You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/286637583

Influence of children’s oral health promotion on parents’ behaviours,


attitudes and knowledge

Article  in  Acta odontologica Scandinavica · December 2015


DOI: 10.3109/00016357.2015.1122836

CITATIONS READS

9 148

5 authors, including:

Mimmi Tolvanen Vuokko Anna-Marketta Anttonen


University of Oulu University of Oulu
127 PUBLICATIONS   2,460 CITATIONS    115 PUBLICATIONS   1,323 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Satu Lahti
University of Turku
163 PUBLICATIONS   3,990 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Oral health promotion View project

School Oral Health Project Nepal View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Vuokko Anna-Marketta Anttonen on 04 March 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Acta Odontologica Scandinavica

ISSN: 0001-6357 (Print) 1502-3850 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iode20

Influence of children’s oral health promotion on


parents’ behaviours, attitudes and knowledge

Mimmi Tolvanen, Vuokko Anttonen, Marja-Leena Mattila, Hannu Hausen &


Satu Lahti

To cite this article: Mimmi Tolvanen, Vuokko Anttonen, Marja-Leena Mattila, Hannu
Hausen & Satu Lahti (2016) Influence of children’s oral health promotion on parents’
behaviours, attitudes and knowledge, Acta Odontologica Scandinavica, 74:5, 321-327, DOI:
10.3109/00016357.2015.1122836

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.3109/00016357.2015.1122836

Published online: 11 Dec 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 438

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 1 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iode20
ACTA ODONTOLOGICA SCANDINAVICA, 2016
VOL. 74, NO. 5, 321–327
http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00016357.2015.1122836

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Influence of children’s oral health promotion on parents’ behaviours, attitudes


and knowledge
MIMMI TOLVANENa,b, VUOKKO ANTTONENc, MARJA-LEENA MATTILAd,e, HANNU HAUSENf,g, and SATU LAHTIa
a
Department of Community Dentistry, Institute of Dentistry, University of Turku, Turku, Finland; bFinnBrain Study Group, Department of
Psychiatry and Turku Brain and Mind Center, University of Turku, Turku, Finland; cDepartment of Pedodontics, Cariology and Endodontology,
Institute of Dentistry, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland; dCity of Turku Welfare Division, Public Dental Health Care Clinic, Turku, Finland;
e
Department of Public Health, University of Turku, Turku, Finland; fFinnish Dental Association, Helsinki, Finland; gInstitute of Dentistry, University
of Oulu, Oulu, Finland

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Objective The aim was to compare the changes in parents’ oral health-related behaviour, Received 18 May 2015
knowledge and attitudes in 2001–2003 and 2003–2005, during a 3.4-year-intervention in Pori and in Revised 16 November 2015
the reference area Rauma, Finland. Materials and methods The study population consisted of Accepted 17 November 2015
parents of children who participated in the oral health promotion programme in Pori (all 5th and Published online
6th graders who started the 2001–2002 school year in the town of Pori, n ¼ 1691) and the parents 9 December 2015
of same-aged children in a reference town (n ¼ 807). In 2001–2003, the promotion was targeted KEYWORDS
only to the children in Pori. In 2003–2005, the promotion was targeted also to parents, for example Behaviour; change; children;
via local mass media. The statistical significances of the differences in parents’ self-reported oral health; parents
behaviour, knowledge and attitudes, and changes in these, were evaluated using Mann-Whitney
U-tests and confidence intervals. Results In 2001–2003, the trend in changing behaviours was in
favour of parents in Pori. Mothers in Pori also improved their knowledge and the attitude
‘importance of brushing for health and appearance’. In 2003–2005, the trend in changing
behaviours was rather similar in both towns, which may be due to diffusion of the oral health
intervention to Rauma via the media. Conclusions The results suggest that health promotion
targeted to children, which in previous studies has been shown to be successful in improving
children’s behaviours, also helped their parents in mending their habits.

Introduction The aim of this study was to compare the changes in


parents’ oral health-related behaviour, knowledge, and atti-
Children’s health-related behaviours are influenced by those of
tudes in Pori and in the reference area Rauma in 2001–2003
their parents.[1–7] Also children may have active participation
and 2003–2005. In Pori, the OHP was targeted only to children
in their family’s health, such as willingness to contribute, and
in 2001–2003, and also to parents in 2003–2005. We also
manage their and their family members’ health and well-
wanted to evaluate if the factor structure of attitudes related to
being.[8,9] However, there is very limited knowledge on if and
oral health and hygiene among parents was similar to that
how children’s behaviours transfer to their parents. It has been
among their children.
reported that health education activities that children learnt in
school influenced parents’ self-management behaviour of
asthma.[10] Pre-school children were also able to transmit Materials and methods
oral health-related knowledge acquired at school to their The study population consisted of parents of children who
parents and that included a change in oral health routine of participated in the 3.4-year community level programme of
their family members.[11] oral health promotion (OHP) in Pori (n ¼ 1691) and parents of
In 2001  2005 in Pori, Finland, a 3.4-year community-level children in the reference group in Rauma (n ¼ 807), a town
programme of oral health promotion (OHP) using a multi-level located 50 km from Pori.
approach was targeted to all children who were 11–12 years In 2001–2005, the OHP was implemented among all 5th and
old at the beginning of the study and to the people involved in 6th graders (11- and 12-year-olds) in the town of Pori at the
their lives. Children exposed to the OHP improved their baseline, except for mentally disabled and handicapped
behaviours during the follow-up more than children in a children attending special schools. The OHP aimed (i) to
neighbouring town where the children were not exposed to increase children s knowledge of oral health problems and
any specific intervention.[12] their prevention, (ii) to change their oral health-related

CONTACT Mimmi Tolvanen mimmi.tolvanen@utu.fi Department of Community Dentistry, Institute of Dentistry, University of Turku, FIN-20014, Finland

ß 2015 Taylor & Francis


322 M. TOLVANEN ET AL.

behaviour and, in particular, (iii) to provide social support for and took the parents’ questionnaires home to one of their
the children in the experimental group. The promotion parents’ to fill out and returned the questionnaires in sealed
programme was targeted to the schoolchildren and persons envelopes. Thus, the parents responded independently of the
involved in the children’s everyday life using various children. All parents who had filled out any of the question-
approaches and was carried out in close co-operation with naires and answered any of the questions concerning behav-
the local health and school authorities and local media, which iours were included in the study. Most of these parents were
provided good coverage throughout the programme. mothers. The numbers of children/parents to whom the
questionnaires were administered and the response rates are
presented in Table 1. A great majority of the children remained
The first phase in 2001–2003 in the study population throughout the entire study period,
The first survey was conducted in Fall 2001 and the second in but every year there were a few new participants and dropouts
Spring 2003. The first phase of the programme was imple- due to moving. The same was true for the parents, but we
mented between those time points. In the first phase, the didn’t ask the same parent to answer every year. For the
promotion programme was targeted only to the school- reference town, no identification variable was available to
children. During a pupil-oriented school campaign, children produce longitudinal data. Thus, the data consist of three
acquired relevant, sufficient, and correct information by separate cross-sectional sets, for which the trends were
preparing different oral health promotion projects, and as an compared.
exercise for improving social skills they presented these The questionnaires contained structured and open-ended
projects in different contexts. The main themes were brushing questions concerning oral-health related behaviours (31 items),
teeth twice a day with fluoride toothpaste, using xylitol attitudes (17 items), knowledge (five items) and background
information, such as age and parent’s occupational level, which
products after meals, and promoting healthy eating habits,
was measured with open-ended question and dichotomized to
such as avoiding constant nibbling. Community analyses of the
low (blue collar workers) and high (white collar workers).[13]
presence of soft drink or candy vending machines at schools
The questions included in this study have been found to be
and possibilities to buy candies or soft drinks elsewhere during
reliable [14] and valid [15,16] among children of these parents.
the school day were conducted by school classes. The results
Reliability for behaviours was measured with Cronbach’s alpha,
were discussed with the pupils and school nurses.
which was 0.85. Validity was measured with correlations
between reported behaviour and clinically visible plaque and
The second phase in 2003–2005 gingivitis. In the follow-ups, the correlation between gingivitis
and toothbrushing increased with age (11-year-olds, r ¼ 0.09;
The second phase of the OHP took place between the second 12-year-olds, r ¼ 0.13; 13-year-olds, r ¼ 0.26; 14-year-olds,
and the third (Spring 2005) survey. In this latter part of the r ¼ 0.29; 15-year olds, r ¼ 0.40; p50.05, except 11-year-olds).
study, in addition to children, the OHP was targeted also to
persons involved in the children’s everyday life. The main
themes were increasing the children’s and parents’ knowledge Measures
of oral health hazards in the school environment and
The oral health behaviours investigated included consumption
increasing the daily toothbrushing frequency among the
of sugared drinks (soft drinks, sports drinks, juices, chocolate,
entire community. The parents organized a meeting to discuss
sugared tea/coffee), sugared snacks (sweets, candy bars,
the healthy school environment, such as avoiding vending chocolate, ice cream, cookies, bakery), and xylitol products
machines, encouraging pupils to eat free school lunches and (chewing gum, lozenges), as well as toothbrushing with
assessing the availability of drinking water instead of soft fluoride toothpaste and using dental floss. Behaviours were
drinks. In the campaign ‘Once a day is not enough’, oral health measured on 7-point Likert-like scales with alternatives that
personnel provided information on recommended toothbrush- described the frequency of the behaviour. Alternatives varied
ing frequency at schools, stores, fairs and in the media. The from 34-times a day or more frequently to less than twice a
local mass media also put healthy school environment month or never. Behaviours were recoded to describe the
regularly on the agenda. For more details concerning the weekly frequencies: 34 times a day or more frequently ¼ 24.5,
OHP, see Tolvanen et al.[12] twice a day ¼ 14, once a day ¼ 7, 2–3 times a week ¼ 2.5, once
In the town of Rauma, Finland, where neither oral health a week ¼ 1, twice a month ¼ 0.45, less than twice month or
promotion nor other interventions were implemented, a similar never ¼ 0. This enabled calculation of sum variables for
group of children and parents served as a reference group. The sugared drinks and snacks and xylitol products.
data for this study were collected using questionnaire surveys Knowledge was measured by parents’ agreement on the
at baseline (2001), in the middle (2003), and 3.4 years after correctness of the five statements related to toothbrushing: ‘a
launching the OHP in Pori (2005) and simultaneously in the person has to brush his/her teeth twice a day’; ‘a person can
reference area, Rauma. prevent caries lesions by using fluoride toothpaste’; ‘to ensure
a sufficient supply of fluoride, one has to brush teeth with
fluoride toothpaste at least twice a day’; ‘a person can prevent
Questionnaires and respondents
caries lesions by using xylitol products after meals’; and ‘a
Similar questionnaires were used for children and parents in person can reduce the risk of developing new caries lesions by
both towns. The children filled out the questionnaires at school omitting one sweet snack a day’. The 4-point Likert-scaled reply
ACTA ODONTOLOGICA SCANDINAVICA 323

alternatives varied from totally agree to totally disagree. developing caries lesions’ and items 7–8 in ‘importance of
Knowledge variables were the sum scores of these five items, brushing for acceptance’.
scaled to have a range of 6–24, where 24 was the best/
healthiest score.
Statistical analyses
Attitudes of the parents were determined based on their
opinions on 17 items: whether toothbrushing is important: for For parents’ 2001 data, multiple group CFA (MGCFA) was used
avoiding caries lesions (item 1), for avoiding tooth discolour- to test for the equivalence of the factorial structure across
ation (item 2), for a fresh feeling in the mouth (item 3), for children and parents (configural invariance). The fit indexes
better appearance (item 4), for fresh breath (item 5), for used were model chi-square and its significance, normed chi-
healthy gingiva (item 6), for acceptance by child (item 7), and square (2/df), normed fit index (NFI), comparative fit index
for acceptance by spouse (item 8). They were also asked (CFI) and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA).
whether they think toothbrushing is important before going Model 2 should be non-significant and values 2/df55, NFI
to work (item 9), visiting a dentist (item 10), participating in and CFI40.90 and RMSEA50.08 indicate a reasonably good
sports/hobbies (item 11), going to a party (item 12), or fit .[17]
meeting their best friends (item 13). In addition, they were For attitude factors and knowledge, Cronbach’s alphas were
asked whether they would feel bothered if they themselves calculated to assess the reliability of the scales. For further
(item 14), their child (item 15), their spouse (item 16), or their analyses, attitude variables were coded as sum scores of the
friend (item 17) developed caries lesions. The 4-point Likert- items loading to factor in question. Attitude sum scores were
like scaled reply alternatives varied from very important to scaled to have a range of 6–24, 24 being the best/healthiest
not at all important. score, similar to knowledge. The mean weekly frequencies for
In our previous study among children, principal component behaviours and mean sum scores for knowledge and attitudes
analyses (PCA) with varimax rotation revealed four factors in 2001, 2003, and 2005, as well as their changes in 2001–2003
describing the attitudes and explaining 63% of the common and 2003–2005 were evaluated for both towns. The statistical
variance.[16] The factors were named as ‘importance of significances of the differences in parents’ self-reported
brushing for social situations’, ‘importance of brushing for behaviour, knowledge and attitudes between the towns were
health and appearance’, ‘concern about developing caries evaluated with Mann-Whitney U-tests and confidence intervals
lesions’ and ‘importance of brushing for acceptance’. The items and between time points (within the towns) with confidence
among children were nearly the same as among parents, with intervals. We also conducted generalized linear models for
the following amendments. Children were asked about behaviours, knowledge and attitudes using a combined cross-
parents’ and friends’ acceptance, whilst parents were asked sectional dataset and using year and town and their interaction
about child’s and spouse’s acceptance. Children were also term as factors. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS
asked about the importance of toothbrushing before going to 16.0 and AMOS 16.0 software. The Ethics Committee of the
school, disco, meeting their girlfriend/boyfriend, and meeting Northern Ostrobothnia Hospital District and the City of Pori
their best friend. For parents, these four items were replaced by gave their approval for the study.
just three items involving work, a party, and meeting their best
friend. Bother about developing caries lesions concerned their
Results
own, mother’s, father’s and friend’s mouth among children and
their own, child’s, spouse’s and friend’s mouth among parents. At baseline, there were no statistically significant differences
For parents, a similar factor structure as among children would between the towns in mean ages of the responding parents;
include items 9–13 in the factor ‘importance of brushing for 40.0 years among mothers and 42.4 among fathers in Pori, and
social situations’, items 1–6 in ‘importance of brushing for 39.4 years among mothers and 40.8 among fathers in Rauma.
health and appearance’, items 14–17 in ‘concern about In Pori, the responding mothers were more likely to have a

Table 1. Description of the population.*


Semester Pori, n (%) Rauma, n (%)
Fall 2001 Population of 5th and 6th graders 1691 807
Questionnaires returned 1527 (90) 693 (86)
Respondent: mother 1332 (87) 581 (84)
Respondent: father 168 (11) 99 (14)
Respondent: else/unknown 27 (2) 13 (2)
Spring 2003 Population of 6th and 7th graders 1651 850
Questionnaires returned 1346 (82) 620 (73)
Respondent: mother 998 (74) 346 (56)
Respondent: father 153 (11) 56 (9)
Respondent: else/unknown 195 (15) 218 (35)
Spring 2005 Population of 8th and 9th graders 1659 827
Questionnaires returned 1292 (78) 523 (63)
Respondent: mother 1083 (84) 434 (83)
Respondent: father 193 (15) 84 (16)
Respondent: else/unknown 16 (1) 5 (1)
*The children were mainly the same during the whole study, just a few new participants and dropouts due to moving. The same was true of
parents, but not the same parent every year.
324 M. TOLVANEN ET AL.

higher occupational level than were the responding mothers in sugary snacks among fathers and also toothbrushing and
Rauma (71% vs 66%, p ¼ 0.034). Among the responding fathers, drinking soft drinks among fathers in Rauma.
no such difference was observed (57% vs 58%, respectively). The differences in knowledge and the attitude ‘health and
The results of the MGCFA revealed good fit for the appearance’ that emerged in 2003 between mothers in Pori
unconstrained model, indicating configural invariance, i.e., and Rauma, had faded by 2005, since mothers in Rauma gained
the same factor structure across children and parents. All more knowledge and the attitude ‘health and appearance’
indicators had relatively high standardized loadings on the relapsed among mothers in Pori (Table 3). The trend in
factor they were assumed to load and the correlations between changing knowledge and attitudes was similar in both towns.
the latent variables were rather low, indicating good conver- Results of the generalized linear modelling revealed that,
gent validity and discriminant validity (Figure 1). The model among mothers, statistically significant factors were time for
was good fit for three criteria: NFI ¼ 0.912, CFI ¼ 0.921, and toothbrushing (p ¼ 0.031), flossing (p ¼ 0.001) and attitude
RMSEA ¼ 0.045 (90% CI ¼ 0.043–0.047), but not according to 2 ‘health and appearance’ (p ¼ 0.026), town for eating sugary
estimates: 2(226) ¼ 2045.361 (p50.001), 2/df ¼ 9.05. snacks (p50.001) and both time and town for using xylitol
Cronbach’s alphas were 0.748 for knowledge, 0.800 for products (p-values ¼ 0.027 and 0.002, respectively), for drinking
concern about developing caries lesions, 0.728 for the import- sugary drinks (p-values50.001 and 0.016, respectively) and for
ance of brushing for social situations, 0.699 for the importance knowledge (both p-values50.001). Among fathers, statistically
of brushing for health and appearance and 0.835 for the significant factors were time for knowledge (p ¼ 0.044) and
importance of brushing for acceptance. both time and town for eating sugary snacks (p-values ¼ 0.020
and 0.009, respectively).

The first phase in 2001–2003


Discussion
At the baseline, the mothers in Rauma were more likely to
consume xylitol products and sugary snacks than were In 2001–2003, the trend in changing behaviours was in favour
mothers in Pori; and for sugary snacks, this association of parents in Pori compared to the reference town, Rauma.
remained throughout the study (Table 2). In 2003, fathers in During this first phase of the intervention, the OHP was
Pori were statistically significantly more likely to brush their targeted only to the children in Pori who also improved their
teeth more frequently and eat sugary snacks less frequently behaviours more than children in Rauma,[12] so the improve-
than were fathers in Rauma. ments among parents in Pori can be assumed to be a result of
During the first phase, the trend in changing behaviours was health promotion targeted to children transmitted to their
in favour of parents in Pori. Compared to Rauma, fathers in Pori parents. Mothers in Pori also improved their knowledge and
tended to be more successful in changing their behaviours in one of the attitudes, ‘health and appearance’. Some of the
four and mothers in all five behaviours studied (Table 2). The changes were quite small. Still, they should not be considered
improvements within the towns were statistically significant for meaningless, since even small individual changes that take
using xylitol products and drinking sugary drinks among place among the whole population may lead to significant
mothers in Pori, for drinking sugary drinks among mothers in improvements at the population level.[18] Factor analysis
Rauma, and for toothbrushing and using xylitol products revealed a similar factor structure for attitudes among parents
among fathers in Pori. The biggest changes in both towns were as was obtained among their children, indicating that, even if
improvements in drinking soft drinks among mothers and attitudes change due to growing up, the measuring may be
flossing among fathers and, in Pori, also the use of xylitol done with similar instruments, easing the studying and
products among both parents. comparison of attitudes.
At the baseline in 2001, no statistically significant differences In the second phase in 2003–2005, the OHP in Pori was
between the towns were found for knowledge and attitudes, targeted also to other persons involved in the children’s
either among mothers or fathers (Table 3). In 2001–2003 everyday life, including parents. The intervention was likely to
mothers in Pori improved statistically significantly their know- diffuse also to Rauma, because the local mass media drew
ledge and the attitude ‘health and appearance’. Thus, in 2003 aspects of intervention, especially toothbrushing, regularly on
mothers in Pori had a statistically significantly higher score for the agenda, and the towns are located only 50 km apart and
knowledge and the attitude ‘health and appearance’ than had share some local media. Due to this ‘noise’, these results are
mothers in Rauma. hard to interpret when it comes to influencing factors.
The differences between parents in Pori and Rauma at the
second time point and the similarities at the third time point
The second phase in 2003–2005
strengthen the interpretation of the result that children can
In 2003–2005, the trend in changing behaviours was pretty transfer knowledge and behaviour to their parents, since the
similar in both towns among both mothers and fathers (Table only exposure parents had between the first two time points
2). The statistically significant changes within the towns were was whatever their children shared with them. At the baseline,
improvements in toothbrushing among mothers in Rauma, these children were 11–12 years old, entering their early
flossing among mothers in Pori and eating sugary snacks teenage years and were maybe more likely to share what they
among fathers in Rauma. The biggest changes in both towns had learned in such a way that it may affect their parents, than
were improvements in flossing among mothers and eating very young children or children in their late teens.
ACTA ODONTOLOGICA SCANDINAVICA 325

Figure 1. Standardized estimates of the confirmatory factor analysis for the attitudinal factor model.

Even though fathers tended to have unhealthier behaviours contributed to the changes. Our data consisted of three
than mothers did, the changes in Pori indicated that, in 2001– cross-sectional datasets, because we did not have any identi-
2003, they also tended to be more successful in improving fication variable in the reference town, Rauma, and even in Pori
those, whilst in 2003–2005 mothers tended to be more where we have, we didn’t ask the same parent to answer every
successful in improving their behaviours than fathers were. In year. This also resulted in parental data including mostly
Rauma, similar trends could not be seen. mothers and so few fathers that their results cannot be
It has previously been reported that children’s health generalized. In the children’s data, there were only a few drop-
promotion will influence also parents’ behaviour.[10,11] Our outs. In the first collection point, 1649 (98%) out of 1691
findings support previous reports that children influence their children returned the questionnaire. Of these 1649 children,
parents on the issues emphasized in health promotion 1483 (90%) also returned the last questionnaire, so there were
targeted to children, indicating that influencing is reciprocal. only 166 (10%) drop-outs. There were no differences in
This may create a snowball effect, but, on the other hand, it baseline behaviours between children who were followed-up
may put the child at risk for cross-pressure if influential throughout the study and those who dropped out during the
intervention is not in line with child’s parents’ thoughts or follow-up, but the children who dropped out were more likely
behaviours. Children are able to contribute and manage their to be boys than girls. Those children probably were not at
own health and well-being as well as that of other family school the day the questionnaires were filled out or had moved
members and they may function as health promoting to another town. The response rates were rather high also
actors.[19,20] The perspective of the children as social actors among parents, especially considering that children took the
will also create ethical demands and responsibility.[21] The parents’ questionnaires home and returned them after the
children are then seen not only as the health promoting agent parent had filled it in, presumably independently of the child. If
of their own health, but also the promoters of the health of they had filled it out together with the child, it could lead to
their families. bias, but we think that the children filling in their question-
The strengths of our study include large representative naires at school reduces the risk of that bias, as well as risk of
datasets, a long follow-up time and a sufficiently large proxy reporting. A more probable source of bias is using self-
reference group to allow us to consider how growing up reports on behaviours, which is often considered to harm
326 M. TOLVANEN ET AL.

Table 2. Mean weekly frequencies of oral health-related behaviors in 2001, 2003 and 2005 and their changes, separately for different towns and time periods and for
mothers and fathers.
Mean Change (95% CI) Change %
Respondent Town 2001 2003 2005 2001–2003 2003–2005 2001–2003 2003–2005
Brushing with Mother Pori 13.1 13.0 13.4 0.1 (0.5–0.3) 0.3 (0.1–0.7) 1 3
fluoride toothpaste Rauma 13.1 12.6 13.3 0.5 (2.9–2.0) 0.7 (0.0–1.4) 4 6
Father Pori 10.9 11.8** 11.1 1.0 (0.0–2.0) 0.8 (1.7–0.2) 9 6
Rauma 10.7 10.0** 11.2 0.7 (2.2–0.8) 1.1 (0.4–2.7) 7 11
Flossing Mother Pori 1.6 1.7 2.2 0.1 (0.2–0.4) 0.5 (0.1–0.8) 5 26
Rauma 1.7 1.7 2.1 0.1 (0.4–0.5) 0.4 (0.2– 0.9) 4 26
Father Pori 0.8 1.0 1.0 0.2 (0.5–0.9) 0.0 (0.6–0.6) 24 1
Rauma 0.6 0.7 0.7 0.1 (0.4–0.6) 0.0 (0.6–0.6) 11 4
Using xylitol products Mother Pori 7.2** 8.1 8.4 1.0 (0.2–1.7) 0.3 (0.6–1.1) 13 3
Rauma 8.6** 8.7 9.4 0.1 (1.2–1.5) 0.7 (0.8–2.2) 2 8
Father Pori 3.9 6.1 6.1 2.2 (0.8–3.7) 0.0 (1.7–1.8) 58 0
Rauma 6.5 6.5 6.3 0.0 (2.8–2.9) 0.2 (3.2–2.9) 0 3
Drinking sugary drinks Mother Pori 15.2 11.5 11.5 3.6 (4.8–2.5) 0.0 (1.1–1.1) 24 0
Rauma 16.0 12.9 12.9 3.1 (5.2–1.0) 0.0 (2.3–2.3) 19 0
Father Pori 18.6 18.9 19.4 0.3 (2.9–3.6) 0.5 (2.8–3.7) 2 2
Rauma 21.8 20.1 16.7 1.7 (4.8–1.4) 3.5 (8.3–.4) 8 17
Eating sugary snacks Mother Pori 6.9** 7.3** 6.9** 0.4 (0.1–0.9) 0.4 (1.0–0.2) 6 6
Rauma 7.9** 8.5** 8.3** 0.6 (0.3–1.5) 0.2 (1.3–1.0) 8 2
Father Pori 7.3 7.0* 6.4 0.3 (1.7–1.2) 0.7 (2.0–0.7) 3 10
Rauma 8.8 9.5* 6.8 0.7 (2.0–3.4) 2.7 (5.2–0.3) 8 29
**p50.01 for difference between the towns; *p50.05 for difference between the towns; Mann-Whitney U-test. Statistically significant changes are in italics. Changes
and change percentages have been calculated by using exact values of the mean frequencies.

Table 3. Sum scores of oral health-related knowledge and attitudes (scale 6–24) in 2001, 2003 and 2005 and their changes, separately for different towns and time
periods and for mothers and fathers.
Mean Change (95% CI) Change %
Respondent Town 2001 2003 2005 2001–2003 2003–2005 2001–2003 2003–2005
Knowledge Mother Pori 21.8 22.4* 22.3 0.6 (0.4–0.8) 0.1 (0.3–0.1) 3 1
Rauma 21.5 21.8* 22.3 0.3 (0.2–0.6) 0.5 (0.1–0.9) 1 2
Father Pori 20.8 21.6 21.7 0.8 (0.0–1.5) 0.2 (0.5–0.8) 4 1
Rauma 20.9 21.2 21.1 0.3 (0.8–1.3) 0.1 (1.1–0.9) 1 0
Concern about developing Mother Pori 18.1 17.9 18.0 0.3 (0.6–0.1) 0.1 (0.3–0.4) 1 0
caries lesions Rauma 17.8 17.8 17.6 0.0 (0.5–0.6) 0.2 (0.8–0.4) 0 1
Father Pori 17.1 17.6 17.2 0.5 (0.4–1.4) 0.4 (1.4–0.5) 3 2
Rauma 17.4 17.7 17.1 0.3 (1.1–1.8) 0.6 (2.1–0.9) 2 3
Importance of brushing for Mother Pori 22.0 22.0 21.9 0.0 (0.2–0.2) 0.0 (0.3–0.2) 0 0
social situations Rauma 22.0 22.0 21.9 0.0 (0.3–0.3) 0.0 (0.4–0.3) 0 0
Father Pori 20.1 20.3 20.3 0.3 (0.5–1.0) 0.0 (0.8–0.7) 1 0
Rauma 20.0 19.7 19.6 0.3 (1.5–0.8) 0.0 (1.3–1.2) 2 0
Importance of brushing for Mother Pori 22.9 23.1* 22.9 0.2 (0.0–0.3) 0.2 (0.4–0.0) 1 1
health and appearance Rauma 22.8 22.9* 23.1 0.1 (0.1–0.3) 0.2 (0.1–0.4) 0 1
Father Pori 22.0 22.0 21.6 0.0 (0.6–0.5) 0.3 (1.0–0.3) 0 2
Rauma 22.2 22.3 22.1 0.1 (0.7–0.8) 0.2 (1.0–0.5) 0 1
Importance of brushing Mother Pori 20.0 19.9 19.5 0.1 (0.5–0.3) 0.4 (0.8–0.0) 1 2
for acceptance Rauma 20.1 20.0 19.9 0.1 (0.7–0.5) 0.2 (0.8–0.5) 0 1
Father Pori 19.7 20.0 19.6 0.2 (0.7–1.2) 0.3 (1.4–0.7) 1 2
Rauma 20.4 19.9 19.0 0.5 (2.0–1.0) 0.9 (2.6–0.7) 2 5
*p50.05 for difference between the towns; Mann-Whitney U-est. Statistically significant changes are in italics. Changes and change percentages have been calculated
by using exact values of the mean frequencies.

validity and reliability, but, in this case, both validity and values, which indicate that the model should not be rejected
reliability were good. Also a possible source of bias are children based only on the model chi-square.[17,22] Sample size greater
who moved to Pori during the study, but we don’t think this than 200 has been suggested to provide sufficient statistical
has influenced the results much, because they have been power for this kind of analysis, but in the same time, a 2 value
exposed to the intervention since they moved in. has been suggested to be highly sensitive to sample size,
Another strength of this study is the number of attitudinal especially if the sample size is bigger than 200.[17] Other
variables, allowing us to use factor analysis as a scientific basis indexes used indicated reasonably good fit and good conver-
when defining attitudes, which is more preferable than using gent and discriminant validity. We tested only configural
single items when studying attitudes. MGCFA resulted in invariance, but not invariance with respect to factor loadings
significant 2 values. In this case, those can be due to rather (metric invariance), because the items were not exactly the
large sample sizes, big correlations in the model, and the four- same. We decided to use confidence intervals when evaluating
class categorical variables, all of which contribute to high 2 the statistical significances of the changes. If the CI does not
ACTA ODONTOLOGICA SCANDINAVICA 327

include zero, the change is statistically significant and the same [5] Mattila ML, Rautava P, Sillanpää M, Paunio P. Caries in five-year-old
is true if the CI’s of two groups do not overlap. We also children and associations with family-related factors. J Dent Res.
2000;79:875–881.
conducted generalized linear models with one combined [6] Poutanen R, Lahti S, Tolvanen M, Hausen H. Parental influence on
cross-sectional dataset with time, town and their interaction children’s oral health-related behavior. Acta Odontol Scand.
as factors. There were differences between the time points and 2006;64:286–292.
the towns. None of the interaction terms were statistically [7] Mattila ML, Rautava P, Ojanlatva A, et al. Will the role of family
significant, but some were close. This may indicate that the influence dental caries among seven-year-old children? Acta Odontol
Scand. 2005;63:73–84.
differences in changes between the towns were not big
[8] Kickbush I, Gordon J, Kropf D, O’Toole L. Learning well-being: a policy
enough to be detected, especially since most changes were priority for children and youth. A process for change. Universal
similar, even though those occurred in different time periods. Education Foundation. Meerbeke, BE: ABC Drukkerij 2011.
When interpreting the results, it must be kept in mind that this [9] Christensen P. Difference and similarity: How children competence is
cross-sectional modelling procedure considers time as a constituted in illness and its treatment. In: Hutchby I & Moran Ellis J
(eds). Children and social competence: Arenas of action. London, UK:
categorical grouping variable within the data, not as time
Falmer Press, 1998; 187–202.
points. [10] Evans D, Clark NM, Levison MJ, et al. Can children teach their parents
The results suggest that successful health promotion about asthma. Health Educ Behav. 2001;28:500–511.
targeted to children may be transferred on to their parents [11] Garbin CAS, Garbin AJI, dos Santos KT, Lima DP. Oral health
and also help them in mending their habits. Future oral health education in schools: promoting health agents. Int J Dent Hygiene.
2009;7:212–216.
promotions should be targeted, especially for children,
[12] Tolvanen M, Lahti S, Poutanen R, et al. Changes in children’s oral
because children are participants and mediators of the health health-related behavior, knowledge and attitudes during a 3.4-yr
promotion, not only receivers. The children are the keys for the randomized clinical trial and oral health-promotion program. Eur J
future and can carry the results far over many years. Oral Sci. 2009;117:390–397.
[13] Statistics Finland 2001. Tilastokeskus 2001. Ammattiluokitus 2001.
Helsinki (in Finnish).
Acknowledgements [14] Poutanen R, Lahti S, Hausen H. Oral health-related knowledge,
attitudes, and beliefs among 11 to 12-year-old Finnish schoolchildren
This study was supported by The Finnish State Research Funding and with different oral health behaviors. Acta Odontol Scand. 2005;63:10–
Finnish Dental Society Apollonia. We thank the children, parents and 16.
teachers in Pori and in Rauma, Finland, for their cooperation in this study. [15] Poutanen R. Boys and girls as health-promoting actors  determin-
ants of oral health-related lifestyle among 11- to 12-year-old
schoolchildren. Acta Universitatis Ouluensis D 942. Oulu: University
Declaration of interest of Oulu, 2007. URI: http://herkules.oulu.fi/isbn9789514285615/
The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are [16] Tolvanen M, Lahti S, Hausen H. Changes in toothbrushing
responsible for the content and writing of the paper. frequency in relation to changes in oral health-related knowledge
and attitudes among children–a longitudinal study. Eur J Oral Sci.
2010;118:284–289.
References [17] Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling.
New York, NY: Guilford, 2005;73–180.
[1] Tuckett D. Sociology as a science. In: Tuckett D, ed. An introduction [18] Rose G. Sick Individuals and Sick Populations. Int J Epidemiol.
to medical sociology. London, UK: Tavistock Publications, 1976/ 1985;14:32–38.
1980;15. [19] Christensen P. The health-promoting family: a conceptual framework
[2] Rossow I, Rice J. Concordance of parental and adolescent health for future research. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59:377–387.
behaviors. Soc Sci Med. 1994;38:1299–1305. [20] Montgomery-Andersen RA, Borup I. Family support and the child as
[3] Åstrøm AN, Jakobsen R. The effect of parental dental health behavior health promoting agent in the Arctic–‘‘the Inuit way’’. Rural Remote
on that of their adolescent offspring. Acta Odontol Scand. Health. 2012;12:1977
1996;54:235–241. [21] Christensen P, Prout A. Working with ethical symmetry in social
[4] Åstrøm AN. Parental influences on adolescents’ oral health behavior: research with children. Childhood. 2002;4:477–497.
two-year follow-up of the Norwegian Longitudinal Health Behavior [22] Hoe SL. Issues and procedures in adopting structural equation
Study participants. Eur J Oral Sci. 1998;106:922–930. modeling technique. J Appl Quant Method. 2008;3:76–83.

View publication stats

You might also like