You are on page 1of 46

Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2013 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 1(S), 2– 47 2326-5523/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/2326-5523.1.S.2

Psychobiology of Altered States of Consciousness

Dieter Vaitl Niels Birbaumer


University of Giessen University of Tübingen and University of Trento

John Gruzelier and Graham A. Jamieson Boris Kotchoubey and Andrea Kübler
Imperial College London University of Tübingen
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Dietrich Lehmann Wolfgang H. R. Miltner


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University Hospital of Psychiatry University of Jena

Ulrich Ott Peter Pütz


University of Giessen Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and
Mental Health

Gebhard Sammer Inge Strauch


University of Giessen University of Zurich

Ute Strehl Jiri Wackermann


University of Tübingen Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and
Mental Health

Thomas Weiss
University of Jena

The article reviews the current knowledge regarding altered states of consciousness
(ASC) (a) occurring spontaneously, (b) evoked by physical and physiological stimu-
lation, (c) induced by psychological means, and (d) caused by diseases. The emphasis

Dieter Vaitl, Center for Psychobiology and Behavioral Mental Health, Freiburg, Germany; Inge Strauch, Depart-
Medicine and Department of Psychology, University of ment of Clinical Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich,
Giessen, Giessen, Germany; Niels Birbaumer, Institute of Switzerland.
Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, Uni- This work was supported by the Institute for Frontier
versity of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany, and Center for Areas of Psychology and Mental Health, Freiburg, Ger-
Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Trento, Trento, It- many. The Altered States of Consciousness (ASC) Con-
aly; John Gruzelier and Graham A. Jamieson, Department sortium, founded in 1998, was devoted to the neurophys-
of Neuroscience and Psychological Medicine, Imperial iological, psychological, and behavioral components of
College London, London, United Kingdom; Boris Ko- altered states of consciousness. Several psychophysiologi-
tchoubey, Andrea Kübler, and Ute Strehl, Institute of Med- cal laboratories participated in this endeavor and ex-
ical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University changed their results, ideas, and concepts at four consec-
of Tübingen; Dietrich Lehmann, The KEY Institute for utive special symposia held in Sils-Maria, Switzerland,
Brain–Mind Research, University Hospital of Psychiatry, from 1999 to 2002.
Zurich, Switzerland; Wolfgang H. R. Miltner and Thomas Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-
Weiss, Department of Biological and Clinical Psychology, dressed to Dieter Vaitl, Clinical and Physiological Psychol-
University of Jena, Jena, Germany; Ulrich Ott and Gebhard ogy, Department of Psychology, University of Giessen,
Sammer, Center for Psychobiology and Behavioral Medi- Otto-Behaghel-Str 10, D-35394, Giessen, Germany.
cine, University of Giessen; Peter Pütz and Jiri Wacker- E-mail: dieter.vaitl@psychol.uni-giessen.de
mann, Department of Empirical and Analytical Psycho- This article is reprinted from Psychological Bulletin,
physics, Institute for Frontier Areas of Psychology and 2005, Vol. 131, No. 1, 98 –127.

2
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 3

is laid on psychological and neurobiological approaches. The phenomenological anal-


ysis of the multiple ASC resulted in 4 dimensions by which they can be characterized:
activation, awareness span, self-awareness, and sensory dynamics. The neurophysio-
logical approach revealed that the different states of consciousness are mainly brought
about by a compromised brain structure, transient changes in brain dynamics (discon-
nectivity), and neurochemical and metabolic processes. Besides these severe altera-
tions, environmental stimuli, mental practices, and techniques of self-control can also
temporarily alter brain functioning and conscious experience.

Consciousness has come under renewed sci- dated in a descriptive sense. This appeared then
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

entific investigation, with respect to both the to be the only way of synthesizing divergent
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

various levels of consciousness and the content approaches, given the fact that there were no
of consciousness, through advances in the mod- standard experimental procedures and compre-
els and methods of cognitive neuroscience (for hensive theoretical concepts for the diverse phe-
a review, see Gazzaniga, 2000; Mesulam, 2000; nomena of ASC that developed and continue to
Zeman, 2001). Everyday conscious awareness develop in relative isolation. This was exempli-
is but the tip of an iceberg, underneath which fied by the anthropological cross-cultural stud-
there is a realm of relatively uncharted pro- ies of Bourguignon (1966, 1973), which
cesses, which are likely to be just as complex as showed that in most cultures there were forms
those of so-called altered states of conscious- of institutionalized ASC that seemed to emerge
ness (ASC), which have tended to defy system- from common, basic psychobiological
atic elucidation; the brain is functionally in a capacities.
constant state of flux and alteration. There are The anthropological studies have revealed
now attempts to systematically explore, concep- the ubiquity of and the similarity between phe-
tualize, and place these phenomena within the nomena. Recently, a number of theories have
context of neuroscience, which is the subject of argued for common psycho- and neurophysio-
this review. We examine advances in under- logical processes underlying different ASC
standing the more extreme phenomena tradi- such as meditation, trance, and shamanism.
tionally included under the rubric of ASC (Lud- Mandell (1980), for instance, postulated hyper-
wig, 1966) that have accumulated about their synchronous hippocampal–septal (ictal or inter-
neurophysiological, cognitive, and biological ictal) activity occurring during microseizures to
underpinnings, together with interrelations be- be the biological bases for ecstatic and mystical
tween the various domains, including phenom- states. In the same vein, Persinger (1983)
enological analysis. As will be seen, so far the claimed that mystical experiences are associ-
evidence is largely empirical in nature, and ated with transient microseizures in the tempo-
there is no overarching model for ASC. How- ral lobe in nonepileptic individuals. However, it
ever, strategies toward this end are proposed in is questionable whether such transient paroxys-
the Discussion section. mal processes can account for the multiplicity
ASC as part of the general experiential and and diversity of ASC. In contrast, the present
behavioral repertoire have a long history, and review aims at a broader approach by shedding
ASC were widely accepted earlier when ideas light on both experiential and neurophysiologi-
were being explored of consciousness expan- cal diversities of ASC and by mapping the fea-
sion, meditation, LSD, and mystical practices. tures these various states have in common. Be-
Myriads of topics were considered but were yond the descriptive approach, explanatory
widely scattered and poorly defined. Several mechanisms are proposed for cases in which the
attempts were made to group the seemingly database and empirical evidence are strong
divergent phenomena and to organize their mul- enough to be discussed in detail.
tiplicity. For example, in reviewing the litera- In the past, some researchers attempted to
ture on the hypnagogic state, Schacter (1976) identify a common core of subjective changes
showed that the psychological and physiologi- produced by pharmacological and psychologi-
cal features characterizing the drowsy interval cal procedures (Dittrich, von Arx, & Staub,
between waking and sleeping could be eluci- 1985; Ludwig, 1966) or to describe ASC as
4 VAITL ET AL.

characteristic patterns of changes on the dimen- by their origin, that is, spontaneous, induced, or
sions of experience ranging from sensation to pathological. In the table some irreversible
self-awareness (Farthing, 1992; Pekala, 1991; pathological conditions have been included, al-
Tart, 1980). These approaches were mainly though some authors restrict the term ASC to
based on questionnaires and were helpful in reversible, short-term conditions only (Farthing,
delineating subjectively different ASC. Here it 1992). The multitude and heterogeneity of
will be seen that the application to ASC of states induced by pharmacological agents was
newly developed methods and techniques in not included here. These states are not consid-
cognitive neuroscience, including multichannel ered to be unimportant—to the contrary; how-
electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoen- ever, the members of the ASC Consortium fo-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cephalography (MEG), neuroelectric and neu- cused their research mainly on clinical condi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

romagnetic source imaging, positron emission tions or physiological and psychological


tomography (PET), and functional magnetic induction methods, which represent a broad
resonance imaging (fMRI), provide new in- field to be reviewed all by themselves (for re-
sights into altered brain functioning.1 cent reviews of pharmacological-induced ASC
Apart from focusing on such findings, this and affected neurotransmitter systems, see
review also includes models of neuroscientific Aghajanian & Marek, 1999; Parrott, 2001; Sny-
approaches to various ASC. The evidential base der, 1996; Vollenweider & Geyer, 2001). The
is mainly devoted to a multilevel approach, in- classification provided in Table 1 has been used
cluding biological, behavioral, cognitive, and to structure our review.
subjective domains, with the focus being largely
on studies including more than one domain. As
shown in Table 1, altered states can be classified Spontaneously Occurring ASC

During daily activities, spontaneous fluctua-


Table 1 tions in wakefulness, alertness, and vigilance
Domains Associated With Alterations of occur. They are subjectively experienced as be-
Consciousness Classified by Their Origin or ing part of phenomenal awareness, which oscil-
Method of Induction lates on the wakefulness– drowsiness–sleep on-
Origin Alteration set continuum. Four major classes of spontane-
Spontaneously ously occurring ASC are addressed here: states
occurring States of drowsiness of drowsiness, daydreaming, hypnagogic states,
Daydreaming and sleep and dreaming. Among the spontane-
Hypnagogic states ously occurring ASC, near-death experiences
Sleep and dreaming
Near-death experiences
Physically and Extreme environmental 1
Only EEG and MEG allow for the time resolution to
physiologically conditions (pressure, observe online changes of mental processes, both reflecting
induced temperature) summation of synchronized synaptic inflow to apical den-
Starvation and diet drites of the cortex. Whereas EEG originates from vertically
Sexual activity and orgasm oriented pyramidal cell columns, MEG reflects tangential
Respiratory maneuvers electromagnetic dipoles mainly located in the cortical sulci.
Sensory deprivation, Spatial resolution of EEG and MEG approaches 2 mm
Psychologically homogenization, and under special conditions (such as early evoked sensory
induced overload potentials and fields; for a review on EEG, see Rockstroh,
Rhythm-induced trance Elbert, Canavan, & Birbaumer, 1989). Subcortical activity
(drumming and dancing) can be visualized only with blood-flow-metabolic measures
Relaxation such as PET and fMRI. Time resolution of both is in the
Meditation order of seconds; spatial resolution is in the order of milli-
Hypnosis meters. Both record the increases or decreases of blood flow
Biofeedback as a consequence of neural activity, particularly local den-
Disease induced Psychotic disorders dritic field potentials (Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath, &
Coma and vegetative state Oeltermann, 2001). PET, through the injection of radioac-
Epilepsy tive ligands, allows specification of local accumulation of
Pharmacologically several metabolic products, such as glucose, dopamine,
induced (Not reviewed) fluoride, phosphor, opiates, and others (for an overview of
brain imaging, see Toga & Mazziotta, 2000).
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 5

are an infrequent and peculiar class of phenom- and sleepiness and drowsiness. Drowsiness
ena, detailed later in this section. states open ways to so-called hypnagogic states
at sleep onset, which are treated below as a
States of Drowsiness special, stand-alone class of ASC.

Variations of vigilance within the normal Daydreaming


sleep–wake cycle usually do not induce ASC.
Alterations of consciousness may occur with Daydreaming refers to spontaneous, subjec-
extreme sleep deprivation, resulting in short ep- tive experiences in a no-task, no-stimulus, no-
isodes of immediate sleep onset (microsleep; response situation (see also the sections on hyp-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Oswald, 1962) or amnesic automatic behavior nagogic states and sensory deprivation). Day-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Guilleminault, Billard, Montplaisir, & Dement, dreaming also includes unintended thoughts
1972). Subjective experience features narrowed that intrude inadvertently into the execution of
attention, reduced volitional potential and mo- intended mental tasks (Uleman & Bargh, 1989)
tivation, decrease of memory, and impaired and undirected ideas in thought sampling during
cognition. Time awareness may be significantly wakefulness (Klinger, 1978). Daydreaming
affected: Wackermann, Pütz, and Miener clearly differs from reality-focusing mentation,
(2001) found verbal overestimates of elapsed and several characteristics of daydreaming
time intervals (1– 40 min). Behavioral correlates strongly differ from those of rapid eye move-
of drowsiness and sleepiness comprise perfor- ment (REM) dreams; for example, daydreaming
mance decrements in psychomotor tasks, partic- showed a 100% increase of familiar settings and
ularly response omissions, and increased reac- an about 20% increase in emotional involve-
tion times in stimulus detection or discrimina- ment (Strauch & Meier, 1996). Implicitly, day-
tion tasks. As for physiological correlates, dreaming, when juxtaposed with night dream-
various parameters have been used to study ing, seems to imply waking in contrast to sleep,
fluctuations of vigilance and drowsiness (e.g., although the nature of daydreaming’s position
EEG, electrooculography, autonomic mea- on a vigilance scale remains a subject of day-
sures). Changes include a shift of EEG spectra dreaming studies. Much of the present day-
to slower frequencies (Davis, Davis, Loomis, dreaming literature concerns therapeutic appli-
Harvey, & Hobart, 1937); reduced latency/ cations of Freud’s free-association technique,
amplitude of event related potentials (P300; the proposed royal road to the unconscious
Harsh, 1994); slow eye movements, disappear- (Freud, 1900). Another large part of this litera-
ance of saccades, and reduced “blinks” (Ma- ture originates from James’s (1890) “stream of
tousek, & Petersén, 1979); and an increase in thought” concept and predominantly reports
pupil diameter variability (Lavie, 1979; Lowen- phenomenology, taxonomy, and psychological
stein & Loewenfeld, 1964). Established objec- and social conditions of daydreaming (Giambra,
tive EEG-based vigilance measures include 1999 –2000; Pope & Singer, 1978).
mostly EEG spectral measures and indices Neurophysiological studies on daydreaming
(Herrmann, Kubicki, & Röhmel, 1988; Jung, are rare. Cunningham, Scerbo, and Freeman
Makeig, Stensmo, & Sejnowski, 1997; Makeig (2000) reported that there were EEG power
& Inlow, 1993; Matejcek, 1982; Matousek & spectral signs of lowered vigilance prior to day-
Petersén, 1983; Stampi, Stone, & Michimori, dreaming being signaled by the subjects. A
1993); global brain state descriptors based on study on subjective experiences during day-
multichannel EEG have also been used (Wack- dreaming conditions and EEG power spectra
ermann, 1999). For a comprehensive discussion revealed correlations between distinctive fea-
of objective and subjective measures to assess tures in EEG spectra and cognition styles (Le-
sleepiness, see Curcio, Casagrande, and Bertini hmann, Grass, & Meier, 1995). One hundred
(2001). Assessment and classification of states twenty EEG spectral (power) variables and 20
of drowsiness is difficult owing to varying con- cognition or emotion variables resulted in four
cepts like activation continuum (Lindsley, significantly different, independent pairs of ca-
1960), wakefulness, alertness, vigilance (Head, nonical variables: Pair 1 had prominent 2– 6 Hz
1923; cf. Davies & Tune, 1970; Ulrich & and (left) 13–15 Hz EEG power with reality-
Gschwilm, 1988; Weinberg & Harper, 1993), remote, sudden undirected ideas of low recall
6 VAITL ET AL.

quality; Pair 2 had lowered 10 –13 Hz and phenomena were first described by J. Müller
15–25 Hz and (medial right) increased 4 – 6 Hz (1826/1967) as “fantastic visual phenomena”
power, with sudden undirected ideas but with (p. 20ff) occurring usually, but not exclusively,
good recall and visual imagery yet without emo- at sleep onset. Maury (1848) coined for them
tion. These two pairs belong to the hypnagogic the term hypnagogic, from Greek hypnos
family, whereas the remaining pairs were of the (sleep) and agogo (I bring). Schacter (1976)
awake type: Pair 3 had 10 –11 Hz and 19 –30 Hz described them as “dreamlets.” Subjects usually
power, with goal-oriented, concatenated report short visual percepts like faces, land-
thoughts related to the present and future, with scapes, and natural or social scenes that may or
little emotion; Pair 4 had a power profile may not be related to previous daytime experi-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

roughly inverted to Pair 2, with reality- and ence. These percepts may be of pseudohalluci-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

future-oriented thoughts and positive emotion. natory (i.e., with preserved insight of unreality)
Another study showed that EEG power spec- or truly hallucinatory (i.e., experienced as if
tral characteristics during a daydreaming condi- real) character. In contrast to dreams, hypnago-
tion were close to characteristics in ganzfeld gic experiences are usually rather static, without
conditions (see section on sensory homogeniza- narrative content, and the subject is not in-
tion) but differed from those during sleep onset volved as an actor (cf. Sleep and Dreaming
(Wackermann, Pütz, Büchi, Strauch, & Leh- section). Hypnagogic states have been exten-
mann, 2002). These studies examined relatively sively studied for their peculiar phenomenology
long EEG epochs of 16 s and 30 s; however, the (Leaning, 1925; Leroy, 1933; Linschoten, 1955)
basic units of mentation processes were esti- and symbolic character (Silberer, 1909). Hori,
mated to be well in the subsecond time range Hayashi, and Morikawa (1994) conceived of
(around 100 ms; Newell, 1992). Microstate hypnagogic states as a unique period that cannot
analysis of EEG data offers resolution in this be accurately categorized as either waking or
time range and has found that the human brain sleeping, and with unique behavioral, electro-
electric state is quasi-stable for fractions of sec- physiological, and subjective characteristics.
onds, then rapidly reorganizes into another Similar phenomena occurring at the transition
state, so that brain activity can be parsed into from sleep to wakefulness are called hypnop-
sequences of quasi-homogeneous temporal seg- ompic (Myers, 1904); here, however, it is diffi-
ments (microstates). Different classes of cult to differentiate hypnagogic imagery from
thoughts in a daydreaming condition were remnants of dream imagery. Hypnagogic-like
found to belong to different classes of micro- phenomena may also occur in daytime periods
states of about 120 ms duration (Lehmann, of reduced wakefulness and possibly superim-
Strik, Henggeler, Koenig, & Koukkou, 1998). posed over adequate sensory perceptions of the
Whether there are microstate classes specific for environment (cf. Mavromatis, 1987; Schacter,
daydreaming still needs to be investigated. In 1976; Sherwood, 2002). Subjective experience
sum, the seemingly continual stream of con- in hypnagogic states comprises vivid, mostly
sciousness is discontinuous, consisting of a se- very brief episodes of usually visual (86%) and
quence of concatenated, psychophysiological acoustical (8%) imagery with other sensory mo-
building blocks (“atoms of thought”; Lehmann dalities occurring less frequently and with an
et al., 1998) that follow each other in fractions average recall rate of 35%. There is more
of seconds and whose functional significance is awareness of the real situation in hypnagogic
identifiable as classes of subjective experiences. states than in dreaming (Hori et al., 1994). The
Further microstate analyses are needed to iden- prevalence for frequent hypnagogic states is
tify the psychophysiological building blocks of estimated at 37% (Ohayon, Priest, Caulet, &
daydreaming and their syntax, that is, their rules Guilleminault, 1996). Behavioral correlates are
of concatenations. sparse, for example, leg or arm jerks (“sleep
starts”) associated with illusionary body move-
Hypnagogic States ments (American Sleep Disorders Association,
1990; Sherwood, 2002). As for physiological
Hypnagogic states are transient states of de- correlates, an association between short flashes
creased wakefulness characterized by short ep- of dreamlike imagery and drop-offs in alpha
isodes of dreamlike sensory experience. These EEG activity was first noticed by Davis et al.
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 7

(1937). By definition, hypnagogic states are re- stage-controlled conditions, the general phe-
lated to sleep onset, that is, Sleep Stage 1 ac- nomenology of dreaming has been exhaustively
cording to Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968), but examined, ranging from night terrors to lucid
may occur even with presleep alpha EEG (Foul- dreams. From the outset, the association be-
kes & Schmidt, 1983; Foulkes & Vogel, 1965). tween various physiological signals and quality
Kuhlo and Lehmann (1964) studied hypnagogic of dream content has been investigated on dif-
states and their EEG correlates during drowsi- ferent levels, with intent to validate the subjec-
ness and sleep onset: Spontaneous, transient, tive experiences (Arkin, Antrobus, & Ellman,
fragmentary nonemotional visual and auditory 1978).
impressions of varying complexity were re- At the sleep-stage level, dreams from REM
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ported that were mostly experienced as unreal and non-REM states, although discriminable on
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and were associated with flattened or deceler- average (Monroe, Rechtschaffen, Foulkes, &
ated alpha and/or slow theta EEG activity; the Jensen, 1965), have proved to differ primarily in
authors postulated a gradual progression from rates of recall and extent, with REM dreams
hypnagogic hallucinations to fragmentary being better recalled and being longer (Antro-
dreams (cf. Lehmann et al., 1995). Systematic bus, 1983). Given comparable lengths, varia-
comparisons of hypnagogic phenomena with tions between sleep stages were a less signifi-
perceptual phenomena that were observed with cant factor (Foulkes & Schmidt, 1983). Dreams
reduced sensory input (cf. section on sensory outside of REM can be as bizarre and emotional
homogenization and “ganzfeld”) led us to con- as REM dreams, and REM dreams can be as
ceptualize a broader class of “hypnagoid” phe- mundane and bland as dreams from other stages
nomena (Wackermann, Pütz, Büchi, Strauch, & (Strauch & Meier, 1996).
Lehmann, 2000, 2002), of which the true hyp- More specifically, physiological variables
nagogic hallucinations are a special case, and in have been selected by apparent face validity.
spite of distinctly different brain functional The scanning hypothesis, implying that inci-
states at sleep onset and in ganzfeld, subjective dence and directional changes of eye move-
experience exhibits very similar features. ments during REM sleep are related to the
dreamer’s viewing pattern (Ladd, 1892), first
Sleep and Dreaming yielded promising results (Roffwarg, Dement,
Muzio, & Fisher, 1962), but these could not be
Dreaming, which is not restricted to REM replicated (L. Jacobs, Feldman, & Bender,
sleep but has been found to take place in all 1972). Weak associations were also found be-
sleep stages, represents an ASC, characterized tween on– off eye movements versus visualiza-
by a virtual sense of reality, a wide range of tion and cognitive elaboration (Foulkes & Pope,
primarily visual perceptions, covert speech, mo- 1973). Furthermore, little correspondence has
tor activities, emotions, and social interactions. existed between phasic muscle activation in
Typical of dreams are a narrative structure and arms and legs and dreamed motor activity (R.
elaborations, ranging from the realistic to the Gardner, Grossman, Roffwarg, & Weiner,
fantastic. However, parallel cognitive process- 1975), between middle ear muscle activity and
ing and metacognition are reduced, while the auditory perceptions (Pessah & Roffwarg,
emphasis is on the here and now. A virtual 1972), between speech muscle activation and
sense of reality (except for the rare category of verbal activity (Shimizu & Inoue, 1986), and
lucid dreams, discussed below) and a predomi- between phasic bursts of the facial corrugator
nant ego involvement are the only continuous and zygomatic muscles and emotions (Gerne &
features of dreaming; all of the other features Strauch, 1985).
are more or less phasic events (Strauch & In the majority of spontaneous dreams, self-
Meier, 1996). reflection and control of the dream are only
Since the discovery of REM sleep by Aser- moderately present (Purcell, Moffitt, & Hoff-
insky and Kleitman in 1953, in the majority of mann, 1993). Lucidity during REM sleep—
dream research studies, physiological record- when not associated with microawakenings—
ings have been primarily used to determine represents a rather rare and exceptional category
sleep stages and the appropriate time to awaken of dreaming, whereby the dreamer is aware of
a subject. By collecting reports under such the dream and capable of influencing dream
8 VAITL ET AL.

events. Selected subjects can be taught to bring online but only retrospectively while one is
such experiences about more frequently and to awake. Owing to the functional state shifts in-
signal onset of lucidity, for example, by inten- volved in dreaming and recall (Koukkou & Le-
tional eye movements. Such prearranged sig- hmann, 1983), a subject’s ability to recall a
nals, executed and recorded during REM sleep, dream may be incomplete or totally inhibited.
allow the further investigation of experiential– Furthermore, the conversion of a dream experi-
physiological parallelism (LaBerge, 2000). ence into a verbal report may go along with
Recently, Foulkes (1996) reviewed 40 years transpositions and omissions, which makes it
of dream research. He came to the conclusion more difficult to reliably assign at a given mo-
that the results of psychophysiological studies ment a physiological signal to a psychological
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

on the whole have tended to be weak and un- event. Therefore, as far as the altered state of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

replicable and that “the bloom definitely was off dreaming is concerned, the wide gap between
the psychophysiological association by 1980” physiology and psychology still has to be
(Foulkes, 1996, p. 613). bridged.
A more basic quest for the locations of the
generators of dreaming is of current interest. Near-Death Experiences
Murri, Arena, Siciliano, Mazzotta, and Murato-
rio (1984) reported frequent loss of dream recall The typical core elements of near-death ex-
after unilateral posterior lesions but rarely after periences include (a) a feeling of peacefulness
anterior lesions. Solms (1997) observed that and well-being, (b) a separation from the body
patients with bilateral lesions in the occipito- (out-of-body experience), (c) a dark tunnel ex-
temporal region had no visual dreams and that perience, (d) a brilliant light associated with
posterior cortical or bilateral frontal lesions mystical feelings of love and union, and (e) a
were associated with a global cessation of heavenly landscape (often relatives, religious
dreaming. He has claimed that the forebrain is a figures, or beings of light appear and finally
common path to dreaming, whereas Hobson initiate the return to the body). Other elements
maintains that dreaming is “a result of the ex- are the hearing of music, a slowing of time and
citation of forebrain circuits by impulses arising speeding of thoughts, and a panoramic life re-
in the ascending activation systems of the brain view. The incidence of near-death experiences
stem . . . and basal forebrain” (Hobson, 1999, p. is estimated to lie between 10% and 50% of all
171). Present imaging techniques (e.g., PET) near-death situations and is independent of gen-
during REM have yielded different results: ac- der, age, and profession (Schroeter-Kunhardt,
tivation of extrastriate visual cortices, attenua- 1993). The circumstances of the close brush
tion of primary visual cortices, activation of with death (e.g., from illness, accident, suicide,
limbic and paralimbic regions, and attenuation or anesthetics) have only a minor influence on
of frontal association areas (Braun et al., 1998). the occurrence and features of the near-death
A detailed discussion of the psychophysiology experiences (Greyson, 2000). This invariance
of sleeping and dreaming research was recently suggests a specific neurophysiological mecha-
published in Behavioral and Brain Sciences nism taking place in the dying brain. Accord-
(Harnad, 2000). Up to now, no coherent model ingly, several hypotheses have been formulated
of the neural underpinnings of dreaming has regarding the neurophysiological processes (ce-
been evolved that integrates the findings from rebral anoxia, depletion of neurotransmitter res-
lesion and imaging studies. At best, these dif- ervoirs, release of endorphins, general disinhi-
ferent approaches may further clarify which bition of the brain) and structures (limbic sys-
brain areas are specifically activated during var- tem, septohippocampal formation, temporal
ious sleep stages; however, they will not be able lobes, visual cortex) that have been involved in
to decode the content of dreaming. the generation of near-death experiences
The results of numerous studies undertaken (Blackmore, 1996; Morse, Venecia, & Milstein,
to relate the experience of dreaming to accom- 1989; Saavedra-Aguilar & Gomez-Jeria, 1989).
panying bodily processes have so far proved to Because no experiments can be performed to
be modest, if not discouraging. Psychophysio- induce real near-death experiences, the neuro-
logical dream research is faced with the insol- physiological explanatory models are mainly
uble problem that dreaming cannot be reported based on speculations and analogue experi-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 9

ments that produce ASC with features similar to illusions, or states of trance. However, although
those observed during near-death experiences, most of these reports have captured large public
such as a body acceleration reducing the cere- interest (i.e., Messner, 2001), little systematic
bral blood supply until a loss of consciousness research is available on the effects of such en-
occurs (Whinery, 1997); the administration of vironments on states of consciousness.
drugs, like ketamine (Jansen, 1997); or the elec- Research on psychological effects of extreme
tromagnetic stimulation of the temporal lobes environmental conditions covers a broad range
(Persinger, 1999). After a critical evaluation of of life-threatening environments. Conditions
these approaches Greyson (2000) concluded explored include exposure to high altitudes in
that no single approach is able to account for all mountaineers of Himalayan expeditions, hyper-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of the features of near-death experiences. Nev- baric underwater conditions on divers, low tem-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ertheless, the study of those elements that occur perature conditions on members of Arctic and
in many conditions, like the out-of-body expe- Antarctic research groups, extensive heat on
rience, improves the prospects of elucidating extreme athletes bicycling or walking through
the brain mechanisms underlying the entire se- desserts, and exposure to microgravity during
quence of events. space flights.
In one of the few empirical studies (Brugger,
Conclusions Regard, Landis, & Oelz, 1999), the incidence
and the circumstances of ASC were investigated
The bulk of evidence on ASC whenever they retrospectively by structured interview on the
occur spontaneously supports changes in corti- effects of exposure to altitudes above 8,500 m
cal activity and arousal levels. They are subjec- without supplementary oxygen in eight world-
tively experienced as dreamlike, illusionary, class climbers. Additionally, subjects were ex-
and hallucinatory and, in many cases, are quite amined by neuropsychological tests and electro-
deviant from normal alert, waking, and vigilant encephalographic and MRI techniques within a
states. These changes are transient in nature and week following the interview. All but one of the
immediately vanish when the central arousal mountaineers reported distortions of the body
system returns to normal levels either by vol- schema to be the major type of somesthetic
untary control, biological rhythms (sleep–wake experience followed by visual and auditory hal-
cycles), or resuscitation. lucinations. These illusions and hallucinations
were reported more frequently at altitudes
Physically and Physiologically Induced ASC above than below 6,000 m. Hallucinations and
illusions were not related to brain abnormalities
Besides pharmacological induction, a wide and were suggested to be consequences of se-
variety of physical and physiological methods vere hypoxia and social deprivation and acute
and maneuvers may result in alterations of con- stress conditions experienced during mountain-
sciousness or even lead to short- or long-term eering. The majority of studies on the negative
loss of consciousness. One pivotal class of cir- effects of exposure to extremely high altitudes
cumstances or situations under which these det- on cognitive, emotional, and behavioral func-
rimental effects have been observed includes tioning so far suggest that visual and auditory
extreme environmental conditions and starva- illusions or hallucinations seem mainly to be
tion. In contrast to these possibly life-threaten- caused by the development of severe hypoxia
ing environmental and behavioral conditions, and acute mountain sickness in those subjects
there are other physiological conditions that who did not adjust properly prior to exposure to
may result in ASC, such as sexual activity and high altitudes or who stayed at extreme heights
orgasm, described by the French expression la for too long a period (Hackett & Roach, 2001).
petite mort, and forced respiratory maneuvers. Although similar anecdotal reports on the
emergence of illusions, hallucinations, cogni-
Extreme Environmental Conditions tive dysfunctions, and negative emotional states
exist for professional and experienced scuba
Behaving in extreme environments has been divers (Chowdhury, 2000), no systematic re-
reported anecdotally to induce ASC, including search is available on what incidence, types, and
auditory or visual hallucinations, somesthetic circumstances of exposure to hyperbaric condi-
10 VAITL ET AL.

tions are associated with the emergence of ASC phenomena of cosmonauts or astronauts (Carr,
or different cognitive and behavioral dysfunc- 1991) during spaceflights. Again, it is assumed
tions. Although some experimental studies in that such responses are paroxysmal conse-
simulated hyperbaric conditions reported decre- quences of primary diseases or of sudden ill-
ments in attention, memory, vision, audition, nesses (Dudley Rowley, 1997).
and sensation (Charles, Allimann, & Ragot,
2001; Massion, Fabre, Mouchnino, & Obadia, Starvation and Diet
1995), a number of studies did not (Abraini et
al., 1997; Bast-Pettersen, 1999). The emergence Starvation and extreme diet are accompanied
of ASC or different cognitive and behavioral by a number of (patho)physiological alterations
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dysfunctions is supposed more likely to be re- and are associated with cognitive, social, emo-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

lated to present paroxysmal symptoms resulting tional, attentional, and behavioral changes (e.g.,
from individual narcotic potencies of gases con- Maddox & Long, 1999; Peterson & Mitchell,
tained in the breathing mixtures (Abraini, 1995) 1999). In strong dieters as well as in anorectic
inhaled by divers while being in deep water for patients, extreme starvation and diet might in-
long periods of time (Tetzlaff et al., 1999). duce ASC, as expressed by an attentional bias
There is also no systematic study available on for food stimuli, disturbed body image, illu-
the effects of prolonged heat exposure in ex- sions, or hallucinations.
treme athletes walking or bicycling through On the basis of studies using a modified ver-
desserts. sion of the Stroop test, short-term diet was
Overwintering in Arctic or Antarctic environ- shown not to be associated with an attentional
ments is a common strategy for investigating bias. In this test, subjects were requested to
the effects of extreme environments on psycho- name the color of words belonging to different
logical and physiological adaptation, perfor- categories (food and body image vs. neutral in
mance, and well-being. Although reports of hal- relation to these categories) but not to attend to
lucinations, sensory illusions, and hypnotic the content. It was found that extreme dieters
states have also been presented for these envi- and anorectic patients show increased reaction
ronments, systematic research is missing. From times to food-related words not observed in
some studies of cognitive and behavioral dys- normal subjects or fasting persons (e.g., Ben-
functions (Bhargava, Mukerji, & Sachdeva, Tovim & Walker, 1991; Huon & Brown, 1996).
2000; Brennen, Martinussen, Hansen, & Hjem- Additionally, anorectics but not dieters or con-
dal, 1999) and emotional disorders (Haggarty, trols responded with increased reaction times to
Cernovsky, Kermeen, & Merskey, 2000), it can words related to body image.
be inferred that most ASC are strongly associ- Data belonging to the scope of the review are
ated with the emergence of different diseases often available only for anorectic patients, that
and illnesses (e.g., shining, cottage fever, infec- is, for starvation in its pathological version.
tious diseases, brain injuries) or with acute Starvation in anorectic patients was demon-
stress following interpersonal conflicts between strated to be associated with decreased regional
group members (Kahn & Leon, 1994; Palinkas, cerebral blood flow in the right hemisphere that
1992; Wood, Lugg, Hysong, & Harm, 1999). normalized after weight gain (Delvenne et al.,
Additionally, such states might be possible con- 1996, 1997). Additionally, it was shown that
sequences of sudden failures of life-supporting early and late components of evoked potentials
technical equipment being perceived as extreme to somatosensory stimulation were decreased
stress conditions (Palinkas, Gunderson, John- (e.g., Lautenbacher, Pauls, Strian, Pirke, &
son, & Holland, 2000). Krieg, 1991) and that spectral power of EEG
Despite some evidence that prolonged expo- background activity was reduced in the alpha-1
sure to microgravity might affect cognitive band in strong dieters and anorectic patients, but
functioning and motor behavior in negative also there was decreased theta EEG power over
ways (Brubakk, 2000; Fowler, Comfort, & the right parietal cortex during haptic explora-
Bock, 2000; Friederici & Levelt, 1990; Manzey tion tasks that remained still modified after
& Lorenz, 1999), no systematic studies are weight gain (Grunwald et al., 2001).
available indicating that hallucinations, sensory In summary, there exists some evidence for
illusions, or other subtypes of ASC are common an attentional bias to food in extreme dieters
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 11

that seems to be accompanied by changes in Redouté et al. (2000) presented their male sub-
regional cerebral blood flow and EEG power. jects with visual sexual stimuli in the PET scan-
However, further research is needed to clarify ner. They found a highly significant bilateral
the basis of these alterations. increase of regional cerebral blood flow in the
claustrum and putamen, which was positively
Sexual Activity and Orgasm correlated with perceived sexual arousal. An-
other activated region was the nucleus accum-
In several studies over the past 50 years, bens and the rostral part of the anterior cingulate
various changes in the electrical activity of the gyrus. The nucleus accumbens serves as the
brain during sexual arousal and orgasm have common final end-path of the dopamine incen-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

been described. Mosovich and Tallaferro (1954) tive system and therefore plays an unspecific
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

reported rapid low-voltage activity during early role in the organization of sexual responses (see
stages of sexual arousal that was followed by Rolls, 1999). The magnitude of activation of the
slow high-voltage paroxysmal activity during latter correlated strongly with penile tumes-
orgasm. Heath (1972) recorded subcortical and cence. Stimulation of this region leads to erec-
cortical EEG with implanted electrodes in two tion in monkeys (Dua & MacLean, 1964). Like-
patients and found slow wave and spike activity wise, Rauch et al. (1999) found activation in the
with interspersed fast activity mainly in the rostral region of the anterior cingulate gyrus
septal region during orgasm. These waveforms during sexual and competitive arousal. In con-
matched paroxysmal discharges in epilepsy. trast, activity in temporal lobes decreased dur-
H. D. Cohen, Rosen, and Goldstein (1976) ing an increase of sexual arousal (Redouté et al.,
showed that alpha waves in both hemispheres 2000).
during baseline give way to high-amplitude To summarize, subcortical paroxysmal and
4-Hz activity over the right parietal lobe during right hemispheric high-amplitude slow activity
orgasm, which was not visible during a “faked appear to be related to the partial loss of con-
orgasm.” This interhemispheric asymmetry was sciousness during orgasm. Along with sexual
confirmed by Tucker and Dawson (1984). They arousal and orgasm, a lateralized right hemi-
found less alpha power and higher coherence in spheric activation occurs. These modifications
central and posterior regions of the right com- of the EEG clearly classify orgasm as a specific
pared with the left hemisphere during sexual ASC not comparable to any other psychophys-
arousal induced by imagery. The results were iological states.
replicated by A. S. Cohen, Rosen, and Goldstein
(1985) with erotic films. Flor-Henry (1980) Respiratory Maneuvers
stated that in epilepsy patients with seizures
leading to orgasm, the orgasmic response was Among methods of inducing ASC, changes
triggered by discharges in the right hemisphere. in breathing patterns are of significant interest.
In contrast to the described profound changes in Most breath manipulations are based primarily
the EEG, Graber, Rohrbaugh, Newlin, Varner, on Yoga and Zen practices, and they include
and Ellingson (1985) found only a small de- passive breath mindfulness and breathing (Lich-
crease in alpha activity during masturbation and stein, 1988). This requires the trainee to focus
ejaculation. attention on breathing and allows slow and shal-
Tiihonen et al. (1994) confirmed the results low respiration to emerge. In meditation tech-
of lateralized right hemispheric activation dur- niques, breath manipulation works with mantra
ing orgasm with single positron emission com- chanting, counting, and maintaining a fixed
puted tomography. They found increased re- gaze on an external object or cue word. Deep
gional cerebral blood flow at the right prefrontal breathing consists of taking a deep breath, re-
cortex. taining the breath, and exhaling slowly. The
Neurosurgery and brain lesions after injury method of slow diaphragmatic breathing and
have revealed the involvement of frontal and paced respiration is also a procedure that alone
temporal areas in the inhibitory control of sex- or in combination with other meditative tech-
ual behavior (Freeman, 1973; Terzian & Dalle niques leads to tension reduction in body mus-
Ore, 1955) and septal nuclei in the control of culature and to the balance of neurovegetative
sexual arousal (Gorman & Cummings, 1992). regulatory systems. The mechanisms of these
12 VAITL ET AL.

breathing techniques are based on increases in Forced respiration during hyperventilation


pCO2, resulting in hypercapnia. Breath holding has rapid and far-ranging physiological effects
(for 5–10 s) and shallow, slow breathing pro- via its alteration of pH and depletion of CO2 in
ducing hypoventilation are two methods with the body, resulting in acute or chronic respira-
which to create mild hypercapnia including a tory alkalosis (hypocapnia). The cerebral circu-
slow heart rate, dilation of the peripheral vas- lation is highly sensitive to respiratory alkalosis,
culature, stimulation of gastric secretion, de- which develops within the first 15–20 s of hy-
pressed cortical activity, and a global sensation perventilation. As a consequence, pronounced
of mild somnolence (Lichstein, 1988). This nor- hypocapnia (PaCO2/22 mmHg or less) affects
mally occurs in the transition from wakefulness regional and local cerebral hemodynamics, cir-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to drowsiness, in hypnagogic states, and in culation, and oxygen supply. Hyperventilation-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sound sleep (Naifeh, Kamiya, & Sweet, 1982). induced changes in EEG (slow waves in the
Similar hypercapnia effects can also be frontal leads, hypersynchronization) were found
evoked by inhaling a CO2-enriched breathing to be identical to the hypoxia-induced changes,
air mixture. Meduna (1950) reported that hyper- such as arteriole vasospasm, ischemic foci, and
capnia may result in typical near-death experi- redistribution of the blood flow between various
ences, such as bodily detachment and the per- brain regions (Paulson & Sharbrough, 1974). In
ception of being drawn toward a bright light. both cases, fainting, obnubilation, depersonal-
Both phenomena were associated with power ization, and similar forms of ASC may occur.
increases in EEG low-frequency bands. A re- The interaction between respiratory maneu-
cent study by Terekhin (1996), however, failed vers, such as hyperventilation and alteration of
to confirm these findings. Here a CO2-enriched consciousness, can also be considered under
breathing air mixture did not lead to slow-wave clinical perspectives. Hyperventilation repre-
EEG and concomitant experiences of obnubila- sents a well-established EEG activation proce-
dure aimed at enhancing epileptiform dis-
tion, depersonalization, and derealization.
charges, which may result in impairment of
These differences may largely be traced back to
consciousness (absence). Typical ictal absences
the varying amount and duration of inhaling a
have been precipitated by hyperventilation in
CO2-enriched air mixture.
about 90% of untreated patients (Panayiotopou-
Breathing has also been manipulated by tech- los, 2001).
niques that lead to hypocapnia, such as shaman- Independent of respiration-induced hyper-
ism practices, ritual dances, holotropic breath- and hypocapnia, there is another, more gentle
ing (Grof, 1976), and rebirthing. In this context, respiratory maneuver that is an important con-
breathing plays a pivotal role. Involuntary hy- stituent of yogic (Pranayama) breathing exercis-
perventilation often accompanies hard physical es—that is, unilateral nostril breathing, where
work, long-lasting emotional tension, and en- the airflow is forced through each nostril in turn.
during mental effort. Hyperventilation is also Stancak, Hönig, Wackermann, Lepicovska, and
involved in panic attacks and their clinical Dostalek (1991) studied young subjects during
symptoms (e.g., dyspnea, vertigo, palpitations, bilateral and 15 min of unilateral nostril breath-
chest pain, numbness or tingling, depersonaliza- ing. Besides cardiovascular changes (e.g., in-
tion, fear of losing control; W. N. Gardner, creased respiratory sinus arrhythmia), they
1996). When healthy subjects were required to observed lowered peak power of beta-2 EEG
voluntarily hyperventilate, 83% experienced activity in the frontal leads during unilateral
syncopes marked by an incipient fall (Lempert, nostril breathing, indicating a relaxation-
Bauer, & Schmidt, 1994). Arrhythmical my- specific effect, as found in the study by G. D.
cloni occurred in 90% of 42 syncopal episodes. Jacobs, Benson, and Friedman (1996). In addi-
Visual (e.g., colored patches, bright lights, gray tion, they found a homolateral relationship be-
haze) and auditory (e.g., roaring noises, scream- tween the nostril airflow and EEG theta activity,
ing) hallucinations were reported by 60%. Com- which they attributed to a lateralized modula-
monly, subjects described a state of impaired tion of the subcortical generators of EEG theta-
external awareness, disorientation, weightless- band during unilateral nostril breathing.
ness, detachment, and loss of voluntary motor The similarity of electrocortical brain func-
control. tions during hyperventilation and ASC is some-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 13

what striking, although their origins may largely auditory stimulation was strongly minimized
vary (for discussion, see Schwartz, 1995). Gen- temporarily. A further method was based on the
erally, this suggests that ASC-like phenomena use of floatation tanks (Lilly, 1977), where vi-
can be evoked by changes in the respiratory sual and auditory restriction is supplemented by
pattern and that they share common basic mech- a significant reduction of tactile stimulation. In
anisms of respiratory– circulatory– electrocorti- addition to sensory restriction by isolation
cal interaction (Grossman, Janssen, & Vaitl, chambers, floatation-based minimal stimulation
1986). further induces the illusion of low gravity and a
sensation of true floatation (Suedfeld, 1980).
Conclusions It has been shown that restricted environmen-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tal stimulation significantly affects a number of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The preceding data suggest that physical ex- physiological functions in humans, including
haustion, metabolic challenges, and sexual ac- reductions of plasma levels of epinephrine, nor-
tivity exert, directly or indirectly, a strong in- epinephrine (Hamad, Fine, & Turner, 1996;
fluence on brain functions. Here it becomes Lilly, 1956), and stress hormones (Schulz &
evident that the subjective experiences may dif- Kaspar, 1994; Turner & Fine, 1983). Further-
fer considerably according to the deficits or more, restriction of sensory input was shown to
surpluses in energy supply to the brain, pro- be associated with an increase of beta-endor-
voked by extreme environmental conditions or phin levels in the peripheral blood not seen in a
behavioral practices. relaxation control condition (Turner & Fine,
1983).
Psychologically Induced ASC Further effects of restricted stimulation have
been demonstrated for memory functions, cre-
In Eastern cultures, methods for altering con- ativity, perception and signal detection, social
sciousness are embedded in religion and the cognition, and the readiness to change one’s
philosophy of human destiny and personal attitudes on social phenomena inducing in-
growth, whereas the Western approach mainly creased motivation to change critical and mal-
focuses on scientific exploration of human con- adaptive behavior patterns (for a summary, see
sciousness by denuding its alterations of reli- Atkinson, 1993; Suedfeld, 1980). Autobio-
gious context and philosophical interpretation. graphical life episodes were retrieved more in-
The induction procedures reviewed in the fol- tensely and recalled more pleasantly after par-
lowing paragraphs cover a wide range from ticipation in a restricted stimulation treatment as
practices such as meditation and drumming to compared with a normal rest condition (Sued-
experimental stimulation procedures and clini- feld & Eich, 1995). Furthermore, when exposed
cal interventions. to a recognition memory test for words and
unfamiliar faces (validated on neurological pa-
Sensory Deprivation, Homogenization, and tients with left and right hemispheric lesions,
Overload respectively) before and after restricted stimu-
lation, subjects showed better performance fol-
Sensory deprivation. Abolishing or mini- lowing restricted stimulation than following a
mizing external sensory input to the human control condition. Memory performance indi-
brain affects all levels of human functioning. cated a significantly greater enhancement of
Suedfeld (1980) has suggested differentiation right hemispheric processing after floatation
between sensory deprivation following total than after the control condition (Raab & Gru-
sensory loss due to trauma or disease and con- zelier, 1994). Additionally, it was shown that
ditions in which environmental stimulation is subjects recovering from electroconvulsive
not blocked totally but limited in terms of its shock therapy showed less severe memory loss
variability, patterning, meaningfulness, or phys- when exposed to restricted stimulation immedi-
ical intensity. ately after shock therapy as compared with a
Research on restricted environmental stimu- condition in which subjects simply returned to
lation has usually been conducted by exposing their hospital room (Suedfeld, Ramirez, Re-
subjects to specially designed chambers (Zubek, mick, & Fleming, 1989). Evaluation of the ef-
Hughes, & Shephard, 1971) where visual and fects of floatation isolation on creativity showed
14 VAITL ET AL.

an increase in vigor and a maintenance of curi- fewer time estimation errors, behavioral activity
osity scores (Forgays & Forgays, 1992). Fur- levels were poorer (Sugimoto, Kida, Teranishi,
thermore, tests on creative thinking following a & Yamamoto, 1968). When groups were exam-
30-min restricted stimulation period as com- ined before and after restricted environmental
pared with a control condition revealed that stimulation with floatation and without floata-
novel ideas generated after restricted stimula- tion, using a tactile object-discrimination task
tion were more creative than those developed in carried out with each hand separately while
office sessions. Restricted stimulation was asso- blindfolded and a recognition memory test for
ciated with trends toward higher levels of vigor words and unfamiliar faces, the floatation group
and lower levels of tension, anger, depression, showed a significantly greater enhancement of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

fatigue, and confusion (Suedfeld, Metcalfe, & right hemispheric processing after floatation
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bluck, 1987). than was found when retesting the controls. The
Sensory hallucinations do not represent a direction of the lateral imbalance was similar to
common phenomenon of restricted stimulation. hypnosis (Raab & Gruzelier, 1994).
In one study (Schulman, Richlin, & Weinstein, To what extent changes of perception during
1967), only 2% of subjects reported percepts of restricted environmental stimulation relate to
an outside stimulus not being physically pres- different neural activities of the human brain
ent, but 74% of subjects reported on the expe- has not been established systematically. Al-
rience of visual percepts that appeared under though EEG studies during restricted environ-
some control of the subjects. Earlier studies by mental stimulation have shown changes of al-
Zuckerman and his group (Zuckerman, Persky, pha-, beta-, and theta-band activities in general
& Link, 1969) have shown that in most cases of (see Suedfeld, 1980; Zubek, 1969), systematic
sensory minimization, hallucinatory percepts studies on brain electric concomitants of visual,
are transient and impersonal in quality and auditory, and other sensory changes, illusions,
mostly nondynamical and nonpsychopathologi- and hallucinations are widely lacking. ASC
cal in nature. They are mostly addressing the have been seen only in subjects who have had a
visual modality and are composed of simple number of prior experiences with such phenom-
features, like sudden occurring flashes, colors, ena due to prior exposure to meditation or con-
or changes of hues (Zuckerman et al., 1969). sumption of psychedelic drugs or who were
Hallucinatory percepts occur most frequently instructed that such phenomena might arise
during states of medium to high physiological while they were exposed to restricted stimula-
arousal. Some studies have also pointed out that tion environments (Zubek, 1969). Whether
reports on such illusions and hallucinations ASC are a common consequence of sensory
might more likely be the result of instruction deprivation has not been settled by systematic
given to subjects prior to the exposure than empirical research.
phenomena occurring spontaneously (Suedfeld, Sensory homogenization (“ganzfeld”).
1980). Ganzfeld is a technical term for an unstructured,
Further studies have reported changes of sat- perfectly homogeneous perceptual, usually vi-
uration and luminosity of colors (Zubek, 1969), sual field (Avant, 1965; Wertheimer, 1923/
have noted misperception of motion and the 1938). A longer exposure (from minutes to a
shape of objects, or have found evidence for few tens of minutes) to visual ganzfeld (a ho-
afterimages (Zubek, 1969). Evidence for im- mogeneous red light field) and auditory gan-
pairment of depth perception, visual acuity, size zfeld (monotone noise, e.g., “white” or “pink”
constancy, discrimination and contrast of noise) may induce an ASC characterized by
brightness, and kinesthetic acuity (Zubek, 1969) episodes of imagery ranging from simple sen-
could not be replicated (Suedfeld, 1980). sory impressions to hallucinatory, dreamlike ex-
The same is true for time perception. For periences. As for subjective experience, visual
example, confinement of subjects for 72 hr did imagery is most frequently reported; less fre-
not affect subjects’ estimation of time spent in quently reported are percepts involving other
an isolation chamber independently of their be- sensory modalities. Procedurally, ganzfeld is
havioral activities. Over- or underestimation of closely related to sensory deprivation, but the
time was associated with periods of higher be- physical level of sensory stimulation is kept at
havioral activity, whereas during periods of medium to high levels. Subjectively, ganzfeld
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 15

imagery is similar to hypnagogic imagery (see a cortico–striato–thalamo– cortical loop model


Hypnagogic States). It was hypothesized that of psychosensory processing. It was suggested
ganzfeld induces a kind of experimental hypna- that a deficient thalamic filter, which had been
gogic state (Braud, Wood, & Braud, 1975; conceptualized in animal models by using the
Schacter, 1976), that is, a decrease of vigilance prepulse inhibition paradigm, enables sensory
(see States of Drowsiness). Little is to be said overload of the cortex. Progress in knowledge
about behavioral correlates: Because of envi- about the relationship between sensory overload
ronmental restrictions, subjects in ganzfeld can and brain function and accordingly on con-
show little overt behavior, except verbal re- sciousness-related functions is mainly based on
sponses. Subjects’ verbal expression is often clinical research, in areas including schizophre-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

less organized than in waking-state reports (see, nia, intensive care unit syndrome, autism, and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

e.g., Honorton et al., 1990). This may be due use of hallucinogens.


partly to the unusual, dreamlike nature of gan- In schizophrenia (see Psychotic Disorders),
zfeld experience and not necessarily to impair- the subject’s ability to inhibit irrelevant stimuli
ment of cognitive functions. It was assumed from the environment is impaired. This deficit is
(Honorton & Harper, 1974; Parker, 1975) that hypothesized to cause sensory overload in
ganzfeld induces a state favorable to “extrasen- schizophrenic patients and successively lead to
sory perception,” which would then constitute a disorganization and thought disorders (Ludwig,
special class of ASC correlates; however, ex- 1972; Ludwig & Stark, 1973). A cluster of
perimental findings are still controversial (Bem psychiatric symptoms that are unique to the
& Honorton, 1994; Wiseman & Milton, 1999). intensive care unit environment have been sug-
Time awareness is little affected by ganzfeld; gested to be caused, among other reasons, by
verbal estimates of elapsed time intervals (5–15 sensory overload or monotony (Glide, 1994;
min) showed a tendency to underestimate com- Steinhart, 1978; Vasquez & Chitwood, 1975).
parable with the waking state (Wackermann et The effects of sleep deprivation on physiologi-
al., 2001). cal and immune system functions have been
A large study comparing physiological cor- discussed as one starting condition for sensory
relates of ganzfeld, sleep onset, and relaxed overload in the intensive care unit (Halm &
waking state showed EEG spectra in ganzfeld to Alpen, 1993; Schwab, 1994). McGuire, Basten,
be more similar to wakefulness than to the sleep Ryan, and Gallagher (2000) have suggested that
onset state, with slightly accelerated alpha ac- intensive care unit psychoses are delirium states
tivity; there were no EEG markers indicating that are exclusively caused by organic stressors.
decreased vigilance (Wackermann et al., 2000, It has been speculated that autism is partly a
2002). Thus, the hypothesis of a hypnagogic consequence of sensory overload caused by a
basis to ganzfeld imagery has not been sup- reduced ability for sensory filtering in the
ported. In a follow-up study, EEG correlates of framework of an unidentified neurological dis-
vivid imagery were studied with selected “high order (Bristol-Power & Spinella, 1999).
responders” to ganzfeld. Intraindividual com- One of the rare studies with healthy subjects
parisons against no-imagery EEG revealed (Brauchli, Michel, & Zeier, 1995) found both
mostly triphasic courses, consisting of (a) an effects on subjective ratings and increased au-
initial alpha increase, (b) a burst of accelerated tonomic arousal during conditions of massive
alpha-2 activity (10 –12 Hz), and (c) a deceler- sensory stimulation but failed to reveal effects
ation and reduction of alpha rhythm and an on electrophysiological brain activity. Obvi-
undulating increase of beta-2 (18 –21 Hz) and ously, a particular vulnerability of brain func-
beta-3 (21–30 Hz) power, indicating a transition tion is necessary for sensory stimulation to be-
from relaxation to outward directed and fluctu- come an overstimulation.
ating attention and finally preparation for the
mentation report (Wackermann, Pütz, & Brae- Rhythm-Induced Trance (Drumming and
unig, 2002). Dancing)
Sensory overload. There is a long tradition
of attempting to induce ASC by sensory over- Drumming and dancing have been practiced
load. However, models of sensory overload are since ancient times to induce altered states and
rare. Vollenweider and Geyer (2001) proposed are still common today. They may result in
16 VAITL ET AL.

trancelike states characterized by a “narrowing induction. Rhythmic body movements are ac-
of awareness of immediate surroundings, or un- companied by recurrent shifts in body fluids,
usually narrow and selective focusing on envi- especially in the blood. In addition, respiration
ronmental stimuli” and by “stereotyped behav- tends to synchronize with movements and in-
iors or movements that are experienced as being duces the heart rate oscillations known as respi-
beyond one’s control” (American Psychiatric ratory sinus arrhythmia. In this way, rhythmic
Association, 1994, p. 729). In the case of drum- movements may result in a respiratory–
ming and dancing, the rhythmic body move- cardiovascular synchronization with increased
ments become synchronized with the beat and blood pressure oscillations that stimulate the
finally seem to happen automatically, without carotid baroreceptors. The effects of barorecep-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

effort or voluntary control. Self-reflective think- tor stimulation are not confined to a slowing of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ing ceases when the subject becomes increas- the heart rate; they also reduce cortical arousal
ingly absorbed in the action. In addition, alter- and excitability (Rau, Pauli, Brody, Elbert, &
ations also include a distortion of the time Birbaumer, 1993; Vaitl & Gruppe, 1995). Many
sense, unusual bodily sensations (e.g., feeling effects of barostimulation—like augmented
light, warm, energized), vivid imagery, and pain thresholds, increased theta activity, and
strong positive emotions (e.g., joy, happiness, reduced muscular reflexes—resemble typical
ecstasy) in conjunction with the impression of features of trance states. To investigate whether
becoming one with the rhythm. cardiovascular oscillations induced by body
In contrast to the ubiquity of the phenomena, movements contribute to trance induction, a se-
modern science has scarcely investigated the ries of experiments was conducted using a tilt
psychobiological foundations of these human table. The subjects were rhythmically tilted be-
activities. Neher (1961, 1962) was the first to tween ⫺6° head-down and 12° head-up. The
study the effects of monotonous drumming on tilting movements were triggered by respiration
the EEG. He found that drum beats (3– 8 Hz) that was paced at 0.125 Hz. In one condition,
can induce EEG waves of the same frequency the respiratory oscillations of the heart rate were
(“auditory driving”), and he speculated that this amplified; in the other condition they were
phenomenon may be responsible for the facili- dampened (Vaitl, Sammer, & Ott, 2000). Dur-
tation of trance states. Later on, the role of ing the condition with increased heart rate vari-
physiological stimulation was questioned by ability, the power in the EEG theta band was
Rouget (1980), who emphasized the impact of increased and the cross-spectral power between
the social setting. This issue was further ex- pupil oscillations and respiration was reduced,
plored by Maxfield (1990), who studied the indicating cortical inhibition. The subjects re-
effects of different beat rhythms on the EEG ported drowsiness, disorientation, and even hal-
and subjective experiences. She found more lucinations. They reached moderate hypnoidal
theta EEG activity while the subjects were lis- scores on the Phenomenology of Consciousness
tening to rhythmic monotonous and patterned Inventory (Pekala, 1991). Furthermore, it was
drum beats than when they heard unstructured found that those subjects responding differently
beat sequences. The alterations in conscious- to the two tilting conditions had significantly
ness included changes in time sense and body higher scores on Tellegen’s Absorption Scale
image, enhanced imagery, and other experi- (Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974; German version
ences resembling descriptions of a shaman’s by Ritz & Dahme, 1995) compared with the
journey. In a study by Maurer, Kumar, Wood- nonresponders. This finding was replicated in a
side, and Pekala (1997), relaxation and similar second study with continuous blood pressure
shamanic-type experiences (e.g., dissociation measurement. Again, baroreflex sensitivity cor-
from the body, tunnel experiences) were evoked related significantly with the absorption score
by monotonous drumming, especially in me- (Ott, Sammer, & Vaitl, 2002) pointing to a
dium and highly hypnotizable subjects. connection between the cardiovascular respon-
Besides rhythmical auditory stimulation, the siveness and the personality trait of absorption,
social setting, and personality traits (e.g., ab- which is positively correlated with hypnotic
sorption), a fourth factor—namely, the rhyth- susceptibility (Kumar & Pekala, 1988).
mic body movements during drumming and In summary, the synchronization of the car-
dancing—may play an important role for trance diovascular system and the augmented stimula-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 17

tion of the baroreflex by increased blood pres- relaxation and the control group. The peripheral
sure oscillations may contribute to the efficacy physiological effects are most likely to follow
of rhythmic trance-induction procedures, and the reduced cortical arousal measured by EEG
this should be taken into account in addition to (Narita, Morozumi, & Yagi, 1987). Beyond
sensory stimulation, personality characteristics, these general changes in electrocortical activity
and situational variables. during relaxation, more specific alterations are
likely to occur. For instance, Isotani, Tanaka,
Relaxation and collaborators (2001) investigated the spe-
cific engagement of different brain regions dur-
There is a wide range of techniques by which ing hypnosis-based suggestion of relaxation
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

relaxation and ASC can be induced. The com- compared with suggestion of negative emotion,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

monly applied and clinically established meth- such as anxiety. Relaxation showed maximally
ods for body relaxation are progressive muscle stronger activity compared with anxiety in the
relaxation, autogenic training, biofeedback, and left superior temporal gyrus (Brodman Area
meditative practices. The cognitive– behavioral 22). During the two induced emotional states,
model of relaxation (Smith, Amutio, Anderson, brain activity shifted to the right hemisphere in
& Aria, 1996) suggests that three elements are the frontotemporal regions during negative
basic to all forms of relaxation: (a) focusing, the compared with positive emotions (relaxation).
ability to maintain concentration on and return The excitatory frequency EEG band of beta-2
attention to simple stimuli (acoustic or visual) (18.5–21.0 Hz) showed the strongest relaxation-
for an extended period of time; (b) passivity, the related effect on location, with the source grav-
ability to refrain from goal-directed and analytic ity center located less right sided during relax-
thoughts; and (c) receptivity, the ability to tol- ation than during anxiety. The lateralized dif-
erate and accept unusual or paradoxical experi- ferences of brain activity during the two
ences. Benson (Benson, 1975; Greenwood & conditions were clearly not mirror symmetric in
Benson, 1977) proposed that almost all relax- the two hemispheres. Relaxation showed stron-
ation techniques elicit a general “relaxation re- gest activity differences in left temporal areas,
sponse” consisting of physiological changes and negative emotion in right frontal areas.
that are mainly evoked by decreased autonomic In addition, neuroimaging studies have sug-
nervous system activity, such as slowing of gested that various brain regions and systems
heart rate, slow and shallow breathing, periph- are involved when subjects intentionally try
eral vasodilation, reduced oxygen consumption, eliciting decreases in sympathoadrenergic tone.
and decrease in spontaneous skin conductance Critchley, Melmed, Featherstone, Mathias, and
responses (cf. Lichstein, 1988; Shapiro & Leh- Dolan (2001) used PET to investigate cerebral
rer, 1980). Specifically, these effects are mainly activity relating to biofeedback-assisted relax-
brought about by a reduced sympathoadrenergic ation and modulation of sympathetic activity.
reactivity, but not, as often erroneously be- The voluntary control of bodily arousal was
lieved, by increased parasympathetic activity. closely related to enhanced activity of the ante-
Neurophysiologically, the relaxation re- rior cingulate cortex, which is thought to be
sponse is most frequently accompanied by engaged in the integration of cognitive control
changes in EEG indicating reduced cortical strategies and bodily responses. As Critchley,
arousal. Insofar as relaxation techniques are fo- Corfield, Chandler, Mathias, and Dolan (2000)
cused on reduced oculomotor activity or refrain demonstrated, these relations were lateralized.
from vivid imagery of overt movements, in- The observed predominantly left cingulate ac-
creases in EEG alpha frequency may be ex- tivity was associated with the intention to relax,
pected to occur (Peper, 1971). G. D. Jacobs and which contrasted with right cingulate activity
his coworkers (1996) found in novices under- observed during task-related states of sympa-
going the progressive muscle relaxation tech- thetic arousal.
nique while listening to taped relaxation in- Almost all relaxation techniques apply vari-
structions a significant reduction in the EEG ous interventions to prevent subjects from fall-
beta frequency band, exclusively in the frontal ing asleep after they have left the state of full
lobe sites. In none of the other 13 electrode sites alertness and approach the critical threshold of
could EEG differences be observed between the sleep onset (showing significant EEG signs,
18 VAITL ET AL.

such as K-complex and sleep spindles). There- scendental consciousness (Travis & Wallace,
fore, trainees gradually learn to circumvent all 1997). Results such as these led to the sugges-
usual sleep-promoting activities and imageries tion that such autonomic activity reflected the
as soon as they have left the stage of full alert- transition of awareness from active thinking
ness by initiating the relaxation response. The processes to the silent yet alert state of transcen-
better they are trained, the longer they can stay dental consciousness (Travis, 1993).
within this particular intermediate stage be- Changes in the EEG during meditation, such
tween decreased arousal and falling asleep. This as an increase in alpha activity, are nonspecific
provides the opportunity for making new expe- and do not support the notion of a unique state
riences similar to those described for hypnago- of consciousness. This holds true also for in-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

gic states. These novel experiences may be sub- creased EEG coherence found during transcen-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

jectively considered as ASC. dental meditation, which has been interpreted as


In summary, the various techniques applied reflecting an “ordering of the mind” (see Fen-
to induce a relaxation response are viable tools wick, 1987, for a critical discussion). Recent
to reduce autonomic and central arousal, which research comparing subjects who have under-
may, in turn, predispose the body and the mind taken meditation for periods longer than 3 years
for accepting unusual or paradoxical experi- with those practicing for less than 6 months has
ences occurring during ASC. Basically, they are shown a difference in slow wave theta activity
not directly aiming at ASC, but whenever they (Aftanas & Golocheikine, 2001) similar to ear-
occur, they are treated as tolerable side effects. lier findings (Hebert & Lehmann, 1977). Long-
In this sense, relaxation techniques are helpful term meditation was characterized by increased
and indispensable adjuncts in the realm of ASC. theta EEG activity over the frontal region. The
intensity of the blissful experience correlated
Meditation with increases in theta power in anterior–frontal
and frontal–midline regions. Moreover, long-
A great variety of meditation techniques exist term meditation was associated with increased
that can be divided into those involving move- theta synchronization between the prefrontal
ments, like walking, dancing, and singing, and and the posterior association cortex, peaking in
the “silent” meditation methods, which are usu- the left prefrontal region. This is compatible
ally practiced in a characteristic sitting position. with investigations into EEG coherence during
Instructions on how to meditate are manifold, the experience of positive and negative emo-
ranging from more receptive “wide focus” tech- tions, which suggest that theta activity, peaking
niques to those requiring intensive concentra- in the left prefrontal region, is indicative of
tion on external objects, imaginings, or parts of emotionally positive experiences (Aftanas, Var-
the body. The transcendental meditation tech- lamov, Pavlov, Makhnev, & Reva, 2001), and
nique, introduced to the West by Maharishi with reported emotional differences between
Mahesh Yogi (1966), is a yogic “mantra” med- short- and long-term meditators (Easterlin &
itation: A syllable is silently repeated in a pas- Cardeña, 1998).
sive, effortless manner. The procedure is quite Recently, several theory-based studies have
standardized, and it has been investigated more been carried out that link specific meditation
frequently than other methods (Murphy & Don- techniques and experiential characteristics to
ovan, 1997). certain changes in brain function. Lehmann,
The subjective experience during transcen- Faber, Achermann, et al. (2001) investigated an
dental meditation has been described as a state advanced subject who was able to enter volun-
of blissful mental quiescence where thoughts tarily distinct meditative states by applying dif-
are absent but consciousness remains—referred ferent meditation techniques. They found differ-
to as a state of “transcendental consciousness” ent centers of gravity of gamma EEG activity
(Travis, 1993). The occurrence of transcenden- related to the nature of the respective meditation
tal consciousness has been frequently associ- technique (visualization: right posterior; verbal-
ated with short periods of breath suspension, ization: left central; “dissolution of the self”:
while breath period, magnitude of heart rate right anterior) using a repeated measurements
deceleration, and skin conductance level have design. Lazar et al. (2000) conceptualized med-
been found to be greater during periods of tran- itation as voluntary regulation of attention.
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 19

Their subjects, who had practiced a breathing that are normally voluntary, or to experience
meditation for many years, were trained to med- changes in belief, perception, or memory that
itate during fMRI. They found an activation of are at odds with the actual state of affairs.
neural structures involved in attention and in the Hypnosis may be self-induced or other-induced.
control of the autonomic nervous system and a However, the relationship between autohypno-
signal decrease apparently related to the slower sis and heterohypnosis is poorly understood.
breathing during the meditation phases. In a Traditional hypnotic phenomena have also been
similar way, certain experiential features of shown to be able to be produced by suggestion
meditative states, like the sense of becoming alone without the administration of a formal
one with the object of meditation, have been induction procedure, although the interpretation
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

linked to the blocking of a parietal cortical area of these findings is controversial. Today hypno-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

during meditation (revealed by single positron sis has many clinical applications, including
emission computed tomography) that represents relief of pain, as an adjunct to facilitate the
the position of the body in three-dimensional effectiveness of specific psychotherapeutic in-
space (Newberg & d’Aquili, 2000). terventions, and is under investigation as a mod-
In summary, it seems promising that the tra- ulator of immune system functioning.
ditional meditation methods are becoming more The effectiveness of hypnotic suggestion in
and more examined scientifically. A cognitive producing corresponding changes in experience
analysis of the meditation methods and the (i.e., hypnotic susceptibility) varies greatly from
study of trained subjects within established and individual to individual and is a highly stable
newly devised experimental paradigms will attribute of the person. Current theories of hypno-
help to elucidate the psychophysiological bases sis emphasize the enactment of self-directed cog-
of the human capability to enter into different nitive strategies (Spanos, 1986), response expec-
ASC through the intentional self-regulation of tancies (Kirsch, 2000), or alterations in central
attention. executive control (Bowers, 1992; Farvolden &
Woody, 2004) as processes mediating response to
Hypnosis hypnotic suggestion. Assessment of susceptibility
and psychological research have focused on be-
After 200 years of inquiry, there is still great havioral responses during hypnosis, but the core
controversy over how hypnosis should be de- phenomena lie at the level of experience (Shee-
fined. Throughout its history, the field has been han, 1992). Identical behavioral responses often
divided between those who evoke distinctive mask widely divergent phenomenology and cog-
psychological processes and those who appeal nitive strategies evoked in different individuals by
to the ordinary processes that underlie everyday the same hypnotic suggestion (Sheehan & McCo-
behavior. The altered state view we take in this nkey, 1982), and it is essential that adequate stud-
article attempts to develop the former approach ies of hypnotic phenomena ensure they are com-
within a contemporary neurophysiological paring subjects with similar phenomenological as
framework. Hypnosis is a procedure that may or well as behavioral responses (Woody & McCon-
may not involve an ASC, and the latter is not key, 2003).
necessary for most hypnotic phenomena The neurophysiology of hypnosis has been
(Cardeña, Lynn, & Krippner, 2000). the subject of numerous studies over the past 40
Traditionally, hypnosis is induced by a ritual, years, reporting on multiple changes in many
administered by the hypnotist and believed in different physiological parameters (for reviews,
by the subject. Induction rituals may vary see, e.g., Crawford, 1994; Crawford & Gruze-
greatly in content. For example, progressive lier, 1992; Gruzelier, 1998, 2000). The bulk of
relaxation instructions, riding an exercise bike, this research has used conventional evoked po-
and sounding a gong have all been reported as tential or frequency analysis of EEG data (De
successful induction rituals. The induction ritual Pascalis, 1999; Ray, 1997; Williams & Gruze-
is followed by a series of verbal suggestions for lier, 2001). For example, a number of studies
seemingly unusual changes in experience, be- investigated electrophysiological characteristics
havior, and cognitive control. Suggestions may of hypnotic susceptibility. These studies re-
be given to behave in ways that are experienced ported various differences between high- and
as involuntary, to be unable to behave in ways low-hypnotically susceptible subjects in differ-
20 VAITL ET AL.

ent EEG bands. For example, higher power in for the subjects with low susceptibility but was
the theta frequency band in some studies, higher abolished in highly hypnotizable subjects fol-
gamma power over the right hemisphere, and lowing instructions of hypnosis.
higher global dimensional complexity all were Lehmann, Faber, Isotani, and Wohlgemuth
reported for high-susceptible subjects (for an (2001) investigated intracerebral source loca-
overview and recent results, see Isotani, Leh- tions during hypnotically suggested arm levita-
mann, et al., 2001). However, until now no tion versus willful initiation of arm raising in
neurophysiological measure or set of measures highly hypnotizable subjects. They found
has been able to reliably categorize data as sources for delta and theta EEG activity more
obtained from hypnotized or nonhypnotized in- posteriorly and for alpha and beta-1 activity
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dividuals (Wagstaff, 1998). Positive findings more anteriorly in the hypnotic arm levitation
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

are rarely replicated. Besides the large amount condition, suggesting the co-occurrence of elec-
of available data, the methodological heteroge- trophysiological characteristics of lowered vig-
neity and complexity of reported findings make ilance and increased attention.
clear comparisons very difficult. One reason for Friederich et al. (2001) compared the effects
this diversity may be that differences in specific of hypnotic analgesia and distraction on pro-
suggestions between studies induce correspond- cessing of noxious laser heat stimuli in highly
ing variability in observed behaviors, for exam- susceptible subjects. They examined whether
ple, emotions, postures, relaxation, and so forth hypnotic analgesia and distraction share similar
(e.g., Isotani, Tanaka, et al., 2001). cortical mechanisms (Crawford, Knebel, &
Gruzelier (2000), for the past decade, has Vendemia, 1998). Friederich et al. (2001) found
consistently advocated the view of hypnosis as significantly reduced pain reports during both
a form of frontal inhibition. Replicated neuro- hypnotic analgesia and distraction compared
psychological findings (Gruzelier & Warren, with the control condition but significantly
1993; Kallio, Revonsuo, Hamalainen, & Gruze- smaller amplitudes of the late laser-evoked
lier, 2001) show impaired letter fluency (left brain potential components during distraction as
frontal) but not category fluency (left temporal) compared with hypnotic analgesia. Further-
performance during hypnosis for subjects high more, coherence analysis of neural oscillations
but not low in hypnotic susceptibility. This sug- between different areas of the brain indicated a
gests that for hypnotizable subjects, hypnosis is significant decrease of coherence within the
associated with inhibition of the left dorsolat- gamma band between somatosensory and fron-
eral prefrontal cortex (Gruzelier, 1998). Selec- tal sites of the brain while subjects were hyp-
tive influences within the cingulate have also notized as compared with the control condition.
been inferred from evidence of the maintenance This loss of coherence between somatosensory
of the error-related negativity wave in concert and frontal brain areas during hypnotic analge-
with an abolition of the ensuing positivity wave sia was hypothesized to reflect a breakdown of
in highly hypnotizable subjects during hypnosis functional connectivity between the brain areas
(Kaiser, Barker, Haenschel, Baldeweg, & Gru- involved in the analysis of the somatosensory
zelier, 1997). Some recent studies investigated aspects of the noxious input and areas organiz-
hypnotic analgesia to test the hypothesis that at ing the emotional and behavioral responses to
least part of the phenomena occurring under pain. Thus, hypnotic analgesia and distraction
hypnosis might also be explained by a dissoci- appear to involve different brain mechanisms,
ation between functional subunits organizing probably an early attentional filter for distrac-
conscious behavior. Thus, Croft, Williams, tion but a dissociation between early sensory
Haenschel, and Gruzelier (2002) analyzed EEG and later higher order processing of noxious
component frequencies in the period following input for hypnotic analgesia. These results are
painful electrical stimulation of the right hand in consistent with neodissociation theories of hyp-
a control condition, during hypnosis, and after nosis and hypnotic pain control (Hilgard & Hil-
hypnotic analgesia suggestion. Prefrontal gard, 1983; Miller & Bowers, 1993) and recent
gamma EEG activity localized in the anterior studies demonstrating a dissociation between
cingulate cortex predicted the intensity of sub- processing of somatosensory and affective in-
jects’ pain ratings in the control condition. This formation during hypnotic analgesia by both
relationship remained unchanged by hypnosis behavioral and regional cerebral blood flow
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 21

data (Rainville, Carrier, Hofbauer, Bushnell, & tions. The modulation of cortical activity is usu-
Duncan, 1999). ally achieved through a training process involv-
These studies suggest that hypnosis affects ing real-time representation of EEG parameters
integrative functions of the brain and induces an paired with positive reinforcement to facilitate
alteration or even a breakdown of communica- successful operant learning of the desired re-
tion between subunits within the brain respon- sponse (Egner & Gruzelier, 2001). Many au-
sible for the formation of conscious experience thors have demonstrated operant conditioning
(see Functional Integration and Breakdown of and self-regulation of various EEG parameters
Connectivity). in animals and humans in experimental and
clinical settings (Birbaumer, 1977, 1984;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Biofeedback Birbaumer, Elbert, Rockstroh, & Lutzenberger,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1981; Kamiya, 1969; Kuhlman, 1978; Plotkin,


Contemporary research supports the view 1976; Sterman, 1977). Sterman and Shouse
that biofeedback and instrumental learning of (1980) demonstrated that epileptic seizure inci-
neuronal activity is a promising noninvasive dents can be reduced by the conditioned en-
methodology to manipulate brain activity as an hancement of a low beta-band 12–15 Hz EEG
independent variable and observe ASC as the rhythm, sensorimotor rhythm (SMR or
dependent variable (see Birbaumer & Kimmel, ␮-rhythm), over sensorimotor cortex. These
1979). The objective of biofeedback is a greater findings are interpreted as improved response
awareness and voluntary control over physio- inhibition by SMR feedback training. Sterman
logical processes. Various methods have proven (1996) supposed that the SMR training may
to be effective in supporting people’s efforts to lead to increased inhibitory activity of thalamic
exert control over physiological processes, such nuclei interacting with somatosensory and sen-
as heart rate, blood pressure, vasomotor re- sorimotor cortex. In the same vein, Egner and
sponses and temperature, respiratory activity, Gruzelier (2001, 2004) postulated that similar
gastrointestinal reactions, and levels of muscu- inhibitory activity may account for alleviation
lar tension (for a review, see Schwartz, 1995). of performance errors in student volunteers dur-
In the late 1960s and the early 1970s, biofeed- ing sustained attention tasks deficit, as in sub-
back of EEG alpha rhythm was believed to alter jects with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
states of consciousness (Kamiya, 1969). It had der. Learning the ability to increase relative and
been shown that subjects could learn to discrim- absolute SMR (12–15 Hz) amplitude correlated
inate whether they were in an alpha or in a beta positively with improvement of attentional per-
state, and by means of the brain wave biofeed- formances and reduction of impulsiveness,
back, they could produce alpha brain waves at whereas the opposite was true for the enhance-
will. At a certain level of alpha wave produc- ment of beta-1 (15–18 Hz). The relationship
tion, the verbal reports of these subjects sug- between learning to enhance SMR over senso-
gested a state of serenity, meditative mood, and rimotor cortex and reduced impulsiveness again
happiness. This led some researchers to hypoth- supports the notion that inhibitory processes are
esize that states of consciousness can also be mediating behavioral and cognitive improve-
modified by brain wave feedback, as with med- ments. Furthermore, the combination of SMR
itation and hypnosis. and beta-1 EEG training has provided evidence,
Recently, neurofeedback has been developed with both behavioral and event-related brain
as a form of biofeedback linked to aspects of the potentials (ERP), of being effective in integrat-
electric activity of the brain such as frequency, ing sensory input and counteracting fast motor
location, or amplitude of specific EEG activity. response tendencies and error-prone behavior.
It therefore aims directly at altering electrocor- Similar frequency-specific effects of neurofeed-
tical processes associated with cortical excit- back were found for cognitive processes, such
ability, arousal, and central control of motor as working memory (Vernon et al., 2003) and
performance. For this purpose, EEG compo- attention (Egner & Gruzelier, 2004).
nents are extracted online and fed back to sub- Another EEG parameter is the slow cortical
jects by various devices producing acoustic and potentials (SCP). Negative shifts of slow EEG
visual signals that can be shaped with operant changes reflect widespread depolarization of
conditioning strategies or cognitive manipula- apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons
22 VAITL ET AL.

(Birbaumer, Elbert, Canavan, & Rockstroh, recently, replicable efficacy of training to el-
1990) and decrease of thresholds for paroxys- evate theta over alpha activity for enhancing
mal activity. In healthy subjects, it has exten- music performance (including ratings of in-
sively been confirmed that SCP can be brought terpretative imagination) in conservatoire stu-
under voluntary control (Birbaumer et al., dents has been reported (Egner & Gruzelier,
1991). Thus, it was obvious to use the SCP 2003). Improvements in attention and
feedback as an adjunctive treatment for drug- semantic working memory in medical stu-
refractory epilepsy. Several studies have shown dents support this view (Egner & Gruzelier,
that epilepsy patients are able to acquire self- 2001, 2004; Vernon et al., 2003). If EEG/
control skills after an extensive training of SCP MEG reflects ASC, as discussed in this arti-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

self-regulation, resulting in a significant de- cle, learning to induce those EEG/MEG pat-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

crease of seizure rate (Kotchoubey et al., 1996, terns underlying ASC should lead to those
1999; Kotchoubey, Strehl, et al., 2001; Rock- states.
stroh et al., 1993).
Moreover, the self-regulation of SCP can be Conclusions
used for brain– computer communication. It
may be needed for patients suffering from neu- There is a huge body of literature linking
rological diseases leading to total motor paral- ASC induced by psychological induction pro-
ysis caused by amyotrophic lateral sclerosis cedures to changes in brain activity. Although
(ALS) or brain stem infarct (“locked-in” pa- early studies were in most cases limited to the
tients). Brain– computer interfaces provide them mere description of correlations, the emerging
with a system for communication, where a cur- new approaches try to develop functional mod-
sor on a screen is moved (vertically or horizon- els that explain how distinct changes in cogni-
tally) by changes in self-regulated SCP in both tion and consciousness are produced and devise
directions, that is, by shifts in SCP negativity. specific experimental paradigms to test these
Birbaumer and his group have developed a models.
thought translation device and a training proce-
dure that enables patients with ALS, after ex- Disease-Induced ASC
tensive training, to verbally communicate with
other people without any voluntary muscle con- There are numerous pathological processes
trol (Kübler, Kotchoubey, Kaiser, Wolpaw, & that can result in various degrees of ASC. Most
Birbaumer, 2001). of these disorders affect, directly (within the
Currently there are growing applications of brain) or indirectly (in the periphery), structures
neurofeedback that show that patients have ben- that are responsible for dramatic shifts on the
efited by learned self-control of EEG parame- wakefulness– coma axis toward clouding of
ters as EEG mechanisms underlying psycho- consciousness, obnubilation, somnolence, so-
pathological and neurological disorders are bet- por, and coma. Regional brain lesions as well as
ter understood. This was underpinned by high-level deefferentation and deafferentation
Gruzelier, Hardman, Wild, and Zaman (1999), (e.g., through a spinal cord injury or ALS) are
who trained schizophrenic patients by SCP not reviewed here, because they typically result
feedback to reduce their interhemispheric im- in a loss of circumscribed functions without
balance, which as been shown to be syndrome disturbing consciousness in general. Neverthe-
related (for a review, see Gruzelier, 1999a). less, interest in the psychological concomi-
Despite the fact that neurofeedback is a tants—for example, of the so-called locked-in
powerful tool for gaining self-control over syndrome— has increased in the past few years,
brain electric activity, the lack of biofeed- and new devices have been developed that en-
back-induced ASC may indicate that biofeed- able communication with these patients
back of brain responses of one or a few elec- (Birbaumer et al., 1999). There exist many other
trodes (i.e., brain areas) is not an induction degenerative, developmental, and organic brain
method powerful enough to induce such al- diseases that are accompanied by alterations of
terations. This conclusion, however, may be consciousness (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, Mor-
premature before serious attempts to produce bus Parkinson, dementia with Lewy bodies,
ASC appear in the scientific literature, and frontotemporal dementia). Although these phe-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 23

nomenological changes have been reported fre- cordingly, hallucinations can be regarded as a
quently, they have rarely been investigated sys- state of hyperattentiveness to intrinsic self-
tematically with respect to the interaction be- generated activity in the absence of appropriate
tween subjective experiences and specific brain sensory input. In support of this proposition, a
malfunctions. Owing to this scarcity of empiri- coincidence between hallucinations and gamma
cal evidence, these brain diseases are not re- activity has been demonstrated in a patient with
viewed here. pseudosomatic hallucinations, later diagnosed
as schizophrenic (Baldeweg, Spence, Hirsch, &
Psychotic Disorders Gruzelier, 1998). Neuromagnetic recording has
also lent substance to theory. Tononi and Edel-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Alterations of conscious experience in psy- man (2000) have demonstrated that conscious
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

chosis are coming under the scrutiny of cogni- experience is underpinned by numerous neuro-
tive neuroscience, exemplified most keenly by nal groups, representing differentiated states
hallucinations and delusions as well as the cog- distributed in the thalamocortical system.
nitive disintegration and splitting of psychic Through corticothalamic and cortico-cortico re-
functions that defines schizophrenia. An over- entrant interactions, these rapidly bind together
riding concept is that alterations of conscious- into an integrated neural process or functional
ness arise from defective connectivity or from cluster. Furthermore, with conscious experience
defective interactions between distributed brain the neuromagnetic response becomes stronger
regions (Fletcher, 1998; Lawrie et al., 2002). and the cluster more widespread to include fron-
Germane to this approach is the evidence that tal, parietal, temporal, and occipital cortices.
patients with metachromatic leukodystrophy, in This in turn is accompanied by increased coher-
whom there is a disorder of frontal white matter, ence between distant brain regions, is charac-
present cognitive symptoms that include terized by strong and rapid interactions between
thought disorder, hallucinations, and delusions groups of neurons, and is subject to wide indi-
(Hyde, Ziegler, & Weinberger, 1992). In addi- vidual differences. Tononi and Edelman (2000)
tion, phencyclidine, which inhibits N-methyl-D- examined the PET results of medicated schizo-
aspartate glutamatergic transmission, produces phrenic patients during simple cognitive tasks.
positive psychotic symptoms (Thornberg & They found that despite similarities in the to-
Saklad, 1996). Historically, the disconnection pography of cluster boundaries, functional in-
concept guided research into the nature of in- teractions within clusters differentiated patients
terhemispheric connection in schizophrenia on from controls while levels of activity and func-
the basis of the split-brain model, giving way to tional specialization within the cortex were
disordered functional interhemispheric connec- spared. They noted that a coherent network may
tivity in the absence of evidence of structural be disrupted by multiple pathophysiological
disconnection (Gruzelier, 1979; Nasrallah, mechanisms, many of which have been impli-
1985). cated in schizophrenia at one time or another.
A leading hypothesis is that it is temporally Integrative circuits of the basal ganglia, thal-
disconnected and abnormal patterns of oscilla- amus, and frontal cortex have also been invoked
tory activity that contribute to abnormal mental in modeling schizophrenia as a disorder of in-
events and whose underpinning involves inte- tegration between the sensory systems of con-
grative thalamocortical circuits. Considering sciousness and the motor systems of thought.
first electrophysiological evidence, gamma os- Feinberg and Guazzelli (1999) adopted Gray-
cillations, circa 40 Hz, have been implicated in biel’s (1997) speculation that innate cognitive
the “binding” together of regions subserving pattern generators exist in the motor systems of
conscious perception (Llinas & Pare, 1991). thought, akin to innate motor pattern generators,
Llinas, Ribary, Joliot, and Wang (1994) have proposing that the basal ganglia along with the
proposed that activity of the specific and non- frontal cortex are involved in the planning of
specific thalamocortical systems underpins con- motor acts and are central to the planning and
scious experience, conceptualizing the specific sequencing of cognitive processes. As with the
nuclei as providing the content of experience theory of Llinas and colleagues (1994), above,
and the ascending nonspecific thalamic system hallucinations, delusions, and disorganized
as providing level of alertness and context. Ac- thought were hypothesized to arise through de-
24 VAITL ET AL.

rangement of the ability to distinguish exoge- crostates (see Fine Structure of Brain Func-
nous from endogenous activity, but here the tional States and States of Consciousness).
emphasis is on the failure to distinguish self
from other (see also Feinberg, 1978). This leads Coma and Vegetative State
to fragmentation of the senses of self and will,
resulting in a subsequent distortion of the Coma and vegetative state are typically re-
boundaries of self. garded as a decrease or even a complete loss of
The diffuse ascending thalamic systems have consciousness. However, in light of modern
also been implicated in the hemispheric activa- data obtained with PET and ERP, these condi-
tional imbalances found in both schizophrenia tions can alternatively be conceived of as in-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and schizotypy. These disorders have been as- volving far-going fragmentation of the field of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sociated with an activated syndrome, under- awareness, whose single modules may continue
pinned by a left hemispheric activational pref- to work independently of other modules, prob-
erence, and a withdrawn syndrome having the ably switched off. Very simple stimuli may not
opposite lateralized imbalance (Gruzelier, be the best means to probe the function of these
1999a, 2002). Neuroleptic drugs can modify disintegrated modules; rather, the optimal level
and reverse cognitive and electrophysiological of task complexity is required.
lateral asymmetries. Developmental origins for Coma is defined as a complete or nearly
dispositional imbalances have been posited, in complete loss of all basic functions of con-
keeping with their manifestation in schizotypy. sciousness: vigilance, mental contents or expe-
In animals, behavioral asymmetries occur rience, and selective attention (Niedermeyer,
through the influence of genes, hormones, and 1999). The basis of this condition is a deep
early experiences, and in human infants there dysfunction of brain stem structures regulating
are spontaneous asymmetries in gesture and sleep and wakefulness. A primary cortical dam-
emotion soon after birth that influence the ap- age is not necessary for coma. The main behav-
proach–withdrawal balance in social encounters ioral sign of coma is a profound suppression of
(see Gruzelier, 1999a). In schizotypy, factors in all responses, which ranges from weak re-
the second decade of life may also produce sponses to intensive stimuli to complete nonre-
atypical connectivity. Both early and late puber- sponsiveness. Several scales have been devel-
tal timing have been associated with a propen- oped to measure these differences in degree
sity for unreality experiences (Kaiser & Gruze- denoted as the “depth” of coma (e.g., Gentle-
lier, 1999), thought to reflect individual differ- man, 1999).
ences in regressive events such as synaptic Patients surviving coma often pass to another
pruning halted by the onset of puberty (Fein- condition called vegetative state (Wade & John-
berg, 1982; Saugstad, 1994). ston, 1999). In contrast to coma, most brain
In sum, interruptions ranging from minor stem functions are preserved in this condition,
asynchrony to complete uncoupling between but all cortical functions are assumed to be lost.
the conjunction of specific and nonspecific tha- Thus, vigilance is preserved, and patients have
lamic systems, and in turn between the content close to normal sleep–wakefulness cycles.
and context of consciousness, could give rise to However, mental contents and selective atten-
many aspects of anomalous processing in tion are believed to be lost. Subcortical re-
schizophrenia and schizotypy (Gruzelier, sponses are usually preserved and often even
1999a, 1999b; Oke & Adams, 1987). Such enhanced, but responses mediated by the cortex
anomalies include disturbances of sensory pro- are lacking. In many patients, however, very
cessing, sensory gating, and magnocellular weak and inconsistent “cognitive” responses are
functions; of perceptual aberrations, of halluci- observed by doctors and the personnel. This
nations, and with attributions of schizophrenia borderline condition is referred to as minimal
to a waking dream; of dysregulation of orient- consciousness state (American Congress of Re-
ing, arousal, alertness, and attention; of mis- habilitation Medicine, 1995; Whyte, DiPas-
matches between ongoing and past experiences quale, & Vaccaro, 1999). Clinical differentia-
that may lead to erroneous and delusional think- tion between vegetative state and minimal con-
ing (for a review, see Behrendt & Young, in sciousness state is extremely difficult and based
press; Gruzelier, 1999b); and to truncated mi- on the very subtle distinction between subcor-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 25

tical and weak cortical responses (American prove conscious experience and control. The
Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine, 1995; Pi- subjective state of these patients can be deduced
lon & Sullivan, 1996; Strauss, Ahwal, Day, & only in those cases regaining consciousness af-
Shavelle, 2000). ter coma (Childs & Merger, 1996) or if these
Background EEG in coma usually shows se- paralyzed patients learn to communicate with a
vere slowing of the dominant rhythms. Coma brain– computer interface, as described in the
variants with dominating theta or even alpha Biofeedback section. Deep brain stimulation
rhythms have been described as well. These with electrodes implanted in the nonspecific
relatively fast rhythms, however, do not indi- thalamic nuclei or other parts of the distributed
cate a higher level of vigilance or a better prog- consciousness system (Dehaene, 2001)—
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nosis (Berkhoff, Donati, & Basetti, 2000; Ka- described in the Hypnosis section—resulted in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

plan, Genoud, Ho, & Jallon, 2000). PET dem- restoration of some degree of communicative
onstrates a depression of brain metabolism abilities and conscious experience of previously
whose level is comparable with that during bar- unconscious cognitive processing (Cohadon &
biturate anesthesia (Rudolf, 2000; Tommasino, Richer, 1993). Any inferences of ASC in those
Grana, Lucignani, Torri, & Fazio, 1995). In the states from ERP, EEG, or other brain-imaging
vegetative state, the EEG is usually moderately methods need external validation through be-
slowed (4 – 6 Hz); the dominant delta-rhythm or havioral, psychological, or clinical criteria. Pa-
extremely flat EEG are rarely observed, typi- tients recovering from coma or vegetative state
cally only in cases of anoxic origin. The decre- usually remain amnesic. At present, conclusions
ment of metabolic activity in subcortical struc- or judgments about the (altered) states of con-
tures in the vegetative state is deeper than in sciousness in coma, vegetative state, or locked-
sleep but less severe than in coma. In contrast, in-syndrome without such external validation
cortical metabolism may even be more strongly are impossible.
suppressed than in coma (Rudolf, 2000; Tom-
masino et al., 1995). Epilepsy
Early evoked potentials to simple stimuli in
both coma and vegetative state can vary from Cortical seizures with their typical pattern of
normal through different degrees of suppression paroxysmal activity are an excellent example of
up to complete disappearance. In coma, the the tight connection between neuroelectric
degree of intactness of the evoked potentials (pathological) changes and conscious experi-
serves as a predictor of outcome (Kane, Butler, ence: Location, extension, and intensity of the
& Simpson, 2000; Kane et al., 1996). This is, neurographic signs are correlated with the qual-
however, not true for vegetative state (Zeitl- ity and intensity of the psychological event be-
hofer et al., 1991). fore, during, and after the seizures.
In coma and vegetative states, late compo- The common underlying neurophysiological
nents (100 –500-ms poststimulus) of ERP to principles of many different types of epileptic
complex stimulus material such as semantic or seizures are hypersynchronizations of extensive
syntactic mismatch are often absent. However, neuronal tissue. Loss of consciousness occurs
Kotchoubey, Lang, et al. (2001) found that even only if large enough cortical tissue in critical
in completely nonresponsive states, 15% to areas is involved and the hypersynchronization
20% of patients show, for example, an N400 causes interruption of normal functioning of the
component to semantically incorrect endings or involved neuronal pool or deactivates structures
a P300 component to simple auditory target involved in regulation of consciousness and at-
stimuli. However, absence of evoked brain re- tention (see Table 3). Partial seizures concern
sponses to simple, automatic stimuli does not local areas of the brain and are excellent exam-
predict absence of evoked brain responses to ples of the “modularity” of consciousness and
complex stimuli, and vice versa, in coma and the underlying brain mechanisms (for a review,
vegetative states (Fischer et al., 1999; Gott, see Niedermeyer & Lopes da Silva, 1999).
Rabinovicz, & DiGiorgio, 1991; Mutschler et Dependent on the anatomical origin of the
al., 1996). The presence of late components in paroxysmal neuroelectric discharge, patients
ERP indicates intact cognitive processing of the experience motor activity, sensory symptoms,
presented material in cortical areas but does not or cognitive, emotional, or autonomic altera-
26 VAITL ET AL.

tions: Particular seizures of the medial temporal content and context of consciousness require a
lobe with the underlying hippocampal and other distinct functional organization of integrative
limbic structures such as the amygdala lead to neuronal circuits whose nature is still far from
characteristic and well-described ASC such as being clearly understood (for a discussion, see
dreamy states, distortions of time sense (Ban- Dehaene & Naccache, 2001).
caud, Brunet-Bourgin, Chauvel, & Halgren,
1994; Vuilleumier, Despland, Assal, & Regli, Discussion
1997), religious experience (Saver & Rabin,
1997), and altered affect (Tisher et al., 1993). Phenomenology of ASC: A Four-
Visual and auditory hallucinations are particu- Dimensional Descriptive Sdstem
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

larly frequent after discharges of the memory


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

structures of the medial temporal lobe and the Facing the variety of ASC, one easily feels
connected hippocampal and cortical regions. lost in the plentitude of disparate and seemingly
Stored memories are excited together with emo- irreducible phenomena. The aim of phenome-
tional responses in a structured or seemingly nology of ASC, as far as it serves science, is not
chaotic fashion (Bien et al., 2000; Brinciotti, Di to account for every tiny detail of experience
Sabato, Matricardi, & Guidetti, 2000; Carmant but, rather, to introduce a reasonably simple
et al., 1996; Gloor, 1990). Out-of-body experi- reference system, a system of dimensions al-
ence and autoscopy (seeing one’s body in ex- lowing for descriptive reduction of observed
trapersonal space) are thought to be due to a behavior or reported experience.
paroxysmal dysfunction of the temporoparietal In the beginnings of scientific interest into
junction in a state of partially and briefly im- ASC, authors constructed lists of basic dimen-
paired consciousness (Blanke, Landis, Spinelli, sions or characteristics of ASC (Ludwig, 1966;
& Seeck, 2004). Tart, 1975). In an attempt at synthesis of those
earlier predecessors, Farthing (1992) ended up
Conclusions with no less than 14 dimensions, a rather eclec-
tic listing of psychological functions and se-
The multiple pathological processes that can lected domains of experience; other authors pro-
result in various degrees of ASC may be viewed posed up to 26 dimensions (Pekala, 1991). For
as impairing brain functions at different func- the purpose of this review, we propose a four-
tional levels that seem to be hierarchically or- dimensional descriptive system that, as shown
ganized. Usually, loss of consciousness—for below, allows one to embed most of the ASC
example, during coma—is a consequence of treated in this report. The dimensions were ob-
severely affected brain stem functions, whereas tained by a synoptic overview of the experien-
during vegetative states brain stem functions are tial material reported from various ASC and a
preserved but most cortical functions are lost. phenomenological reduction to their essential
On a higher cortical level, paroxysmal neuro- features; that is, they are not results of a factor
electric discharges (e.g., epileptic seizures) re- analytic or clustering technique. First we want
sult in altered conscious experiences. The study to briefly characterize the four dimensions, and
of the waxing and waning of symptoms and the then we apply the system to groups of ASC
concomitant physiological changes shows that under study.
the normal stream of consciousness critically 1. Activation in its broadest biological mean-
depends on integrated neural processes and ing refers to the readiness of an organism to
functional clusters subserved by coherent neu- interact with its physical or social environment.
ronal networks, for example, through cortico- Activation is one of the most important dimen-
thalamic and cortico-cortico reentrant interac- sions in objective description of behavior, even
tions. Whenever these integrative networks are in organisms to which we do not (or only hes-
interrupted—for example, by minor asynchrony itatingly) attribute consciousness. In subjective
or complete uncoupling—ASC are likely to oc- experience, activation is represented as being
cur. The findings on disease-induced ASC, at a alert, awake, responsive, and ready to act and
first glance, support the notion of functional and react; the dimension spans high arousal, excite-
neurobiological modularity. However, the dy- ment, and agitation as well as low arousal, re-
namics of altered conscious processes and the laxation, and inertia. Decrease of activation un-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 27

der a certain threshold is incompatible with Activation may be unchanged or reduced (–) in
awareness of reality and results under normal deep relaxation, and sensory dynamics may be
physiological conditions in sleep. unaffected or moderately intensified (⫹).
2. Awareness span refers to the variability of Undoubtedly, other dimensions could also be
the contents available to attention and conscious useful in describing specific features of altered
processing. Awareness span ranges from nar- states. One might think of internally generated
row, focused attention directed at a singular cognitive processes changing in a typical man-
content (e.g., in an intense mental activity) to ner during some of the states listed in Table 2.
broad, extended awareness embracing “all the Especially disease-induced altered conscious
things” in a single grasp (e.g., contemplating the states like schizophrenia and epilepsy are char-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

horizon). Variability of awareness span is ac- acterized by severely impaired cognitive pro-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cessible to the subject and is reported mostly in cesses. The preceding reviews of spontaneously
post hoc, reflective evaluation. occurring and voluntarily induced ASC also
3. Self-awareness refers to the other pole of describe a multitude of alterations with respect
the bipolar self–world structure of human expe- to the amount and the quality of cognitive pro-
rience. In a reflective attitude, all experience is cessing. In view of the diversity of those
“mine,” that is, related to the subject’s self. In changes and the many facets of cognitive pro-
the flow of immediate experience, however, the cesses—thinking, emotion, memory, imag-
degree of self-reference may largely vary from ery—an additional dimension of this type ap-
“forgetting oneself” in absorption or exaltation pears to be too complex to be rated on a single
to an intensified feeling of one’s unique being, scale with adjustable anchor points and scale
“I, here and now.” Variability of self-awareness factors. At present, therefore, we favor to con-
is also subjectively accessed and reported fine the descriptive system to four dimensions
mostly in post hoc, reflective evaluation. that allow a rather broad description. Obvi-
4. Sensory dynamics comprise the variety of ously, if a specific ASC is under scrutiny, ad-
changes in the sensory and perceptual compo- ditional and more specific markers have to com-
nent of subjective experience. With varying plement the four dimensions.
states of consciousness, sensation may be re- Table 2 contains the proposed assignments of
duced (higher thresholds, anesthesia) or en- ASC, classified by the induction methods, as
hanced (lower thresholds, hyperesthesia); some they resulted from expertise given by the re-
ASC are characterized by a particular produc- view. The table shows that most ASC can be
tion of sensations and perceptions even without unequivocally classified within the system. In
an adequate physical stimulus (e.g., synesthesia, some cases, the plus and minus signs may indi-
dreams, hallucinations). The sensation compo- cate variations in the course of induction and
nent is partially accessible to physiological as- maintenance of the respective state. For exam-
sessment (e.g., by sensory threshold measure- ple, within the ganzfeld-induced ASC (see Sen-
ments), but the most important changes in rich- sory Homogenization) we can distinguish “pre-
ness, vividness, structure, and contents of the paratory” and “productive” phases (Wacker-
perceptual component are revealed exclusively mann, Pütz, & Braeunig, 2002); the preparatory
by subjects’ reports. phase is characterized by widened awareness
These four dimensions are well anchored in span, preserved self-awareness, and reduced
everyday experience, and their definition does sensory dynamics, whereas the productive
not require reference to any special or altered phase features focused awareness span, usually
state of consciousness. On the contrary, peculiar reduced or absent self-awareness (“absorp-
characteristics of ASC, often considered to be tion”), and emerging hallucinatory sensory ex-
their unique and irreducible features, can be perience. Sensory homogenization thus effectu-
easily translated into the four-dimensional sys- ates alternating states with the signatures – ⫹ ⫹
tem (see Table 2). For example, the often de- – and – – – ⫹.
scribed “oceanic experience” (Dittrich et al., The entries meditation, hypnosis, and bio-
1985) results primarily from expansion (⫹) of feedback in Table 2, deserve a special mention
the awareness span with simultaneous reduction in this context. The omnipresence of plus and
(–) of self-awareness, while the changes in other minus signs in this group is no counterargument
dimensions play an unspecific, modulating role: against the applicability of the four dimensions;
28 VAITL ET AL.

Table 2
Phenomenological Dimensions of Altered States of Consciousness
Self- Sensory
Activation Awareness span awareness dynamics
(⫹ aroused (⫹ wide ⫺ (⫹ present (⫹ increased
Induction method ⫺ relaxed) narrow) ⫺ absent) ⫺ decreased)
Spontaneously occurring

States of drowsiness ⫺ ⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫺


Daydreaming ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫹
Hypnagogic states ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫹
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Sleep and dreaming ⫺ ⫹ ⫹ ⫹


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Near-death experiences ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫹
Physically and physiologically induced

Extreme environmental conditions ⫹ ⫺ ⫹ ⫹


Starvation and diet ⫹ ⫺ ⫹ ⫺
Sexual activity and orgasm ⫹ ⫺ ⫺ ⫺
Respiratory maneuvers ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫺
Psychologically induced

Sensory deprivation ⫺ ⫹ ⫹/⫺ ⫺


Sensory homogenization ⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹
Sensory overload ⫹ ⫺ ⫺ ⫹
Rhythm-induced trance ⫹ ⫹ ⫺ ⫹
Relaxation ⫺ ⫺ ⫹ ⫺
Meditation ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺
Hypnosis ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺
Biofeedback ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺
Disease induced

Psychotic disorders ⫹/⫺ ⫹/⫺ ⫺ ⫹/⫺


Coma and vegetative state ⫺ ⫺ ⫺ ⫺
Epilepsy ⫹ ⫺ ⫺ ⫹/⫺

rather, it reflects the variety of particular induc- by biofeedback techniques or hypnotic proce-
tion procedures and their intended effects. This dures as well. This observation also presents a
can be exemplified by different meditation tech- challenge for the future studies into ASC,
niques: Activation is reduced in transcendental namely, to differentiate, in terms of their phe-
meditation but increased in tantric meditation; nomenology, the variety of ASC occurring
the awareness span is narrowed in concentrative within each of the broad categories defined by
meditation but widened in mindfulness medita- the induction methods. We believe that the pro-
tion; self-awareness is reduced in states of med- posed four-dimensional descriptive system pro-
itative absorption (samadhi) whereas other vides a useful basis for such studies.
meditation techniques cultivate a distanced, Of the four dimensions listed above, the ac-
self-reflecting attitude; finally, sensory dynam- tivation dimension is nearest to the description
ics may also differ between meditation tech- in terms of physiology, by reference to unspe-
niques depending on the object of meditation, cific activation systems in the brain, to auto-
be it a paradox riddle (kôan), a syllable (man- nomic nervous systems, and to the correlative
tra), the subject’s own breath, or a detailed changes in bodily processes in general. The
visualization of Buddha’s figure. It is thus im- other three dimensions may be related to central
possible to specify unidirectional alterations for integrative and regulatory functions of brain
the category of meditation in its entirety, and subsystems (awareness span and self-aware-
this holds true for the diversity of states induced ness) or to specific sensory systems and higher
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 29

Table 3
Neuroanatomical Substrates of Attention and Arousal (the Limited Capacity Control System)
Neuroanatomical Consequences
system Function of lesion
Prefrontal cortex Working memory, comparison with stored Lack of planning and self-control,
(dorsolateral) contents short attention span
Orbitofrontal cortex Motivational meaning Pseudopsychopathy, apathy
Anterior cingulate Mobilization, error monitoring Akinetic mutism (combined with
cortex extensive prefrontal lesion)
Inferior (right) parietal Disengagement of attention Attentional neglect
cortex
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Reticular and midline Selective attention (“reentrant” Loss of attentional modulation and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

thalamus corticothalamic oscillation) consciousness


Basal ganglia (striatum) Modulation of cortical excitation thresholds Over- or underselectivity of
attention and motor behavior
(i.e., schizophrenia)
Basal forebrain Cholinergic modulation of cortical arousal Loss of consciousness and explicit
and memory memory (i.e., morbus
Alzheimer)
Mesencephalic reticular Cholinergic, glutamatergic and adrenergic Coma, vegetative state
formation arousal of the forebrain
Left frontal operculum “Mirror neurons,” discrimination between Autism (lack of interest in others),
and right anterior self and others, self-awareness, empathy psychopathy (sociopathy)
parietal cortex (“theory of mind”)
Ventromedial prefrontal Combining immediate goals with long-term Loss of planning
cortex goals, cost–benefit analysis for empathy
system
Cerebellum (cerebellar- Coordination of physical activity and Autism (lack of cognitive
frontal pathway) cognitive operations with external stimuli coordination, focus on isolated
stimuli)

level binding and regulatory subsystems (sen- the disorders that result after lesions of these
sory dynamics). Singular dimensions of the pro- subsystems.
posed system can thus be linked to the organi- The limited capacity control system (LCCS)
zational levels as specified in the hierarchical becomes active only if processing of new, com-
scheme presented in the concluding section of plex, or vitally important information and vol-
this report. Some of the underlying neurophys- untary decisions occur. The appearance of con-
iological systems and mechanisms are dis- sciousness at a physiological level therefore
cussed in the following sections. consists of an increase in cortical complexity
(Tononi & Edelman, 1998) and widespread re-
Changes in Arousal Systems
duction of cortical excitation thresholds regu-
ASC are obviously related to alterations of lated by the brain systems enumerated in Table
those brain systems responsible for the regula- 3. The subcortically mediated increase in exci-
tion of consciousness, arousal, and selective tation of forebrain structures requires a certain
attention. While these systems were usually minimal anatomical extension and duration. Re-
characterized as “unspecific,” causing wide- entrant pathways particularly between thalamic
spread excitatory changes in the cortex, more and cortex guarantee reverberation of neuronal
recently several specific subsystems serving cir- activity over a certain time period, usually lon-
cumscribed functions in the regulation of atten- ger than 100 ms, necessary for maintaining the
tion, wakefulness, and sleep were identified (for mutual information exchange (Baars, 1988; Lli-
a review, see Dehaene, 2001). In Table 3 we nas, 2001). ASC may result from various devi-
provide a schematic summary of the functional ations of inter- and intraregional neuronal infor-
brain systems involved in the regulation of at- mation exchange of the LCCS and the con-
tention and consciousness, listing also some of nected brain modules; that is, dreamy states
30 VAITL ET AL.

may result from excitation of posterior sensory functional cell assemblies, a number of studies
cortical association areas without concomitant have shown that each cell assembly can be
prefrontal activation of working memory en- characterized by its own high-frequency oscil-
abling consistent planning, which results in hal- lations. Among the carrier frequencies identi-
lucinations typical for active dreaming (Hob- fied for such oscillations, activities within the
son, Pace-Schott, & Stickgold, 2000). gamma band have become the most prominent
one and were demonstrated to be critical for
Functional Integration and Breakdown of normal conditions of attention, the ability of
Connectivity learning and memory formation, language, and
meaningful motor behaviors. In contrast, a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ASC induced by a decrease of sensory input, breakdown of the connectivity between large
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

homogenization, sensory overload, or strong groups of cell assemblies was suggested to be a


rhythmic patterns of input are associated with a basis for hallucinatory states and states of illu-
strong reduction in the naturally occurring sion during schizophrenia, the emergence of
range of variability in sensory input. Therefore, hypnotic states, and the loss of consciousness
the reduction of sensory input seems to be an induced by anesthesia. Such breakdown of co-
important element to induce ASC; for example, herent oscillations turns complex groups of cell
some methods use the narrowing of sensory assemblies into functionally independent units
input and attention as a core element for induc- and seems to be associated with serious distur-
tion (e.g., hypnosis). bances of stimulus representations and other
Illusions or hallucinations are much more cognitive and behavioral functions.
difficult to explain. Obviously, many different
types of hallucinatory experience exist, each Fine Structure of Brain Functional States
asking for a different neuroscientific explana- and States of Consciousness
tion. Daydreaming and ganzfeld hallucinations
are associated with simultaneous awareness of The state of consciousness changes spontane-
the imaginary nature of the experience. Hallu- ously over time, during development from birth
cinations during dreaming and hypnosis can be to childhood to adulthood, during the circadian
recognized as such retrospectively, whereas cycle, and in shorter near-periodicities. These
schizophrenic patients insist on the reality of changes imply a hierarchy of states of con-
their hallucinated experiences. Brain-imaging sciousness in terms of their characteristic dura-
experiments in schizophrenic individuals during tion and stability (Tart, 1975); for example, the
active auditory hallucinations indicate the acti- sleep–wake cycle follows the circadian period-
vation of a medial temporal lobe– basal gan- icity, and minor fluctuations of vigilance occur
glia–thalamic circuit in the absence of external on a time scale of hours or minutes, whereas
stimulation. In contrast, hypnotically induced Wundt’s “fluctuations of attention” (Wood-
hallucinations are characterized by a dynamic worth & Schlosberg, 1954) are changes in the
dissociation between frontal and parietal range of seconds. This time scale of hierarchi-
connectivity. cally embedded states of consciousness may be
Recent research on the question of how the extended to the subsecond range, as indicated
brain binds together different features of inter- by spatial analyses of brain electric fields.
nal or external stimuli into meaningful repre- Multichannel recordings of spontaneous or
sentations has suggested that such binding evoked brain electric fields can be transformed
might be organized by synchronous neural ac- into series of instantaneous scalp field configu-
tivation within groups of cells with specialized rations, that is, momentary maps showing land-
functional properties. Whereas stimuli with low scapes of different topographies. These brain
complexity are assumed to be represented by field map series exhibit brief epochs of quasi-
only a few of such cell assemblies with re- stable map topographies, separated by rapid,
stricted topographical distribution, stimuli com- discontinuous changes. Accepting that the brain
posed of many complex features are hypothe- at each moment in time is in a particular state,
sized to be represented by larger cell assemblies and considering that different spatial configura-
with widespread topographical organization. tions of the brain electric field must have been
Since Hebb (1949), who first suggested such generated by different sets of active neurons,
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 31

then, one should conclude that different brain some tendency to reversed sequencing among
field configurations indicate different states. certain classes (Lehmann et al., in press).
These brief brain states that occur in the Yet another approach to studies of the neural
subsecond range are called microstates to dis- basis of states of consciousness has involved
tinguish them from global brain macrostates methods of nonlinear dynamics applied to brain
with durations ranging from seconds to minutes. electric activity (Pritchard, 1997) with bearing
Average microstate durations in spontaneous on consciousness-related topics, for example,
EEG, identified by various segmentation proce- differences between sleep stages (Röschke &
dures, have been within the range of 60 –150 ms Aldenhoff, 1991; cf. Achermann, Hartmann,
(Lehmann, Ozaki, & Pal, 1987; Pascual- Gunzinger, Guggenbühl, & Borbély, 1994) and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1995; Strik & increased dimensional complexity in schizo-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Lehmann, 1993). phrenic individuals as compared with controls


Microstates may be classified by their field (Koukkou, Lehmann, Wackermann, Dvorak, &
topographies, compared between groups or Henggeler, 1993; Saito et al., 1998). Although
states in terms of their mean duration and/or of nonlinear complexity measures have been
the percentage of time covered. Topographies mostly calculated from one-dimensional time
of spontaneously occurring microstates in nor- series derived from single scalp loci, these
mal individuals have differed for different men- methods can be applied to the brain in its en-
tation modes, thus suggesting that microstates tirety, considering brain electric activity as a
might be building blocks of consciousness or, spatially distributed process (Babloyantz, 1989;
metaphorically, “atoms of thought” (Lehmann see Dvorak, 1990, on global dimensional com-
et al., 1998). Microstates have differed during plexity). Various linear measures of covariance
maturation (Koenig et al., 2002); have differed complexity of multidimensional brain electric
in awake, drowsy, and REM states (Cantero, data that are less computation intensive and
easier to interpret have also been proposed
Atienza, Salas, & Gomez, 1999); have shown
(Palus, Dvorak, & David, 1990; Pézard et al.,
greater variability in schizophrenia (Merrin,
1996; see Wackermann, 1996, on omega
Meek, Floyd, & Callaway, 1990); and have
complexity).
shown shortened duration, specifically for mi-
Global measures of brain functional states are
crostates of certain classes, in untreated, first- of particular interest for studies of conscious-
episode schizophrenics (Koenig et al., 1999; ness, because consciousness involves the high-
Koukkou, Lehmann, Strik, & Merlo, 1994; Le- est, integrative functions of the brain. The
hmann et al., in press) and chronic schizophren- global complexity measure, omega, provides a
ics (Strelets et al., 2003). single dimension onto which functional states
Mean duration and frequency of occurrence of the whole brain can be mapped. Systematic
provide a first-order description of micro- variations of global omega complexity with
states; a higher order analysis may focus on sleep stages have been demonstrated (Szelen-
the rules of concatenations of the “atoms,” berger, Wackermann, Skalski, Niemcewicz, &
that is, their “syntax.” Analyses of transitions Drojewski, 1996). However, brain states related
between microstates of different classes have to different states of consciousness may project
shown that transition probabilities are class to the same position on a one-dimensional con-
dependent and have revealed asymmetries of tinuum. Accordingly, a three-dimensional sys-
transition probabilities between classes tem of global descriptors—integral power (⌺),
(Wackermann, Lehmann, Michel, & Strik, generalized frequency (⌽), and spatial com-
1993). This indicates that sequences of mi- plexity (⍀)— has been proposed, which allows
crostates are not entirely random but follow a clear separation of sleep stages and indicates
certain probabilistic syntactic rules. Follow- that at least two dimensions may be required to
ing the general interpretation of microstates account for the variety of wakefulness–sleep
as building blocks of consciousness, altera- transitions (Wackermann, 1999). These results
tions of consciousness may reflect changes of have demonstrated applicability of the global
microstate sequencing rules. Indeed, micro- brain state description for consciousness-related
state syntax in untreated, first-episode schizo- studies and have motivated their use in explor-
phrenics has shown clear alteration, with ing other, more subtle alterations. For example,
32 VAITL ET AL.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Different ways to induce altered states of consciousness. Lines within the pyramid
emphasize the strong interdependence of the different levels.

omega complexity as well as global dimen- Final Conclusion and Directions for Future
sional complexity has been found to increase in Research
schizophrenic patients versus controls (Saito et
al., 1998). The variety of ASC as reported above points
Microstate analysis and multichannel EEG to the vulnerably and modifiability of brain dy-
complexity analysis have much in common: namics. Consciousness depends critically on the
They share the focus on the spatial aspect of proper functioning of several brain systems that
brain functional states, they operate on the same may be impaired, for example, by damage to
data—that is, on sequences of scalp field maps brain tissue, neurotransmitter imbalances, hypo-
interpreted as a series of momentary electrical or hypersynchronization, disconnectivity in the
brain states or as a trajectory in a multidimen- firing of neuronal assemblies, and fluctuations
sional state space—and there is a one-to-one in arousal. On the other hand, brain functioning
correspondence between basic notions of the may be modified by alterations in perceptual
two analytical approaches. Higher global spatial input as well as by the use of cognitive self-
EEG complexity thus corresponds to a higher regulation strategies. The following hierarchical
multiplicity of microstates, and both can be scheme (see Figure 1) summarizes the different
interpreted as a higher diversity of actually ac- levels at which brain functioning may be al-
tive brain electric generators involved in the tered. (Table 4 shows that the different condi-
corresponding state of consciousness. Indeed, tions and methods reviewed in this article pro-
convergent findings of microstate and complex- duce ASC by affecting brain functioning on
ity analyses in schizophrenia (Koenig et al., different levels.)
1999; Koukkou et al., 1994; Merrin et al., 1990; During the ordinary waking state, subjective
Saito et al., 1998; Strelets et al., 2003) support reality is created continuously by processes in
this theoretical construct. Thus, microstate anal- the brain. The maintenance of everyday con-
ysis and global complexity analysis seem to sciousness requires intact brain tissue, meta-
provide a unifying framework for assessment bolic homeostasis, a moderate level of arousal,
and interpretation of brain functional states un- a balanced interplay of inhibitory and excitatory
derlying states of consciousness and their networks, and midrange environmental condi-
alterations. tions. As soon as one of these prerequisites for
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 33

Table 4
Levels of Brain Functioning and the Induction of Altered States of Consciousness
Organizational level Techniques, methods, and conditions
Self-control Biofeedback, hypnosis, meditation, relaxation
Arousal Drowsiness, hypnagogic states, sleep, dreaming
Sensory input, environment Rhythmic stimulation, sensory deprivation, and homogenization
Neurochemical and metabolic processes Hyperventilation, starvation and diet, near-death experiences
Brain dynamics Epilepsy, psychotic disorders
Structural (brain tissue) Coma
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

reliable assembly formation is lacking, altera- been provided from MEG recordings (Preissl et
tions of consciousness are likely to occur. Fur- al., 2001; Tononi & Edelman, 1998), which
thermore, by applying psychological procedures avoid some but not all of the pitfalls of surface
like hypnosis or meditation, it seems possible to EEG. In addition, magnetic resonance-based
voluntarily suppress or restrict the formation of noninvasive brain analysis, particularly of lim-
assemblies, resulting in hypnotic analgesia or bic and other subcortical regions (i.e., thalamus)
meditative states of sustained absorption. that are not accessible to EEG, ECoG, and MEG
To test the predictions made on phenomeno- recordings, when combined with simultaneous
logical changes occurring during certain altered recording of EEG will provide complete new
states (see Table 2), on the functional meaning insight into the delicate cortico-subcortical in-
of neuroanatomical systems (see Table 3), and terplay during ASC, as proposed above and as
on the levels of brain functioning provoked by indicated in Table 3. Magnetic resonance spec-
the different induction methods (see Table 4), troscopy, for example, allows neurochemical
some methodological and conceptual ap- analysis of local brain areas during ASC, and
proaches seem to be fruitful and may guide diffusion tensor imaging may provide us with
future research on ASC. completely unknown anatomical connections
Methodological aspects. First, the dynam- between different, often far distant brain areas
ics of micro- and macrostates and the existence during the diverse ASC.
of local perceptual binding in the brain and Third, the inclusion of anatomically confined
quantification of local nonlinear dynamics can lesions and disorders with ASC symptoms, such
be inferred only by direct cortical recording as those depicted in Table 3, may allow the
with electrocorticograms (ECoG) or MEG. independent manipulation of those brain areas
Both approaches allow the analysis of macro- thought to be responsible for a particular ASC
potentials, local field potentials, and oscillations as an independent variable coupled with obser-
undisturbed by the resistance of bone, cerebro- vation of the ASC as a dependent variable. That
spinal fluid, and skin, which act as filters, par- is, reversible inactivation of left operculum with
ticularly for the high-frequency gamma band transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcra-
and electrical brain activity resulting from sulci, nial direct current stimulation may allow one to
composing 60% of all cortical surface. specify the role of “mirror” neuronal fields for
Second, a millimeter-scale localization of the experience of self-alienation comparable to
spontaneous and evoked electrocortical activity those states found in the autistic disorder spec-
is mandatory to observe the subtle changes dur- trum. Furthermore, operant conditioning (self-
ing most of the described ASC and to qualify regulation and biofeedback) of local electro-
the proposed ASC dimensions. ECoG recording magnetic phenomena thought to underlie a par-
is confined to the rare cases in which subdural ticular ASC could be used to test the postulated
or cortical placement of electrodes is ethically correlations.
acceptable, such as preepileptic screening, re- Conceptual aspects. First, the develop-
cording during deep brain stimulation elec- ment of more sophisticated measures of brain
trodes for the treatment of Parkinson’s disease, activity and methods for the manipulation of
dystonia, and chronic pain and brain tumor sur- distinct neuronal units must be complemented
gery. Substantial insight into ASC has already by the proper definition of the altered states
34 VAITL ET AL.

under investigation. In the psychological ap- space; the narrative– descriptive variety of
proach to ASC, the notion of “state” is still states of consciousness implicates such a parti-
laden with the heritage of descriptive psycho- tion of the C-space. Explorative partitioning of
(patho)logy: States are conceived as syndromes the B-space could be done in a similar way,
or clusters of enumerable features. This is con- using physiologically well-defined functional
sistent with the strategy of access to the ASC states as the external criterion.
via reports of subjective experience (first- Fifth, the challenge, then, can be described as
person perspective). However, history shows mapping between the space of functional brain
that natural sciences largely benefited from (a) a states and states of consciousness. The question
transition from enumerative descriptions to of whether these mappings are to be conceptu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

measurable variables, (b) selection of minimal alized as two sides of a one-to-one, invertible
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sets of variables needed to assess the state of the isomorphism or rather one-to-many or many-to-
object under study, and (c) conceptualizing dy- many mappings should remain open. For prac-
namics of state changes in terms of such ab- tical reasons (noise, sources of variance unac-
stract state spaces. We believe that the progress counted for, etc.), locations in both state spaces
of our science lies along similar directions. will always be “blurred,” that is, determined
Second, future research becomes more com- with limited resolution, and thus the empirically
plex by virtue of the fact that we must think in established mappings between B and C will be
terms of two spaces, namely, that of states of necessarily of probabilistic nature. The virtue of
consciousness and that of functional brain the state space approach lies not in perfect one-
states; we refer to them as the C-space and the to-one assignment but rather in assessment of
B-space, respectively. The four-dimensional de- general features of the coarse-grained
scriptive system proposed may be considered a mappings.
first step toward a construction of the C-space. With increasing knowledge of the neural cor-
Development and validation of measurement relates of consciousness, the formerly strange
tools (questionnaires, inventories) should fol- and hard to explain phenomena of ASC become
low. There is no guarantee that the four dimen- increasingly understandable as a natural conse-
sions represent a minimal set, but experience quence of the workings of the brain. This re-
shows that state space approaches work best view of the current state of the art of scientific
with reasonably low numbers of dimensions. research on ASC clearly supports this perspec-
Third, concerning the construction of the B- tive and hopefully stimulates further investiga-
space, our problem is not a lack of measurable tions into the psychobiology of ASC.
data. Owing to the progress of modern neuro-
imaging methods, we have rather too much
data. Novel data-analytical strategies are re-
quired to reduce the plenitude of momentary References
spatial distributions of brain activation to a lim- Abraini, J. H. (1995). Evidence for inert gas narcosis
ited number of “base modes.” Following this mechanisms in the occurrence of psychotic-like
reduction, a minimal number of state variables, episodes at pressure environment. NeuroReport, 6,
constituting the reduced representation and thus 2435–2439.
assessing the global brain functional state, Abraini, J. H., Martinez, E., Lemaire, C., Bisson, T.,
should be defined. The global descriptors of deMendoza, J. L. J., & Therme, P. (1997). Anxi-
spatially distributed brain electric data men- ety, sensorimotor and cognitive performance dur-
tioned in the section Fine Structure of Brain ing a hydrogen-oxygen dive and long-term con-
Functional States and States of Consciousness finement in a pressure chamber. Journal of Envi-
provide an example of this general strategy, ronmental Psychology, 17, 157–164.
Achermann, P., Hartmann, R., Gunzinger, A., Gug-
which, however, has been confined to electrical
genbühl, W., & Borbély, A. A. (1994). Is the EEG
brain fields data measured from the scalp generated by a chaotic attractor? Dimension anal-
surface. ysis of all-night sleep EEG. Journal of Sleep Re-
Fourth, the syndromelike notion of state, search, 3, 1.
abandoned above in favor of products of mea- Aftanas, L. I., & Golocheikine, S. A. (2001). Human
surable dimensions, may reappear at a later anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha
stage redefined as compact regions of the state reflect emotionally positive state and internalised
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 35

attention: High-resolution EEG investigation of Bast-Pettersen, R. (1999). Long-term neuropsycho-


meditation. Neuroscience Letters, 310, 57– 60. logical effects in non-saturation construction div-
Aftanas, L. I., Varlamov, A. A., Pavlov, S. V., Makh- ers. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine,
nev, V. P., & Reva, N. V. (2001). Affective picture 70(1), 51–57.
processing: Event-related synchronisation within Behrendt, R.-P., & Young, C. (in press). Hallucina-
individually defined human theta band is modu- tions in schizophrenia, sensory impairment and
lated by valence dimension. Neuroscience Letters, brain disease: A unifying model. Behavioral and
303, 115–118. Brain Sciences.
Aghajanian, G. K., & Marek, G. J. (1999). Serotonin Bem, D., & Honorton, C. (1994). Does psi exist?
and hallucinogens. Neuropsychopharmacology, Replicable evidence for an anomalous process of
21(2), S16 –S23. information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 115,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine. 4 –18.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(1995). Recommendations for use of uniform no- Benson, H. (1975). The relaxation response. New
menclature pertinent to patients with severe alter- York: Morrow.
ations in consciousness. Archives of Physical Med- Ben-Tovim, D. I., & Walker, M. K. (1991). Further
icine and Rehabilitation, 76, 205–209. evidence for the Stroop test as a quantitative mea-
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnos- sure of psychopathology in eating disorders. Inter-
tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th national Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 609 –
ed.). Washington, DC: Author. 613.
American Sleep Disorders Association. (1990). Inter- Berkhoff, M., Donati, F., & Basetti, C. (2000). Post-
national classification of Sleep disorders: Diag- anoxic alpha (theta) coma: A reappraisal of its
nostic and coding manual (ICSD). Rochester, MN: prognostic significance. Clinical Neurophysiology,
Author. 111, 297–304.
Antrobus, J. (1983). REM and NREM sleep reports: Bhargava, R., Mukerji, S., & Sachdeva, U. (2000).
Comparison of word frequencies by cognitive Psychological impact of the Antarctic winter on
Indian expeditioners. Environment and Behavior,
classes. Psychophysiology, 20, 562–568.
32(1), 111–127.
Arkin, A. M., Antrobus, J. A., & Ellman, S. J. (Eds.).
Bien, C. G., Benninger, F. O., Urbach, H., Schramm,
(1978). The mind in sleep: Psychology and psy-
J., Kurthen, M., & Elger, C. E. (2000). Localizing
chophysiology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
value of epileptic visual auras. Brain, 123, 244 –
Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953, September 4).
253.
Regularly occurring periods of eye motility and
Birbaumer, N. (1977). Operant enhancement of EEG-
concomitant phenomena during sleep. Science,
theta activity. In J. Beatty & H. Legewie (Eds.),
118, 273–274. Biofeedback and behavior (pp. 135–145). New
Atkinson, R. (1993). Short-term exposure to REST: York: Plenum Press.
Enhanced performance on a signal-detection task. Birbaumer, N. (1984). Operant control of slow brain
In A. F. Barabasz & M. Barabasz (Eds.), Clinical potentials: A tool in the investigation of the poten-
and experimental restricted environmental stimu- tial’s meaning and its relation to attentional dys-
lation: New developments and perspectives (pp. function. In T. Elbert, B. Rockstroh, W. Lutzen-
93–100). New York: Springer. berger, & N. Birbaumer (Eds.), Self-regulation of
Avant, L. L. (1965). Vision in the ganzfeld. Psycho- the brain and behaviour (pp. 227–239). Berlin,
logical Bulletin, 64, 246 –258. Germany: Springer-Verlag.
Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of conscious- Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Canavan, A. G. M., &
ness. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Rockstroh, B. (1990). Slow potentials of the cere-
Press. bral cortex and behavior. Physiological Reviews,
Babloyantz, A. (1989). Estimation of correlation di- 70, 1– 41.
mensions from single and multi-channel record- Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T., Rockstroh, B., Daum, I.,
ings: A critical view. In E. Basar & T. H. Bullock Wolf, P., & Canavan, A. (1991). Clinical-
(Eds.), Brain dynamics: Progress and perspectives psychological treatment of epileptic seizures: A
(pp. 122–130). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. controlled study. In A. Ehlers (Ed.), Perspectives
Baldeweg, T., Spence, S., Hirsch, J., & Gruzelier, J. and promises in clinical psychology (pp. 81–96).
(1998, August 22). Gamma-band EEG oscillations New York: Plenum Press.
in a patient with somatic hallucinations. Lancet, Birbaumer, N., Elbert, T. Rockstroh, B., & Lutzen-
352, 620 – 621. berger, W. (1981). Biofeedback of event-related
Bancaud, J., Brunet-Bourgin, F., Chauvel, P., & Hal- slow potentials of the brain. International Journal
gren, E. (1994). Anatomical origin of deja vu and of Psychophysiology, 16, 389 – 415.
vivid “memories” in human temporal lobe epi- Birbaumer, N., Ghanayim, N., Hinterberger, T.,
lepsy. Brain, 117, 71–90. Iversen, I., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A., et al.
36 VAITL ET AL.

(1999, March 25). A spelling device for the para- altitude climbers. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol-
lysed. Nature, 398, 297–298. ogy, and Behavioral Neurology, 12(1), 67–71.
Birbaumer, N., & Kimmel, H. D. (Eds.). (1979). Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M., Salas, R. M., & Gomez,
Biofeedback and self-regulation. New York: Wi- C. M. (1999). Brain spatial microstates of human
ley. spontaneous alpha activity in relaxed wakefulness.
Blackmore, S. (1996). Near-death experiences. In G. Brain Topography, 11, 257–263.
Stein (Ed.), The encyclopedia of the paranormal Cardeña, E., Lynn, S. J., & Krippner, S. (Eds.).
(pp. 425– 441). Amherst, NY: Prometheus. (2000). Varieties of anomalous experience: Exam-
Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., & Seeck, M. ining the scientific evidence. Washington, DC:
(2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of American Psychological Association.
neurological origin. Brain, 127, 243–258. Carmant, L., Carrazana, E., Kramer, U., Helmers,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Bourguignon, E. (1966). World distribution and pat- S. L., Holmes, G. L., Black, P. M., & Mikati,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

terns of possession states. In R. Prince (Ed.), M. A. (1996). Pharyngeal dysesthesia as an aura in


Trance and possession states (pp. 3–34). Montreal, temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 37, 911–913.
Quebec, Canada: R. M. Burket Memorial Society. Carr, G. P. (1991). Human experience in space. Cu-
Bourguignon, E. (1973). Religion, altered states of tis, 48, 289 –290.
consciousness and social change. Columbus: Ohio Charles, A., Allimann, F., & Ragot, P. (2001). Diving
State University Press. and cognitive performance: The selective impair-
Bowers, K. S. (1992). Imagination and dissociation ment of control processes. In F. Columbus (Ed.),
in hypnotic responding. International Journal of Advances in psychology research (Vol. 2, pp. 185–
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 253–275. 192). Huntington, NY: Nova Science.
Brauchli, P., Michel, C. M., & Zeier, H. (1995). Childs, N. L., & Merger, W. N. (1996). Brief report:
Electrocortical, autonomic, and subjective re- Late improvement in consciousness after posttrau-
sponses to rhythmic audio-visual stimulation. In- matic vegetative state. New England Journal of
ternational Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 53– Medicine, 334, 24 –25.
66. Chowdhury, B. (2000). The last dive: A father and
Braud, W. G., Wood, R., & Braud, L. W. (1975). son’s fatal descent into the ocean’s depths. New
Free-response GESP performance during an exper- York: HarperCollins.
imental hypnagogic state induced by visual and Cohadon, F., & Richer, E. (1993). Deep cerebral
acoustic ganzfeld techniques: A replication and stimulation in patients with post-traumatic vegeta-
extension. Journal of the American Society for tive state: 25 cases. Neurochirurgie, 39, 281–292.
Psychical Research, 69, 105–113. Cohen, A. S., Rosen, R. C., & Goldstein, L. (1985).
Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., EEG hemispheric asymmetry during sexual arous-
Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., et al. (1998, al: Psychophysiological patterns in responsive, un-
January 2). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual responsive, and dysfunctional men. Journal of Ab-
cortices and their projections during human rapid normal Psychology, 94, 580 –590.
eye movement sleep. Science, 279, 91–95. Cohen, H. D., Rosen, R. C., & Goldstein, L. (1976).
Brennen, T., Martinussen, M., Hansen, B. O., & Electroencephalographic laterality changes during
Hjemdal, O. (1999). Arctic cognition: A study of human sexual orgasm. Archives of Sexual Behav-
cognitive performance in summer and winter at 69 ior, 5, 189 –199.
degrees N. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, Crawford, H. J. (1994). Brain dynamics and hypno-
561–580. sis: Attentional and disattentional processes. Inter-
Brinciotti, M., Di Sabato, M. L., Matricardi, M., & national Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Guidetti, V. (2000). Electroclinical features in chil- Hypnosis, 42, 204 –232.
dren and adolescents with epilepsy and/or mi- Crawford, H. J., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1992). A mid-
graine, and occipital epileptiform EEG abnormal- stream view of the neuropsychophysiology of hyp-
ities. Clinical Electroencephalography, 31, 76 – nosis: Recent research and future directions. In E.
82. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hyp-
Bristol-Power, M. M., & Spinella, G. (1999). Re- nosis research (pp. 227–266). New York: Guilford
search in screening and diagnosis in autism: A Press.
work in progress. Journal of Autism and Develop- Crawford, H. J., Knebel, T., & Vendemia, M. C.
mental Disorders, 29, 435– 438. (1998). The nature of hypnotic analgesia: Neuro-
Brubakk, A. O. (2000). Man in extreme environ- physiological foundation and evidence. Contempo-
ments. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medi- rary Hypnosis, 15, 22–33.
cine, 71(9, Sect. 2, Suppl.), A126 –A130. Critchley, H. D., Corfield, D. R., Chandler, M. P.,
Brugger, P., Regard, M., Landis, T., & Oelz, O. Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Cerebral
(1999). Hallucinatory experiences in extreme- correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: A
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 37

functional neuroimaging investigation in humans. space and polar field analogs. Human Performance
Journal of Physiology, 523, 259 –270. in Extreme Environments, 2(1), 119 –127.
Critchley, H. D., Melmed, R. N., Featherstone, E., Dvorak, I. (1990). Takens versus multichannel recon-
Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Brain activ- struction in EEG correlation exponent estimations.
ity during biofeedback relaxation: A functional Physics Letters Series A, 132, 101–106.
neuroimaging investigation. Brain, 124, 1003– Easterlin, B. L., & Cardeña, E. (1998). Cognitive and
1012. emotional differences between short- and long-
Croft, R. J., Williams, J. D., Haenschel, C., & Gru- term Vipassana meditators. Imagination, Cogni-
zelier, J. H. (2002). Pain perception, 40 Hz oscil- tion and Personality, 18, 69 – 81.
lations and hypnotic analgesia. International Jour- Egner, T., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2001). Learned self-
nal of Psychophysiology, 46, 101–108. regulation of EEG frequency components affects
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Cunningham, S., Scerbo, M. W., & Freeman, F. G. attention and event-related brain potentials in hu-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(2000). The electrocortical correlates of daydream- mans. NeuroReport, 12, 4155– 4159.
ing during vigilance tasks. Journal of Mental Im- Egner, T., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Ecological va-
agery, 24, 61–72. lidity of neurofeedback: Modulation of slow wave
Curcio, G., Casagrande, M., & Bertini, M. (2001). EEG enhances musical performance. NeuroRe-
Sleepiness: Evaluating and quantifying methods. port, 14, 1221–1224.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 41, Egner, T., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2004). EEG biofeed-
251–263. back of low beta band components: Frequency-
Davies, D. R., & Tune, G. S. (1970). Human vigi- specific effects on variables of attention and event-
lance performance. London: Staples Press. related brain potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology,
Davis, H., Davis, P. A., Loomis, A. L., Harvey, E. N., 115, 131–139.
& Hobart, G. (1937, November 12). Changes in Farthing, G. W. (1992). The psychology of conscious-
human brain potentials during the onset of sleep. ness. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Science, 86, 448 – 450. Farvolden, P., & Woody, E. Z. (2004). Hypnosis,
Dehaene, S. (Ed.). (2001). The cognitive neurosci-
memory, and frontal executive functioning. Inter-
ence of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT
national Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Press.
Hypnosis, 52, 3–26.
Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a
Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary
cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic ev-
discharge: Implications for thinking and its disor-
idence and a workspace framework. In S. Dehaene
ders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 4, 636 – 640.
(Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness
Feinberg, I. (1982). Schizophrenia: Caused by a fault
(pp. 1–37). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Delvenne, V., Goldman, S., Biver, F., De Maertalaer, in programmed synaptic elimination during ado-
V., Wikler, D., Damhaut, P., & Lotstra, F. (1997). lescence? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17,
Brain hypometabolism of glucose in low-weight 319 –334.
depressed patients and in anorectic patients: A Feinberg, I., & Guazzelli, M. (1999). Schizophrenia:
consequence of starvation? Journal of Affective A disorder of the corollary discharge systems that
Disorders, 41(1), 69 –77. integrate the motor systems of thought with the
Delvenne, V., Goldman, S., De Maertelaer, V., Si- sensory systems of consciousness. British Journal
mon, Y., Luxen, A., & Lotstra, F. (1996). Brain of Psychiatry, 174, 196 –204.
hypometabolism of glucose in anorexia nervosa: Fenwick, P. (1987). Meditation and the EEG. In
Normalization after weight gain. Biological Psy- M. A. West (Ed.), The psychology of meditation
chiatry, 40, 761–768. (pp. 104 –117). Oxford, England: Oxford Univer-
De Pascalis, V. (1999). Psychophysiological corre- sity Press.
lates of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. In- Fischer, C., Morlet, D., Bouchet, P., Luaute, J., Jour-
ternational Journal of Clinical and Experimental dan, C., & Salord, F. (1999). Mismatch negativity
Hypnosis, 47, 117–143. and late auditory evoked potentials in comatose
Dittrich, A., von Arx, S., & Staub, S. (1985). Inter- patients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 1601–
national study on altered states of consciousness 1610.
(ISASC): Summary of the results. German Journal Fletcher, P. (1998). The missing link: A failure of
of Psychology, 9, 319 –339. fronto-hippocampal integration in schizophrenia.
Dua, S., & MacLean, P. D. (1964). Localization for Nature Neuroscience, 1, 266 –267.
penile erection in medial frontal lobe. American Flor-Henry, P. (1980). Cerebral aspects of the orgas-
Journal of Physiology, 207, 1425–1434. mic response: Normal and deviational. In R. For-
Dudley Rowley, M. (1997). Deviance among expe- leo & W. Pasini (Eds.), Medical sexology (pp.
ditioners: Defining the off-nominal act through 256 –262). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
38 VAITL ET AL.

Forgays, D. G., & Forgays, D. K. (1992). Creativity Gloor, P. (1990). Experiential phenomena of tempo-
enhancement through flotation isolation. Journal ral lobe epilepsy: Facts and hypotheses. Brain,
of Environmental Psychology, 12, 329 –335. 113, 1673–1694.
Foulkes, D. (1996). Dream research: 1953–1993. Gorman, D. G., & Cummings, J. L. (1992). Hyper-
Sleep, 19, 609 – 624. sexuality following septal injury. Archives of Neu-
Foulkes, D., & Pope, R. (1973). Primary visual ex- rology, 49, 308 –310.
periences and secondary cognitive elaboration in Gott, P. S., Rabinovicz, A. L., & DiGiorgio, C. M.
stage REM: A modest conformation and an exten- (1991). P300 auditory event-related potentials in
sion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 37, 107–118. nontraumatic coma: Association with Glasgow
Foulkes, D., & Schmidt, M. (1983). Temporal se- Coma Score and awakening. Archives of Neurol-
quence and unit composition in dream reports from ogy, 48, 1267–1270.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

different stages of sleep. Sleep, 6, 265–280. Graber, B., Rohrbaugh, J. W., Newlin, D. B., Varner,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Foulkes, D., & Vogel, G. (1965). Mental activity at J. L., & Ellingson, R. J. (1985). EEG during mas-
sleep onset. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 70, turbation and ejaculation. Archives of Sexual Be-
231–243. havior, 14, 491–503.
Fowler, B., Comfort, D., & Bock, O. (2000). A Graybiel, A. M. (1997). The basal ganglia and cog-
review of cognitive and perceptual-motor perfor- nitive pattern generators. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
mance in space. Aviation Space and Environmen- 23, 459 – 469.
tal Medicine, 71(9), A66 –A68. Greenwood, M. M., & Benson, H. (1977). The effi-
Freeman, W. (1973). Sexual behavior and fertility cacy of progressive relaxation in systematic desen-
after frontal lobotomy. Biological Psychiatry, 6, sitization and a proposal for and alternative com-
97–104. petitive response: The relaxation response. Behav-
Freud, S. (1900). Die Traumdeutung [The interpreta- iour Research and Therapy, 15, 337–343.
tion of dreams]. Leipzig, Germany: Deuticke. Greyson, B. (2000). Near-death experiences. In E.
Friederich, M., Trippe, R. H., Özcan, M., Weiss, T., Cardeña, S. J. Lynn, & S. Krippner (Eds.), Vari-
eties of anomalous experience: Examining the sci-
Hecht, H., & Miltner, W. H. R. (2001). Laser
entific evidence (pp. 315–351). Washington, DC:
evoked potentials to noxious stimulation during
American Psychological Association.
hypnotic analgesia and distraction of attention sug-
Grof, S. (1976). Realms of the human unconscious:
gest different brain mechanisms of pain control.
Observation from LSD research. New York: Vi-
Psychophysiology, 38, 768 –776.
king.
Friederici, A. D., & Levelt, W. J. (1990). Spatial
Grossman, P., Janssen, K. H. L., & Vaitl, D. (Eds.).
reference in weightlessness: Perceptual factors and
(1986). Cardiorespiratory and cardiosomatic psy-
mental representations. Perception & Psychophys- chophysiology. NATO ASI Series. New York: Ple-
ics, 47, 253–266. num Press.
Gardner, R., Grossman, W. I., Roffwarg, H. P., & Grunwald, M., Ettrich, C., Assmann, B., Dahne, A.,
Weiner, H. (1975). The relationship of small limb Krause, W., Busse, F., & Gertz, H. J. (2001).
movements during REM sleep to dreamed limb Deficits in haptic perception and right parietal
action. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37, 147–159. theta power changes in patients with anorexia ner-
Gardner, W. N. (1996). The pathophysiology of hy- vosa before and after weight gain. International
perventilation disorders. Chest, 2, 516 –534. Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 417– 428.
Gazzaniga, M. (Ed.). (2000). The new cognitive neu- Gruzelier, J. H. (1979). Synthesis and critical review
rosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. of the evidence for hemisphere asymmetries of
Gentleman, D. (1999). Improving outcome after trau- function in psychopathology. In J. H. Gruzelier &
matic brain injury: Progress and challenges. Brit- P. Flor-Henry (Eds.), Hemispheric asymmetries of
ish Medical Bulletin, 55, 910 –926. function in psychopathology (pp. 647– 672). Am-
Gerne, M., & Strauch, I. (1985). Psychophysiological sterdam: Elsevier.
indicators of affect patterns and conversational sig- Gruzelier, J. H. (1998). A working model of the
nals during sleep. In W. P. Koella, E. Rüther, & H. neurophysiology of hypnosis: A review of the ev-
Schulz (Eds.), Sleep ’84 (pp. 367–369). Stuttgart, idence. Contemporary Hypnosis, 15, 3–21.
Germany: Gustav Fischer. Gruzelier, J. H. (1999a). Functional neuro-psycho-
Giambra, L. M. (1999 –2000). The temporal setting, physiological asymmetry in schizophrenia: A re-
emotions, and imagery of daydreams: Age changes view and reorientation. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
and age differences from late adolescent to the 25(1), 91–120.
old– old. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, Gruzelier, J. H. (1999b). A review of the implications
19, 367– 413. of early sensory processing and subcortical in-
Glide, S. (1994). Maintaining sensory balance. Nurs- volvement for cognitive dysfunction in schizo-
ing Times, 90(17), 33–34. phrenia. In C. Tamminga (Ed.), Review of psychi-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 39

atry: Vol. 25. Schizophrenia in the molecular age Herrmann, W. M., Kubicki, S. K., & Röhmel, J.
(pp. 29 –76). New York: American Psychiatric As- (1988). Vigilance classification system: Develop-
sociation. ment, sample values, and applications in nootropic
Gruzelier, J. H. (2000). Redefining hypnosis: Theory, drug research. Zeitschrift für Gerontopsychologie
methods and integration. Contemporary Hypnosis, und Psychiatrie, 1, 1–33.
17, 51–70. Hilgard, E. R., & Hilgard, J. R. (1983). Hypnosis in
Gruzelier, J. H. (2002). A Janusian perspective on the the relief of pain. Los Altos, CA: William Kauf-
nature, development and structure of schizophre- mann.
nia and schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research, Hobson, J. A. (1999). The new neuropsychology of
54(1–2), 95–103. sleep: Implications for psychoanalysis. Neuro-
Gruzelier, J. H., Hardman, E., Wild, J., & Zaman, R. Psychoanalysis, 1, 157–183.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(1999). Learned control of slow potential inter- Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

hemispheric asymmetry in schizophrenia. Interna- (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cogni-
tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 34, 341–348. tive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral
Gruzelier, J. H., & Warren, K. (1993). Neuropsycho- and Brain Sciences, 23, 793– 842.
logical evidence of reductions on left frontal tests Honorton, C., Berger, R. E., Varvoglis, M. P., Quant,
with hypnosis. Psychological Medicine, 23, 93– M., Derr, P., Schechter, E. I., & Ferrari, D. C.
101. (1990). Psi communication in the ganzfeld: Exper-
Guilleminault, C., Billard, M., Montplaisir, J., & iments with an automated testing system and a
Dement, W. C. (1972). Altered states of conscious- comparison with a meta-analysis of earlier studies.
ness in disorders of daytime sleepiness. Journal of Journal of Parapsychology, 54, 99 –139.
Neurological Sciences, 26, 377–393. Honorton, C., & Harper, S. (1974). Psi-mediated
Hackett, P. H., & Roach, R. C. (2001). High-altitude imagery and ideation in an experimental procedure
illness. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, for regulating perceptual input. Journal of the
107–114. American Society for Psychical Research, 68,
Haggarty, J., Cernovsky, Z., Kermeen, P., & Mer- 156 –168.
skey, H. (2000). Psychiatric disorders in an arctic Hori, T., Hayashi, M., & Morikawa, T. (1994). To-
community. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, pographical EEG changes and the hypnagogic ex-
357–362. perience. In R. D. Ogilvie & J. R. Harsh (Eds.),
Halm, M. A., & Alpen, M. A. (1993). The impact of Sleep onset: Normal and abnormal processes (pp.
technology on patients and families. Nursing Clin- 237–253). Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
ics of North America, 28, 443– 457. ical Association.
Hamad, N. M., Fine, T. H., & Turner, J. W. (1996). Huon, G. F., & Brown, L. B. (1996). Task depen-
Effects of restricted environmental stimulation dence in color-naming latency among dieters. In-
therapy (REST) on plasma levels of epinephrine ternational Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 405–
and norepinephrine. Biofeedback and Self- 410.
Regulation, 21, 359 –360. Hyde, T. M., Ziegler, J. C., & Weinberger, D. R.
Harnad, S. (Ed.). (2000). Sleep and dreaming [Spe- (1992). Psychiatric disturbances in metachromatic
cial issue]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6). leukodystrophy: Insights into the neurobiology of
Harsh, J. R. (1994). Event-related potential changes psychosis. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry,
during the wake-to-sleep transition. In R. D. Ogil- 49, 401– 406.
vie & J. R. Harsh (Eds.), Sleep onset: Normal and Isotani, T., Lehmann, D., Pascual-Marqui, R. D.,
abnormal processes (pp. 309 –326). Washington, Kochi, K., Wackermann, J., Saito, N., et al. (2001).
DC: American Psychological Association. EEG source localization and global dimensional
Head, H. (1923). The conception of nervous and complexity in high- and low-hypnotizable sub-
mental energy: II. Vigilance: A physiological state jects: A pilot study. Neuropsychobiology, 44, 192–
of the nervous system. British Journal of Psychol- 198.
ogy, 14, 126 –147. Isotani, T., Tanaka, H., Lehmann, D., Pascual Mar-
Heath, R. G. (1972). Pleasure and brain activity in qui, R. D., Kochi, K., Saito, N., et al. (2001).
men. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 154, Source localization of EEG activity during hypnot-
3–18. ically induced anxiety and relaxation. Interna-
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A tional Journal of Psychophysiology, 41, 143–153.
neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley. Jacobs, G. D., Benson, H., & Friedman, R. (1996).
Hebert, R., & Lehmann, D. (1977). Theta bursts: An Topographic EEG mapping of the relaxation re-
EEG pattern in normal subjects practising the tran- sponse. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21, 121–
scendental meditation technique. Electroencepha- 129.
lography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42, 397– Jacobs, L., Feldman, B., & Bender, M. B. (1972). Are
405. the eye movements of dreaming sleep related to
40 VAITL ET AL.

the visual images of dreams? Psychophysiology, 9, microstate in acute, neuroleptic-naı̈ve schizo-


393– 401. phrenics at rest. European Archives of Psychiatry
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, 205–211.
(chap. 9). New York: Holt. Koenig, T., Prichep, L. S., Lehmann, D., Valdes-
Jansen, K. L. R. (1997). The ketamine model of the Sosa, P., Braeker, E., Kleinlogel, H., et al. (2002).
near-death experience: A central role for the N- Millisecond by millisecond, year by year: Norma-
methyl-D-aspartate receptor. Journal of Near- tive EEG microstates and developmental stages.
Death Studies, 16, 5–26. NeuroImage, 16, 41– 48.
Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Stensmo, M., & Sejnowski, Kotchoubey, B., Lang, S., Baales, R., Herb, E., Mau-
T. (1997). Estimating alertness from the EEG rer, P., Mezger, G., et al. (2001). Brain potentials
power spectrum. IEEE Transactions on Biomedi- in human patients with severe diffuse brain dam-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cal Engineering, 44, 60 – 69. age. Neuroscience Letters, 301, 37– 40.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Kahn, P. M., & Leon, G. R. (1994). Group climate Kotchoubey, B., Schneider, D., Strehl, U., Uhlmann,
and individual functioning in an all women’s Ant- C., Blankenhorn, V., Fröscher, W., & Birbaumer,
arctic expedition team. Environment and Behavior, N. (1996). Self-regulation of slow cortical poten-
26, 669 – 697. tials in epilepsy: A retrial with analysis of influ-
Kaiser, J., Barker, R., Haenschel, C., Baldeweg, T., encing factors. Epilepsy Research, 25, 269 –276.
& Gruzelier, J. (1997). Hypnosis and event-related Kotchoubey, B., Strehl, U., Holzapfel, S., Blanken-
potential correlates of error processing in a Stroop- horn, V., Fröscher, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1999).
type paradigm: A test of the frontal hypothesis. Negative potential shifts and the prediction of the
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, outcome of neurofeedback therapy in epilepsy.
215–222. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 683– 686.
Kaiser, J., & Gruzelier, J. H. (1999). Timing of Kotchoubey, B., Strehl, U., Uhlmann, C., Holzapfel,
puberty and syndromes of schizotypy: A replica- S., König, M., Fröscher, W., et al. (2001). Modi-
tion. International Journal of Psychophysiology, fication of slow cortical potentials in patients with
34, 237–248.
refractory epilepsy: A controlled outcome study.
Kallio, S., Revonsuo, A., Hamalainen, H., & Gruze-
Epilepsia, 42, 406 – 416.
lier, J. H. (2001). Anterior brain functions and
Koukkou, M., & Lehmann, D. (1983). Dreaming:
hypnosis: A test of the frontal hypothesis. Interna-
The functional state-shift hypothesis. A neuro-
tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
physiological model. British Journal of Psychia-
nosis, 49, 95–108.
try, 142, 221–231.
Kamiya, J. (1969). Operant control of the EEG alpha
rhythm and some of its effects on consciousness. Koukkou, M., Lehmann, D., Strik, W. K., & Merlo,
In C. T. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness M. C. (1994). Maps of microstates of spontaneous
(pp. 489 –501). New York: Wiley. EEG in never-treated acute schizophrenia. Brain
Kane, N. M., Butler, S. R., & Simpson, T. (2000). Topography, 6, 251–252.
Coma outcome prediction using event-related po- Koukkou, M., Lehmann, D., Wackermann, J.,
tentials: P3 and mismatch negativity. Audiology Dvorak, I., & Henggeler, B. (1993). Dimensional
and Neuro-Otology, 5, 186 –191. complexity of EEG brain mechanisms in untreated
Kane, N. M., Curry, S. H., Rowlands, C. A., Minara, schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 33, 397–
A. R., Lewis, T., Moss, T., et al. (1996). Event- 407.
related potentials: Neurophysiological tools for Kübler, A., Kotchoubey, B., Kaiser, J., Wolpaw,
predicting emergence and early outcome from J. R., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Brain– computer
traumatic coma. Intensive Care in Medicine, 22, communication: Unlocking the locked in. Psycho-
39 – 46. logical Bulletin, 127, 358 –375.
Kaplan, P. W., Genoud, D., Ho, T. W., & Jallon, P. Kuhlman, W. N. (1978). EEF feedback training: En-
(2000). Clinical correlates and prognosis in early hancement of somato-sensory cortical activity.
spindle coma. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophys-
584 –590. iology, 44, 290 –294.
Kirsch, I. (2000). The response set theory of hypno- Kuhlo, W., & Lehmann, D. (1964). Das Einschlafer-
sis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 42, leben und seine neurophysiologische Korrelate
274 –301. [Experience during the onset of sleep and its neu-
Klinger, E. (1978). Modes of normal conscious flow. rophysiological correlates]. Archiv für Psychiatrie
In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of und Zeitschrift für die Gesamte Neurologie, 205,
consciousness (pp. 91–116). London: Plenum 687–716.
Press. Kumar, V. K., & Pekala, R. J. (1988). Hypnotizabil-
Koenig, T., Lehmann, D., Merlo, M., Kochi, K., Hell, ity, absorption, and individual differences in phe-
D., & Koukkou, M. (1999). A deviant EEG brain nomenological experience. International Journal
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 41

of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 36(2), 80 – imagery and abstract thoughts. International Jour-
88. nal of Psychophysiology, 29, 1–11.
LaBerge, S. (2000). Lucid dreaming: Evidence and Lempert, T., Bauer, M., & Schmidt, D. (1994). Syn-
methodology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, cope: A videometric analysis of 56 episodes of
962–963. transient cerebral hypoxia. Annals of Neurology,
Ladd, G. T. (1892). Contribution to the psychology 36, 233–237.
of visual dreams. Mind, 1, 299 –304. Leroy, E. B. (1933). Les visions du demi-sommeil
Lautenbacher, S., Pauls, A. M., Strian, F., Pirke, [The visions during being half asleep]. Paris: Al-
K. M., & Krieg, J. C. (1991). Pain sensitivity in can.
anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Biological Lichstein, K. L. (1988). Clinical relaxation strate-
Psychiatry, 29, 1073–1078. gies. New York: Wiley.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Lavie, P. (1979). Ultradian rhythm in alertness: A Lilly, J. C. (1956). Mental effects of reduction of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

pupillometric study. Biological Psychology, 9, 49 – ordinary levels of physical stimuli on intact,


62. healthy persons. Psychiatric Research Reports, 5,
Lawrie, S. M., Buechel, C., Whalley, H. C., Frith, 1–9.
C. D., Friston, K. J., & Johnstone, E. C. (2002). Lilly, J. C. (1977). The deep self. New York: Simon
Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in & Schuster.
schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucina- Lindsley, D. B. (1960). Attention, consciousness,
tions. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 1008 –1011. sleep and wakefulness. In J. Field, H. W. Magoun,
Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, & V. E. Hall (Eds.), Handbook of physiology (Sect.
G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional 1, Vol. 3, pp. 1553–1593). Washington, DC:
brain mapping of the relaxation response and med- American Physiological Society.
itation. NeuroReport, 11, 1581–1585. Linschoten, J. (1955). Über das Einschlafen [On fall-
Leaning, F. E. (1925). An introductory study of hyp- ing asleep]. Psychologische Beiträge, 2, 70 –97,
nagogic phenomena. Proceedings of the Society 266 –298.
Llinas, R. R. (2001). I of the vortex. Cambridge, MA:
for Psychical Research, 35, 289 – 409.
MIT Press.
Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jean-
Llinas, R. R., & Pare, D. (1991). Commentary on
monod, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., & Pizzagalli, D.
dreaming and wakefulness. Neuroscience, 44,
(2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency
521–535.
during volitionally meditation-induced, altered
Llinas, R. R., Ribary, U., Joliot, M., & Wang, X. J.
states of consciousness, and experience of the self.
(1994). Content and context in temporal thalamo-
Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 108, 111–
cortical binding. In G. Buzaki, R. Llinas, W.
121. Singer, A. Berthoz, & Y. Christen (Eds.), Tempo-
Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Galderisi, S., Herrmann, ral coding in the brain (pp. 251–271). Berlin,
W. M., Kinoshita, T., Koukkou, M., et al. (in Germany: Springer-Verlag.
press). EEG microstate duration and syntax in Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T.,
acute, medication-naı̈ve, first-episode schizophre- & Oeltermann, A. (2001, July 12). A neurophysi-
nia: A multi-center study. Psychiatry Research: ological investigation of the basis of the fMRI
Neuroimaging. signal. Nature, 412, 150 –157.
Lehmann, D., Faber, P., Isotani, T., & Wohlgemuth, Lowenstein, O., & Loewenfeld, I. E. (1964). The
P. (2001). Source locations of EEG frequency sleep–waking cycle and pupillary activity. In E. B.
bands during hypnotic arm levitation: A pilot Hague (Ed.), Annals of the New York Academy of
study. Contemporary Hypnosis, 18, 120 –127. [Er- Sciences: Vol. 117. Photo-neuro-endocrine effects
ratum in Contemporary Hypnosis, 18, 220.] in circadian systems, with particular reference to
Lehmann, D., Grass, P., & Meier, B. (1995). Spon- the eye (pp. 142–156). New York: New York
taneous conscious covert cognition states and brain Academy of Sciences.
electric spectral states in canonical correlations. Ludwig, A. M. (1966). Altered states of conscious-
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, ness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 15, 225–
41–52. 234.
Lehmann, D., Ozaki, H., & Pal, I. (1987). EEG alpha Ludwig, A. M. (1972). “Psychedelic” effects pro-
map series: Brain micro-states by space-oriented duced by sensory overload. American Journal of
adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalography Psychiatry, 128, 1294 –1297.
and Clinical Neurophysiology, 67, 271–288. Ludwig, A. M., & Stark, L. H. (1973). Schizophrenia,
Lehmann, D., Strik, W. K., Henggeler, B., Koenig, sensory deprivation, and sensory overload. Journal
T., & Koukkou, M. (1998). Brain electric micro- of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157, 210 –216.
states and momentary conscious mind states as Maddox, R. W., & Long, M. A. (1999). Eating dis-
building blocks of spontaneous thinking: I. Visual orders: Current concepts. Journal of the American
42 VAITL ET AL.

Pharmacological Association Washington, 39, (Ed.), Carbon dioxide therapy (pp. 23– 40).
378 –387. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Mahesh Yogi, M. (1966). The science of being and Merrin, E. L., Meek, P., Floyd, T. C., & Callaway, E.,
the art of living. London: International SRM. III. (1990). Topographic segmentation of waking
Makeig, S., & Inlow, M. (1993). Lapses in alertness: EEG in medication-free schizophrenic patients. In-
Coherence of fluctuations in performance and EEG ternational Journal of Psychophysiology, 9, 231–
spectrum. Electroencephalography and Clinical 236.
Neurophysiology, 86, 23–35. Messner, R. (2001). Die Freiheit, aufzubrechen,
Mandell, A. J. (1980). Toward a psychobiology of wohin ich will: Ein Bergsteigerleben. Munich,
transcendence: God in the brain. In J. M. Davidson Germany: Piper.
& R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The psychobiology of Mesulam, M. M. (Ed.). (2000). Principles of behav-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

consciousness (pp. 379 – 464). New York: Plenum ioral and cognitive neurology. Oxford, England:
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Press. Oxford University Press.


Manzey, D., & Lorenz, B. (1999). Human perfor- Miller, M. E., & Bowers, K. S. (1993). Hypnotic
mance during spaceflight. Human Performance in analgesia: Dissociated experience or dissociated
Extreme Environments, 4(1), 8 –13. control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102,
Massion, J., Fabre, J. C., Mouchnino, L., & Obadia, 29 –38.
A. (1995). Body orientation and regulation of the Monroe, L. J., Rechtschaffen, A., Foulkes, D., &
center of gravity during movement under water. Jensen, J. (1965). Discriminability of REM and
Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium and NREM reports. Journal of Personality and Social
Orientation, 5, 211–221. Psychology, 2, 456 – 460.
Matejcek, M. (1982). Vigilance and the EEG: Psy- Morse, M. L., Venecia, D., & Milstein, J. (1989).
chological, physiological and pharmacological as- Near-death experiences: A neurophysiologic ex-
pects. In W. M. Hermann (Ed.), EEG in drug planatory model. Journal of Near-Death Studies,
research (pp. 405–508). Stuttgart, Germany: 8, 45–53.
Mosovich, A., & Tallaferro, A. (1954). Studies on
Gustav Fischer.
EEG and sex function orgasm. Diseases of the
Matousek, M., & Petersén, I. (1979). Automatic mea-
Nervous System, 15, 218 –220.
surement of the vigilance level and its possible
Müller, J. (1967). Ueber die phantastischen Gesi-
application in psychopharmacology. Pharmako-
chtserscheinungen: Eine physiologische Untersu-
psychiatrie, Neuropsychopharmakologie, 12, 148 –
chung [On fantastic visual phenomena: A physio-
154.
logical investigation]. Munich, Germany: W.
Matousek, M., & Petersén, I. (1983). A method for
Fritsch. (Original work published 1826)
assessment of alertness fluctuations from EEG Murphy, M., & Donovan, S. (1997). The physical
spectra. Electroencephalography and Clinical and psychological effects of meditation: A review
Neurophysiology, 55, 108 –113. of contemporary research with a comprehensive
Maurer, R. L. S., Kumar, V. K., Woodside, L., & bibliography 1931–1996. Sausalito, CA: Institute
Pekala, R. J. (1997). Phenomenological experience of Noetic Sciences.
in response to monotonous drumming and hypno- Murri, L., Arena, R., Siciliano, G., Mazzotta, R., &
tizability. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, Muratorio, A. (1984). Dream recall in patients with
40(2), 130 –145. focal cerebral lesions. Archives of Neurology, 41,
Maury, A. (1848). Hypnagogic hallucinations (or 183–185.
sensory errors in the intermediate state between Mutschler, V., Chaumeil, C. G., Marcoux, L., Wio-
waking and sleeping). Annales Médico-Psy- land, N., Tempe, J. D., & Kurtz, D. (1996). Etude
chologiques du Système Nerveux, 11, 26 – 40. du P300 auditif chez des sujets en coma post-
Mavromatis, A. (1987). Hypnagogia: The unique anoxique: Données preliminaires [Auditory P300
state of consciousness between wakefulness and in subjects in a post-anoxic coma: Preliminary
sleep. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. data]. Neurophysiologie Clinique, 26(3), 158 –163.
Maxfield, M. C. (1990). Effects of rhythmic drum- Myers, F. W. H. (1904). Human personality and its
ming on EEG and subjective experiences. Unpub- survival of bodily death (Vol. 1). New York: Long-
lished doctoral dissertation, Institute of Transper- mans, Green.
sonal Psychology, Menlo Park, CA. Naifeh, K. H., Kamiya, J., & Sweet, D. M. (1982).
McGuire, B. E., Basten, C., Ryan, C., & Gallagher, J. Biofeedback of alveolar carbon dioxide tension
(2000). Intensive care unit syndrome: A dangerous and levels of arousal. Biofeedback and Self-
misnomer. Archives of Internal Medicine, 160, Regulation, 7, 283–299.
906 –909. Narita, T., Morozumi, S., & Yagi, T. (1987). Psycho-
Meduna, L. J. (1950). The effect of carbon dioxide physiological analysis during autogenic training.
upon the functions of the brain. In L. J. Meduna Advances in Biological Psychiatry, 16, 72– 89.
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 43

Nasrallah, H. A. (1985). The unintegrated right cere- Parrott, A. C. (2001). Human psychopharmacology
bral hemispheric consciousness as alien intruder: of Ecstasy (MDMA): A review of 15 years of
A possible mechanism for Schneiderian delusions empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology:
in schizophrenia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 26, Clinical and Experimental, 16, 557–577.
273–282. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann,
Neher, A. (1961). Auditory driving observed with D. (1995). Segmentation of brain electrical activity
scalp electrodes in normal subjects. Electroen- into microstates: Model estimation and validation.
cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 13, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering,
449 – 451. 42, 658 – 665.
Neher, A. (1962). A physiological explanation of Paulson, O. B., & Sharbrough, F. W. (1974). Physi-
unusual behavior in ceremonies involving drums. ological and pathophysiological relationship be-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Human Biology, 34, 151–160. tween the EEG and the cerebral blood flow. Acta
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Newberg, A. B., & d’Aquili, E. G. (2000). The neu- Neurologica Scandinavica, 50, 194 –220.
ropsychology of religious and spiritual experience. Pekala, R. J. (1991). Quantifying consciousness: An
Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(11), 251–266. empirical approach. New York: Plenum Press.
Newell, A. (1992). Precis of unified theories of cog- Peper, E. (1971). Feedback regulation of the alpha
nition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 425– electroencephalogram activity through control of
492. the internal and external parameters. In T. X. Bar-
Niedermeyer, E. (1999). A concept of consciousness. ber, L. V. DiCara, J. Kamiya, N. E. Miller, D.
Italian Journal of Neurological Science, 20, 7–15. Shapiro, & J. Stoyva (Eds.), Biofeedback and self-
Niedermeyer, E., & Lopes da Silva, F. (1999). Elec- control (pp. 314 –332). Chicago: Aldine Atherton.
troencephalography (4th ed.). Baltimore: Williams Persinger, M. A. (1983). Religious and mystical ex-
& Wilkins. periences as artifacts of temporal lobe function: A
Ohayon, M. M., Priest, R. G., Caulet, M., & Guil- general hypothesis. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
leminault, C. (1996). Hypnagogic and hypnopom- 57, 1255–1262.
pic hallucinations: Pathological phenomena? Brit- Persinger, M. A. (1999). Near-death experience and
ish Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 459 – 465. ecstasy: A product of the organization of the hu-
Oke, A. F., & Adams, R. N. (1987). Elevated tha- man brain? In S. Della Sala (Ed.), Mind myths:
lamic dopamine: Possible link to sensory dysfunc- Exploring popular assumptions about the mind
tions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, and brain (pp. 85–99). Chichester, England: Wi-
589 – 603. ley.
Oswald, I. (1962). Sleeping and waking: Physiology Pessah, M. A., & Roffwarg, H. P. (1972, November
and psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier. 17). Spontaneous middle ear muscle activity in
Ott, U., Sammer, G., & Vaitl, D. (2002). Baroreflex man: A rapid eye movement sleep phenomenon.
responsiveness correlates with the trait of absorp- Science, 178, 773–776.
tion. International Journal of Psychophysiology, Peterson, C. B., & Mitchell, J. E. (1999). Psychoso-
45(1–2), 16 –17. cial and pharmacological treatment of eating dis-
Palinkas, L. A. (1992). Going to extremes: The cul- orders: A review of research findings. Journal of
tural context of stress, illness and coping in Ant- Clinical Psychology, 55, 685– 697.
arctica. Social Science & Medicine, 35, 651– 664. Pézard, L., Nandrino, J. L., Renault, B., Massioui,
Palinkas, L. A., Gunderson, E. K. E., Johnson, J. C., F. E., Allilaire, F., Muller, J., et al. (1996). De-
& Holland, A. W. (2000). Behavior and perfor- pression as a dynamical disease. Biological Psy-
mance on long-duration spaceflights: Evidence chiatry, 39, 991–999.
from analogue environments. Aviation, Space and Pilon, M., & Sullivan, S. J. (1996). Motor profile of
Environmental Medicine, 71(Suppl. 1), A29 –A36. patients in minimally responsive and persistent
Palus, M., Dvorak, I., & David, I. (1990). Remarks vegetative states. Brain Injury, 10, 421– 437.
on spatial and temporal dynamics of EEG. In I. Plotkin, W. B. (1976). On the self-regulation of the
Dvorak & A. V. Holden (Eds.), Mathematical ap- occipital alpha-rhythm: Control strategies, states
proaches to brain functioning diagnostics (pp. of consciousness, and the role of physiological
369 –385). Manchester, England: Manchester Uni- feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
versity Press. General, 195, 66 –99.
Panayiotopoulos, C. P. (2001). Treatment of typical Pope, K. S., & Singer, J. L. (1978). The stream of
absence seizures and related epileptic syndromes. consciousness. London: Plenum Press.
Paediatric Drugs, 3, 379 – 403. Preissl, H., Flor, H., Lutzenberger, W., Duffner, F.,
Parker, A. (1975). Some findings relevant to the Freudenstein, D., Grote, E., & Birbaumer, N.
change in state hypothesis. In J. D. Morris, W. G. (2001). Early activation of the primary cortex
Roll, & R. L. Morris (Eds.), Research in parapsy- without conscious awareness. Neuroscience Let-
chology (pp. 40 – 42). Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow. ters, 308, 193–196.
44 VAITL ET AL.

Pritchard, W. S. (1997). A tutorial review of the use Röschke, J., & Aldenhoff, J. (1991). The dimension-
and benefits of non-linear dynamical analysis of ality of human’s electroencephalogram during
the EEG. In F. Angeleri, S. Butler, S. Giaquinto, & sleep. Biological Cybernetics, 64, 307–313.
J. Majkowski (Eds.), Analysis of the electrical Rouget, G. (1980). La musique et la trance [Music
activity of the brain (pp. 3– 44). New York: Wiley. and trance]. Paris: Gallimard.
Purcell, S., Moffitt, A., & Hoffmann, R. (1993). Rudolf, J. (2000). Beitrag der Positronen-Emissions-
Waking, dreaming, and self-regulation. In A. Mof- tomographie zur diagnostischen Zuordnung und
fitt, M. Kramer, & R. Hoffmann (Eds.), The func- prognostischen Einschätzung postanoxischer Hirn-
tions of dreaming (pp. 197–260). Albany, NY: schäden [Relevance of positron emission tomog-
SUNY Press. raphy as to diagnosis and prognosis of postanoxic
Raab, J., & Gruzelier, J. (1994). A controlled inves- cerebral dysfunctions]. Fortschritte der Neurolo-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tigation of right hemispheric processing enhance- gie und Psychiatrie, 68, 344 –351.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ment after restricted environmental stimulation Saavedra-Aguilar, J. C., & Gomez-Jeria, J. S. (1989).
(REST) with floatation. Psychological Medicine, A neurobiological model for near-death experi-
24, 457– 462. ences. Journal of Near-Death Studies, 7, 205–222.
Rainville, P., Carrier, B., Hofbauer, R. K., Bushnell, Saito, N., Kuginuki, T., Yagyu, T., Kinoshita, T.,
M. C., & Duncan, G. H. (1999). Dissociation of Koenig, T., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., et al. (1998).
sensory and affective dimensions of pain using Global, regional and local measures of complexity
hypnotic modulation. Pain, 82, 159 –171. of multichannel EEG in acute, neuroleptic-naive,
Rau, H., Pauli, P., Brody, S., Elbert, T., & Birbaumer, first-break schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry,
N. (1993). Baroreceptor stimulation alters cortical 43, 794 – 802.
activity. Psychophysiology, 30, 322–325. Saugstad, L. F. (1994). The maturational theory of
Rauch, S. L., Shin, L. M., Dougherty, D. D., Alpert, brain development and cerebral excitability in the
N. M., Orr, S. P., Lasko, M., et al. (1999). Neural multifactorially inherited manic– depressive psy-
activation during sexual and competitive arousal in chosis and schizophrenia. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, 18, 189 –203.
healthy men. Psychiatry Research, 91, 1–10.
Saver, J. L., & Rabin, J. (1997). The neural substrates
Ray, W. J. (1997). EEG concomitants of hypnotic
of religious experience. Journal of Neuropsychia-
susceptibility. International Journal of Clinical
try and Clinical Neuroscience, 9, 498 –510.
and Experimental Hypnosis, 45, 301–313.
Schacter, D. L. (1976). The hypnagogic state: A
Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of
critical review of the literature. Psychological Bul-
standardized terminology, techniques and scoring
letin, 83, 452– 481.
system for sleep stages of human subjects. Wash-
Schroeter-Kunhardt, M. (1993). A review of near
ington, DC: Public Health Service, U.S. Govern- death experiences. Journal of Scientific Explora-
ment Printing Office. tion, 7, 219 –239.
Redouté, J., Stoléru, S., Grégoire, M.-C., Costes, N., Schulman, C. A., Richlin, M., & Weinstein, S.
Cinotti, L., Lavenne, F., et al. (2000). Brain pro- (1967). Hallucinations and disturbances of affect,
cessing of visual stimuli in human males. Human cognition, and physical state as a function of sen-
Brain Mapping, 11, 162–177. sory deprivation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 25,
Ritz, T., & Dahme, B. (1995). Die Absorption-Skala: 1001–1024.
Konzeptuelle Aspekte, psychometrische Kennw- Schulz, P., & Kaspar, C. H. (1994). Neuroendocrine
erte und Dimensionalität einer deutschsprachigen and psychological effects of restricted environ-
Adaptation [The Absorption Scale: Conceptual as- mental stimulation technique in a flotation tank.
pects, psychometric properties, and dimensionality Biological Psychology, 37, 161–175.
of a German adaptation]. Diagnostica, 41, 53– 61. Schwab, R. J. (1994). Disturbances of sleep in the
Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Birbaumer, N., Wolf, P., intensive care unit. Critical Care Clinics, 10, 681–
Düchting-Röth, A., Reker, M., et al. (1993). Cor- 694.
tical self-regulation in patients with epilepsies. Ep- Schwartz, M. S. (1995). Breathing therapies. In M. S.
ilepsy Research, 14, 63–72. Schwartz & Associates (Eds.), Biofeedback: A
Rockstroh, B., Elbert, T., Canavan, A., & Birbaumer, practitioner’s guide (2nd ed., pp. 248 –287). New
N. (1989). Slow brain potentials and behavior. York: Guilford Press.
Baltimore: Urban. Shapiro, S., & Lehrer, P. M. (1980). Psychophysio-
Roffwarg, H. P., Dement, W. C., Muzio, J. N., & logical effects of autogenic training and progres-
Fisher, C. (1962). Dream imagery: Relationship to sive relaxation. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation,
rapid eye movements of sleep. Archives of General 5, 249 –255.
Psychiatry, 1, 235–258. Sheehan, P. W. (1992). The phenomenology of hyp-
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford, nosis and the experiential analysis technique. In E.
England: Oxford University Press. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hyp-
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 45

nosis research (pp. 364 –389). New York: Guilford Strauss, D. J., Ahwal, S., Day, S. M., & Shavelle,
Press. R. M. (2000). Life expectancy of children in veg-
Sheehan, P. W., & McConkey, K. M. (1982). Hyp- etative and minimally conscious states. Pediatrics
nosis and experience: The exploration of phenom- and Neurology, 23, 312–319.
ena and process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Strelets, V., Faber, P. L., Golikova, J., Novototsky-
Sherwood, S. (2002). Relationship between the hyp- Vlasov, V., Koenig, T., Guinotti, L. R. R., et al.
nagogic/hypnopompic states and reports of anom- (2003). Chronic schizophrenics with positive
alous experiences. Journal of Parapsychology, 66, symptomatology have shortened EEG microstate
127–150. durations. Clinical Neurophysiology, 14, 2043–
Shimizu, A., & Inoue, T. (1986). Dreamed speech 2051.
and speech muscle activity. Psychophysiology, 23, Strik, W. K., & Lehmann, D. (1993). Data-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

210 –214. determined window size and space-oriented seg-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Silberer, H. (1909). Bericht über eine Methodik, mentation of spontaneous EEG map series. Elec-
gewisse symbolische Halluzinations-Erscheinun- troencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiol-
gen hervorzurufen und zu beobachten [Report on a ogy, 87, 169 –174.
method to induce and observe certain symbolic Suedfeld, P. (Ed.). (1980). Restricted environmental
hallucinatory phenomena]. Jahrbuch für Psycho- stimulation: Research and clinical applications.
analytische und Psychopathologische Forschung, New York: Wiley.
1, 513–525. Suedfeld, P., & Eich, E. (1995). Autobiographical
Smith, J. C., Amutio, S., Anderson, J. P., & Aria, memory and affect under conditions of reduced
L. A. (1996). Relaxation: Mapping an uncharted environmental stimulation. Journal of Environ-
world. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 21, 63– mental Psychology, 15, 321–326.
90. Suedfeld, P., Metcalfe, J., & Bluck, S. (1987). En-
Snyder, S. H. (1996). Drugs and the brain. New hancement of scientific creativity by flotation
York: Scientific American Books. REST (restricted environmental stimulation tech-
Solms, M. (1997). The neuropsychology of dreams. A nique). Journal of Environmental Psychology, 7,
clinico-anatomical study. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 219 –231.
Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A social- Suedfeld, P., Ramirez, C. E., Remick, R. A., & Flem-
psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, ing, J. A. (1989). Reduction of post-ECT memory
and “trance logic.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, complaints through brief, partial restricted envi-
9, 449 –502. ronmental stimulation (REST). Progress in Neuro-
Stampi, C., Stone, P., & Michimori, A. (1993). The Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,
alpha attenuation test: A new quantitative method 13, 693–700.
for assessing sleepiness and its relationship to the Sugimoto, S., Kida, M., Teranishi, T., & Yamamoto,
MSLT. Sleep Research, 22, 115. A. (1968). Time estimation during prolonged sen-
Stancak, A., Hönig, J., Wackermann, J., Lepicovska, sory deprivation. Japanese Journal of Aerospace
V., & Dostalek, C. (1991). Effects of unilateral Medicine and Psychology, 6(1), 1–5.
nostril breathing on respiration, heart rhythm and Szelenberger, W., Wackermann, J., Skalski, M., Ni-
brain electrical activity. Neurosciences, 17, 409 – emcewicz, S., & Drojewski, J. (1996). Analysis of
417. complexity of EEG during sleep. Acta Neurobio-
Steinhart, M. J. (1978). Treatment of delirium: A logiae Experimentalis, 56, 165–169.
reappraisal. International Journal of Psychiatry in Tart, C. T. (1975). States of consciousness. New
Medicine, 9(2), 191–197. York: Dutton.
Sterman, M. B. (1977). Effects of sensomotor EEG Tart, C. T. (1980). A systems approach to altered
feedback training on sleep and clinical manifesta- states of consciousness. In J. M. Davidson & R. J.
tions of epilepsy. In J. Beatty & H. Legewie (Eds.), Davidson (Eds.), The psychobiology of conscious-
Biofeedback and behavior (pp. 167–200). New ness (pp. 243–270). New York: Plenum Press.
York: Plenum Press. Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to
Sterman, M. B. (1996). Epilepsy and its treatment absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorp-
with EEG biofeedback. Annals of Behavioral Med- tion”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility.
icine, 8, 21–25. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268 –277.
Sterman, M. B., & Shouse, M. N. (1980). Quantita- Terekhin, P. I. (1996). The role of hypnocapnia in
tive analysis of training, sleep EEG and clinical inducing altered states of consciousness. Human
response to EEG operant conditioning in epilep- Physiology, 22, 730 –735.
tics. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuro- Terzian, H., & Dalle Ore, G. (1955). Syndrome of
physiology, 49, 558 –576. Klüver and Bucy reproduced in man by bilateral
Strauch, I., & Meier, B. (1996). In search of dreams. removal of the temporal lobes. Neurology, 5, 373–
Albany, NY: SUNY Press. 380.
46 VAITL ET AL.

Tetzlaff, K., Friege, L., Hutzelmann, A., Reuter, M., The psychophysiology of circulation– brain inter-
Hoell, D., & Leplow, B. (1999). Magnetic reso- action (pp. 89 –104). Frankfurt, Germany: Lang.
nance signal abnormalities and neuropsychological Vaitl, D., Sammer, G., & Ott, U. (2000). Rhythmic
deficits in elderly compressed-air divers. European tilting controlled by respiration: Effects on heart
Neurology, 42(4), 194 –199. rate, EEG, basic information processing, and con-
Thornberg, S. A., & Saklad, S. R. (1996). A review sciousness. International Journal of Psychophysi-
of NMDA receptors and the phencyclidine model ology, 35(1), 41.
of schizophrenia. Pharmacotherapy, 16, 82–93. Vasquez, E., & Chitwood, W. R. (1975). Postcar-
Tiihonen, J., Kuikka, J., Kupila, J., Vainio, P., Ai- diotomy delirium: An overview. International
raksinen, J., Eronen, M., et al. (1994). Increase in Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 6, 373–383.
cerebral blood flow of right prefrontal cortex in Vernon, D., Egner, T., Cooper, N., Compton, T.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

man during orgasm. Neuroscience Letters, 170, Neilands, C., Sheri, A., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2003).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

241–243. The effect of training distinct neurofeedback pro-


Tisher, P. W., Holzer, J. C., Greenberg, M., Benja- tocols on aspects of cognitive performance. Inter-
min, S., Devinsky, O., & Bear, D. M. (1993). national Journal of Psychophysiology, 47, 75– 85.
Psychiatric presentations of epilepsy. Harvard Re- Vollenweider, F. X., & Geyer, M. A. (2001). A
view of Psychiatry, 1, 218 –219. systems model of altered consciousness: Integrat-
Toga, A. W., & Mazziotta, S. C. (2000). Brain map- ing natural and drug-induced psychosis. Brain Re-
ping. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. search Bulletin, 56, 495–507.
Tommasino, C., Grana, C., Lucignani, G., Torri, G., Vuilleumier, P., Despland, P. A., Assal, G., & Regli,
& Fazio, F. (1995). Regional cerebral metabolism F. (1997). Astral and out-of-body voyages: Heau-
of glucose in comatose and vegetative state pa- toscopy, ecstasis and experimental hallucinations
tients. Journal of Neurosurgery and Anesthesiol- of epileptic origin. Revue Neurologique, 153, 115–
ogy, 7, 109 –116. 119.
Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998, December 4). Wackermann, J. (1996). Beyond mapping: Estimat-
Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, ing complexity of multichannel EEG recordings.
1846 –1851. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 56, 197–208.
Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (2000). Schizophrenia Wackermann, J. (1999). Towards a quantitative char-
and the mechanisms of conscious integration. acterisation of functional states of the brain: From
Brain Research Interactive, 31, 391– 400. the non-linear methodology to the global linear
Travis, F. T. (1993). Respiratory, autonomic, and description. International Journal of Psychophys-
EEG correlates of transcendental consciousness iology, 34, 65– 80.
experiences during transcendental meditation prac- Wackermann, J., Lehmann, D., Michel, C. M., &
tice. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 18(1), Strik, W. K. (1993). Adaptive segmentation of
574, 15. spontaneous EEG map series into spatially defined
Travis, F. T., & Wallace, R. K. (1997). Autonomic microstates. International Journal of Psychophys-
patterns during respiratory suspensions: Possible iology, 14, 269 –283.
markers of transcendental consciousness. Psycho- Wackermann, J., Pütz, P., Büchi, S., Strauch, I., &
physiology, 34, 39 – 46. Lehmann, D. (2000). A comparison of ganzfeld
Tucker, D. M., & Dawson, S. L. (1984). Asymmetric and hypnagogic state in terms of electrophysiolog-
EEG changes as method actors generated emo- ical measures and subjective experiences. Pro-
tions. Biological Psychology, 19, 63–75. ceedings of the 43rd P. A. Convention, 302–315.
Turner, J. W., & Fine, T. H. (1983). Effects of Wackermann, J., Pütz, P., Büchi, S., Strauch, I., &
relaxation associated with brief restricted environ- Lehmann, D. (2002). Brain electrical activity and
mental stimulation therapy (REST) on plasma cor- subjective experience during altered states of con-
tisol, ACTH, and LH. Biofeedback and Self- sciousness: Ganzfeld and hypnagogic states. Inter-
Regulation, 9, 115–126. national Journal of Psychophysiology, 46, 122–
Uleman, J. S., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (1989). Unin- 145.
tended thought. New York: Guilford Press. Wackermann, J., Pütz, P., & Braeunig, M. (2002).
Ulrich, G., & Gschwilm, R. (1988). Vigilanz— EEG correlates of ganzfeld-induced hallucinatory
Ordnungszustand oder ordnende Kraft? [Vigi- experience. International Journal of Psychophysi-
lance—State of order or organizing force?]. ology, 45, 17.
Fortschritte der Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 56, Wackermann, J., Pütz, P., & Miener, M. (2001). EEG
398 – 402. correlates of distorted time experience: Frequency
Vaitl, D., & Gruppe, H. (1995). CNS modulation bands and topography. Brain Topography, 13, 326.
induced by hemodynamic changes. In D. Vaitl & Wade, D. T., & Johnston, C. (1999). The permanent
R. Schandry (Eds.), From the heart to the brain: vegetative state: Practical guidance on diagnosis
PSYCHOBIOLOGY OF ALTERED STATES OF CONCIOUSNESS 47

and management. British Medical Journal, 319, Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Exper-
841– 844. imental psychology. New York: Holt.
Wagstaff, G. F. (1998). The semantics and physiol- Woody, E. Z., & McConkey, K. M. (2003). What we
ogy of hypnosis as an altered state: Towards a don’t know about the brain and hypnosis but need
definition of hypnosis. Contemporary Hypnosis, to: A view from the Buckhorn Inn. International
15, 149 –165. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
Weinberg, W. A., & Harper, C. R. (1993). Vigilance 51, 309 –338.
and its disorders. Behavioral Neurology, 1, 59 –78. Zeitlhofer, J., Steiner, M., Oder, W., Obergotts-
Wertheimer, M. (1938). Laws of organization in per- berger, S., Mayr, N., & Deecke, L. (1991). Prog-
ceptual forms. In W. Ellis (Ed.), A source book of nostische Wertigkeit evozierter Potentiale in der
Gestalt psychology (pp. 71– 88). London: Rout- neurologischen Frührehabilitation bei Patienten im
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published apallischen Syndrom [The prognostic value of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1923) evoked potentials in early neurologic rehabilitation


Whinery, J. E. (1997). Psychophysiologic correlates of patients with the apallic syndrome]. Zeitschrift
of unconsciousness and near-death experiences. für EEG–EMG, 22, 10 –14.
Journal of Near-Death Studies, 15, 231–258. Zeman, A. (2001). Consciousness. Brain, 124, 1263–
Whyte, J., DiPasquale, M. C., & Vaccaro, M. (1999). 1289.
Assessment of command-following in minimally Zubek, J. P. (1969). Sensory deprivation: Fifteen
conscious brain injured patients. Archives of Phys- years of research. New York: Appleton-Century-
ical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 80, 653– 660. Crofts.
Williams, J. D., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2001). Differen- Zubek, J. P., Hughes, G. R., & Shephard, J. M.
tiation of hypnosis and relaxation by analysis of (1971). A comparison of the effects of prolonged
narrow band theta and alpha frequencies. Interna- sensory deprivation and perceptual deprivation.
tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp- Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 3, 282–
nosis, 49, 185–206. 290.
Wiseman, R., & Milton, J. (1999). Does psi exist? Zuckerman, M., Persky, H., & Link, K. E. (1969).
Lack of replication of an anomalous process of The influence of set and diurnal factors on auto-
information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 125, nomic responses to sensory deprivation. Psycho-
387–391. physiology, 5, 612– 624.
Wood, J. A., Lugg, D. J., Hysong, S. J., & Harm,
D. L. (1999). Psychological changes in hundred- Received April 1, 2003
day remote Antarctic field groups. Environment Revision received June 28, 2004
and Behavior, 31, 299 –337. Accepted August 16, 2004 䡲

You might also like