You are on page 1of 10

Scandinavian Journal of Management (2013) 29, 367—376

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : h t t p : / / w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / s c a m a n

It’s the body (that does it)! The production of


knowledge through body in scientific
learning practice
Assunta Viteritti *

Department of Social Science (DiSS), University of Rome ‘‘Sapienza’’, Via Salaria 113, 00198, Italy

KEYWORDS Summary The article focuses on the scientist’s body with the aim of analyzing how knowledge
Body; is produced by the body in scientific practice. This process is mediated by links and interactions
Learning; with the sociomaterial dimension of working practices and may be defined as a learning process.
Practice; The paper investigates how mastery of the body is produced, perceived, defined and narrated by
Science; researchers engaged in laboratory practice. The paper shows how the scientist’s corporeal
Sociomateriality; learning is gradual, the fruit of daily disciplining, and it is only through a process of embodiment
Knowledge that the body develops sensitive ability and expert competency. The article includes three
episodes of knowledge embodiment in the form of immersion in practice and ex-post reflection on
the practice itself.
# 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction (Houdart, 2008; Viteritti, 2012), which are not merely pre-
conditions for the carrying out of experiments or practice
This article investigates the process of the production of aids of secondary importance. On the contrary, the adoption
knowledge through the body as a situated and sociomaterial of these gestures and postures develops in practice through
process. Combining a learning approach with the corporeal daily familiarizing with the objects in the laboratory (Knorr
turns in social sciences (Yakhlef, 2010), this article analyzes Cetina, 1999). In the article, three episodes of learning as
the processes activated when researchers in the laboratory bodily knowing in practice are investigated. These episodes
acquire bodily expertise. Within the situated learning theory are examples of embodied knowledge in the form of immer-
(Gherardi, 2011; Lave & Wenger, 1991), the corporeal pro- sion in practice and ex-post reflection on the practice itself.
cesses in which the researchers’ bodies become expert occur The first episode narrates the bodily learning process of
through a wealth of sociomaterial and intra-corporeal rela- researcher, who was observed while dissecting cell colonies
tionships with artifacts, and other bodies (such as those of under the chemical hood, and may be defined as disciplined
animals) involved in working practices. The scientists’ bodies immersion of the body in working practice. The practice
learn to adopt postures, gestures and particular manual skills analyzed is an experimental technique in which the cells
are treated, conserved and multiplied. This technique
requires mastery, confidence, concentration and familiarity
with the tools of the practice, such as the stereomicroscope,
* Tel.: +39 06 49918442/3394212348. the cell lines and all the instruments necessary for dissection.
E-mail addresses: assunta.viteritti@uniroma1.it, Eva’s whole body is involved in developing an expert profes-
assu.viteritti@gmail.com. sional vision (Goodwin, 1994) under the chemical hood. The

0956-5221/$ — see front matter # 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.scaman.2013.09.002
368 A. Viteritti

two successive episodes narrate how two experts recall, entities. In the contemporary debate we witness what Yakh-
relive and relate their corporeal learning processes in writ- lef calls the corporeal turn (2010) in Sociology, which others
ing. At my request, the two experts supplied me with a brief had previously referred to as embodied turn (Hassard et al.,
narrative expressing the process of embodiment of knowl- 2000) or body turn (Bischur, 2011). This topic had already
edge through a text that performs the re-writing of bodily been discussed by Biagioli (1995) in his study of the scientist’s
expertise by recalling learning episodes. body and bodily skills: expert human bodies shape them-
selves symmetrically in their relationship with other material
Theoretical background: the body in social objects.
One important source which inspire the work is Laboratory
science
Studies (Knorr Cetina, 1999; Latour & Woolgar, 1979; Latour,
1987; Pickering, 1992), which gave prominence to the role
Social sciences have often reduced scientific research to the played by artifacts and practice networks. In Latour’s view,
institutes that carry it out and to a mere rational activity. The human beings establish a crucial and symmetrical relation-
result is a general displacement of the scientist’s body as ship with the objects and artifacts they use in practice.
shaped by scientific practice (Bischur, 2011). In the last Another source that inspires this paper is that of practice-
decades, many studies have opposed this disembodiment based studies, which developed starting from the practice-
and marginalization of the body by placing it centre-stage turn in social sciences (Nicolini, Gherardi, & Yanow, 2003;
in social sciences. Although the studies focusing on the body Schatzki, Knorr Cetina, & Von Savigny, 2001) and contributed
have not been central to social sciences, there are a number to renewing the conception of organization as a texture of
of significant contributions to this subject. In anthropology daily practices (Gherardi, 2006). From this viewpoint, knowl-
and in the essay Techniques of the Body by Mauss (1934), edge is situated, negotiated, emerging and embedded into
there is nothing natural in the body, which is first and fore- activities, and materiality (technologies, artifacts, tools) is
most a cultural product. In the author’s view, body techni- also a form of situated and distributed cognition (Hutchins,
ques have settled a series of learning procedures within the 1995). In practice-based studies, corporeity is constructed
body from time immemorial. Mauss believes that human and contributes to constructing working practices. Another
beings use their bodies as instruments and are engaged in reference point for the aims of this study, also situated within
gesturing practices that shape the bodies that perform them. the framework of practice-based studies, is the situated
Nevertheless, the idea of the body as an instrument fails to learning approach by Lave and Wenger (1991), who propose
take the other instruments with which it interacts into a reinterpretation of the concept of learning, which is not to
sufficient consideration. From a sociological perspective, be considered merely a consequence of teaching. Learning is
Simmel (1998) was the first to speak of sensorial knowledge. rather a social practice (Illeris, 2009). It is situated and takes
He observed how sensorial perception activates knowledge place through active participation in a community of practi-
processes, thus generating social texture. Successively, from tioners where activities, matter, space and time are shared.
Merleau Ponty’s phenomenology perspective (1945), the It is through participation in practice that the intention of
body becomes the centre of perception, part of the world: learning is expressed and conscious abilities are learnt. These
every corporeal experience (seeing, hearing, touching, etc.) perspectives are at the basis of the theoretical and empirical
provokes emotions. In the author’s view, the body represents reflections carried out in this article, which intends to cap-
pre-social, pre-reflexive knowledge: it is firstly an instrument ture the corporeal learning experience in the laboratory, a
of perception, then one of knowledge. The body theme is also place where knowledge is at its most malleable, fluid, tacit
central to the works of Foucault (1975). In his analysis of total (Collins, 2010; Polanyi, 1966) and sociomaterial (Orlikowski,
institutions, the body is docile and disciplined by institutional 2007). Knowledge is not just a heritage that is accumulated
authority: it is induced to adopt the mandatory social pos- and diffused, the result of individual learning and teaching
tures that shape it. Goffman (1969) too considers the role of processes, but, as we shall try to show here, is a social,
corporeality to be relevant in social relationships. The body cultural practice (Bruni & Gherardi, 2007; Lahire &
becomes the principal vehicle for interaction in the social Rosenthal, 2008; Nersessian, 2006), which is embodied, dis-
context of daily life: gestures define exchange in social tributed and shaped collectively starting from the scientists’
situations. Post-modernist, feminist and queer studies (Fuss, spatial and temporal immersion in the world of material
1991; Tretheway, 1999) gradually brought the body back into practice. On observing the research inspired by ethnometho-
the analysis of social practices, founding their reflections on dology and symbolic interactionism, we can find different
the consideration that it is the body that performs the examples regarding the body as a mediator of social learning.
practice. A first example to be remembered is Sibum’s study on
While social and organizational studies in the 20th century brewers (1998). In it, the gestures and measuring done by
had turned the body into a missing mass, contemporary hand perpetuate a type of craftsmanship that has become
philosophical theories (Butler, 1993; Deleuze & Guattari, scientific and technical knowledge through the centuries.
1972; Foucault, 1975; Haraway, 1991) and then social Another example is a study by Becker (1963) on marijuana
sciences (Crossley, 2001) and organizational studies (Hassard, users, who learn how to get high and how to recognize and
Holliday, & Willmott, 2000) have contributed to giving phy- exploit the effects of substance usage through socialization
sical substance to the body. In this perspective, the body is no of the senses. The studies by Goodwin (2003) on sensorial
longer opposed to the mind, it is not just an instrument, but learning and professional vision are even more specifically
an actor that affects and is affected by practice, a learning relevant. In all the examples that the author cites, he refers
body which shapes itself (Crossley, 2004), a knowing body to sensorial knowledge and how this is cultivated through
socially constructed by its relationship with other material practice.
It’s the body (that does it)! 369

What happens, however, when the body investigated is 2001; Bruni, 2003; Clifford & Marcus, 1986; Goodwin, 2003),
that of the scientist? Knorr Cetina (1999) highlights the through observation, interviews, and personal accounts from
disappearance of the scientist’s body from science studies. the scientists themselves. This paper forms part of a wider
Two main factors have contributed to what Knorr Cetina study carried out in a leading Italian research laboratory
refers to as disembodiment, meaning by that the lack of active in the field of stem cell research applied to disorders
focus of science (but also of the studies on science) on the of the brain, in particular Huntington’s disease. Stem cell
body. The first is the massive adoption of technical instru- research, which is the core scientific interest in the labora-
ments into scientific practice. These have largely substituted tory under examination, is a relatively new field and has
body functions up to the point where the scientist’s body enjoyed controversial success (Franklin, 2005). It is, how-
becomes a mere servant to the experimental machine. The ever, still a developing sector which is extremely competi-
second is the gradual acceptance of the scientists themselves tive, ethically delicate and of major importance in social and
underestimating the body as being merely a silent and docile scientific debate, due to the scenarios it opens up and its
archive of experience and competency at the service of the potential developments in the biomedical field. I conducted
mind and its discoveries. The empirical research that is semi-structured interviews, attended meetings, seminars,
presented in the following sections intends to bring the working round-tables: I observed people at work, at the
scientist’s body back into scientific practice, following the microscope, under the chemical hood, at the computer. I
works of other authors, such as Biagioli (1995) and Bischur watched them write up their logbooks, while they were
(2011). Biagioli places the knowledge of the scientist’s body taking notes, preparing presentations, working together
in the domain of tacit knowledge and courtliness. Tradition- and coordinating their activities, talking amongst themselves
ally, he says, the history and philosophy of science were in the corridors or in the presence of an unexpected, surpris-
represented as disciplines dealing with the products of minds ing or disappointing result. I attempted to follow the shop
rather than bodies. Slowly, this picture began to change, as a work and shop talk activities referred to by Lynch (1985)
result of the works of some philosophers of science, such as closely. These are activities that distill daily practices and
Khun and Feyerabend, who started to consider science as an render them particular from a social and material viewpoint.
experimental practice based on tacit knowledge. The con- Gradually, the researchers began to ponder with me how one
struction of scientific knowledge occurs in the connection learns through working practice, and started to consider
established in the laboratory between the scientists’ obser- themselves not only to be working heads in experimental
vations and the linguistic categories created and used to practice, but also rediscovered the importance of their
name the physical world. Science is therefore seen as the hands, eyes, bodies, touch, posture and sight. Then, when
tacit result of their training and the gestalt they develop I decided to write about body learning, I reflected that it
through this process. This is particularly evident when we would be possible to have the researchers take part in the
analyze how young researchers engage in laboratory prac- stories I wanted to relate. The three following episodes aim
tices. Knowledge, in scientific practice, lies not in textbooks, to accompany the body as it learns through sociomaterial
but in the successful replication of experiments. In this sense, practices, and were collected by different means: through
scientists perform science through the knowledge acquired observation, photos, interviews and brief personal accounts
by the body. Bischur’s work also follows the same perspec- in the form of episodes written by the researchers them-
tive, considering the body of the scientist at work not only as selves. The episodes try to provide a thick description
an instrument but as a kind of archive of knowing. His (Geertz, 1973) of how the sensitivity of the researcher’s
reflections are relevant to the purposes of this article for body is refined, focusing on some specific moments of prac-
another reason: Bischur’s study investigates the relationship tice: working postures (e.g. at the bench or under the
between the scientists’ bodies and those of the animals chemical hood), using technological devices, taking care of
utilized and sacrificed in the laboratory (Birke, 2012; Birke, biotechnological partners (such as cells, for example), learn-
Arluke, & Michael, 2007; Lynch, 1988). This mutual corpore- ing to observe the various cell cultures (embryonic, cerebral,
ality is taken into consideration in two of the episodes human and animal) through the microscope, learning to
presented in this article. Bischur’s work (and the stories I perceive through touch when working with transgenic ani-
collected in the field) shows how in scientific practice animals mals, etc. The gradual way in which the researchers learn
and their bodies are transformed into scientific objects, through the above-mentioned practices matches that of the
epistemic things, analytical animals, and this inevitably process where knowledge is embodied. He or she has accu-
raises some ethical issues. The above-mentioned studies mulated knowledge that has become tacit, but can be
bring the body back to the centre of scientific practice, socially transmitted working side by side professionally. In
considering it not as a subsidiary element but as a vehicle the first of the three accounts, that of Eva, a particularly
for embodied and practical knowledge. delicate and stressful practice is described — dissecting the
This article aims to move a step forward in this analytical colonies of stem cells. This allowed Eva to develop a certain
direction by showing how knowing and learning occurs and is corporeal mastery in the tacit sense. In the other two stories
performed through the constant interconnection and articu- we find Elisa and Luca, two laboratory researchers who writes
lation with the social, technical and biotechnical materials of short narratives on how their bodies learnt to stay in a
practice. laboratory. I asked two expert researchers to re-evoke and
relate brief episodes of embodied knowledge in writing,
Material and methods enrolling them as my partners in observation. My reason
for adopting this method was that self-narrative places
Empirical research was carried out using ethnographical corporeal experience centre-stage, and tells the story of a
methods (Atkinson, Coffey, Delamont, Lofland, & Lofland, body that learns through practice. Both kinds of stories tell of
370 A. Viteritti

bodies that have learned how to discipline themselves and hands and instruments must be treated with alcohol con-
are now able to demonstrate their practical competence. tinuously. This job is entrusted to people who already have a
These episodes are given as examples of reflection on the certain degree of manual dexterity and who know how to
action by bringing the reflection in at the end, and therefore manage the rapid succession of events and any possible
the texts elaborated are meant to be taken as post hoc unexpected occurrences. It is an extremely delicate task
reflexive practice (Czarniawska, 2009). for two reasons. The first is due to cost — cell lines are very
expensive, and in this case we are dealing with human
The body’s immersion in sociomaterial embryonic stem cell lines, which cannot be produced in Italy
and therefore must be purchased abroad. The second is that
practice: Eva dissects human embryonic
if the lines are not correctly dissected, they become useless
stem cell lines under the chemical hood or damaged, and this reflects also on the timing for the
development of the experiments. Cell dissection is not a
One day I decide to observe Eva as she prepares to work under relevant activity, but just part of the routine. However, it
the hood in order to dissect the colonies of human stem cells. requires mature ability as the cells are fundamental partners
I have often observed researchers under the chemical hood, in laboratory work, and need to be dissected well in order to
working with human or animal stem cells in the laboratory. I perform well. Dissecting human embryonic cells means being
have learnt to recognize the delicacy involved in this activity, capable of keeping them in culture, keeping them alive,
due to their particular concentration and attention to pos- making them reproduce without differentiating: in short,
ture and because they prepare their field of action with such knowing how to look after them. If one knows how to do
care. They work in sterile conditions under the chemical hood this, one has already developed the ability to handle cellular
and carry out diverse functions, which must be carefully material and has acquired a certain experience. On various
protected from any risk of contamination. The episode of occasions alongside researchers in cell chambers, I have
Eva shows how the learning process, which consists in the heard comments such as ‘‘Look, these are fine, look how
acquisition and mastery of techniques through the body, is a they adhere well’’ or ‘‘No, these are worse, look how small
sociomaterial practice. Eva and the materials she is in con- and separated they are’’. Essentially, a researcher working
tact with are interdependent and this is what Koschmann, Le under the chemical hood can evaluate the health of the cells
Baron, Goodwin, and Feltovich (2011) call ‘‘a simple matter by looking in the microscope, but is also capable of evaluating
of trust’’ i.e. the embodiment of expert knowledge consists a whole series of parameters which cause him or her to say
in learning how to establish a relationship of trust with the ‘‘Look, these are growing well, they’re multiplying well’’
material field of practice. By now, Eva is not a novice: she has with the naked eye. Knowing how to see how the cells are
gained some practical experience. She no longer needs a cannot be learnt by oneself, but only by observing with
tutor, someone who checks the time she takes, indicates the others, both with the naked eye and the help of the micro-
various steps, corrects her mistakes, describes and explains scope, aided by the glance and the advice of others.
how to carry out actions and also their expected results, This skill forms part of the professional vision training
constantly by her side. She is able to coordinate her gestures referred to by (Goodwin, 1994), and acquiring it is a slow,
and postures, identify and utilize instruments, follow proce- highly sensitive and always participatory process. As men-
dures, adapt a protocol or write up a result. Eva is able to tioned previously, it is a routine operation, but one that
operate with a certain degree of self-sufficiency within the requires great concentration and total discipline of the body
socio-material confines of the laboratory, and has accumu- involved in the operation, because if the cells are not dis-
lated greater experience. She has developed sensorial sected properly, they will die. I ask if I can stay alongside her,
experience and is capable of controlling her field of action take some photos and film her at work and she agrees.
with greater dexterity. Eva is now self-sufficient and must She prepares herself for working under the chemical hood:
face the task of dissecting the colonies of stem cells under she dons gloves, cleans the workbench with alcohol and
the chemical hood, all alone with only her body and her meticulously lays out all the necessary items under the hood
senses. Eva is skillful in facing a particularly stressful situa- as once the work begins she will not be able to move away
tion and controlling her body in the course of the events. from the sterile conditions under it without running the risk
Under the chemical hood, the cells are plated, filtered, of contaminating the cells. Stereomicroscopic technology is
dissected (in order to favor the growth of new colonies) used for viewing and dissecting under the chemical hood: a
and the processes of cellular characterization, proliferation, special microscope allows the researcher to observe the
multiplication and differentiation begin. When operating biological material in 3D. To do this, Eva positions her eyes
under the chemical hood, the researchers need to monitor in correspondence with an opening in the glass of the hood. To
the timing carefully (from reserving the hood itself to plan- better explain how the use of the stereomicroscope is learnt,
ning the work and managing the time available). Under the I let Eva speak of the difficulties she experienced.
hood, the temporization of events becomes fundamental and
no time can be wasted, especially as when the cells are ‘‘The stereomicroscope is shaped anatomically, but with it
removed from the incubator and taken under the hood, they you see an image that you should see vertically, because
are subjected to particular environmental stress. For this you’re looking at it from above, horizontally. When you’re
reason, once the cells are under the hood work must never be working with it, you’re caught on the hop because you’re
interrupted and must proceed swiftly. This is an extremely looking at something below you, but you see it in front of
delicate activity, which requires both concentration and you, and you’re moving your hands in relation to a thing
dexterity. The researcher who is about to work under the you’re not looking at with your eyes but rather through
chemical hood must dress carefully. Gloves must be worn and binoculars, so it’s really difficult because you regulate the
It’s the body (that does it)! 371

movement of your hands much better by touching an she has a very fine needle extracted from a sealed plastic
object you can see with the naked eye: you know how envelope, which she uses for dissecting, and which she later
to move and the capacity for precision that your hands tells me will appear ‘‘big as a very sharp knife’’. In her
have. On the other hand, in these focuses you start to outstretched position, Eva is concentrated and evidently
understand that your hands have to move absolutely under pressure, it seems that she doesn’t use only her hands,
imperceptibly, but that movement in your plate, through her eyes and the stereomicroscope to dissect the cells, but
binoculars, through the oculars, becomes a huge move- rather her whole body. She is completely immersed in her
ment (. . .). Inside the colony, which is quite round, you vision at the stereomicroscope, looking ahead at what she
draw a grid with a needle point and then you take each really has beneath her. Under the lenses, her hands now
tiny piece of the grid and put it on a new plate. When you appear gigantic, the needle big. The cell colonies, invisible to
do this, the movement of your hand is infinitesimal but the naked eye, are a population she knows well because she
when you observe that movement, which can’t be seen by has seen them so often and knows how to deal with them, and
the naked eye, through the stereomicroscope, it’s huge. to her eyes they now appear distinct and clearly visible. Eva
To trace the grid inside the colony, you have to make knows she has to be extremely careful with the cells, which
parallel lines intersected with perpendicular ones. The are faithful and constant partners in the operation and a
movement is inexistent, you move your hand so gently but guarantee of success in differentiation experiments. Out-
decidedly in an instant and almost imperceptibly, but stretched, silent, holding her breath, rapidly coordinating
when you look through the stereomicroscope and see her eyes, her hands, the artifact, the cells and the space
the line you traced, it’s so precise! You need to coordinate under the hood, Eva has disciplined her corporeal coordina-
your vision to the movement you have to make’’. tion in the sociomaterial space. The procedure, which neces-
sitated a re-articulation of some of her body’s perceptions, is
The relationship with the stereomicroscope makes a kind not merely cognitive. Certainly, Eva has had to learn to use
of sensorial readjustment necessary. The process of embody- the protocols, the specific procedures, the measurements.
ing the use of the instrument requires not only that the Dissecting the cells, however, is in the first place a perceptive
microscope become a kind of extension or prolongation of task and it was necessary for her to develop corporeal
the expert’s vision, but also requires a re-articulation of the mastery of the specific environment. To the ethnographer’s
link between vision, hand and artifacts (e.g. the chemical eyes, the ability demonstrated by Eva in re-articulating her
hood, the microscope, the cells, the needle, the plate), as corporeal functions in order to mediate with the artifacts is
that which is below is seen from above, that which is tiny clear evidence of a knowing that has become embodied. This
becomes large. Thus Eva’s body re-orients itself, and her activity should not be considered an individual task, rather it
gestures, postures and movements are spatially reorganized is a collective practice, part of a professional knowledge. Eva
to achieve a new order where the relationships between the has learnt this working practice from others and will in turn
artifacts and the body become linked. In daily working teach others the type of practical activities that must be
practices, the use of the instruments becomes embodied shared, transferred and learnt. In fact, she flanks two PhD
through mediation with the artifacts. The technologies that students who are learning from her how to treat stem cells
come into play are not mere prostheses which extend and and all the methodologies that go with them. The knowledge
amplify the perceptive functions, but require new articula- of these practices is not in Eva’s head, but is produced and
tion, a learning process of new corporeal functions. Let us acquired by her body through interaction with the socio-
follow closely how this process of perceptive re-articulation material world. It is not her eye, or her hand, or the needle
between the microscope, the biotechnological object — the that dissects, but the interconnection between all the cog-
cells — and the corporeal functions occurs under the chemical nitive, social, corporeal and material events in the field.
hood. Eva takes the plate with the cells from the incubator, Embodying the uses of artifacts in a disciplined way produces
places it under the chemical hood and, before sitting down, an ability, which allows Eva to feel no distinction between her
tells me: body and the artifacts themselves. Eva’s hands become more
‘‘(. . .) please stay, but I have to ask you something, I’d certain, her perceptive spectrum and instrumental manip-
rather you didn’t ask me any more questions now, look on ulation are incremented, resulting in a more extensive cor-
if you like, watch what I’m doing up close, but I have to poreal competency distributed throughout the space in
concentrate hard and I won’t be able to answer any which the body is operating. The embodiment of distributed
questions. I have to be very careful, I’ll almost have to knowledge in the social and material field of practice
try not to breathe, for what I’m about to do I have to stay requires the body to be re-articulated in order to acquire
almost motionless’’. what Knorr Cetina (1999) has named the golden touch, that
ability of knowing how to touch and handle the events with
I therefore position myself alongside her, trying not to which you are interacting effectively. The body in the labora-
cause any obstruction. I can see her hands under the chemical tory seeks trust, and in doing so anticipates that which it
hood, the cell container and how she positions herself for cannot yet know fully. It is involved in a kind of athletic work
using the stereomicroscope. Eva sits at the hood, back made up of postures, which are sometimes taught, some-
straight, almost rigid, and leans forward slightly. The whole times improvised, experimented and learnt. Eva’s sensitive
operation is carried out adopting this posture, with her arms body moves autonomously, but is still afflicted by fears and
up to the elbows under the sterile hood. She positions her wonderings. She experiments the dexterity of the senses
eyes on the oculars and with her hands manages the plate (touch, sight, hand), and in doing so one can glimpse the
with the cells on the microscope base. She holds the little tension between an emerging creativity, fruit of mastering
plate with six small wells in her left hand, while in her right practice, and the necessary disciplining of the body. The
372 A. Viteritti

expert’s experience becomes progressively more profound was a daily occurrence. After a few months, I was assigned
and the body more skilled. to two lines of transgenic mice and had to study the
The episode of Eva shows a scientist at work while she uses formation of certain neuronal populations in the course
her body not only as an instrument but also as a resource for of their development. These populations are generated at
knowing-in-practice. In this sense, the body is an integral a very early stage, after only 9.5 days of embryonic
part of a situated practice and it is a knowledgeable body development, and at that stage a mouse embryo is about
trained by learning postures, gestures and professional half the size of a cherry pit! So I had to have the mice
visions, which are essential part of the scientific practice. mate, and take the embryos starting from 9.5 up to birth. I
Fully immersed in practice, the body is therefore shaped by started to collect pregnant females from both lines, and
its relationship with the technical and biotechnological not knowing how to palpate, a lot of those I sacrificed
materials in the laboratory (chemical hood, stereomicro- didn’t have embryos. . . I felt really cruel, sacrificing
scope, cells, etc.). Objects are partners in laboratory work, animals uselessly. I began to ask how I could tell, with a
and are subject to training: together in their relationships, degree of certainty, whether a female was pregnant or
they show their shared expertise in the space and time of not. They told me I could use the palpating technique. . .
practice. but it was difficult to recognize the presence of embryos
at such an early stage: however I could try. It’s done by
touching the area of the belly of the pregnant female that
Bodily learning and self-reflectivity: Elisa, corresponds to the uterus with the thumb and index finger.
Luca and the animal bodies In particular, the index finger is positioned on the belly and
the thumb on the back, in correspondence with each
other. They told me that if there were embryos, I would
The following two episodes are further examples of how
feel a series of little balls under my fingers. . . one after
scientists learn through their bodies. To investigate this
another, a bit like a pearl necklace. Obviously, the animal
bodily learning I used a self-reflection methodology and
isn’t happy about it, so the first few times I didn’t even
asked the scientists to describe the process of learning
manage to hold it, never mind keep it still. I therefore
embodiment. I asked the two researchers to write a brief
learnt to immobilize it, wait till it relaxed and then I
account of episodes, which evoked a description of their
started to understand how to palpate it. Once, when I
learning through the body. Thanks to the relationship of trust,
began palpate. . . I noticed something that seemed like a
which had built up over the observation period, this request
sphere, I sacrificed the female. . . but no embryos. . . prob-
did not sound weird or inappropriate to them. The two
ably they were the kidneys! In truth, there was no neck-
researchers immediately agreed to write their stories in
lace-type formation. . . Anyway, I repeated the
response to the question: do you remember when you first
experiment, this time concentrating on feeling for the
had the feeling that your body had gained mastery of labora-
necklace. . . it was there, I could feel it. . . so I sacrificed the
tory practice? Can you recall a specific episode that illus-
female. . . but there were no embryos. . . probably in this
trates this process? Researchers were asked to adopt a post
case it was feces. I realized that the pressure I exerted
hoc reflexive perspective to re-evoke and relate brief epi-
when I palpated should be less penetrating, more exter-
sodes of embodied knowledge in writing (Pullen, 2006). Two
nal. I repeated the experiment, palpating a female for
issues arose from the analysis of the texts produced by them:
15 minutes and using a non-pregnant female as a control
(1) The postures and words used to describe the process of
mechanism. . . I memorized the shape and the position of
learning through the body are ‘‘gendered’’ (I will elaborate
the kidneys in the control, then I tried to memorize the
further on this point at the end of this section); (2) Both
form and above all the size of the feces. . . I went back to
choose to recount an episode centered on their body in
the supposedly pregnant female, there were some little
relation to laboratory animals.
spheres, rounder and more external than the kidneys, and
In the laboratory, Elisa deals with transgenic animals
there were lots of them. . . hoping I wasn’t making another
(mainly mice and guinea pigs), which are used for transplants
mistake, I sacrificed the female. . . there were embryos, 13
when cellular differentiation processes have to be verified,
to be exact! I kept them as a souvenir for a few months,
or for testing or checking neural development processes in
stored at 4 8C. . . from that day on, I can say that I’ve learnt
mice affected by Huntingdon’s disease. Elisa works daily in
how to palpate a pregnant female mouse without making a
the pound, where the animals live and where she raises and
mistake (well, hardly ever. . .)’’.
takes care of them, and in the laboratory, where she exam-
ines tissue samples from the animals and supports the work of
Elisa’s story tells of touch. The sensitive body learns to
the other researchers. She is renowned in the laboratory for
feel, perceive, accumulate memories, compare sensitivity,
being able to ‘‘get her hands round things’’ (an expression I
make mistakes and then new attempts. Elisa re-learns her
heard her colleagues use several times) and in fact she
sensitivity at work, in her writing she re-evokes the practice,
described how she learnt to ‘‘palpate’’ a pregnant female
the ability to modulate and correct gestures. In her account,
mouse from which embryos could be extracted. This is her
Elisa terms pressure the capacity for prehension (Bessy &
story.
Chateauraynaud, 1995). Through her new bodily capacity,
‘‘This technique is used when embryos at a very preco- she matured the perceptive experience: then she it related in
cious gestational stage are needed, or it’s used to try and her text, not forgetting to include the mistakes that show
evaluate the precise gestational age. When I started to how she achieved competency of touch. The body kept her on
work with mice, I was doing my thesis in a Developmental the alert. Elisa palpated, touched, grasped the object in
Biology laboratory where working with mouse embryos order to reach a judgment. Her text shows us how she used
It’s the body (that does it)! 373

her body, holding the object at a distance. In her account, your body and neck (which is, in fact, extremely rigid) in
Elisa preserved the memory of her previous experiences, the correct position, your eyes on the microscope and your
which allowed her to reutilize that knowledge in other arms and hands with the tweezers under it. That first
situations. Elisa’s knowledge of the hands was acquired time, in fact, I managed to take a sample of the area of the
through time and experience: it is social, as the idea of that brain I wanted (the striate) only with the 4th or 5th
certain prehension as competence to be matured comes from embryo. Naturally it took me ages and most times I ruined
external suggestions which she had to embody into her the embryo. It must be said that at the beginning you can
personal capacity for pressure. Her discipline of the body lose your coordination very easily, and above all find
and dexterity of touch are the result of this process, in which yourself in difficulty searching for the tissue, as the
she had to find her own perceptive pathway, act creatively embryo is reduced to such a ‘‘pulp’’ that it’s hard to
and in a formative way in order to reach a judgment and a distinguish the brain from the other parts. After a few
result. In relating her tactile competence, Elisa has rendered weeks I tried again, but beforehand I got out the sketches
it explicit, recognizable and communicable. and looked at the textbook illustrations regarding the
Luca is one of the senior researchers in the laboratory. positioning of the different areas of the brain in mouse
Many times, I have observed Luca working at the bench with embryos at the developmental stage in question, so that I
the people he coordinates, in meetings and seminars, in could find reference points that could help me in orienting
lessons and have had several interviews with him. Luca is myself under the microscope. I immediately noted that I
keen for me to understand what they do in the laboratory, had acquired greater familiarity, I could pinpoint the parts
and tries to keep me informed of any developments in the I had seen in the textbook illustrations under the micro-
research or any expected or disappointing results. During one scope. My movements were more confident, and I made
of my visits to the laboratory I see Luca busy at the micro- fewer unnecessary ones. I managed to hold the embryo
scope. I realize that he is involved in a long and painstaking still with one pair of tweezers and with another pair cut
job — the dissection (or sample-taking) of nerve tissue from into the area in question to free the brain. After that, I
mouse embryos at the 12th—14th day of embryonic devel- held the brain firmly with one pair of tweezers and with
opment. As I watch, I decide to ask him to write of how he the other I cut away and removed the striate, freeing it
learnt this procedure. He gave me this account two days from pieces of adjacent tissue. I found myself more at
later. ease, and although my movements were still rather slow,
they were more precise compared to the previous time.
‘‘Dissection is used to take samples of specific embryonic This second time I managed to recuperate the cerebral
tissues or organs in order to study them. It’s a difficult striate of about 6 samples out of ten. On the following
procedure, and getting the embryos requires us to sacri- occasion, my rapidity, coordination and precision im-
fice the mouse. At this stage, the embryos are very small proved even more’’.
and situated in line one after another in the uterine tube,
just like the beads of a rosary. Their dimensions are very The expert’s story begins far from the body, which
much reduced, and the dissection is carried out using a appears only afterwards and remains in the background.
dissecting microscope in order to amplify the image, and At the beginning of the story, the body is almost an impedi-
special dissecting tweezers with very fine, sharp-pointed ment, something that makes the researcher waste time. The
ends for moving and cutting the tissue precisely. The first most difficult thing is coordinating with the material ele-
time I carried out this procedure was when I was a student ments, supporting the micro-movements of the body under
about 16 years ago, after only a few months’ experience in the microscope. Luca finds himself in the same situation as
the laboratory. I remembered having seen this operation Eva: he has to re-articulate the corporeal functions that are
done two or three times, and previously I had only done to connect with the biological material — the artifacts
some of the preliminary steps such as sacrificing the (tweezers and microscope), the eyes on the microscope
animal (the worst part in the whole procedure) and and arms and hands with the tweezers under it — and
recuperating the uteri with the embryos inside. I had also notwithstanding all this, ‘‘the neck always stays rigid’’. Luca
attempted to free the embryos from the muscular tissue illustrates all the difficulties in composing the socio-mate-
of the uterus and the vitelline sac. I remember how the rial events through his self-perceptive experience. His body
first time I was fascinated by how similar they were to does not appear to respond — it is rigid, and at first the
human ones, just like I’d seen in textbooks, with all the artifacts seem to be alienating elements. However, codified
main parts (head, paws, tail, heart, liver, spinal cord, knowledge come to his aid, and when Luca recognizes how
eyes) already formed. It was a strange sensation, which I those biological elements (head, paws, tail, heart, liver. . .)
however tried to ‘‘rationalize’’ right away. Those animals were ‘‘just like I had seen in textbooks’’ he is able to
and embryos were there for a precise scope: that is, rationalize his ‘‘strange sensation’’. These animals are
because they were necessary for the experiments, other- raised for this purpose: their bodies are transformed into
wise they would never have existed. Before beginning, the scientific objects. There are no practices or rituals in the
person I was doing the experiment with the first time sacrifice in question, but a sequences of procedures through
showed me the procedure using sketched diagrams. The which the animal’s body is transformed into an abstract
first time, the most difficult part is coordinating your analytical object capable of generating meaning for the
movements so that they follow the micro-movements members of the group (Lynch, 1988). This sacrifice trans-
under the microscope without being too much or too forms the animal’s body into a scientific object. To Luca, the
clumsy, and consequently destroying the embryo and little embryos seem like rosary beads, while to Elisa, a pearl
the tissue. Furthermore, a lot of time is wasted in placing necklace. The story gives us a close-up of the fatigue that
374 A. Viteritti

disciplining and incorporating knowledge involves (Yakhlef, an inter-objectual and inter-subjective space. The learn-
2010). Luca has reread his story several times, making some ing process, which is enduring and endless (both for
little changes as he was struck by how it might risk appearing novices and seniors), consists in this progressive, and also
‘‘too crude’’. He re-perceived his body and almost became problematic, process of embodiment. Bodies and objects
shy about it, wanting to be sure that the mind were free of are partners, they are trained to exhibit their shared
‘‘strange sensations’’. In his account, Luca veers between learning experience in the space and time of laboratory
abstract and embodied knowledge, and comes down in favor practice. In the scientist’s body, this experience takes the
of the latter. form of bodily skills. The working practice in which the
In both episodes, the learning process is narrated as a bodies are immersed is therefore a vital experience
bodily interaction of scientists and animals. Natural animals stretched between forms of disciplining, as well as a
are turned into analytical animals (Birke, 2012; Bischur, creative experience. In the laboratory, the bodies are
2011; Lynch, 1988), they are exhibited as artifacts produced disciplined through action, they become docile in order
by human intervention and transformed into scientific to connect and articulate with the objectuality of acting.
objects. In both accounts, the researchers distance them- The workspace imposes posture formation, and the body is
selves from the animals, which are objectified as products of thus transformed and perfected constantly and meticu-
the experimental process. Sacrificed animals thus become lously. Discipline of acting increases the body’s power, and
investigative tools to be treated with the care reserved for this power is expressed in tiny things, details, in that
the most valuable objects. In the laboratory, working with which Foucault calls day-to-day mysticism (1975). As we
animal bodies is certainly an extraordinary practice that is saw in the stories, practice imposes a corpus of rules,
delegated to particularly expert researchers able to perform prescriptions, coded knowledge inscribed in the materi-
this abstraction process from bodies to tissues, cells and any ality of the objects and bodies, which become expert and
other element that will be treated in experimental practice competent as well as disciplined. Training practice is
and transformed into research data, diagrams and results. invention in acting, and is always a specific method of
Scientists, says Bischur (p. 426), turn mutilated bodies into doing that particular activity and a disciplined and
mathematical data, photographs, fragments of tissue. In detailed action that is also creative. The corporeal experi-
doing so, they distance themselves from the ethical issues ence is at the outset vital and perceptive, as the body is
posed by the use of animals. It is a process of the scientific still enacted in its immersion in the world of measure-
objectification of animal bodies, which transforms them ments, postures, control of the senses, space governed by
into scientific models. ‘‘Indeed, perhaps the only tombstone time, but the sensitive competency is already evident
for all these animals, their only epitaph, is the mountain even in the little episodes of clumsiness and insecurity
of knowledge we call biomedical science’’ (Birke, 2012, we can note. Learning, daily discovery, means the appro-
p. 173). priation of perceptions, of the sensitive expertise of the
hands: it is the embodiment of materiality.
Scientific work inscribes and forms professional knowl-
Discussion and conclusions edge, language and identities that are exhibited in corporeal
expertise. In the adoption of skilled postures, scientific
The aim of the article is to question the disembodiment of knowledge is enacted as craft practice (Gamble, 2004; Sen-
science as the researchers’ bodies learn from daily working net, 2008), continuous do-it-yourself, whose focus goes from
practice, together with others and in strict interconnection the standards and aesthetic aspects of the action to manual
with the objects and materiality from which they learn. Two abilities, creative intelligence and interpretative capacity;
processes are investigated through the stories collected in from the necessity of recognizing and sticking to routines to
the fieldwork. The first is the embodiment of practical knowl- the necessary situated adaptations (to protocols, methodol-
edge as immersion in and with the socio-materiality of work ogies, artifacts) and the formative ability which addresses
practices, a process in which the knowing body is both the itself continuously to knowledge yet unknown. In this way, a
object and subject of learning. The second process analyzed dual relationship between shaping the body and shaping
is the self-reflection of the expert body, which re-evokes and knowledge is implemented. Day after day, in the expert body
relates the bodily learning process in relation to social, — the hands, the glance, the movements of the trunk, the
technical, bio-technological and animal materiality in the posture, the flourish, the tense and uncertain expression,
laboratory. In both processes investigated emerges that both concentrated and absorbed — embodied knowledge, the
the learning occurs through the relationship between daily knowledge of the scientist at work is inscribed and
the body, the social and material space of laboratory prac- subsides. Practice — the regularity impressed upon the com-
tices, and is formed within circuits of perceptive immersion petencies exhibited by people and embodied into routine
and reflection. daily activities — binds bodies and materiality together in
The scientist’s corporeal learning is gradual, the fruit expert embodiment.
of daily disciplining, and it is only through a process of The scientist’s body, as described by the gradualness of
embodiment that the body develops sensitive ability and learning in the stories presented, is therefore operated and
expert competency. As the scientists’ narratives show, immersed in its practice. It acquires experience continuously
having a body (or better, being a body) means learning from the objects it works with and is linked to, re-articulating
the effects produced by the expert connection with other its functions in this link with the artifacts. It is capable of self-
entities, both human and non-human. The body becomes perception and reflexive self-narration. From this new view-
expert in scientific practice only if it engages in this kind point, practices are embodied and learning — acquiring
of sociomaterial learning, which is attached to events in gradual, practical expertise — is produced by the body’s
It’s the body (that does it)! 375

effort in the course of this action, through daily immersion in Gamble, J. (2004). Retrieving the general from the particular: The
the socio-material learning environment. In this sense, the structure of craft knowledge. In J. Muller, B. Davies, & A. Morais
learning process is always unfinished, because laboratory (Eds.), Reading Bernstein, researching Bernstein (pp. 189—203).
practice, even in its routine operations, is made up of London: Routledge.
Geertz, C. (1973). Thick description: Toward an interpretive theory
repetitions but is also open to modifications and variations
of culture. The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays, New
which require a docile, perceptive, concentrated body, able York: Basic Books3—30.
to coordinate itself with the material world and continuously Gherardi, S. (2006). Organizational knowledge: The texture of
question the role of objects. In this direction, the expert workplace learning. Oxford; Malden, MA, Carlton: Blackwell
embodiment of practical knowledge is always an unfinished Publishing.
and never-ending sociomaterial process. Gherardi, S. (2011). Organizational learning: The sociology of prac-
tice. In M. Easterby-Smith & M. Lyles (Eds.), The Blackwell
handbook of organizational learning and knowledge management
Acknowledgments (pp. 43—65). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Goffman, E. (1969). Strategic interaction. Philadelphia: University of
I would like to thank Silvia Gherardi who had the courtesy, Pennsylvania Press.
and I hope also the pleasure, to read and discuss the different Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist,
versions of this article with me. I would also like to thank all 96, 606—633.
the researchers of the Cattaneo laboratory for their avail- Goodwin, C. (2003). Il senso del vedere. Roma: Meltemi.
ability and interest in my ethnographic work. Haraway, D. (1991). Simians, cyborgs and women: The reinvention of
nature. New York: Routledge.
Hassard, J., Holliday, R., & Willmott, H. (Eds.). (2000). Body and
References organization. London: Sage.
Houdart, S. (2008). La cour des miracles: Ethnologie d’un Labora-
Atkinson, P., Coffey, A., Delamont, S., Lofland, J., & Lofland, L. toire Japonais. Paris: CNRS Éditions.
(Eds.). (2001). Handbook of ethnography. London: Sage. Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Becker, H. S. (1963). Outsiders: Studies in the sociology of deviance. Illeris, K. (2009). Contemporary theories of learning. London: Rou-
New York: The Free Press. tledge.
Bessy, C., & Chateauraynaud, F. (1995). Expert et faussier, pour un Knorr Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make
sociologie de la perception. Paris: Mètailié. knowledge. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Biagioli, M. (1995). Tacit knowledge, courtliness, and the scientist’s Koschmann, T., Le Baron, C., Goodwin, C., & Feltovich, P. (2011). Can
body. In S. L. Foster (Ed.), Choreographing history (pp. 69—81). you see the cystic artery yet? A simple matter of trust. Journal of
Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Pragmatics, 43, 521—541.
Birke, L. (2012). Animal bodies in the production of scientific Lahire, B., & Rosenthal, C. (2008). La cognition au prisme des
knowledge: Modelling medicine. Body & Society, 18, 156—178. sciences sociales. Paris: Éditions des archives contemporaines.
Birke, L., Arluke, A., & Michael, M. (2007). The sacrifice: How Latour, B. (1987). Science in action. Cambridge: Harvard University
scientific experiments transform animals and people. West Press.
Lafayette: University Press. Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1979). Laboratory life: The construction of
Bischur, D. (2011). Animated bodies in immunological practices: scientific facts. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Craftsmanship, embodied knowledge, emotions and attitudes Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate periph-
toward animals. Human Studies, 34, 407—429. eral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bruni, A. (2003). Lo studio etnografico delle organizzazioni. Roma: Lynch, M. (1985). Art and artifact in laboratory science: A study of
Carocci. shop work and shop talk in a research laboratory. Boston: Rou-
Bruni, A., & Gherardi, S. (2007). Studiare le pratiche lavorative. tledge.
Bologna: Il Mulino. Lynch, M. (1988). Sacrifice and the transformation of the animal
Butler, J. (1993). Bodies that matter: On the discursive limits of sex. body into a scientific object: Laboratory culture and ritual
New York: Routledge. practice in the neurosciences. Social Studies of Science, 18,
Clifford, J., & Marcus, G. E. (Eds.). (1986). Writing culture: The 265—289.
poetics and politics of ethnography. Berkeley, CA: University of Mauss, M. (1934). Les techniques du corps. Journal de Psychologie,
California Press. XXXII, 3—4.
Collins, H. (2010). Tacit and explicit knowledge. Chicago: The Uni- Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris:
versity of Chicago Press. Éditions Gallimard.
Crossley, N. (2001). The social body: Habit, identity and desire. Nersessian, N. J. (2006). The cognitive-culture systems of the re-
London: Sage. search laboratory. Organization Studies, 27, 125.
Crossley, N. (2004). The circuit traner’s habitus: Reflexive body Nicolini, D., Gherardi, S., & Yanow, D. (2003). Knowing in organiza-
techniques and the sociality of the workout. Body and Society, tion. A practice-based-approach. New York: ME Sharpe Inc.
10, 37—69. Orlikowski, W. J. (2007). Sociomaterial practices: Exploring technol-
Czarniawska, B. (2009). Analisi etnografica di un’agenzia di stampa. ogy at work. Organization Studies, 28, 1435—1448.
Roma: Carocci. Pickering, A. (1992). Science as practice and culture. Chicago:
Deleuze, G., & Guattari, F. (1972). Capitalisme et schizophrénie. University of Chicago Press.
L’anti-¨dipe. Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit. Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. New York: Doubleday &
Foucault, M. (1975). Surveiller et punir. Paris: Gallimard. Company Inc.
Franklin, S. (2005). Stem cells R us: Emergent life forms and the Pullen, A. (2006). Gendering the research self: Social practice and
global biological. In A. Ong & S. J. Collier (Eds.), Global assem- corporeal multiplicity in the writing of organizational research.
blages: Technology, politics and ethics as anthropological pro- Gender, Work and Organization, 13, 277—298.
blems (pp. 59—78). New York and London: Blackwell. Schatzki, T. R., Knorr Cetina, K., & Von Savigny, E. (Eds.). (2001). The
Fuss, D. (1991). Inside/out. Lesbian theory, gay theories. London: practice turn in contemporary theory. London: Routledge.
Routledge. Sennet, R. (2008). The craftsman. London: Allen Lane.
376 A. Viteritti

Sibum, H. O. (1998). Les gestes de la mesure. Joule, les pratiques de Viteritti, A. (2012). Sociomaterial assemblages in learning scientific
la brasserie et la science. Annales: Histoire, Science Sociale, 4/5, practice: Margherita’s first PCR. Tecnoscienza: Italian Journal of
745—774. Science & Technology Studies, 1, 29—48http://www.tecnoscien-
Simmel, G. (1998). Excursus sulla sociologia dei sensi. Sociologia, za.net/index.php/tsj/article/view/91.
Milano: Comunità. Yakhlef, A. (2010). The corporeality of practice-based learning.
Tretheway, A. (1999). Disciplined bodies: Women’s embodied identi- Organization Studies, 31, 409—430.
ties at work. Organization Studies, 3, 423—450.

You might also like