You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232453978

I Bask in Dreams of Suicide: Mental Illness, Poetry, and Women

Article in Review of General Psychology · September 2002


DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.6.3.271

CITATIONS READS

112 2,343

2 authors:

James C. Kaufman John Baer


University of Connecticut Rider University
482 PUBLICATIONS 20,846 CITATIONS 102 PUBLICATIONS 7,603 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Creativity Theory View project

Creativity and Domains View project

All content following this page was uploaded by James C. Kaufman on 21 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Review of General Psychology Copyright 2002 by the Educational Publishing Foundation
2002, Vol. 6, No. 3, 271–286 1089-2680/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2680.6.3.271

I Bask in Dreams of Suicide: Mental Illness, Poetry, and Women


James C. Kaufman John Baer
Educational Testing Service Rider University

A consistent research finding in creativity research has been the tendency of poets—
especially female poets—to suffer from mental illness. We explore (a) Why poets? and
(b) Why female poets? We posit that poetry may attract those with a predisposition
toward illness, the domain of poetry may particularly reward those who exhibit illness,
and unusual aspects of the domain of poetry writing may increase the likelihood of
poets succumbing to illness. These domain-specific aspects of writing poetry affect men
and women alike. In addition, the greater difficulty that women tend to experience in
ignoring extrinsic motivational constraints may cause successful female poets to have
an even higher incidence of psychological stress, and of mental illness, than male poets.

If wild my breast and sore my pride shine” (p. 34), and he argued that “male domi-
I bask in dreams of suicide nation of resources alone could explain why
If cool my heart and high my head
I think, “How lucky are the dead.” women have the best prospects in literature. It
(Dorothy Parker, 1991) doesn’t require a well-equipped laboratory, a
full orchestra, or a large block of marble to
Our hospital was famous and had housed many great
poets and singers. Did the hospital specialize in poets write a masterpiece of fiction or poetry” (p. 36).
and singers, or was it that poets and singers specialized Even in literature, however, the opportunities
in madness? . . . What is it about meter and cadence for a creative woman to succeed have been
and rhythm that makes their makers mad? (and, to some extent, continue to be) severely
(Susanna Kaysen, 1994, p. 48)
limited (for an in-depth study, see Spender,
“It is hard to feel a sense of mystery about 1993).
why there are more eminent men than women,” But there is reason to suspect that the greater
Ravenna Helson (1990) wrote, when one “real- (albeit still limited) access that the field of cre-
izes that social roles have not been structured so ative writing has offered women—female poets
that many women would ever become high in particular— has come at a large psychologi-
achievers” (p. 46). But although a history of cal cost. Given that poetry is a field in which
gender discrimination has often made it more women have been able to create with a greater
difficult for women to succeed, there are some degree of freedom and with a greater chance of
arenas throughout history that have been more having their work recognized for its creativity,
open than others to talented women. Simonton there is sad irony in reports such as Kaufman’s
(1994) noted that “until recently, creative writ- (2001b) that mental illness plagues female poets
ing was the only area where women could really far more than either male poets or women in
general.
In this article, we first discuss what is known
James C. Kaufman, Educational Testing Service, Prince-
of linkages between creativity and mental ill-
ton, New Jersey; John Baer, Department of Undergraduate ness, especially in the domain of poetry and
Education, Rider University. among female poets. We then explore reasons
We would like to thank Allison B. Katz and Susan L. why poets, and especially female poets, may be
Martin for their editorial assistance and Walter Emmerich, unusually susceptible to mental illness. Next,
Ann M. Gallagher, Claudia A. Gentile, David K. Hecht,
Nadeen L. Kaufman, Patrick C. Kyllonen, Jean E. Pretz, we examine several theories regarding the high
Robert J. Sternberg, and Alyssa M. Walters for their com- incidence of mental illness among poets in gen-
ments, suggestions, and advice. eral and one theory regarding the especially
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- high incidence of mental illness among female
dressed to James C. Kaufman, who is now at the Depart-
ment of Psychology, California State University, San Ber-
poets.
nardino, 5500 University Parkway, San Bernardino, Cali- There is some debate over the nature and
fornia 92470. E-mail: jamesckaufman@earthlink.net extent of the relationship between creativity and
271
272 KAUFMAN AND BAER

mental illness (e.g., Jamison, 1993; Rothenberg, among those in artistic professions (e.g., writ-
1990; Kaufman & Sternberg, 2000), but such a ing, music, art, and theater) than among those in
relationship clearly exists. Although it is easy to nonartistic professions (e.g., athletics, business,
criticize methodologies or participant selection and politics), as well as higher rates among
(see Rothenberg, 1995, 2000), a wide range of family members of those in artistic professions.
studies incorporating diverse methods suggest It is also worth pointing out that Ludwig found
that there must be some type of connection. poets to have among the highest rates of depres-
Next, we address some of the key studies that sion and psychosis of all of the groups.
have examined creative writers and mental In a similar vein, Post (1994) analyzed bio-
illness. graphical data on 291 eminent men, categoriz-
Some investigations have examined creative ing them according to the Diagnostic and Sta-
individuals and evaluated them for mental ill- tistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edi-
ness, such as Andreasen’s (1987; Andreasen & tion; American Psychiatric Association, 1994)
Glick, 1988) comparison of rates of mental ill- when applicable. He found that visual artists
ness among 30 creative writers attending the and writers were more likely to suffer from a
University of Iowa Writers Workshop. An- personality disorder than were members of
dreasen paired the writers with 30 matched con- other groups, and writers were more likely to
trols (nonwriters with similar demographic suffer from depression. Although poets were
characteristics), in addition to studying the first- more likely to be categorized with bipolar dis-
degree relatives of both groups. The creative orders, they had lower rates of overall affective
writers had significantly higher rates of mental and personality disorders than did fiction writ-
illness (24 of the 30 suffered from an affective ers and playwrights.
disorder) than the control group (9 of 30) and Jamison (1989) studied British writers (and
also had a tendency toward bipolar illness. The artists) and found that a significantly higher
relatives of the writers were more likely than the percentage of them suffered from some form of
relatives of the nonwriters to show both higher mental illness (particularly affective disorders
creativity and higher rates of affective disorders [38%]) than would be expected from illness
(unipolar and bipolar depression). These results rates in the general population. Her sample size
are consistent with similar, earlier work con- (in total, 47 people) was small, however, for
ducted by Andreasen and Canter (1974) and comparisons between different types of writers.
later work conducted by Ludwig (1994). It is It is worth noting that the highest rate of bipolar
worth noting that, in Ludwig’s study of 59 illness (the most extreme of the disorders she
women writers and a control group, more writ- studied) was found within her sample of poets.
ers had parents with psychopathology, and more Jamison (1993) also found suicide rates in 18th-
writers were abused as children. Perhaps the century British poets to be far higher than ex-
most striking finding was the differing rate of pected, whereas Piirto (1998a), in a review of
depression in Ludwig’s writers and controls: the lives of 80 women creative writers, found
59% versus 9%. consistent themes of depression and self-de-
Whereas experimental work such as An- structive acts. Martindale (1972) found higher
dreasen’s is one method of studying creative rates of psychosis and pathology in a sample of
writers and mental illness, the methodology of eminent poets than found in the general popu-
“historiometric” research allows for much lation. Post (1996) found higher rates of mental
larger sample sizes than are possible in experi- pathology (as diagnosed postmortem) in a pop-
mental work. Historiometric work involves ulation of writers than in the general population.
analysis of biographical data of historical fig- Preti, De Biasi, and Miotto (2001) found more
ures. One of the largest such investigations was suicides among literary creators than music cre-
Ludwig’s (1995) examination of more ators, and Preti and Miotto (1999) found signif-
than 1,000 eminent individuals who were the icantly higher rates of suicide among poets and
subjects of major biographies written between fiction writers than among visual artists (archi-
1960 and 1990. More than one fourth of this tects, painters, and sculptors).
sample was composed of writers of some type: One particular group of writers who experi-
poets, nonfiction writers, and fiction writers. ence an especially high incidence of mental
Ludwig found higher rates of mental illness illness is female poets. Kaufman (2001b) con-
MENTAL ILLNESS 273

ducted two historiometric studies on mental ill- counterparts. This finding, in and of itself, is
ness and eminent creative writers. The first, consistent with a great deal of past literature
which examined 1,629 writers, showed that fe- revealing, for example, that women report suf-
male poets were significantly more likely to fering from depression at a much higher rate
suffer from mental illness than both other types than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1990, 2001). The
of women writers (fiction writers, playwrights, combination of this finding and the finding that
and nonfiction writers) and male writers (fiction poets (both male and female) are more likely to
writers, poets, playwrights, and nonfiction writ- have mental illness than other creative writers
ers). The second study, which examined 520 leads to a possible cumulative effect for female
eminent women, showed that poets were more poets that may make them especially suscepti-
likely to suffer from mental illness than jour- ble to mental illness.
nalists, politicians, actresses, and visual artists. In attempting to discuss the reasons behind
This finding has been given the preliminary this phenomenon, we have chosen to focus on
label the “Sylvia Plath” effect. The effect was mental illness as a whole. As a result of space
named after the famed poet who committed considerations, we had to select which aspects
suicide as she is a common image of a female of the question to study. In an attempt to explore
poet who suffered from mental illness. in depth issues that are not as frequently exam-
Some caveats regarding these studies are ined, we do not delve into detail about the
needed. Obviously, approximately half of the specific mental illnesses involved. Questions re-
overall population is female. In most of these garding how schizophrenia, bipolar depression,
studies (e.g., Kaufman, 2001b; Ludwig, 1995), and unipolar depression (for example) specifi-
only one quarter of the sample was female, and cally interact with creativity and different cre-
some studies (e.g., Post, 1994) included only ative acts have been covered in great detail, and
men. This discrepancy may limit the generaliz- we would refer interested readers to Runco and
ability of the findings. Richards (1997). In addition, Sass and Schuld-
Another key issue is that these studies berg (2001) have covered creativity and schizo-
were, for the most part, conducted with emi- phrenia, and Goodwin and Jamison (1990) have
nent creators. There are simply so many more provided an extensive overview of manic
data available on eminent (as opposed to “ev- depression.
eryday”) creative people that well-known art- The two central questions addressed, then,
ists and writers are often a more appealing are as follows: (a) Why poets? and (b) Why
group to examine. However, the choice of female poets? We hope that the answers to these
which group to study may be an important questions will shed light on why female poets
one. Richards (1993a, 1993b, 1999) has are at such risk.
pointed out that this distinction may explain
conflicting research findings, in that eminent
creators may be more prone to suffer from Why Poets?
mental illness than less eminent creators. In-
deed, some studies (Kaufman, 2001a; Lud- In this section, we argue that several factors
wig, 1995) have shown that the extremely associated with poetry combine to produce an
eminent may be more likely to suffer from additive effect: the types of people who are
mental illness than those who are merely very drawn to poetry, the inability of poetry to as-
good. Can conclusions we might draw on suage mental illness, the impact that the field
female poets extend equally well to Sylvia has on poets, and a possible age effect. First, the
Plath and to a college student writing poetry nature and style of poetry draw people who may
in her journal? Not necessarily, although we be more likely to be unstable. Second, unlike
believe similar factors may come into play. other forms of writing, poetry does not alleviate
Why is mental illness so prevalent among mental illness. Third, implicit expectations from
successful women writers, and especially emi- the field may result in successful poets being
nent female poets? As described earlier, there is expected to be ill. Finally, poets typically peak
evidence that women creative writers in general at a younger age, when mental illness is more
experience more mental illness than their male likely to strike.
274 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Nature and Style of Poetry was also found in the visual arts in regard to
artistic style; more expressive work was more
The nature and style of poetry may make it linked with mental illness. On a case study
more introspective and expressive than other basis, Silverman and Will (1986) analyzed how
forms of writing. Nonfiction writers have been Sylvia Plath’s depression worsened when she
found to use a more “paradigmatic” thinking shifted from a more traditional poetic style to a
style, whereas creative writers (i.e., fiction writ- more expressive style.
ers and poets) have been found to use a more This extra introspection that may be involved
“narrative” thinking style (Kaufman, 2000). A
in writing poetry may also be associated with
paradigmatic thinking style is realistic and log-
mental illness. Nolen-Hoeksema, Larson, and
ical, whereas a narrative thinking style sees
Grayson (1999) found that women who suffer
connections and is focused on what might be
and could be (Bruner, 1986). Similarly, nonfic- from depression are more likely to engage in
tion writers have been found to be more likely rumination, whereas men are more likely to
to use an “executive” thinking style, and cre- distract themselves. This finding was also pro-
ative writers have been found to be more likely duced in a nondepressed sample of eighth grad-
to use a “legislative” thinking style (Kaufman, ers (Sethi & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1997).
in press). Legislative thinkers prefer to create The link between rumination and depression
things and to be self-directed. Executive think- can run both ways. Not only are individuals
ers prefer to follow directions, to carry out who suffer from depression more likely to ru-
orders, and to work under a great deal of struc- minate (and, perhaps, turn these ruminations
ture (Sternberg, 1988, 1997). into poetry), but such rumination can also have
Efforts to distinguish the thinking styles of a deleterious effect on their depression (Kueh-
nonfiction and creative writers (as just de- ner & Weber, 1999). If poets— especially fe-
scribed) have received more attention than com- male poets—are already inclined toward de-
parisons of the thinking styles of poetry and pression, then perhaps the actual act of writing
fiction writers, but one recurring theme is that poems may add to their mental instability.
poetry may be more expressive, emotional, and This type of a direct relationship of creative
introspective than fiction. The concept of fiction style affecting psychopathology is one of five
being more concerned with reality and poetry relationships described by Richards (1981,
being more focused on emotions, internal feel- 1999) in her typology of creativity and psycho-
ings, and introspection has been raised in both pathology. Another involves the fact that pa-
psychology theory (e.g., Gardner, 1993) and thology may affect creativity. We explore this
psychoanalytical research (e.g., Szajnberg, relationship next.1
1992). The connection between poetry and the People with mental disorders who also con-
strong expression of emotions is also raised in
sider themselves to have artistic and creative
educational research (e.g., Whalen, 2000) and
talent may naturally gravitate toward the me-
research on literature (e.g., Olsen, 1998). Obvi-
ously, no one claims that all poetry is expressive dium of poetry precisely because of its personal
and abstract and that all fiction is reality based nature. VanTassel-Baska (1996) explained that
and concrete. Certainly, however, poetry may female poets would channel their emotional dis-
more often be expressive, abstract, and intro- turbances into their writing, whereas women
spective than fiction. fiction writers (such as Charlotte Bronte and
The tendency toward being more expressive Virginia Woolf) found their mental instabilities
may make one more prone to illness. Ludwig’s to interfere with their creative work. Runco
(1998) investigation of more than 1,000 people (1998) has examined in depth the way that
in 18 different professions showed that people Plath’s enormous personal investment in her
who pursued professions that were more objec- poetry was connected with her depression.
tive and formal were less likely to be mentally
unstable (e.g., suffer from illnesses such as 1
Her other three types include an indirect relationship of
manic depression, depression, and mood disor- pathology to creativity, an indirect relationship of creativity
ders) than those who pursued professions that to pathology, and a third factor that affects both creativity
were subjective and emotive. Such a pattern and pathology.
MENTAL ILLNESS 275

An additional possibility is that the linguistic In addition, it is not clear that writing poetry
style of poetry is such that it may appeal more would have the same benefits as other kinds of
to people who are prone to suffer from mental writing. Pennebaker and Seagal (1999) found
illness. Lynton and Salovey (1997) assigned that the formation of a narrative was essential
participants to different writing conditions and for mental and physical benefits. Many poems
then measured their mood. They found that be- do not have narratives; most stories and plays
ing in a bad mood was associated with writing do. In addition, participants who wrote for a
about abstract concepts, and being in a good longer and more intensive duration benefited
mood was associated with writing about con- more than participants who wrote for a shorter
crete concepts and a narrative style. Poetry amount of time (Páez, Velasco, & González,
tends to tackle more abstract concepts, whereas 1999). Do poets write for the same duration
fiction may often deal with more concrete is- each day as fiction writers?
sues. In addition, although there are certainly The psychological impact of the acts of writ-
many narrative poems, a narrative style is none- ing a piece of fiction and writing a poem may be
theless more consistent with writing fiction. different. This is an area that needs more re-
search, and because it is not yet known whether
poetry writing has a different psychological ef-
Poetry’s Link With Mental Illness fect than fiction writing, the hypotheses that
follow are speculative and based on what we
A second possibility is that whereas other acknowledge is an undemonstrated empirical
forms of creative writing may alleviate mental difference.
illness, poetry does not. This is counterintuitive Why could poetry not prove therapeutic?
to such forms of psychotherapy as “poetry ther- Could poets actually be harmed instead of
apy” (e.g., Anderson, 1999; Rojcewicz, 1999). helped by the act of writing poetry? The answer
Research clearly supports the therapeutic value may be found in Bandura’s (1977, 1997) theory
of writing, but the value of writing poetry is less of self-efficacy. Bandura focused on the impor-
clear. tance of thinking positively about one’s abili-
Lepore (1997) found that students who en- ties. If people believe in their aptitudes—and
gaged in expressive writing before an anxiety- see evidence of competence—they will tend to
provoking event (graduate entrance examina- be more persistent in their efforts to succeed and
tions) reported a significant reduction in depres- less likely to be anxious or depressed. People
sive symptoms relative to a control group. This high in self-efficacy will tend to focus on pos-
research is consistent with Pennebaker’s (1997) sibilities rather than limits and inadequacies.
notion that there is an association between writ- One reason for this perception is the feeling of
ing about an emotional experience and im- being in control. When people perceive them-
provements in physical and mental health. selves to have an internal locus of control, they
Yet, conversely, expressive writing about a will tend to give themselves more credit for
traumatic experience may be deleterious to their successes (Rotter, 1990). An internal locus
one’s health if adequate support and therapy are of control is also associated with having an
not in place (Honos-Webb, Harrick, Stiles, & optimistic explanatory style, which is often
Park, 2000). A study of suicidal and nonsuicidal highly associated with a variety of good out-
poets showed that suicidal poets were more comes, such as better health (Peterson, 2000).
likely to use words associated with the self (as Yet, many people in the arts, because of the
opposed to the collective), perhaps an indicator very “mystical” nature of how a person creates,
of more emotional and expressive work (Stir- may (even unconsciously) credit “divine inspi-
man & Pennebaker, 2001). If the expressive ration” for their work. Piirto (1998b) found that
work is focused on negative experiences, then many writers— even to this day—see their
the writer may experience an increase in nega- source of inspiration as being found in a
tive mood (Marlo & Wagner, 1999). Another “muse.” Poets, in particular, were likely to
study showed that people who reported that credit such a “muse” for their creative work
writing served a cathartic function were more (Piirto, 1998b). Even if such a misattribution
likely to suffer from poor health (Pennebaker, results from superstition or modesty, it may
1989). produce a perceived external locus of control.
276 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Writers (and especially poets) may mentally the “gatekeepers” of the domain. The gatekeep-
assign credit—and, indirectly, locus of con- ers of the domain of creative writing include
trol—to such a muse, and this may give them professors, agents, literary critics, and magazine
less sense of self-efficacy. This, in turn, may editors. If the field has expectations or precon-
make them more prone to depression and other ceptions, then these beliefs may eventually in-
emotional disorders. Belief in external agents fluence other components, such as the work of
(of which a “muse” would be just one example) people involved in the creative activity.
has been found to be linked with a decreased Where would these preconceptions be
ability to recognize one’s own active role in rooted? One frequent stereotype is that male
achieving desired outcomes (Gilbert, Brown, poets tend to write about less personal issues
Pinel, & Wilson, 2000). Writers who credit a (such as war, spirituality, and “pure” love),
muse for their ideas and what they write—and whereas female poets turn inward for inspira-
who are thereby making external attributions of tion. A content analysis of poetry and prose
the causes of their successes—may have a could provide insights into whether this stereo-
greater likelihood of suffering from such disor-
type is true or false.4 Regardless of whether it is
ders as anxiety or depression because they have
grounded in fact, however, such a stereotype
a lower sense of self-efficacy than writers who
may still exist in the conceptions of the gate-
do not invoke a muse to explain their
accomplishments. keepers. The field may accept the stereotype
This phenomenon may particularly affect and unconsciously create expectations.
women, who have been found to be more likely Perhaps one cause of Kaufman’s (2001b)
to have an external, rather than internal, locus of “Sylvia Plath” effect is that creative writing
control (Sherman, Higgs, & Williams, 1997; gatekeepers may unconsciously expect a quality
Smith, Dugan, & Trompenaars, 1997).2 Women poem written by a woman to be heartfelt and
who have low self-esteem—and who may there- moving without holding similar expectations
fore be at higher risk of showing signs of illness for a poem written by a man. This dichotomy
such as depression—are known to have a more could create a situation in which female poets
external locus of control (for both success and who suffer from mental illness and pour out
failure) than women with higher self-esteem their troubles in their poetry may actually be
(Chubb, Fertman, & Ross, 1997).3 more likely to have their work praised (and
Thus, even though writing may have thera-
peutic rewards, these rewards may not be appli- 2
It is worth noting that some psychologists (e.g., Gilli-
cable to writing poetry. Indeed, some poets may gan, 1982) object to such findings on gender differences,
be inadvertently engaged in a pattern of external and these results should be interpreted cautiously.
attribution and control that may not merely de- 3
One key variable in determining the possible risk to
prive them of these rewards but actually be mental health may be whether writers perceive themselves
as being in control of their muse. Some writers may see the
detrimental. muse as a resource for inspiration over which they have
power. In this situation, they maintain an internal locus of
Implicit Expectations of Illness control. The muse serves a beneficial purpose—it is a re-
source to be tapped—and these writers’ primary reason for
Many of the studies that have revealed a writing is still intrinsic motivation (i.e., enjoyment). But
other writers may conceive of their muse in a different way.
connection between poetry and mental illness They may envision the muse as the sole source of ideas and
have examined eminent writers. Perhaps one see themselves as a vessel for their creative works. In this
confounding variable in these studies is that the (extreme) situation, they are not writing for their own rea-
publishing field (editors, publishers, agents, and sons but, rather, are viewing themselves as pawns. These
writers would be moved by external forces, which can lead
so forth) unconsciously rewards poetry that is to an emphasis on extrinsic motivation instead of intrinsic
produced by mental illness. The field is an im- motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000). As Deci and Ryan (1980,
portant component of the creative system; Csik- 1985) have argued, intrinsic motivation is a key ingredient
szentmihalyi (1999) described creativity as an in higher self-esteem, better self-determination, and more
interaction of domain, person, and field. A spe- positive emotions.
4
Such a study could involve a format similar to
cific person produces a creative work in a par- Schaller’s (1997) examination of the work of John Cheever,
ticular domain (such as mathematics or fiction Kurt Cobain, and Cole Porter or Stirman and Pennebaker’s
writing), and this work is then given to the field, (2001) ongoing use of computer programs and text analysis.
MENTAL ILLNESS 277

published) than female poets who produce less and mental illness may be strengthened by the
intimate work. Similarly, women fiction writers poet’s age. Other writers typically peak later,
who suffer from mental illness may be at a and by that time in their lives, they are less
disadvantage in trying to get their work known likely to be affected by illness. Poets peak ear-
if the gatekeepers of the fiction writing field lier, when mental illness is most likely to be an
prefer work that is less personal. issue.
The gatekeepers may very well have these
ingrained stereotypes about women writers
without necessarily being aware of them. Banaji Summary
and her colleagues (Banaji & Hardin, 1996;
The answer to the question “Why poets?”
Blair & Banaji, 1996; Greenwald & Banaji,
may very well be that several different and
1995), for example, have found that stereotyp-
unrelated factors build up and compound each
ing can occur unconsciously; even when people
other. People who may be more likely to suffer
may explicitly say they are not prejudiced
from mental illness may be more drawn to writ-
against a particular group, implicit measures
ing poetry. Other types of writers may accrue
reveal that such a bias may very well exist.
therapeutic effects from their writing, but poets
may gain fewer benefits and may actually be
Age and Mental Illness harmed. The “gatekeepers” who decide which
poems are selected into journals and magazines
Simonton has done extensive research on
may have implicit ideas about what makes an
productivity, creativity, and age and has found
“appropriate” poem, thereby skewing eminent
that people in the arts peak earlier than people in
poets toward topics and ideas that might be
the sciences (Simonton, 1990). More specifi-
more related to mental illness. And, finally,
cally, and more appropriate to this article, poets
poets peak earlier, at a more vulnerable time for
peak markedly earlier than novelists (Simonton,
the onset of mental illness. Each of these issues
1975, 1989); indeed, poets produce twice as
may not in itself be enough to result in the
much of their lifetime output in their 20s as
connection between poetry and mental illness,
novelists do (Simonton, 1984). This earlier pro-
but the combined weight may create a group of
ductivity may be one reason why poets can
writers who are at risk.
produce great work yet still die younger than
other writers and artists (Cassandro, 1998).
Many mental illnesses—particularly bipolar Future Directions
depression, the most common illness studied in
conjunction with creativity (e.g., Jamison, One future direction to be explored is the
1993)—are more common at younger ages than question of style versus form. Is it poetry that is
older ages. The onset of bipolar affective illness more likely to be linked to mental illness, or is
is often remarkably early; 20% of patients have it styles associated with poetry (e.g., expressive-
shown evidence of the disorder as early as ad- ness and emotionality)? There is some evidence
olescence, and the peak of the disorder is in the focusing only on writing style. Pennebaker and
20s (Loranger & Levine, 1978). Indeed, adoles- King (1999) analyzed language dimensions, lin-
cence and young adulthood are considered to be guistic profiles, and personality variables. They
particularly essential periods in determining found that using the first-person singular (I, me,
whether an individual will develop bipolar dis- and my) and writing in the present tense were
order, unipolar depression, phobias, and drug two factors that loaded on a dimension labeled
and alcohol abuse (Burke, Burke, Regier, & “immediacy.” This “immediacy” dimension
Rae, 1990). Bipolar depression and most other was significantly correlated with “neuroticism.”
mood disorders are more common in younger Stirman and Pennebaker (2001) found that sui-
adults than in older adults (Eaton et al., 1989). cidal poets were more likely to use first-person
If mental illness is most likely to be an issue singular than a control group of nonsuicidal
during young adulthood, particularly one’s 20s, poets. These results indicate that perhaps fiction
and poets are most likely to produce their great- writers who use the first-person singular would
est output during their young adulthood, also in be more prone to mental illness than poets who
their 20s, then the connection between poetry use the more distant style of third person.
278 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Another possible avenue for future research this difference, Jamison (1993) hypothesized, is
would be to not only analyze the style of the that an elevated mood may actually be a hypo-
work in greater detail but also analyze the writ- manic state that would reflect “greater distract-
ers’ lives in greater detail. Many writers with ibility, irritability, increases in seeking out of
mental illness have affective disorders such as other people, and alcohol abuse” (p. 139). The
manic depression (Jamison, 1993), a disorder key seems to be that it is more important for the
particularly known for periods of intense afflic- mood to be balanced than for it to be necessarily
tion and periods of comparative peace. How good.
would writers’ most troubled and least troubled At times, perhaps, mental illness (especially
times be correlated with their most productive in its most severe forms) may impede and in-
and least productive periods? Would writers terfere with creativity. Singer (1966) empha-
produce the most when they are the most af- sized that the positive daydreaming and fanta-
flicted or the least afflicted? sizing that are involved with imaginative
Andreasen and Glick (1988) suggested that thought are not necessarily associated with the
creative people would be most productive when more negative flights of fancy that may be man-
their illnesses are under good control. Zausner ifested in such disorders as schizophrenia.
(1998) used chaos theory to contend that phys- Rothenberg (1990) advanced the theory that
ical illness could have one of four effects on an many of the mental processes involved in cre-
artist’s work, one being that illness would trans- ativity are often healthy and productive. A cre-
form his or her creative process and the art ator’s mental illness may have nothing to do
produced. Could a similar transformation occur with the act of creation itself and may, if any-
with mental illness, with mental disorders play- thing, impede the process; the production of a
ing an integral role in the type of work pro- piece of writing requires coherent thought, not
duced? Berman (1995) has argued that artists something one associates with mental illness.
are the least productive during the calm periods However, Jamison (1993) argued that periods of
of their lives. But is the reverse true? Could the clear and logical thought are certainly consis-
moments of tranquility be just as essential to the tent with the cyclical nature of such illnesses as
creation process? manic depression. Shapiro and Weisberg (2000)
Certainly, several psychologists would argue argued that much of the past research that has
that the mental processes involved in the cre- examined these questions, however, may have
ation of great work and the mental processes been confounded by the confusion between
involved in an emotional illness may simply mental illness and normal correlates with cre-
coexist and not interact. Richards and Kinney ativity in the cognitive, affective, and motiva-
(1990), for example, studied people who were tional dimensions. More in-depth research that
diagnosed with bipolar and unipolar depression focuses on how mental illness affects the cre-
and asked them when their most creative mood ative process and different types of creative
states occurred. A majority of the patients (more writing is needed if any larger conclusions are
than 75%) reported that they were most creative to be drawn.
when they were in “normal” or “mildly ele-
vated” mood states, as opposed to more severe Why Female Poets?
mood states.
Jamison’s (1989, 1993) study of writers and In this section, it is argued that differences in
artists produced different results for those who the ways in which women and men deal with
had been treated for manic depression and those extrinsic motivational constraints may contrib-
who had not. Those who had not been treated ute to the observed differences in rates of men-
had similar trends in terms of both productivity tal illness among writers. Such factors appear to
and mood ratings: When they were in a better have a greater impact on women than on men,
mood, they were more likely to be productive. and this effect would be especially striking
Ratings of mood and productivity diverged, among women writers who have achieved crit-
however, among writers and artists who had ical acclaim. This theory is based on the follow-
been treated for manic depression. Productivity ing chain of reasoning.
peaks occurred before and after mood peaks by First, women tend to show greater concern
approximately 3 to 4 months. One reason for with maintaining interpersonal relationships
MENTAL ILLNESS 279

than men, and men tend to be less sensitive to themselves into maternal and caretaking roles”
the effects of interpersonal communications (p. 160) far more than boys.
than women. This results in greater attention by Gilligan (1982; Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Gil-
women to salient extrinsic constraints in the ligan, Lyons, & Hanmer, 1990; Gilligan, Ward,
environment (Claim 1). Second, high levels of & Taylor, 1988) has written extensively about
creativity require one to “defy the crowd” and to the different developmental trajectories of boys
ignore extrinsic motivational constraints (Claim and girls in the United States. One consistent
2). Third, because women are more attuned to finding is that girls are more sensitive to inter-
the needs of others and have more difficulty personal communications than boys. Girls value
ignoring extrinsic motivational constraints than group cohesiveness more, and they emphasize
men, the act of creating at high levels produces more than boys the importance of maintaining
more stress in female creative writers than in good relationships with others. Gilligan’s work
male creative writers (Conclusion 1). Finally, has been confirmed by other researchers in psy-
highly successful female creative writers—and chology and women’s studies (Belenky,
especially poets—typically suffer from greater Clinchy, Goldberger, & Tarule, 1986; Hancock,
psychological stress than both other women 1989; Pool, 1994).
who are not successful writers and successful These findings are consistent with personality
male creative writers. This heightened level of research showing that, across many different
stress causes psychological distress and results cultures, women score themselves as being
in a higher incidence of mental illness (Conclu- more “agreeable” on five-factor personality in-
sion 2). ventories (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae,
We first present evidence supporting 2001). In addition, women have been found to
Claims 1 and 2. These are the basic premises of assign more positive ratings to other people on
our argument, and they lead to the final two all of the Big Five personality traits (neuroti-
links in the chain of the argument, which are the cism, extraversion, openness to experience,
conclusions. agreeableness, and conscientiousness) than men
(Winquist, Mohr, & Kenny, 1998).
Claim 1 There is experimental evidence of this gender
difference in sensitivity to interpersonal com-
Piirto (1998a), who surveyed 80 successful munications that relates directly to creative per-
women creative writers about various aspects of formance. Baer (1997) instructed eighth-grade
their writing and personal lives, found that these participants (66 girls and 62 boys) to write
women “experienced conflict between the so- original poems and stories under conditions fa-
cial expectations of being a woman and being a voring both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation.
writer” (p. 61). This conflict, rooted in women’s In the intrinsic motivation condition, partici-
role in Western society as caretakers, begins at pants were told that their poems and stories
least as early as middle school and is not limited would not be evaluated; in the extrinsic condi-
to women who become writers. It is character- tion, participants were led to expect evaluation,
istic of 20th-century American women in and the importance of the evaluation was made
general. highly salient. The poems and stories were
“Caretaking has to be done,” Bateson (1989) judged for creativity by experts. Results re-
noted, and “somebody’s got to be the mommy” vealed a significant Gender X Motivational
(p. 140). Although the ability to empathize with Condition effect. Among boys, there were vir-
others, a willingness to pay close attention to tually no differences in creativity ratings under
others’ needs, and the interpersonal communi- intrinsic and extrinsic conditions; among girls,
cation skills needed by caretakers can be prac- however, these differences were quite large,
ticed and learned by all human beings, women with a significant drop in performance under
have traditionally taken up the role of caretaker extrinsic constraints.
in Western society more than men (and it is In three later studies conducted with middle
therefore immediately understood what Bateson school students, Baer (1998) found that both
meant when she stated that someone must be anticipated evaluation and rewards had a signif-
“the mommy”). From a very early age, Bateson icant impact on the creative performance of
(1989) argued, “girls are encouraged to imagine girls but not boys. Similarly, Kogan (1974),
280 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Cropley and Feuring (1971), and Katz and Poag assuming) share this trait of greater concern
(1979) found that girls were more susceptible to with interpersonal relationships and extrinsic
differences in testing situations and testing in- constraints that has been demonstrated in other
structions than boys taking divergent-thinking women.
tests. Baer (1997, 1998) argued that girls’
greater sensitivity to interpersonal communica- Claim 2
tions and desire to please others made them
more susceptible than boys to messages that Amabile (1996) and her colleagues have
would affect their levels of intrinsic and extrin- shown, in a wide variety of studies, that extrin-
sic motivation. sic constraints decrease creativity (and, con-
A caveat is necessary: With the exception of versely, that higher levels of intrinsic motiva-
Piirto’s (1998a) study of successful women tion are associated with higher levels of creative
writers, all of the research cited in this section performance). Although there may be condi-
refers to women (or girls) in general, not spe- tions under which extrinsic constraints do not
cifically to highly creative women or successful hinder and may even enhance creativity (e.g.,
women writers. What is true of other women see Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996, as well as
may not be true of the small subgroup— highly the “updates” that Amabile added to her original
successful writers—whose high incidence of [1983] book in its 1996 republication), the gen-
mental illness we are attempting to understand. erally negative impact on creative performance
In this regard, it has also been suggested that of attending to extrinsic constraints has been
creativity may be correlated with either biolog- well documented. Hennessey (Hennessey, Am-
ical or psychological androgyny. For example, abile, & Martinage, 1989; Hennessey &
Hassler (1992), in an analysis of 117 compos- Zbikowski, 1993) has also demonstrated exper-
ers, instrumentalists, and nonmusicians, offered imentally the importance of being able to ignore
evidence suggesting that the salivary testoster- extrinsic constraints, even when they are highly
one levels of male composers were lower than salient, and of the possibility of increasing this
those of male instrumentalists and nonmusi- ability through training.
cians and that the testosterone levels of female Amabile’s (1996) research evidence stemmed
composers were higher than those of female from studies of everyday or garden-variety cre-
instrumentalists and nonmusicians. In addition, ativity, however, not the high levels of creativity
Chrisler (1991) found that androgynous individ- associated with the women creative writers whose
uals (according to Bem’s, 1974, Sex-Role In- tendency to suffer from mental illness is being
ventory) scored higher on the verbal, but not the addressed here. Amabile (1996) did provide a
figural, forms of the Torrance Tests of Creative great deal of anecdotal evidence of the negative
Thinking (Torrance, 1962). effects of extrinsic constraints on genius-level cre-
Barron and Harrington (1981) listed a num- ativity and the importance for creators of minimiz-
ber of studies of femininity, masculinity, and ing these constraints (e.g., see pp. 5–16), as did
androgyny and reported that indices of each of Sternberg and Lubart (1995), whose book title
these three traits “were sometimes positively Defying the Crowd emphasized the importance of
and sometimes negatively associated with indi- being able to stand alone if one is to create at the
ces of creative achievement, ability, or self- highest levels. Moreover, Amabile (1996) argued
concept” (p. 458). This remains an intriguing that creativity is a continuum, with factors such as
possibility but one that, at present, is substanti- the salience of extrinsic motivational constraints
ated at best weakly (Baer, 1999, in press; Thur- important at all levels. Although many agree with
ston & Runco, 1999). If such a connection be- Amabile, there is not a strong consensus among
tween androgyny and creativity exists, how- creativity theorists on this issue of the similarity of
ever, it might have implications for our theory; creative processes underlying different levels of
specifically, this could mean that highly creative creative performance. However, even those who
women may be unlike other women in regard to suggest that creativity is a discontinuous concept,
attention to interpersonal communications and with qualitatively different processes affecting ge-
relationships. We recognize that more work nius-level creativity and more common garden-
needs to be done to assess whether highly suc- variety creativity, generally acknowledge the im-
cessful women writers do in fact (as we are portance for the creator of maintaining high levels
MENTAL ILLNESS 281

of intrinsic motivation and of not being distracted incidence of mental illness than male creative
by extrinsic constraints (e.g., see Gruber, 1981, writers (and female poets to have the highest
and Gruber & Davis, 1988). rate of mental illness among writers). And, in-
deed, this pattern has been observed (Kaufman,
Conclusion 1 2001b; Ludwig, 1995).

Success as a creative writer means (among Summary


other things) receiving increased public atten-
tion and acclaim, and it also brings more critical Piirto’s (1998a) study of contemporary
review of one’s work. All writers, successful women creative writers showed that these
and undiscovered alike, may sometimes fanta- women exhibit the “same personality character-
size about unknown future audiences of their istics and drive as men writers, but they also
work (and may even at times be haunted by the experience the conflict of being women and
thought of others’ anticipated evaluations of reconciling family duties with their creative
that work); among successful writers, however, work” (p. 68). The explanation that is being put
such thoughts are much more likely to be sa- forward here, based on a quite different line of
lient, because people actually are already read- evidence and argument, parallels Piirto’s con-
ing and reacting to their words (and presumably clusion with the exception that our emphasis is
will be doing so in the future). They have reason on differing ways in which women and men
to assume that there will be an audience for experience and deal with extrinsic environmen-
whatever they are currently working on, and tal constraints. Women creative writers in con-
book sales, awards, financial compensations, re- temporary Western society will tend to struggle
views, and so forth also remind them with great more than men with the increased salience of
force and regularity of the extrinsic motiva- others’ expectations and evaluations that ac-
tional factors that are attached to the kind of company creative success. Because poets in
creative work they are doing. general are already more at risk for mental
Why should having difficulty ignoring extrin- illness than other creative writers, female poets
sic constraints result in increased stress? Pro- suffer from a “double whammy” of factors con-
ducing work of great creativity while extrinsic tributing to mental illness.
constraints remain salient is much more difficult Piirto (1998a) suggested that young writers
than producing creative work when one’s mo- might either find comfort in her findings or
tivation is primarily intrinsic (Amabile, 1996). decide, on the basis of the difficulties such a life
The likely result of such tension is higher levels is likely to entail, to reject creative writing in
of psychological stress. And to the extent that favor of a more psychologically safe profession.
one is unable to ignore salient extrinsic con- It is hoped that the current analysis will also
straints (something that is generally more diffi- help women creative writers understand—and
cult for women than for men), the effort ex- perhaps find better ways to deal healthfully and
pended in suppressing one’s tendencies to at- productively with—the special demands of suc-
tend to such factors is likely to be tension cess and recognition as a writer.
producing.
Future Directions
Conclusion 2
Much work will need to be done to test the
According to many theories of mental illness, validity and the limits of the theory being pro-
such as the general diathesis–stress theory and posed. For example, do female poets—and es-
most psychodynamic theories, increased levels pecially successful female poets— exhibit the
of stress will typically lead to a greater likeli- same pattern of greater sensitivity to extrinsic
hood of mental illness. Women creative writers motivational constraints that has been observed
have higher levels of stress than their male in other women (in comparison with equally
counterparts, and poets have a higher incidence successful male poets)? This assumption needs
of mental illness than other creative writers to be verified. Also, do successful women cre-
(Kaufman, 2001b). Female creative writers ative writers who are not poets experience sim-
should therefore be expected to have a higher ilar patterns (in comparison with their male
282 KAUFMAN AND BAER

counterparts) in regard to (a) the amount of higher incidence of psychological stress, and of
attention they give to such extrinsic constraints mental illness, than male poets.
and (b) the effects of such attention to extrinsic A great deal of research has sought to deter-
motivators on their levels of stress and mental mine what types of mental illness are associated
illness? Research in these areas could help ei- with creativity. The illnesses examined have
ther to extend or to show the limits of the range included bipolar depression (e.g., Jamison,
of our theory. 1989, 1993; Richards, Kinney, Lunde, Benet, &
There are also questions regarding the under- Merzel, 1988), affective disorder (Richards,
lying processes that mediate these effects. Do Kinney, Daniels, & Linkins, 1992), psychoti-
male and female poets have similar or different cism (Eysenck, 1995), schizophrenia (e.g., An-
mechanisms for dealing with (or ignoring) ex- dreasen, 1987; Andreasen & Glick, 1988), neg-
trinsic constraints? And what techniques or ative schizotypy–social anhedonia (Claridge,
training might allow those who have difficulty 1997; Cox & Leon, 1999), and unipolar depres-
putting aside or ignoring such extrinsic motiva- sion (Post, 1996). Although all of this research
tors to acquire an “immunity” to them? Answers has certainly been valuable, it represents only a
to these questions would help us understand piece of the puzzle. If there is to be a complete
how the observed effects are produced and per- understanding of the ways in which mental ill-
haps give clues as to how to lessen the negative ness and creative writing interact, the form and
effects, which appear to fall most heavily on content of the writing must be examined at a
female poets, of the increasingly salient extrin- similar level of detail.
sic constraints that inevitably follow success Understanding why poets, and especially fe-
and recognition as a writer. male poets, so often succumb to mental illness
is of more than theoretical importance. It might
Conclusion also allow poets, their loved ones, their mentors,
and their therapists to act in ways that lead to
The adage that creativity and “madness” are better mental health among poets. It might, for
linked together is by and large supported by the example, make it possible for women writers to
existing research. This idea is currently being learn ways to deal more effectively with the
explored in many new ways and venues. One various extrinsic constraints that are likely to
especially well-established connection is that follow success (perhaps in ways analogous to
between the writing of poetry and the incidence the “immunization” studies that Hennessey and
of mental illness, and one of the more puzzling her colleagues conducted to help children keep
and unfortunate parts of this linkage is the un- extrinsic motivators in perspective; Hennessey
expectedly high rate of mental illness among et al., 1989; Hennessey & Zbikowski, 1993).
female poets. The purpose of this article was to explore
There are many possible reasons why poets possible reasons for the high rate of mental
may be more likely to experience mental illness illness among female poets. As such, it has been
than people who either write in other genres or somewhat speculative. We hope that these spec-
do not write at all. Poetry may attract those with ulations will not only help psychologists better
a predisposition toward mental illness, and the understand why so many talented female poets
domain of poetry may particularly reward those suffer from mental illness but also increase and
who exhibit mental illness. Unusual aspects of focus research on this topic. Empirical studies
the domain of poetry writing, such as the typical of writers from both past and present times can
age at which poets peak and the possible failure be used to eventually help writers of future
of poetry to provide the kinds of psychological generations.
benefits that often come with other kinds of
writing, may also increase the likelihood that
poets will succumb to mental illness. These References
domain-specific aspects of writing poetry affect Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of
men and women alike. In addition, the greater creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.
difficulty that women tend to experience in ig- Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update
noring extrinsic motivational constraints may to the social psychology of creativity. Boulder,
cause successful female poets to have an even CO: Westview Press.
MENTAL ILLNESS 283

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnos- Brown, L. M., & Gilligan, C. (1992). Meeting at the
tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th crossroads: Women’s psychology and girl’s devel-
ed.). Washington, DC: Author. opment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Anderson, R. N. (1999). The healing powers of cre- Press.
ativity: Using poetry in psychotherapy. Journal of Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds.
Individual Psychology, 55, 256 –265. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Andreasen, N. C. (1987). Creativity and mental ill- Burke, K. C., Burke, J. D., Regier, D. A., & Rae,
ness: Prevalence rates in writers and their first- D. S. (1990). Age at onset of selected mental
degree relatives. American Journal of Psychiatry, disorders in five community populations. Archives
144, 1288 –1292. of General Psychiatry, 47, 511–518.
Andreasen, N. C., & Canter, A. (1974). The creative Cassandro, V. J. (1998). Explaining premature mor-
writer: Psychiatric symptoms and family history. tality across fields of creative endeavor. Journal of
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 15, 123–131. Personality, 66, 805– 829.
Andreasen, N. C., & Glick, I. D. (1988). Bipolar Chrisler, J. C. (1991). The effect of premenstrual
affective disorder and creativity: Implications and symptoms on creative thinking. In D. L. Taylor &
clinical management. Comprehensive Psychia- N. F. Woods (Eds.), Menstruation, health, and
try, 29, 207–216. illness (pp. 73– 83). New York: Hemisphere.
Baer, J. (1997). Gender differences in the effects of Chubb, N. H., Fertman, C. I., & Ross, J. L. (1997).
anticipated evaluation on creativity. Creativity Re- Adolescent self-esteem and locus of control: A
search Journal, 10, 25–31. longitudinal study of gender and age differences.
Baer, J. (1998). Gender differences in the effects of Adolescence, 32, 113–129.
extrinsic motivation on creativity. Journal of Cre- Claridge, G. (Ed.). (1997). Schizotypy: Implications
ative Behavior, 32, 18 –37. for illness and health. Oxford, England: Oxford
Baer, J. (1999). Gender differences in creativity. In University Press.
M. A. Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Costa, P., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001).
creativity (pp. 753–758). San Diego, CA: Aca- Gender differences in personality traits across cul-
demic Press. tures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of
Baer, J. (in press). Gender differences in creativity. In Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 322–331.
M. A. Runco (Ed.), Creativity research handbook Cox, A. J., & Leon, J. L. (1999). Negative schizo-
(Vol. 3). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. typal traits in the relation of creativity to psycho-
Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic pathology. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 25–
stereotyping. Psychological Science, 7, 136 –141. 36.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying Cropley, A. J., & Feuring, E. (1971). Training cre-
theory of behavioral change. Psychological Re- ativity in young children. Developmental Psychol-
view, 84, 191–215. ogy, 4, 105.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self efficacy: The exercise of Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Implications of a sys-
control. New York: Freeman. tems perspective for the study of creativity. In R. J.
Barron, F., & Harrington, D. M. (1981). Creativity, Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of human creativity
intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of (pp. 313–338). New York: Cambridge University
Psychology, 32, 439 – 476. Press.
Bateson, M. C. (1989). Composing a life. New York: Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical
Penguin Books. exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In
Belenky, M., Clinchy, B., Goldberger, N., & Tarule, L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental so-
J. (1986). Women’s ways of knowing: The devel- cial psychology (pp. 39 – 80). New York: Aca-
opment of self, voice, and mind. New York: Basic demic Press.
Books. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motiva-
Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psycholog- tion and self-determination in human behavior.
ical androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical New York: Plenum.
Psychology, 42, 155–162. Eaton, W. W., Kramer, M., Anthony, J. C., Dryman,
Berman, L. E. A. (1995). An artist destroys his work: A., Shapiro, S., & Locke, B. Z. (1989). The inci-
Comments on creativity and destructiveness. In B. dence of specific DIS/DSM-III mental disorders:
Panter (Ed.), Creativity and madness (pp. 59 – 80). Data from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment
Burbank, CA: AIMED Press. Area Program. Acta Psychiatrica Scandi-
Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and navica, 79, 163–178.
controlled processes in stereotype priming. Jour- Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, effects of reward: Reality or myth? American Psy-
1142–1163. chologist, 51, 1153–1166.
284 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of Jamison, K. R. (1989). Mood disorders and patterns
creativity. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- of creativity in British writers and artists. Psychi-
versity Press. atry, 52, 125–134.
Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind (Rev. ed.) New Jamison, K. R. (1993). Touched with fire. New York:
York: Basic Books. Free Press.
Gilbert, D. T., Brown, R. P., Pinel, E. C., & Wilson, Katz, A. N., & Poag, J. R. (1979). Sex differences in
T. D. (2000). The illusion of external agency. instructions to “be creative” on divergent and non-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, divergent test scores. Journal of Personality, 47,
690 –700. 518 –530.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice. Cambridge, Kaufman, J. C. (2000, November). Narrative and
MA: Harvard University Press. paradigmatic thinking styles in creative writers
Gilligan, C., Lyons, N., & Hanmer, T. (Eds.). (1990). and journalists. Paper presented at the meeting of
Making connections: The relational worlds of ad- the International Society for Intelligence Research,
olescent girls at Emma Willard School. Cam- Cleveland, OH.
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Kaufman, J. C. (2001a). Genius, lunatics, and poets:
Gilligan, C., Ward, J. V., & Taylor, J. M. (1988). Mental illness in prize-winning authors. Imagina-
Mapping the moral domain. Cambridge, MA: Har- tion, Cognition, and Personality, 20, 305–314.
vard University Press. Kaufman, J. C. (2001b). The Sylvia Plath effect:
Goodwin, F. K., & Jamison, K. R. (1990). Manic- Mental illness in eminent creative writers. Journal
depressive illness. Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- of Creative Behavior, 35, 37–50.
versity Press. Kaufman, J. C. (in press). Narrative and paradigmatic
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit thinking styles in creative writers and journalists.
social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and ste- Journal of Creative Behavior.
reotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4 –27. Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Are there
Gruber, H. E. (1981). Darwin on man: A psycholog- mental costs to creativity? Bulletin of Psychology
ical study of scientific creativity (2nd ed.). Chi- and the Arts, 1, 38.
cago: University of Chicago Press. Kaysen, S. (1994). Girl, interrupted. New York: Vin-
Gruber, H. E., & Davis, S. N. (1988). Inching our tage.
way up Mount Olympus: The evolving-systems Kogan, N. (1974). Creativity and sex differences.
approach to creative thinking. In R. J. Sternberg Journal of Creative Behavior, 8, 1–14.
(Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 243–270). Kuehner, C., & Weber, I. (1999). Responses to de-
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. pression in unipolar depressed patients: An inves-
Hancock, E. (1989). The girl within. New York: tigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles the-
Random House. ory. Psychological Medicine, 29, 1323–1333.
Hassler, M. (1992). Creative musical behavior and Lepore, S. J. (1997). Expressive writing moderates
sex hormones: Musical talent and spatial ability in the relation between intrusive thoughts and depres-
the two sexes. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 17, 55– sive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social
70. Psychology, 73, 1030 –1037.
Helson, R. (1990). Creativity in women: Outer and Loranger, A. W., & Levine, P. M. (1978). Age at
inner views over time. In M. A. Runco & R. S. onset of bipolar affective illness. Archives of Gen-
Albert (Eds.), Theories of creativity (pp. 46 –58). eral Psychiatry, 35, 1345–1348.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ludwig, A. M. (1994). Mental illness and creative
Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M., & Martinage, M. activity in women writers. American Journal of
(1989). Immunizing children against the negative Psychiatry, 151, 1650 –1656.
effects of reward. Contemporary Educational Psy- Ludwig, A. M. (1995). The price of greatness. New
chology, 14, 212–227. York: Guilford Press.
Hennessey, B. A., & Zbikowski, S. M. (1993). Im- Ludwig, A. M. (1998). Method and madness in the
munizing children against the negative effects of arts and sciences. Creativity Research Journal, 11,
reward: A further examination of intrinsic motiva- 93–101.
tion training techniques. Creativity Research Jour- Lynton, H., & Salovey, P. (1997). The effects of
nal, 6, 297–307. mood on expository writing. Imagination, Cogni-
Honos-Webb, L., Harrick, E. A., Stiles, W. B., & tion, and Personality, 17, 95–110.
Park, C. L. (2000). Assimilation of traumatic ex- Marlo, H., & Wagner, M. K. (1999). Expression of
periences and physical-health outcomes: Cautions negative and positive events through writing: Im-
for the Pennebaker paradigm. Psychotherapy: The- plications for psychotherapy and health. Psychol-
ory, Research, Practice, Training, 37, 307–314. ogy and Health, 14, 193–215.
MENTAL ILLNESS 285

Martindale, C. (1972). Father absence, psychopathol- Richards, R. (1981). Relationship between creativity
ogy, and poetic eminence. Psychological Re- and psychopathology: An evaluation and interpre-
ports, 31, 843– 847. tation of the evidence. Genetic Psychology Mono-
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Sex differences in de- graphs, 103, 261–324.
pression. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Richards, R. (1993a). Everyday creativity, eminent
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Gender differences in creativity, and health: “Afterview” for CRJ issues
depression. Current Directions in Psychological on creativity and health. Creativity Research Jour-
Science, 10, 173–176. nal, 3, 300 –326.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. Richards, R. (1993b). Everyday creativity, eminent
(1999). Explaining the gender difference in depres- creativity, and psychopathology. Psychological In-
sive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social quiry, 4, 212–217.
Psychology, 77, 1061–1072. Richards, R. (1999). Affective disorders. In M. A.
Olsen, W. (1998). Poetry and emotion. Chicago Re- Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of cre-
view, 44, 149 –168. ativity (Vol. 1, pp. 31– 43). San Diego, CA: Aca-
Páez, D., Velasco, C., & González, J. L. (1999). demic Press.
Expressive writing and the role of alexythimia as a Richards, R., & Kinney, D. K. (1990). Mood swings
dispositional deficit in self-disclosure and psycho- and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 3,
logical health. Journal of Personality and Social 202–217.
Psychology, 77, 630 – 641. Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Daniels, H., & Linkins,
Parker, D. (1991). Portable Dorothy Parker (Rev. K. (1992). Everyday creativity and bipolar and
ed.). New York: Viking. unipolar affective disorder: Preliminary study of
Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Confession, inhibition, and personal and family history. European Psychia-
disease. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in exper- try, 7, 49 –52.
imental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 211–244). Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., &
New York: Academic Press. Merzel, A. P. C. (1988). Creativity in manic-de-
pressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and
Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Writing about emotional
control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
experiences as a therapeutic process. Psychologi-
ogy, 97, 281–288.
cal Science, 8, 162–166.
Rojcewicz, S. (1999). Medicine and poetry: The state
Pennebaker, J. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Linguistic
of the art of poetry therapy. International Journal
styles: Language use as an individual difference.
of Arts Medicine, 6, 4 –9.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
Rothenberg, A. (1990). Creativity and madness. Bal-
1296 –1312.
timore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Seagal, J. D. (1999). Forming a Rothenberg, A. (1995). Creativity and mental illness.
story: The health benefits of narrative. Journal of American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 815– 816.
Clinical Psychology, 55, 1243–1254. Rothenberg, A. (2000). Creativity and psychopathol-
Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism. Ameri- ogy. Bulletin of Psychology and the Arts, 1, 54 –58.
can Psychologist, 55, 44 –55. Rotter, J. B. (1990). Internal versus external control
Piirto, J. (1998a). Themes in the lives of contempo- of reinforcement: A case history of a variable.
rary U.S. women creative writers at midlife. American Psychologist, 45, 489 – 493.
Roeper Review, 21, 60 –70. Runco, M. A. (1998). Suicide and creativity: The
Piirto, J. (1998b). Understanding those who create case of Sylvia Plath. Death Studies, 22, 637– 654.
(2nd ed.). Scottsdale, AZ: Gifted Psychology Runco, M. A., & Richards, R. (1997). Eminent cre-
Press. ativity, everyday creativity, and health. Green-
Pool, R. (1994). Eve’s rib: Searching for the biolog- wich, CT: Ablex.
ical roots of sex differences. New York: Crown. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and
Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology: A extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new
study of 291 world-famous men. British Journal of directions. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
Psychiatry, 165, 22–34. ogy, 25, 54 – 67.
Post, F. (1996). Verbal creativity, depression and Sass, L. A., & Schuldberg, D. (Eds.). (2001). Cre-
alcoholism: An investigation of one hundred ativity and the schizophrenia spectrum [Special
American and British writers. British Journal of issue]. Creativity Research Journal, 13(1).
Psychiatry, 168, 545–555. Schaller, M. (1997). The psychological consequences
Preti, A., De Biasi, F., & Miotto, P. (2001). Musical of fame: Three tests of the self-consciousness hy-
creativity and suicide. Psychological Reports, 89, pothesis. Journal of Personality, 65, 291–309.
719 –727. Sethi, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1997). Gender
Preti, A., & Miotto, P. (1999). Suicide among emi- differences in internal and external focusing
nent artists. Psychological Reports, 84, 291–301. among adolescents. Sex Roles, 37, 687–700.
286 KAUFMAN AND BAER

Shapiro, P. J., & Weisberg, R. W. (2000). Positive Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York:
affect and creativity: Symptom of psychopathol- Cambridge University Press.
ogy or component of creative process? Bulletin of Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the
Psychology and the Arts, 1, 58 – 60. crowd. New York: Free Press.
Sherman, A. C., Higgs, G. E., & Williams, R. L. Stirman, S. W., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2001). Word
(1997). Gender differences in the locus of control use in the poetry of suicidal and non-suicidal poets.
construct. Psychology and Health, 12, 239 –248. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 517–523.
Silverman, M. A., & Will, N. P. (1986). Sylvia Plath Szajnberg, N. M. (1992). Psychoanalysis as an ex-
and the failure of emotional self-repair through tension of the autobiographical genre: Poetry and
poetry. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 55, 99 –129. truth, fiction and reality. International Review of
Simonton, D. K. (1975). Age and literary creativity: Psycho-Analysis, 19, 375–387.
A cross-cultural and transhistorical survey. Jour- Thurston, B. J., & Runco, M. A. (1999). Flexibility.
nal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 259 –277. In M. A. Runco & S. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia
Simonton, D. K. (1984). Genius, creativity, and lead- of creativity (pp. 729 –732). San Diego, CA: Aca-
ership. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. demic Press.
Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent.
Simonton, D. K. (1989). Age and creative productiv-
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
ity: Nonlinear estimation of an information-pro-
VanTassel-Baska, J. (1996). The talent development
cessing model. International Journal of Aging and
process in women writers: A study of Charlotte
Human Development, 29, 23–37. Bronte and Virginia Woolf. In K. Arnold, K. D.
Simonton, D. K. (1990). Psychology, science, and Noble, & R. F. Subotnik (Eds.), Remarkable
history. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. women (pp. 295–316). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton
Simonton, D. K. (1994). Greatness: Who makes his- Press.
tory and why. New York: Guilford Press. Whalen, S. (2000). Expressing attitudes and emotions
Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction through poetry. Journal of Student Health, 70, 301–
to the experimental study of inner experience. New 302.
York: Random House. Winquist, L. A., Mohr, C. D., & Kenny, D. A.
Smith, P. B., Dugan, S., & Trompenaars, F. (1997). (1998). The women positivity effect in the percep-
Locus of control and affectivity by gender and tion of others. Journal of Research in Personal-
occupational status: A 14 nation study. Sex ity, 32, 370 –388.
Roles, 36, 51–77. Zausner, T. (1998). When walls become doorways:
Spender, D. (1993). Women of ideas: And what men Creativity, chaos theory, and physical illness. Cre-
have done to them (Rev. ed.). London: Routledge ativity Research Journal, 11, 21–28.
& Kegan Paul.
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Mental self-government: A Received November 5, 2001
theory of intellectual styles and their development. Revision received March 20, 2002
Human Development, 31, 197–224. Accepted March 22, 2002 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like