You are on page 1of 6

Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134 Original Article

Memory span measured by the spatial span


tests of the Cambridge Neuropsychological
Test Automated Battery in a group of Brazilian
children and adolescents
Rosani Aparecida Antunes Teixeira, Elaine Cristina Zachi,
Daniela Tsubota Roque, Anita Taub, Dora Fix Ventura

Abstract  –  The neuropsychological tests of spatial span are designed to measure attention and working memory.
The version of the spatial span test in the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)
evaluates these functions through the recall of sequences of spatial locations presented to the subject. Objective:
The present study investigated how age, gender and educational level might affect the performance of the non-
verbal system. Methods: A total of 60 children and adolescents aged 6 to 18 years were assessed (25 males and 35
females). Results: The results showed no gender differences in test performance. Children with six or more years
of education showed better performance than children with less than three years of education. Older children
had more schooling and thus were able to recall a greater number of items. Span length values proved similar
to a previous large normative study which also employed the CANTAB Spatial Span (De Luca et al., 2003).
Conclusion: The similarity in performance of the Brazilian children and adolescents studied and the group of
Australian participants examined by the cited authors, despite the socio-cultural and economical differences,
points to the suitability of the task for the assessment of attention and working memory in Brazilian children.
Key words: neuropsychological tests, CANTAB, spatial span, working memory, children, educational status.

Capacidade de memória medida por testes de retenção espacial da bateria neuropspicologica computadorizada
de Cambridge em um grupo de crianças e adolescentes brasileiros
Resumo  –  Testes neuropsicológicos de span visuoespacial são construídos para avaliar amplitude atencional e
memória de trabalho. Na versão do teste de span visuoespacial da Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated
Battery (CANTAB) essa avaliação é feita através de sequências de localização espacial. Objetivo: O presente estudo
investiga como a idade, o sexo e a escolaridade podem afetar o desempenho nesse sistema não-verbal. Métodos:
Foram avaliadas 60 crianças e adolescentes com idades entre 6 e 18 anos (25 meninos; 35 meninas). Resultados:
Os resultados não mostraram diferenças no desempenho do teste entre os sexos. O grupo de crianças com seis
ou mais anos de escolaridade foi melhor do que a de crianças menores de três anos de estudo. Nossos resultados
sugerem que as crianças mais velhas e, portanto, com maior tempo de escolaridade conseguem se lembrar de
um número maior de itens. Os valores de amplitude foram similares aos obtidos em um experimento normativo
prévio amplo no qual foi utilizado o teste Spatial Span do CANTAB (De Luca et al., 2003). Conclusão: Estas
concordâncias de desempenhos entre crianças e adolescentes brasileiros e o grupo de participantes australianos
examinados pelos autores citados nos mostram que apesar das diferenças sócio-culturais e econômicas existentes,
a tarefa é adequada para avaliação de atenção e memória de trabalho em crianças brasileiras.
Palavras-chave: testes neuropsicológicos, CANTAB, spatial span, memória de trabalho, criança, nível de
escolaridade.

¹Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo SP, Brazil; 2Instituto de Psiquiatria, Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo,
São Paulo SP, Brazil.
Rosani Aparecida Antunes Teixeira  –  Av. Prof. Mello Morais, 1721 / Bloco A / Sala D-9 - 05508-030 São Paulo SP - Brazil. Email: rosanit@usp.br
Disclosure: The authors reports no conflicts of interest.
Received March 02, 2011. Accepted in final form May 05, 2011.

Teixeira RAA, et al.     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     129
Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134

Memory span is “the ability to grasp a number of dizs- touch-screen computerized system that provides an accu-
crete units in a single moment of attention and to reprodu- rate neuropsychological assessment and latency recording.
ce them immediately”.1 Memory span assessment measures Some tests require a press pad, mainly those measuring
the ability to recall series of discrete stimuli, such as digits, reaction time.
letters, words, sounds, immediately after their presentation. The Spatial Span test is a computerized version of the
Practically every type of material can be used in test span Corsi Block Tapping Task6 and assesses the ability to re-
capacity.1 member a sequence of squares lighting up on the screen.
The Spatial Span Test is frequently considered a non- The Corsi Block Tapping task was developed in the early
verbal analogue of the Digit Span Test, which measures 1970s as a visuospatial counterpart to the verbal-memory
the capacity of visuospatial memory.2 The most frequently span task and has frequently been used to assess visuospa-
cited theoretical model in recent research on verbal short- tial short-term memory performance in adults, children
term memory development has been the Baddeley and and patients with neuropsychological deficits. Vandieren-
Hitch3 working memory model. donck and collaborators7 explored the information-pro-
Working memory is a limited capacity system serving cessing operations measured by the Corsi Blocks Tapping
to keep “active” a limited amount of information for a task within the working-memory framework developed by
brief period of time, and then to operate on it. The Badde- Baddeley and Hitch.3
ley and Hitch working memory model includes a central
component, the central executive, and three sub-systems: Methods
the phonological loop, the visuospatial sketchpad and the Subjects included 60 children and adolescents aged 6 to
episodic buffer.4 18 years (9.26±2.79 ys) with varying levels of formal edu-
In its original formulation3 this model proposed the cation (3.91±2.55 years). The children were recruited from
existence of two separate systems involved in working me- the Escola de Aplicação, Universidade de São Paulo, São
mory. These two systems handled different classes of infor- Paulo, Brazil. Exclusion criteria were history of head injury
mation: the articulatory loop handles speech-based (pho- and/or psychiatric illnesses. The procedures were approved
nological) information that allows auditory information by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Universi-
to be held through a rehearsal mechanism that prevents its ty Hospital of the University of São Paulo (SISNEP CAAE:
rapid decay, and the visuospatial sketchpad handles visual 0026.0.198.000-06) and written informed consent was ob-
(e.g., color) and spatial (e.g., location) information allo- tained from participants or their guardians, prior to testing.
wing this information to be maintained and manipulated.4 The demographic characteristics of the participants are
The episodic buffer is an addition to the original summarized in Table 1.
working memory model. It was proposed by Baddeley in The Spatial Span Test is a test from the Cambridge
order to handle phenomena that were not covered by the Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTABe-
first model. In the revised model he intended to bring to- clipse)8 that assesses working memory capacity, and is a
gether and integrate the information from the other com- visuospatial analogue of the Digit Span test. In the Spatial
ponents of working memory, together with information Span Test, white squares are shown, some of which briefly
about time and order.4
The present study is concerned with memory for spa-
tial information and therefore assesses the visuospatial Table 1. Demographic features of children and adolescents in-
sketchpad proposed by Baddeley and Hitch. The aim was cluded in the study.
to investigate how age, gender and educational level might AGE Years of
affect the performance of the non-verbal system, using a (yrs) Total N Males Females schooling (SD)
modern computerized instrument called the Cambridge 6 7 4 3 1 (0)
Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) 7 10 4 6 1.8 (0.42)
(Cambridge Cognition, 2005). The CANTAB was deve- 8 8 2 6 2.57 (0.53)
loped over 20 years ago at the University of Cambridge
9 8 3 5 3.25 (0.46)
by Robbins and Sahakian 5 for evalutation of cognitive
10 13 3 10 4.35 (0.49)
function.
The CANTAB is a computerized neuropsychological 11 5 3 2 5 (0)
battery consisting of 22 tests for assessing memory, atten- 12-18 9 5 4 8.22 (2.33)
tion and executive function. This software tool has been Total 60 24 36
widely used since the 1990s. Subjects are tested using a SD: standard deviation.

130     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     Teixeira RAA, et al.
Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134

change colour in a variable sequence. For each trial, nine sual span test for the items span forward and backward,
randomly arranged white squares are shown on the screen. attempts forward and backward, or errors forward and
One by one the squares light up in colour, in a variable backward (Table 2).
sequence and children were instructed to remember the Children and adolescents were divided into groups
sequence. At the end of the presentation, the children are according to years of schooling. Group 1 was comprised
required to touch each of the boxes that had lit up, in the by children with only one year of schooling, Group 2 by
same order in the first part of the test, and again in reverse those with two years of schooling, Group 3 three years of
order in the second part. The task begins with the simplest schooling, Group 4 four years of schooling, Group 5 five
level of a two box sequence. After each successful trial, the years of schooling, and Group 6 by children with six or
number of boxes in the sequence is increased one by one more years of schooling.
to a maximum of nine. If the child’s response was incorrect When the results of the same test were compared, no
at any particular level, an alternate sequence of the same statistical difference between the scores of forward and
length was presented. The test is terminated when the child backward Spatial Span was observed (p≥0.05) (Table 3).
fails three consecutive trials at any one level.9 The numbers of attempts increased with age, while the
The measure obtained was the longest visual span, de- numbers of errors decreased with age in almost all groups
fined by the maximum number of boxes the subject cor- (Table 4).
rectly touched. With regard to the variable years of schooling, sta-
tistical differences were found between groups with 1 yr
Data analysis (p<0.01), 2 yrs (p<0.01), 3 yrs (p=0.01) versus 6 yrs on
To test whether the data were normally distributed, forward span scores, and also between the 1 yr (p<0.01), 2
Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were run (Z=0.65, not signi- yrs (p=0.01), 3 yrs (p=0.01), 4 yrs (p=0.02) groups versus
ficant). Intergroup comparison between gender groups the 6 year Group, on the backward span scores.
and among different schooling levels were performed by The analysis as a function of age compared the follo-
analysis of variance (ANOVA). Associations between span wing groups: 6 year-old children, 7-year-olds, 8-year-olds,
scores and age were analyzed using regression models. The 9-year-olds, 10-year-olds, 11-year-olds and 12-18 year-old
significance level used was 0.05. children.
Statistical differences were found for age between the
Results group of 6-year-olds (p=0.04) compared with 10 and 12-
No statistical differences were found between male 18 year-olds, 7 year-olds (p<0.01), as well as between the
(n=24) and female (n=36) participants’ results on the vi- group of 9 year-olds (p<0.01) compared with 10 and 12 ye-

Table 2. Performance by gender.


Sex (n) Span forward Span backward Attempts forward Attempts backward Errors forward Errors backward
Male (24) 5.04 (1.54) 4.92 (1.65) 7.92 (2.30) 7.96 (2.35) 12.20 (6.01) 12.12 (5.39)
Female (36) 5.02 (1.36) 4.70 (1.65) 7.6 (1.81) 7.37 (2.26) 12.22 (5.02) 10.94 (5.01)
Mean (standard deviation).

Table 3. Performance by years of schooling.


Years of Forward Backward
schooling Span Mean Span Mean Forward attempts Backward attempts Forward errors Backward errors
(n) (SD) (SD) P-Value Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
1 (9) 3.66 (0.5) 3.33 (0.86) ≥0.05 6.0 (1.0) 5.66 (1.32) 10.33 (3.27) 8.33 (2.91)
2 (11) 4.63 (1.2) 4.45 (1.36) ≥0.05 7.0 (1.48) 7.18 (1.94) 11.63 (5.42) 10.91 (4.64)
3 (10) 4.8 (1.13) 4.3 (1.41) ≥0.05 7.40 (1.07) 7.70 (3.23) 11.80 (2.89) 12.40 (7.64)
4 (11) 5.2 (1.13) 4.8 (0.78) ≥0.05 8.70 (2.05) 7.50 (1.35) 14.90 (7.26) 12.0 (4.89)
5 (9) 5.2 (1.39) 5.1 (1.10) ≥0.05 8.0 (2.21) 7.70 (1.70) 12.10 (4.38) 10.60 (3.47)
≥6 (10) 6.6 (1.57) 6.3 (1.76) ≥0.05 9.40 (2.27) 9.80 (2.09) 12.40 (7.54) 14.20 (5.30)
SD (standard deviation).

Teixeira RAA, et al.     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     131
Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134

Table 4. Performance of different age groups.


Age Forward attempts Backward attempts Forward errors Backward errors
(n) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
6 (7) 3.57 (0.53) 3.28 (0.95) 9.71 (2.36) 9.00 (2.94)
7 (10) 4.50 (1.80) 4.50 (1.35) 12.20 (6.01) 10.60 (4.99)
8 (8) 5.00 (0.75) 4.00 (0.92) 13.12 (4.76) 10.50 (3.96)
9 (8) 4.87 (1.45) 4.50 (1.69) 12.12 (4.29) 11.87 (8.23)
10 (13) 5.30 (0.94) 4.84 (0.55) 14.46 (6.09) 11.61 (4.50)
11 (5) 4.80 (1.78) 5.40 (1.34) 9.61 (2.07) 11.20 (4.54)
≥12 (9) 6.66 (1.65) 6.44 (1.81) 11.66 (7.61) 14.55 (5.50)
SD (standard deviation).

A B
r=0.53, p<0.01, y=2.47+0.27*x r=0.57, p<0.01, y=1.80+0.31*x
10 10
9 9
8 8
7 7
Span (Backward)
Span (Forward)

6 6
5 5
4 4
3 3
2 2
1 1
0 0
4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20
Age (years) Age (years)

Figure 1. Regression lines representing Spatial Span Performances according to age, Pearson’s coefficient (r), p-values, and regression equations.
[A] forward trial and [B] backward trial.

Table 5. Means and standard deviations for SSP by age group and
ar-olds, in the forward span scores, and between the groups
gender in present study and DeLuca17 study.
of 6-year-olds (p<0.01), 7 year-olds (p=0.02), 8 year-olds
(p=0.01), and 9 year-olds (p=0.03) compared with 12-18 SSP
year-olds, in the backward span scores. Present study DeLuca study
Linear regression analysis showed significant predic- N SPAN (SD) N SPAN (SD)
tion for better span performances by increasing age for 6-7 (years)
both forward trials and backward trials (Figure 1). Some     Male 8 4.50 (1.19)
points corresponded to more than one participant (e.g.     Female 10 4.00 (0.94)
four 6-year-old children showed the same span length on 8 -10 (years)
forward trial).
    Male 9 4.77 (1.30) 13 5.71 (0.91)
    Female 19 5.21 (0.91) 16 5.56 (1.09)
Discussion
11-14 (years)
In the present study, an automated version of the
    Male 7 5.42 (1.51) 13 6.46 (1.51)
spatial span task was used that is a variation on the Cor-
    Female 4 6.50 (2.51) 16 6.00 (1.20)
si Block-Tapping task, a manual test in which the target
locations are presented as nine wooden cubes fixed to a 15-19 (years)
wooden board.8     Male 1 5.04 (1.50) 18 7.76 (1.34)
Our results showed no statistical difference betwe-     Female 2 5.50 (0.70) 39 6.63 (1.50)

132     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     Teixeira RAA, et al.
Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134

en the sexes for span performance, although girls had a Indeed, the fact that the groups with 1 to 3 years of scho-
higher average than boys at ages of between 8 to 14 years oling committed significantly more errors than those with
on the forward span, while boys outperformed girls on the more than 6 years of education may reflect an influence
backward span. This finding corroborates previous results of age. This increase in forward spatial span performance
obtained with the Corsi Block-Tapping task in American with advancing age was also found using the Corsi Block-
children and young adults,9 as well as in Brazilian chil- Tapping task.14,15
dren10 where no statistical differences were found betwe- The total number of errors is lower in reverse order be-
en boys and girls. Similarly, Postma et al.11 also found no cause the test is stopped when the subject makes three con-
statistical differences between the sexes in spatial working secutive errors. In direct order, mistakes tend to be made
memory. This is also true with regard to verbal span, as throughout the test and therefore the total number of trials
concluded in the meta-analysis by Lynn and coauthors12 and errors is higher. In reverse order, mistakes tend to be
which showed no gender differences in the Wechsler di- made successively and in the early stages of the test. Post-
git span subtest. We believe that cognitive performance hoc comparisons confirmed that older children have better
differen­ces between genders, regardless of age, are relati- performance on the spatial span task. This has also been re-
vely small and can only be demonstrated by testing large ported for other testing situations in previous studies.2,13,15,16
number of subjects. An extensive normative study was conducted by De-
Statistical comparisons of our results as a function of Luca and collaborators.17 These authors investigated the
years of schooling revealed no differences between forward development of executive function over lifespan, in sub-
and backward SSP. Nevertheless, we noted that scores for jects from 8 to 64 years old, and used several subtests of the
the backward SSP were consistently lower than for the CANTAB, including the Spatial Span test. Figure 2 depicts
forward SSP. a comparison between our results and those obtained by
We also report age differences in performance compa- the cited authors.
ring younger and older participants, showing that perfor- A comparison between our results and those obtained
mance improves with increasing age. by DeLuca and collaborators17 showed that the span length
The Rosen et al. (1997)13 study in undergraduate stu- scores obtained by 5 out of 6 age groups in our sample
dents showed that the forward and backward tasks reflect were within the range of norms described in that study. As
different levels of processing complexity or different types a limitation, our study sample included only one male and
of representations. In their study however, no difference two females in the 15 to 19 years age group. A larger sample
between forward and backward scores were found, im- will provide further support for comparison.
plying that both tasks required a similar level of processing Despite these limitations, the comparison between the
complexity. present data and results from the DeLuca and collabora-
Kessels and collaborators2 also found no differences be- tors17 study shows similarities in performance between the
tween the forward and backward condition on the Corsi Brazilian children and adolescents studied here and the
Block-Tapping Task (spatial span), but confirmed that the group of Australian participants examined by these au-
Digit Span (verbal span) backward was more difficult than thors, despite the socio-cultural and economic differences.
the forward condition. In conclusion, our results do not show statistical diffe-
The analysis of the results as a function of educational rences in performance on the Spatial Span Test due to gen-
level revealed differences in performance among partici- der but observed differences in performance among partici-
pants categorized in the first years of schooling and parti- pants in the first years of schooling compared to those in the
cipants in the final ranges of high school education, sho- final ranges of high school education, show that performan-
wing that performance improved with increased schooling, ce improves with greater schooling, and the concomitant ad-
and concomitant advancement in age. This expected result vancement in age. Our results also indicate a high applicabi-
had previously been demonstrated using the Corsi Block- lity of the Spatial Span of CANTAB for the Brazilian sample.
Tapping Task in children.10 The ease of administration of the CANTAB allows its utili-
Forward and backward span scores were positively cor- zation independent of subjects’ culture. The study showed
related with age, suggesting improvement in span perfor- that the Spatial Span CANTAB test is a useful tool for asses-
mance with development. Luciana and Nelson9 suggested sing pediatric samples in both research and clinical settings.
that memory capacity measured by the span task does not
reach functional maturity by the age of 12. According to Funding sources  –  This study was supported by grants
the authors, 18 years is the age at which adult levels of per- from the Foundation for Research Support of the State
formance are attained on the CANTAB Spatial Span task. of São Paulo (FAPESP) Projeto Temático 02/12733-8 (to

Teixeira RAA, et al.     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     133
Dement Neuropsychol 2011 June;5(2):129-134

DFV), Doctoral Fellowship (05/53823-8) (ECZ), and Un- function through use of the Cambridge Neuropsychological
dergraduate Fellowship (08/590001-8) (DTR). RAAT had a Testing Automated Battery: performance in 4 to 12-year-old
Doctoral Fellowship from the National Council for Scien- children. Dev Neuropsychol 2002;22:595-624.
tific and Technological Development (CNPq) and DFV 10. Lopes EJ, Lopes RFF, Galera CA. Memória de trabalho
holds a CNPq 1A Research Fellowship. viso-espacial em crianças de 7 a 12 anos. Est Psicol 2005;10:
207-214.
References 11. Postma A, Jager G, Kessels RPC, Koppeschaar HPF, Honk JV.
1. Blankenship AB. Memory span: a review of the literature. Sex differences for selective forms of spatial memory. Brain
Psychol Bull 1938;35:1-25. Cog 2004;54:24-32.
2. Kessels RPC, Van de Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AMA. The Back- 12. Lynn R, Irwing P. Sex differences in mental arithmetic, digit
ward Span of the Corsi Block-Tapping Task and Its Association span, and g defined as working memory capacity. Intelligence
With the WAIS-III Digit Span. Assessment 2008;15:426-434. 2008;36:226-235.
3. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of 13. Rosen VM, Engle RW. Forward and backward serial recall.
working memory. Neuropsychology 1994;8:485-493. Intelligence 1997;25:37-47.
4. Baddeley A. The episodic buffer: a new component of work- 14. Pagulayana KF, Buscha RM, Medina KL, Bartok JA, Kriko-
ing memory? Trends Cog Sciences 2000;4:417-423. rian R. Developmental normative data for the Corsi Block-
5. Robbins TW, James T, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, McInnes Tapping Task. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006;28:1043-1052.
L, Rabbitt PM. CANTAB: a factor analytic study of a large 15. Santos FH, Mello CB, Bueno OFA., Dellatolas G. Cross-cul-
sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dement Geriatr Cogn tural differences for three visual memory tasks in Brazilian
Disord 1994;5:266-281. children. Percep Motor Skills 2005;101:421-433.
6. Milner B. Interhemispheric differences in the localization of 16. Luciana MP. Practitioner review: computerized assessment of
psychological processes in man. Br Med Bull 1971;27:272-277. neuropsychological function in children: clinical and research
7. Vandierendonckl A, Kemps E, Fastame MC, Szmalec A. Work- applications of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Testing
ing memory components of the Corsi blocks task. Br J Psy- Automated Battery (CANTAB). J Child Psychol Psychiatry
chol 2004;95:57-79. 2003;44:649-663.
8. Cambridge Cognition. CANTABeclipse. Test Administration 17. De Luca CR, Wood SJ, Anderson V, Buchanan JA, Proffitt TM,
Guide / Manual version 3.0.0. Cambridge: Cambridge Cogni- Mahony K, Pantelis C. Normative data from the CANTAB. I:
tion Limited. 2006. development of executive function over the lifespan. J Clin
9. Luciana M, Nelson CA. Assessment of neuropsychological Exp Neuropsychol 2003;25:242-254.

134     Memory span: Brazilian children and adolescents     Teixeira RAA, et al.

You might also like