You are on page 1of 6

Research Report

Psychological Science

Valid Facial Cues to Cooperation and Trust: 21(3) 349­–354


© The Author(s) 2010
Reprints and permission:
Male Facial Width and Trustworthiness sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797610362647
http://pss.sagepub.com

M. Stirrat and D.I. Perrett


University of St Andrews

Abstract
Decisions about whom to trust are biased by stable facial traits such as attractiveness, similarity to kin, and perceived
trustworthiness. Research addressing the validity of facial trustworthiness or its basis in facial features is scarce, and the results
have been inconsistent. We measured male trustworthiness operationally in trust games in which participants had options to
collaborate for mutual financial gain or to exploit for greater personal gain.We also measured facial (bizygomatic) width (scaled
for face height) because this is a sexually dimorphic, testosterone-linked trait predictive of male aggression.We found that men
with greater facial width were more likely to exploit the trust of others and that other players were less likely to trust male
counterparts with wide rather than narrow faces (independent of their attractiveness). Moreover, manipulating this facial-width
ratio with computer graphics controlled attributions of trustworthiness, particularly for subordinate female evaluators.

Keywords
face, trust, bizygomatic width, dominance, social attributions, human evolution
Received 5/22/09; Revision accepted 8/7/09

The human face is perhaps the most salient source of interper- (Takahashi, Yamagishi, Tanida, Kiyonari, & Kanazawa, 2006)
sonal information, especially with strangers. People can judge and male symmetry (Zaatari & Trivers, 2007) predict less
extroversion and conscientiousness accurately from the face at cooperative behavior in economic games. These facts have
levels slightly above chance (Penton-Voak, Pound, Little, & been explained in terms of sexual selection for male domi-
Perrett, 2006). Research has shown consensus in perceptions nance and sexual dimorphism.
of facial trustworthiness (Zebrowitz, Voinescu, & Collins, Recently, relationships have been found among male domi-
1996), but evidence for validity in these judgments is patchy. nance, reactive aggression, and variation in a facial metric.
Decisions about whom to trust are biased by stable facial Weston, Friday, and Lio (2007) have shown that human adults
traits such as attractiveness (Wilson & Eckel, 2006), similarity are sexually dimorphic in skull bizygomatic width corrected
to kin (DeBruine, 2002), and perceived trustworthiness (van’t for upper facial height. Variation in human male bone growth,
Wout & Sanfey, 2008). Some evidence has shown that people specifically cranial growth, is related to testosterone effects in
can judge from a face whether someone would be at ease with adolescence (Verdonck, Gaethofs, Carels, & de Zegher, 1999).
deception (Berry, 1990; Bond, Berry, & Omar, 1994); how- Carré and McCormick (2008) found that variation in facial-
ever, others have failed to replicate this result (Masip & Gar- width ratio predicted male participants’ other-rated domi-
rido, 2001) and have found either no correlation or a negative nance, reactive aggression in the lab, and number of ice hockey
correlation between judgments of faces and objective mea- penalties.
sures of trustworthiness (Hassin & Trope, 2000; Zebrowitz Carré and McCormick (2008) measured reactive aggres-
et al., 1996). Whereas transient expressions can support accu- sion as the amount of times a participant punished counter-
rate evaluation of noncooperation (Verplaetse, Vanneste, & parts for stealing his or her points in a point-subtraction task.
Braeckman, 2007) and perceptions of trustworthiness seem to As such, this is a measure of a prosocial behavior that has been
relate to facial expression (Oosterhof & Todorov, 2009), little called altruistic punishment (see Fehr & Gachter, 2002, for
evidence exists showing that biases based on stable facial
traits are valid.
Corresponding Author:
Some research has shown evidence against the validity of Michael Stirrat, University of St Andrews, School of Psychology,
judgments of facial trustworthiness. Although attractive faces St Mary’s Quad, St Andrews, Fife KY16 0HE, United Kingdom
are trusted (Wilson & Eckel, 2006), male attractiveness E-mail: michael@perceptionlab.com
350 Stirrat, Perrett

discussion). We reasoned that the ability to punish may be bizygomatic width to upper face height of participants from
based in greater physical robustness or dominance, which their photographs (see Fig. 1).
would reduce consequences from agonistic interactions. Such
status may also lead to increased antisocial behavior and Analysis. Participants’ play was summarized as the proportion
reduced cooperation, similar to that seen by Zaatari and Triv- of decisions to trust and the proportion of decisions to recipro-
ers (2007). We were therefore interested to explore whether cate with male images, female images, and all images. Spear-
male variation in facial-width ratio relates to (a) cooperation in man correlations were calculated to test for relationships
economic games (Experiment 1) and (b) trust judgments of between participant facial-width ratio, trust, and reciprocation.
others (Experiments 2 and 3).
Results and discussion
Experiment 1: Male Trust and Reciprocation Women, on average, trusted 45% and reciprocated with 69%
Method of their counterparts. The facial-width ratios of female partici-
pants did not correlate with any of their trust game behavior
Participants. A total of 143 White heterosexual University of (see Table 1).
St Andrews students participated (107 women, 36 men; mean Male participants trusted 51% of counterparts and recipro-
age = 21.6 years, SD = 2.3 years, range = 18–35 years). cated with 72%, on average. The facial-width ratios of male
participants showed no relation to trust decisions but did relate
Stimuli. A different set of 50 male and 50 female White Uni- to decisions to reciprocate (rs = –.40, n = 36, p = .015, prep =
versity of St Andrews students (ages 18–25) were photo- .94; see Table 1). Male participants with higher facial-width
graphed in neutral pose 1 year before the current experiment. ratios (wide faces) were more likely to exploit their counter-
parts’ trust than were male participants with lower facial width
Procedure: trust game. Participants were informed that they
would play a series of economic games for real money. They
then gave consent, completed demographic questionnaires,
and were photographed. Participants were informed that the
outcomes of their games would depend on their decisions and
the previous behavior of their counterparts. We used a binary
choice version of the trust game (Berg, Dickhaut, & McCabe,
1995; Huck, Ruchala, & Tyran, 2006).
Participants played 49 games with male counterpart images
and 50 games with female counterpart images. For each game,
the only information available about counterparts was a facial
image masked to exclude hair and clothing. Images were split by
sex into two blocks of games, counterbalanced in order of pre-
sentation, and within each block, images were randomly allo-
cated for participants to make either the first or second move.
The first mover chose between ending the game—each
player would receive £3 GBP ($5.00)—or entrusting money to
his or her counterpart. If the first mover trusted the second,
then £2 ($3.00) was added, and the second mover decided to
either split the money fairly—each player would receive £4
($6.50)—or unfairly—the first mover would receive £2
($3.00), and the second mover would receive £6 ($10.00). The
first move was therefore an operational measure of trust, and
the second a measure of trustworthiness.
Before the games started, participants completed examples
of possible games and possible payoffs to familiarize them
with possible outcomes. Participants reported counterparts
they recognized. These trials were omitted from analysis. Par- Fig. 1. Illustration of how bizygomatic width and upper face height were
ticipants were paid at an exchange rate of £1 for every £100 calculated from two-dimensional images. All measured images were aligned
earned; earnings ranged from £3 to £6 ($5.00 to $10.00). and scaled to the same horizontally level eye points. Bizygomatic width was
calculated as the maximum horizontal distance from the left facial boundary
to the right facial boundary. Upper face height was calculated as the vertical
Facial measure. Following the methodology of Carré and distance from the highest point of the upper lip to the highest point of the
McCormick (2008), we measured and calculated the ratio of eyelids. Facial-width ratio was calculated as width divided by height.
Valid Facial Cues to Cooperation and Trust 351

Table 1. Correlations Between Participants’ Facial-Width Ratio 100


and Trust-Game Behavior in Experiment 1 R2 = 16%

Participants Trusting Face (%)


All Male Female
images images images

Gender and game behavior rs p rs p rs p


Male players (n = 36) 50
Trust −.25 .14 −.24 .15 −.19 .26
Reciprocation −.40 .015 −.34 .04 −.41 .013
Female players (n = 107)
Trust .13 .17 .09 .35 .13 .16
Reciprocation .16 .10 .10 .29 .15 .11
Note: Statistically significant results (p < .05) are highlighted in boldface.
0
1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0
Face Width-to-Height Ratio
(slim faces). In Experiment 2, we tested whether participants’
Fig. 2. Results from Experiment 2: percentage of participants who trusted
judgments of trustworthiness related to variation in male the stimulus face as a function of the face’s width ratio. The regression line
facial-width ratio. is also shown.

Experiment 2: Perceived Trustworthiness Image attractiveness negatively correlated with facial-width


Method ratio (R = –.32, p < .01, prep = .96) and positively correlated
with trust decisions (R = .54, p < .0005, prep = .99). Including
Participants. A total of 62 White heterosexual University of image attractiveness in a regression analysis revealed that
St Andrews students participated (45 women, 17 men; mean attractiveness (~30%) and facial-width ratio (~6%) indepen-
age = 20.32 years, SD = 1.5 years, range = 18–25 years). dently explained variance in trust decisions (Model: R2 = .351,
p < .0005, prep = .99; image attractiveness: β = 0.465, p < .0005,
Stimuli. We presented images of 67 White male University of prep = .99; facial-width ratio: β = –2.48, p = .023, prep = .92.
St Andrews students (mean age = 20.8 years, SD = 2.7 years) It is clear that male facial-width ratio is a cue to male trust-
who attended the university at least 2 years before the partici- worthiness and that it predicts trust placed in male faces. What
pant cohort and were therefore unknown to participants. These is not clear is whether this ratio is driving trust judgments;
images had been previously rated for attractiveness by 41 raters although we have excluded attractiveness as a correlate in our
(24 women, 17 men; mean age = 21 years). Measures of facial- analysis, it is possible that facial-width ratio correlates with
width ratio were the same as in Experiment 1 (see Fig. 1). other facial cues. In Experiment 3, we examined the effect of
manipulating the facial-width ratio of images on decisions to
Procedure. Trust games were conducted as in Experiment 1 trust when other dimensions were held constant.
except that participants were assigned to play only as first
mover for 67 games, with male counterpart images presented
in random order. Experiment 3: Manipulation of Facial Images
Method
Analysis. We calculated the proportion of participants who
trusted each image. We tested for normality using the Participants. The experiment included 285 heterosexual on-
Kolmogorov-Smirnoff test. Trust (p > .20, prep < .72), attrac- line participants of diverse ethnicities (208 women, 77 men;
tiveness (p = .07, prep = .85), and facial-width ratio (p > .20, mean age = 23.2 years, SD = 4.4 years, range = 18–35 years;
prep < .72) were normally distributed. 84% reporting White ethnicity).

Stimuli. Twelve male images were manipulated to alter facial-


Results and discussion width ratio; each of these base images was a composite of
Images were trusted by 46% (SD = 21%) of participants on three facial photographs to mask identity for use in on-line
average. Decisions were consistent across participants (Cron- experiments. These images were manipulated in shape (Row-
bach’s α = .88). A least squares regression analysis revealed land & Perrett, 1995), both to increase and to decrease facial-
that facial-width ratio predicted 16% of the variation in trust width ratio (see Fig. 3).
decisions, β(66) = –3.96, R2 = .157, p = .001, prep = .99 (see To control for artifacts of the manipulation process, we
Fig. 2), with wider faces trusted less. warped images using three different transforms calculated from
352 Stirrat, Perrett

calculated. We found no significant difference in decisions


between the three participant groups in their mean responses,
Kruskal-Wallis’s H(2) = .11, p = .95, prep = .12, nor did we find
significant differences between participant mean responses to
the three different manipulations, Friedman’s N(2) = 285, p =
.22, prep = .70. We also found no significant difference in mean
responses between male and female participants (U = 8,004.5,
p = .99, prep = .04. We therefore pooled data across participant
sex and groups and tested for a directional effect of image
manipulation by chi-square. Men and women differed in age
(U = 6,315.5, p = .005, prep = .88) and dominance (U = 6,316,
p = .039, prep = .89) and were therefore split to test for correla-
tions between trustworthiness judgments and other variables.

Fig. 3. Examples of transformed images used as stimuli in Experiment 3. The


image on the right was transformed to have a high facial-width ratio, and the Results and discussion
one on the left was transformed to have a low facial-width ratio.
Of the participants, 120 showed no bias or more often chose
the images with higher facial-width ratio as more trustworthy,
three different male image sets (n = 19, n = 36, and n = 49; age and 165 chose the images with lower facial-width ratio as
range = 18–27 years) collected at different times from different more trustworthy. Participants were significantly more likely
cohorts of students. All images were delineated with 179 feature to choose the images with lower facial-width ratio as trust­
points. For each data set, the highest and lowest facial-width- worthy, χ2(1, N = 285) = 7.11, p < .005, prep = .97.
ratio images (measured as for Experiments 1 and 2) were No significant correlations were found between male aver-
selected, and for high and low groups, the average location of age choices and age, self-rated dominance, or self-rated attrac-
feature points was calculated. Images were manipulated by tiveness (ps > .61, prep < .43). However, a significant negative
warping base images by 25% of the linear difference between the correlation was found between female average choice of the
average locations of the feature points of the low facial-width- image with low facial-width ratio and self-rated dominance
ratio group and the high facial-width-ratio group: positive (rs = –.17, p = .018, prep = .93). There was no relationship
manipulations for high facial-width transforms and negative between female trustworthiness judgments and self-rated
manipulations for low facial-width transforms. We therefore attractiveness (p = .98, prep = .07) or age (p = .47, prep = .52).
used 72 transformed images as our final stimuli: 12 base images Male facial-width ratio drives perceptions of trustworthi-
warped in two directions using three different transforms. The ness, especially for less dominant female evaluators. This
distribution of facial-width ratio of these 72 images did not differ result validated our interpretation of Experiments 1 and 2.
from the facial-width ratio distribution of the original image sets. Subordinate females may be more vulnerable to exploitative
males and therefore pay more attention to their attributes in
Procedure. Participants were informed that they would be line with their interaction outcome dependency (Erber &
viewing multiple pairs of images and that they would be asked Fiske, 1984).
to (a) decide which of each pair looked more trustworthy and
(b) complete a short questionnaire. Participants gave consent.
Participants were randomly split into three groups and were General Discussion
presented with all 12 identity pairs. Presentation of images Experiment 1 showed that the ratio of facial (bizygomatic)
was counterbalanced such that each participant saw all 12 width to height predicts male reciprocation behavior in trust
identities using all three transforms; each participant group games such that wider faced males are more likely to exploit
saw different identities matched with different transforms. The trust than are slimmer faced males. In Experiment 2, partici-
order of presentation of the 12 pairs of images was random- pants were less likely to trust male counterparts with wide
ized, as was whether the higher or lower facial-width-ratio rather than slim faces (independent of their attractiveness).
image of each pair was on the left or right side of the screen. Moreover, in Experiment 3, manipulating face width with
Participants were given a forced choice to indicate which computer graphics controlled attributions of trustworthiness,
image of each pair looked more trustworthy. After the experi- particularly for subordinate female evaluators. These results
ment, they completed a short questionnaire including self- clearly demonstrate that facial width-to-height ratio is used as
rated attractiveness and self-rated social dominance (from a valid cue to trustworthiness. It is less clear how this cue has
Puts, Gaulin, & Verdolini, 2006) on 7-point Likert-type scales. originated and is maintained.
It is possible that overgeneralization of emotion perception,
Analysis. For each participant, the proportion of trust for high where the similarity of an individual’s neutral expression to
facial-width-ratio or low facial-width-ratio manipulations was standard expressions such as anger or happiness biases
Valid Facial Cues to Cooperation and Trust 353

impressions of traits (Montepare & Dobish, 2003; Oosterhof & References


Todorov, 2008), may influence decisions on whether to trust.
This theory might explain our results if, for instance, both Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity and
(a) men in a neutral pose with a higher facial-width ratio looked social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10, 122–142.
angrier than men with a lower facial-width ratio and (b) men Berry, D.S. (1990). Taking people at face value: Evidence for the ker-
with angry-looking faces were disposed to greater exploitation nel of truth hypothesis. Social Cognition, 8, 343–361.
of trust. Both of these hypotheses remain to be tested. Bond, C.F., Berry, D.S., & Omar, A. (1994). The kernel of truth in
Weston et al. (2007) suggested that the sexual dimorphism judgments of deceptiveness. Basic and Applied Social Psychol-
in facial-width ratio has resulted from sexual selection “operat- ogy, 15, 523–534.
ing mainly through mate choice” (p. 4). In accordance with this Carré, J.M., & McCormick, C.M. (2008). In your face: Facial metrics
theory, women would favor men with wide faces for reproduc- predict aggressive behaviour in the laboratory and in varsity and
tion. Explanations of why male attractiveness (Takahashi et al., professional hockey players. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:
2006) and symmetry (Zaatari & Trivers, 2007) are negatively Biological Sciences, 275, 2651–2656.
correlated with cooperation in economic games similarly Carré, J.M., McCormick, C.M., & Mondloch, C.J. (2009). Facial
derive from sexual selection. Males of higher quality would not structure is a reliable cue of aggressive behavior. Psychological
need to demonstrate as much cooperative display to attract Science, 20, 1194–1198.
female attention. Our data, however, show that men with wide DeBruine, L.M. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust.
faces are neither attractive nor trusted and are therefore incon- Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269,
sistent with female choice for wider faced males—although our 1307–1312.
experiments did not address mate choice directly. Erber, R., & Fiske, S.T. (1984). Outcome dependency and attention
A second explanation put forward by Zaatari and Trivers to inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social
(2007) for men with attractive, symmetric faces being nonco- Psychology, 47, 709–726.
operative is that men with “superior ability . . . in situations Fehr, E., & Gachter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans.
involving aggression” (p. 226) should be less cooperative. Nature, 415, 137–140.
This interpretation fits better with our data. Physical strength Hassin, R., & Trope, Y. (2000). Facing faces: Studies on the cogni-
and fighting ability are apparent in male faces (Sell et al., tive aspects of physiognomy. Journal of Personality and Social
2009), and male facial-width ratio has been suggested as a Psychology, 78, 837–852.
possible honest signal to dominance in male-male competition Huck, S., Ruchala, G.K., & Tyran, J.-R. (2006). Competition fosters
as it correlates with ratings of propensity to reactive aggres- trust (Discussion Paper No. 06-22). Copenhagen, Denmark:
sion (Carré, McCormick, & Mondloch, 2009). Although reac- University of Copenhagen, Department of Economics.
tive aggression (altruistic punishment in the point-subtraction Masip, J., & Garrido, E. (2001). Is there a kernel of truth in judge-
task) is prosocial, we would interpret the validity of partici- ments of deceptiveness? Anales de Psicologia, 17, 101–120.
pant perceptions of antisocial untrustworthiness to derive from Montepare, J.M., & Dobish, H. (2003). The contribution of emotion
the same characteristic, that is, a robust physical build. Men perceptions and their overgeneralizations to trait impressions.
with “robust” wide faces may be able to both prosocially pun- Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 237–254.
ish and antisocially exploit with relative impunity. The pro- Oosterhof, N.N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face
pensity to exploitative behavior seen in our data appears to evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
have created a generalized, somewhat valid stereotypic asso- USA, 105, 11087–11092.
ciation of wide facial structure with untrustworthiness. Oosterhof, N.N., & Todorov, A. (2009). Shared perceptual basis of
Finally, and contrary to Weston et al. (2007), who argued emotional expressions and trustworthiness impressions from
that facial-width-ratio sexual dimorphism in skull shape may faces. Emotion, 9, 128–133.
have been sexually selected by female choice in the hominid Penton-Voak, I.S., Pound, N., Little, A.C., & Perrett, D.I. (2006). Per-
line, our data suggest that female choice (indeed, partner sonality judgements from natural and composite facial images:
choice in social contexts by both sexes), if it has had any effect More evidence for a “kernel of truth” in social perception. Social
in this aspect of human evolution, has selected against, rather Cognition, 24, 607–640.
than for, this sexual dimorphism, and variation may have been Puts, D.A., Gaulin, S.J.C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and
maintained through intrasexual selection and competitive dis- the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolu-
play between males. The decrease in sexual dimorphism and tion and Human Behavior, 27, 283–296.
reduction of physical robustness accompanying hominid evo- Rowland, D.A., & Perrett, D.I. (1995). Manipulating facial appear-
lution may have facilitated the cooperation and trust that a ance through shape and color. IEEE Computer Graphics and
functional human society demands. Applications, 15(5), 70–76.
Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., &
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Gurven, M. (2009). Human adaptations for the visual assessment
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with of strength and fighting ability from the body and face. Proceed-
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. ings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276, 575–584.
354 Stirrat, Perrett

Takahashi, C., Yamagishi, T., Tanida, S., Kiyonari, T., & Kanazawa, Weston, E.M., Friday, A.E., & Lio, P. (2007). Biometric evidence
S. (2006). Sex, attractiveness and cooperation in social exchange. that sexual selection has shaped the hominin face. PLoS ONE,
Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 315–329. 2(8), e710. Retrieved from http://www.plosone.org/article/
van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A.G. (2008). Friend or foe: The effect of info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0000710
implicit trustworthiness judgments in social decision-making. Wilson, R.K., & Eckel, C.C. (2006). Judging a book by its cover:
Cognition, 108, 796–803. Beauty and expectations in the trust game. Political Research
Verdonck, A., Gaethofs, M., Carels, C., & de Zegher, F. (1999). Effect Quarterly, 59, 189–202.
of low-dose testosterone treatment on craniofacial growth in boys Zaatari, D., & Trivers, R. (2007). Fluctuating asymmetry and behav-
with delayed puberty. European Journal of Orthodontics, 21, ior in the ultimatum game in Jamaica. Evolution and Human
137–143. Behavior, 28, 223–227.
Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can Zebrowitz, L.A., Voinescu, L., & Collins, M.A. (1996). “Wide-eyed”
judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in and “crooked-faced”: Determinants of perceived and real honesty
predicting cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, across the life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28, 260–271. 22, 1258–1269.

You might also like