You are on page 1of 120

Dr. Yunisrina Qismullah Yusuf, M. Ling.

Phonetics vs. Phonology? (Rangelov,


2005)
Phonetics studies the nature of speech sounds:
• their production by the vocal tract (articulatory phonetics)
• their perception by the auditory system (auditory
phonetics)
• their physical properties as sound waves (acoustic
phonetics)

Phonology studies the ways in which speech sounds form


systems and patterns:
• the relationship between how sounds are pronounced and
how they are stored in the mind
• which phonetic distinctions are significant enough to signal
differences in meaning
• the ways sounds are organized within words
“The ear hears phonetics, but the brain hears
phonology.” - Dennis Preston
That is, your ear is capable of processing whatever linguistic sounds
are given to it (assuming someone with normal hearing), but your
language experience causes your brain to filter out only those sound
patterns that are important to your language(s).
Phonologists are often as interested in patterns related to
the manner of articulation as they are the patterns of the
speech waves.
Phoneticians, meanwhile, would have no way to analyze
their data sets if they didn't have phonological categories
to help organize them.
How does P&P work?
Let’s observe the following:

(1) The first sound the word fight is produced by


bringing together the top teeth and the
bottom lip, and then blowing air between
them.
 illustrates the fact that we use our vocal
tract to produce speech.
How does P&P work?
(2) The word war is produced with one
continuous motion of the lungs, tongue, lips,
and so on, yet we interpret this motion as a
series of three separate speech sounds:
w-a-r.
 illustrates the fact that words are not
physically one continuous motion but are
psychologically a series of discrete units
called segments.
How does P&P work?
(3) The words pea, see, and key all have the
same vowel, even though the vowel in each
word is spelled differently.
 illustrates that a single segment can be
represented by a variety of spellings
 observations (2) and (3) can be used to
justify a phonemic alphabet, a system of
transcription in which one symbol uniquely
represents one segment.
How does P&P work?
(4) p and b are alike in that they are both
pronounced with the lips; p and k are
different in that k is not pronounced with the
lips.
 illustrates the fact that segments are
composed of smaller units called distinctive
features. Thus, “labial” (referring to the lips)
is a distinctive feature shared by p and b, but
not by p and k.
How does P&P work?
(5) The vowels in the words cab and cad are longer than
the same vowels in cap and cat.
 illustrates that two segments can be the same on
one level of representation but different on another.
Thus, the vowels in cab, cad, cap and cat are the
same on one level (the vowel a), but different on
another level (long a in cab and cad; short in cap and
cat).
 These systematic variations between levels of
representation can be stated in terms of phonological
rules (e.g. vowels are lengthened in a particular
context).
Research in P&P
• Look at how sound systems have changed over
periods of time.
• You can examine specific languages to discover
the rules that apply to their phonology and how
these compare to other languages.
• You can also use phonology within a country to
look at specific dialects and compare them to one
another.
• You can study the characteristics of sounds
(vowels and consonants) of a language, dialect,
accent.
Vocal tract
The vocal tract consists of the passageway between
the lips and nostrils on one end and the larynx
(which contains the vocal cord) on the other.

It is important in the study of phonology because:


• it is used to produce speech.
• it refers to the physical properties that are used to
describe the psychological units of phonology.
Vocal tract
1. Lips
2. Teeth
3. Alveolar ridge, the bony ridge
right behind the upper teeth
4. Hard palate, the bony dome
constituting the roof of the mouth
5. Velum (soft palate), the soft tissue
immediately behind the palate
6. Uvula, the soft appendage hanging
off the velum
7, 8, 9, 10. Tongue
11. Epiglottis, the soft tissue which covers
the vocal cords during eating, thus
protecting the passageway to the lungs
12. Pharynx, the back wall of the throat
behind the tongue
13. Larynx, containing the vocal cords
14. Esophagus, the tube going to the stomach
15. Trachea, the tube going to the lungs
Anatomy of vocal organs
Vocal tract
In short, the vocal tract is a tube which produces sound
when air from the lungs is pumped through it.

Different speech sounds are produced by manipulating


the lips, tongue, teeth, velum, pharynx and vocal cords,
thus, changing the shape of this tube.

The primary importance of the vocal tract is the fact that


phonological units and rules are described in terms of
these physical properties of the vocal mechanism.
Phonemic Alphabet
One type of segment that we perceive when we hear speech is
termed the phoneme.

Conventional orthography (e.g. spelling) does not provide an


adequate means of representing the phonological structure of
words.

For example: pea and key both contain the same vowel, but in pea
the vowel is spelled ea and in key is spelled ey. Consequently,
phonemic alphabet is developed where one symbol always
corresponds to a single phoneme.
So, the phonemic alphabet of the vowel in pea, see, me and key
as /i/.
Phonemic Alphabet
Phonemic transcription is always enclosed in
slashes to distinguish it from conventional
orthography.

From the previous example, we perceive all the


words as having the same vowel by
transcribing them as: pea /pi/, see /si/, me
/mi/ and key /ki/.
Phonemic Alphabet of English VOWELS
No. Phonemic Examples No. Phonemic Examples
symbol Symbol

1 /i/ Seat (kursi) 9 /u/ Suit (setelan; sesuai)

2 /ɪ/ Sit (duduk) 10 /ʊ/ Soot (jelaga)

3 /e/ Say (ucap - present) 11 /о/ Sewed (menjahit – past)

4 /ɛ/ Said (ucap – past) 12 /ɔ/ Sought (dicari)

5 /æ/ Sad (sedih) 13 /aɪ/ Sight (penglihatan)

6 /ʌ/ Suds (buih soda) 14 /aʊ/ South (selatan)


(unstressed
7 = Alone, butter
/ə/

8 /a/ Sod (tanah) 15 /ɔɪ/ Soy (kedelai)


Physical dimensions of the phonemes
The vowel phonemes (e.g. percepts – psychological units)
are described in terms of their physical dimensions:
1. Tongue height: for any articulation corresponding to
one of these vowel phonemes, the tongue is either
relatively high in the mouth (/i, ɪ, u, ʊ/), mid (/e, ɛ, ʌ
(ə), о/), or low (/æ, a, ɔ/). Compare see /si/ (high)
and say /se/ (mid).
2. Frontness: for any articulation corresponding to one
of these vowel phonemes, the tongue is either
relatively front (i, ɪ, e, ɛ, æ/) or back (/ʌ (ə), a, u, ʊ,
о, ɔ/). Compare see /si/ (front) and sue /su/ (back).
Physical dimensions of the phonemes
3. Lip Rounding: for any articulation corresponding
to one of these vowels phonemes, the lips are
either relatively round (/u, ʊ, о, ɔ/) or spread (/i,
ɪ, e, ɛ, æ, ʌ (ə), a/). Compare so /so/ (round)
and say /se/ (spread).
4. Tenseness: for any articulation corresponding to
one of these vowel phonemes, the vocal
musculature is either relatively tense (/i, e, u, о,
ɔ/) or lax (/ɪ, ɛ, æ, ʌ, ə, a, ʊ/). Compare aid /ed/
(tense) and Ed /ɛd/ (lax).
Distinctive feature
Each vowel phoneme is a composite of values (+ or -) along several dimensions
that constitute distinctive features. For example, /i/ and /ɔ/ are not really
units in themselves, but rather each is a bundle of features.

/i/ = +high /ɔ/ = -high


-low +low
-back +back
+tense +tense
-round +round
Vowel quadrangle
Monophthongs, Diphthongs and
Tripthongs of English
Vowels that are pronounced at one and the same place  monophthongs,
and English has 12 of them.

Vowels that change character during their pronunciation, that is, they begin
at one place and move towards another place  diphthongs, and English has
8 of them.

A glide from one vowel to another and then to a third, all produced rapidly
and without interruption  triphthongs, and English has 5 of them.

Compare for example the monophthong in car with the diphthong in cow, or
the monophthong in girl with the diphthong in goal. The vowels of cow and
goal both begin at a given place and glide towards another one .
In goal the vowel begins as if it was [ə], but then it moves towards [ʊ].
Therefore it is written [əʊ], as in [gəʊl] goal, with two symbols, one for how it
starts and one for how it ends.
Some people speak of triphthongs for groups of diphthongs + schwa (ə), for
example:
[məʊər] mower
English monophthongs
English monophthongs (12 monophthongs):
1. /i:/  see, unique, feel
2. /ɪ/  wit, mystic, little
3. /e/  set, meant, bet
4. /æ/  pat, cash, bad
5. /ɑ/  half, part, father
6. /ɒ/  not, what, cost
7. /ɔ/  port, caught, all
8. /ʊ/  wood, could, put
9. /u/  you, music, rude
10. /ʌ/  bus, come, but,
11. /ə/  alone, butter
12. /ɜ/  beard, word, fur
English diphthongs
English diphthongs (8 diphthongs):
Centering diphthong:
1. three (3) ending in /ə/ : /ɪə/, /eə/, /ʊə/
• /ɪə/ : beard, weird, fierce, ear, beer, tear
• /eə/: aired, cairn, scarce, bear, hair,
• /ʊə/: moored, tour, lure, sure, pure

Closing diphthong
2. three (3) ending in /ɪ/: /eɪ/, /aɪ/, /ɔɪ/
• /eɪ/ : paid, pain, face, shade, age, wait, taste, paper
• /aɪ/: tide, time, nice, buy, bike, pie, eye, kite, fine
• /ɔɪ/: void, loin, voice, oil, boil, coin, toy, Roy

3. two (2) ending in /ʊ/: /əʊ/, /aʊ/


• /əʊ/: load, home, most, bone, phone, boat, bowl
• /aʊ/: loud, gown, house, cow, bow, brow, grouse
Diphthongs trajectory
Triphthongs
English triphthongs: 5 closing diphthongs with
/ə/ added on the end.
1. /eɪ/ + /ə/ = /eɪə/, as in layer, player
2. /aɪ/ + /ə/ = /aɪə/, as in lire, fire
3. /ɔɪ/ + /ə/ = /ɔɪə/, as in loyal, royal
4. /əʊ/ + /ə/ = /əuə/, as in lower, mower
5. /aʊ/ + /ə/ = /auə/, as in power, hour
Triphthongs trajectory
Closing ending in /ɪ/ + /ə/ Closing ending in /ʊ/ + /ə/
Exercise 1
1. Describe each of the following vowel phonemes of English in terms of
tongue height, frontness, lip rounding, and tenseness.
a. /æ/ b. /о/ c. /e/ d. /ʌ/ e. /a/ f. /ɔ/ g. /ʊ/ h. /i/
2. The symbol /ʌ/ represents the vowel in:
a. pat b. pet c. pot d. put e. putt
3. Which symbol represents the vowel in look?
a. /ʌ/ b. /u/ c. /ʊ/ d. /o/ e. /a/
4. Match each of the following words with its phonemic vowel.
a. sues _____ /ɔ/
b. sews _____ /aʊ/
c. sows _____ /i/
d. sighs _____ /u/
e. sees _____ /ɪ/
f. says _____ /ɛ/
g. Sis _____/aɪ/
h. sauce _____ /o/
Phonemic Alphabet of English CONSONANTS
No. Symbol Example No. Symbol Example

1 /p/ pat, zipper, cap 13 /ʃ/ shoe, thresher, rush


2 /b/ bat, fiber, cab 14 /ʒ/ ___, treasure, rouge
3 /t/ tab, catty, cat 15 /h/ ham, ahead, ___
4 /d/ dab, caddy, cad 16 /tʃ/ chain, sketchy, beseech
5 /k/ cap, dicker, tack 17 /dʒ/ Jane, edgy, besiege
6 /g/ gap, digger, tag 18 /m/ mitt, simmer, seem
7 /f/ fat, safer, belief 19 /n/ knit, sinner, seen
8 /v/ vat, saver, believe 20 /ŋ/ ___, singer, sing
9 /Ɵ/ thin, ether, breath 21 /l/ light, teller, coal
10 /ð/ then, either, breathe 22 /r/ right, terror, core
11 /s/ sue, lacy, peace 23 /w/ wet, lower, ___
12 /z/ zoo, lazy, peas 24 /y, j yet, layer, ___
(2015)/
Phonemic Alphabet of English
CONSONANTS
Note: English words which appear to end in /w/ and /y/
are analyzed as ending in vowels in this system. For
example, cow = /kaʊ/ and sky = /skaɪ/.

The consonant phonemes (e.g. percepts – psychological


units) are described in terms of their physical
dimensions:
1. Place of Articulation
2. Manner of Articulation
3. Voicing
Place of Articulation
For any articulation corresponding to one of these consonant
phonemes, the vocal tract if constricted at one of the
following points:
1. Bilabial: (from bi ‘two’ + labial ‘lips’, the primary constriction
is at the lips (/p, b, m, w/). Compare pea /pi/ (bilabial) and
tea /ti/ (non-bilabial).
2. Labiodental: (from labio ‘lip’ + dental ‘teeth’). The primary
constriction is between the lower lip and upper teeth (/f, v/).
Compare fee /fi/ (labiodental) and see /si/ (non-labiodental),
3. Interdental: (from inter ‘between’ + dental ‘teeth’). The
primary constriction in between the tongue and the upper
teeth (/ɵ, ð/). Compare thigh /ɵaɪ/ (interdental) and shy
/ʃaɪ/ (non-interdental).
Place of Articulation
4. Alveolar: (from alveolar ridge), the primary constriction is
between the tongue and the alveolar ridge (/t, d, s, z, n,
l/). Compare tea /ti/ (alveolar) and key /ki/ (non-alveolar).
5. Palatal: (from palate), the primary constriction is
between the tongue and the palate (/ʃ, ʒ, tʃ, dʒ, r, y(j)/).
Compare shoe /ʃu/ (palatal) and sue /su/ (non-palatal).
6. Velar: (from velum), the primary constriction is between
the tongue and the velum (/k, g, ŋ/). Compare coo /ku/
(velar) and two /tu/ (non-velar).
7. Glottal: (from glottis, which refers to the space between
the vocal cords), the primary constriction is at the glottis
(/h/). Compare hoe /ho/ (glottal) and so /so/ (non-glottal).
Places of the articulation in the vocal
tract
Manner of Articulation
For any articulation corresponding to one of these
consonant phonemes, the vocal tract is constricted in
one of the following ways:
1. Stops: two articulators (lips, tongue, teeth, etc.) are
brought together such that the flow of air through the
vocal tract is completely blocked (/p, b, t, d, k, g, ʔ/).
Compare tea /ti/ (stop) and see /si/ (non-stop).
2. Fricatives: Two articulators are brought near each
other such that the flow of air is impeded but not
completely blocked. The flow of air through the
narrow opening creates friction, hence the term
fricative (/f, v, ɵ, ð, s, z, ʃ, ʒ, h/). Compare zoo /zu/
(fricative) and do /du/ (non-fricative).
Manner of Articulation
3. Affricates: Articulations corresponding to affricates
are those that begin like stops (with a complete
closure in the vocal tract) and end like fricatives (with
a narrow affricate).
4. Nasals: A nasal articulation is one in which the airflow
through the mouth is completely blocked but the
velum is lowered, forcing the air through the nose
(/m, n, ŋ/). Compare no /no/ (nasal) and doe /do/
(non-nasal).
5. Liquids and Glides: Both of these terms describe
articulations that are mid-way between true
consonants (e.g. stops, fricatives, affricates and
nasals) and vowels, although they are both generally
classified as consonants.
Manner of Articulation
Liquid is a cover term for all l-like and r-like
articulations (/l, r/). Compare low /lo/ (liquid) and doe
/do/ (non-liquid).
Glide refers to an articulation in which the vocal tract
in constricted, but not enough to block or impede the
airflow (/w, y/). Compare way /we/ (glide) and bay
/be/ (non-glide).

Consonants can be divided into obstruents (stops,


fricatives and affricates, which are formed by
obstructing airflow, causing a strong gradient of air
pressure in the vocal tract) and sonorants (nasals,
liquids and glides, no obstruction).
Voicing
For any articulation corresponding to one of
these consonant phonemes, the vocal cords
are either vibrating (or voiced, /b, d, g, v, ð,
z, ʒ, j, m, n, ŋ, r, l, w, y/) or not (or voiceless,
/p, t, k, f, ɵ, s, ʃ, tʃ, h/). Compare zoo /zu/
(voiced), and sue /su/ (voiceless). Stops,
fricatives and affricates come in voiced and
voiceless pairs (except for /h, ʔ/); nasals,
liquids and glides are all voiced, as are vowels.
Consonant Phonemes of English

Note: previously, the IPA symbols for /ʃ/ was /š/, /ʒ/ was /ž/, /tʃ/ was /č/, and /dʒ/ was /j/. This
change was due to the manner that affricates can be described as a stop plus a fricative.
Consonant Phonemes of English
The previous chart shows the consonant phonemes of
English in terms of three physical dimensions: place of
articulation, manner of articulation and voicing.

For example: /p/ is a voiceless (voicing) bilabial (place)


stop (manner), /v/ is a voiced labiodental fricative, /tʃ/ is
a voiceless palatal affricate, /ŋ/ is a voiced velar nasal,
and so on.

Therefore, each consonant phoneme constitutes a


distinctive feature.
/p/ = +bilabial /ŋ/ = +velar
+stop +nasal
-voice +voice
Phonemes
Even though phonemes and distinctive features are
described in physical terms, they are actually
psychological entities: no one has ever uttered a
phoneme or a distinctive feature. But, when we
talk, we utter a physical speech signal which we
interpret as containing phonemes, which in turn
consist of distinctive features.

Phoneme is the smallest contrastive linguistic unit


which may bring about a change of meaning
(Gimson, 2008).
Phonemes
Consider the following sentence:
[ðə kæt ɪz ɒn ðə mæt]
(1) The cat is on the mat.

If we change the first consonant of the noun cat and insert [h]
instead we get the sentence:
[ðə hæt ɪz ɒn ðə mæt]
(2) The hat is on the mat.

…which does not have the same meaning.

Again, if we substitute [b] for [k], we get


[ðə bæt ɪz ɒn ðə mæt]
(3) The bat is on the mat.
Phonemes
The three strings of sound [kæt], [hæt] and [bæt]
differ only because of their initial sound, and thus
are potentially three different words. Obviously the
set of sounds uttered here are identical. So the
difference lies in the order in which these sounds
appear: [k]and [h] permute in the first two
examples. If we permute [k] and [m] we change the
meaning of the sentence and hence we aren’t
speaking about the same thing.

In our examples we produce a change in meaning


through a substitution of segments in a string of
sounds. These segments are called phonemes.
Phonemes
Imagine you’re in London and you want to go to Bond Street.
You ask a couple: “Excuse me, could you tell me where Bond
Street is?”. They both answer in chorus: “Second left and then
right”, which can be transcribed as:

(5a) [sekənd left ən ðen raɪt]


(5b) [sekənd left ən ðen Raɪt]

Both have given you the same information although you


perceive a difference in the sounds used, that is, the woman
has used [r], the regular English / r / sound, whereas the man
used the rolled lingual [R] instead. They are transcribed
phonetically respectively as [raɪt] and [Raɪt].
Phonemes
This difference in the pronunciation, which allows you to
assume that the wife is English and the husband Scottish,
doesn’t entail a change in meaning. The two segments [r]
and [R] can be used indifferently since there is no change
of meaning: the difference between the two is said to be
phonetic.

But…this was not the case for the substitution of [h] for
[k] in [kæt] - [hæt], which brings about a change in
meaning and is said to be phonological (or phonemic).
Minimal Pairs
A phoneme is a speech sound that can make one word
different from another in meaning. So, when there is a
difference between two otherwise identical strings of
sound and this difference results in a change of meaning,
these two strings are said to constitute a minimal pair.

If we substitute one segment for another and this results


in a change in meaning the two segments belong to two
different phonemes. Thus [k] and [h] are realizations of
two different phonemes: /k/ and /h/, because
substituting one for the other as first element of the
string
[-æt] gives two different words:
/kæt/ (cat) and /hæt/ (hat)  minimal pairs.
Minimal Pairs
The phonemes of a given language form a system in which they are all
opposed to one another. The procedure can theoretically be applied to each
phoneme of the language. This is because even though all phonemes of a
given language form a system, oppositions in that language are organized in
such a way that consonants can only be opposed to consonants and vowels to
vowels.

VOWEL SOUND MINIMAL PAIRS


/ɪ/ and /i:/ (sit and seat)
/e/ and /ɪ/ (desk and disk)
/e/ and /eɪ/ (wet and wait)
/æ/ and /ʌ/ (bat and but)
/əʊ/ and /ɔ:/ (so and saw)
/ɒ/ and /əʊ/ (not and note)
/æ/ and /e/ (bad and bed)
/ɑ:/ and /ɜ:/ (fast and first)
Minimal Pairs
CONSONANT SOUND MINIMAL PAIRS
/b/ and /v/ (berry and very)
/b/ and /p/ (buy and pie)
/n/ and /ŋ/ (thin and thing)
/l/ and /r/ (alive and arrive)
/ʧ/ and /t/ (catch and cat)
/s/ and /ʃ/ (sea and she)
/f/ and /v/ (fan and van)
/f/ and /h/ (fat and hat)
/f/ and /θ/ (free and three)
/s/ and /θ/ (sing and thing)
/ð/ and /z/ (with and whizz)
/ʤ/ and /z/ (page and pays)
/d/ and /ʤ/ (bad and badge)
Minimal Pairs
INITIAL CONSONANT SOUNDS
initial /f/ and /p/ (fast and past)
initial /k/ and /g/ (came and game)
initial /t/ and /d/ (two and do)

FINAL CONSONANT SOUNDS


final /k/ and /g/ (back and bag)
final /m/ and /n/ (am and an)
final /t/ and /d/ (hat and had)
Allophones
A phoneme can be pronounced in different ways
according to its context.

Compare (AE):
• The difference between /t/ in : tea, eat, writer, eighth,
two, mountain
• The difference between /i:/ in: see, seed, seat, seen
• /i/ - /i:/

Therefore, a phoneme may have more than one


realization. The different realizations of a phoneme are
called allophones of that phoneme. The allophone is a
variant of a phoneme.
Allophones
An allophone is one of a set of multiple possible spoken sounds (or
phones) used to pronounce a single phoneme (Jakobson, 1980).
Allophones happen because of the position of a phoneme and the
phonetic characteristics of neighboring sounds. – man vs tap  /a/

All allophones of a phoneme share the same set of distinctive features


but each one can also show additional features. For example the
phoneme /p/ is realized as [ph] in [ph ɪt], as it would be every time it
occurs in a word as initial consonant before a vowel, and as [p] in all
other cases.
Please, prom, pray
Pit, pat, pot  [ph]  [phIt] [phat] [phɔt]

/a/ in hana /hana/ (Aceh Utara, Pidie), /hanɛa/ (Aceh Besar)


Allophones
[ph] and [p] are said to be allophones because:
1) they can both be described as voiceless bilabial
plosives and
2) if we substitute one for the other we do not get any
change in meaning and the production is not considered
incorrect by native speakers.

Two or more sounds are allophones of the same


phoneme if:
a) they have a predictable, complementary distribution;
b) they do not create a semantic contrast;
c) and they are phonetically similar.
Allophones

Because of allophones…
• slashes enclose phonemes: /t/
• square brackets enclose allophones: [t]
This is an important distinction!
Types of Distribution in Allophones:
Types of Distribution in Allophones:
1. Contrastive distribution: Two sounds are said to
be contrastive if replacing one with the other
results in a change of meaning. Example:
‘cat’ [khæt] and ‘hat’ [hæt].
2. Complementary distribution: phones appear in
differing environments; are allophones of the same
phoneme. Example: ‘top’ [thɔp] and ‘stop’ [stɔp].
3. Free Variation: phones appear in exactly the
same environments; no difference in meaning; are
allophones of the same phoneme. Example:
‘economics’ or ‘end’, [i] or [ɛ] initially.
Allophones
Free variation is exceedingly common, and, along
with differing intonation patterns, variation in
allophones is the most important single feature in
the characterization of regional accents. So, more
systematic instances of allophones may be due to
regional “accent”:
• The case of the two /r/: [r] and [R], which can
occur in exactly the same context without
change of meaning, hence with an identical set
of distinctive features but accompanied by non-
distinctive features indicating that the speaker
is, for example, a Scotsman.  The Brave
Allophones
Allophones
Allophones
Based on its production, there are 3 kinds of
allophones:
1. Aspiration
2. Assimilation
3. Elision
Allophones
1. Aspiration: an interval air heard between the end of the
plosive and the following vowel. It is represented by the
symbol [h]. Only voiceless plosives may be aspirated; /p/, /t/
and /k/ in the initial position. It is characterized by strong
explosion of breath or puff.
Allophones
Aspiration may be strong or weak, depending on
the context.

Strong aspiration: voiceless plosives are strongly


aspirated in initial stressed position. Examples: pen
[phen], potato [pə’theɪtəʊ].

Weak aspiration: voiceless plosives are weakly


aspirated in unstressed syllables and in final
position. Examples: pot [phɔt], tomorrow
[tə’mɔrəʊ].
Allophones
2. Assimilation: the influence of a sound on a neighboring
sound so that the two become similar or the same
(Salzmann, 2004).

Assimilate  to incorporate

It is a common phonological process by which one sound


becomes more like a nearby sound. This can occur either
within a word or between words. In rapid speech, for
example, "handbag" is often pronounced [ˈhæmbæɡ].

As in this example, sound segments typically assimilate to a


following sound (this is called regressive or anticipatory
assimilation), but they may also assimilate to a preceding
one (progressive assimilation).
Allophones
While assimilation most commonly occurs between
immediately adjacent sounds, it may occur between sounds
separated by others ("assimilation at a distance").

Examples:
• White pepper /waɪt ‘pepə/
If we pronounce this phrase rapidly, the phoneme /t/ in the
word “white” /waɪt/ becomes /p/ because of the influence
of the phoneme /p/ in the word “pepper’ /pepə/. So the
phrase becomes /waɪt’pepə/
• On the house /ɒn ðə ‘haʊs/
If we pronounce this phrase rapidly, the phoneme /ð/ in
the word “the” /ðə/ becomes /n/, because of the influence
of the phoneme /n/ in the word “on” /ɒn/. So the phrase
becomes /ɒn nə ‘haʊs/
Allophones
3. Elision: the omission of a sound for phonological reasons (Algeo, 1999).

It is an instance of complete sound deletion; for examples:


• in consonant clusters, such as facts (deletion of [t]) or fifths (deletion of
[θ]) to ease the articulation process
• when unstressed, the word and often loses its [d]
• entire unstressed syllables are often elided from longer words, such
as comfortable and family, wednesday

In English as spoken by native speakers, elision comes naturally, and it is often


described as ‘slurred’ or ‘muted.’ Often, elision is deliberate. It is a common
misconception that contractions automatically qualify as elided words, which
comes from slack definitions. Not all elided words are contractions and not all
contractions are elided words (for example, ‘going to’ → ‘gonna’: an elision
that is not a contraction; ‘can not’ → ‘cannot’: a contraction that is not an
elision).
Phonological rules
It often happens in language that the phonetic
environment before or after a sound influences
how this sound is pronounced  sounds become
compromised.

When a compromise occur in articulation, changes of


the sound(s) will follow.

Generalizations (simplifications
/oversimplifications) about the patterning of
allophones can be stated as phonological rules.
Phonological rules
A phonological rule is a formal way of expressing a
systematic phonological process or diachronic
(historical) sound change in language.

The relationship between the phonemic


representations of words and the phonetic
representations that reflect the pronunciation of words
is rule-governed.

The concept of rule is central to phonology. Why?


Phonological rules
Hayes (2007):

(1) rules are language-specific: it is not a universal rule,


nor some kind of general principle of speech
articulation. The shortening rule of English (e.g. fifths
(deletion of [θ]) is part of the phonological pattern of
the English language, and must be learned in some
form by children acquiring English.
(2) rules are usually productive in the sense that they
extend to novel cases. “Madu” and “dikit” are not
words of English, but if they become words, we can be
confident that they would obey the rules and be
pronounced [mædʊ] and [dhɪkhɪt].
Phonological rules
(3) rules give rise to well-formedness intuitions. If a
phonetician, or a speech synthesizer, were to create
exceptions to the rule, English speakers sense the
awkwardness of the result. In other words, rule violations are
sensed intuitively.
(4) phonological rules are untaught. Instead, they are learned
intuitively by children from the ambient language data, using
mechanisms that are as yet unknown. In this respect,
phonological rules are very different from rules that are
imparted by direct instruction, like (for example) the rules for
traffic lights, or rules of normative grammar like “don’t end a
sentence with a preposition.”
(5) phonological rules are evidently a form of unconscious
knowledge. No matter how hard we try, we cannot access our
phonological rules through introspection.
Types of phonological rules
The following is a representation of the process:

Phonemic form

rules

phonetic form

In other words, phonological rules apply to the phonemic form to produce the
phonetic form.

Phonological rules can be divided into seven types:


1. Assimilation
2. Dissimilation
3. Insertion
4. Deletion
5. Metathesis
6. Strengthening
7. Weakening
Types of phonological rules
Assimilation: When a sound changes one of its features to be more
similar to an adjacent sound, with respect to some similar phonetic
property.
Phone book /fonbuk/  /fombuk/

Place assimilation in nasals:


• I can ask [ɑɪ kæn æsk]
• I can see [ɑɪ kæn si]
• I can bake [ɑɪ kæm beɪk]
• I can play [ɑɪ kæm pleɪ]
• I can go [ɑɪ kæŋ goʊ]
• I can come [ɑɪ kæŋ kʌm]

 The nasal has the same place of articulation as the stop following it:
/n/  [m] / __C[labial]
/n/  [ŋ] / __ C[velar]
/n/  [n] / elsewhere
Types of phonological rules
Dissimilation: When a sound changes one of its features to
become less similar to an adjacent sound, usually to make the
two sounds more distinguishable, with respect to some
property.
Fifths /fifɵs/  /fifs/

Dissimilation rules are less common than assimilation rules, at


least in English.

One example of a dissimilation rule is fricative dissimilation,


where /θ/ changes to [t] following another fricative:
• fifth phonemically fɪfθ], phonetically often realized as [fɪft]
• sixth phonemically [sɪksθ], but often realized as
[sɪkst]
Types of phonological rules
Insertion: When an extra sound is added between two others; a sound
appears in the surface of a phonetic form which is not in the
underlying phonemic form  epenthesis

Warmth /wɔrmɵ/  /wɔrmpɵ/

Insertion of voiceless stops


• Dance /dænØs/  [dænts]
• Strength /strɛŋØɵ/  [strɛŋkɵ]
• Hamster /hæmØstər/ [hæmpstər]

/Ø/  [C[voiceless stop]] / between a nasal and voiceless fricative

 voiceless stop is inserted between a nasal and a voiceless fricative


 The inserted stop has the same place of articulation as the
following nasal
Types of phonological rules
Deletion: When a sound, such as a stressless syllable or a weak
consonant, is not pronounced; when a sound which is present in the
underlying phonemic form is not expressed at all in the surface
phonetic form.

Hand [hænd]  [hæn]


List [lɪst]  [lɪs]

The deletion of /h/:


He handed her his hat.
/hi hændəd hər hɪz hæt/
[hi hændəd Øər Øɪz hæt]

/h/  [Ø] / in unstressed syllables

 /h/ is deleted in unstressed syllables


Types of phonological rules
Metathesis: When there is a change in order of sounds; refers to a reordering
of segments.
prescribe  perscribe
ask  aks

One way words evolve over time is through metathesis, which is the
transposition of sounds or syllables in a word. After some time, if enough
people pronounce the word in that way, the new pronunciation can
eventually be adopted  best known for the description of historical sound
changes (sporadic).

Modern English ‘bird’, ‘first’ have earlier forms ‘brid’ & ‘frist’.

Here are some more examples:


• comfortable → comfterble
• iron → iern
• asterisk → asteriks
• cavalry → calvary
Types of phonological rules
Strengthening: When a sound becomes stronger 
fortition

English Aspiration
/C[voiceless stops]/  [C[aspirated]] / $[stress]__

 Voiceless stops become aspirated at the beginning of


stressed syllables
 Aspirated stops are considered stronger because the
duration of voicelessness is much longer than in
unaspirated stops
Types of phonological rules
Weakening: When a sound becomes weaker  lenition

English Flapping
/C[alveolar oral stop]/  [ɾ] / V[stress]__ V[unstress]

 An alveolar oral stop /t/ or /d/ becomes a flap when it


occurs after a stressed V and before an unstressed V
 The flap is weaker because it is shorter and obstructs
air less than the alveolar stops
Rule Writing
Same or different phonemes?
If the sounds are allophones of the same phoneme,
describe the environments where each allophone
occurs.
• The environment should be described as a
natural class, using sound properties
• One allophone may have the environment
“elsewhere”

Now…you are ready to write a phonological rule


Rule Writing
When one phoneme has multiple allophones, we write a
phonological rule (or rules) to determine where each
allophone appears

Conceptually:
- The phoneme appears in its basic form in the mental
lexicon
- When it needs to be changed into a different allophone,
a phonological rule applies to make that adjustment
- Phonological rules are part of the mental grammar of a
native speaker
Rule Writing
How to write a phonological rule:

(1) Choose one allophone as basic


- If one allophone has the environment ‘elsewhere’,
pick this one as basic
- Otherwise, if one allophone has an environment
that is a more general natural class, pick this one as
basic
- If no allophone has a more general environment,
just pick one as the basic one
Rule Writing
(2) The basic allophone is the “name” of the phoneme (what
to put inside the / /)
- This is the allophone we will get when no phonological rule
applies

(3) For each non-basic allophone of the phoneme, write a


phonological rule
- The rule states which segment (or segments) it applies to
- The rule then states which properties need changing in
order to turn the basic form of the phoneme into the
appropriate allophone
- The rule also states the environment in which it applies
Rule Writing
Conceptually, a phonological rule says, “When
phoneme /P/ appears in the designated context,
change it into allophone [P'].”

Proposal: It is sound properties like “voiced” or


“nasal” that the mental grammar manipulates, not
individual speech sounds like [m]

- Therefore: Always write your phonological rule in


terms of sound properties, even when only one
sound is affected!
Rule Writing
Here is how we will express phonological rules in our model of mental
grammar:
A → B / X __ Y

A The sound(s) affected by the rule


B The property(ies) that the rule changes
/ ‘In the environment of’
__ Where the affected sound(s) are located with respect to the context
X Preceding context, if any
Y Following context, if any

Always state A, B, X, Y in terms of properties


Rule-writing
Phonological rules are commonly used as a notation (to capture sound-
related operations and computations the human brain performs when
producing or comprehending spoken language. They may use phonetic
notation or distinctive features or both.

The basic format for specifying phonological rules is as follows:

The Form: A  B / C __ D

This format is meant to be read as “A becomes B in the environment following


C and preceding D.”

A: affected segment
B: the change
C & D: the context or environment.
Rule Writing
Consider the following words:
• write -- ride
• rope -- robe
• lock -- log
• cute -- cued
• pick -- pig
• tap -- tab
Is there a difference in the vowel sounds? Yes ! The change-triggering
consonants /p t k/ all differ in the same way from their counterparts /b
d g/; they are voiceless, whereas the counterparts are voiced.

A -> B/C___D “A becomes B following C and preceding D”.


V -> V:/___C (voiced) Vowels are lengthened preceding voiced
consonants.
Rule-writing
Conventional Symbols:
Ø  B / C___D “insert B between C & D”
A  Ø / C __ D “delete A between C & D”

# Word boundary
C [-syllabic] segment
V [+syllabic] segment
+ Morpheme boundary
Rule-writing
(udel.edu/~koirala/phonology/day5.pdf)
Rule-writing
Rule-writing
Rule-writing
Rule-writing
Rule-writing
Acoustic Phonetics
Acoustic phonetics is a branch of phonetics that deals with the
physical characteristics of sound waves which carry speech
sounds between mouth and ear (transmission of sound).

Phonetics refers to the physiological and acoustic parts of the


following diagram, while phonology resides in the brain:
Praat
Speech sound waves can be analyzed in terms of its acoustic
properties:

PRAAT: computer program that enables visualizing , playing ,


annotating , and analyzing of sound object in terms of its acoustic
properties (e.g. frequency , pitch , etc.).
Praat
Pulses & pitch: In the oscilllogram: pulses are indicated by
blue solid lines = phonation mode (voiced)

Oscillogram

In the spectrogram, here is the pitch track of the


voice, i.e. what you perceive as high and low
frequencies. Spectrogram
Praat
Tiers are used to segment a speech waveform and attach labels for each
segment for further processing:

Speaker

Words

Phonemes
Praat
Speech acoustic analysis can be realized by using spectogram & oscillogram.

Oscillogram: represents speech signals  amplitude (vertical axis) and time


or total duration (horizontal axis)

Amplitude

Time
Praat
Spectrogram: graphic representation of sounds in terms of their component
frequencies.
Represents:
• frequency (vertical axis)
• time (horizontal axis).
• third dimension (dark
shading  acoustic energy
(F1, F2, F3, F4)
Praat
Frequency is the number of cycles completed per second  measured in
Hertz (Hz).
When the cycle meets the axis for the second time, one cycle is completed.
Sine wave is the simplest kind of periodic wave; the lowest frequency of a
sine wave component is fundamental frequency (F0).

One cycle
Praat
Spectrogram shows
formants’ concentration
of acoustic energy.
Vowels are characterized
by four formants
(F1, F2, F3, F4).
Praat

Formants in PRAAT
are also shown by
red dotted lines in
the spectrogram.

Recording in a sound-
proof room will make
the dotted lines less.
Praat
Formants can be digitally tracked by formant-based speech production
and linear predictive coding (LPC) (Harrington, 2010).

These formants are numbered from the lower to higher frequency.


The different shapes of the vocal tract and the different positions of
the tongue generate different formant patterns.

Therefore, different vowels are produced by the change of positions of


the tongue and thereby changing the shape of the vocal tract.

They are usually classified by the part of the tongue that is raised:
front, middle or back, and according to the degree of rising which
takes place, namely: close, half-close, half-open and open.

For example, /i/ is located at the front of the mouth and produced
with unrounded lips and tongue, while /o/ is located at the back of the
mouth and produced with rounded lips and tongue.
Praat
Comparison of formant values is precarious across
speakers of different sex.
For adult females, the length of the vocal tract is around
13 cm and for adult males, it can vary to over 18 cm
(Maragakis, 2008).
The vocal tracts of women are shorter; therefore, they
have higher resonance frequencies than those of men
(Flynn, 2011).
Female’s formant frequencies are roughly 10% to 15%
higher; therefore, they produce clearer speech compared
to males (Foulkes & Docherty, 1999; Simpson, 2009;
Wang & van Heuven, 2006).
Data for Vowel Analysis
(1) WORD LIST
Speakers produce words that contain the target vowels from a word list
(elicited speech). The rationale of word list is to have control over the
phonetic environment of the vowels being investigated (King, 2006). How?

• embed the words in a carrier sentence  speakers read the word


list and repeat each word a number of times.
For example: “Say ___ again.”

• design questionnaires to elicit single words


that illustrate the target vowels
For example:
Interviewer: “What number is this?”
Interviewee: “Four.”

• Lexical sets

• Read speech
Data for Vowel Analysis
Preferably, the vowels occur in an identical
environment, where stops and fricatives are
favored as they have minimum effect on vowels
(King, 2006), such as in:
• [sVs] contexts
• [hVd] contexts
• [bV] or [bVt]
• [hV]
Data for Vowel Analysis
Target Words (Long and Short Vowels) for Monopthongs (source: Manueli,
Pillai, & Dumanig, 2010:

No. Vowel Word 1 (short) Word 2 (long)


1 ɪ bib bid
2 iː beep bead
3 e beck beg
4 a back bag
5 ʌ buck bug
6 ɑː bard barb
7 ɒ pot pod
8 ɔː bought board
9 ʊ put could
10 uː boot booed
11 ɜː burp bird
Data for Vowel Analysis
Target Words for Diphthongs from Pillai (2014):

No Vowel Word
1 eɪ bayed
2 aɪ bide
3 ɔɪ Boyd
4 əʊ bode
5 aʊ bout
6 ɪə beard
7 ʊə poor
8 eə bear
Data for Vowel Analysis
Lexical Set: the linguistic concept of lexical sets is a group of
words that share a specific form or meaning. This means each
word in the group refers to a similar pronunciation of a
particular group of words in a language.

A well-known design lexical set is Wells (1987) lexical set to


explain the varieties of English.

Watt and Tillotson (2001) examined the fronting of /o/ in


Bradford English based on the GOAT lexical set.
Wells lexical set was also used to explain the varying degrees
of accents in the British Isles (Foulkes & Docherty, 1999) and
to specifically describe changes in the London vowel system of
young and elderly informants from inner and outer London
(Torgersen, Kerswill & Fox, 2006).
Data for Vowel Analysis
The standard lexical sets by Wells (1987) (reproduced from Wells, 1987, p.
123):
The standard lexical sets
No. RP GenAm keyword
1. ɪ ɪ KIT
2. e ɛ DRESS
3. æ æ TRAP
4. ɒ ɑ LOT
5. ʌ ʌ STRUT
6. ʊ ʊ FOOT
7. ɑː æ BATH
8. ɒ ɔ CLOTH
9. ɜː 1 ɜr NURSE
10. iː i FLEECE
11. eɪ eɪ FACE
12. ɑː ɑ PALM
13. ɔː ɔ THOUGHT
14. əʊ o GOAT
15. uː u GOOSE
16. aɪ aɪ PRICE
17. ɔɪ ɔɪ CHOICE
18. aʊ aʊ MOUTH
19. ɪə1 ɪr NEAR
20 ɛə 1 ɛr SQUARE
21. ɑː 1 ɑr START
22. ɔː1 ɔr NORTH
23. ɔː 1 or FORCE
24. ʊə 1 ʊr CURE
1with /r/ following before a vowel only.
Data for Vowel Analysis
However, there are also shortcomings in the use of
lexical sets because phonological systems and
phonetic realizations are always evolving (Ferragne
and Pellegrino, 2010).

For this reason, the Wells lexical set has been


modified in some studies. An example is a study by
Hickey (1999) on Dublin English. More words were
added, which are MEAT, GIRL, DANCE and PRIDE as
those are deemed necessary to capture the vowel
realizations of Dublin English.
Data for Vowel Analysis
Read Speech
• Data are also collected from segments of read speech.
• Common used text in English phonetics: The North
Wind and The Sun (NWS)
• Using specific texts to collect vowels may result in not
obtaining all of the target vowels under study.
• King (2006): if the text is not exclusively created to
contain all target sounds; there is no control over
influences on vowel quality from different
environments and on the frequency of the target
sounds.
Data for Vowel Analysis
(3) Spontaneous Speech
• Speech that is unrehearsed and produced spontaneously,
because vowels that are collected in citation form may be
hyper-articulated or articulated too carefully and do not
correspond to true representations of the vowels.
• Source: Interviews, have the speakers describe pictures,
recordings of a set of monologues such as the news and
commentaries , a televised series of talk shows, telephone
exchanges , two speakers chat freely and unmonitored for
some time while being recorded
• But, the disadvantage of obtaining the vowels from
spontaneous speech is that it may not cover all of the
vowels being investigated, and further prone to effects
from elision, intonation, stress, vowel reduction and other
phenomenon related to connected speech.
Practice 1
Before recording:
• Choosing target words in English which covers all of the
monophthongs
• Choose two respondents, one boy and one girl
• They must be of the same age, born and bred in the same
area
• Have no dental problems and healthy

Recording directly from your laptop to Praat:


• Find a quiet room
• Turn off all electronic devices (AC, fan, cellphone, fridge, etc)
• Close the doors and windows
• Maintain the quietness: if while recording the room is noisy,
stop, wait until it is quiet again and start recording
• Have each respondent to repeat the word 30 times
Practice 2
Saving your sound file:
• Transfer to WAV file
• Annotate to Textgrid file
• A TextGrid object consists of a number of tiers.
Interval tier is a connected sequence of labeled
intervals, with boundaries in between. Point tier
is a sequence of labeled points.
• For interval tiers, create INTERVIEWER,
CONSULTANT’S CODE, VOWEL, and DURATION
• For point tiers, create F1 and F2
Practice 3:
duration
Measuring your vowel:

F1 and F2
in Hertz
(Hz)
Practice 4
• Insert your measurements in Exel:

Boy /o/from"topi" time Duration F1(Hz) F2(Hz) F1(Bark) F2(Bark)

1 1,564 482 574 1149 5.37 9.41

2 6,861 488 574 1149 5.37 9.41

3 11,632 721 533 1232 5.02 9.88

Girl /o/from"topi" time Duration F1(Hz) F2(Hz) F1(Bark) F2(Bark)

31 0.980 600 737 1469 6.67 11.06

32 3,675 660 645 1423 5.95 10.84

33 6,455 687 776 1674 6.96 11.93

• Conversion from Hz to Bark:


=13*ATAN(0.00076*D2)+3.5*ATAN((D2/7500)*(D2/7500))
*D refers to the column in Exel of the F1 or F2 measurement in
Hertz
Practice 5
• Conducting t-tests: to test the similarities or
differences between the boy and girl vowel
production, use VassarStats: Website for
Statistical Computation at
http://vassarstats.net/
• Choose t-Tests & Procedures
• Use Two-Sample t-Test for Correlated Samples
• After calculation, choose p two-tailed for
results
Practice 6
• Plotting vowels in Exel:
Boy [o] Girl [o] Girl [o] Boy [o]
F2 (Bark) F2 (Bark)
17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7
2 2

3 3

4 4

5 5

F1 (Bark)
F1 (Bark)
6 6

7 7

8 8

9 9

10 10

11 11

• Left: scatter plot, right: vowel plot (in average)


Practice 7
• Writing up a simple report:

Our data is taken from the word “topi” to extract the vowel /o/ of Bahasa
Indonesia. We had recorded one boy and one girl age…, born and bred … who
is living in … .
The result of our measurements are as follows: (copy paste your
measurements from Exel)
The measurement of the tokens in graph is as follows: (copy paste your vowel
scatter plot from Exel)

Based on the graph above, there are a number of outliers (mention this if
there are any). These outliers are caused by…(the first measurement of the
researchers) or (the production from the speaker him/herself). Therefore, we
conducted a second measurement on these outliers, and the outcome is as
the following: (copy paste the corrected graph from Exel)

The measurement of the tokens in average in graph is as follow: (copy paste


your vowel plot from Exel)

In conclusion, the data shows that the sound [o] from Girl is lower/higher
than Boy, but more fronted/back than Boy.
Syllables
A syllable is a unit of organization for a
sequence of speech sounds. For example, the
word water is composed of two syllables:
wa-ter.

A syllable is typically made up of a syllable


nucleus (most often a vowel) with optional
initial and final margins (typically, consonants).
Syllables
Types of Syllables
There are six types of syllables:
1. A closed syllable ends in a consonant. The vowel has a short
vowel sound, as in the word bat.
2. An open syllable ends in a vowel. The vowel has a long vowel
sound, as in the first syllable of apron.

3. A vowel-consonant-e syllable is typically found at the end of a


word. The final e is silent and makes the next vowel before it long,
as in the word name.
4. A vowel team syllable has two vowels next to each other that
together say a new sound, as in the word south.
5. A consonant+l-e syllable is found in words like handle, puzzle, and
middle.
6. An r-controlled syllable contains a vowel followed by the letter r.
The r controls the vowel and changes the way it is pronounced, as
in the word car.

You might also like