You are on page 1of 237

THE ROLE OF NARCISSISM IN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIP INITIATION

By

Robert A. Ackerman

A DISSERTATION

Submitted to
Michigan State University
in partial fulfillment of the requirements
for the degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Psychology

2011
ABSTRACT

THE ROLE OF NARCISSISM IN ROMANTIC RELATIONSHIP INITIATION

By

Robert A. Ackerman

Two studies were designed to clarify the roles of normal, grandiose, and vulnerable

narcissistic traits in the initiation of romantic relationships. Study 1 examined whether these

traits were differentially predictive of relationship initiation motives connected to self-

enhancement (i.e., motives that concern increasing positive sentiments towards the self), self-

protection (i.e., motives that concern decreasing negative sentiments towards the self), and

intimacy. The question of what types of partners (i.e., admiring, desirable, and caring) are

considered attractive by people with differing levels of these narcissistic dimensions was also

investigated. Normal narcissism was connected to the diminished adoption of self-enhancement

and self-protection relationship initiation motives. The grandiose narcissistic traits, in contrast,

were generally related to the increased adoption of self-enhancement motives and an increased

attraction towards desirable romantic partners. Last, the vulnerable narcissistic traits were

connected to the increased adoption of both self-enhancement and self-protection relationship

initiation motives. Study 2 examined the roles of these traits in actual dyadic interactions using a

paradigm similar to speed-dating. Normal narcissism was associated with the expression of

dominant and confident behaviors. The grandiose narcissistic traits were generally linked to the

expression of dominance and gregariousness; moreover, participants with greater levels of the

grandiose traits were considered more attractive and desirable by their dates. Findings for the

vulnerable narcissistic traits were not as straightforward as there were few connections between

these traits and outcomes in the interpersonal interactions. Taken together, this dissertation
demonstrates that the different narcissism dimensions play unique and important roles in the

romantic relationship initiation process.


TO MY FAMILY

iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First and foremost, I thank my advisors, Debby Kashy and Brent Donnellan. This

dissertation would not have been possible without them. I also thank my undergraduate research

assistants who assisted me in collecting these data—Jessie Trierweiler, Mollie Blanchard-Brown,

Alyssa Segal, Ashley Pratt, Jason Maise, Miles Armaly, Sarah Effner, Shannon Postel, and

Lauren Ruben. Finally, I thank my family.

v
TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES vii

LIST OF FIGURES xi

CHAPTER 1: GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1

CHAPTER 2: INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 1 21

CHAPTER 3: METHOD FOR STUDY 1 23

CHAPTER 4: RESULTS FOR STUDY 1 34

CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION FOR STUDY 1 54

CHAPTER 6: INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 2 62

CHAPTER 7: METHOD FOR STUDY 2 72

CHAPTER 8: RESULTS FOR STUDY 2 80

CHAPTER 9: DISCUSSION FOR STUDY 2 108

CHAPTER 10: GENERAL DISCUSSION 115

APPENDICIES 196

REFERENCES 221

vi
LIST OF TABLES

TABLE 1: Summary of Hypotheses for the Different Narcissism Dimensions


at Each Stage of Romantic Relationship Initiation 127

TABLE 2: Pattern Coefficients from an Exploratory Factor Analysis of the


Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory (RIM) 130

TABLE 3: Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables


in Study 1 133

TABLE 4: Zero-order correlations between RIM variables and


attachment-related avoidance in Study 1 134

TABLE 5: Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables and


dependent variables in Study 1 135

TABLE 6: Parameter Estimates from Path Analysis using the Narcissistic


Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits to Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations 136

TABLE 7: Parameter Estimates from an Unconstrained Multiple-Groups Analysis


for Men and Women using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory Subscales to
Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations 137

TABLE 8: Parameter Estimates from Path Analysis using the Pathological


Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits to Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations 138

TABLE 9: Parameter Estimates from an Unconstrained Multiple-Groups Analysis


for Men and Women using the Pathological Narcissism Inventory Scales to
Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations 139

TABLE 10: Means and Standard Deviations for Romantic Targets as a Function
of Target Presentation Order 140

TABLE 11: Zero-order correlations between primary dependent variables in


Study 1 141

TABLE 12: Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables and


dependent variables in Study 1 142

TABLE 13: Multiple Regression Analyses using the Narcissistic Personality


Inventory Subscales to Predict Attraction towards the Romantic Targets 143

TABLE 14: Multiple Regression Analyses using the Pathological Narcissism

vii
Inventory Scales to Predict Attraction towards the Romantic Targets 144

TABLE 15: Means and Standard Deviations of Attraction towards Romantic


Targets 145

TABLE 16: Predicting Attraction towards Admiring Target with Narcissistic


Personality Inventory Traits 146

TABLE 17: Predicting Attraction towards Desirable Target with Narcissistic


Personality Inventory Traits 148

TABLE 18: Simple Slopes for each Remote Associations Test (RAT)
Condition for the Relation between the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
Subscale of Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Attraction towards the Desirable
Target 150

TABLE 19: Predicting Attraction towards Caring Target with Narcissistic


Personality Inventory Traits 151

TABLE 20: Predicting Attraction towards Admiring Target with Pathological


Narcissism Inventory Traits 153

TABLE 21: Predicting Attraction towards Desirable Target with Pathological


Narcissism Inventory Traits 155

TABLE 22: Predicting Attraction towards Caring Target with Pathological


Narcissism Inventory Traits 157

TABLE 23: Simple Slopes for each Remote Associations Test (RAT)
Condition for the Relation between the Pathological Narcissism Inventory
Scale of Narcissistic Vulnerability and Attraction towards the Caring Target 159

TABLE 24: Illustration of Asymmetric Block Design 160

TABLE 25: Social Relations Model Variance Partitioning for Dating Record
Variables in Study 2 161

TABLE 26: Reciprocity Correlations for Dating Record Variables in Study 2 163

TABLE 27: Zero-order correlations between narcissism variables in Study 2 164

TABLE 28: Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the


Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects
for the Interpersonal Adjective Scale-Revised (IAS-R) Dating Record Variables 165

viii
TABLE 29: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Interpersonal
Behavior from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 166

TABLE 30: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Interpersonal


Behavior from the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI)
Traits 167

TABLE 31: Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the


Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects
for the Appeal Dating Record Variables 168

TABLE 32: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Admiration and


Desirability, and Dates’ Attraction to Participants, from the Participants’
Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 169

TABLE 33: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Admiration and


Desirability, and Dates’ Attraction to Participants, from the Participants’
Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 170

TABLE 34: Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ NPI traits
and their dates’ perceptions of them as Warm-Agreeable is Moderated by
participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability 171

TABLE 35: Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ NPI traits
and their dates’ perceptions of them as Admiring is Moderated by participants’
perception of their dates’ desirability 172

TABLE 36: Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ PNI traits
and their dates’ perceptions of them as Warm-Agreeable is Moderated by
participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability 173

TABLE 37: Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ PNI traits
and their dates’ perceptions of them as Admiring is Moderated by participants’
perception of their dates’ desirability 174

TABLE 38: Zero-order correlations between dates’ perceptions of the


participants’ gregarious-extraversion, assured-dominance, arrogance-calculation,
desirability, and attractiveness 175

TABLE 39: Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the


Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Perceiver
effects for the Rapport Dating Record Variables 176

TABLE 40: Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the

ix
Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects
for the Rapport Dating Record Variables 177

TABLE 41: Predicting Participants’ Disclosure, Trust, and Connection to their


Dates in the Social Interactions from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality
Inventory (NPI) Traits 178

TABLE 42: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants as Trusting and


Responsive in the Social Interactions from the Participants’ Narcissistic
Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 179

TABLE 43: Predicting Participants’ Disclosure, Trust, and Connection to


their Dates in the Social Interactions from the Participants’ Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 180

TABLE 44: Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants as Trusting and


Responsive in the Social Interactions from the Participants’ Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 181

TABLE 45: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Admiring Dates


with the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 182

TABLE 46: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Admiring Dates


with the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 183

TABLE 47: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Desirable Dates


with the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 184

TABLE 48: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Desirable Dates


with the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 185

TABLE 49: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Caring Dates


with the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits 186

TABLE 50: Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Caring Dates


with the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits 187

x
LIST OF FIGURES

FIGURE 1: Higher order structure of Pathological Narcissism Inventory 188

FIGURE 2: Scree plot of eigenvalues from an exploratory factor analysis of


items from the relationship initiation motives inventory 189

FIGURE 3: Path analysis model for predicting the Relationship Initiation


Motives Inventory subscales from the Narcissistic Personality Inventory
subscales 190

FIGURE 4: Path analysis model for predicting the Relationship Initiation


Motives Inventory subscales from the Pathological Narcissism Inventory
subscales 191

FIGURE 5: The Interpersonal Circumplex 192

FIGURE 6: Diagram of a single-variable mediational model 193

FIGURE 7: Mediational Model for Grandiose Narcissism-Attraction Link 194

xi
CHAPTER 1: GENERAL INTRODUCTION

Narcissism corresponds to an investment in the self and is characterized by several

intrapersonal and interpersonal strategies aimed at self-esteem regulation (P. F. Kernberg, 1998).

Although usually discussed in contexts pertaining to pathology, theorists have noted that such

self-investment can occur for healthy individuals and that it can be connected to self-esteem

regulation strategies that are normal and adaptive (e.g., O. F. Kernberg, 1975; Stone, 1998).

Given that the social realm is one of the primary venues through which the self is regulated,

examination of the role of narcissism within close relationships represents an especially

important opportunity to understand the construct. It also offers an excellent opportunity to better

understand how basic relationship processes (e.g., relationship initiation, interdependence) are

impacted by individual differences involving an excessive self-focus.

Unfortunately, empirical work on the role of narcissism in romantic relationships has


1
been scarce. The research that has been conducted suggests that narcissists adopt a game-

playing approach to love (Campbell, Foster, & Finkel, 2002), are less committed in their

relationships (Campbell & Foster, 2002), and report an increased susceptibility to infidelity

(Buss & Shackelford, 1997). Although this research has helped to illuminate how narcissism

manifests itself in ongoing close relationships, we still know very little about how narcissists

initiate such relationships in the first place (for a notable exception, see Campbell, 1999). Such

research is important because before we can fully understand the processes that occur in ongoing

relationships, we must first gain insight into the motivations and behaviors that initially gave rise

to them. Moreover, the field’s overreliance on a single measure to assess the construct (i.e., the

Narcissistic Personality Inventory) has led to disparate research findings (e.g., narcissism being

1
When using the term “narcissist,” I am referring to someone with elevated scores on particular
continuous dimensions of personality rather than implying that narcissism is typological.
1
linked to both self-esteem and aggressive behavior) and the formulation of theoretical models

(e.g., the Agency Model of Narcissism; see Campbell, Brunell, & Finkel, 2006) that conflate

adaptive and maladaptive features of personality.

Accordingly, the present research will make use of the distinction between normal and

pathological narcissism (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). It will also distinguish between the

pathological narcissistic expressions of grandiosity and vulnerability. As will be argued, each of

these traits should be associated with somewhat different strategies to regulate self-esteem.

Moreover, such strategies should be related to various aspects of the relationship initiation

process, such as the motivations that individuals possess for pursuing relationships, what

individuals find attractive, how the self is presented in initial encounters, and what individuals do

to ensure the possibility of a second encounter. The goal of this dissertation is therefore to

uncover the different strategies associated with these traits and their resulting consequences for

the initiation stages of a romantic relationship.

The Conceptualization and Measurement of Narcissism

The construct of narcissism has a long history in psychology and has acquired myriad

meanings over the years (see, e.g., Ellis, 1898; Freud, 1914). Nevertheless, most contemporary

conceptualizations of narcissism are indebted to the theoretical work of O. F. Kernberg, Kohut,

and Millon. According to these theorists, narcissists possess abnormal self-structures and exhibit

pathological self-esteem regulation strategies (O. F. Kernberg, 1998; Millon, 1996). In line with

this conceptualization, it has been posited that narcissists are grandiose and self-absorbed (O. F.

Kernberg, 1975; Kohut, 1966; Millon, 1996), exhibitionistic (O. F. Kernberg, 1975; Kohut,

1966; Millon, 1996), entitled (O. F. Kernberg, 1975; Millon, 1996), exploitative (O. F. Kernberg,

1975; Millon, 1996), incapable of experiencing empathy (O. F. Kernberg, 1975; Millon, 1996),

2
and that they fluctuate between feelings of superiority and inferiority (O. F. Kernberg, 1975;

Kohut, 1966). Though it has been historically conceptualized as a disorder and discrete category,

many contemporary researchers in both social/personality (e.g., Raskin & Terry, 1988) and

clinical psychology (e.g., Pincus, Ansell, Pimentel, Cain, Wright, & Levy, 2009) now

conceptualize narcissism as a continuous trait.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) of psychiatric disorders first included

Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD) as a diagnosable entity in its third edition. Criteria for

the DSM-III primarily reflected the insights of O. F. Kernberg, Kohut, and Millon (Levy,

Reynoso, Wasserman, & Clarkin, 2009). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin &

Hall, 1979)—the most frequently used measure of narcissism within social and personality

psychology (see Cain, Pincus, & Ansell, 2008)—was based on the anticipated criteria for NPD in

the DSM-III. As such, it placed a heavy emphasis on those features of narcissism that involved

grandiosity, exhibitionism, entitlement, and exploitativeness.

Although the content for the NPI was purportedly derived from these anticipated DSM-

III criteria, conceptualizations of narcissism in social/personality psychology are potentially

different from those found in the neighboring disciplines of psychiatry and clinical psychology

given that social/personality psychologists have generally emphasized features that reflect more

resilience and extraversion (Miller & Campbell, 2008). It is therefore notable that Pincus and his

colleagues (e.g., Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010) have suggested that a distinction be made between

normal narcissism and pathological narcissism.

Pincus and Lukowitsky (2010) assert that whereas normal narcissism involves self-

regulatory strategies that are generally adaptive and stem from a healthy personality

organization, pathological narcissism is characterized by maladaptive self-regulatory strategies

3
that generally produce distress and impairment (Pincus & Lukowitksy, 2010). Accordingly, they

proposed that normal narcissism is manifested in behaviors such as asserting dominance, holding

positive self-illusions, and engaging in achievement striving (Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). In

contrast, pathological narcissism is manifested in vulnerable self-states and characteristics that

more closely reflect the criteria identified in the DSM for NPD.

After an exhaustive review of the literature, Cain et al. (2008) further identified two

primary ways in which pathological narcissism is expressed: Grandiosity and Vulnerability.

Narcissistic Grandiosity is characterized by several intrapersonal (e.g., engagement in grandiose

fantasies, feelings of superiority, entitled beliefs) and interpersonal (e.g., exploitive and

exhibitionistic behavior) strategies aimed at enhancing the self. Pincus et al. (2009) developed

several subscales to capture this expression of narcissistic dysfunction (see Figure 1). Among

these were Grandiose Fantasy (i.e., a tendency to fantasize about unlimited admiration and

success), Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement (i.e., a tendency to engage in ostensibly selfless acts

to reinforce the grandiose self), and Exploitativeness (i.e., a tendency to engage in interpersonal

manipulation). Pincus et al. (2009) found that the Exploitativeness facet was related to more

domineering interpersonal problems whereas the Grandiose Fantasy and Self-Sacrificing Self-

Enhancement facets were associated with more intrusive and overly-nurturant interpersonal

problems, respectively.

Narcissistic Vulnerability reflects the possession of a fragile self-concept and the frequent

experience of emotional dysregulation. In their research, Pincus et al. (2009) developed several

subscales to assess the features of Narcissistic Vulnerability (see Figure 1). These included

Contingent Self-Esteem (i.e., the tendency to experience self-esteem as substantially labile and

dependent upon external sources), Hiding the Self (i.e., a reluctance to portray the self as

4
imperfect and incompletely self-sufficient), Devaluing (i.e., the inclination to devalue those

individuals who do not reinforce the grandiose self as well as the inclination to experience

disappointment/shame for needing this reinforcement), and Entitlement-Rage (i.e., a tendency to

experience anger over unmet expectations of special favors or treatment).

In contrast to the scales assessing Narcissistic Grandiosity, the Narcissistic Vulnerability

subscales were consistently associated with lower self-esteem scores (Pincus et al., 2009).

Moreover, they displayed the strongest positive connections with shame, primitive defenses,

impaired reality testing, and low moral values (Pincus et al., 2009). The vulnerability subscales

of Devaluing and Entitlement-Rage were most strongly positively related to aggression (Pincus

et al., 2009). When examining their relations to interpersonal problems, Pincus et al. (2009)

found that whereas the Devaluing and Entitlement-Rage subscales were connected to vindictive

problems, the Hiding the Self and Contingent Self-Esteem subscales were related to avoidant and

exploitable problems, respectively.

According to Pincus and Lukowitsky (2010), social/personality psychologists have

primarily studied normal narcissism. This focus is most likely due to social/personality

psychologists’ focus on the experiences of nonclinical populations (especially college student

samples) as well as their almost exclusive reliance on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory

(NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1979) to operationalize narcissism in their research—a measure that Pincus

and Lukowitsky (2010) believe assesses normal narcissism. Nevertheless, there is still some

content on the inventory that assesses some of the maladaptive features traditionally associated

with narcissism (e.g., Ackerman, Witt, Donnellan, Trzesniewski, Robins, & Kashy, 2011).

Indeed, researchers have argued that the NPI seems to assess both adaptive and maladaptive

features (e.g., Barry, Frick, & Killian, 2003; Cain et al., 2008; Watson & Morris, 1991), and

5
recent analyses exploring the dimensional structure of the NPI have corroborated this notion

(Ackerman et al., 2011; Corry, Merritt, Mrug, & Pamp, 2008).

Most recently, Ackerman et al. (2011) found that the NPI consists of three related but

distinct dimensions: Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and

Entitlement/Exploitativeness. Leadership/Authority captures content related to assertiveness,

dominance, and self-perceived leadership ability. It is therefore most similar in form to Pincus

and Lukowitsky’s (2010) conceptualization of normal narcissism (Ackerman et al., 2011).

Individuals with higher levels of this construct report higher levels of extraversion, self-esteem,

Behavioral Activation System (BAS) activation, and self-control (Ackerman et al., 2011; Corry

et al., 2008; Emmons, 1984; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995; Watson & Biderman, 1993). These

individuals also report higher levels of self-monitoring, surgency, warmth, and dominance

(Emmons, 1984), as well as an increased tendency to attribute the cause of positive events to an

internal, global, and stable source (Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995). Combined with the fact that higher

levels of this construct are associated with lower levels of neuroticism, depression, anxiety,

Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) activation, and contingent self-esteem (Ackerman et al.,

2011; Emmons, 1984; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995; Watson & Biderman, 1993), the

Leadership/Authority trait is generally considered a rather adaptive feature of personality.

Grandiose Exhibitionism, on the other hand, represents a somewhat more maladaptive

feature of the NPI than Leadership/Authority. Conceptually speaking, this trait appears to capture

the expression of narcissistic grandiosity. The Grandiose Exhibitionism subscale is characterized

by a combination of thoughts and behaviors related to vanity, exhibitionism, and beliefs of

superiority. People with higher levels of this trait report greater levels of extraversion and BAS

6
activity (Ackerman et al., 2011). Moreover, they express anger over not getting their way and are

somewhat oblivious to the feelings and needs of others (Ackerman et al., 2011).

Entitlement/Exploitativeness represents the most maladaptive feature of the NPI among

the three dimensions highlighted by Ackerman et al. (2011). Entitlement/Exploitativeness refers

to thoughts and behaviors that reflect beliefs of special privilege and a willingness to use others

for personal gain. In terms of Pincus and Lukowitsky’s (2010) distinction between grandiosity

and vulnerability, Entitlement/Exploitativeness appears to better represent an expression of

narcissistic vulnerability. However, unlike the PNI subscales used to assess narcissistic

vulnerability, this NPI subscale is narrower in scope and seems to reflect a set of strategies used

to cope with the experience of vulnerable self- and affect-states. Of all the facets of personality

embedded within the NPI, Entitlement/Exploitativeness has been shown to have the most

pernicious outcomes. Research has shown that this facet is related to higher levels of

interpersonal manipulation, antisocial tendencies, devaluation of others, neuroticism,

disagreeableness, entitlement-rage, and contingent self-esteem (Ackerman et al., 2011).

Entitlement/Exploitativeness has also been linked to lower levels of relationship satisfaction and

college adjustment among roommates (Ackerman et al., 2011).

Despite their apparent differences, each of the aforementioned narcissistic dimensions is

fundamentally concerned with self-esteem regulation. What appear to differentiate these

narcissistic dimensions are the particular strategies that are used to regulate self-esteem. Hepper,

Gramzow, and Sedikides (2010) recently found that individuals with greater levels of trait

narcissism (operationalized using a short-form of the NPI) are likely to use both self-

enhancement (i.e., positivity embracement, favorable construals, and self-affirming reflections)

and self-protection (i.e., defensiveness) strategies. Given the divergent patterns of empirical

7
associations found for the narcissistic expressions of grandiosity and vulnerability, it is probable

that each particular expression of narcissistic dysfunction may be connected to different self-

esteem regulation strategies.

Wright, Lukowitsky, Pincus, and Conroy (2010) recently remarked that PNI Narcissistic

Grandiosity appears to reflect maladaptive forms of self-enhancement. Similarly, Tritt, Ryder,

Ring, and Pincus (2010) commented that PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity is characterized by

approach motivation. It is plausible that the strategies used by individuals with greater levels of

NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism also primarily reflect self-enhancement. Indeed, each of the

intrapersonal (e.g., grandiose self-views and preoccupation with physical appearance) and

interpersonal (e.g., attention-seeking behaviors) strategies associated with this trait appear to be

in the service of affirming a grandiose self. Moreover, Grandiose Exhibitionism has been found

to be connected with increased BAS activity (Ackerman et al., 2011).

Unlike these NPI and PNI operationalizations of narcissistic grandiosity,

Entitlement/Exploitativeness appears to reflect a collection of strategies that have as their goal

self-protection. Indeed, individuals with higher levels of this narcissistic trait have been found to

possess self-esteem that is somewhat depleted and more labile and contingent upon external

sources (Ackerman et al., 2011). It therefore appears that both entitled beliefs and exploitive

behavior seem to be enacted in the service of trying to protect an inferior self (for similar

arguments, see Bishop & Lane, 2002; Zitek, Jordan, Monin, & Leach, 2010). PNI Vulnerability

has also been argued to be characterized by avoidance motivation (Tritt et al., 2010). Combined

with the emphasis placed on a fragile self-concept and the inclusion of content that appears to

serve the goal of self-protection (e.g., Hiding the Self), I expect PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability to

function in a similar manner as Entitlement/Exploitativeness.

8
To sum up, past work has suggested a distinction between normal and pathological

narcissism as well as a distinction between the pathological narcissistic traits of grandiosity and

vulnerability. Moreover, the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and the Pathological Narcissism

Inventory appear to assess related but distinct expressions of some of these traits. The present

research uses the Narcissistic Personality Inventory to assess normal narcissism (i.e.,

Leadership/Authority) and relatively normative expressions of narcissistic grandiosity (i.e.,

Grandiose Exhibitionism) and vulnerability (i.e., Entitlement/Exploitativeness). It also uses the

recently developed Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al., 2009) to assess more

clinically relevant expressions of narcissistic grandiosity and vulnerability (i.e., Narcissistic

Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability, respectively). To be clear, these five dimensions are

not meant to exhaustively cover the definition of narcissism. Nonetheless, I believe that they

capture broad phenotypic expressions of the construct that can provide a good foundation for

better understanding the relation between narcissism and relationships. Thus, I will focus on

these five dimensions when theorizing about connections between narcissism and romantic

relationship initiation.

The Role of Narcissism in Romantic Relationship Initiation

Bredow, Cate, and Huston (2008) propose a conceptual model of romantic relationship

initiation that involves four stages. In the first stage, Appraisal of Initial Attraction, individuals

discern whether they are attracted to a potential partner. Bredow et al. (2008) emphasize that

individuals’ motives for pursuing the relationship are particularly important in this stage. If there

is attraction in the first stage, individuals then decide whether to make an overture (i.e., the

second stage). Such a decision depends upon how confident the individual is that the potential

partner will accept her or his overture. Bredow et al. (2008) state that such confidence hinges

9
upon two factors: (1) whether the potential partner is perceived to be open to the individual’s

motives, and (2) whether the potential partner is perceived to be attracted to the individual. If

only one of these factors is true, then the individual will have moderate confidence; however, if

both of them are true, the individual is argued to have high confidence.

The third stage in Bredow et al.’s (2008) model is Strategic Self-Presentation. Whereas

moderate confidence from the second stage leads to strategic self-presentation (i.e., selective

presentation of particular aspects of the self), high confidence leads to self-expression (i.e.,

presentation of the true self). The final stage in Bredow et al.’s (2008) model is Build-Up of

Rapport. Bredow et al. (2008) suggest that a build-up of rapport will lead to a successful

encounter whereas the lack of such a build-up will lead to an encounter that is terminated. The

present research examines the role of narcissism within three of these stages in detail.

Specifically, it investigates the roles of normal and pathological narcissistic traits in the

Appraisal of Initial Attraction, Strategic Self-Presentation, and the Build-Up of Rapport.

The Appraisal of Initial Attraction

When two people meet for the first time, they each carry with them particular motivations

(e.g., to fall in love). These motivations in turn have an impact on what characteristics they will

find attractive in a potential partner (Bredow et al., 2008). To the extent that there is a match

between the individual’s motives for pursuing a relationship and the potential partner’s

characteristics, then there will be romantic attraction (Bredow et al., 2008). Therefore, a

satisfactory understanding of the role of narcissism in romantic relationship initiation will first

require an exploration of the reasons that narcissists have for entering such relationships.

Campbell et al. (2006) proposed that narcissists possess agentic goals (e.g., the goal to

gain status and self-esteem) that drive them to pursue romantic relationships. According to

10
Campbell et al. (2006), potential romantic partners that are perceived to be ideal and provide

admiration will be desired by narcissists because they raise self-esteem. Moreover, potential

romantic partners that are of high status (e.g., through being very physically attractive or well-

known) will be desired because they help to enhance status. Such proposals, however, are based

on a conceptualization of narcissism that predominantly reflects self-enhancing rather than

defensive tendencies. This conceptualization seems to derive from other research showing that

the NPI is positively correlated with extraversion and approach-related motivations (e.g.,

Emmons, 1984; Foster & Trimm IV, 2008; Sedikides & Luke, 2008).

Although previous research corroborates the notion that narcissists’ goals tend to be self-

focused and individualistic (Emmons, 1984; Watson & Biderman, 1993), it does not appear that

all narcissists are excessively extraverted or fully driven by appetitive motivations (see

Ackerman et al., 2011). Indeed, the entitled and exploitative features of narcissism tend to be

either unrelated, or even negatively related, to constructs such as Extraversion and Behavioral

Activation System (BAS) activity (see Ackerman et al., 2011). These findings lend support to the

idea that the grandiose and vulnerable expressions of narcissism are differentially related to

processes related to self-enhancement and self-protection. Such differences should likely play a

role in the adoption of distinct relationship motives.

Critical to this point, Cooper, Shapiro, and Powers (1998) surmised that two predominant

distinctions seem to underlie most theories of motivation: approach/avoidance motivation (e.g.,

Carver & White, 1994; Gray, 1987) and internal/external focus (e.g., Bakan, 1966; Bowlby,

1988). By applying these two pairs of dimensions to the realm of sexual behavior, Cooper et al.

(2008) derived four general motivational categories that include Enhancement (i.e., self-focused

motivations that entail enhancing pleasure), Intimacy (i.e., other-focused motivations that entail

11
increasing intimacy), Coping (i.e., self-focused motivations that concern the regulation of

negative affect and self-esteem threats), and Approval (i.e., other-directed motives that involve

the avoidance of disapproval from others). Although used by Cooper et al. (1998) to explain the

motives behind sexual behavior, this model may be especially useful to better understand the

motivations behind narcissists’ pursuit of romantic relationships.

Thus, I expect that those narcissistic traits reflective of self-enhancement (i.e., Grandiose

Exhibitionism and Narcissistic Grandiosity) will be positively associated with Enhancement

motives for pursuing romantic relationships. In contrast, I expect those narcissistic traits

reflective of self-protection (i.e., Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Narcissistic Vulnerability) to

be positively associated with Coping motives for pursuing romantic relationships. Finally, given

the self-focused nature of narcissism, I expect that all five of the narcissistic traits measured in

this research will be negatively related to Intimacy relationship motives.

These narcissistic traits should also have an impact on what partner characteristics

individuals find attractive. Campbell (1999) initially proposed the Self-Orientation Model to

account for the role of narcissism in romantic attraction. The model stipulates that narcissists’

level of attraction to a partner depends upon the partner’s perceived ability to provide the

narcissist with self-enhancement. Whereas some partners (i.e., individuals who are admiring

and/or possess highly desirable characteristics) possess characteristics that represent

opportunities for self-enhancement and are perceived as attractive, other partners (i.e.,

individuals who are interested in emotional intimacy or possess expectations of caring) do not

represent opportunities for self-enhancement and are therefore perceived as less attractive.

Campbell (1999) proposed admiration (i.e., the potential romantic partner’s provision of

praise and other positive feedback about the self) as a possible self-enhancement strategy for

12
narcissists during romantic attraction. Given that individuals with higher levels of grandiose

narcissistic traits are expected to adopt enhancement relationship motives and to seek out such

admiration in an approach-oriented manner, I hypothesize that such individuals will be especially

attracted to targets who readily provide such attributes (i.e., targets who are perceived to be

admiring and interested). Further evidence for this hypothesis comes from Campbell’s (1999)

second study in which he found that the strongest correlations between the NPI and romantic

attraction to the admiring target occurred for the Superiority and Vanity subscales. Due to their

preoccupation with obtaining positive feedback about the self and enhancing their self-image,

however, I hypothesize that individuals with higher levels of grandiose traits will also

demonstrate an aversion to caring targets.

Campbell (1999) further proposed identification as another possible self-enhancement

strategy for narcissists during romantic attraction. Identification refers to the association of the

self with an ideal and high status other (i.e., a perfect other). Interestingly, O. F. Kernberg (1975)

discussed how narcissists sometimes seek an ideal other for the purposes of protection. It is

possible that individuals possessing higher levels of narcissistic vulnerability will be more

attracted to those individuals who display highly desirable characteristics. Indeed, such

individuals may be perceived as sources of protection for an acknowledged vulnerable self. In

support of this conjecture, Campbell (1999) found that the NPI Entitlement scale was strongly

correlated with ratings of attraction towards a perfect and uncaring target. I therefore predict that

individuals with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits (i.e., Entitlement/Exploitativeness

and Narcissistic Vulnerability) will be more romantically attracted to perfect targets (called

“desirable” targets in the present research) and less romantically attracted to caring targets.

13
Taken together, several hypotheses concerning the role of narcissistic traits in initial

attraction can be proposed. First, I predict that all five narcissistic dimensions (i.e.,

Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, Entitlement/Exploitativeness, Narcissistic

Grandiosity, and Narcissistic Vulnerability) will be associated with reduced intimacy

motivations for entering romantic relationships and a decreased attraction towards potential

romantic targets who are caring. Second, given the consistent associations found between

Leadership/Authority and positive psychological states (Ackerman et al., 2011), I predict that

persons with higher levels of this trait will report lower levels of coping motivations. Third, I

predict that individuals with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism and Narcissistic

Grandiosity will possess greater enhancement motivations for entering relationships and that

they will report increased attraction towards potential romantic targets who admire them. Last, I

predict that individuals with higher levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Narcissistic

Vulnerability will possess greater levels of coping motivations and that they will report increased

attraction towards potential romantic targets who are desirable.

Strategic Self-Presentation

When individuals are not fully confident that the other person will be receptive to their

advances or is attracted to them, Bredow et al. (2008) propose that individuals engage in

strategic self-presentation in an effort to make the self more attractive to others. According to

Bredow et al. (2008), individuals can use three strategies to pique the interest of the other person.

These include appearing likable, competent/capable, or morally virtuous. In order to appear

likeable, people may ingratiate the other person and/or draw attention to their own positive

qualities. To appear competent/capable, people may emphasize those personal qualities that

14
convey talent, resources, and accomplishment. Last, to appear morally virtuous, people may

draw the other person’s attention to those personal qualities that convey honesty and integrity.

Previous research examining the impressions that narcissists make upon unacquainted

individuals has revealed some interesting findings. Oltmanns, Friedman, Fiedler, and Turkheimer

(2004), for instance, examined what traits can be gleaned from 30-second thin slices of behavior

based on videos of military recruits. The military recruits that were nominated by their peers to

be higher in narcissism were rated by unacquainted individuals to be higher in extraversion, to be

more physically attractive, and to be worthy of getting to know better (see also Friedman,

Oltmanns, Gleason, & Turkheimer, 2006). Friedman, Oltmanns, and Turkheimer (2007) further

found that individuals nominated by their peers to be narcissistic (as well as those with higher

scores on the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality narcissistic subscale) were

perceived by unacquainted coders to take advantage of others, believe they are superior, and

prefer being the center of attention. For the most part, the narcissistic traits connected with

grandiosity therefore appear to be most salient upon initial acquaintance.

Other research has more directly examined how different facets of narcissism are related

to initial personality impressions (e.g., Back, Schmukle, & Egloff, 2010). Back et al. (2010)

showed that individuals’ NPI total scores were positively linked to peers’ ratings of their

popularity in a group setting. Moreover, this connection was primarily driven by the

Exploitativeness/Entitlement dimension of the NPI identified by Emmons (1984, 1987). Using a

lens model approach (see Brunswick, 1947), Back et al. (2010) demonstrated that individuals

with higher levels of Exploitativeness/Entitlement exhibited more humorous verbal expressions,

were flashier and neater in their style of dress, conveyed nonverbal behavior that was more self-

15
assured, and gave more charming facial expressions. These researchers were able to further show

that these behavioral cues were what led to higher popularity ratings.

However, Back et al. (2010) used the Emmons (1984, 1987) dimensional solution for the

NPI, and so their conclusions may need to be qualified because this specification of the

Exploitative/Entitlement dimension actually contains some items representing grandiosity and

exhibitionism (e.g., “I am more capable than other people” and “I get upset when people don't

notice how I look when I go out in public”). Therefore, what may actually be leading to stylish

dress and likeable expressions may be the attention-seeking tendencies associated with

exhibitionism rather than entitled beliefs and exploitative tendencies per se. This would be

consistent with the correlations found between the NPI facet of Self-Admiration/Self-Absorption

and those behavioral cues involving flashy and neat dress, charming facial expression, and self-

assured body movements (Back et al., 2010).

In light of the above findings, I hypothesize that the expression of narcissistic grandiosity

(i.e., Grandiose Exhibitionism and Narcissistic Grandiosity) will be associated with self-

presentational strategies that involve competence/capability. Given their need for admiration,

people with greater levels of grandiosity will be likely to boast about their accomplishments and

draw attention to their highly desirable attributes. As such, they will likely take great pains to

appear physically attractive and to convey a sense of self-assurance throughout the interaction.

Moreover, given their exhibitionistic nature, such people will likely come across as gregarious.

Vulnerable individuals, on the other hand, are likely to use a different strategy. As

discussed previously, vulnerable individuals are expected to be motivated to pursue romantic

relationships to cope with negative emotions and to bolster a relatively inadequate self-image. As

such, they are expected to be particularly attracted to those individuals who possess desirable

16
qualities. Rather than utilizing a strategy that involves presenting the self as competent and/or

capable, it is more likely that these individuals will use ingratiation as a strategy to win over the

“perfect” potential partner. Combined with their relatively introverted nature, I therefore predict

that individuals with higher levels of narcissistic vulnerability will engage in warm, admiring,

and submissive behaviors towards those whom they perceive to be desirable.

In summary, I first predict that individuals with higher levels of grandiose traits (i.e.,

Grandiose Exhibitionism and Narcissistic Grandiosity) will use self-presentational strategies that

involve the demonstration of competence and/or capability. As a result of this, I expect such

individuals to be perceived as self-assured, extraverted, charming, and good looking during an

initial encounter. Moreover, given the nature of the strategy being used, I also expect individuals

with greater levels of grandiose traits to be perceived as arrogant. Second, I predict that

individuals with higher levels of vulnerable traits (i.e., Entitlement/Exploitativeness and

Narcissistic Vulnerability) will use self-presentational strategies that involve ingratiation when

they are interacting with someone they perceive to be desirable. Consequently, I expect

individuals with higher levels of these traits to be perceived as friendly, submissive, and

admiring by those individuals they consider to be desirable.

Building Rapport and Setting the Stage for a Second Encounter

The establishment of rapport is generally considered important for the initial

development of a romantic relationship (Bredow et al., 2008). Therefore, it is advantageous for

individuals to be able to initiate a conversation with the other person and work to foster mutual

feelings of affinity. As was discussed previously, strategic self-presentation is one method that

can be used to generate feelings of attraction. Bredow et al. (2008) also assert that affinity can be

17
effectively engendered within an interaction by showing interest, being attentive, finding

common areas of interest, and engaging in self-disclosure.

Self-presentational strategies that involve charm, confidence, and expressiveness have

been found to result in higher ratings of likeability (Back et al., 2010). Likewise, being well-

groomed and physically attractive also can lead to higher ratings of interpersonal attractiveness

(Back et al., 2010; Vazire, Naumann, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008). Based on these findings, it is

likely that individuals with higher levels of grandiose traits (i.e., Grandiose Exhibitionism and

Narcissistic Grandiosity) will elicit higher ratings of romantic attraction from their interaction

partners and that this relation will be mediated by their interaction partners’ perceptions of them

as being more gregarious and desirable (i.e., charming, popular, and good looking).

In the previous section, I predicted that individuals with higher levels of narcissistic

vulnerability will engage in warm, admiring, and submissive behaviors towards those whom they

perceive to be desirable. Previous research has found ingratiation to be a rather effective strategy

to foster attraction so long as it is not too extreme (Vonk, 2002). That being said, I expect that

individuals’ levels of narcissistic vulnerability will be related to their interaction partners’ ratings

of romantic attraction. Importantly, however, this will only be the case when vulnerable

individuals perceive their partners to be desirable (i.e., it is not expected that they will ingratiate

other interaction partners that they do not perceive to be desirable). Therefore, I predict that the

relation between individuals’ levels of narcissistic vulnerability and their interaction partners’

ratings of romantic attraction will be moderated by the individuals’ perceptions of their

interaction partners being desirable. Moreover, in the cases where the individual does perceive

the partner to be desirable, the relation between the individuals’ degree of vulnerable narcissism

18
and their partners’ ratings of romantic attraction will be mediated by the interaction partners’

perceptions of the individual as being submissive, warm, and admiring.

As mentioned previously, expressing interest in the other person and engaging in self-

disclosure also work to foster attraction. Indeed, self-disclosure is connected to enhanced

perceptions of intimacy (Laurenceau, Barrett, & Pietromonaco, 1998) and the establishment of

such a connection is likely to lead to increased attraction. However, past research suggests that

individuals with narcissistic traits are unlikely to desire intimacy (Emmons, 1989). Indeed,

Campbell (1999) argued that intimacy represents a detriment to self-enhancement as any flaws

associated with the narcissist will be exposed. Thus, I predict that the grandiose and vulnerable

narcissistic traits will be linked to lower rates of self-disclosure. Moreover, given their excessive

self-preoccupation, individuals with higher levels of the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic

traits will also be perceived by their partners to be less responsive. Consequently, although the

self-presentational strategies associated with the narcissistic traits may help to foster initial

attraction, it is unlikely that such encounters will be characterized by feelings of closeness.

In summary, several predictions can be made concerning the role of narcissistic traits in

the establishment of rapport during initial encounters. First, individuals with higher levels of

grandiose traits (i.e., Grandiose Exhibitionism and Narcissistic Grandiosity) will be rated as

more attractive because they are perceived to be more gregarious and desirable. Second, when

individuals with higher levels of vulnerable traits (i.e., Entitlement/Exploitativeness and

Narcissistic Vulnerability) perceive potential partners to be desirable, these partners will rate

them as more attractive because they perceive the individuals with vulnerable traits to be

submissive, warm, and admiring. Third, the measures of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism

will be negatively related to self-reported rates of disclosure during initial encounters. Fourth,

19
individuals with greater levels of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism will be perceived by their

partners to be less responsive. Fifth, the measures of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism will be

negatively related to feelings of closeness.

Present Research

The present research was designed to clarify the roles played by normal narcissism,

narcissistic grandiosity, and narcissistic vulnerability in the romantic relationship initiation

process. Study 1 examines whether these traits are differentially associated with separate motives

for entering relationships. It also evaluates whether individuals’ attraction towards potential

romantic partners with particular characteristics represents an actual means for grandiose and

vulnerable individuals to regulate self-esteem. Study 2 uses a paradigm similar to speed-dating to

further examine the role of these narcissistic traits in the attraction process in real-life social

settings, as well as to investigate whether the grandiose and vulnerable expressions of narcissism

are differentially related to strategies involved in self-presentation and the building of rapport.

Table 1 presents a summary of the hypotheses for each of the narcissistic attributes at the

different stages of romantic relationship initiation.

20
CHAPTER 2: INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 1

Study 1 aimed to illuminate the roles of normal, grandiose, and vulnerable narcissistic

traits in the appraisal of initial attraction. As was discussed previously, it is plausible that each of

the narcissistic traits may be associated with different motives for relationship initiation. Those

narcissistic traits indicative of inflated self-views and approach-related behavioral strategies (i.e.,

Grandiose Exhibitionism and Narcissistic Grandiosity), for instance, will likely be associated

with relationship motives that reflect self-enhancement. In contrast, those narcissistic traits

reflective of depleted self-views and self-protective behavioral strategies (i.e.,

Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Narcissistic Vulnerability) will likely be connected to

relationship motives that represent an escape from or amelioration of negative self-states.

In addition to demonstrating that each of these narcissistic traits is associated with

somewhat different motives for initiating a romantic relationship, Study 1 investigates whether

each of these traits is also associated with attraction towards different types of romantic targets. I

hypothesize that those traits indicative of grandiosity will be positively associated with attraction

towards romantic targets who provide admiration. In contrast, those traits indicative of

vulnerability should be positively associated with attraction towards romantic targets who are

considered desirable. Last, I predict that each of the measured narcissist traits will be negatively

associated with attraction towards romantic targets who are considered caring.

Although previous research has assumed that narcissists use their romantic relationships

as a means to regulate self-esteem (see Campbell, 1999), this assumption has never been

empirically tested. The present research therefore experimentally manipulates participants’ self-

esteem levels to evaluate whether the relations between narcissistic traits and attraction towards

particular romantic targets become stronger. The presence of an interaction between the self-

21
esteem manipulation and the relations of these narcissistic traits to attraction towards romantic

targets would provide good evidence that such partner choices actually represent a self-esteem

regulation strategy.

22
CHAPTER 3: METHOD FOR STUDY 1

Study 1 involved two phases. In the first phase, participants were asked to complete the

Narcissistic Personality Inventory and the Pathological Narcissism Inventory online prior to a

visit to the laboratory. In the second phase, participants were asked to come into the laboratory to

complete a measure of relationship motivations (i.e., the Relationship Initiation Motives

Inventory) and to make ratings of attraction towards three different romantic targets described to

be desirable, caring, or admiring. Importantly, before participants made these ratings of

attraction, their self-esteem levels were manipulated via the provision of false feedback

regarding their performance on a cognitive task.

The primary independent variables in this study included the NPI traits of

Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness, and the PNI

traits of Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability. The primary dependent variables

included the relationship initiation motivation variables (i.e., Intimacy, Hedonism, Enhancement,

Coping, and Self-Affirmation), attachment-related avoidance, and participants’ ratings of


2
romantic attraction towards the desirable target, admiring target, and caring target.

Participants

All participants were recruited through the Human Participation Research system

maintained by the psychology department at Michigan State University. The study description on

the website requested students who were not currently involved in a romantic relationship and

who were at least 18 years old (see Appendix 1). A total of 504 participants signed up for the

2
All materials used in Study 1 are contained in Appendix 1.

23
3
first phase of the study. Of these participants, 414 of them completed the second phase of the

study. Unfortunately, nine of these participants could not be linked up to the online data because

the emails that they provided at the second phase did not match those provided in the first phase.

Further, six participants were dropped because they did not respond affirmatively to the question,

“Did you answer these questions honestly?” on the online questionnaire. Finally, out of the 394

participants remaining, 48 were dropped due to their relationship status (i.e., 43 were dating

exclusively, four were living with their significant other, and one was divorced).

The sample that I used for the analyses therefore consisted of 346 undergraduate college

students. Most of the participants were women (60.4%; two participants did not report their

gender) and were 18 (28.6%), 19 (23.1%), 20 (21.7%), and 21 (26.3%) years old (one participant

did not report her/his age). Most participants (78%) were White (Not Hispanic/Not Latino) and

reported being either Single and Not Dating (80.3%) or Dating casually (19.7%). Almost all of

the participants self-identified as heterosexual (97.1%).

Materials

Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI). The NPI (Raskin & Terry, 1988) consists of a

set of 40 forced-choice items that aim to measure narcissism as a trait in nonclinical populations.

For each item, participants were asked to choose between a narcissistic alternative (e.g., “I like to

be the center of attention”) and a non-narcissistic alternative (e.g., “I prefer to blend in with the

crowd”). Previous work using the NPI has computed either the NPI total score (i.e., the sum or

average of participants’ responses to all of the items) or subscales derived from published

dimensional solutions (e.g., Emmons, 1984, 1987; Raskin & Terry, 1988).

3
This number includes participants that originally signed up for Study 2 but participated in
Study 1.
24
The present research made use of the dimensional solution provided in Ackerman et al.

(2011). The NPI subscales therefore included Leadership/Authority (11 items; α = .77, average

inter-item r = .23; M = 0.52, SD = 0.26), Grandiose Exhibitionism (10 items; α = .71; average

inter-item r = .19; M = 0.37, SD = 0.24), and Entitlement/Exploitativeness (4 items; α = .40;

average inter-item r = .15; M = 0.20, SD = 0.24). Participants’ scores for each of these subscales

were the average number of narcissistic alternatives that they endorsed, with higher scores

reflecting higher levels of the trait. Note that men and women did not significantly differ in their

average levels of Leadership/Authority (men: M = 0.53, SD = 0.27; women: M = 0.52, SD =

0.26; t(342) = 0.38, p = .703), Grandiose Exhibitionism (men: M = 0.38, SD = 0.25; women: M =

0.36, SD = 0.22; t(342) = 0.55, p = .584), and Entitlement/Exploitativeness (men: M = 0.21, SD =

0.23; women: M = 0.20, SD = 0.25; t(342) = 0.44, p = .661).

Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI). This measure consists of 52 items intended

to assess the pathological expression of narcissism (Pincus et al., 2009). Participants were asked

to rate how well each statement described them on a 6-point scale that ranged from 0 (Not at all

like me) to 5 (Very much like me). Pincus et al. (2009) originally established the existence of

seven subscales (see Figure 1). Recent work on the PNI, however, has established the existence

of higher order factors of Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability (Wright et al.,

2010).

Whereas Narcissistic Grandiosity (18 items; α = .81, average inter-item r = .19; M = 2.89,

SD = 0.60) is comprised of subscales that reflect maladaptive self-enhancement strategies (i.e.,

Grandiose Fantasy, Exploitativeness, and Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement), Narcissistic

Vulnerability (34 items; α = .93, average inter-item r = .29; M = 2.28, SD = 0.74) is comprised of

subscales that reflect deficient emotional and self-regulation (i.e., Entitlement-Rage, Hiding the

25
Self, Contingent Self-Esteem, and Devaluing). These superordinate scales of Narcissistic

Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability were used in this research. Note that men and women

did not significantly differ in their average levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity (men: M = 2.92, SD

= 0.60; women: M = 2.87, SD = 0.60; t(342) = 0.66, p = .511) and Narcissistic Vulnerability

(men: M = 2.22, SD = 0.74; women: M = 2.32, SD = 0.74; t(342) = -1.25, p = .212).

Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory. This 25-item measure was adapted from the

Sex Motives Scale developed by Cooper et al. (1998). Given that the original scale by Cooper et

al. (1998) was designed to assess individuals’ reasons for having sex, several modifications were

made so that the scale would reflect reasons for initiating romantic relationships. These

modifications included changing the instructions, changing the scale anchors, dropping

inappropriate subscales, adding a new subscale, and changing the wording for particular items.

Altogether, two scales assessed self-focused, approach-oriented reasons for beginning

relationships (i.e., the Hedonism Motive and Enhancement Motive subscales), two scales

assessed self-focused, avoidance-oriented reasons for beginning relationships (i.e., the Self-

Affirmation Motive and Coping Motive subscales), and one scale assessed other-focused,

approach-oriented reasons for beginning relationships (i.e., the Intimacy Motive subscale).

Participants were first presented with the following stem: “I often begin romantic relationships

with new partners …” Participants were then asked to indicate their degree of agreement with a

series of statements on a 5-point Likert-type scale that ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5

(Strongly Disagree).

The Intimacy Motive subscale (α = .86, average inter-item r = .56; M = 3.97, SD = 0.71)

included five items that assessed participants’ motive to develop emotional intimacy (e.g., “To

better understand the other person” and “To have another person truly understand me”). The

26
Self-Affirmation Motive subscale (α = .87, average inter-item r = .58; M = 2.57, SD = 0.93)

included five items that assessed participants’ motive to use the relationship to bolster their less

than adequate self-image (e.g., “Because it makes me feel like I’m a more interesting person”

and “To reassure myself that I am desirable”). The Hedonism Motive subscale (α = .78, average

inter-item r = .40; M = 2.93, SD = 0.83) included five items that assessed participants’ motive to

pursue excitement and physical pleasure (e.g., “Just for the excitement of it” and “To satisfy my

sexual needs”). The Enhancement Motive subscale (α = .84, average inter-item r = .52; M = 2.29,

SD = 0.88) included five items that assessed participants’ motive to affirm and enhance their

inflated self-image (e.g., “To impress others” and “Because they remind me I’m a great person”).

The Coping Motive subscale (α = .85, average inter-item r = .54; M = 2.33, SD = 0.88) included

five items that assessed participants’ intentions to use relationships as a means to regulate

negative affect (e.g., “To cope with upset feelings” and “To cheer myself up”). Participants’

scores for each subscale were computed as the mean of their responses, with higher scores

reflecting higher levels of the motive.

Attachment-Related Avoidance. To provide an additional means of assessing

participants’ desire for intimacy, the attachment-related avoidance subscale of the Experiences in

Close Relationships (ECR; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) questionnaire was included in the

current study. This subscale assesses participants’ discomfort with closeness. Participants were

asked to indicate how much they agreed with 18 statements on a 7-point scale that ranged from 1

(Disagree Strongly) to 7 (Agree Strongly). Example items included, “I prefer not to show others

how I feel deep down” and “I get uncomfortable when someone wants to be very close to me.”

Each participant’s score was the average of these items (α = .91, average inter-item r = .36; M =

3.40, SD = 1.00), with higher scores indicating higher levels of attachment-related avoidance.

27
Remote Associations Test (RAT). This test, created by Mednick (1962), was used as

part of a procedure to manipulate participants’ state levels of self-esteem. The RAT consists of a

series of three words that are somehow linked together. When presented with the three words

(e.g., Car-Swimming-Cue) on the computer, each participant was asked to come up with the

fourth word that links them together (e.g., “Pool”) and to type it in the blank space on the screen.

The present research used normative data compiled by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) on

RAT item difficulties to construct a cognitive task consisting of 10 easy items, seven difficult

items, and seven moderately difficult items. Between 85% and 100% of the participants in

Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) solved the 10 easy items within a 30-second time span.

Similarly, the seven difficult items were correctly solved by 0% to 10% of the participants in the

30-second time span, and the seven moderately difficult items were correctly solved by 49% to

51% of the participants in the 30-second time span. Participants were presented with these 24

items in a mixed order.

Manipulation Check. The manipulation check consisted of the State Self-Esteem Scale

(Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) and two additional questions reported by Jarry and Kossert (2007)

to ensure that participants paid attention to the feedback that was provided by the experimenter.

The State Self-Esteem Scale included 20 items (α = .90, average inter-item r = .33; M = 3.59, SD

= 0.61) that assessed participants’ current levels of self-esteem at the moment (e.g., “I feel good

about myself” and “I feel that others respect and admire me”). Although the State Self-Esteem

Scale technically consists of three separate subscales (i.e., Social Self-Esteem [7 items; α = .84,

average inter-item r = .43; M = 3.73, SD = 0.76], Performance Self-Esteem [7 items; α = .84,

average inter-item r = .44; M = 3.56, SD = 0.73], and Appearance Self-Esteem [6 items; α = .82,

average inter-item r = .43; M = 3.46, SD = 0.69]), some previous research using the scale as a

28
manipulation check has collapsed across the subscales (e.g., Heatherton & Vohs, 2000). In

contrast, other research has examined the subscales separately and has found the Performance

Self-Esteem subscale to be the most impacted by the manipulation (Jarry & Kossert, 2007).

The present research examined each of the three subscales with the expectation that the

Performance Self-Esteem subscale would be the most affected by the self-esteem manipulation.

The last two questions posed to participants in the manipulation check were taken from Jarry and

Kossert (2007) and concern participants’ attention to their performance on the RAT (note that the

RAT is presented to participants as a test of Integrative Orientation or IO for short): “How many

items on the IO test do you think you solved?” and “Please provide your own assessment of your

IO ability using the following 0 to 10 scale, with 0 being the worst and 10 being the best.”

Romantic Targets. Developed by Campbell (1999), this stimulus consisted of a written

scenario and the presentation of three potential romantic targets (the target described as ‘needy’

in Campbell [1999] was not found to be connected to any of the NPI facets and was therefore not

used in the present research). As reported in Campbell (1998, p. 78), the scenario was as follows:

Imagine the following situation: You are at a party with some people you know and some

people you don’t know. A friend of yours introduces you to [three] people of the opposite

sex who are single—we’ll call them by their initials: J.G., C.F., and B.W. Later, you run

into your friend in the kitchen and she tells you about these people in more detail. Your

friend describes the [three] single people you met— J.G., C.F., and B.W.—as follows…

At this point, participants were asked to refer to a handout that contained the three

romantic targets (note that photographs were not attached to the descriptions of the targets). As

was done in Campbell (1999), each target was identified by a pair of initials so that participants

could easily envision the target being a man or a woman. Moreover, the order in which the three

29
romantic targets was presented on the sheet was counterbalanced across participants. The

Admiring target was presented as follows: “J.G. told me that he/she thought you were terrific!

J.G. thought you were the best looking person at the party and that you were very charming. J.G.

also thought you were one of the most intelligent people he/she had ever met.” The Desirable

target was presented to participants as follows: “C.F. is really a great person. C.F. is very popular

and good-looking. He/she is also one of the top students at the school and is rated as one of the

best soccer players in the nation.” The Caring target was presented as follows: “B.W. is a really

caring person. B.W. told me that he/she’s looking for a person to date with whom he/she could

be close, intimate friends. B.W. is one of the most sensitive, sharing, compassionate, and friendly

people I know.”

Attraction Scale. This scale was developed by Campbell (1999) to assess participants’

attraction to the different romantic targets. It consists of five items on 7-point Likert-type scales.

Participants were asked to use a scale with the anchors of 1 (not at all) and 7 (very) to answer

three questions: “How attractive do you find this person?”, “How desirable would you find this

person as a dating partner?”, and “How much would you actually like to date this person?”

Participants were then asked to use the scale anchors of 1 (very bad) and 7 (very good) to

answer, “How would you feel about yourself if you were dating this person?”, and to use the

scale anchors of 1 (disapproving of me) and 7 (approving of me) to answer, “How do you think

your friends would feel about you if you were dating this person?” Participants’ scores for the

attraction scales were the mean of their responses to these five questions, with higher scores

reflecting greater levels of attraction. Attraction scores were computed for the admiring target (α

= .87, average inter-item r = .56; M = 4.73, SD = 1.13), the desirable target (α = .88, average

30
inter-item r = .60; M = 5.12, SD = 1.25), and the caring target (α = .91, average inter-item r =

.68; M = 5.26, SD = 1.20).

Procedure

Before signing up for an in-lab session, participants were asked to complete the NPI and

PNI online. This was done to ease the burden placed on participants’ time in the laboratory when

they came in for the second session. Participants arrived to the laboratory in groups of one to

four. The laboratory was housed in the basement of the psychology building and consisted of a

large room that was connected to a hallway with three smaller rooms. Each of the three smaller

rooms contained a desk with a computer. The larger room contained a long table in the center

and computers along the perimeter of the room. Upon arrival, participants were asked to sit at the

table in the larger part of the laboratory. After providing informed consent, the experimenter told

participants:

We are interested in the relations between different aspects of personality and romantic

attraction. This study will have three parts. In the first part, we will ask you to complete

some measures related to motivation. In the second part, we will ask you to take a test

that assesses an aspect of your cognitive ability. Finally, in the third part of the study, you

will complete some other personality measures and we will ask you to read several

different personality profiles and rate how attractive you find each one.

The experimenter then randomly assigned participants to one of four rooms and asked

them to complete the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory on the computer. Once

participants completed the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory, the experimenter

implemented a procedure designed to manipulate participants’ state levels of self-esteem. The

self-esteem manipulation used in this study was very similar to procedures that have been used in

31
previous research (Heatherton & Vohs, 2000; Jarry & Kossert, 2007). Participants completed a

cognitive task that contained a combination of easy, difficult, and moderately difficult questions,

and were then provided with false feedback that ostensibly informed them of their standing on a

cognitive ability. This study used a cover story to ensure that participants believed the cognitive

ability being tested was important and relevant to their lives. The cover story used in this study

drew heavily on the script reported in Jarry and Kossert (2007; see pp. 42-43). Each participant

was told:

As mentioned at the beginning, I will now ask you to take a test that assesses an

important cognitive ability called Integrative Orientation, or IO for short. The IO ability

involves the capacity to discern the relationships among different kinds of information

and to use that information to effectively solve analytical problems. IO has been

extensively researched for the past 10 years and has been found to be an important

predictor of success in college and in future employment. Have you heard of the IO

ability in your psychology courses [asked casually]?

After this speech, the experimenter introduced participants to the test purportedly used to

assess IO (i.e., the RAT). Participants were then presented with two example RAT problems to

ensure that they understood the task. After participants conveyed that they understood, the

experimenter provided them with the Partner Descriptions sheet with the romantic targets that

they would need to refer to later in the study. The experimenter then escorted participants back to

their computers to complete the IO test. Participants were then presented with a set of RAT

problems of varying difficulty (i.e., 10 easy items, 7 difficult items, and 7 moderately difficult

items).

32
Depending upon the room that participants were randomly assigned to at the beginning of

the study, participants experienced one of three experimental conditions: (a) the Success

Condition, (b) the Control Condition, or (c) the Failure Condition. Once participants completed

their set of problems, the computer provided them with immediate feedback on their purported

performance. In the Success Condition, the computer informed participants, “You performed

very well and scored within the top 15% of all students tested at Michigan State University.” In

the Control Condition, the computer did not provide participants with any feedback. In the

Failure Condition, the computer informed participants, “You did not perform very well and

scored within the bottom 30% of all students tested at Michigan State University.”

After providing feedback, the computer directed participants to complete the State Self-Esteem

Scale and then to answer the two questions explicitly connected to the IO task. After this,

participants were asked to refer to the Partner Descriptions sheet that they were given before

with the three descriptions of the romantic targets and they were asked to rate how attractive they

found each one using the Attraction Scale. Once they completed this, participants were thanked

and fully debriefed.

33
CHAPTER 4: RESULTS FOR STUDY 1

Narcissism and Relationship Initiation Motivations

Before proceeding to analyze associations between the narcissistic dimensions and the

Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was

conducted on the Pearson product-moment correlation matrix of the 25 items from the

Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory to evaluate the dimensional structure of the instrument.

Path analyses were then used to test the first set of hypotheses concerning the relations of the

narcissistic dimensions to the various relationship initiation motives. To provide an additional

test of the relation between these narcissistic dimensions and the desire for intimacy, multiple

regression analyses were performed with attachment-related avoidance as the criterion. Each set

of analyses was evaluated for gender moderation.

Exploratory factor analysis of relationship initiation motives inventory. SPSS

version 19.0 was used to conduct the EFA. Principal axis factoring was used as the estimation

method and the factors were rotated using the oblique promax procedure. Based on guidelines

proposed by Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, and Strahan (1999), the number of factors to

extract was decided upon through a combination of examination of the scree plot of eigenvalues

and evaluation of the interpretability of the factors in the solution. Results revealed four

eigenvalues above 1.0 (8.835, 3.505, 1.727, and 1.502). Figure 2 displays a scree plot of the

eigenvalues. As can be seen, the last drop occurs between the fifth and fourth eigenvalues,

therefore suggesting a four-factor solution. In addition, the most noticeable bends occurred

between the second and third eigenvalues (difference = 1.778) and between the fourth and fifth

eigenvalues (difference = 0.522). Together with the fact that five dimensions were initially

34
anticipated, two-, four-, and five-factor solutions were therefore extracted and subsequently

evaluated in regards to their interpretability.

The two-factor solution appeared to isolate the intimacy content from the self-

affirmation, coping, enhancement, and hedonism content. Thus, it represented a clean break

between the self-focused motives and the other-focused motive. Nonetheless, the self-focused

factor was somewhat difficult to interpret given that it combined diverse content related to

sensation-seeking, coping, and self-enhancement. The four-factor solution improved upon this

because in addition to isolating the intimacy content, it divided up the self-focused motivation

content into areas reflecting sensation-seeking, coping, and self-enhancement. The five-factor

solution did not appear to improve upon the four-factor solution. That is, it represented a

virtually identical solution with the exception that the item “Because I feel horny” defined an

additional fifth factor. In light of this, a four-factor solution was extracted. Table 2 shows the

pattern loadings for this solution.

As Table 2 shows, there was clear evidence for the existence of the Intimacy,

Enhancement, Coping, and Hedonism motive subscales. However, items from the Self-

Affirmation subscale appeared to primarily load on the Coping factor. I therefore decided to

retain the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales for Intimacy, Enhancement, and

Hedonism for use in the analyses. In addition, I decided to calculate an aggregated Coping/Self-

Affirmation subscale that averaged across the Coping and Self-Affirmation subscale items (10

items; M = 2.45, SD = 0.85; α = .91, average inter-item r = .51). This Coping/Self-Affirmation

subscale consequently reflected coping with negative self-images and affect in general.

Zero-order correlations between the narcissism measures, the relationship initiation

motives inventory measures, and the attachment-related avoidance measure. Table 3

35
presents zero-order correlations between the NPI subscales and the PNI subscales. The NPI

subscales were all moderately correlated with one another, with the largest association occurring

between Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism. Moreover, the PNI Vulnerability

scale was strongly correlated with the PNI Grandiosity scale. Further, whereas the NPI subscales

were all moderately correlated with the PNI Grandiosity scale, only the NPI

Entitlement/Exploitativeness scale was associated with the PNI Vulnerability scale.

Table 4 shows the zero-order correlations between the primary dependent variables. As

can be seen, all of the self-focused Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales were

strongly associated with each other. In contrast, the Intimacy subscale was modestly associated

with Coping/Self-Affirmation and moderately correlated with Enhancement. Attachment-related

avoidance demonstrated a modest positive association with Coping/Self-Affirmation.

Table 5 displays the zero-order correlations between the narcissism measures and the

primary dependent variables. NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism was positively connected to

Hedonism and Enhancement. Moreover, NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness was positively

connected to each of the self-focused motives and negatively connected to the Intimacy motive

subscale. PNI Grandiosity was positively connected to all of the self-focused motives. PNI

Vulnerability demonstrated a similar pattern of relations with the exception that it exhibited a

modest positive association with the Intimacy motive subscale. Table 5 also shows that NPI

Grandiose Exhibitionism was negatively related to attachment-related avoidance whereas PNI

Vulnerability was positively related to avoidance.

Path analysis evaluating relations between NPI traits and relationship initiation

motivations. A path analysis was used to test the hypotheses concerning the relation of the NPI

traits to the various relationship initiation motives. As shown in Figure 3, the NPI subscales of

36
Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness were specified

as exogenous variables in the model. Moreover, the four Relationship Initiation Motives

Inventory subscales of Intimacy, Coping/Self-Affirmation, Hedonism, and Enhancement were

treated as the endogenous variables. Correlations were freely estimated between the NPI

subscales. Likewise, correlations were freely estimated between the residuals for the

Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales. As such, this model was fully saturated.

Table 6 shows the parameter estimates for this model (estimated using AMOS version

19.0). As hypothesized, Leadership/Authority negatively predicted the Coping/Self-Affirmation

motive. Similarly, Grandiose Exhibitionism positively predicted the Hedonism and Enhancement

motives whereas Entitlement/Exploitativeness positively predicted the Coping/Self-Affirmation

motive and negatively predicted the Intimacy motive. In contrast to what was predicted,

however, there was no evidence that the Leadership/Authority or Grandiose Exhibitionism

subscales were connected to lower levels of the Intimacy motive.

Although not hypothesized, Leadership/Authority also negatively predicted the

Enhancement motive. Given the zero-order relation between these variables, this finding

provides evidence of a suppression effect and suggests that controlling for the overlap with

grandiosity and vulnerability makes the connection between normal narcissism and this motive

clearer. In addition, Entitlement/Exploitativeness positively predicted the Hedonism and

Enhancement motives. This suggests that self-enhancement may be another important reason that

entitled individuals initiate their romantic relationships.

Gender moderation. Gender moderation was evaluated for in the previous analysis by

performing a multiple-groups analysis. Table 7 displays the parameter coefficients from the

unconstrained multiple-groups analysis model for men and women. Imposing equality

37
constraints on the structural weights in the model (i.e., the coefficients for the paths between the

NPI subscales and the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales) for men and women
2
adversely affected model fit, χ (12) = 27.00, p = .008. Examination of the modification indices

suggested removing equality constraints for the following paths: (a) Leadership/Authority to

Enhancement; (b) Entitlement/Exploitativeness to Coping/Self-Affirmation; and (c)

Entitlement/Exploitativeness to Intimacy. Freely estimating these path coefficients for men and
2
women resulted in reasonable model fit, χ (9) = 16.34, p = .06, thereby suggesting that men and

women differed in regards to these particular variable relations.

Examination of the unconstrained parameters from this less restricted model revealed that

Leadership/Authority was not significantly related to the Enhancement motive for men, b = -

0.26, SE = .26, β = -.08, p = .301; however, Leadership/Authority was significantly related to the

Enhancement motive for women, b = -0.72, SE = .21, β = -.22, p < .001. This model also showed

that the relation between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Coping/Self-Affirmation was stronger

for men (b = 1.16, SE = .25, β = .31, p < .001) than for women (b = 0.56, SE = .21, β = .17, p =

.009). Finally, whereas there was no significant relation between Entitlement/Exploitativeness

and the Intimacy motive for women (b = -0.18, SE = .21, β = -.06, p = .39), men with higher

levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported lower levels of Intimacy motivations (b = -0.76,

SE = .24, β = -.26, p = .002).

Path analysis evaluating relations between PNI traits and relationship initiation

motivations. A path analysis was also used to evaluate the hypotheses concerning relations

between the PNI traits and the relationship initiation motives. Figure 4 shows the specified

model. As shown, the PNI scales of Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability were

specified as exogenous variables. In addition, the four Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory

38
subscales of Intimacy, Coping/Self-Affirmation, Hedonism, and Enhancement were treated as

the endogenous variables. The PNI scales were allowed to correlate. Likewise, correlations were

freely estimated between the residuals for the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory

subscales. As before, this path analysis was a fully saturated model.

Table 8 presents the parameter estimates. There was no support for the hypothesis that

higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity are connected to greater levels of self-enhancing

relationship initiation motivations. Likewise, there was no support for the hypothesis that

Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability would be negatively related to the

Intimacy motive. In fact, there was even a weak positive relation between Narcissistic

Vulnerability and the Intimacy motive. Nevertheless, the hypothesis that Narcissistic

Vulnerability would positively predict the Coping/Self-Affirmation motive was supported in

these data. Although not originally hypothesized, Narcissistic Vulnerability also positively

predicted the Hedonism and Enhancement motives. These findings suggest that the relationship

initiation motivations endorsed by individuals with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability

serve both self-enhancing and self-protective functions.

Gender moderation. Gender moderation was evaluated for in the previous analysis by

conducting a multiple-groups analysis. Table 9 presents the parameter coefficients for men and

women from the unconstrained multiple-groups analysis model. Imposing equality constraints on
2
the structural weights in the model for men and women did not adversely affect model fit, χ (8)

= 6.39, p = .603. Thus, there was no evidence that gender moderated these effects.

Summary for analyses involving narcissism and the relationship initiation motives

inventory subscales. The measures of normal narcissism, grandiose narcissism, and vulnerable

narcissism were each hypothesized to demonstrate a unique pattern of relations with the

39
relationship initiation motive measures. As hypothesized, Leadership/Authority negatively

predicted the coping relationship initiation motive. Leadership/Authority also negatively

predicted the enhancement relationship initiation motivation. Although results showed that NPI

Grandiose Exhibitionism positively predicted the Hedonism and Enhancement relationship

initiation motives, PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity was unrelated to the self-focused relationship

initiation motives. Consistent with predictions, the vulnerable narcissistic traits positively

predicted the coping relationship initiation motivation. However, whereas NPI

Entitlement/Exploitativeness negatively predicted the intimacy motive, PNI Narcissistic

Vulnerability positively predicted it. Finally, although not originally predicted, the vulnerable

narcissistic traits both positively predicted the self-enhancing relationship motives.

Narcissism and attachment-related avoidance. As an additional test of the association

between narcissism and the desire for intimacy, a series of multiple regression analyses were

performed to evaluate the independent associations of the NPI and PNI traits with the ECR

subscale of attachment-related avoidance. A multiple regression analysis using the NPI traits to

predict attachment-related avoidance found that Leadership/Authority was unrelated to

attachment-related avoidance, b = 0.20, SE = .22, β = .05, p = .367. In contrast, Grandiose

Exhibitionism negatively predicted attachment-related avoidance, b = -1.14, SE = .25, β = -.27, p

< .001; further, Entitlement/Exploitativeness positively predicted avoidance, b = 0.56, SE = .23,

β = .13, p = .014. None of these effects were significantly moderated by gender. Thus, across

both men and women, individuals with higher levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported

more discomfort with intimacy whereas individuals with higher levels of Grandiose

Exhibitionism reported less discomfort with intimacy.

40
A multiple regression analysis using the PNI traits to predict attachment-related

avoidance found that Narcissistic Grandiosity negatively predicted attachment-related avoidance,

b = -0.44, SE = .10, β = -.26, p < .001. Conversely, Narcissistic Vulnerability positively predicted

scores on attachment-related avoidance, b = 0.63, SE = .08, β = .47, p < .001. Additional

analyses including participant gender revealed significant interactions between Narcissistic

Grandiosity and participant gender, b = -0.38, SE = .10, p < .001, and between Narcissistic

Vulnerability and participant gender, b = 0.18, SE = .08, p = .027. To break down these

interactions, simple slopes for men and women were estimated for these relations between the

PNI traits and attachment-related avoidance.

The simple slope for women for the relation between PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity and

avoidance was, b = -0.12, SE = .13, β = -.07, p = .367. Moreover, the simple slope for men for

the relation between PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity and avoidance was, b = -0.88, SE = .15, β = -

.53, p < .001. Thus, the relation between PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity and attachment-related

avoidance appeared to only hold true for men. The simple slope for women for the relation

between PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability and avoidance was, b = 0.48, SE = .10, β = .36, p < .001.

Further, the simple slope for men for the relation between PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability and

avoidance was, b = 0.83, SE = .12, β =.62, p < .001. This suggests that although the relation

between Narcissistic Vulnerability and attachment-related avoidance was positive for men and

women, the magnitude of the association was stronger for men.

In sum, each of the narcissistic dimensions exhibited somewhat divergent relations with

attachment-related avoidance. Leadership/Authority was unrelated to attachment-related

avoidance. Moreover, individuals who possessed greater levels of grandiose narcissism reported

lower levels of attachment-related avoidance, therefore suggesting that they are more

41
comfortable with intimacy. However, evidence for the relation between PNI Grandiosity and

attachment-related avoidance only held for men. In contrast, individuals who possessed greater

levels of vulnerable narcissism reported higher levels of attachment-related avoidance; this

relation was particularly pronounced for men.

Narcissism and Attraction towards the Romantic Targets

The second set of hypotheses in Study 1 concerned whether the different narcissistic

dimensions are associated with differential attraction towards particular romantic targets. Before

testing these hypotheses, the effect of target presentation order on participants’ attraction towards

the romantic targets was evaluated. Multiple regression analyses were then used to test the

primary hypotheses regarding the relations between the narcissistic dimensions and attraction

towards the admiring, desirable, and caring romantic targets.

Main effect of target presentation order on attraction towards the romantic targets.

As discussed earlier, participants received presentations of the admiring, desirable, and caring

romantic targets that differed in their ordering. Altogether, six different presentation orders were

possible (1 = Admiring, desirable, and caring; 2 = Admiring, caring, and desirable; 3 =

Desirable, admiring, and caring; 4 = Desirable, caring, and admiring; 5 = Caring, desirable, and

admiring; 6 = Caring, admiring, and desirable). A series of one-way between-subjects ANOVAs

were used to assess the impact of ordering presentation on participants’ ratings of attraction

towards the different romantic targets. Table 10 shows the means and standard deviations for

participants’ ratings of attraction towards the different targets as a function of the order in which

they were presented.

Ordering was not observed to impact participants’ ratings of attraction towards the

admiring target, F (5, 336) = 0.11, MSE = 1.26, p = .99. Similarly, ordering did not significantly

42
affect participants’ ratings of attraction towards the caring target, F (5, 336) = 1.48, MSE = 1.40,
4
p = .195. However, ordering did impact participants’ ratings of attraction towards the desirable

target, F (5, 336) = 3.30, MSE = 1.49, p = .006. A post-hoc Tukey analysis revealed that

presentation order 2 (where the admiring target was presented first, the caring target was

presented second, and the desirable target was presented last) significantly differed from

presentation order 4 (where the desirable target was presented first, the caring target was

presented second, and the admiring target was presented last). There was no evidence of

differences for the other combinations. This suggests that the position of the description for the

desirable target influenced participants’ subsequent attraction ratings towards the desirable

target. Target presentation order did not moderate any of the effects in the following analyses

examining participants’ attraction towards the desirable target. Unless otherwise noted,

controlling for target presentation order also did not substantially alter results for analyses

involving attraction towards the desirable target.

Zero-order correlations between the narcissism measures and the attraction

measures. Table 11 shows the zero-order correlations between the attraction-towards-target

measures. As can be seen, participants’ scores for the attraction measures concerning the three

romantic targets were modestly related. Moreover, Table 12 shows that NPI

Leadership/Authority was positively correlated with attraction towards a desirable partner. NPI

Grandiose Exhibitionism was also positively correlated with attraction towards a desirable

partner, and to a weaker extent, attraction towards an admiring target. Last, PNI Grandiosity was

positively correlated with attraction towards a desirable partner.

4
A Brown-Forsyth (1974a) test indicated heterogeneity of variance, F (5, 336) = 3.46, MSE =
0.50, p = .005. Nevertheless, the Brown and Forsyth (1974b) robust test of equality of means
revealed a similar pattern of results, FBrown-Forsyth (5, 282.04) = 1.48, p = .197.

43
Multiple regression analyses using NPI subscales to predict attraction towards the

romantic targets. Table 13 shows the results from three multiple regression analyses in which

the three NPI subscales were used to predict participants’ attraction towards each of the romantic

targets. Note that these analyses were examining the independent effects of each of these NPI

subscales collapsing across all three RAT conditions. As can be seen, none of my hypotheses

were supported.

Instead, Table 13 shows that Leadership/Authority significantly predicted participants’

attraction towards the desirable target, such that participants with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority reported being more attracted to the desirable target. Although not

statistically significant, the coefficient for Grandiose Exhibitionism was virtually identical in

magnitude to that of Leadership/Authority. When the effect of target presentation order was

controlled for in these analyses, the relation between Grandiose Exhibitionism and attraction

towards the desirable target became significant.

Gender moderation. Each of the above analyses was rerun with gender as a moderator of

each of the effects. The multiple regression analysis with attraction towards the admiring target

as an outcome revealed a significant interaction between gender and Leadership/Authority, b =

0.61, SE = .27, p = .025, and a significant interaction between gender and Grandiose

Exhibitionism, b = -0.63, SE = .30, p = .035. To follow up these interactions, simple slopes for

men and women were estimated for the relations between the NPI subscales and attraction

towards the admiring target.

The simple slope for women for the effect of Leadership/Authority on attraction towards

the admiring target was, b = -0.34, SE = .32, β = -.08, p = .296. In contrast, the simple slope for

men for this relation was, b = 0.89, SE = .44, β = .21, p = .045. Thus, it is only for men that

44
higher levels of Leadership/Authority predict greater attraction towards the admiring target. The

simple slope for women for the effect of Grandiose Exhibitionism on attraction was, b = 0.91, SE

= .38, β = .19, p = .017. The simple slope for men for this relation was, b = -0.35, SE = .46, β = -

.07, p = .45. These results indicate that only women with higher levels of Grandiose

Exhibitionism report greater attraction towards the admiring target.

A second multiple regression analysis with attraction towards the desirable target as the

outcome revealed a significant interaction between gender and Leadership/Authority, b = 0.85,

SE = .30, p = .005. To break this down, simple slopes for men and women were again estimated.

The simple slope for women was, b = -0.05, SE = .35, β = -.01, p = .893. The simple slope for

men was, b = 1.65, SE = .48, β =.34, p = .001. Consequently, only men with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority reported greater attraction towards the desirable target.

A third multiple regression analysis with attraction towards the caring target as the

outcome did not reveal any significant interactions with gender. Thus, there was no evidence of

gender moderation for these effects.

Multiple regression analyses using PNI scales to predict attraction towards the

romantic targets. Table 14 shows results from three multiple regression analyses in which the

two PNI scales were used to predict participants’ attraction towards each of the romantic targets.

Again, these analyses examined the independent effects of the PNI traits collapsing across the

three RAT conditions. As can be seen in Table 14, none of my hypotheses were supported.

Instead, the PNI Grandiosity scale significantly predicted attraction towards the desirable target,

such that individuals with higher levels of PNI Grandiosity reported being more attracted to the

desirable target.

45
Gender moderation. The above analyses were rerun with gender as a moderator of the

effects. None of the analyses revealed any significant interactions with gender.

Summary for analyses using the narcissism variables to predict attraction towards

the romantic targets. Each of the narcissistic dimensions was hypothesized to be associated

with attraction towards a particular romantic target. In partial support of my hypotheses, I found

that women with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism reported greater levels of attraction

towards the admiring target. However, no other hypotheses were supported. Instead, participants

with higher levels of normal and grandiose narcissistic traits reported greater attraction towards

the desirable target.

Regulation of Self-Esteem through Romantic Attraction

The third set of hypotheses in Study 1 dealt with whether attraction towards particular

romantic targets can serve as a means for narcissists to regulate self-esteem. To examine this,

participants were randomly assigned to one of three RAT conditions before providing their

ratings of attraction towards the three romantic targets. Before performing the main analyses, the

manipulation checks for the RAT condition were assessed. After determining the efficacy of the

self-esteem manipulation, multiple regression analyses with the RAT condition as a moderator

were used to evaluate the main hypotheses. Gender moderation was evaluated for in each of the

primary analyses.

Manipulation checks. Two questions were posed to participants after the IO task to

assess whether they paid attention to the feedback that they had received. The first question

asked participants to report how many problems they believed they had solved successfully.

Because some participants wrote words instead of typing in numbers in response to this question,

some of the data had to be modified. Numbers written in words were transformed into numbers

46
(n = 10). Moreover, expressions such as, “about half” and “75% of them” (n = 11) were treated

as their corresponding numerical values (i.e., 12 and 18, respectively). Other expressions that

conveyed ambiguity (e.g., “most” and “not many”) or were nonsensical (e.g., “40545”) were

treated as missing data (n = 19).

A one-way between-subjects ANOVA was used to determine the effect of the RAT

condition (i.e., success vs. failure vs. control) on participants’ responses to this first question.

Results revealed that the RAT condition had a significant effect on their responses, F (2, 324) =

5.78, MSE = 13.08, p = .003. A post-hoc Tukey analysis showed that participants in the success

condition reported solving significantly more problems (n = 144, M = 7.23, SD = 4.10) than

participants in the failure condition (n = 96, M = 5.67, SD = 3.05), Cohen’s d = 0.42. However,

the success condition did not differ significantly from the control condition (n = 87, M = 6.20,

SD = 3.34), Cohen’s d = 0.27, and the control condition did not differ significantly from the

failure condition, Cohen’s d = 0.17.

The second question after the IO task asked participants to provide their own assessment

of their IO ability using a 10-point scale that ranged from 0 (the worst) to 10 (the best). A one-

way between-subjects ANOVA was used to evaluate the effect of the RAT condition on

participants’ responses to this second question. Results revealed that the RAT condition had a

significant effect on participants’ responses, F (2, 343) = 21.05, MSE = 2.69, p < .001. A post-

hoc Tukey analysis showed that participants in the success condition reported having a

significantly higher IO ability (n = 152, M = 5.28, SD = 1.73) than participants in the control

condition (n = 91, M = 4.62, SD = 1.60), Cohen’s d = 0.39, and the failure condition (n = 103, M

= 3.93, SD = 1.54), Cohen’s d = 0.82, conditions. Likewise, participants in the control condition

reported having a significantly higher IO ability than participants in the failure condition,

47
Cohen’s d = 0.44. In sum, participants’ responses to these first two questions suggest that

participants’ opinions of their performance and ability were indeed impacted by the RAT

condition that they were placed in during the study.

The next set of manipulation checks determined whether the RAT condition impacted

participants’ state levels of self-esteem. A one-way between-subjects ANOVA evaluating the

effect of the RAT condition on participants’ total state self-esteem levels revealed that the RAT

condition did not have an overall impact on the omnibus measure of state self-esteem, F (2, 343)

= 2.82, MSE = 0.36, p = .061. Additional one-way between-subjects ANOVAs showed that the

RAT condition also did not significantly affect participants’ social state self-esteem levels, F (2,

343) = 1.46, MSE = 0.57, p = .233, or their appearance state self-esteem levels, F (2, 343) = 0.06,

MSE = 0.48, p = .938. However, the RAT condition did significantly affect participants’
5
performance state self-esteem levels, F (2, 343) = 7.26, MSE = 0.52, p = .001.

A post-hoc Tukey analysis revealed that participants in the success condition (n = 152, M

= 3.72, SD = 0.63) reported significantly higher performance state self-esteem than participants

in the failure condition (n = 103, M = 3.40, SD = 0.81), Cohen’s d = 0.45, or the control

condition (n = 91, M = 3.47, SD = 0.75), Cohen’s d = 0.37. However, there was no evidence that

participants’ performance state self-esteem levels were different between the failure and control

RAT conditions, Cohen’s d = -0.09. Taken together, the results provide evidence for the efficacy

of the self-esteem manipulation. Although it did not impact the omnibus measure of state self-

esteem, the RAT condition significantly affected participants’ performance state self-esteem

levels as expected.

5
A Brown-Forsyth (1974a) test indicated heterogeneity of variance, F (2, 343) = 3.49, MSE =
0.22, p = .032. Nevertheless, the Brown and Forsyth (1974b) robust test of equality of means
revealed a similar pattern of results, FBrown-Forsyth (2, 280.17) = 6.86, p = .001.

48
Main effect of RAT condition on attraction towards the romantic targets. Table 15

shows the means and standard deviations for attraction towards each romantic target as a

function of the RAT condition. A one-way between-subjects ANOVA evaluating the effect of

the RAT condition on attraction towards the admiring target did not reveal a significant effect of

condition, F (2, 343) = 0.21, MSE = 1.27, p = .81. Likewise, a one-way between-subjects

ANOVA evaluating the effect of the RAT condition on attraction towards the desirable target did

not reveal a significant effect of condition, F (2, 343) = 1.68, MSE = 1.56, p = 19. However, a

one-way between-subjects ANOVA testing the effect of RAT condition on attraction towards the

caring target revealed a significant effect for condition, F (2, 343) = 3.05, MSE = 1.43, p = .049.

A post-hoc Tukey analysis revealed a marginally significant difference between the

success and failure conditions (p = .05), such that participants receiving negative feedback

regarding their performance on the IO task reported greater levels of attraction towards the

caring target. In contrast, no significant differences were found between the success and control

conditions or between the failure and control conditions.

Does the self-esteem manipulation moderate the relations between the narcissism

variables and romantic attraction towards the admiring, desirable, and caring targets? A

series of multiple regression analyses were used to evaluate whether the narcissism-attraction

associations were moderated by the self-esteem manipulation. Two dummy codes were created

to represent the RAT condition: Success (i.e., 1 for the success condition and 0 otherwise) and

Failure (i.e., 1 for the failure condition and 0 otherwise). The reference group when including

these dummy codes in the analysis was therefore the control condition. The NPI and PNI

subscales were each grand-mean centered on their respective means. Interaction terms were

49
formed between each of the dummy coded RAT condition variables and the centered NPI and

PNI scales.

Using the NPI to predict attraction towards the romantic targets. Each multiple

regression analysis used the dummy codes for the RAT condition, the centered NPI subscales,

and the interaction terms between the centered NPI subscales and the dummy codes for the RAT

condition to predict participants’ attraction towards the romantic target. Hierarchical regression

analyses were used to evaluate the incremental change in R-squared that resulted from including

the interaction terms in the model. Similar to the previous multiple regression analyses

examining the independent effects of the NPI variables, I was interested in the extent to which a

block of interaction terms between a particular NPI variable (e.g., Grandiose Exhibitionism) and

the RAT condition dummy codes explained additional variance in the outcome over and above

the inclusion of the other NPI variables and their interaction terms with the RAT condition

dummy codes. Thus, each test of R-squared change for a block of interaction terms corresponds

to the case when it is the final block in the model.

Table 16 presents results from a multiple regression analysis in which participants’

attraction towards the admiring target was the outcome. As can be seen, each of the blocks

containing interactions between the RAT condition dummy codes and the NPI variables did not

account for significant additional variance in participants’ attraction towards the admiring target.

Thus, there was no evidence that the self-esteem manipulation moderated the narcissism-

attraction relations.

Table 17 presents results from a multiple regression analysis in which participants’

attraction towards the desirable target was the outcome. Analyses showed that the blocks

containing the interactions between the RAT condition dummy codes and the NPI variables did

50
not contribute significant additional variance to the outcome. However, when target presentation

order was controlled for in the analysis, the block containing the interactions between the RAT

condition dummy codes and Entitlement/Exploitativeness explained significant additional


2
variance in participants’ attraction towards the desirable target, ΔR = .018, ΔF (2, 329) = 3.18, p

= .043. This finding was followed up by computing simple slopes for the

Entitlement/Exploitativeness-Attraction relation in the success, failure, and control RAT

conditions. As Table 18 shows, Entitlement/Exploitativeness negatively predicted attraction

towards the desirable target in the failure condition. Thus, individuals with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported being less attracted to the desirable target when they

received negative feedback regarding their performance on the IO task. In contrast,

Entitlement/Exploitativeness was unrelated to attraction towards the desirable target in the

success and control conditions.

Table 19 presents results from a multiple regression analysis in which participants’

attraction towards the caring target was the outcome. As seen in Table 19, none of the blocks

containing interactions between the RAT condition dummy codes and the NPI variables

explained significant additional variance in the outcome. Thus, there was little evidence to

suggest that the self-esteem manipulation moderated the relations between the NPI variables and

participants’ attraction towards the caring target.

Gender moderation. Additional hierarchical regression analyses were used to evaluate

whether gender moderated any of the two-way interactions between the NPI subscales and RAT

condition. Out of nine tests, only one suggested moderation. The block containing the effects of

the two-way interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes on
2
attraction towards the caring target was significantly moderated by gender, ΔR = .026, ΔF (2,

51
326) = 4.49, p = .012. To follow this up, simple slopes for the two-way interaction between

gender and Leadership/Authority were estimated for each RAT condition. The simple slope for

the control condition was, b = -0.11, SE = .55, p = .847. The simple slope for the success

condition was, b = -0.25, SE = .42, p = .56. The simple slope for the failure condition was, b =

1.67, SE = .54, p = .002. These results indicate that the relation between Leadership/Authority

and attraction towards the caring target only differed for men and women when they were given

negative feedback on the IO task.

This significant two-way interaction was further broken down by computing simple

slopes for men and women. The simple slope for women in the failure RAT condition was, b = -

0.27, SE = .34, β = -.06, p = .439. Moreover, the simple slope for men was, b = 0.31, SE = .47, β

=.07, p = .509. Thus, whereas women tended to report being less attracted to the caring target in

the failure condition, men tended to report being more attracted to the caring target. Because

both of these simple slopes were not significantly different from zero, however, the finding that

these relations differed for men and women should be treated with caution.

Using the PNI to predict attraction towards the romantic targets. I used the same

strategy outlined before for the PNI scales. Table 20 presents results from a multiple regression

analysis in which attraction towards the admiring target was the outcome. Moreover, Table 21

presents results from a multiple regression analysis in which participants’ attraction towards the

desirable target was the outcome. As can be seen, none of the blocks containing interactions

between the PNI variables and the RAT condition dummy codes accounted for significant

additional variance in the outcomes.

Table 22 presents results from a multiple regression analysis in which participants’

attraction towards the caring target was the outcome. The block containing the two-way

52
interactions between Narcissistic Vulnerability and the RAT condition dummy codes explained

significant additional variance in participants’ attraction towards the caring target. Simple slopes

analysis was used to follow up these significant interactions. Results are presented in Table 23

and show that Narcissistic Vulnerability positively predicted attraction in the failure RAT

condition. Thus, individuals with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability reported being more

attracted to the caring target when they received negative feedback regarding their performance

on the IO task. The other relations were negative but non-significant.

Gender moderation. Hierarchical regression analyses were again used to assess whether

gender moderated any of the two-way interactions between the PNI scales and the RAT

condition dummy codes. None of the tests revealed evidence of gender moderation.

Summary of analyses evaluating moderation of the narcissism-attraction relations

by the self-esteem manipulation. It was hypothesized that lowering participants’ state self-

esteem levels would strengthen the magnitudes of the previously proposed associations between

the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic dimensions and attraction towards particular romantic

targets. Overall, there was little support for this hypothesis. The self-esteem manipulation did not

moderate any of the associations between the grandiose narcissism measures and attraction

towards the romantic targets. Moreover, in contrast to what was hypothesized, individuals with

higher levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported being less attracted to the desirable target

when their state self-esteem was lowered. In a similar manner, individuals with higher levels of

Narcissistic Vulnerability reported being more attracted to the caring target when their state self-

esteem was lowered. Such findings run counter to what was originally predicted.

53
CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION FOR STUDY 1

Study 1 aimed to better understand the roles of normal, grandiose, and vulnerable

narcissistic traits in the appraisal of initial attraction. Towards this end, Study 1 examined

whether these traits were predictive of several relationship initiation motivations. It also

investigated what types of partners are considered attractive by people with differing levels of

these narcissistic dimensions and whether attraction towards these different romantic partners

can serve as a means to regulate self-esteem. Below I summarize the main results from Study 1

for each of the narcissistic dimensions and then briefly discuss conclusions.

Normal Narcissism and the Appraisal of Initial Attraction

A robust finding in the literature is that people with higher levels of Leadership/Authority

possess greater levels of self-esteem (Ackerman et al., 2011; Brown, Budzek, & Tamborski,

2009; Rhodewalt & Morf, 1995; Watson & Biderman, 1993) and experience lower levels of

psychological distress (Emmons, 1984; Watson & Biderman, 1993; Watson & Morris, 1991). It

therefore follows that people with higher levels of this trait are unlikely to pursue their romantic

relationships to improve their self-image or to ameliorate negative emotions. Consistent with

this, participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority reported less frequently pursuing

their romantic relationships to cope with negative emotions, to bolster an inadequate sense of

self, and to enhance a potentially inflated self-image.

This research also found that men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were more

attracted to potential romantic partners that were described to be admiring or desirable. Indeed,

romantic attraction towards such partners may represent adaptive self-esteem regulation

strategies, such as asserting dominance (for attraction towards admiring partners) and

achievement striving (for attraction towards perfect partners; Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010). The

54
finding that more dominant men are attracted to desirable partners is also consistent with

theorizing by evolutionary psychologists that dominant and powerful men pursue especially

attractive female mates (see Kenrick, Trost, & Sheets, 1996).

Grandiose Narcissistic Traits and the Appraisal of Initial Attraction

Participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism reported being more likely to

pursue their romantic relationships for hedonistic reasons and to enhance their self-image. This

suggests that grandiose individuals, at least to some extent, construe romantic relationships as an

avenue to increase positive feelings and sentiments towards the self. This is consistent with other

work that has found vanity and exhibitionism to be moderately positively associated with

strivings for self-presentation (Emmons, 1989). This conclusion needs to be tempered by the

fact, however, that the same results were not obtained for Narcissistic Grandiosity as assessed by

the PNI. Although both measures assess maladaptive self-enhancement strategies, the NPI

Grandiose Exhibitionism subscale appears to capture features that are more explicitly concerned

with the admiration of others—namely, exhibitionism and vanity. As such, these features are

likely to be more closely connected to the adoption of motivations that emphasize the need for

self-enhancement or excitement.

Interestingly, the grandiose narcissistic traits were both negatively related to attachment-

related avoidance. Therefore, people who possessed higher levels of grandiose traits tended to be

more comfortable with intimacy and closeness. This was true across both sexes for Grandiose

Exhibitionism and it was true in men for Narcissistic Grandiosity. Although I had originally

expected that grandiose narcissism would be positively related to attachment-related avoidance,

the current findings are actually consistent with previous research. Indeed, Dickinson and Pincus

55
(2003) found that the majority of the participants that they designated as grandiose narcissistic

subtypes reported possessing secure adult attachment styles.

The present study further hypothesized that grandiose narcissistic traits would be

positively associated with attraction towards admiring targets. Results from Study 1 provided

some support for this hypothesis: Women with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism

reported being more attracted to the targets that were described as admiring. Thus, admiration in

a potential romantic partner may represent an important resource for women who possess greater

levels of vanity and exhibitionism. Unlike Grandiose Exhibitionism, Narcissistic Grandiosity

was unrelated to attraction towards the admiring target.

Although not hypothesized, one of the consistent findings to emerge from Study 1 is that

participants with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits are more attracted to targets that are

described to be desirable. Indeed, both NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism and PNI Narcissistic

Grandiosity positively predicted attraction towards the desirable target. This finding is consistent

with research by Campbell (1999) that found that narcissists use identification as one means to

self-enhance in romantic relationships. The primary caveat to this conclusion is that this means

of self-enhancement appears to be more appropriately reserved for grandiose narcissistic traits as

opposed to vulnerable narcissistic traits.

Vulnerable Narcissistic Traits and the Appraisal of Initial Attraction

The hypothesis that vulnerable narcissistic traits would be positively related to the

endorsement of self-protective relationship initiation motivations was supported in the present

research. Participants with higher levels of NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness and PNI

Narcissistic Vulnerability reported more frequently beginning their romantic relationships to

cope with negative emotions and to bolster an inadequate sense of self. Much previous research

56
has documented associations between narcissistic traits related to entitlement and

exploitativeness and increased negative affect (Emmons, 1987; Watson, Little, Sawrie, &

Biderman, 1992; Watson & Morris, 1991) and unstable self-views (Ackerman et al., 2011;

Brown et al., 2009; Watson et al., 1992). Likewise, research by Pincus and his colleagues (2009)

has shown that facets belonging to the PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability factor are associated with

lower levels of self-esteem and increased shame. Based on these findings, it seems reasonable

that an important criterion for vulnerable individuals in initiating a romantic relationship is the

possibility of ameliorating negative emotions and coping with negative self-views.

Although not hypothesized, there was evidence that participants with higher levels of

vulnerable narcissistic traits also appear to adopt self-enhancing relationship motivations. Indeed,

both NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness and PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability positively predicted

the Hedonism and Enhancement relationship initiation motives. These findings suggest that

people with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits are also concerned with using their

relationships to enhance their self-image and pursue excitement.

This research further predicted that participants with higher levels of vulnerable

narcissistic traits would possess diminished intimacy motivations. The results showed that the

vulnerable narcissistic traits did indeed exhibit significant relations with the intimacy

relationship initiation motive variable; however, the two operationalizations of vulnerable

narcissism exhibited divergent relations. Men with higher levels of NPI

Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported being less likely to pursue their relationships to develop

emotional intimacy with their partners. Thus, at least for men with higher levels of this

normative expression of vulnerable narcissism, emotional intimacy represents an atypical motive

for initiating relationships. This would be consistent with arguments made by Campbell (1999)

57
that intimacy represents a detriment to self-enhancement for narcissists. Indeed, increased

intimacy may exacerbate vulnerability and risk further injury to an already brittle sense of self.

On the other hand, participants with higher levels of PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability

reported pursuing their romantic relationships more often to develop emotional intimacy. To

explore why this might be, I examined partial correlations between the facets of Narcissistic

Vulnerability and the intimacy relationship initiation motive variable after controlling for the

effects of Narcissistic Grandiosity and gender. Whereas the intimacy motive was unrelated to

Entitlement-Rage (r = .02, p = .700), Devaluing (r = .05, p = .323), and Hiding the Self (r = .07,

p = .174), it was significantly related to Contingent Self-Esteem (r = .14, p = .011). This suggests

that intimacy represents a more important motivation for beginning romantic relationships for

individuals whose self-esteem is labile and contingent upon external sources.

As an additional test of whether the vulnerable narcissistic dimensions would be

associated with diminished intimacy motivations, I examined connections between the

vulnerable narcissistic traits and attachment-related avoidance. The vulnerable narcissistic traits

were positively related to attachment-related avoidance, indicating that people who possess

greater levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Narcissistic Vulnerability experience greater

discomfort with intimacy in general. This finding is consistent with research by Dickinson and

Pincus (2003) that has found that vulnerable narcissistic subtypes more frequently report

possessing a fearful avoidant adult attachment style.

When investigating the effects of decreasing participants’ levels of self-esteem on

romantic attraction, I found that the relation between NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness and

attraction towards the desirable target was moderated by the self-esteem manipulation.

Specifically, people with higher levels of NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported being less

58
attracted to desirable partners when their self-esteem levels were lowered. Given their higher

self-reported levels of vulnerability and depression (Ackerman & Donnellan, 2011), participants

with higher levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness may have viewed the desirable targets as a

potential threat to their sense of self-worth. This therefore hints at the participants’ use of

devaluation as a strategy to undermine this threat. Another potential explanation is that, in

general, threatened people will denigrate people of higher status (see Crocker, Thompson,

McGraw, & Ingerman, 1987).

Related to this, participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability reported being

more attracted to the caring target when they experienced a temporary decrease in self-esteem.

Although inconsistent with my predictions, this result is congruent with the previous finding that

Narcissistic Vulnerability is positively related to the intimacy relationship initiation motive.

Taken together, the results suggest that people with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability are

more likely to seek out partners that offer the potential for intimacy when they feel wounded.

Conclusions

Broadly speaking, many of the outcomes investigated in this research can be considered

either self-focused (e.g., pursuing a new romantic relationship to reduce one’s own negative

affect) or other-focused (e.g., pursuing a new romantic relationship to become close to the other

person). Furthermore, the self-focused outcomes can be thought to reflect either self-enhancing

or self-protecting opportunities. Whereas self-enhancing opportunities represent the potential to

increase positive sentiments towards the self, self-protective opportunities represent the potential

to decrease negative sentiments towards the self.

The present research found that the pattern of correlates for the normal, grandiose, and

vulnerable narcissistic traits were all fairly distinct. Normal narcissism was generally associated

59
with outcomes reflecting a diminished self-focus. The grandiose narcissistic traits, in contrast,

were more consistently associated with outcomes that reflected self-enhancement. Further, the

vulnerable narcissistic traits were characterized by outcomes that reflected a mixture of self-

enhancement and self-protection.

Several significant gender differences emerged across the analyses. It is important to

note, however, that none of these gender differences were predicted a priori on the basis of

strong theory. Moreover, the pattern of gender differences was not consistent enough to lend

itself to interpretation. Indeed, given the large number of tests for gender moderation and the few

significant differences that emerged, their occurrence is most likely due to chance. Future

research will be needed to replicate these gender differences and therefore determine whether

they are robust.

An important concern with the Relationship Initiation Motivations measure is that I did

not ask participants how many times they had actually attempted to begin a romantic

relationship. Participants were primarily in their late teens and early twenties, and it is possible

that some of them may not have had much experience in initiating romantic relationships. If this

is true, then the measure may say less about their recollected experiences and more about their

general intentions for beginning romantic relationships in the future. It will be important to

replicate these results in future research with participants that have verified having multiple

experiences with initiating romantic relationships.

Another potential limitation is that the romantic targets in the vignettes were artificial and

unrealistic. Indeed, it is unlikely that many people will encounter replicas of these particular

romantic targets that were described. It will therefore be necessary to make sure that these

findings apply to naturally occurring situations so as to ensure the generalizability of the results.

60
It will also be important use a more natural paradigm that permits the investigation of strategic

self-presentation and the development of rapport in initial interactions.

61
CHAPTER 6: INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 2

The goal of Study 2 was to further illuminate the roles of normal, grandiose, and

vulnerable narcissistic traits in romantic relationship initiation. Unlike Study 1, Study 2

examined the roles of these traits in actual dyadic interactions using a paradigm similar to speed-

dating. Participants were asked to engage in one-on-one interactions with several members of the

opposite sex and subsequently complete questionnaires (i.e., dating records) regarding the

interaction and their date (i.e., interaction partner). Such a paradigm allows the possibility of

addressing additional questions regarding self-presentation and the generation of rapport in

relationship initiation. To be able to address these questions effectively, however, requires a

good understanding of the data that are generated by participants in these sessions. Indeed, the

perceptions that participants have of their interactions and their dates may reflect several

different sources of influence. Although unique features of the interaction between the two

participants may contribute to the perception, it is also possible that the rating is influenced by

general characteristics of the person making the rating or general characteristics of the date that

is being rated.

The Social Relations Model

The Social Relations Model (SRM; Kenny, Kashy, & Cook, 2006) is a theoretical model

of dyadic behavior and a methodological tool that can illuminate the extent to which

interpersonal perceptions are driven by features of the perceiver, the target, or something unique

about the particular relationship between the perceiver and the target. As an example, let us

imagine that Alfredo has a date with Cecilia and that he afterwards rates how attracted he is to

Cecilia. One potential source contributing to this rating is Alfredo’s general tendency to be

attracted to all women. In SRM terminology, this is called Alfredo’s perceiver effect. It

62
represents the consistency of a person’s rating across all partners. Another potential source

contributing to this rating is Cecilia’s general tendency to be rated as attractive by all men. This

is termed the target effect in the SRM. It reflects the degree to which a person is rated similarly

by her or his partners. The final potential source contributing to Alfredo’s rating of attraction to

Cecilia is Alfredo’s unique level of attraction to Cecilia, over and above Alfredo’s general

tendency to be attracted to all women and over and above Cecilia’s general tendency to be seen

as attractive by all men. In the SRM, this is labeled the relationship effect. It reflects the unique

adjustment that one person makes to another after accounting for the perceiver and target effects.

The SRM estimates the variance in these effects for each group (i.e., dating session) and

then pools these estimates across groups. Perceiver variance assesses the extent to which people

differ in their general tendencies to provide high or low ratings across dates. Significant

perceiver variance in attraction for men would indicate that some men report being more

attracted to all of their dates whereas other men report being less attracted to all of their dates.

Target variance captures consensus among participants in their ratings of a particular date.

Significant target variance in attraction for women would indicate that some women are seen as

more attractive by all of their male partners whereas other women are seen as less attractive by

all of their male partners. Finally, substantial variance in the relationship effects would indicate

that attraction has a fundamentally dyadic component and is to some extent specific to the two

particular individuals interacting. Note that unless there are multiple indicators of the dating

record variable, the relationship variance includes error variance.

In addition to deriving estimates of variance in the perceiver, target, and relationship

(plus error) effects, the SRM also routinely generates estimates of reciprocity. Generalized

reciprocity refers to the association between individuals’ perceiver effects and their target

63
effects. In the case of attraction, a significant and negative generalized reciprocity coefficient for

men would indicate that men who report being more attracted to all of their dates tend to be rated

by those female dates as being less attractive. Dyadic reciprocity refers to the association

between a man’s relationship effect with a particular woman and that woman’s relationship

effect with the man. Thus, a positive and significant dyadic reciprocity coefficient for attraction

would indicate that a man who reports being especially attracted to a particular female date tends

to be seen as especially attractive by that date.

Narcissism and the SRM Effects

An SRM analysis of the dating record variables in Study 2 can provide important

information for the subsequent analyses. From a descriptive standpoint, the relative percentage of

variance in the dating record variables accounted for by the perceiver, target, and relationship

effects can shed insight on how much the variables reflect individual differences versus

relationship-specific phenomenon. In other words, it is possible to examine whether variability in

attraction ratings is more so a function of general characteristics of the two individuals involved

or something specific to their particular relationship. In addition, examination of the SRM

variances has practical implications for the analyses. If enough variance exists in the SRM

effects, it is possible to correlate individual-difference variables (e.g., narcissism) with them.

Correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM perceiver effects for the

dating record variables would provide information on whether participants’ levels of narcissism

are associated with their general tendencies to perceive their dates in a particular way. For

instance, a negative correlation between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the SRM perceiver

effect for attraction would indicate that participants with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness consistently report being less attracted to their dates.

64
Correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the dating

record variables, on the other hand, would provide information on whether people who are

higher in narcissism are seen by members of the opposite-sex in a certain way. A positive

correlation between Grandiose Exhibitionism and the SRM target effect for attraction, for

example, would indicate that participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism are

generally seen as more attractive.

Hypotheses Regarding the Role of Narcissism in Strategic Self-Presentation

As noted previously, because Study 2 involves actual interactions, it allows me to

evaluate the types of self-presentation that individuals with the different narcissistic traits use in

their initial encounters. In addition, I can assess whether certain narcissistic traits are perceived

to be more desirable than others. Last, I can investigate whether the type of self-presentation that

individuals engage in depends upon the impression that they have of their date. Below I present

my hypotheses and my plans for the analyses.

How do narcissists present themselves during the interactions? Given that past

research has revealed a notable connection between certain narcissistic traits related to

grandiosity and flashy styles of dress, charming facial expressions, and self-assured body

postures (Back et al., 2010), I hypothesize that individuals with higher levels of grandiose

narcissistic traits will elicit impressions of greater dominance and gregariousness from their

dates. I also predict that grandiose narcissistic traits will be associated with impressions of

increased arrogance. On the other hand, I expect that more vulnerable narcissistic traits will

generally elicit impressions of decreased warmth and dominance.

As a preliminary step in evaluating these hypotheses, I will examine the zero-order

correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the interpersonal

65
circumplex variables. Because I am primarily interested in the independent effects of grandiosity

and vulnerability, the primary analyses will use multilevel regression with these variables as

predictors. Note that separate analyses will be performed with the NPI traits as predictors and the

PNI traits as predictors.

Are narcissists considered more desirable by their dates? Because past research has

found links between grandiose narcissistic traits and popularity (Back et al., 2010), I hypothesize

that individuals with greater levels of grandiose narcissistic traits will be perceived to be more

desirable (i.e., good looking, charming, and popular) and attractive by their dates. In addition, I

predict that individuals with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits will be perceived by

their dates as less admiring towards them and less attractive.

To evaluate these hypotheses, I will first examine the zero-order correlations between the

narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for these “appeal” variables (i.e., the dates’

perceptions of the participants’ admiration towards the dates, the dates’ perceptions of the

participants’ desirability, and the dates’ attraction towards the participants). The primary

analyses will then use multilevel regression to obtain estimates of the partial relations between

the participants’ levels of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism and their dates’ perceptions of

their appeal. As before, separate analyses will be performed with the NPI traits as predictors and

the PNI traits as predictors.

Are narcissists’ self-presentational strategies dependent upon their impressions of

their dates? It is possible that there are situations in which the self-presentational strategies used

by vulnerable individuals actually work to garner more positive impressions from their dates. For

example, if such individuals perceive a date to be desirable, they may behave in ways that are

more ingratiating and reflect a positive form of submission. In this case, individuals with higher

66
levels of vulnerable traits may be seen as less dominant but also more warm and admiring. I

therefore hypothesize that the relation between participants’ vulnerable traits and their dates’

perceptions of their warmth-agreeableness and admiration will be moderated by the participants’

perceptions of the date being desirable. Note that I do not anticipate such effects for the normal

and grandiose narcissistic traits.

To evaluate these hypotheses, I will perform a series of moderated multilevel regression

analyses in which I assess whether the relations between the participants’ scores on the

vulnerable narcissism variables and their dates’ perceptions of their appeal are moderated by the

participants’ ratings of their dates’ levels of desirability. Two outcomes will be evaluated: the

dates’ perceptions of the participants’ warmth-agreeableness and the dates’ perceptions of the

participants’ admiration towards the date. Again, separate analyses will be performed with the

NPI traits as predictors and the PNI traits as predictors.

Hypotheses Regarding the Processes Linking Narcissism to Attractiveness

One of the other purposes of Study 2 is to examine how each of the narcissistic traits is

related to ratings of attraction in a speed-dating context. Back et al. (2010) were able to show

how particular behavioral cues (e.g., self-assured body postures, charming facial expressions)

emitted by people with higher levels of certain narcissistic traits drive the relations between their

traits and their partners’ ratings of their popularity. In the present work, I am interested in

whether particular interpersonal perceptions mediate the relations between participants’

narcissistic traits and their dates’ attraction towards the participants.

There are three mediational hypotheses altogether. Based on the findings of Back et al.

(2010), I hypothesize that the positive relation between participants’ grandiose narcissism and

their dates’ attraction towards them will be mediated by the dates’ perceptions of their increased

67
levels of gregariousness, dominance, arrogance, and desirability. In addition, I hypothesize that

the negative relation between participants’ vulnerable narcissism and their dates’ attraction

towards them will be mediated by the dates’ perceptions of their decreased levels of dominance,

warmth, and admiration towards the date. Finally, in those cases where participants with higher

levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits perceive their dates to be more desirable, I hypothesize that

the positive relation between participants’ vulnerable narcissism and their dates’ attraction

towards them will be mediated by the dates’ perceptions of their increased levels of warmth and

admiration towards the date.

I will be using the causal steps approach to evaluating mediation proposed by Baron and

Kenny (1986). As will be described later in more detail, two of the requirements that must be

satisfied to be able to conclude that mediation has taken place are that: (a) the independent

variable(s) is significantly related to the outcome; and that (b) the independent variable(s) is

significantly related to the mediator. If, in the previous analyses, I find that participants’ levels of

grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits are not significantly related to their dates’ attraction

towards them, or that these traits are not significantly related to their dates’ perceptions of their

interpersonal behaviors or appeal-related behaviors (i.e., dominance, gregariousness, arrogance,

warmth, admiration, and/or desirability, depending upon the mediating variables hypothesized),

then mediational analyses will not be pursued for the first and/or second hypotheses. Likewise, if

I find that the relation between participants’ levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits and their

dates’ impressions of their admiration and warmth are not significantly moderated by the

participants’ perceptions of their dates’ desirability, then mediational analyses for the third

hypothesis will not be pursued.

Hypotheses Regarding the Role of Narcissism in the Building of Rapport

68
Another set of questions concerns whether certain narcissistic traits serve to shape the

rapport-relevant qualities of the interaction. For instance, do particular narcissistic traits lead

people to disclose less in their interactions, trust their dates less, and/or feel less connected to

their dates? Moreover, do dates tend to trust people with certain narcissistic traits less and/or

perceive them to be less responsive during the interactions? Below I detail my hypotheses

regarding these “rapport” variables and discuss the analytic strategies that I plan on pursuing.

Do narcissists disclose less to their dates, trust their dates less, and/or feel less

connected to their dates? Based on previous research (Emmons, 1989), I predict that the

grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits will be associated with lower self-reported rates of

disclosure. Similarly, I predict that the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits will be

associated with lower self-reported levels of interpersonal connection. Though admittedly

speculative, it is possible that participants with higher levels of vulnerable traits will be less

likely to trust their interaction partners.

To evaluate connections between the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits and self-

reported rates of disclosure, connection, and trust, I will first examine the zero-order correlations

between the narcissism variables and the SRM perceiver effects for the disclosure, connection,

and trust variables. The primary analyses investigating these hypotheses will then use multilevel

regression to predict participants’ self-reported levels of disclosure, connection, and trust from

their own narcissism variables. Separate analyses will be performed using the NPI traits as

predictors and the PNI traits as predictors.

Are narcissists considered less responsive and trustworthy by their dates? Because

of their apparent insensitivity to people’s needs and their increased levels of disagreeableness

(Ackerman et al., 2011), I predict that participants with higher levels of grandiose and vulnerable

69
narcissistic traits will be perceived as less responsiveness by their dates. Moreover, albeit

speculative, I expect that participants with higher levels of grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic

traits will also elicit greater levels of skepticism from their partners.

To evaluate connections between the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits and

informant-reported rates of responsiveness and trust, I will first examine the zero-order

correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the responsiveness

and trust variables. I will then again use multilevel regression analyses to predict the

participants’ dates’ reports of how much they trust the participants and their perceptions of the

participants’ responsiveness from the participants’ narcissism variables. Separate analyses will

be performed with the NPI traits as predictors and the PNI traits as predictors.

Hypotheses Regarding the Role of Narcissism in the Appraisal of Initial Attraction

Finally, Study 2 aimed to further evaluate whether the narcissistic traits are connected

with differential attraction towards particular romantic partners. As in Study 1, I hypothesize that

individuals with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits will be more attracted to dates

whom they perceive to be admiring during the interaction. Moreover, I hypothesize that

individuals with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits will be more attracted to dates

whom they perceive to be desirable during the interaction. Last, I hypothesize that individuals

with higher levels of each of the narcissistic traits will be less attracted to dates whom they

perceive to be caring during the interaction.

To evaluate these hypotheses, I will perform a series of moderated multilevel regression

analyses in which the relation between participants’ levels of narcissism and their attraction

towards their dates is moderated by participants’ perceptions of their dates’ levels of admiration

towards the participants, their dates’ levels of desirability, or their dates’ levels of kindness.

70
Separate analyses will be performed with the NPI traits as predictors and the PNI traits as

predictors.

71
CHAPTER 7: METHOD FOR STUDY 2

Study 2 involved two phases. In the first phase that took place online, participants

completed the NPI and PNI. In the second phase, participants were asked to engage in five-
6
minute dates with several opposite-sex participants and subsequently complete dating records.

In the dating record, participants rated how attracted they were to their date (i.e., the Romantic

Attraction to Target Scale); how admiring, desirable, caring, dominant, submissive, cold-hearted,

arrogant, warm, and gregarious they perceived their date to be (i.e., the Partner Description

Inventory); and how much they disclosed to their dates, how responsive their dates were to them,

how close they felt to their dates, and how much they trusted their dates (i.e., the Encounter

Description Inventory).

The primary independent variables in Study 2 included the NPI traits of

Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness, and the PNI

traits of Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability. The primary dependent variables

in Study 2 included participants’ levels of romantic attraction towards their dates; participants’

perceptions of their dates’ levels of admiration, desirability, dominance, submissiveness,

arrogance, cold-heartedness, warmth, and gregariousness; participants’ self-reported rates of

disclosure during the interaction, participants’ perceptions of their dates’ responsiveness during

the interaction, participants’ perceptions of connection to their dates, and participants’ degree of

trust in their dates.

Participants

All participants were recruited through the Human Participation Research system

maintained by the psychology department at Michigan State University and received course

6
All materials used in Study 2 are contained in Appendix 2.

72
credit or extra credit for their participation. The study description on the website requested

students who were not currently involved in a romantic relationship, who had lived in the

continental United States since they were five years old (so that language barriers would not

interfere with communication on the dates), and who were between 18 and 20 years old. A total
7
of 497 participants signed up for the first phase of the study. Of these participants, 238 of them

8
completed the second phase of the study. However, the online responses for six participants had

to be dropped because they did not respond affirmatively to the question, “Did you answer these

questions honestly?” on the online questionnaire. Thus, complete data from both phases of the

study were available from 232 participants overall.

Data from the second phase of the study came from 33 groups of 6 to 10 undergraduate

college students (i.e., 16 groups of 6, 13 groups of 8, and 4 groups of 10). Each group contained

equal numbers of men and women. Based on the data set of 232 participants, participants were

18 (41.3%) years old, 19 (34.3%) years old, 20 (23.5%) years old, and 21 (0.9%) years or older

(two participants did not report their age). Participants were predominantly White (Not

Hispanic/Not Latino) (82.3%) or Black or African American (11.7%); one participant did not

provide data on her/his ethnicity. Participants reported being Single or Not Dating (78.4%),

Dating casually (13.9%), Dating exclusively (7.4%), and Engaged (0.4%); one participant did

not provide data on her/his relationship status. All participants self-identified as heterosexual.

Materials

7
This number includes participants that signed up for Study 2 but ended up participating in
Study 1.
8
Two additional participants completed the in-lab portion of the study before they completed the
online portion. Because they were privy to the study hypotheses, their online data were not used.
73
Narcissistic Personality Inventory. As in Study 1, subscales for Leadership/Authority

(11 items; α = .78, average inter-item r = .24; M = 0.56, SD = 0.27), Grandiose Exhibitionism

(10 items; α = .69, average inter-item r = .18; M = 0.41, SD = 0.24), and

Entitlement/Exploitativeness (4 items; α = .28, average inter-item r = .09; M = 0.22, SD = 0.23)

were created according to the Ackerman et al. (2011) solution. Participants’ scores for each of

these subscales were the average number of narcissistic alternatives that they endorsed, with

higher scores reflecting higher levels of the trait. Note that men and women significantly differed

in their average levels of Leadership/Authority (men: M = 0.61, SD = 0.26; women: M = 0.50,

SD = 0.26; t(238) = 3.06, p = .002), Grandiose Exhibitionism (men: M = 0.45, SD = 0.23;

women: M = 0.36, SD = 0.23; t(238) = 2.81, p = .005), and Entitlement/Exploitativeness (men: M

= 0.26, SD = 0.24; women: M = 0.18, SD = 0.20; t(238) = 3.05, p = .003).

Pathological Narcissism Inventory. As in Study 1, scales were created for the

superordinate factors of Narcissistic Grandiosity (18 items; α = .84, average inter-item r = .22; M

= 2.93, SD = 0.67) and Narcissistic Vulnerability (34 items; α = .94, average inter-item r = .30;

M = 2.28, SD = 0.77). Participants’ scores were the mean of their responses for each scale, with

higher scores indicating higher levels of the trait. Note that although men and women

significantly differed in their average levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity (men: M = 3.10, SD =

0.62; women: M = 2.76, SD = 0.66; t(238) = 4.10, p < .001), they did not significantly differ in

their average levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability (men: M = 2.21, SD = 0.68; women: M = 2.35,

SD = 0.83; t(238) = -1.40, p = .164).

Romantic Attraction to Target. Participants’ degree of romantic attraction towards each

date with whom they interacted during the lab session was assessed with four questions: (a) “Do

you like this person?”; (b) “Are you attracted to this person?”; (c) “Would you like to date this

74
person?”; and (d) “Do you think this person is desirable?” Participants responded to each

question on a 7-point scale that ranged from 1 (Definitely no) to 7 (Definitely yes). Each

participant’s score for Romantic Attraction was the mean of her or his responses to these four

questions, with higher scores indicating greater levels of attraction (M = 3.90, SD = 1.34; α = .89,

average inter-item r = .66).

Encounter Description Inventory. This measure used and adapted items from Eastwick

and Finkel (2008), Finkel, Eastwick, and Matthews (2007), and Laurenceau et al. (1998) to

describe several aspects of the dyadic interactions during the lab session. These aspects included

Interpersonal Connection, Trust, Disclosure, and Perceived Responsiveness. Participants were

asked to provide their responses to the Interpersonal Connection and Trust items on 7-point

scales that ranged from 1 (Definitely no) to 7 (Definitely yes). Interpersonal Connection assesses

the degree to which participants felt an initial bond with their date (M = 3.49, SD = 1.28; α = .84,

average inter-item r = .64). It was measured with three items (e.g., “Did you feel a real

connection?” and “Were there sparks between you and your partner?”). Trust measured how

much participants were unsuspicious of their dates and was assessed with a single item (i.e.,

“Were you skeptical of your interaction partner?”; reverse scored; M = 5.45, SD = 1.51).

Participants provided responses to the Disclosure and Perceived Responsiveness items on

7-point scales that ranged from 1 (Very Little) to 7 (A Great Deal). Disclosure assessed

participants’ perceptions of their own rate of disclosure during the interaction and was measured

with a single item (i.e., “I disclosed…”; M = 4.10, SD = 1.40). Perceived Responsiveness

measured the degree to which participants perceived their dates to be accepting and responsive to

their disclosure. It was assessed with two items (i.e., “How much did your partner understand

you?” and “Did the other person see you as acceptable?”; M = 5.05, SD = 1.11; α = .77, inter-

75
item r = .64). Participants’ scores for the Interpersonal Connection and Perceived

Responsiveness scales were computed as the mean of their responses.

Partner Description Inventory. This 21-item measure consisted of a list of personality

characteristics and was used to assess how dates were perceived during the lab interactions.

Participants were asked to rate how much each characteristic was descriptive of their date on a 7-

point scale that ranged from 1 (Extremely Inaccurate) to 5 (Extremely Accurate). One part of this

inventory used some of the language reported in Campbell (1999) to assess how well

characteristics associated with being an admiring, desirable, and caring person described

participants’ dates. Thus, participants were asked to rate how much they believed their date was

Admiring (i.e., “Admires me,” “Interested in me,” and “Loves my company”; 3 items, M = 3.69,

SD = 1.13; α = .87, average inter-item r = .69), Desirable (i.e., “Good looking,” “Charming,” and

“Popular”; 3 items, M = 4.23, SD = 1.17; α = .80, average inter-item r = .57), and Caring (i.e.,

“Friendly,” “Caring,” and “Compassionate”; 3 items, M = 5.10, SD = 0.97; α = .82, average

inter-item r = .61).

A second part of this inventory included items from the Interpersonal Adjective Scale,

Revised (IAS-R; Wiggins, Trapnell, & Phillips, 1988) to assess particular octants of the

interpersonal circumplex (see Figure 5). To save time and lessen the participants’ burden, only

adjectives from those octants relevant for the hypotheses and/or that are necessary to create the

fundamental circumplex dimensions of agency and communion were used. Therefore, six octants

were assessed: Assured-Dominant (PA), Cold-Hearted (DE), Unassured-Submissive (HI),

Arrogant-Calculating (BC), Warm-Agreeable (LM), and Gregarious-Extraverted (NO). To

further save time, the two adjectives from each of these octants that were reported in Wiggins et

al. (1988) to exhibit the highest loadings on their corresponding octant were selected for use in

76
this study. Six scales were computed: Assured-Dominant (i.e., “Assertive” and “Dominant”; M =

3.65, SD = 1.25; α = .71, inter-item r = .55), Cold-Hearted (i.e., “Unsympathetic” and

“Warmthless”; M = 2.24, SD = 0.99; α = .54, inter-item r = .37), Unassured-Submissive (i.e.,

“Unaggressive” and “Shy”; M = 3.85, SD = 1.25; α = .40, inter-item r = .25), Arrogant-

Calculating (i.e., “Cunning” and “Sly”; M = 2.82, SD = 1.09; α = .57, inter-item r = .40), Warm-

Agreeable (i.e., “Gentlehearted” and “Tenderhearted”; M = 4.91, SD = 1.09; α = .83, inter-item r

= .71), and Gregarious-Extraverted (i.e., “Friendly” and “Outgoing”; M = 5.21, SD = 1.10; α =

.70, inter-item r = .56). Participants’ scores for each scale were the mean of their responses.

Procedure

Participants completed the NPI and the PNI online and then signed up for the in-lab

session. The in-lab sessions involved the participation of six to ten participants and took place in

a medium-sized room in the Psychology Building. To run a session, it was required that there

were an equal number of men and women and that there was at least three of each gender

present. In the event that not enough participants were available to conduct the session (or

alternatively, that there were extra participants), participants were offered the opportunity to

participate in Study 1 for credit.

The room was set up such that five small tables were evenly spread out along the

perimeter of the room and one longer table was situated in the center of the room. Each of the

tables was adorned with a red table cloth. As participants arrived to the study, they were each

given a bottle of water and were asked to remain quiet until the study began. Once all of the

participants arrived (or it was five minutes past when the study was scheduled to begin), the

experimenter asked participants if any of them knew each other. In the event that any men knew

77
any of the women or any women knew any of the men, one of them was randomly chosen to

participate in Study 1.

Once these preliminary tasks were completed, the experimenter provided participants

with informed consent. After this, the experimenter provided each participant with an

identification tag and a clipboard of dating records. The identification-tags for each participant

had different colors and unique numbers. The clipboard contained several dating records in

which the Romantic Attraction to Target Scale, the Encounter Description Inventory, and the

Partner Description Inventory were embedded.

At this point, the experimenter read the instructions to the participants and explained the

general procedure for the study (see Appendix 3 for the entire script). After explaining the study,

the experimenter asked all of the women (or men) to take a seat at one of the tables along the

perimeter of the room. The experimenter then randomly assigned each of the men (or women) to

take a seat with one of the women (or men). Note that the status of men or women sitting down

or rotating during the study was randomly determined prior to the actual session (see Finkel &

Eastwick, 2009). Once all of the participants had taken a seat, the experimenter told participants:

You will start your first date momentarily. I will warn you when you have one minute

left. I will then ring a bell to let you know to stop your date and to fill out the

questionnaire. I do have one request: Please stop your date within a second or two of

hearing the bell; this is the only way to keep the event on track. While you are actually

interacting with other people, we suggest that you put the clipboard on the floor so it does

not become a distraction. OK, please go ahead and start your first date.

After these five minutes transpired, the experimenter told the participants to stop the

interaction and asked the rotating gender to take a seat at the long table in the center. At this

78
time, participants were told to spend the next five minutes completing a dating record based on

the date with whom they just interacted. After all the participants had completed their dating

records, the rotating gender was asked to rotate clockwise to the next table along the room

perimeter. This cycle of rotating, having a date, and completing a dating record continued until

all the participants had interacted with all other participants of the opposite-sex. Once the session

was over, the experimenter collected all participants’ dating records. Participants were then

thanked and fully debriefed. Note that there was no implemented follow-up as would normally

occur in a real speed-dating event.

79
CHAPTER 8: RESULTS FOR STUDY 2

Preliminary Analyses of the Social Interaction Data

The hypotheses in Study 2 were evaluated using the dating record data obtained from the

speed-dating sessions. As noted in my discussion of the SRM, these ratings reflect multiple

sources of influence, including perceiver effects, target effects, and relationship effects. The

speed-dating design in which each individual interacts with and then rates multiple dates allows

me to tease apart these multiple sources of influence and thereby gain a better understanding of

the dating record variables used in the present research.

The SRM (Kenny et al., 2006) for an asymmetric block design was used to partition

variance in individuals’ social interaction ratings. A block design occurs when one subgroup of

participants (e.g., men) interacts one-on-one with another subgroup of participants (e.g.,

women); it is termed “asymmetric” when the two subgroups can be distinguished on some

characteristic (e.g., gender). Table 24 provides an illustration of the asymmetric block design

used in the present study.

Estimates of the SRM parameters (e.g., perceiver, target, and relationship variance;

reciprocity correlations) were obtained from the program, BLOCKO (Kenny, 1998). Because

BLOCKO does not permit missing data, missing data were mean-imputed (n = 8 for each of the

NPI subscales; n = 10 for PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity; n = 9 for PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability;

and n = 1 for disclosure). In those cases where significant gender differences were observed in

the variable means (i.e., each of the NPI subscales, PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity, and disclosure),

the missing value was imputed with the mean of the variable for the appropriate gender.

Application of the SRM to the dating record variables. Table 25 presents the SRM

variance partitioning for the dating record variables. This table includes the total amount of

80
variance for each variable as well as the relative percentage of variance in the dating record

variable accounted for by the perceiver, target, and relationship (plus error) effects. Note that

estimates of these values are provided for men and women separately. To facilitate comparison,

values for men’s ratings of women and women’s ratings of men are presented one right after the

other. Moreover, only the perceiver and target variances were tested for statistical significance

given that the relationship variances include error.

All of the perceiver variances for the dating record variables were statistically significant.

Participants in the speed-dating sessions were therefore at least somewhat consistent in their

responses across the different dates. For example, in Table 25, close to half of the variance in

participants’ self-reported degree of disclosure was due to their perceiver effects. Thus, some

participants reported disclosing a great deal to all of their dates, but others reported little such

disclosure. For the most part, the relative percentage of variance accounted for by the perceiver

effects for the dating record variables was similar for men and women.

Table 25 also shows that there was significant and substantial target variance for

participants’ ratings of attraction. This indicates that participants within a session tended to agree

with one other on which dates they found attractive. There was also substantial consensus among

participants on who they found to be desirable, dominant, submissive, and gregarious. To a lesser

extent, participants also agreed on who they found to be easy to connect with, who was

responsive to their needs, who was kind, and who was admiring. As Table 25 shows, not all of

the target variances for the dating record variables were statistically significant, thus indicating a

lack of consistency in participants’ ratings of a target for some variables. Indeed, there was little

consensus for cold-heartedness. Again, generally speaking, these relative percentages were

similar for men and women.

81
Table 26 displays the reciprocity correlations for the dating record variables. As Table 26

shows, there was no evidence of generalized reciprocity. Thus, participants’ general perceptions

of their dates were not significantly connected to their dates’ general perceptions of them. In

contrast, there was evidence of significant dyadic reciprocity for romantic attraction,

interpersonal connection, perceived responsiveness, perceived admiration, perceived desirability,

perceived kindness, and perceived warmth-agreeableness. Thus, if the man was especially

attracted to a particular woman, that woman also reported being especially attracted to the man.

This tendency for participants to reciprocate levels of attraction and connection shows that they

were actively engaged during the speed-dating sessions; thus, these findings illustrate the

experimental realism of the paradigm.

Conclusions from SRM analysis. Taken together, these results provide important

baseline information for the study. The findings regarding significant dyadic reciprocity for

variables such as attraction and connection, for instance, show that participants were taking the

task seriously. Moreover, because all of the dating record variables possess a substantial degree

of perceiver variance, it will be possible to examine associations between the narcissism

variables and the perceiver effects to determine whether narcissists see potential dating partners

in a similar way. Further, many of the dating record variables have significant target variance.

Thus, it will be possible to examine whether narcissists are seen in similar ways by potential

dating partners.

Unfortunately, it will not be possible to evaluate whether participants’ levels of the

narcissistic traits are associated with their dates’ perceptions of their cold-heartedness. Related to

this, there are some variables for which the relative percentage of variance accounted for by the

target effects is small for men, or women, or both (i.e., trust, arrogant-calculating, and warm-

82
agreeable). Thus, depending on the dating record variable, there may be some correlations that

cannot be computed due to insufficient variance in the target effects.

Plan of Analysis for Testing Hypotheses

The hypotheses for Study 2 concerned the role of narcissism in self-presentation, the

development of rapport, and the appraisal of initial attraction. As noted in the introduction to

Study 2, the analyses used to test these hypotheses involved some combination of zero-order

correlations and multilevel regression. The zero-order correlations between the narcissism

variables and the SRM perceiver and/or target effects were obtained from the program,

BLOCKO (Kenny, 1998) and are disattenuated for measurement error; moreover, separate

correlations were obtained for men and women and are presented side by side in the

corresponding tables. For each hypothesis that uses both zero-order correlations and multilevel

regression, I present the zero-order correlations first before proceeding to present the results for

the multilevel regression analyses.

Multilevel Modeling (MLM) was used to account for the hierarchical structure of the data

in the primary analyses. The random effects structure was specified to be consistent with the

SRM. Accordingly, separate perceiver and target variances were estimated for men and women.

Moreover, separate covariances between the perceiver and target effects for men and women

were specified to index their corresponding generalized reciprocity coefficients. The relationship

variances for men and women were estimated as residuals in the models and the covariance

estimated between them was used to index dyadic reciprocity. Although a random component for

the group was also initially included in the models, its inclusion was found to often produce non-

positive definite matrices; this was due to the minimal variance across the groups on the outcome

variables (this is sensible given that the groups were randomly formed and the levels of the

83
variables were more or less randomly distributed across members). As a result, the random

component for the group was removed from all the models. Unless otherwise noted, this was the

random structure that was specified across each of the subsequent analyses using MLM. Tests of

the hypotheses concerned the fixed effects in the models.

What Self-Presentational Strategies do Narcissists Generally Use on their Dates?

The first set of hypotheses concerned the self-presentational strategies that narcissists use

when initially interacting with a member of the opposite-sex in a romantic context. Due to the

circumplex structure of the variables examined for this particular set of hypotheses, these

analyses focused on what octant(s) of the interpersonal circle appeared to best describe the self-

presentational interpersonal style of narcissists. Indeed, if a narcissistic dimension is associated

with one octant of the circle, it will likely also be associated with other octants in close

proximity. Consequently, what is important here is the area of the interpersonal circle that is best

predicted by the narcissistic dimension.

Table 27 presents the zero-order correlations between the narcissism variables. As can be

seen, all of the NPI subscales were moderately correlated with each other, with the strongest

association occurring between Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism. Likewise, the

two PNI scales were moderately correlated. Whereas PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity was

moderately correlated with each of the NPI subscales, the PNI Narcissistic Vulnerability scale

was only modestly associated with the NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism and NPI

Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscales.

Do narcissists express the same interpersonal behaviors across their dates?: Zero-

order correlations with SRM target effects. Table 28 presents the zero-order correlations

between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the interpersonal circumplex

84
measures. These correlations assess whether individuals with higher levels of narcissistic traits
9
tended to be seen in similar ways by their dating partners. For instance, are people with higher

levels of grandiose narcissistic traits seen as more arrogant?

As can be seen in Table 28, participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were

seen by their dates as more assured-dominant. Moreover, participants with higher levels of

grandiose narcissistic traits tended to be perceived as more assured-dominant. In addition, men

with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits were seen by their dates as more gregarious-

extraverted. With the exception of a negative correlation between NPI

Entitlement/Exploitativeness and unassured-submissiveness for women, the vulnerable

narcissistic traits were unrelated to perceptions of interpersonal behavior.

Do narcissists express the same interpersonal behaviors across their dates?:

Multilevel regression analyses. A series of MLM analyses were conducted to evaluate how

individuals with higher levels of the narcissism dimensions were perceived by their dates during

the speed-dating interactions. Each analysis included the following predictors: (a) the

participants’ gender (effect coded such that men = 1 and women = -1), the participants’ scores on

each of the narcissism scales (either all three NPI subscales or the two PNI scales), and the

interactions between the narcissism scales (either the NPI subscales or the PNI scales) and

participant gender to predict the participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ interpersonal

behaviors. All continuous predictor variables were grand-mean centered prior to forming the

interaction terms. Five analyses were performed for each set of narcissism traits, one for each

9
Note that there was insufficient target variance to compute correlations between the narcissism
variables and the SRM target effects for cold-heartedness. Likewise, there was insufficient target
variance in the arrogant-calculating variable for women and insufficient target variance in the
warm-agreeable variable for men; therefore, correlations were not estimated with these particular
target effects.
85
interpersonal octant as an outcome (i.e., Assured-Dominant, Arrogant-Calculating, Unassured-

Submissive, Warm-Agreeable, and Gregarious-Extraverted).

Table 29 shows results for the MLM analyses using the three NPI traits to predict

participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ interpersonal behaviors, with each column

displaying a separate analysis. Participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were seen

by their dates as more assured-dominant and more gregarious-extraverted. Table 29 shows,

however, that there was a significant interaction between Leadership/Authority and participant

gender for perceptions of gregarious-extraversion. To break down this interaction, simple slopes

for the effect of participants’ Leadership/Authority on their dates’ perceptions of gregarious-

extraversion were computed separately for men and women. The simple slope of

Leadership/Authority on gregarious-extraversion for women was b = 0.03, SE = .26, p = .914. In

contrast, the simple slope for men was b = 0.86, SE = .33, p = .01. Therefore, whereas men with

higher levels of Leadership/Authority were seen as more gregarious-extraverted and assured-

dominant in these social interactions, women with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were

only seen as more assured-dominant.

Consistent with predictions, participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism

were seen by their dates as being more assured-dominant, more arrogant-calculating, more

gregarious-extraverted, and less unassured-submissive. However, in contrast to what was

hypothesized, Table 29 shows that Entitlement/Exploitativeness was not significantly related to

dates’ perceptions of any of the interpersonal traits.

Table 30 shows results for the MLM analyses using the two PNI traits to predict

participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ interpersonal behaviors. As hypothesized,

participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were perceived by their dates to be

86
more gregarious-extraverted during the interactions. However, this effect was qualified by a

significant interaction between Narcissistic Grandiosity and participant gender. To break this

interaction down, simple slopes for the effect of Narcissistic Grandiosity on perceptions of

gregarious-extraversion were computed separately for men and women. The simple slope of

Narcissistic Grandiosity on perceptions of gregarious-extraversion for women was b = -0.02, SE

= .11, p = .859. The simple slope for men was b = 0.58, SE = .13, p < .001. Therefore, it appears

that only men with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were perceived by their dates to be

more gregarious-extraverted.

Although Narcissistic Grandiosity was not significantly related to dates’ perceptions of

any other interpersonal behaviors, there was a significant interaction between Narcissistic

Grandiosity and participant gender in the prediction of how warm-agreeable participants were

perceived to be during the interactions. Simple slopes of Narcissistic Grandiosity on perceptions

of warmth-agreeableness were again estimated separately for men and women. The simple slope

of Narcissistic Grandiosity for women was b = -0.06, SE = .09, p = .526. The simple slope for

men was b = 0.24, SE = .09, p = .009. Thus, only men with higher levels of Narcissistic

Grandiosity were perceived by their dates to be more warm-agreeable during the interactions.

Unlike Narcissistic Grandiosity, Narcissistic Vulnerability was not significantly related to

dates’ perceptions of any of the interpersonal behaviors. However, there was a marginal

tendency for participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability to be perceived by their

dates as less gregarious-extraverted. Thus, at the very most, participants with higher levels of

Narcissistic Vulnerability were perceived to be slightly more aloof and introverted.

Summary of analyses using participants’ narcissism to predict their dates’

perceptions of their interpersonal behavior. The grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits

87
were predicted to be associated with different self-presentational strategies. Participants with

higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits were hypothesized to behave in a more dominant,

gregarious, and arrogant manner. There was reasonable support for this hypothesis. However,

evidence did not suggest that Narcissistic Grandiosity is linked to interpersonal behaviors in the

arrogant-calculating and assured-dominant octants. In addition, participants with higher levels of

vulnerable narcissistic traits were hypothesized to behave in a more submissive and cold manner.

There was no support for this hypothesis.

How Appealing are Narcissists to their Dates?

In addition to predicting general interpersonal styles of presentation, this research

evaluated whether individuals with higher levels of particular narcissistic dimensions are

perceived to be more or less attractive during the interactions. Furthermore, this research

evaluated whether the narcissistic dimensions are differentially associated with perceptions of

admiration and desirability from dates.

Are narcissists considered appealing by their dates?: Zero-order correlations

between the narcissism variables and SRM target effects. Table 31 presents the zero-order

correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the appeal variables.

These correlations assess whether participants’ narcissistic traits are associated with a tendency
10
for them to be perceived by their dates as more appealing. For instance, are people with higher

levels of grandiose narcissistic traits considered more attractive by all of their dates?

10
Because of insufficient variance in the SRM target effects for the disclosure and trust
variables in women, correlations were not estimated between women’s scores on the narcissism
measures and their SRM target effects for these variables. Likewise, because of insufficient
variance in the SRM target effect for the admiring variable in men, correlations were not
estimated between men’s scores on the narcissism measures and this variable.
88
As seen in Table 31, men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were perceived by

their dates to be more desirable and attractive. Moreover, participants with higher levels of the

grandiose narcissistic traits tended to be perceived as more desirable and attractive as well.

However, the vulnerable narcissistic traits were unrelated to the SRM target effects for the

appeal variables.

Are narcissists considered appealing by their dates?: Multilevel regression analyses.

Multilevel regression analyses using the same set of predictors from the previous section were

pursued. The outcomes in these analyses included: (a) how much the dates perceived the

participants admired them; (b) how much the dates perceived the participants to be desirable; and

(c) how attracted the dates were to the participants.

Table 32 shows the results for the MLM analyses using the NPI traits as predictors. In

contrast to what was hypothesized, participants with greater levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness were not perceived by their dates to be less admiring during the

interactions. Instead, as Table 32 shows, there was a significant interaction between

Leadership/Authority and participant gender on perceived admiration, as well as a marginally

significant interaction between Grandiose Exhibitionism and participant gender on perceived

admiration. To break these interactions down, simple slopes for the effects of

Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism on perceived admiration were estimated

separately for men and women.

The simple slope of Leadership/Authority on perceived admiration for women was b = -

0.24, SE = .19, p = .211; the simple slope of Leadership/Authority on perceived admiration for

men was b = 0.39, SE = .22, p = .084. Moreover, the simple slope of Grandiose Exhibitionism on

perceived admiration for women was b = -0.25, SE = .22, p = .256, and the simple slope of

89
Grandiose Exhibitionism on perceived admiration for men was b = 0.38, SE = .23, p = .11. The

simple slopes suggest that whereas women with higher levels of Leadership/Authority and

Grandiose Exhibitionism tended to be perceived by their dates as less admiring, men with higher

levels of Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their dates as

more admiring. Because none of the slopes were significantly different from zero, however,

these interpretations should be treated with caution.

Consistent with predictions, Table 32 shows that participants with higher levels of

Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their dates to be more desirable. Moreover, there

was a marginally significant interaction between Leadership/Authority and participant gender in

the prediction of participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ desirability. To break this

interaction down, simple slopes for the effect of Leadership/Authority on perceived desirability

were computed separately for men and women. The simple slope of Leadership/Authority for

women was b = -0.07, SE = .30, p = .815. The simple slope for men was b = 0.85, SE = .37, p =

.024. Thus, it appears that only men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were perceived

by their dates to be more desirable.

Also in line with expectations, Table 32 shows that participants with higher levels of

Grandiose Exhibitionism were considered to be more attractive by their dates. However,

Entitlement/Exploitativeness was unrelated to ratings of attractiveness.

Table 33 shows results for the MLM analyses with the PNI traits as predictors. As shown,

neither Narcissistic Grandiosity nor Narcissistic Vulnerability significantly predicted dates’

perceptions of participants’ admiration towards the dates. However, there was a significant

interaction between Narcissistic Grandiosity and gender in the prediction of dates’ perceptions of

participants’ admiration. The simple slope of Narcissistic Grandiosity for women was b = -0.14,

90
SE = .08, p = .083. The simple slope for men was b = 0.11, SE = .08, p = .204. These slope

coefficients suggest that whereas men with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity tended to be

perceived as more admiring by their dates, women with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity

were perceived to be less admiring. Note that although these effects trended in opposite

directions, neither slope was significantly different from zero.

As hypothesized, participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were

perceived by their dates to be more desirable. However, this effect was qualified by a significant

interaction between Narcissistic Grandiosity and participant gender. The simple slope for women

was b = 0.06, SE = .13, p = .656. In contrast, the simple slope for men was b = 0.51, SE = .14, p

< .001. Thus, only men with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were perceived by their

dates to be more desirable.

Also consistent with hypotheses, Table 33 shows that participants with higher levels of

Narcissistic Grandiosity were considered to be more attractive by their dates. In contrast,

Narcissistic Vulnerability was unrelated to ratings of attractiveness.

Summary of analyses using narcissism to predict appeal. Participants with higher

levels of grandiose narcissistic traits were predicted to be perceived as more desirable and

attractive by their dates. These hypotheses were largely supported in the current study. In

addition, participants with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits were predicted to be

perceived as less admiring by their dates and to elicit lower ratings of attraction. There was no

support for these hypotheses.

Are Narcissists’ Self-Presentational Strategies Conditional?

Another set of hypotheses in Study 2 focused on whether vulnerable narcissists’ use of

particular self-presentational strategies is dependent upon their impressions of their date. I

91
hypothesized that vulnerable individuals would be perceived by their dates to be more admiring

and more warm when the vulnerable individuals perceived their dates to be desirable. These

increased levels of admiration and warmth were then predicted to elicit higher ratings of

attraction from their dates.

A series of MLM analyses were conducted to evaluate whether the relations between

participants’ levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits and their dates’ perceptions of their

admiration and warmth-agreeableness are moderated by the degree to which participants

perceived their dates to be desirable. Note that the parameters of primary interest are the

interactions between the narcissism scales and the variable used to index participants’

perceptions of their dates’ levels of desirability (i.e., perceived desirability).

Table 34 shows the results of an analysis using the NPI traits to predict the participants’

dates’ perceptions of the participants’ warmth-agreeableness. As can be seen, the relations

between participants’ NPI traits and their dates’ perceptions of their warmth-agreeableness did

not depend upon how desirable participants perceived their dates to be.

Table 35 shows the results of an analysis using the NPI traits to predict the participants’

dates’ perceptions of the participants’ admiration towards the date. In contrast to what was

hypothesized, there was no significant interaction between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and

perceived desirability. Nevertheless, there was a significant two-way interaction between

Grandiose Exhibitionism and perceived desirability. To break this interaction down, simple

slopes for the effect of Grandiose Exhibitionism on dates’ perceptions of the participants’

admiration were computed at one standard deviation above and below the mean for perceived

desirability. The simple slope at low levels of perceived desirability was b = -0.12, SE = .23, p =

.608. The simple slope at high levels of perceived desirability was b = 0.61, SE = .22, p = .006.

92
This finding indicates that whereas participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism

were perceived as more admiring by their dates when they perceived their dates to be more

desirable, participants’ levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were unrelated to their dates’

perceptions of their admiration when they perceived their dates to be less desirable.

Table 36 shows the results of an analysis using the PNI traits to predict the participants’

dates’ perceptions of the participants’ warmth-agreeableness. There were no significant

interactions between the PNI dimensions and perceived desirability. However, there was a

marginally significant three-way interaction between Narcissistic Grandiosity, participant

gender, and perceived desirability. To break down this three-way interaction, simple slopes for

the two-way interactions between Narcissistic Grandiosity and perceived desirability were

computed separately for men and women. The simple slope for women was b = 0.06, SE = .05, p

= .27. The simple slope for men was b = -0.13, SE = .08, p = .108. Although the parameter

estimates for the two-way interaction terms for men and women marginally differed, neither was

significantly different from zero; thus, these interactions were not broken down further.

Table 37 shows the results of an analysis using the PNI traits to predict the participants’

dates’ perceptions of the participants’ admiration towards the date. As Table 37 shows, there

were no significant interactions between the PNI traits and perceived desirability.

Summary of analyses examining moderation of the relations between narcissism and

self-presentation. Broadly speaking, the hypothesis that more vulnerable individuals would be

perceived as more warm and admiring by their dates when they perceived their dates to be more

desirable was not supported. Because there was no evidence for this hypothesis, the proposed

additional analyses involving mediation were not pursued. Instead, the results revealed that

93
individuals with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their dates to be

more admiring when they perceived their dates to be more desirable.

Why is Grandiose Narcissism Related to Enhanced Attraction?

This next set of analyses evaluated the mediational process hypothesized to account for

the relations between the grandiose narcissism dimensions and ratings of attraction. I expected

that participants’ levels of grandiose narcissism would lead their dates to perceive higher levels

of assured-dominance, gregarious-extraversion, arrogance-calculation, and desirability, and

thereby lead their dates to perceive them as more attractive. Before performing the mediational

analyses, zero-order correlations between the proposed mediators and outcome were first

inspected to evaluate whether the mediators were sufficiently distinct from the outcome and

whether the mediators were sufficiently distinct from one another.

Zero-order correlations between the proposed mediators and outcome. Table 38

presents the zero-order correlations between the variables. As would be expected given the

circumplex structure of the IAS-R, ratings of gregarious-extraversion, assured-dominance, and

arrogance-calculation were all correlated with one another. To prevent redundancy in the

analyses, I chose to focus on perceptions of gregarious-extraversion as a mediator given that

previous analyses revealed relations with this variable were common to both measures of

grandiose narcissism (see Tables 29 and 30).

Table 38 shows that dates’ ratings of participants’ levels of gregarious-extraversion and

desirability were both strongly associated with dates’ ratings of attraction to the participants.

Nevertheless, dates’ ratings of participants’ desirability were so strongly related to dates’ ratings

of attraction to the participants that a mediational analysis with these two variables would

provide little meaningful information. Accordingly, mediational analyses were only pursued with

94
gregarious-extraversion as a mediator of the relation between participants’ levels of grandiose

narcissism and their dates’ ratings of attraction.

Baron and Kenny (1986) steps for mediation. Baron and Kenny (1986) outlined a

causal steps approach to evaluating mediation that has been widely adopted in the social

sciences. According to these authors, a series of conditions must be met to claim that mediation

has taken place. First, the independent variable must significantly predict the outcome variable

(i.e., coefficient c in Panel A in Figure 6; the “total effect”). The logic behind this is that it does

not make sense to speak of mediation if there is not first a path to be mediated (MacKinnon,

2008). Second, the independent variable must significantly predict the mediating variable (i.e.,

coefficient a in Panel B in Figure 6). Third, the mediating variable must significantly predict the

outcome variable while controlling for the independent variable (i.e., coefficient b in Panel B in

Figure 6). Finally, for there to be complete mediation, the regression coefficient for the

independent variable obtained from regressing the outcome variable on the independent variable

and the mediating variable must be near zero (i.e., coefficient c′ in Panel B in Figure 6; the

“direct effect”).

In many cases, researchers are interested in the size of the indirect effect and whether it is

statistically different from zero. This is computed as either the difference in the total and direct

effects (i.e., c - c′) or the product of the a and b paths (i.e., ab). In the multiple regression

context, these two methods of computing the indirect effect are algebraically equivalent.

However, in the context of multilevel modeling, they are not, and methodologists report using

the ab product as the estimate of the indirect effect (MacKinnon, 2008).

95
The Sobel (1982) test is used to test the statistical significance of the indirect effect. The

standard error for the product of paths a and b is given by the following equation (MacKinnon,

2008; p. 52),
2 2 2 2
sab = square root of a sb + b sa ,

where sa is the standard error of the a path and sb is the standard error of the b path. The quotient

of these values (i.e., ab/sab) is approximately distributed as a standard normal score.

It should be noted that the topic of mediation is considerably more complex when

considered within the context of a MLM. This is because mediation can take place across

different levels (e.g., an upper-level independent variable predicting a lower-level mediator that

predicts a lower-level outcome) and the paths between lower-level variables (e.g., the path

connecting a lower-level mediator to a lower-level outcome) can have random effects.

Methodologists are just beginning to grapple with this complexity and offer solutions (see Bauer,

Preacher, & Gil, 2006; Kenny, Korchmaros, & Bolger, 2003; Krull & MacKinnon, 2001).

One solution proposed by Krull and MacKinnon (1999) involves the estimation of three

MLM equations that are analogous to those used in single-level mediation for non-hierarchical

data (see the equations presented in Figure 6). By estimating these three equations with a

properly specified random effects structure, researchers are able to derive appropriate standard

errors of the a and b parameters. These standard errors can then be used to derive an appropriate

standard error for the indirect effect using the Sobel (1982) test. Note that a restriction of this

method for multilevel mediation is that it assumes that the b parameter is fixed across the upper-

level units (see Bauer et al., 2006). Figure 7 shows the general mediational model that was

evaluated in the subsequent analyses.

96
Mediational hypothesis with NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism. To estimate path c, a MLM

was specified in which participants’ dates’ attraction to the participants was predicted to be a

function of the participants’ levels of Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and

Entitlement/Exploitativeness. Participants’ levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were found to

significantly predict their dates’ levels of attraction towards them (b = 0.67, SE = .30, p = .026).

The first step in the Baron and Kenny (1986) causal steps approach was therefore satisfied.

To estimate path a, a MLM was specified in which participants’ dates’ perception of the

participants’ gregarious-extraversion was predicted to be a function of the participants’ levels of

Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness. Participants’

levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism significantly predicted their dates’ perceptions of their

gregarious-extraversion (b = 0.46, SE = .23, p = .048). Consequently, the second step in the

Baron and Kenny (1986) causal steps approach was also satisfied.

To estimate paths b and c′, a MLM was specified in which participants’ dates’ attraction

to the participants was predicted to be a function of the participants’ levels of

Leadership/Authority, Grandiose Exhibitionism, and Entitlement/Exploitativeness, as well as

participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ gregarious-extraversion. Path b in this model

was significant (b = 0.56, SE = .03, p < .001). This finding therefore satisfied the third step in

Baron and Kenny’s (1986) causal steps approach. In addition, path c′ was non-significant (b =

0.39, SE = .23, p = .094). According to Baron and Kenny (1986), there is consequently evidence

for mediation.

To test the significance of the indirect effect, the ab product term was first formed by

multiplying the values obtained from paths a and b (i.e., ab = 0.26). The standard error of the

mediated effect was derived using the formula for the Sobel test presented earlier (sab = 0.13).

97
The quotient of these values resulted in a z-obtained of 1.98. There was thus evidence that

participants’ dates’ perceptions of the participants’ gregarious-extraversion mediated the relation

between participants’ levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism and their dates’ attraction towards them.

Mediational hypothesis with PNI Narcissistic Grandiosity. To estimate path c, a MLM

was specified in which participants’ dates’ attraction to the participants was predicted to be a

function of the participants’ levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability.

Participants’ levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were found to significantly predict their dates’

levels of attraction towards them (b = 0.25, SE = .11, p = .018). The first step in the Baron and

Kenny (1986) causal steps approach was therefore satisfied.

To estimate path a, a MLM was specified in which participants’ dates’ perception of the

participants’ gregarious-extraversion was predicted to be a function of the participants’ levels of

Narcissistic Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability. Participants’ levels of Narcissistic

Grandiosity significantly predicted their dates’ perceptions of their gregarious-extraversion (b =

0.19, SE = .08, p = .022). Consequently, the second step in the Baron and Kenny (1986) causal

steps approach was also satisfied.

To estimate paths b and c′, a MLM was specified in which participants’ dates’ attraction

to the participants was predicted to be a function of the participants’ levels of Narcissistic

Grandiosity and Narcissistic Vulnerability, as well as the participants’ dates’ perceptions of the

participants’ gregarious-extraversion. Path b in this model was significant (b = 0.56, SE = .03, p

< .001). This therefore satisfied the third step in Baron and Kenny’s (1986) causal steps

approach. In addition, path c′ was non-significant (b = 0.15, SE = .08, p = .078). According to

Baron and Kenny (1986), there is consequently evidence for mediation.

98
To test the significance of the indirect effect, the ab product term was first formed by

multiplying the values obtained from paths a and b (i.e., ab = 0.11). The standard error of the

mediated effect was derived using the Sobel test (sab = 0.05). The quotient of these values

resulted in a z-obtained of 2.29. There was therefore evidence that participants’ dates’

perceptions of the participants’ gregarious-extraversion mediated the relation between

participants’ levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity and their dates’ attraction towards them.

Summary of mediational analyses. The hypothesis that the expression of interpersonal

behaviors related to gregarious-extraversion would mediate the connection between grandiose

narcissistic traits and ratings of attraction was supported in this research. Indeed, both measures

of grandiose narcissism provided converging evidence for this hypothesis.

How Does Narcissism Contribute to the Building Up of Rapport in the Social Interaction?

Data from the speed-dating interactions were used to further evaluate how the different

narcissistic dimensions contribute to rapport-relevant features of an initial romantic encounter.

The outcomes examined in this section include self-reported rates of disclosure, trust, and

interpersonal connection, and informant-reported levels of trust and responsiveness. Before

presenting the primary MLM analyses, zero-order correlations between the narcissism variables

and the SRM perceiver effects (for the disclosure, trust, and connection variables) and the SRM

target effects (for the trust and responsiveness variables) are examined.

Do narcissists disclose less to their dates, trust their dates less, and feel less

connected to their dates?: Zero-order correlations between narcissism variables and SRM

perceiver effects. Table 39 presents the zero-order correlations between the narcissism

variables and the SRM perceiver effects for the rapport variables. These correlations capture

whether the narcissism dimensions are associated with a generalized tendency to see interactions

99
as close and characterized by rapport. For example, do people with higher levels of vulnerable

narcissistic traits report feeling less connected to all of their dates?

As seen in Table 39, men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were more skeptical

of their dates. Moreover, participants with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits tended to

also be more skeptical of their dates. Interestingly, whereas women with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness trusted their dates more, women with higher levels of Narcissistic

Vulnerability were more skeptical of their dates. All of the narcissism variables were unrelated to

the SRM perceiver effects for disclosure and connection.

Are narcissists trusted less by their dates and considered less responsive?: Zero-

order correlations between narcissism variables and SRM target effects. Table 40 presents

the zero-order correlations between the narcissism variables and the SRM target effects for the

rapport variables. These correlations assess whether participants’ narcissistic traits are associated

with a tendency for their dates to report having interactions with them that are characterized by
11
more or less rapport. For instance, are people with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits

considered less responsive by all of their dates?

As Table 40 shows, whereas men with higher levels of the grandiose narcissistic traits

were perceived by their dates to be more responsive, women with higher levels of the grandiose

narcissistic traits tended to be perceived as less responsive (though the associations for women

were non-significant). However, the normal and vulnerable narcissistic traits were unrelated to

the SRM target effects for the trust and responsiveness variables.

11
Because of insufficient variance in the SRM target effects for the trust variable in women,
correlations were not estimated between women’s scores on the narcissism measures and their
SRM target effect for this variable.
100
Does narcissism contribute to the development of rapport?: Multilevel regression

analyses. Table 41 displays the results for three MLM analyses in which the NPI traits were

used to predict self-reported levels of disclosure, trust, and connection within the social
12
interactions. As can be seen, none of the NPI traits significantly predicted rates of disclosure

or connection. On the other hand, participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority

reported being less trusting of their dates. In addition, there was a significant interaction between

Entitlement/Exploitativeness and participant gender in the prediction of trust.

To break down this significant interaction, simple slopes for the effect of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness on trust were computed separately for men and women. The simple

slope for women was b = 1.30, SE = .57, p = .025. The simple slope for men was b = -0.14, SE =

.42, p = .738. Thus, only women with higher levels of Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported

being more trusting of their dates.

Table 42 displays the results for two MLM analyses in which the NPI traits were used to

predict the dates’ trust in the participant and the dates’ perceptions of the participants’
13
responsiveness. None of the participants’ NPI traits were significantly related to their dates’

levels of trust. However, there was a significant interaction between Grandiose Exhibitionism

and participant gender. The simple slope for women was b = -0.44, SE = .32, p = .171. The

simple slope for men was b = 0.96, SE = .36, p = .009. These results indicate that women

reported being more trusting of men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism.

12
The women’s target variance for trust was fixed to zero in the random effects statement due to
minimal variance in this parameter.
13
The women’s perceiver variance for dates’ trust in the participant was fixed to zero in the
random effects statement due to minimal variance in this parameter.
101
As Table 42 shows, none of participants’ NPI traits were significantly related to their

dates’ perceptions of their responsiveness. However, there was a significant interaction between

Grandiose Exhibitionism and participant gender. The simple slope for women was b = -0.50, SE

= .24, p = .037. The simple slope for men was b = 0.58, SE = .27, p = .035. These findings

suggest that whereas men perceived women with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism to be

less responsive, women perceived men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism to be more

responsive.

Table 43 displays the results for three MLM analyses in which the PNI traits were used to

predict participants’ self-reported levels of disclosure, trust, and connection within the social
14
interactions. Neither of the PNI traits predicted self-reported rates of disclosure or trust in the

interactions. Interestingly, however, participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity

reported feeling more connected to their dates.

Table 44 presents the results for two MLM analyses in which the PNI traits were used to

predict the dates’ trust in the participant and the dates’ perception of the participants’
15
responsiveness. As can be seen, neither PNI dimension was significantly related to

participants’ dates’ levels of trust in the participant or their dates’ perceptions of the participants’

responsiveness during the interaction.

Summary of analyses using narcissism to predict rapport. I hypothesized that the

grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits would be related to lower levels of self-reported

disclosure during the interactions. I also hypothesized that the grandiose and vulnerable

14
The women’s target variance for trust was fixed to zero in the random effects statement due to
minimal variance in this parameter.
15
The women’s perceiver variance for dates’ trust in the participant was fixed to zero in the
random effects statement due to minimal variance in this parameter.
102
narcissistic traits would be related to lower self-reported levels of interpersonal connection.

There was no support for these hypotheses. In fact, participants with higher levels of Narcissistic

Grandiosity reported feeling more connected to their dates.

In addition, I predicted that participants with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits

would be less trusting of their dates. To the contrary, women with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported having more trust in their dates. Related to this, I expected

that participants would be less trusting of dates who possessed greater levels of grandiose and

vulnerable narcissistic traits. In contrast, men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism

were trusted more by their dates. Last, I hypothesized that the grandiose and vulnerable

narcissistic traits would be associated with lower levels of informant-reported responsiveness.

On the one hand, women with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their

dates to be less responsive. Men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism, however, were

perceived by their dates to be more responsive.

Who do Narcissists Find Attractive in the Social Interactions?

The final set of hypotheses evaluated whether the narcissism dimensions are associated

with differential attraction towards certain romantic partners. For this set of analyses, the

relations between participants’ scores on the narcissism variables and their ratings of attraction

towards their dates were evaluated for moderation by the participants’ ratings of their dates’

levels of admiration, desirability, and kindness. Separate analyses were performed for perceived

admiration as a moderator, perceived desirability as a moderator, and perceived kindness as a

moderator. Moreover, separate analyses were performed with the NPI subscales as predictors and

the PNI scales as predictors, and gender was included as a moderator of each of the effects. In

103
each case, the parameters of primary interest were the two-way interactions between the

narcissism variables and the date perception variable.

Are grandiose narcissists more attracted to admiring dates?: Multilevel regression

analyses. Table 45 shows the results of an MLM analysis using the NPI traits to predict

participants’ attraction towards their dates. There was no significant interaction between

Grandiose Exhibitionism and perceived admiration. Similarly, no significant two-way

interactions were observed between the other NPI traits and perceived admiration.

Table 46 shows the results from an MLM analysis using the PNI traits to predict

participants’ attraction towards their dates. In contrast to what was hypothesized, there was no

significant two-way interaction between Narcissistic Grandiosity and perceived admiration.

However, there was a significant two-way interaction between Narcissistic Vulnerability and

perceived admiration. To break down this interaction, simple slopes were computed for the effect

of Narcissistic Vulnerability on attraction at one standard deviation above and below the mean of

perceived admiration. The simple slope at low levels of perceived admiration was b = 0.15, SE =

.11, p = .169. The simple slope at high levels of perceived admiration was b = -0.17, SE = .10, p

= .093. Although there was a trend for participants with higher levels of Narcissistic

Vulnerability to be less attracted to dates that they perceived to be more admiring during the

interaction, this effect was not significant.

Are vulnerable narcissists more attracted to desirable dates?: Multilevel regression

analyses. Table 47 presents results from an MLM analysis using the NPI traits to predict
16
participants’ attraction towards their dates. There were no significant two-way interactions

between the NPI traits and perceived desirability. However, there was a significant three-way

16
In this analysis, the men’s target variance for attraction was fixed to zero in the random effects
statement due to minimal variance in this parameter.
104
interaction between Leadership/Authority, perceived desirability, and gender. To break this

interaction down, the simple slope of the two-way interaction between Leadership/Authority and

perceived desirability was computed separately for men and women. The simple slope for men

was b = 0.19, SE = .17, p = .27. The simple slope for women was b = -0.24, SE = .12, p = .045.

This finding indicates that the relation between participants’ levels of Leadership/Authority and

their attraction towards their dates is only dependent upon how desirable they perceive their

dates to be for women.

To further break down the two-way interaction between Leadership/Authority and

perceived desirability for women, simple slopes for the effect of Leadership/Authority on

attraction towards the date were computed at one standard deviation above and below the mean

of perceived desirability. The simple slope at high levels of perceived desirability was b = -0.71,

SE = .26, p = .008. The simple slope at low levels of perceived desirability was b = -0.14, SE =

.25, p = .591. These results indicate that whereas women with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority report being less attracted to their dates when they perceive them to be

more desirable, women’s levels of Leadership/Authority are unrelated to their attraction towards

their dates when they perceive them to be less desirable.

Table 48 presents results from an MLM analysis using the PNI traits to predict
17
participants’ attraction towards their dates. As can be seen, there were no significant two-way

interactions between the PNI traits and perceived desirability.

Are narcissists less attracted to caring dates?: Multilevel regression analyses. Table

49 shows the results of an MLM analysis using the NPI traits as predictors. As can be seen, there

were no significant two-way interactions between the NPI traits and perceived kindness.

17
Note that the men’s target variance for attraction was fixed to zero in the random effects
statement due to minimal variance in this parameter.
105
Nevertheless, there was a significant three-way interaction between Grandiose Exhibitionism,

gender, and perceived kindness. To break down this interaction, simple slopes for the two-way

interaction between Grandiose Exhibitionism and perceived kindness were computed separately

for men and women. The simple slope for women was b = -0.26, SE = .22, p = .24. The simple

slope for men was b = 0.86, SE = .34, p = .011. Thus, it is only for men that the relation between

Grandiose Exhibitionism and attraction towards their date is dependent upon how caring they

perceive their date to be during the interaction.

To further break down the significant two-way interaction between Grandiose

Exhibitionism and perceived kindness for men, simple slopes for the effect of Grandiose

Exhibitionism on attraction for men were computed at one standard deviation above and below

the mean for perceived kindness. The simple slope at low levels of perceived kindness was b = -

1.24, SE = .48, p = .01. The simple slope at high levels of perceived kindness was b = 0.42, SE =

.44, p = .343. These findings indicate that whereas men with higher levels of Grandiose

Exhibitionism are less attracted to their dates when they perceive them to be less caring during

the interaction, men’s levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism are unrelated to their attraction towards

their dates when they perceive them to be more caring.

Table 50 shows the results of an MLM analysis using the PNI traits to predict

participants’ attraction towards their dates. As Table 50 shows, there were no significant two-

way interactions between the PNI traits and perceived kindness. However, there was a significant

three-way interaction between Narcissistic Vulnerability, gender, and perceived kindness. To

break this interaction down, simple slopes for the two-way interaction between Narcissistic

Vulnerability and perceived kindness were computed separately for men and women. The simple

slope for women was b = 0.09, SE = .08, p = .231. The simple slope for men was b = -0.16, SE =

106
.10, p = .098. Therefore, although the interaction terms trended in opposite directions for men

and women, neither was significantly different from zero for either gender. These interactions

were consequently not broken down further.

Summary for analyses involving the narcissism dimensions and attraction towards

dates in social interactions. Individuals with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits were

predicted to be more attracted to dates who they perceived to be more admiring during the

interactions. The results did not provide any support for this hypothesis. In addition, participants

with higher levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits were hypothesized to be more attracted to dates

who they perceived to be more desirable during the interactions. There was no support for this

hypothesis. Instead, women with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were found to be less

attracted to their dates when they perceived their dates to be more desirable during the

interactions. Finally, participants with higher levels of each of the narcissistic traits were

expected to be less attracted to dates who they perceived to be more caring. Results did not

provide any support for this hypothesis. To the contrary, findings indicated that men who

perceived their dates to be less caring during the interactions reported even lower levels of

attraction towards these dates.

107
CHAPTER 9: DISCUSSION FOR STUDY 2

Study 2 sought to better understand the roles of the normal, grandiose, and vulnerable

narcissistic traits in strategic self-presentation (i.e., how narcissists are viewed by others) and the

development of rapport (i.e., how narcissists perceive their interactions and how narcissists’

dates perceive their interactions with the narcissists). It also aimed to further illuminate the roles

of these traits in the narcissists’ appraisals of initial attraction towards their dates. To accomplish

these aims, Study 2 used a speed-dating paradigm to investigate the manifestations of these traits

in a real-world context related to romantic relationship initiation. Below I present the main

findings for each of the narcissism dimensions and then offer some basic conclusions.

Normal Narcissism in the Speed-Dating Sessions

Participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were perceived by their dates to

express interpersonal behaviors characterized by increased levels of assured-dominance when

viewed through the lens of the interpersonal circumplex (Wiggins et al., 1988). These findings

are consistent with self-report data showing that people with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority report higher levels of surgency and dominance (Emmons, 1984). They are

also consistent with findings by Back et al. (2010) that people with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority exhibit more self-assured body movements during group interactions. In

general, these findings suggest that an important goal for people with greater levels of this trait—

even within initial encounters with a potential romantic partner—is to establish dominance.

Men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were also perceived by their dates to be

more gregarious-extraverted. Research has shown that people with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority report being more extraverted (Ackerman et al., 2011; Corry et al., 2008;

Emmons, 1984) and also have informants that report them being more extraverted (Ackerman et

108
al., 2011; Hill & Roberts, in press). However, previous work has not generally uncovered gender

differences in the relation between Leadership/Authority and Extraversion. It is possible that the

present finding can be explained in terms of gender role violations. Indeed, the expression of

dominant traits in men within an explicitly romantic context (as opposed to the expression of

such traits in women within the same context) was probably viewed upon more favorably given

that such behaviors are consistent with the traditional masculine gender role.

Along with this, men with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were also perceived by

their dates to be more desirable. This finding contradicts work by Back et al. (2010), which

failed to find links between Leadership/Authority and ratings of likeability. Perhaps the more

explicit nature of the romantic context was responsible for the finding. Indeed, evolutionary

psychologists have long argued that women are more attracted to men with greater levels of

dominance (e.g., Havlicek, Roberts, & Flegr, 2005).

Interestingly, participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority were more skeptical

of all of their dates. Past research has shown, however, that people with higher levels of

Leadership/Authority report possessing lower levels of Machiavellianism (Ackerman et al.,

2011). This finding was therefore unexpected and seems to run counter to the notion that people

with higher levels of this trait should be less cynical of their dates’ intentions. Nevertheless, this

finding may have emerged due to the particular item that was used to assess trust (i.e., were you

skeptical of your interaction partner?). Instead of assessing trust per se, this item may tap into a

person’s ability to detect the degree of self-presentation that is occurring in these brief social

encounters. In this case, it may be that participants with higher levels of Leadership/Authority

are better able to detect that their partners are trying to make a good impression. An important

109
next step in this research is to determine whether such participants are actually accurate in their

inferences about the underlying motivations of their dates.

Finally, women with higher levels of Leadership/Authority reported being less attracted

to dates that they perceived to be more desirable. Thus, women who report being more dominant

and having greater leadership potential find men who are more charming, popular, and good

looking to be less attractive. It is possible that such dates represent a potential threat to the

participants’ assertion of dominance. Even if this is true, however, it remains unclear why the

same finding did not emerge for men.

Grandiose Narcissism in the Speed-Dating Sessions

Participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their dates

to display more agentic interpersonal behaviors. Specifically, they were seen as more arrogant,

dominant, and gregarious during the interactions. Such results are consistent with findings that

individuals with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism tend to be more extraverted

(Ackerman et al., 2011; Hill & Roberts, in press).

Interestingly, men with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity were perceived by their

dates to display relatively more communal interpersonal behaviors. They were seen as more

gregarious and warm during the interactions. These findings conflict with other research using

the HEXACO model of personality which has shown that Narcissistic Grandiosity is largely

unrelated to self-reports of Extraversion and Agreeableness (instead, Narcissistic Grandiosity is

negatively related to Honesty-Humility and positively related to Emotionality; Bresin & Gordon,

2011). The present findings suggest that, at least within the context of an initial interaction with a

potential romantic partner, men with higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity behave in a more

agreeable fashion.

110
Related to this, women tended to agree with each other that men with higher levels of

Narcissistic Grandiosity were more desirable. Such results are consistent with findings by Back

et al. (2010) that participants who are more self-absorbed and self-admiring exhibit more

charming facial expressions during group settings. However, Back et al. (2010) found that they

were not considered more popular, which is somewhat at odds with what was found here. In

addition, Back et al. (2010) did not report gender differences in this result.

Although not hypothesized, an interesting finding to emerge from Study 2 was that

participants with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were perceived by their dates to be

more admiring when the participants perceived that their dates were more desirable. Thus,

participants with higher levels of this trait more freely expressed their admiration for their dates

when they perceived them to be more charming, good looking, and popular. It therefore appears

that they were attempting to win over this “perfect” partner via admiration.

Another group of hypotheses in Study 2 concerned whether dates would be more

attracted to participants with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic traits due to the participants’

enhanced levels of self-assuredness, gregariousness, arrogance, and desirability. To prevent

redundancy and ensure that the mediational analyses were meaningful, I chose to focus on

gregarious-extraversion as a mediator. Results showed that the relation between participants’

grandiose narcissism measures and their dates’ attraction to them was indeed mediated by their

dates’ perceptions of their gregarious-extraversion.

The other hypotheses in Study 2 focused on the development of rapport within the speed-

dating interactions. Consistent with hypotheses, women with higher levels of Grandiose

Exhibitionism were perceived to be less responsive by their dates. This is congruent with work

that suggests that people with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism tend to be oblivious to

111
the needs of others (Ackerman et al., 2011). However, men with higher levels of Grandiose

Exhibitionism were perceived to be more responsive by their dates. Similarly, women reported

being less skeptical of men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism. These findings are

difficult to explain in the context of past research, but appear to fit a general trend in the present

research where men with higher levels of grandiose and dominant traits are perceived more

positively than women with these traits.

It was originally expected that participants with higher levels of grandiose narcissistic

traits would report feeling lower levels of connection and attraction towards caring dates due to

their diminished intimacy motivations. In contrast to what was hypothesized, participants with

higher levels of Narcissistic Grandiosity actually reported feeling more connected to their caring

dates. Likewise, men with higher levels of Grandiose Exhibitionism were less attracted to their

dates when they perceived them to be less caring.

Vulnerable Narcissism in the Speed-Dating Sessions

None of the hypotheses regarding the role of vulnerable narcissistic traits in relationship

initiation were supported in the present study. This suggests the need for seriously reconsidering

how participants with greater levels of vulnerable narcissistic traits initiate their relationships

with romantic partners. It could be that the particular strategies adopted by such participants

were not well-captured by the instruments used in this research. Alternatively, such traits may

matter more for the later stages of relationship development. Apart from these possibilities,

however, there are a few findings worthy of mention.

When the other NPI traits were not controlled, participants with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness were perceived by their dates to be less submissive. Thus, dates

tended to agree with one another that more entitled participants were not shy and unaggressive in

112
their interactions. Research using informants that have known the participants for longer periods

of time (e.g., roommates, mothers) has generally found that participants with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness are seen by informants as less agreeable (Ackerman et al., 2011;

Hill & Roberts, in press).

Participants with higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability, on the other hand, were

perceived by their dates to be less gregarious. Interestingly, Pincus et al. (2009) found that

participants with higher levels of traits related to Narcissistic Vulnerability generally reported

experiencing more interpersonal problems characterized by distrust, difficulty with being

affectionate, difficulty with initiating interactions, and gullibility. The present research

corroborates the notion that more vulnerable individuals would experience difficulty with

initiating interactions as they were found to be more withdrawn during their dates.

I had originally hypothesized that the vulnerable narcissistic traits would be associated

with lower self-reported levels of trust in dates. In contrast, women with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness reported being less skeptical of their dates. Again, this finding may

be due to how trust was measured in the present study. Indeed, it may simply mean that women

with higher levels of entitled beliefs were better detectors of their dates’ self-presentational

tendencies. Future research will be needed to determine whether a similar finding is replicated

with a more face-valid measure of trust.

Conclusions

Broadly speaking, each of the narcissistic traits demonstrated reasonably unique

associations with outcomes in the speed-dating interactions. Normal narcissism was associated

with the expression of dominant behaviors and greater levels of skepticism in dates. The

grandiose narcissistic traits were generally linked to the expression of dominance and

113
gregariousness; moreover, participants with greater levels of the grandiose traits were considered

more attractive and desirable by their dates. Unfortunately, the findings for the vulnerable

narcissistic traits were not as straightforward as there were few connections between these traits

and outcomes in the interpersonal interactions.

Interestingly, there was a general trend for men with greater levels of normal and

grandiose traits to be construed more positively by their female dates. Women with greater levels

of these traits did not accrue such important benefits. As mentioned previously, a potential

reason for this discrepancy resides in the norms that are expected for each gender. Simply put,

men are expected to be agentic whereas women are not (Eagly & Steffen, 1984).

All in all, Study 2 provided an important extension to Study 1 by examining the

manifestations of these narcissistic traits within a speed-dating paradigm. Future research would

benefit from recording participants’ actual behaviors during the speed-dating interactions. This

information would provide added insight into what behavioral cues are being used by

participants to inform their impressions of the participants (see Back et al., 2010).

114
CHAPTER 10: GENERAL DISCUSSION

The purpose of this dissertation was to illuminate the role of narcissism within romantic

relationship initiation. Despite having clear conceptual links to interpersonal behaviors, relatively

little research has investigated the connections between narcissism and close relationships. There

is even less research that has examined the role of narcissism in the beginning stages of a close

relationship. Unlike much of the current work in social and personality psychology which

typically relies solely on one instrument (Cain et al., 2008), I used multiple measures of

narcissism and attempted to delineate the unique roles of normal narcissism (characterized by the

use of adaptive self-esteem regulation strategies and the presence of a healthy self-image),

grandiose narcissism (characterized by the use of maladaptive self-enhancement strategies and

the presence of an inflated self-image), and vulnerable narcissism (characterized by the use of

maladaptive self-protection strategies and the presence of a depleted self-image) within the

initiation of relationships. Using the conceptual stages of romantic relationship initiation

suggested by Bredow et al. (2008), I investigated the roles of these narcissism dimensions within

attraction, self-presentation, and the development of rapport.

Below I summarize my findings for the roles of normal, grandiose, and vulnerable

narcissism in these three stages of relationship initiation. I then discuss the implications of these

results for better understanding the role of narcissism within close relationships. Finally, I cite

chief strengths and limitations of the present research and recommend areas for future study.

The Role of Narcissism in Romantic Relationship Initiation

Bredow et al. (2008) developed a conceptual model that outlines general stages involved

in romantic relationship initiation. According to these authors, romantic first encounters begin

with attraction, potentially proceed to strategic self-presentation, and possibly end with a

115
successful encounter that holds the promise for future relationship development. Although it is

possible that individuals can transition to earlier stages in the course of a romantic first

encounter, it is useful to review these stages in order for the sake of expositional clarity.

The beginning stage in Bredow et al.’s (2008) conceptual model of relationship

initiation—the Appraisal of Initial Attraction—involves processes related to romantic attraction.

This stage is concerned with the motives that individuals have for beginning their relationships

and whether they are congruent with the attributes that a potential romantic partner possesses. In

this dissertation, I predicted that each of the narcissism dimensions would be differentially

related to broader self-enhancement and self-protection motives that would in turn impact the

motives that were adopted for beginning a romantic relationship. The present research found

relatively unique connections between each of the three dimensions of narcissism and motives

for relationship initiation. It also found a few connections between some of the narcissism

dimensions and the partner attributes that were considered most attractive.

Generally speaking, normal narcissists did not pursue their romantic relationships for

self-focused reasons. There was some evidence, however, that grandiose narcissists were

concerned about self-enhancement when they pursued their romantic relationships and that they

were attracted to partners that would fulfill their needs for self-enhancement. In a similar

manner, the motives endorsed by vulnerable narcissists suggest that they pursue their romantic

relationships to both bolster and enhance their self-image.

A subsequent stage in Bredow et al.’s (2008) conceptual model of romantic relationship

initiation—Strategic Self-Presentation—centers on the presentation of the self. Bredow et al.

(2008) proposed that individuals will attempt to make themselves more attractive to others when

they are not fully confident that the other person is attracted to them. Bredow et al. (2008) further

116
suggested that this “strategic self-presentation” can take three forms: (a) appearing likeable, (b)

appearing competent/capable, and (c) appearing morally virtuous. I expected that the relations

between the narcissistic traits and the form of self-presentation adopted would hinge upon

whether the trait was more closely aligned with interpersonal motives involving self-

enhancement or self-protection. If the trait was more closely aligned with self-enhancement, I

predicted that the self-presentational style would entail competence; if the narcissistic trait was

more closely aligned with self-protection (and the narcissistic individual perceived the partner to

be very desirable), I predicted that the self-presentational style would entail likeability.

The current results suggest that normal narcissists generally portray themselves as

competent and capable. This particular self-presentational style appeared to be effective for

normal narcissists as their dates considered them to be rather desirable. Similarly, the gregarious

self-presentation and ingratiation tactics used by grandiose individuals were effective in

generating positive impressions. Broadly speaking, however, there was not good evidence that

the vulnerable narcissistic traits are connected to a particular form of strategic self-

presentation—at least not one that was readily detected by the particular measures used. The

failure to find consistent relations between these traits and interpersonal perceptions may signal

something about the traits themselves. Indeed, unlike some of the traits associated with grandiose

narcissism, the vulnerable narcissistic traits do not have considerable associations with

dispositions like Extraversion. Thus, the behaviors associated with them may be less likely to be

observed in these brief five-minute interactions.

The final stage in Bredow et al.’s (2008) conceptual model of romantic relationship

initiation is the Build-Up of Rapport. To increase the likelihood of a second encounter with

another person, it is generally considered important to develop rapport during the initial

117
encounter. Besides strategic self-presentation, other ways to develop rapport within an

interaction include engaging in self-disclosure and being responsive to the other person’s

disclosure. Further, interactions with increased levels of rapport are likely to be characterized by

higher levels of trust and greater feelings of connection between the two partners involved.

Given the connections between the grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits and excessive

self-focus, I predicted that these traits would engender outcomes that negatively impacted

rapport during these first encounters.

Normal narcissism and vulnerable narcissism did not exhibit many connections with the

rapport-relevant variables in the present research. In contrast, the grandiose traits were associated

with interactions that could be characterized by higher levels of rapport (at least for men).

Although not originally predicted, these findings are consistent with recent work which suggests

that the expression of confidence in an initial encounter is construed positively unless there is

other information to suggest that the confidence may be unfounded (Tenney & Spellman, 2011).

Indeed, within the speed-dating interactions, it is plausible that dates generally presumed that the

grandiose individual was a good judge of his own abilities. It may not be until the date obtains

more accurate information over time that impressions of the grandiose date may change.

In sum, the narcissism dimensions were observed to possess rather unique relations with

several outcomes involved in romantic relationship initiation. Normal narcissism was generally

associated with favorable outcomes. Although the grandiose narcissistic traits demonstrated

many similar outcomes as normal narcissism, these behaviors were likely motivated by self-

enhancement concerns. Finally, the vulnerable narcissistic traits were characterized by motives

for beginning relationships that reflected tendencies towards self-protection and self-

118
enhancement; however, these traits had few connections with interpersonal outcomes related to

strategic self-presentation and rapport.

Rethinking the Role of Narcissism in Romantic Relationships

Close relationships constitute an important arena for self-regulation and clarifying the

role of narcissism within this arena therefore provides important insight into how narcissists

regulate their self-image. This general idea prompted Campbell et al. (2006) to develop the

Agency Model of Narcissism, which attempts to explain the interpersonal self-regulatory

strategies used by narcissists in their romantic relationships.

According to Campbell et al. (2006), narcissists possess certain fundamental qualities

that make them successful in the beginning stages of a relationship but which eventually

deteriorate the quality of the relationship as time progresses. These fundamental qualities

include: a preoccupation with agentic issues over communal issues; an inflated view of the self

across myriad domains; a sense of entitlement; the possession of approach-oriented motivations;

and the exhibition of self-regulatory processes that emphasize the acquisition of self-esteem.

Taken together, these qualities are proposed to be associated with interpersonal skills

such as confidence, charisma, attractiveness, and charm (Campbell et al., 2006), as well as

interpersonal strategies such as game playing, self-promotion, and the attainment of trophy

partners (Campbell et al., 2006). Within the model, Campbell et al. (2006) argue that the

possession of these interpersonal skills give rise to the interpersonal strategies that narcissists

engage in within their relationships. Likewise, engagement in these interpersonal strategies and

the successful attainment of narcissistic esteem reinforce the original interpersonal skills.

The findings from the present research suggest that the normal, grandiose, and vulnerable

narcissistic traits do not reflect the same fundamental qualities and are implicated differently

119
with separate elements of relationships. Whereas normal narcissism is characterized by outcomes

that reflect diminished tendencies towards self-enhancement and self-protection, grandiose

narcissism is more closely connected to self-regulatory processes emphasizing the acquisition of

self-esteem using self-enhancement. Further, vulnerable narcissism is characterized by the

acquisition of self-esteem using both self-enhancement and self-protection strategies. In addition,

the normal, grandiose, and vulnerable narcissistic traits appear to be connected to different sets

of interpersonal skills and interpersonal strategies. Whereas normal narcissism is primarily

linked to the expression of dominance in romantic first encounters, grandiose narcissism is

related to the expression of charm, gregariousness, and dominance. However, vulnerable

narcissism is not clearly related to any particular form of strategic self-presentation. All in all,

these findings suggest that the Agency Model of Narcissism would benefit from making a

distinction between the grandiose and vulnerable expressions of narcissism (as well as between

normal and pathological narcissism more broadly) more explicit.

As it stands, the Agency Model of Narcissism also does not offer much insight into how

the vulnerable expressions of narcissism play out in romantic relationships. One of the insights

gained from the present work is that it is primarily the normal and grandiose narcissistic traits

that are important for the beginning stages of romantic relationships. It may be that the

vulnerable narcissistic traits play a more important role in the later stages of relationship

development. Indeed, research that has examined the role of Entitlement/Exploitativeness in

roommate relationships (where roommates had lived with each other for at least a few weeks)

has shown that this variable has negative implications for relationship satisfaction (Ackerman et

al., 2011). This insight may have important implications for understanding the developmental

course of narcissism within romantic relationships.

120
That being said, it is important to note that the Agency Model of Narcissism was

informed by the paradoxical finding by Paulhus (1998) regarding the impressions that narcissists

make upon their peers. Paulhus (1998) found that although narcissists (operationalized using the

NPI total score) are perceived more favorably upon first acquaintance, they are later perceived

quite negatively. The findings from this dissertation suggest that this conclusion regarding the

longitudinal impact of narcissism on relationships may be a bit misleading. Indeed, whereas

normal and grandiose narcissism may facilitate the initial development of relationships, it may be

vulnerable narcissism (and not normal or grandiose narcissism) which deteriorates or impedes

the long-term stability and quality of relationships.

At a broader level, the findings regarding normal narcissism beg the question of whether

this dimension should truly be considered narcissism. It could be argued that

Leadership/Authority is a poor operationalization of normal narcissism. Indeed, it is often

difficult to distinguish this variable from constructs such as dominance and self-esteem.

Nevertheless, even if new measures were developed to capture the broader features of normal

narcissism, it is debatable whether retaining the term “normal narcissism” would still be useful.

To be fair, many clinical theorists have suggested the existence of “normal narcissism” (e.g.,

Kernberg, 1975; Pincus & Lukowitsky, 2010; Stone, 1998); however, in these contexts, normal

narcissism is generally synonymous with healthy self-esteem regulation. Perhaps the term

narcissism is best reserved for describing those compensatory self-regulatory efforts that stem

from an underlying sense of vulnerability. Rather than labeling adaptive self-esteem regulation

strategies as narcissistic, it may make more sense to study the purported features of normal

narcissism on their own terms. That is, instead of studying normal narcissism, researchers could

study achievement striving, asserting dominance, etc. To continue labeling these behaviors as

121
“narcissistic,” even if preceded by the label “normal,” only serves to obfuscate the

conceptualization of narcissism.

Strengths, Weaknesses, and Future Directions

A major strength of this research was that it investigated the role of narcissism in several

different stages of romantic relationship initiation. In addition to investigating the role of this

construct in romantic attraction, it also examined its manifestations in self-presentation and the

development of rapport. As such, this research provided enhanced insight into the relationship

initiation process. Another important strength was the inclusion of multiple measures of

narcissism. A frequent criticism of research within social and personality psychology is that there

is an overreliance on the NPI to assess narcissism. By including both the NPI and the PNI, I was

able to obtain a broader understanding of the role of narcissism in relationships. Moreover, by

dividing the NPI into subscales, I was able to investigate the differential associations of the

personality constructs embedded within this instrument with the relationship outcomes. Use of

the NPI total score would have obscured these differences.

One limitation across both of the studies was the low alpha coefficient associated with the

NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscale. The low reliabilities of the NPI subscales are a

ubiquitous criticism of the instrument (del Rosario & White, 2005). It is important to point out,

however, that the chief concern with having a low reliability is that it sets an upper limit on the

possible magnitude of the correlations. Nevertheless, the Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscale

still predicted several of the hypothesized criteria. I therefore do not view this limitation as a

serious concern.

Another criticism that could be raised is that the Relationship Initiation Motivations

measure in Study 1 has not been independently validated in separate research. Indeed, it is

122
possible that the motives I used are not representative. However, previous research has generally

uncovered similar motives (see Clark, Shaver, & Abrahams, 1999; Mongeau, Serewicz, &

Therrien, 2004). For instance, Clark et al. (1999) found that some common goals reported by

undergraduate students when they initiated romantic relationships included having sex (similar to

Hedonism), experiencing love (similar to Intimacy), and impressing others outside of

relationship (similar to Enhancement).

The use of the self-esteem manipulation in Study 1 was a notable methodological

strength. An untested assumption in previous research is that narcissists use their romantic

relationships to regulate their self-esteem (e.g., Campbell, 1999; Campbell et al., 2006). By

manipulating participants’ levels of state self-esteem, I was able to empirically evaluate whether

romantic attraction towards particular romantic partners (e.g., grandiose narcissists’ attraction

towards admiring partners) becomes stronger for narcissists whose self-esteem levels are

lowered. Unfortunately, none of the proposed hypotheses were supported.

Examination of the manipulation checks suggested that the self-esteem manipulation was

reasonably effective. Moreover, there were other relations between the narcissism dimensions

and attraction that were strengthened as a result of the self-esteem manipulation. Specifically,

when participants’ self-esteem levels were lowered, those with higher levels of

Entitlement/Exploitativeness became less attracted to the desirable partners and those with

higher levels of Narcissistic Vulnerability became more attracted to caring partners. Taken

together, these facts suggest that my hypotheses may simply have been wrong. It will be

important for future research to examine the possibility that self-esteem regulation is manifested

in areas of close relationships other than romantic attraction.

123
The use of the speed-dating paradigm in Study 2 was an important strength. With this

design, participants were able to interact with several real-life potential romantic partners rather

than viewing and rating artificial person profiles. Nevertheless, one potential concern connected

with the design was the amount of time allotted for participants to interact with their dates. As

the results showed, the vulnerable narcissistic traits demonstrated a remarkable dearth of

associations with the interpersonal perception variables. The five-minute interactions were

perhaps too brief to uncover the strategies used by vulnerable narcissists. Although it is probably

ill-advised to extend the length of time for the speed-dating interactions, use of another paradigm

(e.g., the experimental generation of closeness; see Aron, Melinat, Aron, Vallone, & Bator,

1997) may help to uncover the strategies used by vulnerable narcissists in relationship initiation.

Another important limitation of Study 2 was that self-presentation was inferred through

participants’ perceptions of their dates’ behaviors. That is, I did not record the specific behaviors

that participants engaged in during the interactions. Therefore, I do not technically have evidence

of the particular ways in which participants objectively presented themselves to their dates. It

will be useful for future research to videotape or audio record the sessions so that more objective

qualities of the interaction can be assessed. Indeed, behaviors such as gaze aversion, animation,

and physical proximity can be measured and used to index characteristics of self-presentation

and/or rapport.

Future research should aim to study the role of narcissism at other stages of romantic

relationship initiation. For instance, a neglected stage in the current research was the decision to

make an overture. There are several different ways in which the different narcissism dimensions

can impact this decision. For instance, the increased confidence associated with grandiose

narcissistic traits may make it more likely that a decision will be made to try to initiate a

124
romantic relationship. It will also be important for future research to gain more insight into the

period immediately following romantic relationship initiation. One simple way to do this is to

follow-up the participants from the speed dates after the sessions. By following up participants

after the speed date, there is an enhanced opportunity to better understand the role of narcissism

in relationship development.

General Conclusions

This dissertation demonstrated that the different narcissistic dimensions play unique and

important roles in the romantic relationship initiation process. People with higher levels of each

of these traits were found to have differing motives for beginning relationships. They were also

found to engage in different self-presentational strategies to garner positive impressions.

This dissertation also provided insights into the potential mechanisms responsible for the

deterioration of narcissists’ relationships over time. Along with the differential sets of findings

for the normal, grandiose, and vulnerable narcissistic traits, such insights lend further support to

the argument that researchers who study narcissism should be investigating these dimensions

separately. In the end, further research examining the impact of the grandiose and vulnerable

narcissistic traits on the relationship initiation process will facilitate a better understanding of

narcissism as well as relationship initiation in general.

125
Table 1
Summary of Hypotheses for the Different Narcissism Dimensions at Each Stage of Romantic Relationship Initiation
Narcissism Dimension

Normal Narcissism Grandiose Narcissism Vulnerable Narcissism

Measure Measure Measure

Stage of
NPI Leadership/ NPI Grandiose PNI Narcissistic NPI Entitlement/ PNI Narcissistic
Relationship
Authority Exhibitionism Grandiosity Exploitativeness Vulnerability
Initiation

Appraisal of 1. Decreased adoption 1. Decreased adoption of Intimacy 1. Decreased adoption of Intimacy Motive
Initial Attraction of Intimacy and Coping Motive
Motives
2. Decreased attraction 2. Increased adoption of Hedonism and 2. Increased adoption of Coping and Self-
towards caring targets Enhancement Motives Affirmation Motives
3. Increased attraction towards admiring 3. Increased attraction towards desirable
targets targets
4. Decreased attraction towards caring 4. Decreased attraction towards caring
targets targets

1. Increased expressions of dominance, 1. Generally speaking, decreased


Strategic Self- No hypotheses
gregariousness, arrogance, and charm expressions of warmth and dominance
Presentation

126
Table 1 (cont’d)
Narcissism Dimension

Normal Narcissism Grandiose Narcissism Vulnerable Narcissism

Measure Measure Measure

Stage of
NPI Leadership/ NPI Grandiose PNI Narcissistic NPI Entitlement/ PNI Narcissistic
Relationship
Authority Exhibitionism Grandiosity Exploitativeness Vulnerability
Initiation

2. In cases where vulnerable participants


Strategic Self- perceive their date to be desirable,
Presentation increased expressions of warmth and
admiration
1. Will be considered more attractive by their 1. Generally speaking, will be considered
Building Rapport No hypotheses
dates less attractive by their dates
2. In cases where vulnerable participants
2. Relation between participants’ levels of
perceive their date to be desirable, will be
grandiose narcissism and their dates’
considered more attractive by their dates,
attraction towards them will be mediated by
and this relation will in turn be mediated
their dates’ perceptions of the participants’
by their dates’ perceptions of their warmth
levels of gregariousness and desirability
and admiration
3. Will disclose less to their dates 3. Will disclose less to their dates

127
Table 1 (cont’d)
Narcissism Dimension

Normal Narcissism Grandiose Narcissism Vulnerable Narcissism

Measure Measure Measure

Stage of
NPI Leadership/ NPI Grandiose PNI Narcissistic NPI Entitlement/ PNI Narcissistic
Relationship
Authority Exhibitionism Grandiosity Exploitativeness Vulnerability
Initiation

4. Will be seen as less responsive 4. Will be seen as less responsive


Building Rapport

5. Will feel less connected to their dates 5. Will feel less connected to their dates

Note. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory. PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory.

128
Table 2
Pattern Coefficients from an Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory (RIM)
RIM Item RIM Subscale I II III IV

To receive compliments Enhancement 1.00 -.21 -.05 -.01

To impress others Enhancement .80 -.11 -.08 .06

Because they remind me I’m a great person Enhancement .75 -.00 .09 -.05

To be admired Enhancement .64 .10 .13 .03

To have other person treat me like the


Enhancement .57 -.03 .20 -.07
center of his/her world
To prove to myself that the other person
Self-Affirmation .47 .16 -.20 .16
thinks I’m attractive
To reassure myself that I am desirable Self-Affirmation .39 .37 -.08 .17

To help me deal with disappointment in my


Coping -.23 .96 -.12 -.13
life
To cope with upset feelings Coping -.10 .83 -.14 -.09

Because it helps me feel better when I’m


Coping .08 .75 .07 -.03
feeling low
Because it helps me feel better when I’m
Coping -.03 .64 .20 .06
lonely

129
Table 2 (cont’d)

RIM Item RIM Subscale I II III IV

To help me feel better about myself Self-Affirmation .38 .57 -.02 -.04

To cheer myself up Coping .42 .53 -.01 -.09

Because it makes me feel more self-


Self-Affirmation .05 .43 .09 .34
confident
Because it makes me feel like I’m a more
Self-Affirmation .17 .42 .10 .20
interesting person
To feel close with the other person Intimacy .02 -.10 .85 -.02

Because I want to feel connected to the


Intimacy -.11 .11 .79 .01
other person
Because I want to open up to the other
Intimacy .17 -.14 .77 -.17
person
To better understand the other person Intimacy .07 -.12 .67 .08

To have another person truly understand


Intimacy -.04 .10 .65 -.01
me
Just for the excitement of it Hedonism -.07 -.07 .08 .83

130
Table 2 (cont’d)

RIM Item RIM Subscale I II III IV

Just for the thrill of it Hedonism .05 -.11 -.06 .80

To satisfy my sexual needs Hedonism .05 -.05 .00 .72

Because I feel “horny” Hedonism .07 -.04 -.23 .57

Because it feels good Hedonism -.16 .09 .36 .43

Note. All pattern loadings ≥ .50 are bolded. r between I and II = .69. r between I and III = .18. r between I and IV = .63. r between II
and III = .19. r between II and IV = .60. r between III and IV = .09.

131
Table 3
Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables in Study 1
1 2 3 4 5

NPI Scales
1. Leadership/Authority
2. Grandiose Exhibitionism .43**
3. Entitlement/Exploitativeness .22** .24**
PNI Scales
4. Grandiosity .36** .28** .22**
5. Vulnerability .02 .07 .29** .54**

Note. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory. PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory. **p <
.01.

132
Table 4
Zero-order correlations between RIM variables and attachment-related avoidance in Study 1
Variable 1 2 3 4 5

1. RIM Intimacy
2. RIM Coping/Self-Affirmation .13*
3. RIM Hedonism .07 .56**
4. RIM Enhancement .21** .69** .49**
5. Attachment Avoidance -.06 .11* .09 .09

Note. RIM = Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory. *p < .05. **p < .01.

133
Table 5
Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables and dependent variables in
Study 1
NPI Scales PNI Scales

Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/


Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness

RIM Variables
Intimacy -.09 -.09 -.16** .08 .13*
Coping/Self- -.09 .06 .21** .25** .53**
Affirmation
Hedonism .06 .21** .23** .21** .29**
Enhancement -.02 .17** .25** .32** .49**
Avoidance -.04 -.22** .08 -.01 .33**

Note. RIM = Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory.

134
Table 6
Parameter Estimates from Path Analysis using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits to Predict Relationship Initiation
Motivations
NPI Scales

Leadership/Authority Grandiose Exhibitionism Entitlement/Exploitativeness

RIM Scales b SE β b SE β b SE β

Intimacy -0.12 .16 -.04 -0.12 .18 -.04 -0.41* .17 -.14

Coping/
-0.57** .19 -.18 0.31 .21 .09 0.80** .19 .23
Self-Affirmation

Hedonism -0.21 .18 -.07 0.68** .20 .19 0.69** .19 .20

Enhancement -0.51** .19 -.15 0.66** .21 .18 0.88** .20 .24

Note. RIM = Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05.
**p < .01.

135
Table 7
Parameter Estimates from an Unconstrained Multiple-Groups Analysis for Men and Women using the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory Subscales to Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations
Gender
Women Men
b SE β b SE β
Leadership/Authority (LA)
LA Æ Intimacy -0.33 .21 -.11 0.38 .25 .16
LA Æ Coping/Self-Affirmation -0.97** .23 -.30 0.07 .32 .02
LA Æ Hedonism -0.36 .22 -.12 -0.06 .32 -.02
LA Æ Enhancement -0.98** .23 -.29 0.20 .34 .06
Grandiose Exhibitionism (GE)
GE Æ Intimacy -0.05 .25 -.02 -0.37 .26 -.14
GE Æ Coping/Self-Affirmation 0.60* .27 .16 -0.22 .33 -.06
GE Æ Hedonism 0.67* .26 .19 0.70* .34 .20
GE Æ Enhancement 0.74** .27 .19 0.34 .36 .10
Entitlement/Exploitativeness (EE)
EE Æ Intimacy -0.20 .22 -.07 -0.82** .25 -.28
EE Æ Coping/Self-Affirmation 0.41 .23 .12 1.33** .32 .35
EE Æ Hedonism 0.46* .23 .14 1.07** .32 .28
EE Æ Enhancement 0.72** .24 .21 1.07** .34 .27
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

136
Table 8
Parameter Estimates from Path Analysis using the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI)
Traits to Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations
PNI Scales
Grandiosity Vulnerability
RIM Scales b SE β b SE β

Intimacy 0.01 .08 .01 0.13* .06 .13

Coping/
-0.08 .08 -.06 0.64** .06 .56
Self-Affirmation

Hedonism 0.11 .09 .08 0.28** .07 .25

Enhancement 0.11 .08 .07 0.53** .07 .45

Note. RIM = Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory. SE = Standard error of the


unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

137
Table 9
Parameter Estimates from an Unconstrained Multiple-Groups Analysis for Men and Women using the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory Scales to Predict Relationship Initiation Motivations
Gender
Women Men
b SE β b SE β
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG)
NG Æ Intimacy 0.01 .10 .01 0.02 .11 .02
NG Æ Coping/Self-Affirmation -0.17 .10 -.12 -0.02 .11 -.01
NG Æ Hedonism -0.00 .11 -.00 0.22 .14 .15
NG Æ Enhancement 0.03 .11 .02 0.17 .13 .11
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV)
NV Æ Intimacy 0.17* .08 .18 0.03 .09 .03
NV Æ Coping/Self-Affirmation 0.63** .08 .57 0.71** .09 .61
NV Æ Hedonism 0.28** .09 .26 0.34** .11 .28
NV Æ Enhancement 0.55** .09 .48 0.55** .11 .44
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

138
Table 10
Means and Standard Deviations for Romantic Targets as a Function of Target Presentation Order
Attraction Ratings Towards Target
Admiring Desirable Caring
Target Order n Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

1. Admiring, Desirable, and Caring 50 4.82 0.97 5.10 1.17 5.27 1.12

2. Admiring, Caring, and Desirable 51 4.78 1.28 4.69 1.19 4.90 1.50

3. Desirable, Admiring, and Caring 50 4.70 1.29 5.01 1.29 5.25 1.06

4. Desirable, Caring, and Admiring 97 4.73 1.04 5.49 1.08 5.39 1.16

5. Caring, Desirable, and Admiring 49 4.76 1.14 5.17 1.34 5.46 1.07

6. Caring, Admiring, and Desirable 45 4.68 1.03 4.97 1.36 5.32 1.14

139
Table 11
Zero-order correlations between primary dependent variables in Study 1
Variable 1 2 3
1. Admiring Partner
2. Desirable Partner .16**
3. Caring Partner .11* -.05
Note. *p < .05. **p < .01.

140
Table 12
Zero-order correlations between primary independent variables and dependent variables in
Study 1
NPI Scales PNI Scales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness
Admiring Partner .06 .11* .04 .06 .11
Desirable Partner .16** .15** -.02 .16** .05
Caring Partner -.05 -.10 -.07 .07 .03
Note. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory. PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory. *p <
.05. **p < .01.

141
Table 13
Multiple Regression Analyses using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory Subscales to Predict
Attraction towards the Romantic Targets
Narcissistic Personality Inventory Subscales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness

Romantic Targets b SE β b SE β b SE β

Admiring 0.07 .26 .02 0.47 .29 .10 0.08 .26 .02
Desirable 0.62* .28 .13 0.61 .31 .12 -0.39 .29 -.07
Caring -0.04 .28 -.01 -0.42 .31 -.08 -0.22 .28 -.04
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05.

142
Table 14
Multiple Regression Analyses using the Pathological Narcissism Inventory Scales to Predict
Attraction towards the Romantic Targets
Pathological Narcissism Inventory Scales
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Romantic Targets b SE β b SE β
Admiring 0.01 .12 .01 0.15 .10 .10
Desirable 0.40** .13 .19 -0.09 .11 -.05
Caring 0.16 .13 .08 -0.03 .10 -.02
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. **p < .01.

143
Table 15
Means and Standard Deviations of Attraction towards Romantic Targets
Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Failure Control
(n = 152) (n = 103) (n = 91)

Romantic Target Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

Admiring 4.69 1.14 4.75 1.14 4.78 1.10

Desirable 5.13 1.20 5.26 1.22 4.94 1.34

Caring 5.08 1.22 5.44 1.24 5.34 1.09

144
Table 16
Predicting Attraction towards Admiring Target with Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR
Model Intercept 4.80** 0.12
Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition -0.12 0.16
Failure Condition -0.08 0.17 .001
Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
Leadership/Authority 0.08 0.52
Grandiose Exhibitionism 0.56 0.58
Entitlement/Exploitativeness -0.08 0.50 .013
Interactions between Variables
a
Leadership/Authority by RAT Condition
Leadership/Authority by Success Condition 0.03 0.65
Leadership/Authority by Failure Condition -0.00 0.72 .000
b
Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT Condition
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Success Condition -0.36 0.74
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Failure Condition 0.25 0.77 .002
c
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT Condition
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Success Condition 0.14 0.65
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Failure Condition 0.25 0.73 .000

145
Table 16 (cont’d)

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-
squared obtained by including the Leadership/Authority by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after the
blocks containing the interactions between Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions
2
between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-squared
obtained by including the Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after the
blocks containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions between
2
Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). c = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-squared obtained
by including the Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after the blocks
containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions between
Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes).

146
Table 17
Predicting Attraction towards Desirable Target with Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR

Model Intercept 4.92** 0.14


Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition 0.21 0.17
Failure Condition 0.28 0.18 .010
Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
Leadership/Authority 0.61 0.57
Grandiose Exhibitionism 0.30 0.63
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.64 0.54 .036**
Interactions between Variables
a
Leadership/Authority by RAT Condition
Leadership/Authority by Success Condition 0.18 0.70
Leadership/Authority by Failure Condition -0.34 0.78 .002
b
Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT Condition
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Success Condition -0.05 0.80
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Failure Condition 0.81 0.84 .004
c
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT Condition
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Success Condition -1.16 0.70
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Failure Condition -1.83* 0.79 .016

147
Table 17 (cont’d)

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change
in R-squared obtained by including the Leadership/Authority by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e.,
after the blocks containing the interactions between Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes and the
2
interactions between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this row represents the change in
R-squared obtained by including the Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e.,
after the blocks containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions
2
between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). c = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-squared
obtained by including the Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after the
blocks containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions between
Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes).

148
Table 18
Simple Slopes for each Remote Associations Test (RAT) Condition for the Relation between the
Narcissistic Personality Inventory Subscale of Entitlement/Exploitativeness and Attraction
towards the Desirable Target
Entitlement/Exploitativeness
b SE β
RAT Condition
Success -0.52 .44 -.10
Failure -1.19* .57 -.23
Control 0.64 .54 .12
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05.

149
Table 19
Predicting Attraction towards Caring Target with Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR

Model Intercept 5.31** .13


Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition -0.24 .16
Failure Condition 0.16 .18 .017*
Narcissistic Personality Inventory Traits
Leadership/Authority 0.83 .55
Grandiose Exhibitionism -0.66 .61
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.03 .53 .012
Interactions between Variables
a
Leadership/Authority by RAT Condition
Leadership/Authority by Success Condition -1.46* .68
Leadership/Authority by Failure Condition -0.80 .76 .013
b
Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT Condition
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Success Condition 0.64 .77
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Failure Condition -0.16 .81 .004
c
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT Condition
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Success Condition -0.88 .68
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Failure Condition 0.36 .76 .010

150
Table 19 (cont'd)

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change
in R-squared obtained by including the Leadership/Authority by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e.,
after the blocks containing the interactions between Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes and the
2
interactions between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this row represents the change in
R-squared obtained by including the Grandiose Exhibitionism by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e.,
after the blocks containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions
2
between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and the RAT condition dummy codes). c = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-squared
obtained by including the Entitlement/Exploitativeness by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after the
blocks containing the interactions between Leadership/Authority and the RAT condition dummy codes and the interactions between
Grandiose Exhibitionism and the RAT condition dummy codes).

151
Table 20
Predicting Attraction towards Admiring Target with Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR

Model Intercept 4.78** 0.12


Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition -0.10 0.15
Failure Condition -0.05 0.16 .001
Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
Narcissistic Grandiosity -0.02 0.23
Narcissistic Vulnerability 0.16 0.20 .011
Interactions between Variables
a
Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Success Condition -0.09 0.31
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Failure Condition 0.25 0.32 .004
b
Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Success Condition -0.02 0.25
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Failure Condition 0.06 0.26 .000

152
Table 20 (cont’d)

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-
squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after
2
the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Vulnerability and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this row
represents the change in R-squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT condition dummy codes as the final
block in the model (i.e., after the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Grandiosity and the RAT condition dummy
codes).

153
Table 21
Predicting Attraction towards Desirable Target with Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR

Model Intercept 4.95** 0.13


Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition 0.20 0.17
Failure Condition 0.29 0.18 .010
Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
Narcissistic Grandiosity 0.56* 0.26
Narcissistic Vulnerability -0.29 0.22 .027*
Interactions between Variables
a
Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Success Condition -0.23 0.34
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Failure Condition -0.23 0.35 .002
b
Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Success Condition 0.29 0.28
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Failure Condition 0.24 0.29 .003

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change
in R-squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e.,

154
Table 21 (cont’d)

2
after the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Vulnerability and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this
row represents the change in R-squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT condition dummy codes as the
final block in the model (i.e., after the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Grandiosity and the RAT condition
dummy codes).

155
Table 22
Predicting Attraction towards Caring Target with Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
2
b SE ΔR

Model Intercept 5.35** 0.13


Remote Associations Test Condition
Success Condition -0.25 0.16
Failure Condition 0.10 0.17 .017*
Pathological Narcissism Inventory Traits
Narcissistic Grandiosity 0.30 0.25
Narcissistic Vulnerability -0.32 0.21 .003
Interactions between Variables
a
Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Success Condition -0.07 0.32
Narcissistic Grandiosity by Failure Condition -0.30 0.33 .003
b
Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT Condition
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Success Condition 0.15 0.27
Narcissistic Vulnerability by Failure Condition 0.71* 0.28 .023*

2
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. **p < .01. a = ΔR in this row represents the change in R-
squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Grandiosity by RAT condition dummy codes as the final block in the model (i.e., after

156
Table 22 (cont’d)

2
the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Vulnerability and the RAT condition dummy codes). b = ΔR in this row
represents the change in R-squared obtained by including the Narcissistic Vulnerability by RAT condition dummy codes as the final
block in the model (i.e., after the blocks containing the interactions between Narcissistic Grandiosity and the RAT condition dummy
codes).

157
Table 23
Simple Slopes for each Remote Associations Test (RAT) Condition for the Relation between the
Pathological Narcissism Inventory Scale of Narcissistic Vulnerability and Attraction towards the
Caring Target
Vulnerability
b SE β
RAT Condition
Success -0.17 .16 -.11
Failure 0.38* .18 .24
Control -0.32 .21 -.20
Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05.

158
Table 24
Illustration of Asymmetric Block Design

Target

M1 M2 M3 W1 W2 W3

M1 X X X

M2 X X X

Perceiver
M3 X X X

W1 X X X

W2 X X X

W3 X X X

Note. M = Man. W = Woman.

159
Table 25
Social Relations Model Variance Partitioning for Dating Record Variables in Study 2
Relative Percentage of Variance in
Dating Record Variable Accounted for
by SRM component
Dating Record Variables Perceiver Target Relationship Total
plus error Variance
Interpersonal Behavior
M-W Assured-Dominant 21.21* 16.23* 62.56 1.43
W-M Assured-Dominant 19.15** 30.86** 49.99 1.90
M-W Cold-hearted 47.22** 0.00 52.78 0.95
W-M Cold-hearted 30.37** 0.24 69.39 0.92
M-W Unassured-Submissive 20.30** 24.77** 54.93 1.64
W-M Unassured-Submissive 16.02** 30.58** 53.39 1.68
M-W Arrogant-Calculating 41.03** 2.46 56.51 1.00
W-M Arrogant-Calculating 47.48** 6.75* 45.76 1.16
M-W Warm-Agreeable 44.34** 14.65* 41.01 0.96
W-M Warm-Agreeable 33.95** 4.44 61.61 1.40
M-W Gregarious-Extraverted 16.63** 28.09** 55.28 1.01
W-M Gregarious-Extraverted 12.54* 29.81** 57.65 1.50
Appeal
M-W Admiring 52.89** 6.56* 40.55 1.10
W-M Admiring 49.60** 7.56 42.84 1.23
M-W Desirable 21.98** 37.54** 40.47 1.17
W-M Desirable 17.48** 37.24** 45.27 1.55
M-W Caring 33.13** 12.95** 53.92 0.70
W-M Caring 30.67** 12.27** 57.06 1.16
M-W Romantic Attraction 18.37** 33.62** 48.01 1.58
W-M Romantic Attraction 22.97** 31.65** 45.38 1.99
Rapport
M-W Connection 24.84** 24.56** 50.59 1.39

160
Table 25 (cont’d)
Relative Percentage of Variance in
Dating Record Variable Accounted for
by SRM component
Dating Record Variables Perceiver Target Relationship Total
plus error Variance
Rapport
W-M Connection 21.70** 14.74* 63.57 1.66
M-W Disclose 48.70** 4.86 46.44 1.80
W-M Disclose 48.31** 8.07* 43.62 2.11
M-W Trust 27.83** 0.00 72.17 2.21
W-M Trust 47.23** 11.34** 41.42 2.37
M-W Perceived Responsiveness 38.44** 7.92* 53.64 1.14
W-M Perceived Responsiveness 32.08** 9.32* 58.60 1.40
Note. Estimates of parameters obtained from BLOCKO. M-W = Rating from men to women.
W-M = Rating from women to men. *p < .05. **p < .01.

161
Table 26
Reciprocity Correlations for Dating Record Variables in Study 2
Generalized Reciprocity Coefficients Dyadic Reciprocity
Men Women Coefficients

Dating Record Variables r  r  r


Interpersonal Behavior
Assured-Dominant -.25 -.23 -.05
Cold-hearted nc nc .07
Unassured-Submissive -.25 .36 -.06
Arrogant-Calculating -.13 nc .07
Warm-Agreeable nc .05 .13*
Gregarious-Extraverted .08 -.07 .04
Appeal
Admiring nc .28 .22*
Desirable -.17 -.13 .18**
Caring .01 -.00 .17**
Romantic Attraction -.01 -.14 .26**
Rapport
Connection .29 .35 .37**
Disclose -.19 nc .08
Trust -.22 nc .04
Perceived Responsiveness .43 -.02 .23**
Note. Estimates of parameters obtained from BLOCKO. nc = estimate not computed due to a
lack of significant target variance. *p < .05. **p < .01.

162
Table 27
Zero-order correlations between narcissism variables in Study 2
1 2 3 4 5

1. NPI Leadership/Authority

2. NPI Grandiose Exhibitionism .47**

3. NPI Entitlement/Exploitativeness .35** .31**

4. PNI Grandiosity .34** .37** .32**

5. PNI Vulnerability .10 .17* .23** .47**

Note. NPI = Narcissistic Personality Inventory. PNI = Pathological Narcissism Inventory. *p <
.05. **p < .01.

163
Table 28
Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects for the Interpersonal Adjective Scale-
Revised (IAS-R) Dating Record Variables
NPI Scales PNI Scales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness
SRM Target
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
Effects
Assured-
.37* .19 .52** .22 .05 .17 .15 -.14 -.03 -.08
Dominance
Arrogant- .35 nc .63** nc .12 nc .01 nc .06 nc
Calculating
Unassured- -.27 -.26* -.48* -.26 -.01 -.29* -.20 -.08 .03 -.06
Submissive
Warm- nc -.26* nc -.21 nc -.16 nc -.17 nc -.08
Agreeable
Gregarious- .42* .06 .51* -.04 .08 .04 .39* -.17 -.01 -.14
Extraverted
Note. Estimates of correlations obtained from BLOCKO and are disattenuated for error. nc = estimate not computed due to a lack of
significant target variance. *p < .05. **p < .01.

164
Table 29
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Interpersonal Behavior from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI)
Traits
Assured- Arrogant- Unassured- Warm- Gregarious-
Dominant Calculating Submissive Agreeable Extraverted
b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 3.63** .05 2.82** .06 3.85** .06 4.88** .06 5.17** .06
First-Order Effects
Gender 0.02 .07 -0.26** .06 -0.02 .07 -0.13* .05 -0.07 .05
LA 0.45* .23 0.16 .15 -0.24 .24 -0.00 .16 0.45* .21
GE 0.72** .25 0.41* .16 -0.68* .26 -0.12 .18 0.49* .23
EE -0.20 .25 -0.05 .16 -0.22 .26 0.05 .18 -0.09 .23
Interactions with Gender
LA by Gender 0.27 .23 0.01 .15 0.17 .24 0.27 .16 0.42* .21
GE by Gender 0.33 .25 0.29 .16 -0.37 .26 0.06 .18 0.42 .23
EE by Gender -0.12 .25 -0.13 .16 0.09 .26 0.17 .18 -0.13 .23

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. LA = Leadership/Authority. GE = Grandiose Exhibitionism.
EE = Entitlement/Exploitativeness. *p < .05. **p < .01.

165
Table 30
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Interpersonal Behavior from the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory
(PNI) Traits
Assured- Arrogant- Unassured- Warm- Gregarious-
Dominant Calculating Submissive Agreeable Extraverted
b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 3.61** .06 2.83** .06 3.88** .07 4.88** .06 5.15** .05
First-Order Effects
Gender 0.04 .07 -0.26** .06 -0.03 .07 -0.16** .05 -0.09 .06
NG 0.10 .09 0.08 .06 -0.17 .10 0.09 .06 0.28** .09
NV -0.09 .08 -0.08 .05 0.06 .08 -0.05 .06 † .07
-0.14
Interactions with Gender
NG by Gender 0.16 .09 -0.06 .06 -0.16 .10 0.15* .06 0.30** .09
NV by Gender -0.08 .08 0.06 .05 0.09 .08 -0.02 .06 -0.08 .07

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. NG = Narcissistic Grandiosity. NV = Narcissistic
Vulnerability. *p < .05. **p < .01. †p = .06.

166
Table 31
Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects for the Appeal Dating Record Variables
NPI Scales PNI Scales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness
SRM Target
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
Effects
Admiring nc -.20 nc -.13 nc .03 nc -.15 nc .07
Desirable .36* .05 .48** .27* .10 .12 .40* .01 .16 -.02
Attraction .35* .05 .49** .16 .10 .11 .41** .07 .14 .02

Note. Estimates of correlations obtained from BLOCKO and are disattenuated for error. nc = estimate not computed due to a lack of
significant target variance. *p < .05. **p < .01.

167
Table 32
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Admiration and Desirability, and Dates’
Attraction to Participants, from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits
Admiring Desirable Attraction

b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 3.67** .06 4.20** .06 3.88** .07


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.25** .06 -0.11 .06 -0.16* .07
LA 0.08 .15 0.39 .24 0.35 .27
GE 0.06 .16 0.75** .26 0.68* .30
EE 0.08 .16 0.02 .26 0.04 .30
Interactions with Gender
LA by Gender 0.31* .15 0.46† .24 0.37 .27
GE by Gender 0.32† .16 0.13 .26 0.37 .30
EE by Gender -0.11 .16 -0.09 .26 -0.23 .30

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. LA =


Leadership/Authority. GE = Grandiose Exhibitionism. EE = Entitlement/Exploitativeness. *p <
.05. **p < .01. †p < .06.

168
Table 33
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants’ Admiration and Desirability, and Dates’
Attraction to Participants, from the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI)
Traits
Admiring Desirable Attraction

b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 3.67** .07 4.19** .06 3.87** .07


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.24** .06 -0.11 .07 -0.17* .08
NG -0.02 .06 0.29** .10 0.32** .11
NV 0.00 .05 -0.04 .08 -0.07 .09
Interactions with Gender
NG by Gender 0.12* .06 0.23* .10 0.19 .11
NV by Gender -0.07 .05 0.03 .08 -0.01 .09

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. NG = Narcissistic


Grandiosity. NV = Narcissistic Vulnerability. *p < .05. **p < .01.

169
Table 34
Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ NPI traits and their dates’ perceptions of
them as Warm-Agreeable is Moderated by participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability

b SE

Model Intercept 4.88** .05


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.14* .05
Desirability 0.11** .03
Leadership/Authority 0.04 .16
Grandiose Exhibitionism -0.11 .18
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.07 .18
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender 0.30 .16
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender 0.05 .18
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender 0.20 .18
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Desirability 0.02 .03
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Desirability
Leadership/Authority by Desirability -0.11 .13
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Desirability 0.23 .15
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Desirability 0.04 .13
Three-Way Interactions for NPI traits, Desirability, and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender by Desirability -0.17 .13
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender by Desirability 0.18 .15
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender by Desirability 0.04 .13

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

170
Table 35
Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ NPI traits and their dates’ perceptions of
them as Admiring is Moderated by participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability

b SE

Model Intercept 3.67** .06


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.26** .06
Desirability 0.15** .03
Leadership/Authority 0.11 .15
Grandiose Exhibitionism 0.24 .17
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.07 .16
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender 0.34* .15
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender 0.34* .17
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender -0.04 .17
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Desirability 0.01 .03
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Desirability
Leadership/Authority by Desirability -0.04 .11
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Desirability 0.31* .13
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Desirability 0.09 .12
Three-Way Interactions for NPI traits, Desirability, and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender by Desirability 0.08 .11
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender by Desirability -0.18 .13
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender by Desirability 0.03 .12

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

171
Table 36
Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ PNI traits and their dates’ perceptions of
them as Warm-Agreeable is Moderated by participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability

b SE

Model Intercept 4.88** .05


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.16** .06
Desirability 0.13** .03
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG) 0.10 .06
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV) -0.05 .06
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Gender
NG by Gender 0.15* .06
NV by Gender -0.03 .06
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Desirability 0.02 .03
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Desirability
NG by Desirability -0.04 .05
NV by Desirability 0.04 .04
Three-Way Interactions between PNI traits, Desirability, and Gender
NG by Gender by Desirability -0.09† .05
NV by Gender by Desirability 0.03 .04

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01. †p =
.051.

172
Table 37
Evaluating whether the relation between participants’ PNI traits and their dates’ perceptions of
them as Admiring is Moderated by participants’ perception of their dates’ desirability

b SE

Model Intercept 3.67** .06


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.24** .06
Desirability 0.13** .03
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG) -0.00 .06
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV) -0.00 .05
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Gender
NG by Gender 0.13* .06
NV by Gender -0.06 .05
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Desirability 0.03 .03
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Desirability
NG by Desirability -0.02 .04
NV by Desirability 0.07 .04
Three-Way Interactions between PNI traits, Desirability, and Gender
NG by Gender by Desirability -0.02 .04
NV by Gender by Desirability 0.03 .04

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

173
Table 38
Zero-order correlations between dates’ perceptions of the participants’ gregarious-extraversion,
assured-dominance, arrogance-calculation, desirability, and attractiveness

Variable 1 2 3 4 5

Dates’ Ratings of Participant


1. Perceived Gregarious-Extraversion
2. Perceived Assured-Dominance .46**
3. Perceived Arrogance-Calculation .15* .31**
4. Perceived Desirability .65** .38** .25**
5. Attraction to Participant .54** .31** .29** .81**

Note. df for each correlation = 173 (i.e., n – 2*number of groups – 1). *p < .05. **p < .01.

174
Table 39
Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Perceiver effects for the Rapport Dating Record Variables
NPI Scales PNI Scales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness

SRM Perceiver
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
Effects

Disclosure -.08 -.02 .06 .08 -.03 .09 .11 .11 .09 .18
Trust -.34** .01 -.19 .08 -.17 .22* -.21 -.26** -.10 -.38**
Connection .12 -.04 .12 -.08 -.13 -.08 .18 .23 .07 .00

Note. *p < .05. **p < .01.

175
Table 40
Zero-order correlations between participants’ scores on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) subscales, the Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) scales, and their Social Relations Model (SRM) Target effects for the Rapport Dating Record Variables
NPI Scales PNI Scales
Leadership/ Grandiose Entitlement/
Grandiosity Vulnerability
Authority Exhibitionism Exploitativeness

SRM Target
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
Effects

Trust .19 nc .38 nc .07 nc .14 nc -.08 nc


Responsiveness .34 .10 .57** -.29 .23 -.10 .42* -.09 .06 -.06

Note. Estimates of correlations obtained from BLOCKO and are disattenuated for error. nc = estimate not computed due to a lack of
significant target variance. *p < .05. **p < .01.

176
Table 41
Predicting Participants’ Disclosure, Trust, and Connection to their Dates in the Social
Interactions from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits
a
Disclosure Trust Connection

b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 4.12** .07 5.50** .07 3.49** .07


First-Order Effects
Participant gender -0.13 .08 -0.20* .08 0.24** .06
LA -0.07 .32 -0.66* .31 0.01 .24
GE 0.17 .36 0.23 .35 0.13 .26
EE 0.27 .36 0.58 .35 -0.22 .26
Interactions with Gender
LA by Gender 0.03 .32 -0.24 .31 0.20 .23
GE by Gender -0.05 .36 -0.05 .35 -0.01 .26
EE by Gender -0.03 .36 -0.72* .35 -0.03 .27

Note. a = the women’s target variance for trust was fixed to zero in the random effects statement
due to minimal variance in this parameter. LA = Leadership/Authority. GE = Grandiose
Exhibitionism. EE = Entitlement/Exploitativeness. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized
regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

177
Table 42
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants as Trusting and Responsive in the Social
Interactions from the Participants’ Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits

Dates’ Trust in Perceived


a
Participant Responsiveness

b SE b SE

Model Intercept 5.44** .07 5.02** .06


First-Order Effects
Participant gender 0.19* .07 -0.09 .05
LA -0.04 .22 0.18 .16
GE 0.26 .24 0.04 .18
EE 0.04 .24 0.08 .18
Interactions with Gender
LA by Gender -0.25 .22 -0.05 .16
GE by Gender 0.70** .24 0.54** .18
EE by Gender -0.00 .24 0.11 .18

Note. a = the women’s perceiver variance for partners’ trust was fixed to zero in the random
effects statement due to minimal variance in this parameter. LA = Leadership/Authority. GE =
Grandiose Exhibitionism. EE = Entitlement/Exploitativeness. SE = Standard error of the
unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

178
Table 43
Predicting Participants’ Disclosure, Trust, and Connection to their Dates in the Social
Interactions from the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits
a
Disclosure Trust Connection

b SE b SE b SE

Model Intercept 4.10** .07 5.48** .08 3.52** .07


First-Order Effects
Participant gender -0.13 .08 -0.20* .08 0.18** .06
NG 0.14 .13 -0.03 .13 0.30** .09
NV 0.14 .11 -0.17 .11 -0.09 .08
Interactions with Gender
NG by Gender 0.08 .13 -0.07 .13 -0.11 .09
NV by Gender 0.01 .11 0.19 .11 0.04 .08

Note. a = the women’s target variance for trust was fixed to zero in the random effects statement
due to minimal variance in this parameter. NG = Narcissistic Grandiosity. NV = Narcissistic
Vulnerability. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p <
.01.

179
Table 44
Predicting Dates’ Perceptions of Participants as Trusting and Responsive in the Social
Interactions from the Participants’ Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits

Dates’ Trust in Perceived


a
Participant Responsiveness

b SE b SE

Model Intercept 5.43** .07 5.03** .06


First-Order Effects
Participant gender 0.18* .08 -0.10† .05
NG 0.03 .09 0.09 .07
NV -0.10 .08 -0.06 .06
Interactions with Gender
NG by Gender 0.12 .09 0.11 .07
NV by Gender -0.04 .08 -0.03 .06

Note. a = the women’s perceiver variance for partners’ trust was fixed to zero in the random
effects statement due to minimal variance in this parameter. NG = Narcissistic Grandiosity. NV
= Narcissistic Vulnerability. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p
< .05. **p < .01. †p < .06.

180
Table 45
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Admiring Dates with the Participants’ Narcissistic
Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits

b SE

Model Intercept 3.96** .06


First-Order Effects
Participant gender 0.03 .07
Perceived Partner Admiration 0.56** .04
Leadership/Authority -0.13 .22
Grandiose Exhibitionism -0.90** .24
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.01 .26
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender -0.14 .22
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender -0.31 .24
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender -0.21 .26
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Admiration
Gender by Perceived Admiration -0.08* .04
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Admiration
Leadership/Authority by Perceived Admiration -0.13 .17
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Perceived Admiration 0.02 .18
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Perceived Admiration -0.27 .18
Three-Way Interactions for NPI traits, Gender, and Admiration
Leadership/Authority by Gender by Admiration -0.28 .16
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender by Admiration 0.11 .18
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender by Admiration 0.06 .18

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

181
Table 46
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Admiring Dates with the Participants’ Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits

b SE

Model Intercept 3.92** .06


First-Order Effects
Gender -0.02 .07
Admiration 0.51** .04
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG) 0.02 .09
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV) -0.01 .08
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Gender
NG by Gender 0.00 .09
NV by Gender -0.03 .08
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Admiration
Gender by Admiration -0.14** .04
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Admiration
NG by Admiration 0.08 .06
NV by Admiration -0.14* .06
Three-Way Interactions for PNI traits, Admiration, and Gender
NG by Gender by Admiration 0.04 .06
NV by Gender by Admiration -0.03 .06

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

182
Table 47
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Desirable Dates with the Participants’ Narcissistic
Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits

b SE

Model Intercept 3.91** .04


First-Order Effects
Gender 0.07 .04
Desirability 0.91** .02
Leadership/Authority -0.26 .15
Grandiose Exhibitionism 0.08 .17
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.04 .17
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender 0.16 .15
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender -0.07 .17
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender -0.16 .17
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Perceived Desirability 0.00 .02
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Desirability
Leadership/Authority by Desirability -0.03 .10
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Desirability 0.02 .12
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Desirability 0.17 .11
Three-Way Interactions for NPI traits, Desirability, and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender by Desirability 0.22* .10
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender by Desirability -0.14 .12
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender by Desirability 0.03 .11

Note. The men’s target variance for attraction was fixed to zero in the random effects statement
due to minimal variance in this parameter. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression
coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

183
Table 48
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Desirable Dates with the Participants’ Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits
b SE
Model Intercept 3.89** .04
First-Order Effects
Gender 0.05 .04
Desirability 0.92** .02
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG) 0.06 .06
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV) -0.03 .05
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Gender
NG by Gender 0.00 .06
NV by Gender -0.05 .05
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Desirability
Gender by Perceived Desirability -0.00 .02
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Desirability
NG by Desirability 0.06 .04
NV by Desirability -0.02 .03
Three-Way Interactions for PNI traits, Desirability, and Gender
NG by Gender by Desirability -0.02 .04
NV by Gender by Desirability -0.02 .03

Note. The men’s target variance for attraction was fixed to zero in the random effects statement
due to minimal variance in this parameter. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression
coefficient. **p < .01.

184
Table 49
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Caring Dates with the Participants’ Narcissistic
Personality Inventory (NPI) Traits

b SE

Model Intercept 3.92** .06


First-Order Effects
Gender 0.08 .07
Caring 0.53** .04
Leadership/Authority -0.15 .22
Grandiose Exhibitionism -0.14 .25
Entitlement/Exploitativeness 0.05 .26
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender 0.13 .22
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender -0.28 .25
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender -0.06 .25
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Caring
Gender by Caring -0.06 .04
Two-Way Interactions between NPI traits and Caring
Leadership/Authority by Caring -0.25 .18
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Caring 0.30 .20
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Caring -0.08 .19
Three-Way Interactions for NPI traits, Caring, and Gender
Leadership/Authority by Gender by Caring -0.04 .18
Grandiose Exhibitionism by Gender by Caring 0.56** .20
Entitlement/Exploitativeness by Gender by Caring -0.16 .19

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. **p < .01.

185
Table 50
Predicting Participants’ Attraction towards Caring Dates with the Participants’ Pathological
Narcissism Inventory (PNI) Traits

b SE

Model Intercept 3.92** .06


First-Order Effects
Gender 0.02 .07
Caring 0.51** .04
Narcissistic Grandiosity (NG) 0.22* .09
Narcissistic Vulnerability (NV) -0.06 .08
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Gender
NG by Gender -0.06 .09
NV by Gender 0.00 .08
Two-Way Interaction between Gender and Caring
Gender by Caring -0.08 .04
Two-Way Interactions between PNI traits and Caring
NG by Caring 0.05 .07
NV by Caring -0.04 .06
Three-Way Interactions for PNI traits, Caring, and Gender
NG by Gender by Caring 0.09 .07
NV by Gender by Caring -0.13* .06

Note. SE = Standard error of the unstandardized regression coefficient. *p < .05. **p < .01.

186
Figure 1. Higher order structure of Pathological Narcissism Inventory.

Narcissistic Narcissistic
Grandiosity Vulnerability

Self-
Grandiose Sacrificing Contingent Entitlement- Hiding
Exploitative Devaluing the Self
Fantasy Enhancement Self-Esteem Rage

187
Figure 2. Scree plot of eigenvalues from an exploratory factor analysis of items from the relationship initiation motives inventory.

188
Figure 3. Path analysis model for predicting the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales from the Narcissistic Personality
Inventory subscales.

Intimacy e1

Leadership/
Authority Coping/ e2
Self-Affirmation

Grandiose
Exhibitionism

Hedonism e3
Entitlement/
Exploitativeness

Enhancement e4

189
Figure 4. Path analysis model for predicting the Relationship Initiation Motives Inventory subscales from the Pathological Narcissism
Inventory subscales.

Intimacy e1

Coping/ e2
Grandiosity Self-Affirmation

Hedonism e3
Vulnerability

Enhancement e4

190
Figure 5. The Interpersonal Circumplex.

PA
Assured-Dominant

BC NO
Arrogant- Gregarious-
Calculating Extraverted

DE Cold-Hearted Warm-Agreeable LM

Unassuming-
Aloof-
Ingenious
Introverted
JK
FG
Unassured-
Submissive
HI

191
Figure 6. Diagram of a single-variable mediational model.

Panel A

e1

c
IV DV

DV = i1 + cIV + e1

_______________________________________________________________

Panel B
e3

M
a b

e2

c′
IV DV

DV = i2 + c′ IV + bM + e2

M = i3 + aIV + e3

Note. IV = Independent variable. DV = Outcome variable. M = Mediating variable. Equations


taken from MacKinnon (2008, pp. 48-49).

192
Figure 7. Mediational Model for Grandiose Narcissism-Attraction Link.

e1

Participants’ c Dates’ Attraction to


Grandiose Narcissism Participant

e3

Dates’ Perception of
Gregarious-
Extraversion b
a

e2

Participants’ c′ Dates’ Attraction to


Grandiose Narcissism Participant

193
APPENDICIES

194
APPENDIX 1: STUDY 1 MATERIALS

Description of Study to Participants

Participants MUST be SINGLE (that is, not currently involved in a romantic relationship) to

participate. Moreover, they MUST be at least 18 years old.

Materials for Study 1

Narcissistic Personality Inventory-40


Read each pair of statements and then choose the one that is closer to your own feelings and
beliefs. Indicate your answer by circling the letter "A" or "B" to the left of each item. Please do
not skip any items.

1. A I have a natural talent for influencing people. [Leadership/Authority]

B I am not good at influencing people.

2. A Modesty doesn't become me.

B I am essentially a modest person.

3. A I would do almost anything on a dare.

B I tend to be a fairly cautious person.

4. A When people compliment me I sometimes get embarrassed.

B I know that I am good because everybody keeps telling me so. [Grandiose


Exhibitionism]

5. A The thought of ruling the world frightens the hell out of me.

B If I ruled the world it would be a much better place. [Leadership/Authority]

6. A I can usually talk my way out of anything.

B I try to accept the consequences of my behavior.

7. A I prefer to blend in with the crowd.

B I like to be the center of attention. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

8. A I will be a success.

B I am not too concerned about success.

195
9. A I am no better or no worse than most people.

B I think I am a special person.

10. A I am not sure if I would make a good leader.

B I see myself as a good leader. [Leadership/Authority]

11. A I am assertive. [Leadership/Authority]

B I wish I were more assertive.

12. A I like having authority over people. [Leadership/Authority]

B I don't mind following orders.

13. A I find it easy to manipulate people. [Entitlement/Exploitativeness]

B I don't like it when I find myself manipulating people.

14. A I insist upon getting the respect that is due me. [Entitlement/Exploitativeness]

B I usually get the respect that I deserve.

15. A I don't particularly like to show off my body.

B I like to display my body. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

16. A I can read people like a book.

B People are sometimes hard to understand.

17. A If I feel competent I am willing to take responsibility for making decisions.

B I like to take responsibility for making decisions.

18. A I just want to be reasonably happy.

B I want to amount to something in the eyes of the world.

19. A My body is nothing special.

B I like to look at my body. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

20. A I try not to be a show off.

B I am apt to show off if I get the chance. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

21. A I always know what I am doing.

196
B Sometimes I am not sure of what I am doing.

22. A I sometimes depend on people to get things done.

B I rarely depend on anyone else to get things done.

23. A Sometimes I tell good stories.

B Everybody likes to hear my stories.

24. A I expect a great deal from other people. [Entitlement/Exploitativeness]

B I like to do things for other people.

25. A I will never be satisfied until I get all that I deserve. [Entitlement/Exploitativeness]

B I take my satisfactions as they come.

26. A Compliments embarrass me.

B I like to be complimented. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

27. A I have a strong will to power. [Leadership/Authority]

B Power for its own sake doesn't interest me.

28. A I don't very much care about new fads and fashions.

B I like to start new fads and fashions. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

29. A I like to look at myself in the mirror. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

B I am not particularly interested in looking at myself in the mirror.

30. A I really like to be the center of attention. [Grandiose Exhibitionism]

B It makes me uncomfortable to be the center of attention.

31. A I can live my life in any way I want to.

B People can't always live their lives in terms of what they want.

32. A Being an authority doesn't mean that much to me.

B People always seem to recognize my authority. [Leadership/Authority]

33. A I would prefer to be a leader. [Leadership/Authority]

B It makes little difference to me whether I am a leader or not.

197
34. A I am going to be a great person. [Leadership/Authority]

B I hope I am going to be successful.

35. A People sometimes believe what I tell them.

B I can make anybody believe anything I want them to.

36. A I am a born leader. [Leadership/Authority]

B Leadership is a quality that takes a long time to develop.

37. A I wish somebody would someday write my biography.

B I don't like people to pry into my life for any reason.

38. A I get upset when people don't notice how I look when I go out in public. [Grandiose

Exhibitionism]

B I don't mind blending into the crowd when I go out in public.

39. A I am more capable than other people.

B There is a lot that I can learn from other people.

40. A I am much like everybody else.

B I am an extraordinary person. [Leadership/Authority]

Pathological Narcissism Inventory

Instructions: Below you will find 52 descriptive statements. Please consider each one and
indicate how well that statement describes you. There are no right or wrong answers. On your
scantron sheet, fill in only one answer. Simply indicate how well each statement describes you
as a person on the following 6-point scale:

0 1 2 3 4 5
Not at all Moderately A little A little Moderately Very much
Like me Unlike me Unlike me Like me Like me Like me

1. I often fantasize about being admired and respected. [Grandiose Fantasy]

2. My self-esteem fluctuates a lot. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

198
3. I sometimes feel ashamed about my expectations of others when they disappoint me.
[Devaluing]

4. I can usually talk my way out of anything. [Exploitativeness]

5. It’s hard for me to feel good about myself when I’m alone. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

6. I can make myself feel good by caring for others. [Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement]

7. I hate asking for help. [Hiding the Self]

8. When people don’t notice me, I start to feel bad about myself. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

9. I often hide my needs for fear that others will see me as needy and dependent. [Hiding
the Self]

10. I can make anyone believe anything I want them to. [Exploitativeness]

11. I get mad when people don’t notice all that I do for them. [Entitlement-Rage]

12. I get annoyed by people who are not interested in what I say or do. [Entitlement-Rage]

13. I wouldn’t disclose all my intimate thoughts and feelings to someone I didn’t admire.
[Hiding the Self]

14. I often fantasize about having a huge impact on the world around me. [Grandiose
Fantasy]

15. I find it easy to manipulate people. [Exploitativeness]

16. When others don’t notice me, I start to feel worthless. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

17. Sometimes I avoid people because I’m concerned that they’ll disappoint me. [Devaluing]

199
18. I typically get very angry when I’m unable to get what I want from others. [Entitlement-
Rage]

19. I sometimes need important others in my life to reassure me of my self-worth.


[Contingent Self-Esteem]

20. When I do things for other people, I expect them to do things for me. [Entitlement-
Rage]

21. When others don’t meet my expectations, I often feel ashamed about what I wanted.
[Devaluing]

22. I feel important when others rely on me. [Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement]

23. I can read people like a book. [Exploitativeness]

24. When others disappoint me, I often get angry at myself. [Devaluing]

25. Sacrificing for others makes me the better person. [Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement]

26. I often fantasize about accomplishing things that are probably beyond my means.
[Grandiose Fantasy]

27. Sometimes I avoid people because I’m afraid they won’t do what I want them to do.
[Devaluing]

28. It’s hard to show others the weaknesses I feel inside. [Hiding the Self]

29. I get angry when criticized. [Entitlement-Rage]

30. It’s hard to feel good about myself unless I know other people admire me. [Contingent
Self-Esteem]

200
31. I often fantasize about being rewarded for my efforts. [Grandiose Fantasy]

32. I am preoccupied with thoughts and concerns that most people are not interested in me.
[Contingent Self-Esteem]

33. I like to have friends who rely on me because it makes me feel important. [Self-
Sacrificing Self-Enhancement]

34. Sometimes I avoid people because I’m concerned they won’t acknowledge what I do for
them. [Devaluing]

35. Everybody likes to hear my stories. [Exploitativeness]

36. It’s hard for me to feel good about myself unless I know other people like me.
[Contingent Self-Esteem]

37. It irritates me when people don’t notice how good a person I am. [Entitlement-Rage]

38. I will never be satisfied until I get all that I deserve. [Entitlement-Rage]

39. I try to show what a good person I am through my sacrifices. [Self-Sacrificing Self-
Enhancement]

40. I am disappointed when people don’t notice me. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

41. I often find myself envying others’ accomplishments. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

42. I often fantasize about performing heroic deeds. [Grandiose Fantasy]

43. I help others in order to prove I’m a good person. [Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement]

44. It’s important to show people I can do it on my own even if I have some doubts inside.
[Hiding the Self]

201
45. I often fantasize about being recognized for my accomplishments. [Grandiose Fantasy]

46. I can’t stand relying on other people because it makes me feel weak. [Hiding the Self]

47. When others don’t respond to me the way that I would like them to, it is hard for me to
still feel ok with myself. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

48. I need others to acknowledge me. [Contingent Self-Esteem]

49. I want to amount to something in the eyes of the world. [Grandiose Fantasy]

50. When others get a glimpse of my needs, I feel anxious and ashamed. [Hiding the Self]

51. Sometimes it’s easier to be alone than to face not getting everything I want from other
people. [Devaluing]

52. I can get pretty angry when others disagree with me. [Entitlement-Rage]

Experience in Close Relationships Scale (ECR)

Instructions: The following statements concern how you generally feel in close relationships
(e.g., with romantic partners, close friends, or family members). Respond to each statement by
indicating how much you agree or disagree with it. Write the number in the space provided,
using the following rating scale:

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Disagree Disagree Neutral/ Agree Agree


Disagree Agree
Strongly Slightly Mixed Slightly Strongly

___ 1. I prefer not to show others how I feel deep down. [Avoidance]

___ 2. I worry about being rejected or abandoned.

202
___ 3. I am very comfortable being close to other people. [Avoidance]

___ 4. I worry a lot about my relationships.

___ 5. Just when someone starts to get close to me I find myself pulling away. [Avoidance]

___ 6. I worry that others won’t care about me as much as I care about them.

___ 7. I get uncomfortable when someone wants to be very close to me. [Avoidance]

___ 8. I worry a fair amount about losing my close relationship partners.

___ 9. I don’t feel comfortable opening up to others. [Avoidance]

___ 10. I often wish that close relationship partners’ feelings for me were as strong as my
feelings for them.

___ 11. I want to get close to others, but I keep pulling back. [Avoidance]

___ 12. I want to get very close to others, and this sometimes scares them away.

___ 13. I am nervous when another person gets too close to me. [Avoidance]

___ 14. I worry about being alone.

___ 15. I feel comfortable sharing my private thoughts and feelings with others. [Avoidance]

___ 16. My desire to be very close sometimes scares people away.

___ 17. I try to avoid getting too close to others. [Avoidance]

___ 18. I need a lot of reassurance that close relationship partners really care about me.

___ 19. I find it relatively easy to get close to others. [Avoidance]

___ 20. Sometimes I feel that I try to force others to show more feeling, more commitment to our
relationship than they otherwise would.

___ 21. I find it difficult to allow myself to depend on close relationship partners. [Avoidance]

___ 22. I do not often worry about being abandoned.

___ 23. I prefer not to be too close to others. [Avoidance]

___ 24. If I can’t get a relationship partner to show interest in me, I get upset or angry.

___ 25. I tell my close relationship partners just about everything. [Avoidance]

___ 26. I find that my partners don’t want to get as close as I would like.

203
___ 27. I usually discuss my problems and concerns with close others. [Avoidance]

___ 28. When I don’t have close others around, I feel somewhat anxious and insecure.

___ 29. I feel comfortable depending on others. [Avoidance]

___ 30. I get frustrated when my close relationship partners are not around as much as I would
like.

___ 31. I don’t mind asking close others for comfort, advice, or help. [Avoidance]

___ 32. I get frustrated if relationship partners are not available when I need them.

___ 33. It helps to turn to close others in times of need. [Avoidance]

___ 34. When other people disapprove of me, I feel really bad about myself.

___ 35. I turn to close relationship partners for many things, including comfort and reassurance.
[Avoidance]

___ 36. I resent it when my relationship partners spend time away from me.

Demographics

1) What is your gender? (1=Male; 2=Female)


2) How old are you? (1 = Less than 18 years old; 2 = 18 years old; 3 = 19 years old; 4 = 20
years old; 5 = 21 years old or older)
3) How would you describe your ethnicity? (1 = Black or African American; 2 = White (Not
Hispanic/Not Latino); 3 = Hispanic or Latino; 4 = American Indian; 5 = Asian; 6 =
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander; 7 = Multiracial (Having parents of more than one
race)
4) What is your present religious orientation? (1=Protestant/Christian; 2=Jewish;
3=Catholic; 4=Islamic; 5=No affiliation; 6=Other)
5) What is your current relationship status? (1 = Single (not dating); 2 = Dating casually; 3
= Dating exclusively; 4 = Engaged; 5 = Married; 6 = Separated; 7 = Living with
significant other; 8 = Divorced; 9 = Widowed)
6) Have you ever been divorced? (1=Yes; 2=No)
7) How would you describe your political orientation? (1=Very Conservative;
2=Conservative; 3=Slightly Conservative; 4=Moderate (partly Conservative and partly
Liberal); 5=Slightly Liberal; 6=Liberal; 7=Very Liberal).
8) How would you describe your sexual orientation? (1=Heterosexual, 2=Bisexual,
3=Homosexual)
9) I answered these questions honestly: (1=Yes; 2=No)

204
Relationship Initiation Motives Measure: (Adapted from Cooper, Shapiro, & Powers, 1998)

Instructions: Below is a series of statements that describe reasons that people may have for
beginning romantic relationships. For each statement, please use the following scale to indicate
how much you agree with each of the statements.

Response Scale:
1 = Strongly Disagree
2 = Somewhat Disagree
3 = Neither agree nor disagree
4 = Somewhat Agree
5 = Strongly Disagree
I often begin romantic relationships with new partners …

1) Because I want to be intimate with the other person [Old Intimacy Motive]
2) Because I feel “horny” [Hedonism Motive]
3) To prove to myself that the other person thinks I’m attractive [Self-Affirmation
Motive]
4) To cope with upset feelings [Coping Motive]
5) To express my affection to the other person [Old Intimacy Motive]
6) Because it feels good [Hedonism Motive]
7) Because it makes me feel like I’m a more interesting person [Self-Affirmation
Motive]
8) To help me deal with disappointment in my life [Coping Motive]
9) To make an emotional connection with the other person [Old Intimacy Motive]
10) Just for the excitement of it [Hedonism Motive]
11) Because it makes me feel more self-confident [Self-Affirmation Motive]
12) Because it helps me feel better when I’m lonely [Coping Motive]
13) To become closer with the other person [Old Intimacy Motive]
14) Just for the thrill of it [Hedonism Motive]
15) To reassure myself that I am desirable [Self-Affirmation Motive]
16) Because it helps me feel better when I’m feeling low [Coping Motive]
17) To feel emotionally close to the other person [Old Intimacy Motive]
18) To satisfy my sexual needs [Hedonism Motive]
19) To help me feel better about myself [Self-Affirmation Motive]
20) To cheer myself up [Coping Motive]
21) To be admired [Enhancement Motive]
22) To impress others [Enhancement Motive]
23) To receive compliments [Enhancement Motive]

205
24) Because they remind me I’m a great person [Enhancement Motive]
25) To have other person treat me like the center of his/her world [Enhancement
Motive]
26) To better understand the other person [New Intimacy Motive]
27) Because I want to open up to the other person [New Intimacy Motive]
28) To feel close with the other person [New Intimacy Motive]
29) To have another person truly understand me [New Intimacy Motive]
30) Because I want to feel connected to the other person [New Intimacy Motive]

The Remote Associations Test (RAT) (from Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003)

Condition RAT item Answer

(10 easy items) Cream/skate/water Ice


Cane/daddy/plum Sugar
Political/surprise/line Party
Night/wrist/stop Watch
Duck/fold/dollar Bill
Rocking/wheel/high Chair
Dew/comb/bee Honey
Fountain/baking/pop Soda
Preserve/ranger/tropical Forest
Measure/worm/video Tape

(7 difficult items) Land/hand/house Farm


Hungry/order/belt Money
Forward/flush/razor Straight
Way/ground/weather Fair
Cast/side/jump Broad
Reading/service/stick Lip
Over/plant/horse Power

(7 moderate difficulty items) Dust/cereal/fish Bowl


Palm/shoe/house Tree
Wheel/hand/shopping Cart
Mill/tooth/dust Saw
Fly/clip/wall Paper
Wagon/break/radio Station
Dress/dial/flower Sun

206
The State Self-Esteem Scale (from Heatherton & Polivy, 1991)

Instructions: This is a questionnaire designed to measure what you are thinking at this moment.
There is, of course, no right answer for any statement. The best answer is what you feel is true of
yourself at this moment. Be sure to answer all of the items, even if you are not certain of the best
answer. Again, answer these questions as they are true for you RIGHT NOW.

Each item is scored on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 2 = a little bit, 3 = somewhat, 4 = very
much, and 5 = extremely).

1) I feel confident about my abilities


2) I am worried about whether I am regarded as a success or failure (Reverse)
3) I feel satisfied with the way my body looks right now
4) I feel frustrated or rattled about my performance (Reverse)
5) I feel that I am having trouble understanding things that I read (Reverse)
6) I feel that others respect and admire me
7) I am dissatisfied with my weight (Reverse)
8) I feel self-conscious (Reverse)
9) I feel as smart as others
10) I feel displeased with myself (Reverse)
11) I feel good about myself
12) I am pleased with my appearance right now
13) I am worried about what other people think of me (Reverse)
14) I feel confident that I understand things
15) I feel inferior to others at this moment (Reverse)
16) I feel unattractive (Reverse)
17) I feel concerned about the impression I am making (Reverse)
18) I feel that I have less scholastic ability right now than others (Reverse)
19) I feel like I’m not doing well (Reverse)
20) I am worried about looking foolish (Reverse)

207
Romantic Targets Stimuli (from Campbell, 1999, dissertation)

Instructions: Please read the following passage carefully. You will be asked some questions
related to the passage afterward.

Imagine the following situation: You are at a party with some people you know and some
people you don’t know. A friend of yours introduces you to three people of the opposite
sex who are single—we’ll call them by their initials: J.G., C.F., and B.W. Later, you run
into your friend in the kitchen and she tells you about these people in more detail. Your
friend describes the three single people you met— J.G., C.F., and B.W.—as follows…

Participants are then presented with the three following person descriptions in random order:

Person Description 1

J.G. told me that he/she thought you were terrific! J.G. thought you were the best looking person
at the party and that you were very charming. J.G. also thought you were one of the most
intelligent people he/she had ever met.

Person Description 2

C.F. is really a great person. C.F. is very popular and good-looking. He/she is also one of the top
students at the school and is rated as one of the best soccer players in the nation.

Person Description 3

B.W. is a really caring person. B.W. told me that he/she’s looking for a person to date with
whom he/she could be close, intimate friends. B.W. is one of the most sensitive, sharing,
compassionate, and friendly people I know.

Instructions: Please answer the following questions based on what your friend in the kitchen
told you about J.G.

1) How attractive do you find this person?


1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
2) How desirable would you find this person as a dating partner?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
3) How much would you actually like to date this person?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
4) How would you feel about yourself if you were dating this person?
1 (very bad) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very good)
5) How do you think your friends would feel about you if you were dating this person?
1 (disapproving of me) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (approving of me)

208
Instructions: Please answer the following questions based on what your friend in the kitchen
told you about C.F.

1) How attractive do you find this person?


1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
2) How desirable would you find this person as a dating partner?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
3) How much would you actually like to date this person?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
4) How would you feel about yourself if you were dating this person?
1 (very bad) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very good)
5) How do you think your friends would feel about you if you were dating this person?
1 (disapproving of me) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (approving of me)

Instructions: Please answer the following questions based on what your friend in the kitchen
told you about B.W.

1) How attractive do you find this person?


1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
2) How desirable would you find this person as a dating partner?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
3) How much would you actually like to date this person?
1 (not at all) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very)
4) How would you feel about yourself if you were dating this person?
1 (very bad) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (very good)
5) How do you think your friends would feel about you if you were dating this person?
1 (disapproving of me) 2 3 4 5 6 7 (approving of me)

Instructions: Please answer the following question based on what you have heard about B.W.,
C.F., and J.G.

1) Which of the people would you most like to meet? (please choose one)
a. J.G.
b. C.F.
c. B.W.

209
APPENDIX 2: STUDY 2 MATERIALS

Narcissistic Personality Inventory-40 (same scale as Study 1)

Pathological Narcissism Inventory (same scale as Study 1)

Demographics (same questions as Study 1)

Ideal Partner-Scale (developed for current study)

Instructions: Please use the following scale to describe how well the following characteristics
describe your ideal romantic partner

Response Scale:

1 = Extremely Inaccurate
2
3
4 = Somewhat Accurate
5
6
7 = Extremely Accurate

1) Admires me
2) Is cunning
3) Is interested in me
4) Is good looking
5) Is unsympathetic
6) Loves my company
7) Is charming
8) Is outgoing
9) Is unaggressive
10) Is popular
11) Is friendly
12) Is caring
13) Is tenderhearted
14) Is dominant
15) Is warmthless
16) Is sly
17) Is assertive
18) Is shy
19) Is compassionate
20) Is narcissistic

210
21) Is gentlehearted

211
Interaction Record
Please circle your answer to the following questions on the person with whom you just
interacted.

Definitely Maybe Definitely


No Yes
1. Do you like this person?
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2. Are you attracted to this person?


1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3. Would you like to date this person?


1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4. Do you think this person is


desirable? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5. Do you think this person is attracted


to you? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6. Did you feel a real connection?


1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7. Was there ‘sexual’ chemistry?


1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8. Did you bond with your interaction


partner? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

9. Were there sparks between you and


your partner? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

10. Were you skeptical of your


interaction partner? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Very Somewhat A Great


Little Deal
11. I disclosed…
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

212
12. My partner disclosed…
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

13. How much did your partner


understand you? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

14. Did the other person see you as


acceptable? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Instructions: Please rate how this person behaved with you today using the rating scales
provided below.

Submissive Dominant
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Submissive behavior involves being passive and looking to others to take control or lead.
Dominant behavior involves being assertive, controlling, aggressive, and in charge.

Cold Warm
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Cold behavior involves being uninterested in others, unfriendly, and even quarrelsome.
Warm behavior involves being kind, friendly, and close to others.

213
Interaction Record

Please circle your answer to the following statements on the person with whom you just
interacted.

Extremely Somewhat Extremely


My Interaction Partner…
Inaccurate Accurate Accurate
1. admires me
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

2. is cunning
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

3. is interested in me
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

4. is good looking
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

5. is unsympathetic
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

6. loves my company
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

7. is charming
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8. is outgoing
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

9. is unaggressive
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

10. is popular
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

11. is friendly
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

12. is caring
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

13. is tenderhearted
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

14. is dominant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

214
15. is warmthless
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

16. is sly
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

17. is assertive
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

18. is shy
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

19. is compassionate
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

20. is narcissistic
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

21. is gentlehearted
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

215
APPENDIX 3: SCRIPT USED IN STUDY 2

Script used in Study 2 (note that the following script borrows heavily from the script used by Eli

Finkel):

Hello, and welcome to the Personality in Social Interactions Study. My name is ____,

and I’m one of the research assistants in the RASP Lab. We’re very happy to see you

guys here.

We are interested in how the romantic attraction process works. In the field of close

relationships, researchers have become increasingly interested in the processes that occur

in short-meetings between persons—situations like speed-dating, for example. For this

second part of the study, we have therefore chosen to use a set-up that is in many ways

similar to speed-dating and we ask you to treat each of the interactions you have tonight

as an actual speed-date. Please note, however, that we will not be providing you with

anyone’s phone number or other contact information after the event.

You will notice that we are asking you to complete a number of questionnaires as part of

the study. If you guys take the questionnaires seriously, this study will be very

informative. As such, I hope you view yourself the way I view you: not as a research

subject, but rather as a collaborator on a major research project. We simply couldn’t

conduct this research without your active collaboration.

OK, here’s a brief overview of what will happen here this evening. You will meet every

opposite-sex speed-dater here today for 5 minutes each. After each speed-date, you will

be given about 5 minutes to fill out a brief questionnaire about it. We will warn you when

216
there is a minute left for each date and then ring a bell after each date to signal that time

is up and it’s time to move on. The entire event will take approximately 2 hours.

You will notice that all of you are wearing name-tags with unique numbers. The

numbers will be how you refer to one another on the questionnaires. Women [or men]

will remain seated at the tables situated around the room; men [or women] will rotate

clockwise after each round until you’ve had a chance to date everybody here.

Now let’s talk about the questionnaires. You will see that all the questionnaires are the

same—each is two pages long and they ask you to answer some questions about the

interaction that you just had. First, put in the number of the name-tag of the person you

just interacted with at the top. Then proceed to fill out the questions. Please be as honest

as possible on these questionnaires—no one besides the research team will ever see what

you put down here.

Are there any questions about how the process will work?

As you can see, this room is set up with a group of desks in the center and a group of

desks along with sides of the room. In a moment, I will be asking each of the women to

take a seat at one of the desks along the sides of the room. I will then be asking each of

the men to take a seat with one of the women. After the five minutes is up, I will ask the

men to take a seat back at the center of the room. At this point, I will ask you to complete

the interaction record on the person that you were just with. After everyone has

completed the interaction record, I will ask the men in the center of the room to rotate

clockwise to the next woman along the side of the room. You will continue this process

until each woman has interacted with each man. Does anyone have any questions on this

217
process?

218
REFERENCES

219
REFERENCES

Ackerman, R. A., & Donnellan, M. B. (2011). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory


Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscale: Further evidence of its psychometric properties,
validity, and research utility. Unpublished Manuscript.

Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, M. B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., & Kashy,
D. A. (2011). What does the Narcissistic Personality Inventory really measure?
Assessment, 18, 67-87.

Aron, A., Melinat, E., Aron, E. N., Vallone, R. D., & Bator, R. J. (1997). The experimental
generation of interpersonal closeness: A procedure and some preliminary findings.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363-377.

Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2010). Why are narcissists so charming at first
sight? Decoding the narcissism-popularity link at zero-acquaintance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 132-145.

Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social
psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., & Killian, A. L. (2003). The relation of narcissism and self-esteem to
conduct problems in children: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 32 (1), 139-152.

Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect
effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: New procedures and
recommendations. Psychological Methods, 11, 142-163.

Bishop, J., & Lane, R. C. (2002). The dynamics and dangers of entitlement. Psychoanalytic
Psychology, 19, 739-758.

Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate
problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 634-639.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Parent-Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development.
New York: Basic Books.

Bredow, C. A., Cate, R. M., & Huston, T. L. (2008). Have we met before? A conceptual model
of first romantic encounters. In S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel, & J. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of
Relationship Initiation. New York: Psychology Press.

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult romantic

220
attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.),
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford Press.

Bresin, K., & Gordon, K. H. (2011). Characterizing pathological narcissism in terms of the
HEXACO model of personality. Journal of Psychopathology Behavioral Assessment, 33,
228-235.

Brown, R. P., Budzek, K., & Tamborski, M. (2009). On the meaning and measure of narcissism.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 951-964.

Brown, M. B., & Forsythe, A. B. (1974a). Robust tests for equality of variance. Journal of the
American Statistical Association, 69, 364-367.

Brown, M. B., & Forsythe, A. B. (1974b). The small sample behavior of some statistics which
test the equality of several means. Technometrics, 16, 129-132.

Brunswick, E. (1947). Systematic and representative design of psychological experiments.


Berkeley: University of California Press.

Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Susceptibility to infidelity in the first year of
marriage. Journal of Research in Personality, 31 (2), 193-221.

Cain, N. M., Pincus, A. L., & Ansell, E. B. (2008). Narcissism at the crossroads: Phenotypic
description of pathological narcissism across clinical theory, social/personality
psychology, and psychiatric diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 638-656.

Campbell, W. K. (1998). Narcissism and romantic attraction. Dissertation Abstracts


International, 58, 12B, p. 6866.

Campbell, W. K. (1999). Narcissism and romantic attraction. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 77, 1254-1270.

Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., & Finkel, E. J. (2006). Narcissism, interpersonal self-
regulation, and romantic relationships: An agency model approach. In E. J. Finkel & K.
D. Vohs (Eds.), Intrapersonal processes, interpersonal relationships. New York:
Guilford.

Campbell, W. K., & Foster, C. A. (2002). Narcissism and commitment in romantic relationships:
An investment model analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 484-495.

Campbell, W. K., Foster, C. A., & Finkel, E. J. (2002). Does self-love lead to love for others? A
story of narcissistic game playing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
340-354.

Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective
responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of

221
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333.

Clark, C. L., Shaver, P. R., & Abrahams, M. F. (1999). Strategic behaviors in romantic
relationship initiation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 707-720.

Cooper, M. L., Shapiro, C. M., & Powers, A. M. (1998). Motivations for sex and risky sexual
behavior among adolescents and young adults: A functional perspective. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1528-1558.

Corry, N., Merritt, R. D., Mrug, S., & Pamp, B. (2008). The factor structure of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90 (6), 593-600.

Crocker, J., Thompson, L. L., McGraw, K. M., & Ingerman, C. (1987). Downward comparison,
prejudice, and evaluations of others: Effects of self-esteem and threat. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 907-916.

Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vulnerable
narcissism. Journal of Personality Disorders, 17, 188-207.

del Rosario, P. M., & White, R. M. (2005). The Narcissistic Personality Inventory: Test-retest
stability and internal consistency. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1075-1081.

Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women
and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735-754.

Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Speed-dating: A powerful and flexible paradigm for
studying romantic relationship initiation. In S. Sprecher, A. Wenzel, & J. Harvey (Eds.),
Handbook of Relationship Initiation, New York: Taylor & Francis Group.

Ellis, H. (1898). Auto-eroticism: A psychological study. Alienist and Neurologist, 19, 260-299.

Emmons, R. A. (1984). Factor analysis and construct validity of the narcissistic personality
inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48 (3), 291-300.

Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52 (1), 11-17.

Emmons, R. A. (1989). Exploring the relations between motives and traits: The case of
narcissism. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds). Personality Psychology: Recent Trends
and Emerging Directions. New York: Springer-Verlag Publishing.

Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use
of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272-
299.

Finkel, E. J., & Eastwick, P. W. (2009). Arbitrary social norms influence sex differences in

222
romantic selectivity. Psychological Science, 20, 1290-1295.

Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., & Matthews, J. (2007). Speed-dating as an invaluable tool for
studying romantic attraction: A methodological primer. Personal Relationships, 14, 149-
166.

Foster, J. D., & Trimm IV, R. F. (2008). On being eager and uninhibited: Narcissism and
approach-avoidance motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 (7),
1004-1017.

Freud, S. (1914). On narcissism: An introduction. In A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential Papers on


Narcissism (1986). New York: New York University Press.

Friedman, J. N. W., Oltmanns, T. F., Gleason, M. E. J., & Turkheimer, E. (2006). Mixed
impressions: Reactions of strangers to people with pathological personality traits. Journal
of Research in Personality, 40, 395-410.

Friedman, J. N. W., Oltmanns, T. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2007). Interpersonal perception and
personality disorders: Utilization of a thin slice approach. Journal of Research in
Personality, 41, 667-688.

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.

Havlicek, J., Roberts, S. C., & Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant males’ odour:
Effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biology Letters, 1, 256-259.

Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state
self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 895-910.

Heatherton, T. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2000). Interpersonal evaluations following threats to self: Role
of self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 725-736.

Hepper, E. G., Gramzow, R. H., & Sedikides, C. (2010). Individual differences in self-
enhancement and self-protection strategies: An integrative analysis. Journal of
Personality, 78, 781-814.

Hill, P. L., & Roberts, B. W. (in press). Narcissism, well-being, and observer-rated personality
across the lifespan. Social Psychological and Personality Science.

Jarry, J. L., & Kossert, A. L. (2007). Self-esteem threat combined with exposure to thin media
images leads to body image compensatory self-enhancement. Body Image, 4, 39-50.

Kenny, D. A. (1998). BLOCKO: Version 6 [Unpublished computer program]. University of


Connecticut, Storrs.

223
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic Data Analysis. New York: The
Guilford Press.

Kenny, D. A., Korchmaros, J. D., & Bolger, N. (2003). Lower level mediation in multilevel
models. Psychological Methods, 8, 115-128.

Kenrick, D. T., Trost, M. R., & Sheets, V. L. (1996). Power, harassment, and trophy mates: The
feminist advantage of an evolutionary perspective. In D. M. Buss & N. M. Malamuth,
Sex, power, conflict: Evolutionary and feminist perspectives (pp. 29-53). New York:
Oxford University Press.

Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderline Conditions and Pathological Narcissism. New York: Jason
Aronson.

Kernberg, P. F. (1998). Developmental Aspects of Normal and Pathological Narcissism. In E. F.


Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of Narcissism: Diagnostic, Clinical, and Empirical
Implications (1998). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.

Kernberg, O. F. (1998). Pathological Narcissism and Narcissistic Personality Disorder:


Theoretical Background and Diagnostic Classification. In E. F. Ronningstam (Ed.),
Disorders of Narcissism: Diagnostic, Clinical, and Empirical Implications (1998).
Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.

Kohut, H. (1966). Forms and transformations of narcissism. In Self Psychology and the
Humanities: Reflections on a New Psychoanalytic Approach (1985). Edited by C. B.
Strozier. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.

Krull, J. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (1999). Multilevel mediation modeling in group-based


intervention studies. Evaluation Review, 23, 418-444.

Krull, J. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2001). Multilevel modeling of individual and group level
mediated effects. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36, 249-277.

Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal


process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner
responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1238-1251.

Levy, K. N., Reynoso, J. S., Wasserman, R. H., & Clarkin, J. F. (2006). Narcissistic personality
disorder. In W. O’Donohue & S. O. Lilienfield (Eds.). Sage Handbook of Personality
Disorders (pp. 233-277). New York: Sage Press.

MacKinnon, D. P. (2008). Introduction to Statistical Mediation Analysis. New York: Lawrence


Erlbaum Associates.

Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69,

224
220-232.

Miller, J. D., & Campbell, W. K. (2008). Comparing clinical and social-personality


conceptualizations of narcissism. Journal of Personality, 76 (3), 449-476.

Millon, T. (1996). Disorders of Personality: DSM-IV and Beyond. New York: Wiley.

Mongeau, P. A., Serewicz, M. C., & Therrien, L. F. (2004). Goals for cross-sex first dates:
Identification, measurement, and the influence of contextual factors. Communication
Monographs, 71, 121-147.

Oltmanns, T. F., Friedman, J. N. W., Fiedler, E. R., & Turkheimer, E. (2004). Perceptions of
people with personality disorders based on thin slices of behavior. Journal of Research in
Personality, 38, 216-229.

Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: A


mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1197-1208.

Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N.
(2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory.
Psychological Assessment, 21 (3), 365-379.

Pincus, A. L., & Lukowitsky, M. R. (2010). Pathological narcissism and narcissistic personality
disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 421-446.

Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A narcissistic personality inventory. Psychological Reports,
45 (2), 590.

Raskin, R. N., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic


Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (5), 890-902.

Rhodewalt, F. & Morf, C. C. (1995). Self and interpersonal correlates of the Narcissistic
Personality Inventory. Journal of Research in Personality, 29, 1-23.

Sedikides, C., & Luke, M. (2008). On when self-enhancement and self-criticism function
adaptively and maladaptively. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Self-criticism and self-enhancement:
Theory, research, and clinical implications. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological
Association.

Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation
models. Sociological Methodology, 13, 290-312.

Stone, M. H. (1998). Normal Narcissism: An Etiological and Ethological Perspective. In E. F.


Ronningstam (Ed.), Disorders of Narcissism: Diagnostic, Clinical, and Empirical
Implications (1998). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.

225
Tenney, E. R., & Spellman, B. A. (2011). Complex social consequences of self-knowledge.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 343-350.

Tritt, S. M., Ryder, A. G., Ring, A. J., & Pincus, A. L. (2010). Pathological narcissism and the
depressive temperament. Journal of Affective Disorders, 122, 280-284.

Vazire, S., Naumann, L. P., Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2008). Portrait of a narcissist:
Manifestations of narcissism in physical appearance. Journal of Research in Personality,
42, 1439-1447.

Vonk, R. (2002). Self-serving interpretations of flattery: Why ingratiation works. Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 515-526.

Watson, P. J., & Biderman, M. D. (1993). Narcissistic Personality Inventory factors, splitting and
self-consciousness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 61, 41-57.

Watson, P. J., Little, T., Sawrie, S. M., & Biderman, M. D. (1992). Measures of the narcissistic
personality: Complexity of relationships with self-esteem and empathy. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 6 (4), 434-449.

Watson, P. J., & Morris, R. J. (1991). Narcissism, empathy and social desirability. Personality
and Individual Differences, 12 (6), 575-579.

Wiggins, J. S., Trapnell, P., & Phillips, N. (1988). Psychometric and geometric characteristics of
the Revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS-R). Multivariate Behavioral Research,
23, 517-530.

Wright, A. G. C., Lukowitsky, M. R., Pincus, A. L., & Conroy, D. E. (2010). The higher order
factor structure and gender invariance of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory.
Assessment, 17, 467-483.

Zitek, E. M., Jordan, A. H., Monin, B., & Leach, F. R. (2010). Victim entitlement to behave
selfishly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 245-255.

226

You might also like