The largest and most consistent body of evidence comes from the students in the sample were classified as special education or
correlational studies that link musical training to higher levels of limited English proficiency. The sample was majority African
academic achievement. However, even this literature is not with- American (85.7%), but included students of Hispanic/Latino
out contrary findings, and in any event these studies cannot speak (9.1%), Asian (2.6%) and European (2.6%) descent. The parents or
to whether music education is the cause of these benefits, in part guardians of all students elected for their children to participate in
because these studies cannot address self-selection bias. Causal an intensive afterschool music-education program (see the Proce-
inference demands random assignment, but to date most experi- dures section for a description of the program), but because of
mental studies have focused on the capacity of music education to constraints of space and resources, 135 students (50.9% of the
benefit overall cognitive function as indexed by IQ. Here too there sample) were selected at random to participate in the program via
are mixed findings, and as noted above the capacity for music a lottery; these students were designated as the program group,
education to benefit IQ does not necessarily mean that parallel although those not selected comprised the control group. Selection
benefits would be observed in the areas of academic achievement, occurred at the beginning of the 2010, 2011, and 2012 academic
EFs, or STM. Moreover, most experimental studies have featured years. In the fall of 2012 when data collection began, 50 (37.0% of
single-dose courses of music education that cannot address the the program group) students were entering their third year in the
effects of more sustained courses of music education that vary in program, whereas 32 (23.7%) and 53 (39.3%) students were en-
duration (see Costa-Giomi, 1999, for an exception). Finally, given tering their second or first years, respectively. All students entering
that many correlational and most experimental studies were per- their third year in the program had been enrolled in 2010 –2011
formed with homogenous samples of students of European de- and 2011–2012 academic years. All students entering their second
scent, the extent to which their findings can be generalized to year had been enrolled in 2011–2012, with one exception: Because
students attending school in large, urban districts in the United of extenuating familial circumstances, one student had been en-
States is unclear. rolled in 2010 –2011, was not enrolled in 2011–2012, and returned
It was therefore the purpose of this study to examine the effects to the program in 2012–2013.
of enrollment in a program of music education on academic Table 1 reports the distribution of students by gender, ethnicity,
achievement, EFs, and STM in a majority-minority sample of age, and grade; no differences were observed between groups for
school-age children. The design of the program as implemented any of these factors. Within each grade, students were divided into
(see below) featured assignment to an oversubscribed music edu- two classrooms, A and B. The distribution of students by class-
cation program by lottery and natural variability in program dos- room within grade did not differ by group (see Supplemental Table
age, allowing us to address three specific research questions while S1). The distribution of students by years enrolled in the program
maintaining the ecological validity of the study setting: First, is did not differ by gender or ethnicity, but did differ by age, F(2,
there any effect of program enrollment on students’ academic 135) ⫽ 36.2, p ⬍ .001 and grade, ␹2(14) ⫽ 105.7, p ⬍ .001, such
achievement, EFs, or STM? We hypothesized that students en- that students enrolled in the program for longer periods of time
rolled in the program would exhibit higher levels of academic were significantly older than those in the previous year (see also
achievement and improved performance on measures of EFs and Supplemental Table S2).
STM. Second, are there dosage effects for program enrollment,
such that longer periods of enrollment in the program are associ-
Procedure
ated with better performance on measures of academic achieve-
ment, EFs, and STM? We made the exploratory hypothesis that Students in the program group received an intensive course of
such effects would be observed, based in part on the results of prior music education during the 2010 –2011, 2011–2012, and/or 2012–
studies demonstrating an association between the duration of mu- 2013 academic years inspired by El Sistema, a program of orches-
sical training and academic achievement as indexed by standard- tral music instruction that originated in Venezuela (see the online
ized test scores and grades (Corrigall et al., 2013; Schellenberg, supplementary material for additional details). Each year the pro-
2006). Third, for the subset of students new to the program in its gram ran for 39 weeks, beginning in mid-September 2012 and
most recent year, are the effects of program enrollment on year- concluding in the first week of June 2013. Students enrolled in the
152 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

Table 1
Distribution of Program- and Control-Group Students by Gender, Ethnicity, Age, and Grade

Overall (N ⫽ 265) Program group (n ⫽ 135) Control group (n ⫽ 130) Comparison


Variable n (%) M (SD) n (%) M (SD) n (%) M (SD) ␹2(df) t (df)

Gender
Female 154 (58.1) 82 (60.7) 72 (55.4) .781 (1)
Male 111 (41.9) 53 (39.3) 58 (44.6)
Ethnicity
Asian American 7 (2.6) 3 (2.2) 4 (3.1) 1.61 (3)
African American 227 (85.7) 116 (85.9) 111 (85.4)
Hispanic/Latino 24 (9.1) 11 (8.1) 13 (10.0)
European American 7 (2.6) 5 (3.7) 2 (1.5)
Age 10.2 (2.15) 10.2 (2.09) 10.2 (2.22) ⫺.175 (263)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Grade
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1 31 (11.7) 15 (11.1) 16 (12.3) 2.86 (7)


2 13 (4.9) 7 (5.2) 6 (4.6)
3 40 (15.1) 17 (12.6) 23 (17.7)
4 57 (21.5) 32 (23.7) 25 (19.2)
5 18 (6.8) 8 (5.9) 10 (7.7)
6 31 (11.7) 18 (13.3) 13 (10.0)
7 38 (14.3) 20 (14.8) 18 (13.8)
8 25 (9.4) 12 (8.9) 13 (10.0)
Note. For the program- and control-group columns, percentages are reported with respect to the number of students in each group.

program met for approximately two hours every day school was in the measures of EFs and STM. Fall 2012 testing occurred 2 weeks
session. Each meeting featured 40 min of instruction on an orches- after the program began, whereas Spring 2013 testing occurred 1
tral instrument in a small-group setting and 40 min’ rehearsal in an week before the program ended (see Figure 1 for a summary of
ensemble comprised of players of similar levels of experience and when measures were administered). In both the fall and spring 18
skill. In between small-group and large-ensemble activities, stu- students missed their scheduled testing date due to absence
dents took a snack break lasting approximately 20 min (the re- (though only one student missed both dates), bringing the maxi-
maining 20 min were required for students to travel between mum number of students completing any EFs or memory task to
activities). Of the 135 students participating in the program during 216. These students were not rescheduled, given that this would
the 2012–2013 academic year, 14 (10.4% of those initially en- have required waiting multiple weeks to accommodate the school
rolled) left the program during the school year due to lack of schedule, resulting in unacceptable variability in the time of testing
interest or scheduling conflicts. Given that (a) no student partici- relative to the initiation and conclusion of the program. Absence
pated in the program for fewer than 10 weeks; (b) students who on the date of scheduled testing for both Fall 2012 and Spring 2013
ultimately left the program remained in it for an average of 23 was coded as a binary variable (1 ⫽ absence) and regressed on
weeks (SD ⫽ 6.87 weeks); and (c) the minimum program “dosage” gender, ethnicity, age, grade, classroom, and group (program vs.
required to yield observable effects is unknown, students who left control). Absence for both the fall and spring was unrelated to each
the program were considered part of the program group for pur- of these factors.
poses of analysis. This intent-to-treat approach yields the most On both the fall and spring testing days, students were escorted
conservative estimates of program effects. from their classroom to the school’s computer laboratory by a
All students in Grades 2 through 8 (N ⫽ 234) in both the member of the program staff. All students were familiar with the
program and control groups were scheduled for EFs and STM lab and its computers, as students use the lab regularly during the
testing during one week in the fall of 2012 and 1 week in the spring school year. After students were seated a researcher explained that
of 2013. Students in first grade were not assessed, as pilot testing they would be playing a series of computer games. Students were
had revealed that they struggled with the instructions for some of told that some of the games might seem like tests, but that they

Program Begins Program Ends

EF measures, post-
EF measures, pre- Grades, T1 Test Scores
Grades, T3

mid-Sep., 2012 8 wks. mid-Nov., 2012 mid-Mar., 2013 10 wks. early Jun., 2013

Figure 1. Schematic summarizing the administration of measures relative to the beginning and end of the
program. Note that preprogram EF (executive functions) measures were administered 2 weeks after the program
began, whereas postprogram EF measures were administered 1 week before the program ended. T1 ⫽ first
trimester report card, T3 ⫽ third trimester report card.
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 153

were not like the tests students take in school: no one except the formance; cf. Willoughby et al., 2012) nor adults (when RT
researcher would know how the student did, and he would not tell indicators are preferred; cf. Bialystok & DePape, 2009). For the
anyone. After answering any questions students had, the researcher flanker task, a difference score was calculated as the median RT to
proceeded to guide students through nine computerized tasks of correct incongruent trials (in milliseconds) minus the median re-
EFs and STM (described below), which required approximately 50 action to correct congruent trials, whereas for the go/no-go task the
min to complete. After students completed the tasks, the researcher difference score was calculated as the median RT to correct no-go
thanked them and presented them with a small prize (crayons and trials (in which the child responded to a stimulus that appeared
stickers). rarely) minus the mean RT to correct go trials (in which the child
responded to a stimulus that appeared frequently).
A difference score was also calculated for the trail-making task
Measures
as the median time required to complete the trials with interference
Academic achievement. Students in Grades 2 through 8 were minus the median time required to complete trials without inter-
assigned year-end and trimester grades in English Language Arts ference (Lehto, Juujärvi, Kooistra, & Pulkkinen, 2003). The pro-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(ELA) and math by their primary teachers. For students in Grades portion of trials correct was not calculated as this score would be
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

4 through 8, ELA and math grades were assigned on a numeric directly analogous to the time required to complete the task (the
scale ranging from 60 to 99; for students in Grades 2 and 3, task will not progress until the child correctly connects one dot to
academic grades were assigned on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 the next). Although it was possible to calculate a difference score
(unsatisfactory) to 5 (outstanding). All students in Grades 1 for the Stroop task, the order in which stimuli were presented—a
through 7 took a series of standardized tests in March of 2013, block of shapes, a block of color-neutral words (e.g., “went”),
approximately 10 weeks prior to the conclusion of the program followed by a block of color-words (e.g., “red” printed in blue
(the TerraNova, 3rd ed.; McGraw-Hill, 2008). Students in Grades ink)—meant that the block of trials in which the prepotent ten-
2 through 7 took reading, math, and language arts tests, while dency to name the color-word rather than the color was also the
students in Grade 1 took only the reading and math tests. Scores block in which students were most practiced in providing re-
were reported on a common scale ranging from 450 to 850. A sponses. This conflated practice and Stroop-effects, recommend-
composite measure of standardized test performance was calcu- ing against calculating a difference score across blocks (for addi-
lated as the mean of the scores on the reading, math, and language tional discussion of this issue, see the online supplementary
arts tests for students in Grades 2 through 7 (␣ ⫽ .86) and the mean materials). Instead, the median RT for correct responses for the
of reading and math tests for students in Grade 1 (␣ ⫽ .79). block of trials where the Stroop effect was in place was used as the
EFs. The computerized tasks of EFs and STM were taken measure of efficiency (after Huizinga, Dolan, & van der Molen,
from the Psychology Experiment Building Language, version 12 2006). For the Tower of London task, the dependent variable was
(Mueller & Piper, 2014). Three of these tasks focused on STM, the proportion of trials completed in the minimum number of
and are described below. Of the six EFs tasks, four focused on moves (cf., Huizinga et al., 2006; Lehto et al., 2003), while for the
inhibitory control or flexible attention while the remaining two card-sorting task, it was the proportion of trials in which a perse-
tasks—the Tower of London (Shallice, 1982) and the Wisconsin verative error was made (after Huizinga et al., 2006; Welsh,
Card Sorting Task (Berg, 1948)—were more complex tasks in- Pennington, & Groisser, 1991).
tended to draw upon executive capacities across multiple domains. STM. Three tasks of visual STM were administered to stu-
Tasks were administered in the order in which they are listed in dents: the digit-forward condition of the digit span task (McCarthy,
Table 2. Inhibitory control and flexible attention were assessed 1972), the Corsi block task (Corsi, 1973), and a pictorial span task.
using a go/no-go task (Bezdjian, Baker, Lozano, & Raine, 2009), Note that the digit forward-span task was presented visually, with
a color-word Stroop task (Troyer, Leach, & Strauss, 2006), a numbers appearing on a computer screen in sequence, rather than
flanker task (Erickson & Schultz, 1979) and the trail-making task verbally. For each task, the dependent variable was the mean
as implemented by Atkinson and Ryan (2007). Students also number of items (digits, blocks, or picture-word pairs) correctly
completed a computerized version of the Tower of London task recalled across trials. This corresponds most closely to the point
used by Phillips and his colleagues (Phillips, Wynn, Gilhooly, system used by Brocki and Bohlin (2004) as their indicator of
Della Sala, & Logie, 1999) and a version of the Wisconsin Card performance on the digit span task.
Sorting Task developed for the Psychology Experiment Building Demographics. Data regarding students’ gender, ethnicity,
Language (see Fox, Mueller, Gray, Raber, & Piper, 2013). For date of birth, grade, and classroom were provided by the school.
additional details about the tasks, see the online supplementary Each student’s age at the time of testing was calculated using the
materials. date of testing and their date of birth.
To minimize shared variance due to task, we sought to select a
single indicator for each measure of EFs based on the indicators
Data Analysis
chosen for the same tasks in previous studies. However, for the
flanker, go/no-go, and Stroop tasks both the proportion of trials Our first research question asked whether there was there any
correct and a score based on RT were calculated. Although less effect of the program on students’ academic achievement, EFs, or
parsimonious than a single indicator of performance, calculating STM. To address this question, year-end academic achievement
both these metrics provides information about the accuracy and and Spring 2013 performance on each computerized task was
efficiency of students’ responses, both of which are relevant given modeled as a function of program enrollment for all children in the
that students are neither young children (when accuracy indicators sample using the following equation, in which the -ith child was
display sufficient variability to be a meaningful measure of per- nested within the -jth classroom:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

154

Table 2
Bivariate Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Year-End Measures of Academic Achievement and Executive Functions

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20

1. ELA grades (2 & 3) —


2. ELA grades (4 to 8) — —
3. Math grades (2 & 3) .80ⴱⴱ — —
4. Math grades (4 to 8) — .80ⴱⴱ — —
5. Reading scores .60ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ .51ⴱⴱ .48ⴱⴱ —
6. Math scores .58ⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱ .56ⴱⴱ .67ⴱⴱ .81ⴱⴱ —
7. Language arts scores .77ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .68ⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱ .77ⴱⴱ .65ⴱⴱ —
8. Digit span .30ⴱ .19ⴱ .20 .31ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .52ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ —
9. Flanker, correct .39ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱ .40ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱ —
10. Flanker, diff .13 ⫺.05 .28 ⫺.06 .06 ⫺.03 .02 .03 ⫺.16ⴱ —
11. Go/no-go, correct .42ⴱⴱ .03 .50ⴱⴱ .11 .37ⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱ ⫺.12 —
12. Go/no-go, diff .14 ⫺.05 .17 ⫺.15 ⫺.17ⴱ ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.12 ⫺.12 .03 ⫺.14 ⫺.19ⴱⴱ —
13. Corsi .40ⴱⴱ .22ⴱⴱ .27 .34ⴱⴱ .38ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱ .43ⴱⴱ .30ⴱⴱ .04 .25ⴱⴱ .03 —
14. Memory Span .48ⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱ .49ⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .37ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱ .34ⴱⴱ ⫺.09 .35ⴱⴱ ⫺.11 .32ⴱⴱ —
HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

15. Stroop, correct .18 .01 .18 .04 ⫺.01 ⫺.03 0 .15ⴱ .02 ⫺.06 .04 .01 .01 .06 —
16. Stroop, RT ⫺.09 ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.09 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ ⫺.33ⴱⴱ ⫺.38ⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱⴱ ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.13 .02 ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ —
17. Trail-making .02 ⫺.16ⴱ .02 ⫺.24ⴱⴱ ⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.26ⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱ ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.10 ⫺.32ⴱⴱ ⫺.19ⴱⴱ .13 .30ⴱⴱ —
18. Tower of London .18 .18ⴱ .13 .25ⴱⴱ .15ⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .25ⴱⴱ .15ⴱ .09 ⫺.14ⴱ .17ⴱ ⫺.07 ⫺.22ⴱⴱ ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.08 ⫺.19ⴱⴱ ⫺.15ⴱ —
ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ ⴱⴱ
19. Card-sorting ⫺.21 ⫺.09 ⫺.32ⴱ ⫺.12 ⫺.23ⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.30ⴱⴱ .09 ⫺.18ⴱⴱ .06 .22 .19 .05 .20 .07 ⫺.18ⴱⴱ —
20. Age ⫺.20 ⫺.15 ⫺.12 ⫺.02 .68ⴱⴱ .61ⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱ .36ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ 0 .22ⴱⴱ ⫺.17 .32ⴱⴱ .09 ⫺.05 ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.23ⴱⴱ .11 ⫺.20ⴱⴱ —
N 52 176 52 176 232 232 188 216 205 204 209 201 211 213 213 204 205 215 215 265
M 3.62 85.6 3.71 82.9 641.9 623.1 653.5 4.71 .808 38.4 .920 112.0 4.90 5.33 .857 1,246 10,105 .470 .160 10.2
SD 1.19 9.31 1.45 9.60 40.3 53.4 35.9 .801 .161 37.4 .095 66.0 1.40 1.34 .132 525 9,420 .188 .088 2.15
Note. For pairwise correlations (excepting those involving English Language Arts [ELA] or math grades for subset of participants), N ⫽ [169, 215]. Correct refers to proportion of trials correct.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 155

year-end or spring performanceij ⫽ intercept ⫹genderij ⫹ gradeij Results


⫹ programij ⫹ errorij (1)
Preliminary Analyses
Program was a four-level categorical variable corresponding to
the number of years a student had been enrolled in the program. Table 2 reports bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics
The specific contrast between students who were not in the pro- for year-end measures of academic achievement and Spring 2013
gram (i.e., those for whom program ⫽ 0) and those who had been tasks of EFs and STM for the full sample. Note that these statistics
in the program for 1, 2, or 3 years (program ⫽ [1, 2, 3]) was used are disaggregated by age-group for ELA and math grades, given
to test the hypothesis that students in the program for any length of that the scale on which grades are assigned changes in fourth
time would exhibit higher levels of academic achievement, EFs, or grade, and that these figures exclude students with missing data
STM than their peers who were not in the program. due to outliers or other factors (see the online supplementary
Our second research question examined whether there were material). Two points are notable about the pattern of correlations
dosage effects for the program by comparing each successive presented in Table 2: First, standardized test scores in reading,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

increment in enrollment: 1 versus 0 years, 2 versus 1 year, and so math, and language arts were intercorrelated, r(188, 232) ⫽ [.65,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

on. Omnibus tests (i.e., a type III test of fixed effects) of the .81], p ⬍ .01, supporting the calculation of the composite test
variable program indicated whether significant differences are score. Second, although age was not associated with grades, it was
observed between any two levels of the variable, while a series of associated with standardized test scores, r(188, 232) ⫽ [.46, .68],
Tukey-adjusted contrasts were used to test whether there were p ⬍ .01, and multiple measures of EFs and STM: the digit span
significant differences in the mean estimates for any two levels of task, the proportion of trials correct on the flanker and go/no-go
program. tasks, the Corsi task, RT on the Stroop task, trail-making task
Our third and final research question asked whether the effects difference scores, and the card-sorting task, r(210, 215) ⫽ [|.20,
of enrollment on year-end measures of academic achievement, .49|], p ⫽ [.01, .05].
EFs, and STM would be contingent upon levels of performance on Table 3 reports descriptives and bivariate correlations among
these measures prior to program entry. To test the hypothesis that tasks of EFs and STM for the subset of students who were new to
these conditional effects would be observed, the equation used to the program in the 2012–2013 academic year, as well as the
test the first hypothesis was modified to include the variable fall students in the control group. Fall 2012 and Spring 2013 perfor-
performance, which referred to first-trimester grades or perfor- mance on each measure of EFs and STM was correlated, r(111,
mance on the measures of EFs and STM in the fall 2012 admin- 135) ⫽ [.33, .76], p ⬍ .01, with the exceptions of the difference
istration of the tasks, enrollment, which referred to enrollment in score for the flanker, the Tower of London and the card-sorting
the program during the 2012–2013 academic year, and the inter- task. ELA and math grades were also correlated from the first to
action between these variables. The modified equation was thus: the third trimester (not included in Table 3) among both younger
(Grades 2 and 3) ELA: r(52) ⫽ .87, p ⬍ .01, math: r(52) ⫽ .72,
third-trimester or spring performanceij ⫽ intercept⫹genderij p ⬍ .01, and older students (Grades 4 – 8), ELA: r(173) ⫽ .77, p ⬍
.01, math: r(173) ⫽ .71, p ⬍ .01.
⫹ gradeij ⫹ fall performanceij ⫹ enrollmentij Exploratory analyses revealed that girls answered a higher pro-
⫹ (fall performanceij X enrollmentij) ⫹ errorij (2) portion of incongruent trials correctly in the Spring 2013 admin-
istration of the flanker task at a rate approaching significance,
First-trimester or Fall 2012 assessments corresponded to pre- t(203) ⫽ 1.82, p ⫽ .070, earned significantly higher year-end ELA
program measures only for the subset of participants who were grades, t(226) ⫽ 2.00, p ⫽ .047, and higher scores on standardized
new to the program in the 2012–2013 academic year, and therefore tests of both reading, t(230) ⫽ 2.37, p ⫽ .019, and language arts,
these models were tested using only these participants and con- t(186) ⫽ 3.22, p ⫽ .001. There were no significant differences in
trols. performance on any measures of academic achievement or any
All models were estimated in SAS using the PROC MIXED task of EFs as a function of ethnicity.
procedure with full information maximum likelihood (Alison,
2009), given that all data on the dependent variables were missing
at random, such that missingness of data on each measure was not Was There Any Effect of the Program?
related to gender, ethnicity, age, grade, classroom, program enroll- To examine whether there was any effect of the program on
ment, and, in the case of the EFs and STM tasks, fall 2012 students’ academic achievement, EFs or STM, year-end academic
performance. Degrees of freedom were calculated using Kenward achievement and performance on the Spring 2013 administration
and Roger’s method, given that this method is recommended for of each computerized task was modeled as a function of years in
small samples with missing data (Littell, Stroup, & Freund, 2002). the program using Equation 1. Table 4 presents estimated means
Gender was included in all models as a covariate given that and effects for these models (parameter estimates are available in
preliminary analyses indicated that it was related to multiple the supplementary material; see Supplemental Tables S4 and S5).
dependent variables (see below). All models specified that children Enrollment in the program was associated with composite stan-
were nested within classroom because a series of random-effects dardized test scores that were 10.7 (SE ⫽ 3.90) points higher, on
analyses of variance indicated that significant portions of the average, than those obtained by students who were not enrolled,
variance in standardized test scores and multiple tasks of EFs and t(217) ⫽ 2.74, p ⫽ .007. The corresponding effect size of 0.24 was
STM were attributable to students’ classroom (see Supplemental calculated by dividing the effect estimate by the standard deviation
Table S3). for the composite score. Older students (those in Grades 4 through
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

156

Table 3
Bivariate Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Fall 2012 (F) and Spring 2013 (S) Measures of Executive Functions: New Students and Controls
Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25

1. Digit span (F) —


2. Digit span (S) .53ⴱⴱ —
3. Flanker, correct (F) .31ⴱⴱ .32ⴱⴱ —
4. Flanker, diff (F) 0 ⫺.11 .03 —
5. Flanker, correct (S) .23ⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .56ⴱⴱ .04 —
6. Flanker, diff (S) .15 .01 ⫺.03 .16 ⫺.10 —
7. Go/no-go, correct (F) .16 .44ⴱⴱ .33ⴱⴱ .16 .23ⴱ .05 —
8. Go/no-go, diff (F) ⫺.11 ⫺.14 ⫺.16 ⫺.19 ⫺.02 ⫺.19ⴱ ⫺.15 —
9. Go/no-go, correct (S) 0 .37ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ ⫺.03 .43ⴱⴱ ⫺.11 .35ⴱⴱ .01 —
10. Go/no-go, diff (S) ⫺.15 ⫺.11 .05 ⫺.13 .06 ⫺.06 ⫺.11 .25ⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱ —
11. Corsi (F) .26ⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .03 .30ⴱⴱ .09 .17 ⫺.13 .20ⴱ .10 —
12. Corsi (S) .21ⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ ⫺.07 .29ⴱⴱ .11 .28ⴱⴱ ⫺.28ⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ .08 .38ⴱⴱ —
13. Memory span (F) .44ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .04 .41ⴱⴱ 0 .33ⴱⴱ ⫺.05 .31ⴱⴱ ⫺.01 .46ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ —
14. Memory span (S) .21ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ .28ⴱⴱ ⫺.13 .35ⴱⴱ ⫺.07 .10 .06 .29ⴱⴱ ⫺.03 .16 .22ⴱ .42ⴱⴱ —
15. Stroop, correct (F) .03 .01 .11 .01 .22ⴱ .06 .02 ⫺.17 .29ⴱⴱ .11 .17 .05 .17ⴱ .11 —
16. Stroop, RT (F) ⫺.15 ⫺.27ⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ ⫺.17 ⫺.03 ⫺.16 .02 .05 .04 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.11 ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.10 .19ⴱ —
17. Stroop, correct (S) ⫺.07 .16 .05 .05 .08 ⫺.12 .08 ⫺.13 0 .08 .05 .07 ⫺.03 0 .37ⴱⴱ .12 —
HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

18. Stroop, RT (S) ⫺.11 ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱ ⫺.22ⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ ⫺.06 .01 ⫺.03 ⫺.04 .06 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ ⫺.19ⴱ ⫺.25ⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱ .08 .45ⴱⴱ .27ⴱⴱ —

19. Trail-making (F) ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.16 ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.31ⴱⴱ ⫺.26ⴱⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.13 ⫺.04 ⫺.19ⴱ ⫺.06 ⫺.23ⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.44ⴱⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱ ⫺.04 .17 .20ⴱ .32ⴱⴱ —
20. Trail-making (S) ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.29ⴱⴱ ⫺.25ⴱⴱ ⫺.12 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ .07 ⫺.09 ⫺.03 ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.15 ⫺.28ⴱⴱ ⫺.34ⴱⴱ ⫺.43ⴱⴱ ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.01 .28ⴱⴱ .11 .23ⴱⴱ .46ⴱⴱ —
21. Tower of London (F) .10 .17 .10 .11 .19ⴱ ⫺.06 .23ⴱⴱ ⫺.15 .13 .01 .13 .02 .25ⴱⴱ .22ⴱ .01 ⫺.07 .05 ⫺.01 ⫺.15 ⫺.02 —
22. Tower of London (S) .18ⴱ .24ⴱⴱ .12 .05 .13 ⫺.14 .06 ⫺.07 .21ⴱ ⫺.09 .17 .21ⴱ .28ⴱⴱ .21ⴱ .08 ⫺.17 ⫺.14 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ ⫺.23ⴱⴱ .16 —
23. Card-sorting (F) ⫺.05 ⫺.14 ⫺.11 ⫺.04 .04 ⫺.01 0 ⫺.06 .15 ⫺.06 ⫺.09 ⫺.13 ⫺.01 .19 .04 .04 .13 .05 .10 .21ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.07 —
24. Card-sorting (S) ⫺.07 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.15 ⫺.12 ⫺.23ⴱⴱ .01 ⫺.07 .03 ⫺.13 .14 ⫺.08 ⫺.07 ⫺.13 ⫺.12 ⫺.01 ⫺.09 .03 .09 .11 ⫺.08 ⫺.12 ⫺.19 .02 —

25. Age (F) .43ⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .22ⴱ .05 .22ⴱⴱ .04 .21ⴱⴱ ⫺.26ⴱⴱ .22ⴱ ⫺.07 .40ⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .31ⴱⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.17ⴱ ⫺.17 ⫺.03 ⫺.17ⴱ ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.15 ⫺.10 .24ⴱⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.12 —
N 137 135 133 122 127 127 130 127 131 129 138 133 138 135 138 137 134 127 137 134 138 135 123 135 168
M 4.38 4.61 .659 39.1 .789 42.2 .863 119.2 .907 108.6 4.86 5.17 4.48 4.74 .865 1769 .862 1356 17,046 10,826 .348 .476 .231 .174 10.0
SD .571 .784 .197 59.8 .169 39.7 .151 78.6 .108 67.9 1.46 1.38 1.34 1.34 .135 709.8 .131 542.6 11,676 9275 .170 .192 .092 .094 2.02

Note. For pairwise correlations, N ⫽ [111, 138]; F ⫽ fall 2012 administration of tasks; S ⫽ spring 2013 administration of tasks; diff ⫽ difference score.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 157

Table 4
Means and Effect Estimates for Mixed Models of Year-End Measures of Academic Achievement and Executive Functions

Model-implied mean estimates Effect estimates


0 years 1 year 2 years 3 years Any effect Dosage effects
Measure Est. (SE) Est. (SE) Est. (SE) Est. (SE) t (df) p F(df) p
a
Test scores 625.6 (3.37) 632.4 (5.07) 637.4 (5.64) 639.1 (5.19) 2.74 (217) .007 2.72 (3, 217) .045
ELA grades (2 & 3) 3.60 (.250) 3.23 (.603) 3.95 (.319) 3.74 (.522) .87 (46) .389 .47 (3, 46) .702
ELA grades (4 to 8) 84.0 (1.01) 86.3 (1.79) 85.1 (2.15) 87.9 (1.34)a 3.58 (163) ⬍.001 3.20 (3, 165) .025
Math grades (2 & 3) 4.03 (.276) 3.99 (.948) 3.80 (.490) 4.08 (.600) 1.81 (46) .109 1.45 (3, 46) .347
Math grades (4 to 8) 81.1 (1.00) 84.3 (1.91) 85.6 (2.31) 85.0 (1.38)a 2.56 (163) .011 3.96 (3, 165) .009
Digit span 4.57 (.076) 4.72 (.152) 4.70 (.139) 4.82 (.114) ⫺.15 (205) .882 1.41 (3, 205) .240
Flanker, correct 76.9 (1.58) 83.8 (3.18) 80.3 (2.84) 83.5 (1.17)a 2.63 (194) .009 2.75 (3, 194) .044
Flanker, difference 46.6 (3.89) 17.9 (7.96)c 28.6 (7.11) 34.3 (5.76) ⫺3.73 (192) ⬍.001 5.04 (3, 192) .002
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Go/no-go, correct 90.3 (.94) 94.7 (1.92) 93.7 (1.72) 93.8 (1.42) 2.95 (198) .004 2.96 (3, 198) .034
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Go/no-go, difference 105.5 (6.81) 85.8 (13.4) 127.8 (13.0) 113.1 (10.3) 3.37 (190) .718 1.78 (3, 190) .152
Corsi 5.15 (.131) 5.25 (.263) 5.62 (.242) 5.38 (.195) .47 (202) .640 1.16 (3, 202) .325
Memory span 4.68 (.143) 4.81 (.284) 4.92 (.263) 5.34 (.217)a 2.56 (204) .011 2.80 (3, 202) .041
Stroop, correct 86.1 (1.37) 89.4 (2.72) 83.4 (2.47) 86.3 (2.07) .11 (202) .911 .84 (3, 202) .473
Stroop, RT 1490.8 (52.1) 1264.8 (100.4) 1100.9 (91.9)b 1177.6 (75.8)a ⫺4.53 (193) ⬍.001 7.66 (3, 193) ⬍.0001
Trail-making 11,333 (983.1) 14,004 (1938.3) 11,803 (1739.8) 8935.3 (1420.7) .19 (194) .849 1.67 (3, 194) .174
Tower of London .474 (.021) .490 (.039) .481 (.036) .441 (.030) ⫺.61 (202) .544 .50 (3, 202) .683
Card-sorting .171 (.008) .178 (.018) .155 (.016) .133 (.013)a ⫺2.41 (206) .017 2.66 (3, 203) .049
Note. Bold-face type indicate that the estimate for the variable program was significant. All mixed model estimates for program used no years in the
program as the reference group. Results for tests of any effects report the specific contrast between students in the program for any number of years and
those not in the program; dosage effects report the results of Type III test of fixed effects for the variable program.
a
The Tukey-adjusted comparison between the model-implied estimates for 0 and 3 years in the program was significant. b The comparison between 0
and 2 years in the program was significant. c The comparison between 0 and 1 years in the program was significant.

8) enrolled in the program also earned year-end ELA grades that standardized test scores and both ELA and math grades than their
were 2.45 points higher (SE ⫽ .685), t(163) ⫽ 3.58, p ⬍ .001; d ⫽ peers who had not been in the program (see Figure 2a).
0.30, and year-end math grades that were 3.90 points higher (SE ⫽ Significant effects were also observed for the proportion of trials
1.52), t(163) ⫽ 2.56, p ⫽ .011; d ⫽ 0.42), on average, than their correct on the flanker, F(3, 194) ⫽ 2.75, p ⫽ .044 and go/no-go tasks,
peers, differences that correspond to approximately one-quarter F(3, 198) ⫽ 2.96, p ⫽ .034, the flanker difference scores, F(3, 192) ⫽
and one third of a letter grade (e.g., B vs. A), respectively. 5.04, p ⫽ .002, and the Stroop, F(3, 193) ⫽ 7.66, p ⬍ .001, and
Enrolled students were more accurate than their peers on the card-sorting tasks, F(3, 203) ⫽ 2.66, p ⫽ .049. For the proportion of
flanker (Est. ⫽ .056; SE ⫽ .021), t ⫽ (194) ⫽ 2.63, p ⫽ .009; d ⫽ flanker trials correct and card-sorting tasks, the pattern of differences
0.35, and go/no-go tasks (Est. ⫽ .038; SE ⫽ .013), t ⫽ (198) ⫽ 2.95, among the means estimates was the same as that observed for the
p ⫽ .004; d ⫽ 0.40, but were also more efficient on both the flanker standardized test scores: students in the program for three years
and Stroop tasks. Students who were enrolled in the program exhib- performed significantly better on the tasks than their peers who had
ited shorter difference scores on the flanker (Est. ⫽ ⫺19.7; SE ⫽ not been in the program. For the Stroop task, the Tukey-adjusted
5.27), t ⫽ (192) ⫽ ⫺3.73, p ⬍ .001; d ⫽ 0.50, and faster RTs on the contrasts revealed that students in the program for either two or three
Stroop task (Est. ⫽ ⫺309.7; SE ⫽ 68.4), t ⫽ (193) ⫽ ⫺4.53, p ⬍ years performed significantly better than students who had not been in
.0001; d ⫽ 0.57. Enrolled students also made fewer perseverative the program (see Figure 2b), while these contrasts indicated the
errors on the card-sorting task (Est. ⫽ ⫺.012, SE ⫽ .005), flanker difference scores were significantly shorter only among stu-
t(206) ⫽ ⫺2.41, p ⫽ .017; d ⫽ 0.18, and exhibited better perfor- dents who had been in the program for one year.
mance on the pictorial span task of visual STM (Est. ⫽ .201, SE ⫽ The omnibus test for program was also significant for the
8.13), t(204) ⫽ 2.56, p ⫽ .011; d ⫽ 0.25. pictorial span measure of visual STM, F(3, 202) ⫽ 2.80, p ⫽ .041.
The pattern of differences among the means estimates corre-
Were There Dosage Effects for the Program? sponded to that observed for the both the standardized test scores
and the flanker and card-sorting tasks. Compared to their peers
The effect estimates included in Table 4 indicate that there were who had not been enrolled in the program, students enrolled for 3
significant differences in outcomes by level of program enrollment. years performed significantly better on the pictorial span measure.
The omnibus test for the variable program was significant for the
composite standardized test scores, F(3, 217) ⫽ 2.72, p ⫽ .045. Were the Effects of the Program Contingent Upon
Among students in Grades 4 through 8, effects were also observed for
Levels of Preprogram Performance?
year-end grades in English language arts, F(3, 165) ⫽ 3.20, p ⫽ .025
and math, F(3, 165) ⫽ 3.96, p ⫽ .009, though this effect was not For students new to the program in the 2012–2013 school year,
found among students in Grades 2 and 3. The Tukey-adjusted tests of first trimester grades and fall 2012 tests of EFs and STM were
the differences among the model-implied means estimates indicate truly preprogram measures. Therefore we used this subset of
that students in the program for three years earned significantly higher participants to test our hypothesis that effects of enrollment would
158 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

a
650

645

640

Composite Test Score


635

630

625

620

615
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

610
0 years 1 year 2 years 3 years
Program Enrollment

b
1600
Stroop Reaction Time (msec)

1400

1200

1000

800
0 years 1 year 2 years 3 years
Program Enrollment

Figure 2. (a) Model-implied mean estimates of composite standardized test scores for each year of program
enrollment. Error bars represents two times the SE of the estimate. (b) Model-implied mean estimates of reaction
time to correct responses on interference trials of the Stroop task. Error bars represents two times the SE of the
estimate.

be contingent upon preprogram levels of performance on academic exhibited RTs during the spring 2013 administration that were
achievement, EFs, and STM. A main effect of enrollment was 615.6 msec shorter than their nonenrolled peers, corresponding to
found for difference scores on the flanker task, t(97.6) ⫽ ⫺2.93, an effect size of 1.13 for these students (see Figure 3a). The
p ⫽ .004, such that enrolled students (Est. ⫽ 18.0, SE ⫽ 10.1) program exerted opposite effects for the go/no-go and card-sorting
exhibited significantly shorter difference scores than their peers tasks. For the go/no-go task the effect was large for students who
(Est. ⫽ 50.0, SE ⫽ 4.14), corresponding to a substantial effect size performed well in the fall (d ⫽ .84), but no effect was observed
(d ⫽ 0.81). Significant interactions between fall performance and among students who performed poorly. For the card-sorting task
enrollment were observed for difference scores for the go/no-go the effect of the program was negligible for students who per-
task (B ⫽ ⫺.611, p ⬍ .001), RT for the Stroop (B ⫽ ⫺.486, p ⫽ formed poorly in the fall (d ⫽ .10) but was substantial for students
.002) and card-sorting tasks (B ⫽ .595, p ⫽ .040), whereas who had performed well (d ⫽ 0.30), with enrolled students com-
nearly significant interactions were observed for the proportion mitting perseverative errors on 2.6% fewer trials than their non-
of trials correct on the go/no-go task (B ⫽ .265, p ⫽ .057). A enrolled peers (see Figure 3b).
nearly significant interaction was also observed for the pictorial
span task of STM (B ⫽ .343, p ⫽ .064; see also Supplemental
Discussion
Tables S6 and S7).
As can be seen in Table 5, in the case of the Stroop task students This study examined the effects of enrollment in an ensemble-
who performed poorly (in the 25th percentile) during the fall 2012 based program of music education on students’ academic achieve-
administration of the task and who were enrolled in the program ment, EFs, and STM in a majority-minority sample of school-age
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 159

Table 5
Mean Estimates for Mixed Models of Executive Functions: New Students and Controls

Low preprogram performance High preprogram performance


New students Controls New students Controls
Measure Est. (SE) Est. (SE) Est. (SE) Est. (SE)

Go/no-go, difference 111.7 (16.1) 92.3 (18.7) 82.0 (14.6) 138.7 (10.0)
Stroop, RT 1162.1 (149.2) 1777.7 (72.5) 1224.6 (141.7) 1150.7 (87.8)
Card-sorting .174 (.025) .172 (.015) .146 (.031) .165 (.011)
Note. For Stroop and card-sorting tasks, lower scores indicate better performance: faster reaction times and
fewer errors, respectively. Low preprogram performance was defined as scoring in the 25th percentile on the fall
2012 administration of the measure; high preprogram performance was defined as scoring in the 75th percentile.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

children. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that students work indicating that music education may lead to enhanced cog-
enrolled in the program exhibited higher levels of academic nitive function (Kaviani et al., 2014; Moreno et al., 2011; Portow-
achievement and better performance on select measures of EFs and itz et al., 2009; Schellenberg, 2004) and improved performance on
STM. Although we did not observe dosage effects for program tests of basic academic skills (Degé & Schwarzer, 2011; Forgeard,
enrollment, the number of years’ instruction required to observe Winner, Norton, & Schlaug, 2009; Graziano, Peterson, & Shaw,
differences between students in the program and those not in the 1999; Moreno et al., 2009). Considered in the context of this
program varied by measure. Finally, we observed that among literature, the present results suggest that music education may
students who were new to the program in its most recent year, foster the cognitive capacities that underlie academic achievement,
some effects of enrollment on EFs were contingent upon perfor- or, at the very least, improve the ability to take tests that measure
mance prior to the start of the program. that achievement.
The primary contribution of this study is that these results were Program enrollment for any number of years also led to
obtained using a design that balances concerns of internal and improved accuracy and efficiency on a flanker test of flexible
external validity. The fact that students were randomly assigned to attention, improved accuracy on a go/no-go task, and improved
the program allows for inferences about causation, whereas the performance on a Stroop task and a card-sorting task that draws
low rate of attrition from the program and the conservative method upon multiple components of EFs. Thus, consistent with all
of estimating the program’s effects (i.e., intent to treat) allowed for previous studies examining music and EFs among adults (e.g.,
a valid investigation into the heterochronicity of effects (Schellen- Amer et al., 2013) and children (e.g., Zuk et al., 2014) musical
berg, 2004, 2006). The fact that we studied the program as imple- training was associated with improved performance on some,
mented among a sample of majority-minority children attending but not all, measures of EFs. In some cases, we observed
school in an area of elevated sociodemographic risk extends the improved performance on the same tasks used in previous
generalizability of the findings to encompass music education as it studies, such as the go/no-go (Moreno et al., 2011) or Stroop
is occurs in the field and as it is offered to underserved students. tasks (Degé et al., 2011). However, our results build on these
The results reported here thus build upon findings from previous, previous findings in two important ways: first, most of these
primarily correlational studies demonstrating that music education studies (with the notable exception of Moreno et al., 2011) did
is associated with academic achievement and better performance
not use random assignment and therefore could not speak to
on measures of EFs and STM.
causation. Second, contrary to previous assertions that the ef-
fects of music education on EFs are limited to “low-level tests
Was There Any Effect of the Program? of attention” (Schellenberg & Mankarious, 2012), we observed
Enrollment in the program for any number of years was found significant (albeit modest) effects of music education on a
to exert a positive effect on academic achievement as measured by complex card-sorting task, considered by some to be the “gold
standardized tests in reading, math, and language arts and course standard” of EFs tasks (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001, p. 2).
grades in ELA and math. Effects for course grades were observed We also observed an effect of program enrollment on per-
only among students in Grades 4 to 8, which may be attributable formance on a pictorial measure of visual STM. This finding
to reduced statistical power (the subsample of younger students extends results reported in previous correlational (Lee et al.,
was considerably smaller than that for older students) or the 2007) and quasi-experimental (Bilhartz et al., 1999) studies, in
compressed range of the scale used to assign younger students that it establishes for the first time a causal link between music
grades. Nevertheless, these results are broadly consistent with education and visual STM. It should be noted, however, that a
those yielded by correlational studies (Gouzouasis et al., 2007; significant relationship between program enrollment and per-
Vaughn & Winner, 2000), including those that controlled for formance was not observed on for two other tests of visual
socioeconomic status (e.g., Corrigall et al., 2013; Miksza, 2010; STM. We offer one possible explanation for this pattern of
Schellenberg, 2006; Southgate & Roscigno, 2009; Wetter et al., findings below.
2009). However, they extend these results by using random as- For measures of academic achievement, the size of the effect for
signment to address the issue of selection bias (Schellenberg, enrollment ranged from 0.24 for the composite standardized test
2011), and in so doing build upon the small body of experimental score to 0.30 and 0.42 for course grades in English language arts
160 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

a
2000

Post-Program Stroop Reaction Time (msec)


1800

1600

1400 New Students


Controls

1200
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1000
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

800
Low Pre-Program Performance High Pre-Program Performance

b
0.25
Perseverative Errors (proportion of trials)

0.2

0.15
New Students
Controls
0.1

0.05

0
Low Pre-Program Performance High Pre-Program Performance

Figure 3. (a) Model-implied mean estimates of reaction time to correct responses on interference trials of the
Stroop task among students new to the program and controls at low and high levels of preprogram performance.
Error bars represents two times the SE of the estimate. (b) Model-implied mean estimates of the proportion of
card-sorting task trials featuring perseverative errors among students new to the program and controls at low and
high levels of preprogram performance. Error bars represents two times the SE of the estimate.

and math, respectively. For the EFs tasks, effects ranged from 0.18 reported effects ranging from 0.20 and 0.27. Thus, with the ex-
for the card-sorting task to 0.57 for RT on the Stroop task, whereas ception of the effect of enrollment on the card-sorting task, all the
for the pictorial measure of STM the effect was .25. This range effects of the music education program approximated or exceeded
encompasses the size of the effects reported in previous experi- both Tallmedge’s benchmark and the size of the effects reported
mental studies of the effects of music education on IQ (i.e., 0.35 by Weiland and Yoshikawa (2013).
for Schellenberg, 2004, 0.40 for Kaviani et al., 2014). According
to the guidelines established by Cohen (1988), these effect sizes Were There Dosage Effects for the Program?
range from small to medium. However, as Cohen himself noted,
these guidelines were never intended to be universally applied to Our analyses revealed that there were significant differences in
all findings from data collected in all circumstances, and therefore standardized test scores, year-end grades in ELA and math, select
Tallmadge’s (1977) benchmark of 0.25 may be more relevant here tasks of EFs, and the pictorial STM task by the level of program
(though certainly the extent to which this benchmark can be enrollment. The comparisons among means estimates indicated that
universally applied has also been questioned). In one of the few for both test scores and grades, significant differences were observed
studies that reported effect sizes for an educational intervention (a between students who were not enrolled in the program and those
prekindergarten program) on EFs, Weiland and Yoshikawa (2013) who had been enrolled for three years. For the EF tasks, the effects of
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 161

program enrollment were heterochronous: significant differences tween cognitive function and being a musician, but define musi-
were observed between unenrolled and third-year students for the cians as individuals who have studied music for 6 (Santos-Luiz et
proportion of trials correct on the flanker task and the card-sorting al., 2015), 10 (Hanna-Pladdy & Mackay, 2011), or 20 years (Amer
task. Differences for the Stroop task were observed between unen- et al., 2013).
rolled students and both second- and third-year program students,
whereas disparities for the flanker difference scores were significant
Were the Effects of the Program Contingent Upon
only between unenrolled and first-year students. Differences for the
Levels of Preprogram Performance?
pictorial STM task paralleled those for test scores and grades, with
significant differences only between students who were not enrolled Performance on three of the tasks for which effects of cumula-
in the program and those enrolled for 3 years. tive enrollment were observed—the go/no-go, Stroop, and card-
It is therefore the case that, contrary to our hypothesis, dosage sorting tasks—was also found to vary by enrollment and perfor-
effects— defined as significant differences in outcomes for stu- mance on these tasks prior to beginning the program. For the
dents enrolled in the program for different numbers of years— Stroop task, musical training exerted a remedial or compensatory
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were not observed. Instead, significant differences were observed effect, such that students who performed poorly on the task prior
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

between students who had not been enrolled in the program and to the program achieved much higher scores following training
those who had been enrolled for 1, 2, or 3 years. That said, for than their peers. In contrast, musical training exerted an amplifying
outcomes such as standardized test scores performance continued effect for the go/no-go and card-sorting tasks, with students who
to improve with additional years in the program at the trend level, performed well prior to the program realizing the greatest benefits
suggesting the possibility of a dosage effect that we may have been from musical training. We may speculate that this difference stems
underpowered to detect (Schellenberg, 2006). in part from the nature of the tasks and how performance on those
One implication of these findings is that assessments of music tasks was measured: RT on the Stroop task is a measure of a
education programs— even programs featuring a very intensive relatively simple aspect of inhibitory control, although the differ-
treatment wherein students receive daily instruction every day ence scores for the go/no-go task and, in particular, perseverative
school is in session—may not yield significant findings after a errors on the card-sorting task assess more complex executive
single school year. After 3 years of intense training, effects were processes. Thus students who come to music education with rel-
not observed across all measures of EFs and STM used in this atively poor basic inhibitory abilities may realize particular benefit
study. This could be due to limitations of measurement; as others from engagement in an activity that demands they regularly exer-
have noted (Degé et al., 2011), differences in how EFs and cise these capacities, whereas those already adept at the more
memory are measured may explain discrepant findings across complex task of integrating the subroutines of EFs may become
studies. However, subtle differences in measurement may also even more skilled when presented with repeated opportunities to
explain inconsistencies within studies. Consider our results for the do so.
Stroop task, in which we reported an effect for musical instruction For students who were new to the program and performed
on performance where performance was measured as the median poorly on their preprogram Stroop task, enrollment conferred a
RT for interference trials. With performance thus defined our benefit of 1.13 in magnitude to postprogram performance; for
results are not only consistent with those reported by Bialystok and students who performed well on the preprogram go/no-go and
DePape (2009) and Slevc et al. (2016), who also indexed perfor- card-sorting tasks, enrollment resulted in an improvement equiv-
mance in terms of RT, but with our findings for the flanker and alent to effect sizes of 0.84 and 0.30, respectively. As was the case
go/no-go task. However, had it been feasible to calculate a differ- for the effects of any enrollment, the magnitude of these effects
ence score for the Stroop task we may have observed inconsisten- exceed both Tallmedge’s benchmark and the size of the effects
cies not only with the previous studies cited (neither of which reported by Weiland and Yoshikawa (2013). This is noteworthy
reported a relationship between musical instruction and difference given that educational interventions often fail to yield benefits to
scores), but with results for our other tasks as well. untrained domains (Olesen, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004) and
However, there is another explanation for our pattern of results that the explicit purpose and pedagogical focus of the program
rooted in how the measurement of EFs and STM interacts with under examination was not to improve test scores, grades, or EFs,
heterochronicity in the development of these abilities. Even if we but rather to teach children music. As others have noted (Amer et
had access to the underlying latent constructs that define these al., 2013; Schellenberg, 2004), this may indicate that music edu-
abilities, gaining insight into how different forms of environmental cation has the rare capacity to yield far transfer effects (Barnett &
input (including music education) influenced their development Ceci, 2002), or benefits to extramusical cognitive domains.
would be an involved undertaking, given that this development is
likely to be nonlinear and its openness to environmental input may
Limitations and Future Directions
fluctuate over time. But of course we only have measures that,
with varying degrees of fealty, index certain aspects of these Although this study yielded novel findings regarding the rela-
underlying constructs. As such, we cannot conclude that music tionships between music education, academic achievement, EFs,
education does not influence EFs because we do not observe an and STM, those findings are subject to methodological limitations,
effect of training on the trail-making task. It may take many more particularly with respect to measurement. Chief among these is the
years of instruction than three for instruction to yield benefits to all fact that although measures of EFs and STM were taken concur-
aspects of EFs and STM, or at least for that instruction to yield rent with the start and end of the program year, the first available
benefits that our measures can detect. Certainly this point is measures of academic achievement and behavior were taken from
consistent with correlational studies that find a relationship be- students’ first trimester report cards. These were issued in mid-
162 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

November, at which point the program had been in operation for achievement motivation or self-esteem. There is some evidence
approximately 8 weeks. This may in part explain why effects of that music education is correlated with motivation (Santos-Luiz et
enrollment on EFs were observed for new students, but not for al., 2015), though these findings are subject to third-factor expla-
academic achievement: given how little we know about the timing nations and self-selection bias. Although all three measures of
of the program’s effects, it is possible that even a short period of STM included in our study were build-up tasks (in which a correct
instruction elevated students’ first-trimester performance, thereby answer on a trial with n stimuli results in n ⫹ 1 stimuli on the next
reducing the likelihood of observing significant change in perfor- trial), this process occurred most slowly in the pictorial task for
mance from the first to the third trimester. which significant program effects were observed. This raises the
It should also be noted that while we included measures of STM possibility that the superior performance exhibited by enrolled
(e.g., a forward digit-span task), we did not include measures of students on this task were driven, in part, by enhanced levels of
working memory that required the manipulation of information mastery motivation or persistence. Future experimental research
held in STM by the central executive (e.g., a backward digit-span should explore this possibility, addressing whether music educa-
task). The decision to focus on STM was based on the argument tion causes higher levels of motivation while paying particular
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that musical practice requires the maintenance of information in attention to the possibility that students exhibiting the highest
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

STM (Schellenberg, 2004) and previous research referenced above levels of motivation prior to musical education exhibit the most
that supports this argument (e.g., Amer et al., 2013; Fauvel et al., rapid gains in motivation due to reciprocal, self-promoting inter-
2014; Franklin et al., 2008; Hansen et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2007). actions between students’ dispositional motivation and subsequent
The decision to focus on visual STM was motivated both by the training.
relative dearth of literature addressing the relationship between Another possibility is that music education benefits academic
music education and visual (rather than verbal) memory and the outcomes through its effects on EFs (Hannon & Trainor, 2007;
fact that establishing a causal link between musical instruction and Schellenberg & Peretz, 2008), though the empirical investigations
visual memory would constitute stronger evidence of far transfer. into this possibility have yielded mixed results (cf. Degé et al.,
That said, a future study that systematically examined the relation- 2011; Schellenberg, 2011). Laying aside the issue of mediation,
ship between music education and multiple forms of memory— what processes may account for music’s effects on EFs? The
working and short-term, visual and verbal—would add much to relationship between musical training and prefrontal cortical func-
our understanding of the ancillary benefits of music education. tion (Fujioka et al., 2006; Zuk et al., 2014) and development
Another limitation of our study was that the same tasks of EFs (Hudziak et al., 2014), together with the fact that the prefrontal
and STM were used and in the same order (given the problems cortex is the putative seat of EFs (Casey et al., 2005), may
associated with counterbalancing the order of nine tasks in a implicate the steroid-hormone cortisol in the relationship between
relatively small sample) during both the pre- and postprogram music and EFs. It has long been known that cortisol can cross the
assessments. However, as Moreno et al. (2011) observed this issue blood– brain barrier, thereby gaining direct access to brain tissue.
is minimized by random assignment, in that practice and order Over the past decade, research has emerged indicating that ele-
effects should apply equally to both the treatment and control vated levels of cortisol in the bloodstream are associated with
groups. We also did not control for students’ socioeconomic status poorer performance on tasks of executive EFs (cf. Blair et al.,
or IQ, but again the rationale for doing so—that more affluent or 2011), a relationship that may be mediated by cortisol’s deleterious
intelligent students have access to music education (Schellenberg, effects on the function of the prefrontal cortex. However, other
2004)—applies to nonexperimental designs. Here students had research has demonstrated that for young children, music educa-
equal chance of being assigned to the treatment condition, regard- tion can lead to lower levels of cortisol (Brown, Garnett, Ander-
less of socioeconomic status or IQ. son, & Laurenceau, 2016). Thus it is possible that one way in
Beyond these limitations there are the broader questions of which music education benefits EFs is through effects on cortisol
whether the results observed can be attributed to the influence of levels and prefrontal brain function. Investigating this possibility is
the musical content of the program. It is possible that an after- a priority future research, and is a particularly relevant for pro-
school program that did not include musical training but neverthe- grams serving disproportionate numbers of children at risk, given
less featured “school-like” activities—teacher-directed activities the known associations between poverty and cortisol (Evans &
requiring students to put forth sustained effect (Ceci & Williams, English, 2002; Hill-Soderlund et al., 2015) as well as poverty and
1997)—might benefit children’s academic achievement and EFs prefrontal cortical volume (Hansen et al., 2013).
directly (cf. Schellenberg, 2004) or that the program’s socioemo- It is also possible that the intense training of memory, inhibitory
tional benefits (e.g., to self-esteem; Costa-Giomi, 2004) could control, and, in particular, attention afforded by music education
influence these outcomes indirectly. However, to date experimen- translates to other behavioral contexts not only through frequent
tal studies including a control group of students assigned to non- and prolonged recruitment of areas of the brain including the
musical activities (drama, Schellenberg, 2004; visual arts, Moreno prefrontal cortex, but also through the reorganization of subcortical
et al., 2011) have yielded null results for these students, supporting physiological processes. For example, higher levels of activity in
the assertion that the benefits of these programs are attributable to the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) are thought to reflect a
specific features of music education (Schellenberg, 2006) such as state of calm engagement (Porges, 2007). Accordingly, higher
progressive difficulty and multimodal sensory integration (Bugos baseline levels of PNS activity, as indexed by respiratory sinus
et al., 2007). arrhythmia (the variability in the interbeat interval of the heart), are
Assuming this is the case, the question remains: how or through associated with better performance on tasks of EFs (Marcovitch,
what processes does music achieve these benefits? One possibility Leigh, Calkins, Leerks, O’Brien, & Blankson, 2010). But what is
is that music education confers benefits to conative skills such as not as well understood are the reciprocal effects of musical en-
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 163

gagement on PNS activity. It is possible that repeated and sus- Perception and Performance, 35, 565–574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
tained engagement in music, if associated with improved perfor- a0012735
mance on behavioral measures of EFs, may over time shift PNS Bilhartz, T. D., Bruhn, R. A., & Olson, J. E. (1999). The effect of early
activity to a configuration that supports these cognitive processes. music training on child cognitive development. Journal of Applied
This possibility is underscored by the assertion that the acoustic Developmental Psychology, 20, 615– 636. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0193-3973(99)00033-7
features of music—particularly when experienced within social
Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Cox, M.,
groups—may promote higher PNS activity by signaling a safe Greenberg, M. T., . . . the FLP Investigators. (2011). Salivary cortisol
environment appropriate for a state of calm engagement (Porges & mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early
Lewis, 2009). childhood. Child Development, 82, 1970 –1984. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x
Conclusion Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13:
A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26,
Despite its limitations, this study makes an important contribu- 571–593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2602_3
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion to the literature. It is the first study that uses experimental Brown, E. D., Garnett, M. L., Anderson, K. E., & Laurenceau, J. (2016).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

methods to demonstrate that music education, offered in a natu- Can the arts get under the skin? Arts and cortisol for economically
ralistic context over the course of years, yields benefits to aca- disadvantaged children. Child Development. Advance online publica-
demic achievement, EFs, and STM. As Zuk et al. (2014) noted tion. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12652
when reporting the results of their study, policymakers should Bugos, J. A., Perlstein, W. M., McCrae, C. S., Brophy, T. S., & Beden-
baugh, P. H. (2007). Individualized piano instruction enhances executive
consider carefully the implications of these results, especially in
functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging & Mental
light of reduced access to private music instruction among children
Health, 11, 464 – 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860601086504
at risk (Southgate & Roscigno, 2009) and the potential for music Butzlaff, R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of
education to benefit these students in particular (Catterall, 2012). Aesthetic Education, 34, 167–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3333642
Our results suggest not only that the elimination of music from Cabanac, A. Perlovsky, L., Bonniot-Cabanac, M. C., & Cabanac, M.
public education in the pursuit of higher academic test scores may (2013). Music and academic performance. Behavioural Brain Research,
be counterproductive, but that denying students music education 256, 257–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.023
may deprive them of an opportunity to build the basic cognitive Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the
and behavioral skills necessary for success in nearly every domain developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development?
of school and life. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 104 –110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.tics.2005.01.011
Catterall, J. S. (2012). The arts and achievement in at-risk youth: Findings
References from four longitudinal studies. Washington, DC: National Endowment
for the Arts.
Alison, P. D. (2009). Missing data. In R. E. Millsap & A. Maydeu-Olivares
(Eds.), The Sage handbook of quantitative methods in psychology (pp. Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (1997). Schooling, intelligence and income.
72–90). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/ American Psychologist, 52, 1051–1058. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-
9780857020994.n4 066X.52.10.1051
Amer, T., Kalender, B., Hasher, L., Trehub, S. E., & Wong, Y. (2013). Do Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., & Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves
older professional musicians have cognitive advantages? PLoS ONE, 8, verbal memory. Nature, 396, 128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/24075
e71630. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071630 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
Atkinson, T. M., & Ryan, J. P. (2007). The Use of variants of the trail Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
making test in serial assessment: A Construct validity study. Journal of Comalli, P. E., Jr., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects
Psychoeducational Assessment, 26, 42–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. The
0734282907301592 Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human De-
Baddeley, A., Logie, R., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., & Spinnler, H. (1986). velopment, 100, 47–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1962
Dementia and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental .10533572
Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 603– 618. http:// Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. M. (2013). Music
dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640748608401616 training, cognition, and personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222.
Baker, G. (2014). El sistema: Orchestrating Venezuela’s youth. Oxford, http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222
UK: Oxford University Press. Corsi, P. M. (1973). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the
Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 819B.
learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 612– Costa-Giomi, E. (1999). The effects of three years of piano instruction on
637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.612 children’s cognitive development. Journal of Research in Music Edu-
Bastian, H. G. (2000). Musik (erziehung) und ihre Wirkung.: Eine Lang- cation, 47, 198 –212. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3345779
zeitstudie an Berliner Grundschulen [Music education and its effects]. Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on
Mainz, Germany: Schott. children’s academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem.
Berg, E. A. (1948). A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility Psychology of Music, 32, 139 –152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 15–22. 0305735604041491
Bezdjian, S., Baker, L. A., Lozano, D. I., & Raine, A. (2009). Assessing Degé, F., Kubicek, C., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). Music lessons and intel-
inattention and impulsivity in children during the Go/nogo task. British ligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Perception,
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 365–383. http://dx.doi.org/ 29, 195–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.195
10.1348/026151008X314919 Degé, F., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). The effect of a music program on
Bialystok, E., & Depape, A. M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, phonological awareness in preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2,
and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human 124. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124
164 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive Hansen, M., Wallentin, M., & Vuust, P. (2013). Working memory and
function system (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora- musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of
tion. Music, 41, 779 –793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735612452186
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, Hanson, J. L., Hair, N., Shen, D. G., Shi, F., Gilmore, J. H., Wolfe, B. L.,
64, 135–168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 & Pollak, S. D. (2013). Family poverty affects the rate of human infant
Duncan, G. J., & Murnane, R. J. (2015). Restoring opportunity: The crisis brain growth. PLoS ONE, 8, e80954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal
of inequality and the challenge for American education. Cambridge, .pone.0080954
MA: Harvard University Press. Hille, K., Gust, K., Bitz, U., & Kammer, T. (2011). Associations between
Elpus, K. (2013). Is it the music or is it selection bias? A nationwide music education, intelligence, and spelling ability in elementary school.
analysis of music and non-music students’ SAT scores. Journal of Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 1– 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/
Research in Music Education, 61, 175–194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ v10053-008-0082-4
0022429413485601 Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Holochwost, S. J., Willoughby, M. T., Granger,
Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in visual D. A., Gariépy, J. L., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. J. (2015). The
search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Percep- developmental course of salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol from 12 to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion & Psychophysics, 25, 249 –263. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/ 36 months: Relations with early poverty and later behavior problems.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

bf03198804 Psychoneuroendocrinology, 52, 311–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j


Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple .psyneuen.2014.08.011
stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional ad- Ho, Y. C., Cheung, M. C., & Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves
justment. Child Development, 73, 1238 –1248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ verbal but not visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explo-
1467-8624.00469 rations in children. Neuropsychology, 17, 439 – 450. http://dx.doi.org/10
Fauvel, B., Groussard, M., Mutlu, J., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Eustache, F., .1037/0894-4105.17.3.439
Desgranges, B., & Platel, H. (2014). Musical practice and cognitive Hudziak, J. J., Albaugh, M. D., Ducharme, S., Karama, S., Spottswood, M.,
aging: Two cross-sectional studies point to phonemic fluency as a Crehan, E., . . . the Brain Development Cooperative Group. (2014).
potential candidate for a use-dependent adaptation. Frontiers in Aging Cortical thickness maturation and duration of music training: Health-
Neuroscience, 6, 227. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00227
promoting activities shape brain development. Journal of the American
Fitzpatrick, K. R. (2006). The effect of instrumental music participation
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53, 1153–1161, 1161.e1–
and socioeconomic status on Ohio fourth-, sixth-, and ninth-grade pro-
1161.e2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.06.015
ficiency test performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54,
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related
73– 84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002242940605400106
change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable
Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Practicing a
analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017–2036. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal
.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010
ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE, 3, e3566. http://dx.doi.org/
Jakobson, L. S., Cuddy, L. L., & Kilgour, A. R. (2003). Time tagging: A
10.1371/journal.pone.0003566
key to musicians’ superior memory. Music Perception, 20, 307–313.
Fox, C. J., Mueller, S. T., Gray, H. M., Raber, J., & Piper, B. J. (2013).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2003.20.3.307
Evaluation of a short-form of the Berg Card Sorting Test. PLoS ONE, 8,
Jakobson, L. S., Lewycky, S. T., Kilgour, A. R., & Stoesz, B. M. (2008).
e63885. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063885
Memory for verbal and visual material in highly trained musicians.
Franklin, M. S., Moore, K. S., Yip, C., Jonides, J., Rattray, K., & Moher,
Music Perception, 26, 41–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2008.26
J. (2008). The effects of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology
.1.41
of Music, 36, 353–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735607086044
Fujioka, T., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., Pantev, C., & Trainor, L. J. (2006). One Jäncke, L. (2009). Music drives brain plasticity. F1000 Biology Re-
year of musical training affects development of auditory cortical-evoked ports, 1, 78.
fields in young children. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 129, 2593– Jentzsch, I., Mkrtchian, A., & Kansal, N. (2014). Improved effectiveness of
2608. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl247 performance monitoring in amateur instrumental musicians. Neuropsy-
Gardiner, M. F., Fox, A., Knowles, F., & Jeffrey, D. (1996). Learning chologia, 52, 117–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia
improved by arts training. Nature, 381, 284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ .2013.09.025
381284a0 Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory
Gouzouasis, P., Guhn, M., & Kishor, N. (2007). The predictive relationship (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral
between achievement and participation in music and achievement in and Brain Sciences, 30, 135–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140
core Grade 12 academic subjects. Music Education Research, 9, 81–92. 525X07001185
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14613800601127569 Kaviani, H., Mirbaha, H., Pournaseh, M., & Sagan, O. (2014). Can music
Graziano, A. B., Peterson, M., & Shaw, G. L. (1999). Enhanced learning lessons increase the performance of preschool children in IQ tests?
of proportional math through music training and spatial-temporal train- Cognitive Processing, 15, 77– 84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-
ing. Neurological Research, 21, 139 –152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ 0574-0
01616412.1999.11740910 Kinney, D. W. (2008). Selected demographic variables, school music
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & Gajewski, B. (2012). Recent and past musical activity participation, and achievement test scores of urban middle school stu-
predicts cognitive aging variability: Direct comparison with general dents. Journal of Research in Music Education, 56, 145–161. http://dx
lifestyle activities. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 198. http://dx .doi.org/10.1177/0022429408322530
.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00198 Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes
Hanna-Pladdy, B., & MacKay, A. (2011). The relation between instrumen- prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behav-
tal musical activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology, 25, 378 –386. ior, 31, 354 –364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021895 .004
Hannon, E. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: Effects of Lee, Y., Lu, M., & Ko, H. (2007). Effects of skill training on working
enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive memory capacity. Learning and Instruction, 17, 336 –344. http://dx.doi
Sciences, 11, 466 – 472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008 .org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.02.010
MUSIC, ACADEMICS, AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 165

Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions Portowitz, A., Lichtenstein, O., Egorova, L., & Brand, E. (2009). Under-
of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of lying mechanisms linking music education and cognitive modifiability.
Developmental Psychology, 21, 59 – 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ Research Studies in Music Education, 31, 107–128. http://dx.doi.org/10
026151003321164627 .1177/1321103X09344378
Littell, R. C., Stroup, W. W., & Freund, R. J. (2002). SAS for linear models. Putnam, R. D. (2015). Our kids: The American dream in crisis. New York,
Cary, NC: SAS Institute. NY: Simon & Schuster.
Loehr, J. D., Kourtis, D., Vesper, C., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Robinson, K., & Aronica, L. (2015). Creative schools: The grassroots
Monitoring individual and joint action outcomes in duet music perfor- revolution that’s transforming education. New York, NY: Viking Press.
mance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1049 –1061. http://dx.doi Roden, I., Grube, D., Bongard, S., & Kreutz, G. (2014). Does music
.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00388 training enhance working memory performance? Findings from a quasi-
Marcovitch, S., Leigh, J., Calkins, S. D., Leerks, E. M., O’Brien, M., & experimental longitudinal study. Psychology of Music, 42, 284 –298.
Blankson, A. N. (2010). Moderate vagal withdrawal in 3.5-year-old http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735612471239
children is associated with optimal performance on executive function Santos-Luiz, C., Mónico, L. S. M., Almeida, L. S., & Coimbra, D. (2015).
tasks. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 603– 608. http://dx.doi.org/10 Exploring the long-term associations between adolescents’ music train-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.1002/dev.20462 ing and academic achievement. Musicae Scientiae, 20, 512–527.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

McCarthy, D. (1972). Manual for the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological
Abilities. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Science, 15, 511–514. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004
McGraw-Hill. (2008). Introducing TerraNova: The New standard in .00711.x
achievement (3rd ed.). New York, NY: The McGraw-Hill Companies. Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Long-term positive associations between music
Miksza, P. (2010). Investigating relationships between participation in high lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 457– 468. http://
school music ensembles and extra-musical outcomes: An analysis of the dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.457
Education Longitudinal Study of 2002 using a bioecological develop- Schellenberg, E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music
ment model. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 283–302.
186, 7–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A.,
Schellenberg, E. G., Corrigall, K. A., Dys, S. P., & Malti, T. (2015). Group
& Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions
music training and children’s prosocial skills. PLoS ONE, 10, e0141449.
and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent vari-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141449
able analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 –100. http://dx.doi.org/10
Schellenberg, E. G., & Mankarious, M. (2012). Music training and emotion
.1006/cogp.1999.0734
comprehension in childhood. Emotion, 12, 887– 891. http://dx.doi.org/
Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., &
10.1037/a0027971
Chau, T. (2011). Short-term music training enhances verbal intelligence
Schellenberg, E. G., & Peretz, I. (2008). Music, language and cognition:
and executive function. Psychological Science, 22, 1425–1433. http://
Unresolved issues. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 45– 46. http://dx
dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611416999
.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.11.005
Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., & Besson,
Schellenberg, E. G., & Weiss, M. W. (2013). Music and cognitive abilities.
M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old
In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music (pp. 499 –550). New York,
children: More evidence for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 712–
NY: Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-381460-9.00012-2
723. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn120
Scott, L. (1992). Attention and perseverance behaviors of preschool chil-
Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building
dren enrolled in Suzuki violin lessons and other activities. Journal of
Language (PEBL) and PEBL Test Battery. Journal of Neuroscience
Methods, 222, 250 –259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10 Research in Music Education, 40, 225–235. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
.024 3345684
National Governors Association Center for Best Practices. (2010). Com- Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical
mon Core state standards. Washington, DC: National Governors Asso- Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 298, 199 –209. http://dx
ciation Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers. .doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1982.0082
Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal Slevc, L. R., Davey, N. S., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2016). Tuning
and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuro- the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and exec-
science, 7, 75–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1165 utive functions. Cognition, 152, 199 –211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
Parsad, B., & Spiegelman, M. (2012). Arts education in public elementary .cognition.2016.03.017
and secondary schools: 1999 –2000 and 2009 –10 (NCES 2012– 014). Southgate, D. E., & Roscigno, V. J. (2009). The impact of music on
Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of childhood and adolescent achievement. Social Science Quarterly, 90,
Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. 4 –21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2009.00598.x
Phillips, L. H., Wynn, V., Gilhooly, K. J., Della Sala, S., & Logie, R. H. Tallmadge, G. K. (1977). The Joint Dissemination Review Panel
(1999). The role of memory in the Tower of London task. Memory, 7, IDEABOOK. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
209 –231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/741944066 Troyer, A. K., Leach, L., & Strauss, E. (2006). Aging and response
Pont, G. (2004). Philosophy and science of music in ancient Greece: The inhibition: Normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test. Aging, Neuro-
predecessors of Pythagoras and the contribution. Nexus Network Jour- psychology, and Cognition, 13, 20 –35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
nal, 6, 17–29. 138255890968187
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion of self-
74, 116 –143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009 regulation as a means of enhancing school readiness and early achieve-
Porges, S. W., & Lewis, G. F. (2009). The polyvagal hypothesis: Common ment in children at risk for school failure. Child Development Perspec-
mechanisms mediating autonomic regulation, vocalizations, and listen- tives, 6, 122–128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00209.x
ing. In S. M. Brudzynski (Ed.), Handbook of mammalian vocalizations: Vaughn, K., & Winner, E. (2000). SAT scores of students who study the
An integrative neuroscience approach (pp. 255–264). Amsterdam, the arts: What we can and cannot conclude about the association. Journal of
Netherlands: Academic Press. Aesthetic Education, 34, 77– 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3333638
166 HOLOCHWOST ET AL.

Weber, E. W., Spychiger, M., & Patry, J.-L. (1993). Music makes the surement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 12, 69 –95.
school. Essen, Germany: Verlag Blaue Eule. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15366367.2014.929453
Weiland, C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Impacts of a prekindergarten Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., Blair, C. B., & Investigators, F. L. P., &
program on children’s mathematics, language, literacy, executive func- the Family Life Project Investigators. (2012). Executive function in early
tion, and emotional skills. Child Development, 84, 2112–2130. http://dx childhood: Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental
.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12099 change. Psychological Assessment, 24, 418 – 431. http://dx.doi.org/10
Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991). A normative- .1037/a0025779
developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal Winner, E., & Cooper, M. (2000). Mute those claims: No evidence (yet) for
function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 131–149. a causal link between arts study and academic achievement. Journal of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565649109540483 Aesthetic Education, 34, 11–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3333637
Wetter, O. E., Koerner, F., & Schwaninger, A. (2009). Does musical Zuk, J., Benjamin, C., Kenyon, A., & Gaab, N. (2014). Behavioral and
training improve school performance? Instructional Science, 37, 365– neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-
374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11251-008-9052-y musicians. PLoS ONE, 9, e99868. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal
Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. (2002). The role of retrieval structures in .pone.0099868
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

memorizing music. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 1–32. http://dx.doi.org/10


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.1006/cogp.2001.0759 Received December 15, 2015


Willoughby, M., Holochwost, S. J., Blanton, Z. E., & Blair, C. B. (2014). Revision received October 24, 2016
Executive functions: Formative versus reflective measurement. Mea- Accepted November 15, 2016 䡲

Correction to Czechowski et al. (2016)


In the article, “Like a Rolling Stone: A Mixed-Methods Approach to Linguistic Analysis of Bob
Dylan’s Lyrics” by Konrad Czechowski, Dave Miranda, and John Sylvestre (Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2016, Vol. 10, No. 1, pp. 99 –113. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
aca0000045), Dylan’s album Modern Times (2006) has been mistakenly coded as a 2001 release.
Results in the two last artistic periods of Dylan have thus been corrected (see corrected Tables 2,
3 and 4, as well as Figures 1 and 2). These corrections require nuances regarding some of the more
detailed interpretations of year-related trends in the categorical perspective. In sum, overall, our
main conclusions have not changed.

The online version of this article has been corrected.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/aca0000134

View publication stats

You might also like