You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/14533992

Unconscious mood-congruent memory bias in depression

Article  in  Journal of Abnormal Psychology · March 1996


DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.1.34 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
202 3,314

5 authors, including:

Philip Watkins Steven Verney


Eastern Washington University University of New Mexico
56 PUBLICATIONS   3,912 CITATIONS    64 PUBLICATIONS   1,218 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Does gratitude predict future joy? View project

Improving Native American Elder Access to and Use of Healthcare through Effective Health System Navigation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Philip Watkins on 19 March 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1996, Vol. 105, No. 1, 34-41 0021-843X/96/$3.00

Unconscious Mood-Congruent Memory Bias in Depression


Philip C. Watkins, Karen Vache, Steven P. Verney, Andrew Mathews
and Stephanie Muller Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit
Eastern Washington University

The purpose of this study was to investigate an unconscious or implicit mood-congruent memory
( MCM ) bias in clinical depression. Many studies have shown an explicit memory bias, but no study
has yet found an implicit MCM bias in clinical depression. The authors compared depressed and
control group participants on a conceptually driven implicit memory test. After studying words of
positive, neutral, and negative affective valences, participants produced free associations to various
cues. Implicit memory or priming was demonstrated by the production of more studied than un-
studied words to the association cues. Depressed participants showed more priming of negative
words, whereas controls showed more priming of positivewords, thus supporting the MCM pattern.
Also, no implicit memory deficit was found in depressed participants. These findings are discussed
in the context of several prominent theories of cognition and depression.

Mood-congruent memory (MCM) may be an important cog- avoidance of that party and consequently the avoidance of what
nitive mechanism in the maintenance of depression. M C M re- might be a mood repair behavior. Thus, investigations of MCM
fers to the tendency for depressed or sad individuals to remem- in depression may be important for determining specific cogni-
ber information consistent with their mood (i.e., negative or tive mechanisms that are active in the maintenance of
unpleasant memories). Research indicates that MCM is a ro- depression.
bust phenomenon in depression (for a review, see Blaney, Although MCM has been repeatedly demonstrated (for a re-
1986), and Teasdale (1983) has suggested that MCM might act view, see Blaney, 1986), almost all of these studies have used
to maintain depression. If an individual is sad or depressed, explicit or conscious memory measures. In explicit memory
MCM indicates that more negative memories are accessible. tests the individual consciously uses memory, and recall is di-
Recalling these memories tends to maintain or exacerbate the rected toward a specific learning experience. Most memory re-
depressed condition, which then leads to the recollection of search has used explicit memory tests. However, as Mason and
more negative memories, resulting in a vicious cycle. Graf have stated (1993), "'we now know, explicit memory is
Teasdale (1983) also suggested that MCM might help explain only a small part--the conscious tip of the iceberg--of how
why depressed individuals tend to avoid effective coping activi- memory for recent events influences us in our daily activities"
ties. Depression leads to increased recall of negative events. If (p. 8). Cognitive psychologists have been investigating uncon-
memory is biased in this fashion, the individual may have low- scious or implicit memory (Roediger, 1990; Schacter, 1987).
ered expectations regarding the outcome of coping activities, Implicit memory has been defined as "memory for information
resulting in the avoidance of these coping behaviors. The de- that was acquired during a specific episode and that is expressed
pressed state is then maintained as the result of the avoidance on tests in which subjects are not required, and are frequently
of effective coping behaviors. For example, ifa depressed person unable, to deliberately or consciously recollect the previously
is invited to a party, the tendency to remember negative experi- studied information" (Schacter, 1990, p. 338). If MCM is an
ences with parties in the past leads to lowered expectations for important maintenance mechanism in depression, it could be
enjoyment. These lowered expectations probably lead to the argued that this negative bias actually affects adaptive behavior
in an unconscious fashion rather than in the conscious way that
is usually tapped by explicit memory tests. To return to the ear-
Philip C. Watkins, Karen Vache, Steven P. Verney, and Stephanie lier example, it seems unlikely that a depressed individual who
Muller, Department of Psychology,Eastern WashingtonUniversity:An- is considering a party invitation would consciously attempt to
drew Mathews, Medical Research Council Applied Psychology Unit, remember other party experiences (explicit memory). More
Cambridge, England. likely, memory of past negative experiences unconsciously in-
Preparation of this article was supported in part by a grant from the fluences expectations regarding this event. It is possible that an
Northwest Institute of Advanced Studies. We thank Corby Martin and unconscious memory bias in depressed individuals is an impor-
Robert Carpenter for their assistance in data collection, and Tom Pyle tant factor influencing their negative view of reality. Thus, an
for his advice regarding data analysis. We also wish to express our grat-
implicit MCM bias may be an important cognitive mainte-
itude to Henry Roediger and Paula Hertel for their comments on drafts
of this article. nance mechanism in depression.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Philip To our knowledge, only two published studies have investi-
C. Watkins, Department of Psychology, MS-94, Eastern Washington gated MCM in clinical depression with implicit memory tests.
University,526 5th Street, Cheney, Washington 99004. Electronic mail Recently, Watkins, Mathews, Williamson, and Fuller (1992)
may be sent via Internet to pwatkins@ewu.edu. published a study that did not find a MCM bias in implicit
34
UNCONSCIOUS MEMORY BIAS 35

memory. In this study they compared depressed and control found an implicit memory deficit associated with depression
group participants on an implicit memory test and an explicit (Elliot & Greene, 1992), whereas several studies have not found
memory test. The implicit test used word stem completion (e.g., any overall priming difference with neutral materials (Denny &
complete this stem with the first word that comes to mind." Hunt, 1992; Hertel & Hardin, 1990; Watkins et al., 1992).
los ). The explicit memory test used cued recall and had There is some evidence that although conceptual variables at
identical memory cues as the implicit test. Thus, these two mea- encoding affect conceptually driven implicit tests, individual
sures differed only in whether the participants were consciously differences variables such as amnesia do not result in priming
using memory. Watkins et al. found the well-demonstrated deficits on conceptual tests (Shimamura, 1986). Thus, follow-
MCM bias in the explicit memory measure but found no MCM ing Roediger and McDermott ( 1992 ), we predicted that no im-
bias in the implicit memory test. Denny and Hunt (1992) con- plicit memory deficit would be associated with depression.
ducted a similar study with depressed participants but used A third purpose of this study was to test the elaboration hy-
word fragment completion as their implicit memory test and pothesis of depression proposed by Williams, Watts, MacLeod,
free recall as their explicit test. Their results were consistent and Mathews (1988). They argued that the negative cognitive
with Watkins et al.; they demonstrated MCM in the explicit test biases in depression are best explained by the tendency for de-
but did not find this negative bias in the implicit test. pressed individuals to elaborate negative information. Elabora-
In a commentary on these two studies, Roediger and McDer- tion is a more strategic cognitive process, and it is defined as
mott (1992) made some interesting predictions. They observed "the activation of a representation in relation to other associ-
that both studies used perceptually driven implicit memory ated representations to form new relations between them and
tests, meaning that in these tasks cognitive activities are guided to activate old relationships" (p. 170). In the test phase of this
by the perceptual features of the stimuli rather than by the experiment, participants produced free associations to positive,
meaning of the stimuli. For example, the meaning of los is neutral, and negative cues. If Williams et al. are correct, then
not important to completing this word stem with the first word depressed individuals should produce relatively more associa-
that comes to mind. However, many memory tests do rely on tions to negative cues and fewer associations to positive cues
meaningful processing of the stimuli and so are called concep- than nondepressed controls.
tually driven. Roediger (1990) has pointed out that virtually
all explicit memory tests are conceptually driven because the Method
individual must meaningfully relate memory cues to an earlier
learning experience. Thus, Roediger and McDermott (1992) Design and Overview
argued that the inability to find MCM with perceptually driven
This study used a 2 (Group: depressed individuals and controls) x 3
implicit tests was not surprising, because word meaning should
(Word Valence:positive, neutral, and negative) x 2 (Priming: studied
not be expected to affect tests that do not require conceptual
vs. unstudied words) mixed factorial design. Repeated measures were
processing. However, they predicted that MCM should be found Word Valenceand Priming. The between-subjectsvariable was Group.
in implicit memory if conceptually driven tests are used. This At study, participants were randomly assigned to study one of two word
prediction follows the transfer-appropriate processing ap- sets. Each word set contained positive, neutral, and negativewords. Par-
proach, which states that if cognitive processing at study and ticipants then completed a brief interference task. A free association
test are matched, individuals are more likely to recall the infor- task followed, which included association cues that were related to
mation than if processing at study and test are mismatched words from their studied word set and their unstudied word set.
( Morris, Bransford, & Franks, 1977). For example, if words are
studied in a perceptual fashion but the memory test requires Participants
meaningful processing, the transfer-appropriate processing ap-
proach would predict that individuals will recall less informa- We used two groups of participants in this study: clinically depressed
tion than if both the study task and the memory test are mean- individuals and nondepressed controls. We defined the depressed group
ingfully driven. as participants with Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores of greater
than 15 who met the diagnostic criteria for either major depression or
The purpose of this study was to investigate implicit MCM
dysthymia, as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Men-
using a conceptually driven test. We exposed depressed and con- tal Disorders ( 3rd ed., rev.:DSM-IH-R; American Psychiatric Associ-
trol group participants to positive, neutral, and negative words ation, 1987). Individuals with a reported history of mania were ex-
in the study phase, then administered a conceptually driven im- cluded. Diagnosis was determined through a structured interview, the
plicit memory test. In this test we presented participants with a Schedule for AffectiveDisorders and Schizophrenia, Form C ( SADS-C;
series of association cues and asked them to produce as many Endicott & Spitzer, 1978; see the description below). All interviews of
associates as they could to each cue for 30 s. Using this method- depressed participants were conducted by clinical graduate students.
ology, studies have found that participants reliably produce Interviewersmade independent diagnoses and then presented interview
more target words that they had seen before than targets they data to the principal investigator,who made the final determination of
had not studied (Graf, Shimamura, & Squire, 1985; Kihlstrom, the diagnosis. Diagnostic agreement was reached in 94% of the cases.
Depressed individuals were recruited from undergraduate psychol-
Schacter, Cork, Hurt, & Behr, 1990; Srinivas & Roediger,
ogy classes. A screening study was conducted where students were ad-
1990), thus demonstrating implicit memory. Following Roe-
ministered the BDI along with a number of instruments relating to var-
diger and McDermott (1992), we predicted that an MCM bias ious psychological issues. Individuals with a BDI score of at least 16
would be demonstrated with this memory measure. were scheduled for the experiment. Depressed participants had to have
The use of neutral words allowed us to investigate the possi- a BDI score of at least 16 at both screening and the experimental session.
bility of a general deficit in implicit memory. To date. one study We tested 33 individuals through the experimental protocol as potential
36 WATKINS, VACHE, VERNEY, MULLER, AND MATHEWS

depressed group participants; 13 actually met the criteria described Derry, 1982; see also Watkins et al., 1992, for a description ofa norming
above and were included in the depressed group. No participants were study). The positive and negative word groups were roughly equivalent
taking psychotropic medications. in length and frequency. These words were divided into two equivalent
Control group participants were defined as those individuals who bad sets consisting of positive, neutral, and negative words (see Table 1 ).
BDI scores of 5 or less and who did not meet the DSM-III-R One set served as the studied or primed set and the other as the unstud-
(American Psychiatric Association, 1987) diagnostic criteria for any ied set for each participant. Each word set consisted of 8 positive, 8
mood disorder. They also did not report a significant history of depres- neutral, and 8 negative words. Mean word frequency (using Thorndike
sion as measured by the Past Accounts of Sadness Test (PAST; Watkins & Lorge, 1944) for Word Set A was 28.54 words per million (SD =
& Curtis, 1995). Confirmation o f diagnosis was conducted through in- 35.65 ), and word frequency for Set B was 27.54 words per million (SD
terview with the SADS-C. Controls included in the analysis were = 27.67). Mean length of words was 8.04 letters (SD = 1.65) for Set A
matched to depressed participants with regard to gender, and we also and 7.25 letters (SD = 0.99) for Set B. In each group, Set A served as
attempted to match for age as closely as possible. Each group contained the primed set for 7 participants, and Set B was the primed word set for
10 women and 3 m e n ) Mean ages o f the depressed and control groups 6 participants.
were 19.62 and 19.15, respectively. Both control and depressed partici- We conducted an analysis to determine whether word set had any
pants who were recruited from psychology classes received partial effect on priming. Word set did not interact with priming, F( 1, 24) =
course credit for their participation. 0.50, ns, and no interactions with word set involving Group, Valence,
and Priming reached significance. All words chosen for the study task
were moderately related to certain cue words. In a pilot study, we asked
Materials participants to produce associations to a number o f positive and nega-
tive cues. We then chose targets from the words that several, but not
Clinicalmeasures. The BDI (Beck, 1978; Beck & Steer, 1987) was most, participants produced to the cues. This selection criteria was used
the primary self-report measure that was used for assessing depression so that most participants would not produce the unstudied target words
level. This is perhaps the most frequently used test in depression re- in the association task. If participants produced a high number of un-
search and has good psychometric data supporting its use. studied targets, then a differential priming effect would be ditficult to
To confirm depression diagnosis we used the SADS-C. This interview demonstrate. Neutral words and cues were chosen from those used by
scale is based on the Research Diagnostic Criteria ( RDC; Spitzer, Endi- Srinivas and Roediger (1990). Some lack of equivalence existed be-
cott, & Robbins, 1975, 1978). Because of the similarity of the RDC tween the emotional and neutral words in that the positive and negative
and DSM-II1-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987 ) criteria for words were adjectives and the neutral words were not.
depression, this interview is commonly used for confirming depression
diagnosis. Form C is a shorter interview but still covers depressive symp-
toms comprehensively. This form also provides a derived Hamilton Rat-
Apparatus
ing Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, 1960, 1967) score The word stimuli were presented in the study phase on an IBM-com-
(Endicott, Nee, Cohen, Fleiss, & Sarantakos, 1981). Thus, this in- patible computer. Likewise, the distractor task and the association cues
terview not only covers the DSM-III-R depression diagnostic criteria, in the test phase were also presented by computer. The presentation of
but it also provides a measure o f the severity of depression. the stimuli was programmed through the Micro Experimental Labora-
The PAST is a self-report questionnaire designed to assess an individ- tory software package Schneider, 1988).
ual's history of depression. It contains a simplified description of the
DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987 ) criteria for ma-
Procedure
jor depression and for dysthymia. Following each description the par-
ticipant is asked whether he or she experienced an episode o f depression Following the screening study, appropriate participants were called
as described. For major depression, participants are also asked to esti- and scheduled for experimental sessions. After reading and signing the
mate how many episodes o f depression they have undergone. At the consent form, they were introduced to the study phase of the experi-
conclusion of the questionnaire, they are asked several questions relat- ment. They were first randomly assigned to be exposed to one o f the two
ing to their history of depression (e.g. "Have you ever thought about word sets as described above. The set they were assigned to would serve
suicide?", "Have you ever felt so depressed you sought help from as their studied (primed) word set, and the set they were not assigned to
friends?"). Psychometric data on this instrument appear good. Data served as their unstudied word set (unprimed). Participants were told
were collected on the PAST over several studies with undergraduates that a number of words would appear on the screen, one at a time, and
from psychology courses. Reliability and validity data were collected that their task was to attempt to imagine themselves in a scene that
from 142 participants. Item to total correlations range from .28 to .61 involved themselves and the word presented. They were instructed to
(mean r = .46), and reliability is adequate (coefficient c~ = .81, 12 press a button on the computer keyboard "as soon as you have a clear
items). Validity data also appear good. The total score of the PAST is image of the scene." Following a 1 s "get ready" prompt, each stimulus
moderately correlated with the BDI (r = .43). Thus, although it is re- word appeared on the screen for 15 s. After imagining each scene, par-
lated to report o f current depression, the PAST measures another dis- ticipants were asked several questions about the scene: (a) they were
tinct variable. Similarly, the number of depressive episodes participants asked whether the scene was an actual scene from their past or an imag-
report is moderately related to the BD! ( r = .26, p < .01 ). As predicted, inary one, (b) they were asked whether they were the principal character
women had reliably higher PAST scores than men (t = 2.04, p < .02) of the scene, (c) they were asked to rate the vividness of the scene on a
and were more likely to report a past episode o f major depression (t = 9-point Likert-type scale, and (d) they were asked to rate the pleasant-
1.63, p < .05 ). There was no difference between the genders in report of ness-unpleasantness of the scene on a 9-point Likert-type scale.
past episodes of dysthymia. Thus, data from the PAST roughly corre- Following this sequence, the next stimulus word was presented. Five
spond to known prevalence patterns. Using this measure, we defined a
significant history of depression as two or more episodes of depression.
Wordstimuli. The words used in this study were chosen to represent A total of 22 participants who met the criteria for the control group
positive (pleasant), neutral, and negative (unpleasant) affective va- completed the experimental protocol. Statistical analyses comparing
lence. These positive and negative words were derived from past mood- the entire group of controls to the depressed group yielded essentially
congruent research (e.g., Gotlib, McLachlan, & Katz, 1988; Kuiper & identical results as that reported here.
UNCONSCIOUS MEMORY BIAS 37

Table l
Association Cues and Targets by Valenceand WordSet
Valence

Word set Positive Neutral Negative

Adored-Admired Clothing-Sweater Blamed-Punished


Attractive-Desirable Furniture-Dresser Bleak-Hopeless
Approval-Supported Kitchen utensils-Spatula Criticized-Judged
Hopeful-Optimistic Musical instrument-Piccolo Discontent-Unsatisfied
Loved-Appreciated Sports-Bowling Hated-Detested
Respected-Liked Senses-Hearing Isolated-Abandoned
Successful-Accomplished Types of cloth-Flannel Mistake-Failure
Creative-Talented Vegetables-Cabbage Ugly-Unwanted
Awarded-Praised Animals-Giraffe Downcast-Gloomy
Capable-Competent Colors-Magenta Empty-Lonely
Fulfilled--Gratified Fruits-Peaches Guilty-Accused
Outstanding-Superior Fuels-Propane Humiliated-Ashamed
Smart-Witty Parts of a car-Muffler Ignored-Neglected
Triumph-Overcome Reptiles-Turtles Inadequate-Unworthy
Valuable-Needed Sciences-Geology Unlovable-Rejected
Worthy-Accepted Trees-Willows Worthless-Useless

Note. In each pair, the first word or phrase is the cue, and the word following the dash is the target.

practice trials were presented, followed by the experimental trials. Stim- free association task was a memory test" while they were performing
ulus words were presented in random order. After the presentation of the association task. Then we asked them whether they noticed that
the 24 words in the study phase, all participants were exposed to a brief some of the associations they produced were from the words in the en-
distractor task. They were shown an array of numbers or letters (or coding task. Finally, we included a question regarding their estimation
both) on the screen, and their task was to determine as quickly as possi- of the percentage of positive and negative words in the encoding task.
ble whether there was a letter m located in the array. None of the letter
arrays formed a legitimate English word. This task took approximately
30 s to complete. Results
Participants were then introduced to the test phase. In the introduc-
tion of this phase an attempt was made to dissociate study and test.
Participant Characteristics
They were asked to participate in a "developmental experiment" and
M e a n B D I scores were 29.00 (SD = 9.27) for the depressed
were told that we needed to "develop some materials for a future exper-
g r o u p a n d 2.69 (SD = 1.70) for the controls. M e a n derived
iment." They were told that their task was to produce as many one-
word associations to the cue words as possible and that for some words H R S D scores were 18.46 ( S D = 7.71 ) a n d 4.58 ( S D = 2.23 ) for
(categories) the best way to produce associations was to list as many the depressed a n d control groups, respectively.
instances of the category as possible. However, other cues were not
clearly categories, and with these words "you will probably produce
Analysis of Associations
more associations if you think of words that are closely related to the
word (like synonyms)." In each trial, a "get ready" prompt appeared To test the hypothesis t h a t depressed p a r t i c i p a n t s elaborate
on the screen along with a tone (to reorient the participants ). Following negative i n f o r m a t i o n m o r e t h a n controls, we c o n d u c t e d a 2
the prompt, a cue word appeared and the participant was instructed to
( G r o u p ) X 3 ( C u e Valence) repeated m e a s u r e s analysis o f vari-
produce as many associations to the word as possible. Each association
ance (ANOVA) for n u m b e r of u n s t u d i e d associations produced.
cue remained on the computer screen for 30 s. Participants were given
one practice trial, which was then followed by 48 experimental trials. A n y association t h a t a p a r t i c i p a n t p r o d u c e d t h a t was n o t a stud-
Each of the 48 cue words was related to one of the words from either the ied target was included in this analysis. G r o u p was a between-
studied or unstudied word lists. Responses were recorded through audio subjects factor a n d Cue Valence was a within-subjects factor. We
tape, and the experimenters also made a written record of the critical present this analysis prior to the p r i m i n g analysis because the
responses. results b e a r on how we c o n d u c t e d the p r i m i n g analysis. M e a n s
Immediately following the free association task, participants were ad- for n u m b e r o f associations p r o d u c e d are shown in Table 2. A
ministered the BDI, PAST, and other measures. Then the experimenter large m a i n effect for Cue Valence was found, F ( 2, 48 ) = 315.0 l,
administered the SADS-C interview. Finally they were debriefed. The MSE = 146.46, p < .000 I. T h i s was d u e to all participants pro-
debriefing interview included open-ended questions regarding the par-
d u c i n g a m u c h larger n u m b e r o f associations to n e u t r a l cues
ticipants' perceptions of the purpose ofthe study. Second, we asked par-
ticipants how they came up with the associations in the free association (see Table 2). As predicted, the Valence X G r o u p interaction
task. We also asked more direct questions regarding the participants' was also significant, F ( 2 , 48 ) = 3.26, MSE = 477.59, p < .05.
awareness of the memory components of the implicit test. For example, We investigated this interaction by f o r m i n g a n association bias
we asked them whether they were "consciously trying to come up with score by s u b t r a c t i n g the n u m b e r o f associations p r o d u c e d to
word associations that were from the imagination task." (All partici- negative cues from the n u m b e r of associations p r o d u c e d to pos-
pants responded "'no.") We also asked whether they thought "that the itive cues. We then c o m p a r e d the groups with a t test, a n d the
38 WATKINS, VACHE, VERNEY, MULLER, AND MATHEWS

Table 2 ing scores for each valence by subtracting the number of un-
Number of Unstudied Associations Produced as a studied targets produced from the number of studied targets,
Function of Group and Valence thus incorporating the priming factor into the figure. This num-
ber was then turned into a proportion by dividing it by 8 (the
Group maximum priming score a participant could receive in a va-
Control Depressed lence category). As is evident from this figure, the pattern of
means for the two groups conforms to a mood-congruent pat-
Valence M SD M SD tern. To explore this interaction further, we compared the
groups with planned orthogonal contrasts of priming scores by
Positive 58.54 20.24 65.31 18.75
Neutral 131.54 32.54 131.08 14.29 Valence. We found that the control group had significantly
Negative 46.92 18.83 63.54 13.51 higher priming scores for positive targets than did depressed
participants (p < .05), and conversely depressed participants
had significantly higher priming scores for negative targets (p <
.05). The groups did not differ with regard to priming of neu-
group differences were found to be reliable, t = 2.53, p < . 05. tral targets (p = .90), thus showing no implicit memory deficit
The mean positive association bias score for controls was 11.62, for depressed participants.
and the mean for depressed participants was 1.77. Because of the group and valence differences in number of
associations produced, it could be argued that the number of
opportunities that existed for producing a target was not equiv-
P r i m i n g Analysis
alent across valences. Thus, we formed a dependent variable
An association was coded as a target if the participant pro- that accounted for the number of associations produced. We
duced one of the targets from either the studied or unstudied formed scores for each participant that represented the percent-
target list. Different forms of the words (e.g., failed instead of age of targets produced of the total number of associations pro-
f~ilure) were coded as targets produced. Acceptable responses duced for each valence condition. We then conducted a 2
were defined prior to data collection. Synonyms and words re- (Group) × 3 (Valence) × 2 (Priming) repeated measures
lated to targets were not coded as targets. For example, unsuc- ANOVA. Confirming our primary analysis, the interaction be-
cessful or loser were not coded as targets for the word failure. If tween Group, Valence, and Priming was significant, F(2, 48 ) =
a participant produced a target more than once, this was 3.46, M S E = 2.27, p < .05.
counted as only one target produced.
Prior to conducting our primary analysis,we first analyzed Discussion
number of unstudied targets produced by Group and Valence.
No main effect for Group was found, F( 1, 24) = 0.38, M S E = This study represents the first demonstration of an MCM bias
1.21, n s; the interaction between Group and Valence was not on a conceptually driven implicit memory test. Depressed par-
significant, F(2, 48) = .72, M S E = 1.02, ns. Thus, the groups ticipants were found to have greater priming of negative words
did not differentially respond on our baseline measure. How-
ever, a main effect of Valence was found, F(2, 48 ) = 8.31, M S E
= 1.02, p < .05. This effect paralleled our finding with the asso-
ciation analyses described earlier, in that participants produced Table 3
more neutral unstudied targets than positive or negative. Mean Number ql'Target Words Produced as a Function of
In our primary analysis we conducted a 2 (Group) X 2 Group, Valence, and Priming
(Priming) x 3 (Valence) repeated measures ANOVA for num- Group
ber of targets produced. Number of targets produced included
words produced that were from either the studied or the unstud- Control Depressed
ied word lists. Means for number of targets produced are shown
in Table 3. No main effect was found for Group, F( 1, 24) = Valence M SD M SD
0.58, M S E = 2,84, ns, indicating that the two groups did not Positive
differ in overall number of targets produced. A strong effect for Studied targets 2.85 1,34 2.31 1.03
Priming was found, F ( 1 , 2 4 ) = 63.50, M S E = 1.60, p < .0001, Unstudied targets 0.85 0.80 1.38 1.19
indicating the existence of an implicit memory effect. A main Difference (priming) 2.00 1.35 0.93 1.66
Neutral
effect was found for Valence, F( 2, 48 ) = 13.44, M S E = 1.1 O, p
Studied targets 3.46 1.56 3.46 0.88
< .0001. This was due to all participants producing more stud- Unstudied targets 1.92 1.38 1.85 1.14
ied and unstudied neutral word targets. Neutral words were not Difference (priming) 1.54 1.61 1.61 1.44
primed easier than positive or negative words, however. This was Negative
demonstrated by the absence of a Priming X Valence interac- Studied targets 1.92 1.66 3.23 1.24
Unstudied targets 0.77 0.83 0.77 0.72
tion, F(2, 48) = 0.45, M S E = .89, ns. Difference (priming) 1.15 1.72 2.46 1.13
Most importantly (and as predicted), the three-way interac- Total
tion between Group, Priming, and Valence was significant, F(2, Studied targets 8.23 3.63 9.00 2.48
48) = 5.21, M S E = 0.89, p < .05. The nature of our finding is Unstudied targets 3.54 2.07 4.00 1.73
Difference (priming) 4.69 3.52 5.00 2.61
depicted in Figure 1. This figure was formed by creating prim-
UNCONSCIOUS MEMORY BIAS 39

implicit MCM appears to be consistent with that found in


mood state-dependent recall research. Using a musical mood
induction methodology, Eich and associates have found evi-
dence of mood state-dependent recall in conceptually driven
implicit memory measures, but not in perceptually driven im-
plicit tests (for a review of these studies, see MacAulay, Ryan, &
Eich, 1993).
One difference in the pattern of findings between implicit
tests could be due to participants being more aware that con-
ceptually driven tests involve memory than the perceptually
driven tests (see Bowers & Schacter, 1990). Data from the de-
briefing interviews in this study seem to contradict this hypoth-
esis, however. In the current study, 42% of the participants
claimed they did not recognize that they had produced any as-
sociations from the study phase. In the Watkins et al. (1992)
study, 96% of the participants recognized that they used studied
words to complete the word stems. Because almost half of the
participants in the current study were unaware that they had
produced any studied targets, we defined these participants as
m e m o r y u n a w a r e and re-analyzed our data with this memory
awareness factor. This analysis showed no significant interac-
tions with the other factors (i.e., Group, Priming, Valence).
Thus, awareness of the memory-based aspects of the implicit
Figure 1. Mean priming proportion by group and word valence. Prim- test appeared to have no impact on the implicit MCM effect
ing proportion is number of studied minus unstudied targets produced, found here.
divided by the maximum priming score ( 8 ). It could be argued that referring to our finding as an uncon-
scious m e m o r y bias is problematic (see Schacter, Bowers, &
Booker, 1989). After all, 58% of our participants had at least
and less priming of positive words than controls. We believe some awareness of the memory aspects of the experiment. We
this finding may have important implications for understanding hold that the conscious aspects of this study are largely epiphe-
depression. It may be that the negatively colored view of the nomena, that is, participants unconsciously (unintentionally)
world exhibited by depressed individuals and their avoidance of produced a studied target and then recognized that the associa-
activities that may help repair their mood are influenced by this tion was from the study task. In a series of studies using percep-
unconscious negative memory bias. Although this study may tual identification as their implicit test, Richardson-Klavehn,
lack some generalizability in that the depressed participants Lee, Joubran, and Bjork (1994) showed that the principle
were college students, a recent review has shown that there is difference between implicit and explicit tests was in whether the
little reason to believe that findings from depressed college stu- individuals used intentional recollection. Thus, implicit mem-
dents are different from depressed individuals at large ory tests appear to be tapping incidental retrieval. In fact, all of
(Vredenburg, Flett, & Krames, 1993). We agree with Coyne our participants stated that they were not "consciously trying"
(1994) that Vredenburg et al. confused the question "Are de- to recall items from the study task. However, most were aware
pressed college students different from depressed subjects at that some of their associations were studied targets. Thus, it is
large?" with "Are college students with elevated BDI scores possible that they can unconsciously retrieve an item and then
different from depressed subjects at large?" However, Coyne's become consciously aware that the item was previously studied.
critique does not appear to preclude generalizing from a prop- Future research may determine whether a participant's aware-
erly diagnosed depressed college population. ness that an item was previously studied affects subsequent re-
The finding of the current study contrasts with two other trieval. In this study, however, test awareness had no impact on
studies that did not find an implicit MCM bias (Denny & Hunt, the variables of interest.
1992; Watkins et al., 1992 ). As pointed out earlier, both of these How does our primary finding here fit with prominent theo-
studies used perceptually driven implicit memory tests, whereas ries of cognition and sadness-depression? It seems that a con-
we used a conceptually driven test. It seems that the most ceptually driven MCM bias would be consistent with the
straightforward way to interpret this pattern of results is to as- spreading activation approach of Bower ( 1981 ) and the schema
sert that conceptually driven processes at test reveal MCM in theory of Beck (Kovacs & Beck, 1978). However, neither of
depression, whereas perceptually driven processes do not. Al- these theories make specific predictions about cognitive biases
though we believe this to be the case, other explanations, such in implicit memory. Even if we deduce from these theories the
as increased sensitivity of the test and use of different word sets, prediction of a conceptually driven MCM bias, it would be
are also possible. difficult for these theories to explain the absence of MCM with
Often, mood state-dependent recall and MCM are conceptu- perceptually driven tests. The lack of a mood-congruent bias in
ally linked in theories of emotion and cognition (e.g., Bower, perceptually driven tests and our finding of MCM in a concep-
1981). It is of interest to note that the pattern of findings in tually driven test appear to be handled by the approach of Wil-
40 WATKINS, VACHE, VERNEY, MULLER, AND MATHEWS

liams et al. (1988). They argue that the cognitive bias in depres- Beck, A.T., & Steer, R. ( 1987 ). Beck Depression Inventory manual. San
sion is limited more to elaborative processes rather than the ini- Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanonich.
tial "pick up" or activation of the information in memory. If we Blaney, P.H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bul-
concede that elaboration is a conceptually driven process, then letin, 99, 229-246.
the Williams et al. approach seems to account for the pattern of Blaxton, T.A. ( 1989 ). Investigating dissociations among memory mea-
sures: Support for a transfer-appropriate processing framework.
findings. In fact, several studies have shown that the manipula-
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cogni-
tion of conceptual variables at encoding affect conceptually tion, 15, 657-668.
driven tests much like they influence explicit memory tests Bower, G.H. ( 1981 ). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,
(Blaxton, 1989; Hamann, 1990; Smith & Branscombe, 1987, 129-148.
1988; Srinivas & Roediger, 1990). These conceptual manipula- Bowers, J.S., & Schacter, D.L. (1990). Implicit memory and test aware-
tions have little or no effect on perceptually driven implicit ness. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and
memory tests (for a review, see Roediger, 1990). Cognition, 16, 404-416.
We also tested the elaboration hypothesis of Williams et al. Coyne, J. C. (1994). Self-reported distress: Analog or eratz depression?
(1988) by comparing the number of associations produced to Psychological Bulletin, 116, 29-45.
the valenced cues between the two groups. As predicted, a Denny, E.R., & Hunt, R.R. (1992). Affective valence and memory in
mood-congruent pattern was found. This finding clearly seems depression: Dissociation of recall and fragment completion. Journal
to support the elaboration hypothesis. of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 575-580.
This study also speaks to the issue of whether an implicit Elliot, C.L., & Greene, R.L. (1992). Clinical depression and implicit
memory deficit exists in depression. Although several studies memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,572-574.
Endicott, J., Nee, J., Cohen, J., Fleiss, J.L., & Sarantakos, S. ( 1981 ).
did not find an implicit deficit in depression and sadness
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale extracted from Schedule for
(Denny & Hunt, 1992; Hertel & Hardin, 1990; Watkins et al.,
AffectiveDisorders and Schizophrenia and SADS-C. Archives of Gen-
1992), one study found that depressed individuals did show an eral Psychiatry, 38. 98-103.
implicit memory deficit with word stem completion and homo- Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R.L. ( 1978 ). A diagnostic interview. Archives of
phone spelling (Elliot & Greene, 1992). Although Roediger and General Psychiatry 35, 837-844.
McDermott (1992) pointed out some methodological diffi- Gotlib, I.H., McLachlan, A.L., & Katz, A.N. (1988). Biases in visual
culties with this study, they concluded that these problems attention in depressed and nondepressed individuals. Cognition and
could not account for the priming deficit demonstrated by El- Emotion, 2, 185-200.
liot and Greene (1992). Our result here agrees with the findings Graf, P. (1990). Lifespan changes in implicit and explicit memory. Bul-
of the other studies that used perceptually driven tests. Thus, as letin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 353-358.
predicted by Roediger and McDermott (1992), the bulk of the Graf, P., Shimamura, A.P., & Squire, L.R. ( 1985 ). Priming across mo-
data suggests that implicit memory is intact in depression. De- dalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of
pression, like amnesia and age, is related to a deficit in explicit preserved function in amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
memory, but does not appear to impair implicit memory (e.g., Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11,386-396.
Graf, 1990; Schacter, 1985 ). Hamann, S.B. (1990). Level of processing effects in conceptually-
driven implicit tasks. Journal of Experimental P~ychology."Learning,
To summarize, this study represents the first demonstration
Memory, and Cognition, 16, 970-977.
of an implicit MCM bias in clinical depression. This was dem-
Hamilton, M.A. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neu-
onstrated with a conceptually driven implicit test. By eontrast, rology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56-61.
perceptually driven tests have not revealed MCM in clinical de- Hamilton, M.A. ( 1967 ). Development of a rating scale for primary de-
pression (Denny & Hunt, 1992; Watkins et al., 1992). Thus, as pressive illness. British Journal of School and Clinical Psychoh~gy, 6,
predicted by Roediger and McDermott (1992), conceptually 278-296.
driven implicit memory appears to conform to the same pattern Hertel, P.T., & Hardin, T.S. (1990). Remembering with and without
as that of explicit memory, in which MCM has frequently been awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative.
demonstrated in depression (Blaney, 1986). Second, no im- Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 119. 45-59.
plicit memory deficit was found with depressed individuals. Ev- Johnson, M.H., & Magaro, P.A. ( 1987 ). Effects of mood and severity
idence appears to be converging to support the supposition that on memory processes in depression and mania. Psychological Bulle-
no implicit memory deficit exists in depression, contrasting tin. 101, 28-40.
with the explicit memory deficit that has been repeatedly dem- Kihlstrom, J.E, Schacter, D.L., Cork, R.C., Hurt, C.A.. & Behr, S.E.
onstrated ( for a review, see Johnson & Magaro, 1987). Whereas (1990). Implicit and explicit memory following surgical anesthesia.
it is tempting to conclude that MCM exists in conceptually Psychological Science, 1,303-306.
driven memory and not in perceptually driven memory, this Kovacs, M., & Beck, A.T. (1978). Maladaptive cognitive structures in
depression. American Journal ofPsychiatr); 135, 525-533.
conclusion is premature at this point. More research is needed
Kuiper, N.A., & Derry, P.A. (1982). Depressed and nondepressed
to test this conclusion by using several different perceptually
content self-reference in mild depressives. Journal ~?fPersonality 50,
driven and conceptually driven tests in the same study. 67-80.
MacAulay, D., Ryan, L., & Eich, E. (1993). Mood dependence in im-
References plicit and explicit memory. In P. Graf& E. J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit
American Psychiatric Association. ( 1987 ). Diagnostic and statistical memory. New directions in cognition, development, and neuropsychol-
manual of mental disorders ( 3rd ed., rev.). Washington, 13(?:Author. ogy (pp. 75-94 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Beck, A.T. ( 1978 ). Depression inventory Philadelphia: Center for Cog- Mason, M. E., & Graf, P. ( 1993 ). Introduction: Looking back and into
nitive Therapy. the future. In P. Graf& E. J. Masson (Eds.), Implicit memory. New
UNCONSCIOUS MEMORY BIAS 41

directions in cognition, development, and neuropsychology (pp. 1- cial inferences: The case of category accessibility effects. Journal of
11 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum. Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 361-382.
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of pro- Smith, E.R., & Branscombe, N.R. ( 1988 ). Category accessibility as im-
cessing versus transfer-appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal plicit memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 490-
Learning and VerbalBehavior, 16, 519-533. 504.
Richardson-Klavehn, A., Lee, M. G., Joubran, R., & Bjork, R. A. Spitzer, R.L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1975). Research diagnostic
(1994). Intention and awareness in perceptual identification prim- criteria. Psychopharmacologia Bulletin, 11, 22-25.
ing. Memory and Cognition, 22, 293-312. Spitzer, R.L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic
Roediger, H.L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remem- criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36,
bering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043-1056. 773-782.
Roediger, H.L., & McDermott, K.B. (1992). Depression and implicit Srinivas, K., & Roediger, H.L. (1990). Classifying implicit memory
memory: A commentary. JournalofAbnormalPsychology, 101,587- tests: Category association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory
591. and Language, 29, 389-412.
Schacter, D.L. (1985). Priming of old and new knowledge in amnesic Teasdale, J.D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect,
patients and normal controls. Annals of the New York Academy of or reciprocal relationship? Advances in Behaviour Research and
Science, 444.44-53. Therapy, 5, 3-25.
Schacter, D.L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Thorndike, E. L., & Lorge, I. ( 1944 ). The teacher's word book of30,O00
Journal ~f Experimental Psychology."Learning, Memory, and Cogni- words. New York: Teacher's College, Columbia University.
tion, 13, 501-518. Vredenburg, K., Flett, G. L., & Krames, L. (1993). Analogue versus
Schacter, D. L. (1990). Introduction to "Implicit memory: Multiple clinical depression: A critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin,
perspectives?' Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 28, 338-340. 113, 327-344.
Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. (1989). Intention, awareness, Watkins, P.C., & Curtis, N. ( 1995 ). A self-report instrument for the as-
and implicit memory: The retrieval intentionality criterion. In S. sessment of past episodes of depression. Manuscript in preparation.
Lewandowsky, J. C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.), Implicit memory: Watkins, P.C., Mathews, A., Williamson, D.A., & Fuller, R.D. (1992).
Theoretical issues (pp. 47-65 ). Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum. Mood-congruent memory in depression: Emotional priming or elab-
Schneider, W. ( 1988 ). Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated oration? Journal of Abnorrnal Psychology, 101, 581-586.
system for IBM PC compatibles. Behavior Research Methods, Instru- Williams, J.M.G., Watts, EN., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988).
ments, and Computers, 20, 206-217. Cognitive psychology and the emotional disorders. New York: Wiley.
Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a
dissociable memory function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Received May 3, 1994
Psychology, 24, 490-504. Revision received March 13, 1995
Smith, E.R., & Branscombe, N.R. (1987). Procedurally mediated so- Accepted May 19, 1995 •

View publication stats

You might also like