You are on page 1of 11

Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

Associations between psychosocial factors and generalized


pathological internet use in Chinese university students:
A longitudinal cross-lagged analysis
Yu Tian, PhD a, Yulong Bian, PhD b, Piguo Han, PhD a, Fengqiang Gao a, *, Peng Wang a, **
a
Department of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, China
b
Department of Computer Science and Technology, Shandong University, China

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This short-term longitudinal study examined the reciprocal associations among preexisting psychosocial
Received 22 November 2016 variables (shyness, loneliness, and interpersonal relationships), maladaptive cognitions, and generalized
Received in revised form pathological Internet use (GPIU) in a Chinese sample. A total of 361 university freshmen (138 males;
16 February 2017
mean age ¼ 18.57 years) participated in the study. Cross-lagged panel analysis indicated that (i) the
Accepted 20 February 2017
Available online 23 February 2017
associations among shyness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU were dynamic and bidirectional, and the
increased maladaptive cognitions bidirectionally mediated the relation between shyness and GPIU across
time; (ii) the association between interpersonal relationships and GPIU was dynamic; and (iii) the as-
Keywords:
GPIU
sociation between loneliness and GPIU was dynamic and bidirectional. Additionally, (iv) shyness and
Shyness GPIU had higher stability than interpersonal relationships, loneliness, and maladaptive cognitions did
Loneliness and (v) gender differences were identified in GPIU (T1 and T2). Interventions for GPIU and implications
Interpersonal relationships for future studies were discussed.
Maladaptive cognitions © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Cross-lagged analysis

1. Introduction 
Niculovic, Zivkovi  
c, Manasijevic, & Strbac, 2014).
The anonymity and absence of nonverbal and demographic cues
Using the Internet has become one of the most popular leisure- provided by the Internet can be beneficial to adolescents' well-
time activities among adolescents aged 10e21 years in China (Liu, being by offering relief from emotional distress (Caplan & Turner,
Fang, Deng, & Zhang, 2012). More than 287 million adolescents 2007), as well as enhancing perceptions of social support and
are Internet users, making this age cohort the largest population of self-esteem (Kraut et al., 2002) and expanding the ranges of
Internet users (China Internet Network Information Center interpersonal relationships (Cotten, 2008). However, pathological
[CNNIC], 2016). Previous research in China has reported patholog- Internet use (PIU), which is characterized by excessive or compul-
ical Internet use and dependency in approximately 8% of adoles- sive Internet use and a preoccupation with and loss of control over
cents (Fan et al., 2008; Lei & Yang, 2007; Liu et al., 2012; Luo & Peng, this use, results in negative personal and professional conse-
2008). Concern about the large number of adolescents using the quences (Caplan, 2002; Davis, 2001) and may be detrimental
Internet has led to increased research into the possible adverse (Greenfield & Yan, 2006). The number of adolescent Internet users
effects of Internet use on adolescent well-being, academic in China has increased rapidly from 120 million in 2002 to 287
achievement, and socialization (Chen, 2012; Fan et al., 2008; Lei & million as of the time of publication (CNNIC, 2016), placing growing
Yang, 2007; Liu et al., 2012; Lu & Yeo, 2015; Luo & Peng, 2008; numbers at the risk of developing PIU. This large population and the
increasing trend indicate that investigating how adolescents ac-
quire PIU and developing effective intervention techniques for PIU
* Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, are imperative. A number of studies have demonstrated that ado-
No. 88, East Wenhua Road, Ji'nan, Shandong, 250014, China. lescents with PIU usually suffer from loneliness (Engelberg &
** Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, lu,
Sjoberg, 2004; Turkle, 2011; Young, 1998), shyness (Erog
No. 88, East Wenhua Road, Ji'nan, Shandong, 250014, China.
E-mail addresses: 1427329793@qq.com (Y. Tian), bianyulong_007@hotmail.com Pamuk, & Pamuk, 2013), poor interpersonal relationships (Odaci
(Y. Bian), hanpiguo1215@163.com (P. Han), gaofengqiang@sdnu.edu.cn (F. Gao), & Çikrikçi, 2014; Sanders, Field, Diego, & Kaplan, 2000), cognitive
pengsdnu@163.com (P. Wang).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.048
0747-5632/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188 179

distortion (Lu & Yeo, 2015) and other decreases in well-being (Liu Although numerous studies have shown a significant relation-
et al., 2012; Lu & Yeo, 2015). Therefore, investigating the detailed ship between preexisting psychosocial variables (shyness, loneli-
relationships of these variables and PIU is necessary. ness, and interpersonal relationships), maladaptive cognitions, and
GPIU, few of them have directly tested the directionality of this
association. For example, most studies examining the links be-
2. Literature review
tween shyness and GPIU have looked unidirectionally from either
shyness to GPIU (Caplan, 2002; Casale & Fioravanti, 2011), GPIU to
In identifying the etiology of PIU, the cognitive-behavioral lu et al., 2013), or from shyness to GPIU through
shyness (Erog
model offers theoretical explanations of the origins and patho-
maladaptive cognitions (Tian, Bian, Han, Wang, & Gao, 2015).
genesis of GPIU (Davis, 2001, Fig. 1). Davis (2001) suggested that
However, it may be that the associations among shyness, mal-
psychopathology (such as depression, social anxiety, and substance
adaptive cognitions, and GPIU are bidirectional, with each charac-
dependence) is a distal necessary cause of symptoms of GPIU.
teristic mutually influencing another. An understanding of the
Notably, he argued that underlying psychopathology does not in
directionality of the associations among shyness, maladaptive
itself result in symptoms of GPIU, but is a necessary element in its
cognitions, and GPIU has important practical implications for
etiology. The key factors of GPIU are maladaptive cognitions, which
intervention efforts. Intervention based on incorrect assumptions
are proximal sufficient causes. Furthermore, some studies have
of the nature of the directionality of associations may prove to be
indicated that distal psychopathology renders an individual
ineffective. For example, if the goal of an intervention is to decrease
vulnerable to GPIU through maladaptive cognitions (Caplan, 2005;
GPIU and shyness is not the antecedent but the effect of decreasing
Kalkan, 2012; Li & Wang, 2013; Liu & Peng, 2009; Lu & Yeo, 2015;
maladaptive cognitions, an intervention aimed at decreasing
Mai et al., 2012). Additionally, the social context of the individual
shyness would be unlikely to decrease GPIU. If shyness and mal-
contributes to the causal pathway of GPIUdspecifically, through
adaptive cognitions influence each other over time, interventions
social isolation and a lack of social support from family and friends
can benefit from targeting both shyness and maladaptive
(Davis, 2001; Liu et al., 2012; Lu & Yeo, 2015).
cognitions.
On the basis of the cognitive-behavioral theory described by
To our knowledge, little research has addressed the direction-
Davis (2001) and previous studies (Caplan, 2005; Kalkan, 2012; Li &
ality of associations among preexisting psychosocial variables
Wang, 2013; Liu & Peng, 2009; Lu & Yeo, 2015; Mai et al., 2012),
(shyness, loneliness, and interpersonal relationships), maladaptive
preexisting psychosocial variables (shyness, loneliness, and inter-
cognitions, and GPIU. One study examined causal priority in the
personal relationships) related to GPIU were selected. Specifically,
observed empirical relationship between Internet addiction and
shyness tends to be a distal necessary cause of PIU symptoms,
loneliness. The results indicated that excessive and unhealthy
because the core trait of shyness is social anxiety (Mccabe, 2015;
Internet use increases feelings of loneliness over time (Yao &
Snyder, Smith, Augelli, & Ingram, 1985), which renders an indi-
Zhong, 2014). This study enhanced previous research by using
vidual vulnerable to GPIU through maladaptive cognitions. Addi-
Chinese samples. However, it had some limitations. First, the time
tionally, social support is a significant predictor of interpersonal
span between the two waves of surveys might have been too short
relationships and loneliness (Solomon, Bensimon, Greene, Horesh,
to detect a stable change in Internet addiction and loneliness over
& Eindor, 2015; Zhang, Gao, Fokkema, Alterman, & Liu, 2015),
time. Furthermore, because only two time points were applied in
which tend to be the causal pathway of GPIU rather than its distal
the cross-lagged model, examining the dynamic association be-
necessary cause. Therefore, this study identified maladaptive cog-
tween Internet addiction and loneliness across time was not
nitions as important predictors and proximal causes of PIU,
possible. Excessive and unhealthy Internet use may increase feel-
whereas shyness was identified as the distal necessary cause of
ings of loneliness, and increased loneliness may further increase
GPIU and interpersonal relationships and loneliness as its causal
excessive and unhealthy Internet use. Second, the observed
pathway.

Fig. 1. The cognitiveebehavioral model of pathological Internet use (PIU). Adapted from Davis (2001).
180 Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

empirical relationship between Internet addiction and loneliness males exhibit more interest in the latest information technology
was tested. This method cannot test the measurement invariance and playing games (Niculovi c et al., 2014). These different purposes
across time, which may introduce measurement error in later for Internet use may yield gender differences in specific-field PIU
analysis. Therefore, more than two waves of surveys and latent (SPIU). However, no research has comprehensively described
cross-lagged analysis are necessary. gender differences in SPIU such as daily routines, social behavior,
academic and occupational productivity, sleep patterns, feelings,
2.1. Cross lagged analysis and online activities (e.g., time spent on games, chatting, and
shopping) for a Chinese sample. Hence, additional studies are
Numerous questions in psychological research concern how two necessary to extend previous research by examining gender dif-
or more variables influence each other over time. An example of ferences in these specific fields.
such research questions is “How do rumination and anxiety influ-
ence each other?” Because of ethical limitations, many of these 2.4. The present study
questions cannot be investigated experimentally. However, cross-
lagged panel design can effectively solve these questions. In this To investigate the formation mechanisms of GPIU, and provide
approach, two or more variables are measured on two or more evidence for the prevention of GPIU in university freshmen, pre-
occasions. The cross-lagged associations between the variables existing psychosocial variables (shyness, loneliness, and interper-
over time can be examined while controlling for the effect that the sonal relationships), maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU were
variables have on themselves (i.e., the autoregression; Rogosa, combined in a cognitive-behavioral model. Furthermore, a latent
1980; Schuurman, Ferrer, De, & Hamaker, 2016). An important cross-lagged panel model design was used to investigate the as-
goal of cross-lagged panel studies is to establish causal effects using sociations among preexisting psychosocial variables (shyness,
cross-lagged regression coefficients, and then compare these as- loneliness, and interpersonal relationships), maladaptive cogni-
sociations with respect to their strength. The strongest association tions, and GPIU across time in a Chinese sample. We hypothesized
is judged to provide the most important causal influence that drives that (i) shyness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU influence each
the system, which is also referred to as being “causally dominant.” other over time and that shyness influences GPIU through mal-
This causal influence can assist determining the bidirectional as- adaptive cognitions; (ii) interpersonal relationships and GPIU in-
sociations among variables. Additionally, the autoregressive effects fluence each other over time; and (iii) loneliness and GPIU
of the cross-lagged panel model indicate how stable the individual influence each other over time. Additionally, the self-reported
differences in the scores are over time (Schuurman et al., 2016; scores for GPIU and SPIU were tested for gender difference, as
Talbot et al., 2012). well as shyness, loneliness, interpersonal relationships, and mal-
adaptive cognitions.
2.2. Freshmen of university
3. Method
The beginning of university life is a developmental period for
students. Several theorists (e.g., Collins & Steinberg, 2006; 3.1. Participants
Woodhouse, Dykas, & Jude, 2012) have proposed that university
students shift from a relatively greater reliance on parents for The initial number of young people available for participation
support and interaction to a reliance on interpersonal relationships was 500 participants (279 females and 221 males). All were uni-
(not only family relationships but also peer and other social re- versity students from two educational institutions in Eastern China.
lationships). Interpersonal relationships tend to be a central pre- The questionnaire measurements were taken at the beginning of
occupation for university students (Brown & Klute, 2003). the university year (T1), 3 months later (T2), and 6 months later
Additionally, past studies have demonstrated that heavy Internet (T3). Of the initial sample, 139 adolescents did not complete the
use is related to a decline in family communication and supervision third wave of measurements (participation rate: 72.20%). The lack
(Liu et al., 2012; Park, Kim, & Cho, 2008; Van den Eijnden, of participation was almost entirely owing to sickness or absence.
Spijkerman, Vermulst, Van Rooij, & Engles, 2010). Therefore, the Consequently, the final sample comprised 361 adolescents (223
shift in interpersonal relationships tends to place university stu- females and 138 males), with a mean age at the beginning of the
dents at risk of developing PIU. Furthermore, owing to abundant study of 18.57 years (standard deviation [SD] ¼ 0.88). A series of
leisure time and unlimited Internet access through a range of independent sample t tests was conducted to determine whether
wireless tools, contemporary university students tend to spend participating adolescents differed from nonparticipating adoles-
large amounts of time online, and are therefore very likely to cents regarding any of the variables applied in this study. None of
experience symptoms of PIU (Chen, 2012; Morahan-Martin & these analyses was significant.
Schumacher, 2000). Hence, we believe that university freshmen are A team of researchers from another city came to the university
more likely to develop GPIU than other type school students. to administer a series of self-report questionnaires. No university-
related adults were present during the questionnaire administra-
2.3. Gender difference tion, which was conducted by a researcher during one full class
period. Students were informed that no one in their university
Morahan-Martin and Schumacher (2000) suggested that path- would see their reports and that the researchers would not know
ological Internet users use the Internet for various purposes such as who the students were when processing their collective reports. At
social interaction with online-only contacts (sharing ideas or fan- the end of the session, the students were briefed about the purpose
tasies, meeting new people, talking to others with shared interests, of the research and the absolute anonymity of their identities.
seeking emotional support) and recreational Internet usage Because there were no student names included on the surveys, the
(relaxing, gambling, using adult-only resources, playing games, school staff chose to collect passive consent from parents. Thus,
using virtual reality, wasting time). However, gender differences parents were notified and given the option of refusing to allow their
also exist in Internet use purposes. For example, females engage child's participation. Parental consent forms were distributed to all
more with Web blogs and personal home pages, which can be used the students. Close to 96% of the students' parents returned the
as outlets for self-advertisement and self-expression, whereas consent forms to allow their children's participation.
Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188 181

3.2. Measures 3.3. Analytical approach

3.2.1. Shyness Three cross-lagged panel designs were employed. The first
Shyness was measured using the Chinese adaptation (Wang, model included measures of shyness, maladaptive cognitions, and
Wang, Han, Gong, & Gao, 2009) of the Cheek and Buss Shyness GPIU, the second model included measures of interpersonal re-
Scale (Cheek & Buss, 1981). The questionnaire measures shyness as lationships and GPIU, and the third model included measures of
a deficit of social skills. The Chinese version is a 17-item (instead of loneliness and GPIU. Structural equation modeling was used to test
14) self-reporting scale. Students rated their agreement with the the three hypotheses of the study. All of the models were tested
items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to through maximum likelihood estimation using MPLUS 7.0. In cross-
5 (strongly agree). A sample item for shyness is “I do not find it lagged models, change in each variable over time is modeled using
difficult to ask other people for information.” The Cronbach's a the stability coefficients between time-adjacent measures of each
values for shyness across three time points were 0.79, 0.88, and variable (e.g., T1 GPIU predicts T2 GPIU, and T2 GPIU in turn pre-
0.83. dicts T3 GPIU), and the cross-lagged relations among the three
models are captured by the cross-lagged effects between two var-
3.2.2. Interpersonal relationships iables (e.g., T1 shyness predicts T2 GPIU, and T1 GPIU in turn pre-
The 10-item scale developed by Fang, Wo, and Lin (2005) was dicts T2 shyness). We also tested the configural invariant and
used to measure the quantity of interpersonal relationships (five metric equivalence of each measure across the three occasions (e.g.,
items) and the quality of interpersonal relationships (five items). factor loadings involving the same indicator were specified to be
The participants responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 the same over time, and the errors of the same indicator were also
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample item for the specified to covary over time).
quantity of interpersonal relationships is “has trouble making Additionally, the parceling strategy of the factorial algorithm
friends” and a sample item for the quality of interpersonal re- was used to improve the quality of the indicators and model fit.
lationships is “most of my best friends do not care about my feel- Following Rogers and Schmitt (2004), factor analysis was first
ings or welfare.” Cronbach's a for the quantity of interpersonal performed, and items of each latent variable were ranked from
relationships and quality of interpersonal relationships for the highest to lowest according to the factor loading size. Each parcel
three time points were 0.89e0.94 and 0.85e0.95, respectively, was sequentially assigned the remaining items with the highest
demonstrating the reliability of these two subscales in the present and lowest rankings, alternating direction through the parcels,
sample. until all items were assigned. For example, in the case of 16 items
assigned to four parcels, Parcel #1 ¼ items ranked 1, 8, 9, and 16;
Parcel #2 ¼ items ranked 2, 7, 10, and 15; Parcel #3 ¼ items ranked
3.2.3. Loneliness
3, 6, 11, and 14; and Parcel #4 ¼ items ranked 4, 5, 12, and 13.
Loneliness was measured using the Chinese adaptation (Wang,
The fit of all the cross-lagged models was assessed using the chi-
1995) of the Cheek and Russell Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau,
squared (c2) statistic, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), comparative fit in-
& Ferguson, 1978). This scale consists of 20 self-statements con-
dex (CFI), and root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA).
cerning people's satisfaction with their interpersonal relationships.
Because the chi-squared statistic is sensitive to large samples,
The statements are answered on a 4-point scale for how often
model fit indices were used as the primary criteria to evaluate
(1 ¼ never, 4 ¼ often) subjects feel the statements apply to them-
model fit. The TLI and the CFI range between 0 and 1, with values
selves, with high scores indicating greater perceived loneliness. A
above 0.90 indicating adequate model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). A
sample item for loneliness is “I feel lonely even when I am around
rule of thumb for the RMSEA is that values  0.10 indicate poor fit,
other people.” Cronbach's a values for loneliness for the three time
values between 0.05 and 0.08 indicate reasonable error of
points were 0.88, 0.87, and 0.92.
approximation, and values  0.05 indicate close approximation
(Browne & Cudeck, 1993). The chi-squared test of difference (Dc2)
3.2.4. Maladaptive cognitions was used to compare the fit of the nested models. A nonsignificant
The 14-item scale developed by Li, Zhang, Li, Zhen, and Wang Dc2 test indicates that the two models provide an equal fit to the
(2010) was used to measure maladaptive cognitions regarding so- data, whereas a significant Dc2 suggests that the less constrained
cial convenience and coping with stress. The participants respon- model should be retained.
ded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
(strongly agree). A sample item for maladaptive cognition regarding 4. Results
social convenience was “friends in this online network are more
trustworthy than my real-world friends” and for coping with stress 4.1. Descriptive statistics and difference test
was “people can temporarily relieve pressure by playing online
games.” Cronbach's a values for social convenience and coping with The means and standard deviations of shyness, interpersonal
stress for the three time points were 0.78e0.88 and 0.85e0.91. relationships and loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU are
presented in Table 1. Independent sample t tests were conducted to
3.2.5. GPIU compare males and females through the measures of shyness,
GPIU was measured using the Chinese adaptation (Bai & Fan, interpersonal relationships and loneliness, maladaptive cognitions,
2005) of the Internet Addiction Test (Young, 1998), which as- and GPIU. The results showed that the self-reported scores of
sesses the degree to which Internet use affects people's daily rou- shyness, interpersonal relationships, and loneliness exhibited no
tines, social life, academic and occupational productivity, sleep differences with respect to gender at the three times. However,
patterns, feelings, and online activities (time spent on games, males had a higher GPIU score than females did at T1 and T2
chatting, and shopping) by asking 20 questions (e.g., “How often do (Table 1).
others in your life complain to you about the amount of time you The correlations among all variables are shown in Table 2. Cor-
spend online?”). Student rated their agreement with the items on a relations among T1, T2, and T3 measures of shyness were in the
6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (always). Cronbach's 0.61e0.71 range. Correlations among T1, T2, and T3 measures of
a values for GPIU across three time points were 0.82, 0.88, and 0.86. interpersonal relationships were in the 0.43e0.51 range.
182 Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

Table 1
Means, and standard deviations of shyness, interpersonal relationship and loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, GPIU and gender differences.

Variable Total (n ¼ 361) Male (n ¼ 138) Female (n ¼ 223) t df p

M SD M SD M SD

1. T1 Shyness 45.43 10.05 45.25 10.10 45.55 10.04 0.73 359 0.78
2. T1 IR 31.92 4.73 32.01 4.98 31.86 4.57 0.29 359 0.77
3. T1 Loneliness 39.51 8.51 38.91 8.53 39.89 8.50 1.01 359 0.29
4. T1 MC 33.90 9.35 33.94 9.67 33.88 9.00 2.85 359 0.52
5. T1 GPIU 34.95 10.08 36.17 10.40 34.20 9.82 2.10 359 <0.05
6. T2 Shyness 47.90 10.35 47.23 11.46 48.32 9.61 0.97 359 0.33
7. T2 IR 32.92 4.68 32.74 5.26 33.03 4.29 0.56 359 0.58
8. T2 Loneliness 41.51 18.88 40.71 10.24 42.01 22.64 0.63 359 0.53
9. T2 MC 34.79 10.43 35.10 11.06 34.61 10.92 2.22 359 0.87
10. T2 GPIU 36.86 11.85 38.14 13.08 36.06 11.70 1.00 359 <0.01
11. T3 Shyness 46.85 10.75 45.68 11.23 47.57 10.40 1.62 359 0.11
12. T3 IR 33.08 4.73 33.18 5.05 33.01 4.53 0.34 359 0.73
13. T3 Loneliness 41.51 9.65 40.87 10.37 41.91 9.17 0.08 359 0.32
14. T3 MC 33.83 10.67 34.70 11.35 33.30 10.21 1.22 359 0.22
15. T3 GPIU 37.68 11.78 37.62 11.57 37.71 11.93 0.71 359 0.94

Note: IR ¼ interpersonal relationship, MC ¼ maladaptive cognitions.

Table 2
Correlations of shyness, interpersonal relationship, loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU over a three-wave test.

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

1. T1 Shyness 1
2. T1 IR 0.21** 1
3. T1 Loneliness 0.54** 0.55** 1
4. T1 MC 0.43** 0.08 0.33** 1
5. T1 GPIU 0.39** 0.14** 0.32** 0.71** 1
6. T2 Shyness 0.71** 0.22** 0.50** 0.36** 0.36** 1
7. T2 IR 0.23** 0.47** 0.37** 0.09 0.11* 0.29** 1
8. T2 Loneliness 0.21** 0.24** 0.41** 0.14** 0.17** 0.23** 0.23** 1
9. T2 MC 0.35** 0.15** 0.33** 0.67** 0.63** 0.39** 0.09 0.19** 1
10. T2 GPIU 0.35** 0.17** 0.32** 0.57** 0.74** 0.35** 0.15** 0.22** 0.71 1
11. T3 Shyness 0.61** 0.21** 0.48** 0.31** 0.32** 0.68** 0.26** 0.17** 0.38** 0.35** 1
12. T3 IR 0.21** 0.43** 0.37** 0.08 0.06 0.25** 0.51** 0.23** 0.11* 0.06 0.25** 1
13.T3 Loneliness 0.40** 0.52** 0.67** 0.30** 0.31** 0.44** 0.38** 0.41** 0.34** 0.37** 0.44** 0.43** 1
14. T3 MC 0.30** 0.06 0.23** 0.60** 0.57** 0.34** 0.14** 0.12* 0.69** 0.57** 0.35** 0.12* 0.35** 1
15. T3 GPIU 0.29** 0.14** 0.31** 0.51** 0.61** 0.31** 0.15** 0.24** 0.58** 0.71** 0.40** 0.18** 0.44** 0.69** 1
*
Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
**
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Correlations among T1, T2, and T3 measures of loneliness were in two for maladaptive cognitions, and four for GPIU. The latent var-
the 0.41e0.67 range. Correlations among T1, T2, and T3 measures of iables of shyness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU were specified
maladaptive cognitions were in the 0.57e0.67 range. Correlations to covary over time. Errors of the same indicator were also specified
among T1, T2, and T3 measures of GPIU were in the 0.61e0.74 to covary over time. Additionally, factor loadings involving the
range. The associations among the measures of shyness, interper- same indicator were specified to be the same over time. To deter-
sonal relationships, loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU mine whether the measurement model could be considered
were in the expected direction. That is, shyness, loneliness, mal- invariant or equivalent over time, the model fit of two alternative
adaptive cognitions, and GPIU were positively correlated with each models (differing in number of parameter restrictions) was tested
other, and interpersonal relationships was negatively correlated and compared (Cole & Maxwell, 2003) using the corrected chi-
with shyness, loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU squared difference test (Satorra & Bentler, 2001).
(Table 2). First, a longitudinal confirmatory factor analysis was under-
Additionally, pairwise comparisons indicated that the values for taken, which included all observed and latent variables from each
interpersonal relationships, loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, and time with freely estimated parameters (all the observed indicators
GPIU at T2 were equal to those at T3. However, the values were of latent variables and the errors of each indicator were specified to
significantly greater than those at T1. Additionally, shyness at T3 covary over time). The measurement model consisted of nine latent
was significantly greater than at T2 and shyness at T2 was signifi- variables corresponding to the three variables of the study being
cantly greater than at T1 (see Table 3). measured three times. This measurement model (unconstrained
model) exhibited good fit indexes (see M1 in Table 4). This model
4.2. The cross-lagged associations among shyness, maladaptive was compared with a more restrictive model (metric invariance
cognitions and GPIU model) in which all the indicators of latent variable factors were
specified as being equal across time. Although the data model fit
Establishing Measurement Invariance Over Time. To test hypoth- was good (see M2 in Table 4), the chi-squared test of difference
esis (i), the measurement model's fit for the latent variables of the indicated that the model without metric invariance fit the data
study was examined. Item-parcels were used as indicators of the more closely (Dc2 ¼ 30, Ddf ¼ 18, p < 0.05). Partial metric invari-
latent variables. Four parcels were used as indicators for shyness, ance was then tested by freely estimating the first factor indicator
Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188 183

Table 3
Pairwise comparisons of shyness, interpersonal relationship and loneliness, maladaptive cognitions, GPIU over a three-wave test.

Items T1 (M, SD) T2 (M, SD) T3 (M, SD) F (1, 360) Pairwise comparisons

Shyness 45.43 (10.05) 47.90 (10.35) 49.85 (10.75) 8.40** T1 < T2 < T3
IR 31.92 (4.73) 32.92 (4.68) 33.08 (4.73) 18.99** T1 < T2 ¼ T3
Loneliness 39.51 (8.51) 41.51 (18.88) 41.51 (9.65) 26.03** T1 < T2 ¼ T3
MC 32.17 (9.35) 33.56 (10.43) 33.83 (10.67) 12.21** T1 < T2 ¼ T3
GPIU 34.95 (10.10) 37.35 (11.85) 37.68 (11.78) 28.31** T1 < T2 ¼ T3
*
Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
**
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 4 maladaptive cognitions and GPIU were bidirectional: GPIU and


Fit statistics for model comparison of cross-lagged associations among shyness, maladaptive cognitions positively predicted each other across time;
maladaptive cognitions and GPIU. the associations between shyness and maladaptive cognitions were
Model M1 M2 M3 M4 M5 dynamic and bidirectional: shyness positively predicted maladap-
tive cognitions across time; and increased T2 maladaptive cogni-
Chi-square (c2) 1557 1587 1585 1756 1843
df 459 477 475 497 494 tions (T1 < T2) positively predicted T3 shyness (T3 > T2). The
RMSEA 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.07 association between shyness and GPIU was dynamic: initial GPIU
TLI 0.93 0.98 0.98 0.94 0.93 positively predicted increased shyness at T2 (T1 < T2) (Fig. 2).
CFI 0.94 0.98 0.98 0.93 0.94
Therefore, hypothesis (i) was verified.
Note: M1 ¼ unconstrained model, M2 ¼ metric invariance model, M3 ¼ partial The above paths were indicative of some mediational mecha-
metric invariance model, M4 ¼ autoregressive model, M5 ¼ cross-lagged model. nisms. The link between T1 shyness and T3 GPIU was mediated
through the increased maladaptive cognitions at T2, and the link
between T1 GPIU and T3 shyness was mediated through the
of shyness at T1 (pairwise comparisons indicated the first in- increased maladaptive cognitions at T2. The significance of these
dicator's effect of shyness at T1 ¼ 0.41 vs. T3 ¼ 0.61, exhibiting a mediational paths was tested using bootstrapping. With this aim,
maximum invariance). A good model fit was achieved for the par- 1000 bootstrap samples were created from the original data set by
tial metric invariance model (see M3 in Table 4) and an equally good random sampling with replacement. According to the proposal of
fit to the data was achieved with the unconstrained model Shrout and Bolger (2002), a mediational effect is significant at the
(Dc2 ¼ 28, Ddf ¼ 16, p ¼ 0.11). Thus, the partial metric invariance 0.05 level if the 95% confidence level does not include zero. The
model was used in the subsequent analysis. results indicate that all mediational paths were statistically signif-
Autoregressive Model. The next step was to test an autoregressive icant: the indirect effect of T1 shyness on T3 GPIU through T2
model, which included autoregressive paths between the T1, T2, maladaptive cognitions (95% CI: 0.12e0.15) and the indirect effect
and T3 measures of shyness, interpersonal relationships, and GPIU. of T1 GPIU on T3 shyness through T2 maladaptive cognitions
All autoregressive paths were significant and are displayed in Fig. 2. (0.15e0.19). Thus, maladaptive cognitions played a bidirectional
The fit indices for the autoregressive model were excellent (see M4 mediating role between shyness and GPIU across time.
in Table 4).
Hypothesized Cross-Lagged Model. Building on the above autor- 4.3. The cross-lagged associations between interpersonal
egressive model, the fit of the hypothesized model was tested. It relationship and GPIU
included cross-lagged predictive paths from shyness and mal-
adaptive cognitions to GPIU, from maladaptive cognitions and GPIU Establishing Measurement Invariance Over Time. The measure-
to shyness, and from shyness and GPIU to maladaptive cognitions ment model consisted of six latent variables corresponding to the
across time. The fit indices for this model were excellent (see M5 in two variables of the study measured three times; two parcels were
Table 4). The results indicated that the associations between used as indicators for interpersonal relationships, and four parcels

Fig. 2. Cross-lagged associations among shyness, maladaptive cognitions and GPIU. Note: MC ¼ maladaptive cognitions. Only significant associations are displayed. Coefficients
represent standardized values. *Coefficient is significant at the 0.05 level. **Coefficient is significant at the 0.01 level.
184 Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

Table 5 measures of loneliness and GPIU. All autoregressive paths were


Fit statistics for model comparison of cross-lagged associations between interper- significant and are displayed in Fig. 4. Fit indices were excellent for
sonal relationship and GPIU.
the autoregressive model (see M3 in Table 6).
Model M1 M2 M3 M4 Hypothesized Cross-Lagged Model. Building on the above autor-
Chi-square (c2) 517 528 509 505 egressive model, the fit of the hypothesized model was tested. It
df 120 133 134 130 included cross-lagged predictive paths from loneliness to GPIU, and
RMSEA 0.05 0.05 0.05 0.06 from GPIU to loneliness across time. Fit indices were excellent for
TLI 0.94 0.98 0.95 0.95
this model (see M4 in Table 6). The results indicate that the asso-
CFI 0.95 0.98 0.96 0.94
ciation between loneliness and GPIU was dynamic and bidirec-
Note: M1 ¼ unconstrained model, M2 ¼ metric invariance model,
tional: loneliness positively predicted GPIU across time, whereas
M3 ¼ autoregressive model, M4 ¼ cross-lagged model.
increased GPIU at T2 negatively predicted later loneliness (Fig. 4).
Thus, hypothesis (iii) was verified.
for GPIU. This measurement model showed good fit indexes (see
M1 in Table 5). Additionally, a good model fit was also achieved for 5. Discussion
the metric invariance model (see M2 in Table 5), fitting the data
equally as well as the unconstrained model did (Dc2 ¼ 11, Ddf ¼ 13, 5.1. The reciprocal associations among shyness, maladaptive
p ¼ 0.31). cognitions and GPIU
Autoregressive Model. The next step was to test an autoregressive
model, which included autoregressive paths between the T1, T2, The present study revealed that maladaptive cognitions played a
and T3 measures of interpersonal relationships and GPIU. All bidirectional mediating role between shyness and GPIU, which
autoregressive paths were significant and are displayed in Fig. 3. Fit indicates that shyness and GPIU influence each other through
indices were excellent for the autoregressive model (see M3 in maladaptive cognitions across time. In other words, individuals
Table 5). with severe maladaptive cognitions may suffer from more severe
Hypothesized Cross-Lagged Model. Building on the above autor- shyness and GPIU than others. According to the model described by
egressive model, we tested the fit of the hypothesized model. It Davis (2001), the cognitive dysfunctions are a sufficient determi-
included cross-lagged predictive paths from interpersonal re- nant of subsequent symptoms of GPIU. These symptoms, in turn,
lationships to GPIU, and from GPIU to interpersonal relationships feed back into the subsequent cognitive dysfunctions and shyness.
across time. The fit indices for this model were excellent (see M4 in Generally, this finding was consistent with most previous research
Table 5). The results indicate that the associations between inter- findings that maladaptive cognitions play a crucial role in devel-
personal relationships and GPIU were dynamic: initial interper- oping PIU (Caplan, 2005; Kalkan, 2012; Li & Wang, 2013; Liu &
sonal relationships negatively predicted later increased GPIU Peng, 2009; Lu & Yeo, 2015; Mai et al., 2012).
(T1 < T2); however, increased GPIU negatively predicted final Importantly, GPIU was found to render an individual vulnerable
interpersonal relationships (Fig. 3). Therefore, hypothesis (ii) was to shyness through maladaptive cognitions across time. This is
verified. because individuals with PIU were more satisfied than others with
their Internet usage because they felt that the Internet is more
entertaining, fun, and interactive, helping them to escape from
4.4. The cross-lagged associations between loneliness and GPIU their real-world responsibilities and identity (Chou & Hsiao, 2000).
However, maladaptive cognitions increased later shyness and GPIU.
Establishing Measurement Invariance Over Time. The measure- Additionally, shyness was also found to renders an individual
ment model consisted of six latent variables corresponding to the vulnerable to GPIU through maladaptive cognitions across time.
two variables of the study measured three times; two parcels were Shy people may find it difficult to show warmth and empathy with
used as indicators for loneliness, four parcels for GPIU. This mea- other people in face-to face-conversation owing to their having
surement model showed good fit indices (see M1 in Table 6). poorer interpersonal skills. They may also believe that they are less
Additionally, a good model fit was achieved for the metric invari- liked and accepted by their peers. However, the Internet may also
ance model (see M2 in Table 6), fitting the data equally as well as provide them with anonymity and an absence of nonverbal and
the unconstrained model did (Dc2 ¼ 18, Ddf ¼ 11, p ¼ 0.07). Thus, demographic cues that can facilitate their communication,
the unconstrained model was then used for the subsequent relieving them the anxiety of face-to-face interaction, increasing
analysis. their sense of comfort and social control (Caplan, 2002; Tian et al.,
Autoregressive Model. The next step was to test an autoregressive 2015). However, the apparent benefits may be misleading (i.e.,
model, which included autoregressive paths between T1, T2, and T3 maladaptive cognitions), further increasing later shyness and GPIU.

Fig. 3. Cross-lagged associations between interpersonal relationship and GPIU. Note: IR ¼ interpersonal relationship. Only significant associations are displayed. Coefficients
represent standardized values. *Coefficient is significant at the 0.05 level. **Coefficient is significant at the 0.01 level.
Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188 185

Table 6 assigned a special importance in adolescence, which continues to


Fit statistics for model comparison of cross-lagged associations between loneliness be regarded as a period when loneliness is particularly prevalent. In
and GPIU.
particular, for freshmen who are shifting from a stage wherein they
Model M1 M2 M3 M4 relatively heavily rely on their parents for support to a stage
Chi-square (c2) 401 419 403 417 wherein they generally rely on peer relationships and other social
df 120 131 123 125 relationships, the decline in communication with their parents may
RMSEA 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.05 cause them to suffer from more loneliness and reduced interper-
TLI 0.97 0.97 0.95 0.95
sonal interaction than they do during other periods of their
CFI 0.96 0.96 0.96 0.97
adolescence. However, the Internet significantly enhances their
Note: M1 ¼ unconstrained model, M2 ¼ metric invariance model,
sense of identity, enables closer relationships with friends, and
M3 ¼ autoregressive model, M4 ¼ cross-lagged model.
increases their bonding with people worldwide, which makes them
more likely to use the Internet to develop feelings of belonging and
Additionally, the positive direct effect of GPIU on later increased companionship (Chou, 2001). Gradually, they become more drawn
shyness was also verified. The attraction of the Internet for shy to their online activity, using it as a means to diminish or escape
individuals may be that it enables them to disguise their identities from their struggles with loneliness. This further isolates them
easily and avoid experiencing anxiety in social interactions (Erog lu from interpersonal interactions and thereby causes them to suffer
et al., 2013). However, The more time they spend online, the less more loneliness. Furthermore, the more time they spend online,
time they engage in offline social interactions. This could eventually the less time they engage in offline social interactions, and they
lead to a strong dependency on the Internet, which may lead to may miss out on opportunities to learn interpersonal skills. This
their becoming alienated from normal socialization, thereby could eventually lead to them experiencing rejection and being
perpetuating reduced social and communication skills (Durkee disliked by other people, thereby perpetuating their poor inter-
et al., 2012). Consequently, they may experience rejection in so- personal relationships.
cial activities and suffer increased social anxiety (Ehrensaft, 2005; However, the association between loneliness and GPIU was not
Wentzel & Mcnamara, 1999), which in turn exacerbates their consistent with the findings of previous studies. Yao and Zhong
shyness. (2014) found that excessive and unhealthy Internet use can in-
crease feelings of loneliness over time. This finding may have been
5.2. The reciprocal associations between interpersonal relationships caused by their adopting a different concept of Internet use. In our
and GPIU, loneliness and GPIU study, Internet use was considered as a social phenomenon,
whereas Yao and Zhong focused on people's mental and emotional
According to cognitive-behavioral model, interpersonal re- states, which are adversely affected by the overuse of the Internet.
lationships and loneliness tend to be the causal pathway of GPIU In addition, a different sample was used. University freshmen with
(Davis, 2001). However, this study does not conclusively demon- a mean age of 18.57 years were followed in our study, whereas the
strate that interpersonal relationships and loneliness are causal mean age of Yao and Zhongs' sample was 21.63. The associations
pathways of GPIU, owing to their associations being dynamic and between loneliness and GPIU are dynamic, and therefore, different
bidirectional. Some studies have identified that the cycle of inter- ages may yield different associations. Third, different analysis
personal relationships, loneliness, and PIU involves individual with methods were used. In the present study, the measurement
poor interpersonal relationships and loneliness using the Internet invariance of Internet addiction and loneliness was tested at first in
to compensate for offline social interaction thereby developing PIU. a latent variable model, which can ensure invariance in the mea-
PIU and lack of social interactions in real life then exacerbate their surement of Internet addiction and loneliness across time. By
feelings of loneliness and poor interpersonal relationships (Lu & contrast, the observed Internet addiction and loneliness were
Yeo, 2015; Yao & Zhong, 2014). All these mechanisms may applied directly in Yao and Zhongs' study. This method cannot
contribute to the creation of a “snowball” or “cascade” effect at the ensure the invariance of the measurement of Internet addiction and
beginning of university life, which is a developmental stage at loneliness across time, and may thereby introduce an error in later
which the occurrence of poor interpersonal relationships and cross-lagged analysis.
loneliness predicts increased GPIU. GPIU in turn increases the
likelihood of experiencing further poor interpersonal relationships 5.3. Gender difference of GPIU
and loneliness. Consequently, GPIU stabilizes into trait-like risks
(0.67 / 0.69), whereas interpersonal relationships (0.46 / 0.43) Gender differences were found in GPIU (T1 and T2). To further
and loneliness tend toward unstable “state-like” risks investigate the gender differences, a series of independent t tests
(0.39 / 0.35). were conducted on daily routines, social life, academic and occu-
Heinrich and Gullone (2006) suggested that loneliness is pational productivity, sleep patterns, feelings, and online activities

Fig. 4. Cross-lagged associations between loneliness and GPIU. Note: Only significant associations are displayed. Coefficients represent standardized values. *Coefficient is significant
at the 0.05 level. **Coefficient is significant at the 0.01 level.
186 Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

(time spent on games, chatting, and shopping). The results reveal should be addressed in future research. First, longitudinal studies
that females spend more time on chatting, whereas males spend that span a longer study period, especially beginning earlier in
more time on games; no gender difference were identified in other university adolescence, are needed in order to clarify long-term
fields. These results are consistent with previous research findings stabilities, as well as developmental changes in the associations
(McAndrew & Jeong, 2012; Niculovi c et al., 2014; Tan & Yang, 2014; among preexisting psychosocial variables (shyness, loneliness, or
Tifferet & Vilnai-Yavetz, 2014). This because could be interpreted interpersonal relationships), maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU.
according to gender stereotypes, in which typical male character- Participants in the current study were followed over the course of
istics are considered to consist of independence, autonomy and half a year. Longer studies with longer intervals may further
self-sufficiency in contrary to typical female characteristics such as elucidate transactional development among preexisting psycho-
interdependence, nurture, and concern for others (e.g., Chodorow, social variables (shyness, loneliness, or interpersonal relationships),
1978; Cross & Markus, 1993). In this view, female interpersonal maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU.
dependency is higher than that of males (Sanathara, Gardner, Second, although self-reporting is useful in longitudinal
Prescott, & Kendler, 2003). In particular, university students shift research, we cannot rule out the possibility that the results may be
from a relatively greater reliance on parents for support and influenced by the issue of shared-method variance because all the
interaction to a reliance on not only family relationships, but also study variables were obtained from self-reports. Future research
on peer relationships and social relationships; some freshman may that incorporates data from other informants or sources, such as
suffer from poor interpersonal relationships and loneliness. In this teacher, parental, or peer reports are needed to verify the validity of
situation, females are more likely to communicate online with the present findings.
family and peers for social support, whereas males are more willing Finally, this study was conducted in a Chinese cultural context.
to spend large amounts of time on games for entertainment Although it extends our understanding and sheds light on cultural
(Hupfer & Detlor, 2007; Niculovi c et al., 2014). differences, its generalizability must be further explored. For
example, a recent study found that Koreans are more likely to use
6. Implications for prevention of GPIU social networking sites to increase their social contacts but tended
to spend less time browsing contacts' profiles than Malaysians and
These findings have implications for the treatment and pre- Chinese Internet users do (Lee, Kim, Golden, Kim, & Park, 2016).
vention of GPIU in university freshmen. The results show a trans- Thus, different cultures may encourage different types of GPIU and
actional, dynamic set of associations among shyness, interpersonal exhibit different GPIU formation mechanisms.
relationships and loneliness which combine to produce a feedback
loop that perpetuates GPIU. This finding suggests that interventions Appendix A. Supplementary data
(including those aimed at maladaptive cognitions) can break this
chain in various locations. Cognitive behavioral therapy indicates Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http://
that cognitive restructuring can be applied as therapeutic strategies dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.048.
regarding Internet applications (Davis, 2001; Oikawa et al., 2012).
For example, Young and Case (2004) found that education and References
training about the risks of excessive or compulsive Internet use can
alleviate many symptoms of GPIU. Additionally, interventions Bai, X., & Fan, F. M. (2005). A study on the internet dependence of college students:
should target initial interpersonal relationships and loneliness. The revising and applying of a measurement. Psychological Development and
Education, 4, 99e104. http://dx.doi.org/1001-4918(2005)04-0099-104.
Previous studies have found that decreased social interaction is Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In
related to poor interpersonal relationships and loneliness. Thus, K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136e162).
periodic collective counseling and collective activities have been Newbury Park, CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0049124192021002005.
Brown, B. B., & Klute, C. (2003). Friendships, cliques, and crowds. In G. R. Adams, &
advocated, particularly for university freshmen experiencing a
M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 330e348).
decline in family communication and supervision. These activities Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
can assist students to engage in positive interactions such as Caplan, S. E. (2002). Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: Devel-
opment of a theory-based cognitiveebehavioral measurement instrument.
respecting, encouraging, and helping each other, thereby facili-
Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 553e575. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0747-
tating positive interpersonal relationships and reducing loneliness. 5632(02)00004-3.
Additionally, shyness (0.61e0.66) and GPIU (0.66e0.69) had a Caplan, S. E. (2005). A social skill account of problematic internet use. Journal of
higher level of stability than interpersonal relationships Communication, 55(4), 721e736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2005.
tb03019.x.
(0.43e0.46), loneliness (0.35e0.39) and maladaptive cognitions Caplan, S. E., & Turner, J. S. (2007). Bringing theory to research on computer-
(0.44e0.57) did. These results indicated shyness, loneliness, inter- mediated comforting communication. Computers in Human Behavior, 23,
personal relationships, maladaptive cognitions, and GPIU were 985e998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2005.08.003.
Casale, S., & Fioravanti, G. (2011). Psychosocial correlates of internet use among
somewhat stable at the beginning of university life, suggesting that italian students. International Journal of Psychology, 46(4), 288e298. http://dx.
emerging individual differences persist over time (Guo, Sun, doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2010.541256.
Breitsmith, Morrison, & Connor, 2014). It is also noteworthy that Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 41, 330e339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.330.
shyness and GPIU had a comparatively higher level of stability than Chen, S. K. (2012). Internet use and psychological well-being among college stu-
loneliness, interpersonal relationships, and maladaptive cognitions dents: A latent profile approach. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(6),
did. A construct with a higher level of stability is considered to be 2219e2226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.029.
China Internet Network Information Center (CNNIC). (2016). Statistical report on the
more “trait-like,” whereas a construct with a lower level of stability development of internet in China (p. 8). http://www.cnnic.net.cn/hlwfzyj/
is more “state-like” (Kenny & Zautra, 2001, p. 243). Therefore, the hlwxzbg/qsnbg/ (in Chinese).
treatment and prevention of GPIU should be carried out as early as Chodorow, N. J. (1978). The reproduction of mothering: Psychoanalysis and the soci-
ology of gender. Berkeley: University of California Press.
possible at the beginning of university life.
Chou, C. (2001). Internet heavy use and addiction among taiwanese college stu-
dents: An online interview study. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 4(5), 573e585.
7. Limitations and future directions http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/109493101753235160.
Chou, C., & Hsiao, M. C. (2000). Internet addiction, usage, gratification, and pleasure
experience: The Taiwan college students' case. Computers & Education, 35,
Although the current study elucidated a mechanism for the 65e80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0360-1315(00)00019-1.
development of GPIU in China, it has a number of limitations that Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing meditational models with longitudinal
Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188 187

data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Lu, X., & Yeo, K. J. (2015). Pathological internet use among Malaysia university
Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 558e577. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X. students: Risk factors and the role of cognitive distortion. Computers in Human
112.4.558. Behavior, 45, 235e242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.021.
Collins, W. A., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal Mai, Y., Hu, J., Yan, Z., Zhen, S., Wang, S., & Zhang, W. (2012). Structure and function
context. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. Lerner (Eds.) (6th ed.,Social, emotional of maladaptive cognitions in pathological internet use among Chinese adoles-
and personality developmentHandbook of child psychology:Vol. 3 (pp. cents. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(6), 2376e2386. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1003e1068). New York: Wiley http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658. 1016/j.chb.2012.07.009.
chpsy0316. McAndrew, F. T., & Jeong, H. S. (2012). Who does what on Facebook? Age, sex, and
Cotten, S. R. (2008). Students' technology use and the impacts on well-being. New relationship status as predictors of Facebook use. Computers in Human Behavior,
Directions for Student Services, 124, 55e70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ss.295. 28(6), 2359e2365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.007.
Cross, S. E., & Markus, H. R. (1993). Gender in thought, belief, and action: A cognitive Mccabe, R. E. (2015). Review of the mindfulness and acceptance workbook for social
approach. In A. E. Beall, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The psychology of gender (pp. anxiety and shyness: Using acceptance and commitment therapy to free
55e98). New York: Guilford Press. yourself from fear and reclaim your life. Canadian Psychology, 56(1), 152e153.
Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitiveeBehavioral model of pathological internet use. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038090.
Computers in Human Behavior, 17(2), 187e195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0747- Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (2000). Incidence and correlates of patho-
5632(00)00041-8. logical internet use among college students. Computers in Human Behavior,
Durkee, T., Kaess, M., Carli, V., Parzer, P., Wasserman, C., Floderus, B., et al. (2012). 16(1), 13e29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0747-5632(99)00049-7.
Prevalence of pathological internet use among adolescents in europe: De- Niculovi 
c, M., Zivkovi 
c, D., Manasijevi 
c, D., & Strbac, N. (2014). Study of pathological
mographic and social factors. Addiction, 107(12), 2210e2222. http://dx.doi.org/ internet use, behavior and attitudes among students population at technical
10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03946.x. faculty bor, university of belgrade. Computers in Human Behavior, 39(39), 78e87.
Ehrensaft, M. K. (2005). Interpersonal relationships and sex differences in the http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.020.
development of conduct problems. Clinical Child & Family Psychology Review, Odaci, H., & Çikrikçi, O. € (2014). Problematic internet use in terms of gender,
8(1), 39e63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007s00249-005-0021-y. attachment styles and subjective well-being in university students. Computers
Engelberg, E., & Sjoberg, L. (2004). Internet use, social skills, and adjustment. Cyber in Human Behavior, 32(1), 61e66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.11.019.
Psychology & Behavior, 7(1), 41e47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/ Oikawa, H., Kamagata, K., Arai, M., Fukasawa, A., Yokota, H., Ide, T., et al. (2012).
109493104322820101. Modelling excessive internet use:s revision of R. Davis's cognitive-behavioural
Eroglu, M., Pamuk, M., & Pamuk, K. (2013). Investigation of problematic internet model of pathological internet use. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 42(3),
usage of university students with psychosocial levels at different levels. Pro- 129e139. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10059-011-0018-6.
cedia e Social and Behavioral Sciences, 103(26), 551e557. http://dx.doi.org/10. Park, S. K., Kim, J. Y., & Cho, C. B. (2008). Prevalence of internet addiction and
1016/j.sbspro.2013.10.372. correlations with family factors among South Korean adolescents. Adolescence,
Fang, X. Y., Wo, J. Z., & Lin, X. Y. (2005). Development of chinese college student 172(43), 895e909.
adjustment scale. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 3(2), 95e101. Rogers, W. M., & Schmitt, N. (2004). Parameter recovery and model fit using
Fan, F., Su, L., Cao, F., Gao, X., Jin, Y., Xiao, H., et al. (2008). Development and psy- multidimensional composites: A comparison of four empirical parceling algo-
chometric analysis of internet addiction predict test for adolescents. Chinese rithms. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(3), 379e412. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 1e4 (in Chinese). 1207/S15327906MBR3903_1.
Greenfield, P., & Yan, Z. (2006). Children, adolescents, and the internet: A new field Rogosa, D. (1980). A critique of cross-lagged correlation. Psychological Bulletin, 88,
of inquiry in developmental psychology. Developmental Psychology, 42, 245e258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.2.245.
391e394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.391. Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of lone-
Guo, Y., Sun, S., Breitsmith, A., Morrison, F. J., & Connor, M. D. (2014). Behavioral liness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 42(3), 290e294. http://dx.doi.org/10.
engagement and reading achievement in elementary-school-age children: A 1207/s15327752jpa4203_11.
longitudinal cross-lagged analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(2), Sanathara, V. A., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2003). Interpersonal
332e347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037638. dependence and major depression: A etiological inter-relationship and gender
Heinrich, L. A., & Gullone, E. (2006). The clinical significance of loneliness: A liter- differences. Psychological Medicine, 33(5), 927e931. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
ature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 695e718. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ S0033291703007542.
j.cpr.2006.04.002. Sanders, C. E., Field, T. M., Diego, M., & Kaplan, M. (2000). The relationship of
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure internet use to depression and social isolation among adolescents. Adolescence,
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation, 6(1), 35(138), 237e242.
1e55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for
Hupfer, M. E., & Detlor, B. (2007). Beyond gender differences: Self-concept orien- moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507e514. http://dx.doi.org/10.
tation and relationship-building applications on the internet. Journal of Business 1007/BF02296192.
Research, 60, 613e619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.06.010. Schuurman, N. K., Ferrer, E., De, B. M., & Hamaker, E. L. (2016). How to compare
Kalkan, M. (2012). Productiveness of interpersonal cognitive distortions on uni- cross-lagged associations in a multilevel autoregressive model. Psychological
versity students' problematic internet use. Children and Youth Services Review, Methods, 21(2), 206e221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000062.
34(7), 1305e1308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.03.003. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental
Kenny, D. A., & Zautra, A. (2001). Trait-state models for longitudinal data. In studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4),
L. M. Collins, & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 422e445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//1082-989X.7.4.422.
243e263). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi. Snyder, C. R., Smith, T. W., Augelli, R. W., & Ingram, R. E. (1985). On the self-serving
org/10.1037/10409-008. function of social anxiety: Shyness as a self-handicapping strategy. Journal of
Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J., Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. (2002). Personality & Social Psychology, 48(4), 970e800 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 49e74. http://dx.doi.org/ 3514.48.4.970.
10.1111/1540-4560.00248. Solomon, Z., Bensimon, M., Greene, T., Horesh, D., & Eindor, T. (2015). Loneliness
Lee, S. L., Kim, J. A., Golden, K. J., Kim, J. H., & Park, S. A. (2016). A cross-cultural trajectories: The role of posttraumatic symptoms and social support. Journal of
examination of SNS usage intensity and managing interpersonal relationships Loss & Trauma, 20(20), 1e21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15325024.2013.815055.
online: The role of culture and the autonomous-related self-construal. Frontiers Talbot, L., Stone, S., Gruber, J., Hairston, I., Eidelman, P., & Harvey, A. (2012). A test of
in Psychology, 7(376), 1e12. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00376. the bidirectional association between sleep and mood in bipolar disorder and
Lei, L., & Yang, Y. (2007). The development and validation of adolescent pathological insomnia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 39e50. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Internet use scale. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(4), 688e696 (in Chinese). 1037/a0024946.
Liu, Q. X., Fang, X. Y., Deng, L. Y., & Zhang, J. T. (2012). Parenteadolescent commu- Tan, W. K., & Yang, C. Y. (2014). Internet applications use and personality. Telematics
nication, parental internet use and internet-specific norms and pathological and Informatics, 31(1), 27e38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2013.02.006.
internet use among chinese adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(4), Tian, Y., Bian, Y. L., Han, P. G., Wang, P., & Gao, F. Q. (2015). The effect of shyness on
1269e1275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.010. Internet addiction: The mediating effects of immersion tendency and network-
Liu, M., & Peng, W. (2009). Cognitive and psychological predictors of the negative related maladaptive cognition. Chinese Journal of Special Education, 12, 83e89.
outcomes associated with playing MMOGs (massively multiplayer online Tifferet, S., & Vilnai-Yavetz, I. (2014). Gender differences in Facebook self-
games). Computer in Human Behavior, 25, 1306e1311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ presentation: An international randomized study. Computers in Human
j.chb.2009.06.002. Behavior, 35(6), 388e399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.016.
Li, H., & Wang, S. (2013). The role of cognitive distortion in online game addiction Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from
among Chinese adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review, 35(9), each other. New York: Basic Books.
1468e1475. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2013.05.021. Van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Spijkerman, R., Vermulst, A. A., Van Rooij, T. J., &
Li, D., Zhang, W., Li, X., Zhen, S., & Wang, Y. (2010). Stressful life events and prob- Engles, R. C. M. E. (2010). Compulsive internet use among adolescents: Bidi-
lematic internet use by adolescent females and males: A mediated moderation rectional parentechild relationships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38,
model. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(5), 1199e1207. http://dx.doi.org/10. 77e89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9347-8.
1016/j.chb.2010.03.031. Wang, D. F. (1995). A study on the reliability and validity of the Russel Loneliness
Luo, H., & Peng, Y. (2008). Relationship of Internet addiction and family environ- Scale. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 3(1), 23e25. http://dx.doi.org/
ment and attachment of the youth. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 16(3), 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.1995.01.006.
319e321 (in Chinese). Wang, Q. Q., Wang, P., Han, L., Gong, R. Y., & Gao, F. Q. (2009). The revision of college
188 Y. Tian et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 72 (2017) 178e188

students' shyness scale and its relevant study. Psychological Science, 32(1), Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder.
204e206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1679/j.cnki.1671-6981.2009.01.048. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 1(3), 237e244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.1998.
Wentzel, K. R., & Mcnamara, C. C. (1999). Interpersonal relationships, emotional 1.237.
distress, and prosocial behavior in middle school. Journal of Early Adolescence, Young, K. S., & Case, C. J. (2004). Internet abuse in the workplace: New trends in risk
19(1), 114e125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431699019001006. management. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 7(1), 105e111. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Woodhouse, S. S., Dykas, M. J., & Jude, C. (2012). Loneliness and peer relations in 1089/109493104322820174.
adolescence. Review of Social Development, 21(2), 273e293. http://dx.doi.org/10. Zhang, B., Gao, Q., Fokkema, M., Alterman, V., & Liu, Q. (2015). Adolescent inter-
1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00611.x. personal relationships, social support and loneliness in high schools: Mediation
Yao, M. Z., & Zhong, Z. (2014). Loneliness, social contacts and internet addiction: A effect and gender differences. Social Science Research, 53, 104e117. http://dx.doi.
cross-lagged panel study. Computers in Human Behavior, 30(30), 164e170. org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2015.05.003.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.08.007.

You might also like