You are on page 1of 10

627420

research-article2016
CDQXXX10.1177/1525740115627420Communication Disorders QuarterlyLarson

Article
Communication Disorders Quarterly

Language Screening for Infants and


1­–10
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2016
Reprints and permissions:
Toddlers: A Literature Review of Four sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1525740115627420

Commercially Available Tools cdq.sagepub.com

Anne L. Larson, MS, CCC-SLP1

Abstract
Children from low-income environments are at increased risk of developing language delays which can negatively affect later
academic and social outcomes. As children age, deficits between children with language delays and their typically developing
peers continue to widen. In order to prevent future disabilities, efficient early language screening tools are needed to
identify infants and toddlers who are at risk of language delay as the first step towards providing early intervention. The
purpose of this review was to identify commercially available language screening tools for use with children under three
years of age. The psychometric properties of each tool are described—including a specific focus on technical adequacy of
the measures for use with diverse families. There are currently four tools available for use with infants and toddlers. The
strengths and limitations of each tool are described, as well as the feasibility of using these tools in diverse populations.

Keywords
birth to 3 years, age, language, assessment, delays/disorders, language/linguistics, vocabulary, literacy, diversity

Early intervention with children who are at risk for delay can language ability and academic achievement persist into
help prevent disabilities (Guralnick, 2005). The importance elementary school (Walker, Greenwood, Hart, & Carta,
of early identification is codified into law in the Individuals 1994). Children with strong vocabulary skills learn to read
With Disabilities Education Improvement Act (IDEIA, better, read more, and thus learn more.
2004), which requires all states to provide, “a comprehen- The negative outcomes associated with insufficient early
sive child find system . . . that ensures rigorous standards for language skills predicates the need to identify effective
appropriately identifying infants and toddlers with disabili- language screening tools for use with very young children.
ties for services . . . that will reduce the need for future ser- Given that 22.5% of children under 5 years are living in pov-
vices . . . ” (20 USC § 1435[a]). Early speech and language erty (Child Trends Databank, 2014), identification of early
disorders are prevalent disabilities that can cause later diffi- language screening tools that can be used in low-income
culty (Law, Boyle, Harris, Harkness, & Nye, 2000; Tomblin, populations is also highly relevant. With adequate screening
Zhang, Buckwalter, & Catts, 2000). In a longitudinal inves- tools designed for use with low-income populations, young
tigation of late-talkers who were identified between 24 and children who are at risk for language delays will have an
31 months of age, Rescorla (2009) noted significantly lower opportunity to be identified for further evaluation and may
reading and writing ability at 17 years of age. receive early intervention services when needed.
Compared with their more advantaged peers, children Although the gap in expressive language development
who are living in poverty are particularly at risk of having for children from low SES families compared with their
low language skills (see Hoff, 2006, for a review). In one of peers from higher SES has been identified in laboratory set-
the most well-known studies to examine the effects of tings at 18 months of age (Fernald, Marchman, & Weisleder,
socioeconomic status (SES) on language outcomes, Hart 2013), there is no gold standard early language screening
and Risley (1995) estimated that by age 3, children from tool for use in the community. Furthermore, there are no
upper-income families will have an expressive vocabulary
almost three times larger than their low-income counter- 1
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, USA
parts. By kindergarten, children from low SES families
often have significantly lower achievement—a problem Corresponding Author:
Anne L. Larson, Department of Educational Psychology, University
that is then pronounced by low SES students entering lower of Minnesota, 250 Education Sciences Bldg., 56 East River Road,
quality schools (Lee & Burkam, 2002). The effects of low Minneapolis, MN 55455, USA.
SES and vocabulary on child receptive and spoken Email: lars4959@umn.edu

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


2 Communication Disorders Quarterly 

specific recommendations for screening diverse popula- and quantity of referrals for complete evaluation (Rescorla,
tions of very young children. The purpose of this review is 1989). The CAT/CLAMS and ELM Scales were developed
to identify language assessment tools that can be used for for pediatricians in response to a need to improve on the
screening language skills in children under 3 years of age. Denver Developmental Screening Test, which included only
In addition to examining the technical adequacy of early 19 items relevant for the first 3 years of the child’s life
language screening tools, this review aims to discuss the (Coplan, Gleason, Ryan, Burke, & Williams, 1982). The
feasibility of using these measures with diverse, low- LDS and CDI were later developed as parent-report tools to
income populations who are most at risk of developing lan- be used in a variety of settings.
guage delays.
Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic
Method Auditory Milestone Scale (CAT/CLAMS)
I conducted a systematic review of the literature to identify The CAT/CLAMS is a standardized tool developed for use
language assessments available for use as screening by pediatricians to measure language and visual-motor
tools with very young children. The electronic databases problem-solving development in young children. Several
of Education Resource Information Center (ERIC), researchers have examined the concurrent validity of the
PsychINFO, Education Source, and Linguistics and CAT/CLAMS with comprehensive developmental assess-
Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA) were searched using ments and suggest the use of the CAT/CLAMS as a diag-
the following phrase: “screening AND language AND nostic tool (i.e., Leppert, Shank, Shapiro, & Capute, 1998;
(infant OR toddler).” Searches were restricted to peer- Macias et al., 1998; Rossman et al., 1994); however, Mohay
reviewed journals with no limits on publication dates. A (2007) suggested the CAT/CLAMS should be used as a
total of 59 articles were identified in these databases, and screening tool to identify the need for more comprehensive
an additional 11 unduplicated studies were found using the formal assessment. When only the language portion, the
same search terms in the American Journal of Speech- CLAMS, is used, administration takes less than 5 min
Language Pathology and the Journal of Speech, Language (Belcher, Gittlesohn, Capute, & Allen, 1997). Three studies
and Hearing Research. Articles that discussed the develop- focused on the CLAMS as a screening tool.
ment, psychometric properties, and/or usability of each The CLAMS includes 43 developmental language
screening tool were included but dissertations and techni- milestones for children from birth to 36 months of age.
cal reports were not. Tools were included in the review if Each item is scored (0.25 to 1) based on parent responses
they were (a) narrow-band assessments focused on lan- to standardized questions. Rather than asking parents to
guage understanding or use, (b) designed for use with chil- recall a specific age at which receptive and expressive lan-
dren under 3 years of age, (c) brief assessments administered guage skills were acquired, the CLAMS uses a recognition
and scored in under 10 minutes, and (d) commercially format (i.e., yes/no questions)—a feature that adds to the
available in English. reliability of the measure (Capute, Shapiro, Wachtel,
Four language screening instruments for children under Gunther, & Palmer, 1986). Although normative data are
36 months of age were identified in the initial search, available for the CLAMS, most administrators use the
including: the Early Language Milestone Scale (ELM CLAMS Language Quotient (LQ)—the sum of each com-
Scale-2; Coplan, 1993), The Capute Scales: Cognitive pleted language task, divided by the child’s chronologic
Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Milestone age, and multiplied by 100 (Belcher et al., 1997). Studies
Scale (CAT/CLAMS; Accardo & Capute, 2005), the in this review used a CLAMS LQ of less than 90 (Clark,
Language Development Survey (LDS; Rescorla, 1989), and Jorgensen, & Blondeau, 1995) and a CLAMS LQ of less
the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories than 80 (Belcher et al., 1997; Capute et al., 1986) as the
(CDI; Fenson et al., 1994). A secondary search of the afore- cutoff for children who should be referred for comprehen-
mentioned databases with the full name of each screening sive language evaluation.
tool and a hand search of all references identified additional Capute et al. (1986) examined the relationship between
references, and a total of 19 studies were reviewed. The fol- the CLAMS and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development
lowing discussion reports on these findings and includes (BSID; Bayley, 1969). The researchers examined the con-
information on the psychometric properties, as well as the current and predictive validity of the CLAMS in a sample
feasibility of using each tool. of young children with mild-to-moderate motor delays
between 12 and 30 months of age. They described parents
Literature Synthesis as being from all socioeconomic strata with a range of edu-
cation levels. Capute and colleagues (1986) collected data
Initially, researchers developed screening tools specific to across three time points and summarized scores using the
language development to increase pediatrician accuracy CLAMS LQ and a derived Bayley ratio quotient (BRQ).

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


Larson 3

Authors found strong correlations with the BRQ and mod- ages rather than making comparisons between same-age
erate sensitivity and specificity in the study sample. peers (Mohay, 2007). There are also concerns with using
In a later study, Clark et al. (1995) evaluated the concur- standard scores on the CLAMS. In a review of The Capute
rent validity of the CLAMS with the Sequenced Inventory Scales, Mohay (2007) noted that standard deviations had
of Communication Development (SICD; Hedrick, Prather, large variation depending on child age with an abundance
& Tobin, 1984). Again, researchers described the sample as of young children falling far below the mean, and an over-
being from all socioeconomic strata with 99 children represented group of older children exceeding two or three
between the ages of 14 and 36 months. Unlike other studies standard deviations above the mean. In addition to concerns
where the CLAMS was administered in a pediatrician’s regarding standardization, the comparison tools used in
office, testing in this study was completed in the child’s these studies are out-of-date ranging from 1969 to 1984.
home or in a quiet room at Houston School for Deaf Assuming that, in general, assessments have improved over
Children. Clark et al. (1995) used age quotients to compare time, the use of outdated assessments makes it difficult for
scores on the CLAMS (< 90) with the SICD (< 80), and practitioners to draw conclusions about the accuracy of the
concluded that, compared with the SICD, the CLAMS was CLAMS with today’s population. Finally, even though two
most accurate in identifying expressive language delay for sets of authors described study participants as being eco-
children between 25 to 36 months of age; whereas classifi- nomically diverse (Capute et al., 1986; Clark et al., 1995),
cation accuracy diminished for children who were between neither provided specific breakdowns by number or per-
14 and 24 months old. centage. No authors commented on the racial or ethnic
Belcher et al. (1997) had a slightly different purpose for background of study participants.
their research. They used the CLAMS to evaluate 81 high-
risk preterm infants to determine the need for gestational
age correction at three age intervals. The authors in this
ELM Scale and ELM-2
study did not provide information for participants’ charac- The original ELM Scale included a total of 41 items
teristics beyond a description of medical exclusion crite- arranged chronologically by age of expected develop-
ria. Belcher et al. (1997) compared CLAMS LQ scores ment—the revised ELM-2 (Coplan, 1993) includes 43
with one of three comprehensive developmental assess- items. Both versions group language development into
ments: The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (53%; three categories: auditory receptive abilities, visual lan-
Terman & Merrill, 1976), The BSID (36%), and the guage milestones, and auditory expressive function
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (Coplan & Gleason, 1990; Costarides & Shulman, 1998).
(11%; Wechsler, 1976). Developmental quotients less than The tool can be scored using pass-fail criterion (Coplan et
71 were considered delayed and a CLAMS LQ of 90 was al., 1982) or with a point-scoring technique that allows for
used as the cutoff to determine a need for further evalua- calculation of percentiles, means, and standard deviation
tion. Belcher et al. (1997) noted statistically significant comparisons by age (Coplan & Gleason, 1990). Coplan et
correlations with comprehensive assessments of 9- to al. (1982) estimated that test administrators can gather
14-month-olds and 18- to 24-month-olds (r = .34, p < .02 information on child development in 1 to 3 min through
and r = .75, p < .001, respectively), but no statistically parent report, direct elicitation of skill, and incidental
significant relationship for the youngest study participants observation.
between 3- to 5-months-old (r = .27, p > .05). Using The ELM Scale was originally validated by Coplan et al.
chronological age resulted in the highest correlations (1982) through language evaluation and interviews with 191
(Belcher et al., 1997). children (birth through age 3) and their parents. Study
Researchers in the above-mentioned studies concluded authors demonstrated high sensitivity (97%) and specificity
that the CLAMS is an efficient screening tool with rela- (93%) in a cross-sectional sample of children aged 5 to 36
tively strong, statistically significant relationships with months using a variety of comprehensive standardized
other measures of child language and cognitive develop- assessments, including: BSID, Stanford-Binet Intelligence
ment. Across studies, researchers identified stronger rela- Scale, and Leiter International Performance Scale (Leiter,
tionships with increases in child age, and determined that 1969), and language assessments, including: Receptive-
the CLAMS was most effective in identifying expressive Expressive Emergent Language Scale (REEL Scale; Bzoch
language delay. Despite the relatively positive results from & League, 1971), Preschool Language Scale (PLS;
these studies, however, several issues affect use of the Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 1979), and the Peabody
CLAMS as an early language screening tool. Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn, 1965). Despite using
First, with the exception of Belcher et al. (1997), age mostly standardized measures (with the exception of the
quotients were used as comparisons between the CLAMS REEL Scale), authors did not mention how many children
and other more comprehensive measures. Age quotients can participated in each type of testing, nor did they provide data
be problematic because they compare children of different on the sensitivity or specificity for individual assessments

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


4 Communication Disorders Quarterly 

compared with the ELM. Furthermore, Coplan et al. (1982) Researchers have demonstrated the ELM Scale as an
mentioned that, “failing formal assessment was based upon appropriate screening tool across a variety of populations
the presence of significantly delayed language . . .” (p. 679); with good classification accuracy when compared with
however, they did not adequately describe pass-fail criteria comprehensive assessments. Although the ELM Scale was
for the standardized assessments. not as related to comprehensive measures prior to age 12
The original normative sample for the ELM Scale was months, researchers noted improvements with follow-up
primarily middle class and 80% White (Coplan et al., screenings. As discussed with the CLAMS, however, the
1982); however, additional studies using the ELM Scale research on the ELM Scale is out of date and there are no
have been validated for use with more diverse populations. reports on the classification accuracy of the ELM-2. The
Black, Freeland, Nair, Rubin, and Hutcheson (1988) found most recent studies addressing the utility of the ELM-2 are
high levels of sensitivity (86%) and specificity (100%) at least 10 years old, and none compare ELM-2 scores with
with 48 children with low SES, 60% of whom had mothers comprehensive assessments created after 1993.
without a high school degree. Other researchers examined
the use of the ELM Scale or ELM-2 with children who
have low birth weight (Byrne, Ellsworth, Bowering, &
LDS
Vincer, 1993; Costarides & Shulman, 1998), children who The CLAMS and ELM Scale ask if the child uses at least 50
were born premature and those with physical conditions words, but neither requires parents to list their child’s
that increase the risk for developmental delay (Costarides expressive vocabulary (Rescorla, 1989). In contrast, the
& Shulman, 1998), as well as children diagnosed with frag- LDS uses parent report on a vocabulary checklist. The LDS
ile X syndrome (Mirrett, Bailey, Roberts, & Hatton, 2004). can be completed independently by caregivers in a variety
Costarides and Shulman (1998) found statistically signifi- of settings to identify children at risk for language delay
cant correlations with the ELM-2 and BSID-II (Bayley, around age 2. Rescorla (1989) validated the LDS in four
1993). Mirrett and colleagues (2004) noted moderate to studies with a total of 641 child–caregiver dyads. Families
strong agreement between the ELM-2 and the REEL-2 who participated in the initial studies were both racially and
(Bzoch & League, 1991), but neither study provided data economically diverse, as is noted in detailed breakdowns
on the classification accuracy of the ELM-2—making it for SES, family type (one- or two-parent), parent employ-
difficult to determine the utility of the ELM-2 as a screen- ment status, and parent education (Rescorla, 1989).
ing tool. The LDS includes 310 vocabulary words arranged
Like studies that used the CLAMS, authors who exam- alphabetically in 14 semantic categories: actions, animals,
ined the ELM noted lower levels of predictive validity body parts, clothes, food, household items, modifiers, other,
when screening young children. Despite adequate sensitiv- outdoors, people, personal, places, toys, and vehicles
ity (87%) and specificity (70%) levels in their sample over- (Rescorla, Mirak, & Singh, 2000). Parents mark words they
all, Walker, Gugenheim, Down, and Northern (1989) have heard their child use, write down examples of their
demonstrated poor agreement with the SICD at 12 months child’s longest utterances, and answer questions related to
(sensitivity 0%, specificity 86%). Variations in sensitivity medical history and family demographics—which can be
of the ELM Scale for young children have been attributed to completed in about 10 min (Rescorla, 1989). Three criteria
low criterion validity on comparison tools (Walker et al., (Delay 1, Delay 2, and Delay 3) can be used to determine a
1989)—suggesting the ELM Scale may actually be a better need for further language assessment after screening with
tool for detecting language problems in very young children the LDS (Rescorla, 1989). An additional screening equation
(Black et al., 1988). known as Delay 3+ (Klee, Pearce, & Carson, 2000) incor-
When comparing the ELM Scale with the BSID, Satish porates information about parent concern for their child’s
and colleagues again found lower correlations between the language development and number of cases of otitis media
ELM and BSID for infants under 12 months (Satish et al., up to age 2. Although sensitivity and specificity of the LDS
1988a, 1988b) compared with those 13 months of age and have been shown to increase when using Delay 3+ (Klee
older (Satish et al., 1988b). In these studies, as well as in a et al., 2000), the equation has not been used in other pub-
study by Walker et al. (1989), authors used rescreens for lished research. Across all other studies, the Delay 3 criteria
children who failed the ELM Scale prior to their first birth- (child has fewer than 50 words or no word combinations), had
day. Walker et al. (1989) reported increased specificity the strongest correlations with other similar measures. Data
(70% to 85%) for the second screening attempt, and sug- calculated with Delay 3 criteria are presented in Table 1.
gested gains in specificity due to parents’ increased aware- Original LDS studies reported strong inter-rater reliabil-
ness of developmental milestone attainment following ity (r = .99), sensitivity ranging from 61% to 100%, and
initial completion of the tool. Increased classification accu- specificity between 67% to 96% when compared with other
racy with repeated screening suggests the ELM Scale may measures of vocabulary, language, and cognitive skills
be a useful tool for frequent progress monitoring. (Rescorla, 1989). In many studies, a child’s ability to name

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


Larson 5

Table 1.  Classification Accuracy From Studies of Early Language Screening Tools.
Study n Age in months Comparison measure(s) R SNS % SPC % PPV % NPV %

CLAMS
  Capute, Shapiro, Wachtel, 44 12–30 BSID .73*** 66 79 80 65
Gunther, and Palmer (1986)
  Clark, Jorgensen, and Blondeau 99 14–36 SICD (receptive) .66*** 78 90 44 98
(1995)  SICD (expressive) .65*** 67 95 75 93
  Belcher, Gittlesohn, Capute, and 81 3–6 BSID, WPPSI, or SB .27* 79 30  
Allen (1997)   9–14 .34*  
18–24 .75***  
ELM
  Coplan, Gleason, Ryan, Burke, and 119 5–36 BSID, SB, Leiter, REEL, PLS, 97 93 94 96
Williams (1982) and/or PPVT
  Walker, Gugenheim, Down, and 77 0–36 SICD (Initial Screen) 87 70  
Northern (1989)  SICD (Rescreen) 77 85  
  Black, Freeland, Nair, Rubin, and 48 14, 18 REEL, BSID 86 100  
Hutcheson (1988)
  Byrne, Ellsworth, Bowering, and 71 12, 24 SICD-R 0–68 80–100  
Vincer (1993)
ELM-2
  Costarides and Shulman (1998) 90 4, 12, 24 BSID-II .30*  
  Mirrett, Bailey, Roberts, and 18 9–18 REEL-2 .64–.93^  
Hatton (2004)
LDS
  Rescorla (1989; Study 3) 81 24–30 BSID, Reynell objects and .87*** 87 86  
pictures, and Reynell age
score
  Rescorla (1989; Study 4) 58 22–33 BSID and PLS objects and .79^  
pictures
 Rescorla, Hadicke-Wiley, and 108 22–30 BSID objects .78^ 90 95  
  Escarce (1993; Study 1)
  SB pictures 73 94  
 Rescorla, Hadicke-Wiley, and 92 24 BSID objects .82^ 100 90  
  Escarce (1993; Study 2)
  SB pictures 100 91  
  Klee et al. (1998; Study 1) 64 24–26 Mullen .72*** 91 87 18–37 >99
  Klee et al. (1998; Study 2) 36 36–40 Mullen .53** 67 90  
  Klee, Pearce, and Carson (2000) 64 24–26 Mullen 91 96 77 99
  Rescorla and Alley (2001; Study 1) 422 24–26 BSID objects .69*** 90 94 39 99
  SB pictures .74*** 64 94 39 98
  Rescorla and Alley (2001; Study 2) 66 24–26 Reynell .56–.81*** 94 67 52 97
  Mossabeb, Wade, Finnegan, Sivieri, 178 24, 26 BSID-III 61 82  
and Abbasi (2012)
CDI short-forms
  Fenson et al. (2000; Study 1) 483 8–18 CDI:WG .97**  
  Fenson et al. (2000; Study 2) 911 16–30 CDI:WS .99**  
  Pan, Rowe, Spier, and Tamis- 105 24 BSID-II .54 ***  
Lemonda (2004)  36 PPVT-III .50***  
  Rose, Feldman, and Jankowski 182 12 PPVT-R and verbal fluency .29–.49***  
(2009)

Note. SNS = sensitivity; SPC = specificity; PPV = positive predictive value; NPV = negative predictive value; CLAMS = Cognitive Linguistic Auditory Milestone Scale;
BSID = Bayley Scales of Infant Development; SICD = Sequenced Inventory of Communicative Development; WPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence;
SB = Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale; ELM = Early Language Milestone Scale; REEL = Receptive-Expressive Emergent Language Scale; PLS = Preschool Language Scale;
PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; SICD-R = Sequenced Inventory of Communicative Development–Revised; LDS = Language Development Survey; CDI short-forms
= MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories–Short-Form; CDI:WG = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories Words and Gestures;
CDI:WS = MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories Words and Sentences; PPVT-R = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Revised.
^ = p not reported. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

at least one of five objects on the Bayley Scale or one of 14 Rescorla & Alley, 2001; Rescorla, Hadicke-Wiley, &
pictures on a subtest of the Stanford-Binet: Fourth Edition Escarce, 1993). Failure to name any objects or pictures in
(Thorndike, Hagen, & Sattler, 1986) was used as the stan- these studies resulted in classification of language delay.
dard for normal language development (Rescorla, 1989; Rescorla and Alley (2001) reported concerns with

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


6 Communication Disorders Quarterly 

over-identification based on these criteria and suggested used with children from low-education, and low-income,
that factors unrelated to vocabulary use (i.e., child being families.
uncooperative, shy, or difficult to engage) affected the Pan, Rowe, Spier, and Tamis-Lemonda (2004) used a
results. They attempted to use z-scores from the receptive more diverse sample of 105 low-income children and their
and expressive sections of the Reynell Expressive Language mothers to examine correlations with the CDI-SF Level II
Scale to remediate the problem with the language delay cri- form and the BSID-II (r = .54, p < .001) at age 2. Researchers
teria. Unfortunately, the Reynell, as well as the Infant found modest predictive validity when compared with the
Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1993) used by PPVT-III (Dunn & Dunn, 1997) at age 3 (r = .50, p < .001).
Klee et al. (1998), are based on age scores and are not Interestingly, Pan et al. (2004) noted differences in CDI-SF
standardized. scores depending on mother’s race—where White mothers
In the most recent study examining the sensitivity and reported higher vocabulary scores than African American
specificity of the LDS, Mossabeb, Wade, Finnegan, Sivieri, or Hispanic mothers. They hypothesized that the difference
and Abbasi (2012) used the BSID-III (Bayley, 2006) and may exist due to limitations in assessing children in bilin-
compared Delay 3 criteria with standard scores and expres- gual households (41% of the sample), as well as potential
sive language scaled scores. When comparing LDS scores validity concerns with the PPVT-III in evaluating minority
with expressive scaled scores less than eight (<25th percen- children.
tile), authors found high levels of sensitivity (74%) and Rose, Feldman, and Jankowski (2009) used an economi-
specificity (87%) in this sample. When standardized lan- cally and racially diverse sample of 182 preterm and full-
guage scores were used, however, sensitivity decreased term 1-year-olds when they completed the first predictive
noticeably (61%). In their study, Mossabeb et al. (2012) validity study of the CDI-SF Level I. They compared
used a cutoff score of less than 90 to denote language CDI-SF Level I scores with Peabody Picture Vocabulary
delay—even though the BSID-III classifies scores of 80 to Test–Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) and a test of
89 as low average. Mossabeb et al. (2012) do not report on verbal fluency (Singer, Corley, Guiffrida, & Plomin, 1984)
the classification accuracy of the LDS with other compari- 2 years later. Correlations on the measures in this study
son scores. ranged from .29 to .49 (p < .001). Unfortunately, though,
Rose et al. (2009) used outdated measures to draw their
comparisons, with the rationale that the measures had previ-
CDI ously related to other infant measures in their study.
Like the LDS, the CDI includes a series of assessments that One final study, by Deniz Can, Ginsburg-Block,
use parent-completed vocabulary checklists. With an esti- Golinkoff, and Hirsh-Pasek (2012), examined longitudinal
mated administration time of 30 min (Rescorla, 1989), predictive validity of the CDI-SF Level II with language
however, use of the CDI as a screening tool is impractical. outcomes in kindergarten. After controlling for age, Deniz
Instead, the CDI–Short Form (CDI-SF) can be used by Can et al. (2012) found moderate correlations between the
researchers and clinicians seeking a quick assessment of picture vocabulary naming subtest (r =.41, p < .01) of the
early language (Fenson et al., 2000). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement–III (WJ-III;
Fenson et al. (2000) developed the CDI-SF with simu- Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001), and the syntax (r =
lated short-form scores extracted from the original CDI .32 p < .01) and semantics (r = .27 p < .05) domains of the
norming study (Fenson et al., 1994). The Level I form, Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation Test (DELV;
designed for young children between 8 and 18 months, Seymour, Roeper, & DeVilliers, 2005). Strengths of this
asks parents to review an 89-word vocabulary list for words study include use of reputable comprehensive assessments
their child understands or understands and says. The Level suggesting that the CDI-SF can be used to measure progress
II form, designed for children 16 to 30 months, presents over time, as well as a measure for predicting later language
100 words on either Form A or Form B and asks parents to ability. The sample tested by Deniz Can et al. (2012), how-
select words their child can say. The Level II form also ever, was primarily White and middle- to upper-middle
includes a question asking the parents if their child has class, with 79% of mothers reporting college graduation.
begun to combine words. Total vocabulary scores can be The lack of socioeconomic, racial, and parent education
converted to percentiles using normative data (Fenson et diversity in this study makes it difficult to determine out-
al., 2000). Across age ranges, Fenson et al. (2000) reported comes for children from other backgrounds.
correlations of .97 for Level I and .99 for Level II short- The available evidence on concurrent and predictive
forms when compared with their long-form versions. They validity supports use of the CDI-SF with young children;
noted, however, that the normative group had above-aver- however, across studies, authors noted that CDI-SF scores
age education levels and were less racially diverse than the should be interpreted cautiously in low SES and non-White
population, suggesting that results obtained through the populations. No studies in this review reported on the clas-
CDI-SF should be interpreted with caution when the tool is sification accuracy of the CDI-SF as a screening tool.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


Larson 7

Discussion assessing child performance in unfamiliar contexts with


unfamiliar communication partners (Dale, 1991). As fre-
The purpose of this review was to examine the technical quent observers of their child’s language across a variety of
adequacy and feasibility of four commercially available contexts, parents are able to characterize their child’s lan-
language screening tools for use with children under age 3. guage and can understand the child’s developing articula-
Early identification of infants and toddlers at risk for devel- tion for speech (Pan et al., 2004). Despite these advantages,
oping language delay can lead to early intervention that there is variability in the accuracy of reporting among dif-
may prevent later disabilities. In the following section, I ferent parent groups.
present summaries of the psychometric properties for early Parents who lack specialized training on identifying spe-
language screening tools, as well as their potential useful- cific language milestones may overestimate their child’s
ness within community-based settings with families who abilities (Dale, 1991); while caregivers with low SES may
are living in poverty. I also provide suggestions for future underestimate their child’s vocabulary production (Roberts,
research in the area of early language screening and outline Burchinal, & Durham, 1999). Parent reporting may differ in
the limitations of the review. Last, I summarize implications relation to the child’s age as well. Feldman et al. (2000)
for speech-language pathology (SLP), early childhood found negative relationships between parent education and
practitioners, and researchers. CDI scores at 1 year of age, with a reverse relationship at
age 2. Early language screening tools must be reliable
Psychometric Properties enough to account for variation in parent reporting.
There are also differences related to administration of the
Across studies, researchers identified moderate to strong rela- four tools that affect feasibility of use. Although the CLAMS
tionships between comprehensive language and cognitive and ELM-2 rely on parent report, the tools are conducted
assessments and the CLAMS, ELM, LDS, and CDI-SF. through parent interview and cannot be completed indepen-
Unfortunately, almost all gold standard assessments used as dently. Vocabulary checklists can be completed indepen-
comparisons in these studies were dated before the turn of dently by parents, and are therefore considered to be more
the century—even when more recent comprehensive assess- flexible options for use as screening tools within community
ments were available. For example, studies using the LDS settings. Studies with the LDS have been conducted in doc-
consistently demonstrated high levels of sensitivity and tor’s office waiting rooms (Rescorla, 1989) and through the
specificity across a variety of populations; however, only mail (Klee et al., 1998) with 90% and 50% return rates,
one study (Mossabeb et al., 2012) compared the LDS with respectively. Authors of the LDS and CDI suggest that par-
standard scores on a comprehensive assessment. Early ents can complete these screening tools independently; how-
research on the LDS used arbitrary gold standards based on ever, there may be less confusion when parents are provided
portions of standardized assessments. Authors who repli- with verbal directions (Rose et al., 2009).
cated these early studies continued using this criteria more
than 20 years later (e.g., Rescorla & Alley, 2001). In the
CLAMS study by Belcher et al. (1997), researchers used Future Research
comprehensive measures ranging in publication date from Based on the current literature review and available
1969 to 1986 when more recent tools such as the BSID-II research, there are three areas of research needed to improve
were available. the value of screening tools for infants and toddlers. First,
Research on the ELM-2 and CDI-SF has been completed there is a need for increased diversity in study sample popu-
with more reputable and updated standardized instruments lations—given that more than half of children under age 1
(BSID-II, PPVT-III, REEL-2, and WJ-III); however, psy- are minorities (U.S. Census Bureau, 2012), and more than a
chometric data on these tools is quite limited. With the quarter (26%) of all children under age 5 are living in pov-
ELM-2, for example, researchers only reported on correla- erty (Kids Count Data Center, 2014). With the exception of
tions. Similarly, despite a call for research on the sensitivity studies using the LDS (Rescorla, 1989), the majority of par-
of the CDI-SF (Fenson et al., 2000), more than a decade of ticipants in early language screening research are middle to
research reports only on concurrent and predictive validity. upper SES and primarily White. Research that is inclusive
of diverse populations will help determine the effects of
parent reporting and allow more accurate identification of
Feasibility of Use language delay in children from a variety of backgrounds.
The tools in this review can be considered for use in screen- Second, researchers should determine the concurrent valid-
ing children under 3 years of age. Each tool uses a recogni- ity and classification accuracy of early language screening
tion format, can be completed in under 10 min, and requires tools as they compare with up-to-date comprehensive lan-
little or no training by administrators. Parents’ report of guage assessments. The PLS is the most widely used assess-
young children’s language skills is more reliable than tools ment to determine language delay in young children (Caesar

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


8 Communication Disorders Quarterly 

& Kohler, 2009). Empirical studies are needed using the willingness to move into a comprehensive evaluation and
most recent version of this, and other, comprehensive lan- receive intervention (Bricker et al., 2013).
guage assessments. The tools discussed in this review can also be considered
Perhaps, most important, future research should be for use in evaluation and treatment. As a means of involving
community-based. Researchers should investigate the use families in the evaluation process, SLPs may find it useful
of screening tools within community settings to determine to have parents complete one of the above-mentioned tools
the most effective ways to screen large groups of young before initiating a comprehensive evaluation—to provide a
children in an efficient manner. Communities should means of gathering parent input and comparing results with
develop screening programs, a crucial step in identifying standardized assessments. Similarly, as SLPs and parents
children who may need early intervention (Bricker, Macy, collaborate throughout intervention, SLPs might solicit par-
Squires, & Marks, 2013). Before intervention can be pro- ent assistance in completing early language screening tools
vided, however, researchers must first determine the most as a means of monitoring progress and measuring the effects
appropriate ways to implement screening programs, com- of interventions over time. Researchers might consider
plete cost-benefit analyses, and determine any potential using these early language screening tools as brief measures
negative impacts screening may have. Researchers need to of language ability—to find populations of interest for a
determine the best age(s) for screening and examine the specific study and to test between-group differences in
effects of using language-specific screening tools com- experimental designs.
pared with more comprehensive developmental screeners.
They should also explicitly compare the accuracy of par-
ent-report tools with practitioner-implemented observa-
Conclusion
tional measures. The current research on early language screeners is some-
what outdated and primarily focused on White, middle-
class children. Although overall technical adequacy of early
Limitations language screening tools is acceptable, there is no identifi-
This review was limited to screening tools that exclusively able gold standard. Effective early language screening tools
evaluate communication development—with two tools (LDS should help identify children at risk for language delays and
and CDI-SF) relying on a child’s use and understanding of provide early intervention as soon as concerns arise.
vocabulary as opposed to other communication domains (i.e., Researchers and practitioners in the field of early childhood
social language and articulation). Although vocabulary is a must continue to work together to identify accurate screen-
strong predictor of later academic ability, the use of more ing tools for an increasingly diverse population of infants
comprehensive language screening tools may improve clas- and toddlers. This review provides a starting point for future
sification accuracy. The CLAMS, LDS, and CDI-SF are also research that will continue exploring the feasibility of early
available in other languages; however, this review was lim- childhood screening programs, and the most efficient mea-
ited to research examining the English versions of the screen- sures to identify young children who may benefit from lan-
ing tools. As the diversity of young children continues to guage intervention.
grow, reviews inclusive of research describing screening
tools in other languages will also be important. Acknowledgments
The author thanks Drs. Kristen McMaster and Scott McConnell
Implications for Research and Practice for ongoing support and assistance with this project.

Researchers and practitioners should carefully consider how Declaration of Conflicting Interests
language screening tools relate to the population they work
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
with. Each tool in this review has drawbacks in terms of sam-
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ple characteristics, gold standard comparison tools, or lack of
data on sensitivity and specificity. Despite these concerns, the
tools that were identified can, and should, be useful for a vari- Funding
ety of reasons. First, all tools in this review require parent The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
participation. The American Speech-Language-Hearing authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Association (ASHA) suggests that SLPs involve families in
the identification and verification of concerns related to their References
child’s early language development (ASHA, 2008). Parent Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (2005). The Capute Scales:
involvement in screening is also thought to increase aware- Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic & Auditory
ness of developmental expectations and increase acceptance Milestone Scale (CAT/CLAMS). Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
of screening outcomes—thus, potentially affecting a family’s Brookes.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


Larson 9

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2008). Roles Dale, P. S. (1991). The validity of a parent report measure of
and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in early vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. Journal of Speech and
intervention: Technical report [Technical Report]. Retrieved Hearing Research, 34, 565–571.
from http://www.asha.org/policy Deniz Can, D., Ginsburg-Block, M., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-
Bayley, N. (1969). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New Pasek, K. (2012). A long-term predictive validity study: Can
York, NY: The Psychological Corporation. the CDI Short Form be used to predict language and early
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). literacy skills four years later? Journal of Child Language, 40,
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. 1–15. doi:10.1017/S030500091200030X
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Dunn, L. M. (1965). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle
Development, Third Edition: Technical Manual. San Antonio, Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
TX: The Psychological Corporation. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Belcher, H. M. E., Gittlesohn, A., Capute, A. J., & Allen, M. Test–Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
C. (1997). Using the Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary
Milestone Scale for developmental screening in high- Test–Third Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
risk preterm infants. Clinical Pediatrics, 36, 635–642. Service.
doi:10.1177/000992289703601104 Feldman, H. M., Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Kurs-Lasky,
Black, M. B., Freeland, C. A. B., Nair, P., Rubin, J. S., & M., Janosky, J. E., & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement
Hutcheson, J. J. (1988). Language screening for infants prone properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development
to otitis media. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 13, 423–433. Inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development,
Bricker, D., Macy, M., Squires, J., & Marks, K. (2013). 71, 310–322.
Developmental screening in your community: An integrated Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick,
approach for connecting children with services. Baltimore, S. J., & Stiles, J. (1994). Variability in early communicative
MD: Paul H. Brookes. development. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Byrne, J., Ellsworth, C., Bowering, E., & Vincer, M. (1993). Child Development, 59, 1–185. doi:10.2307/1166093
Language development in low birthweight infants: The first Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., &
two years of life. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur
14, 21–27. Communicative Development Inventories. Applied
Bzoch, K. R., & League, R. (1971). Receptive-Expressive Psycholinguistics, 21, 95–116.
Emergent Language Scale. Baltimore, MD: University Park Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES
Press. differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are
Bzoch, K. R., & League, R. (1991). The Bzoch-League Receptive- evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16, 234–248.
Expressive Emergent Language Test (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: doi:10.1111/desc.12019
ProEd. Guralnick, M. J. (2005). An overview of the developmental sys-
Caesar, L. G., & Kohler, P. D. (2009). Tools clinicians use: A sur- tems model for early intervention. In M. J. Guralnick (Ed.),
vey of language assessment procedures used by school-based The developmental systems approach to early intervention
speech-language pathologists. Communication Disorders (pp. 3–28). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Quarterly, 30, 226–236. doi:10.1177/1525740108326334 Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the every-
Capute, A. J., Shapiro, B. K., Wachtel, R. C., Gunther, V. A., & day experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD:
Palmer, F. B. (1986). The Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Paul H. Brookes.
Milestone Scale (CLAMS): Identification of cognitive defects Hedrick, D. L., Prather, E. M., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Sequenced
in motor-delayed children. American Journal of Diseases of Inventory of Communication Development. Seattle: University
Children, 140, 694–698. of Washington Press.
Child Trends Databank. (2014). Children in poverty. Retrieved from Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape lan-
http://www.childtrends.org/?indicators=children-in-poverty guage development. Developmental Review, 26, 55–88.
Clark, J., Jorgensen, S., & Blondeau, R. (1995). Investigating the doi:10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
validity of the Clinical Linguistic Auditory Milestone Scale. Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act, 20
International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 31, U.S.C. § 1435(a) (2004).
63–75. Kids Count Data Center. (2014). Children in poverty by age group.
Coplan, J. (1993). Early Language Milestone Scale–Second Retrieved from http://www.datacenter.kidscount.org
Edition (ELM Scale-2). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Klee, T., Carson, D. K., Gavin, W. J., Hall, L., Kent, A., & Reece,
Coplan, J., & Gleason, J. R. (1990). Quantifying language devel- S. (1998). Concurrent and predictive validity of an early lan-
opment from birth to 3 years using the Early Language guage screening program. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Milestone Scale. Pediatrics, 86, 963–971. Hearing Research, 41, 627–641.
Coplan, J., Gleason, J. R., Ryan, R., Burke, M. G., & Williams, M. Klee, T., Pearce, K., & Carson, D. K. (2000). Improving the
L. (1982). Validation of an Early Language Milestone Scale positive predictive value of screening for developmental lan-
in a high-risk population. Pediatrics, 70, 677–683. guage disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Costarides, A. H., & Shulman, B. B. (1998). Norm-referenced lan- Research, 43, 821–833.
guage measures: Implications for assessment of infants and Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000).
toddlers. Topics in Language Disorders, 18, 26–33. Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016


10 Communication Disorders Quarterly 

delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. Rescorla, L., Mirak, J., & Singh, L. (2000). Vocabulary growth in
International Journal of Language & Communicative late talkers: Lexical development from 2;0 to 3;0. Journal of
Disorders, 35, 165–188. Child Language, 27, 293–311.
Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2002). Inequality at the starting Roberts, J. E., Burchinal, M., & Durham, M. (1999). Parents’
gate: Social background differences in achievement as report of vocabulary and grammatical development of African
children begin school (Executive Summary). Retrieved from American preschoolers: Child and environmental associa-
http://epsl.asu.edu/epru/articles/EPRU-0603-138-OWI.pdf tions. Child Development, 70, 92–106.
Leiter, R. G. (1969). Leiter International Performance Scale. Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2009). A cog-
Chicago, IL: Stoelting. nitive approach to the development of early language.
Leppert, M. L., Shank, T. P., Shapiro, B. K., & Capute, A. J. Child Development, 80, 134–150. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
(1998). The Capute Scales: CAT/CLAMS—A pediatric 8624.2008.01250.x
assessment tool for the early detection of mental retarda- Rossman, M. J., Hyman, S. L., Rorabaugh, M. L., Berlin, L. E.,
tion and communicative disorders. Mental Retardation and Allen, M. C., & Modlin, J. F. (1994). The CAT/CLAMS
Developmental Disabilities, 4, 14–19. assessment for early intervention services. Clinical Pediatrics,
Macias, M. M., Saylor, C. F., Greer, M. K., Charles, J. M., Bell, 33, 404–409.
N., & Katikaneni, L. D. (1998). Infant screening: The use- Satish, M., McQuiston, S., Dennler, J., Mueller, P., Urrutia, J.,
fulness of the Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener ElShafie, S., & Peters, L. (1988a). Developmental testing, I:
and the Clinical Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic Auditory Correlation of multiple Early Language Milestones (ELM)
Milestone Scale. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, with Bayley [Abstract]. Pediatric Research, 23, 455.
19, 155–161. Satish, M., McQuiston, S., Dennler, J., Mueller, P., Urrutia, J.,
Mirrett, P. L., Bailey, D. B., Roberts, J. E., & Hatton, D. D. (2004). ElShafie, S., & Peters, L. (1988b). Developmental testing, II:
Developmental screening and detection of developmental Correlation of multiple Early Language Milestones (ELM)
delays in infants and toddlers with Fragile X Syndrome. Journal with Bayley [Abstract]. Pediatric Research, 23, 455.
of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25, 21–27. Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T. W., & DeVilliers, J. (2005). The
Mohay, H. (2007). The Capute Scales: Cognitive Adaptive Test/ Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variation, Norm
Clinical Linguistic and Auditory Milestone Scale [Review of Referenced: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX:
the book The Capute Scales: Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Psychological Corporation.
Linguistic and Auditory Milestone Scale, by P. Accardo & A. Singer, S., Corley, R., Guiffrida, C., & Plomin, R. (1984). The
Capute]. International Journal of Disability, Development and development and validation of a test battery to measure dif-
Education, 54, 135–142. doi:10.1080/10349120601151108 ferentiated cognitive abilities in three-year-old children.
Mossabeb, R., Wade, K. C., Finnegan, K., Sivieri, E., & Abbasi, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 44, 703–712.
S. (2012). Language Development Survey provides a useful Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. (1976). Stanford-Binet Intelligence
screening tool for language delay in preterm infants. Clinical Scale: Form L-M. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Pediatrics, 51, 638–644. doi:10.1177/0009922812439244 Thorndike, R. L., Hagen, E. P., & Sattler, J. M. (1986). Stanford-
Mullen, E. M. (1993). Infant Mullen Scales of Early Learning: Binet Intelligence Scale: Fourth Edition. Chicago, IL:
AGS Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Riverside Press.
Pan, E. A., Rowe, M. L., Spier, E., & Tamis-Lemonda, C. (2004). Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Buckwalter, P., & Catts, H. (2000). The
Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low-income association of reading disability, behavioral disorders, and
families: Concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of language impairment among second-grade children. Journal
data. Journal of Child Language, 31, 587–608. doi:10.1017/ of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 473–482.
S0305000904006270 U.S. Census Bureau. (2012, May 17). Most children younger
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A than 1 are minorities, census bureau reports. Available from
screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of http://www.census.gov
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599. Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., & Carta, J. (1994). Prediction
Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in of school outcomes based on early language production and
late-talking toddlers: Support for a dimensional perspective socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 64, 606–621.
on language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Walker, D., Gugenheim, S., Down, M. P., & Northern, J. L.
Research, 52, 16–30. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0171) (1989). Early Language Milestone Scale and language screen-
Rescorla, L., & Alley, A. (2001). Validation of the Language ing of young children. Pediatrics, 83, 284–288.
Development Survey (LDS): A parent report tool for identify- Wechsler, D. (1976). Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and
ing language delay in toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, Primary Scale of Intelligence. New York, NY: Psychological
and Hearing Research, 44, 434–455. doi:10.1044/1092- Corporation.
4388(2001/035) Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001).
Rescorla, L., Hadicke-Wiley, M., & Escarce, E. (1993). Woodcock–Johnson III: Test of Cognitive Abilities and
Epidemiological investigation of expressive language delay Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
at age two. First Language, 13(5), 5–22. doi:10.1177/ Zimmerman, I., Steiner, V., & Pond, R. E. (1979). Preschool
014272379301303702 Language Scale. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.

Downloaded from cdq.sagepub.com at Middle East Technical Univ on April 8, 2016

You might also like