You are on page 1of 16

Child Development, January/February 2012, Volume 83, Number 1, Pages 104–119

When Friends Disappoint: Boys’ and Girls’ Responses to Transgressions of


Friendship Expectations
Julie Paquette MacEvoy Steven R. Asher
Duke University
Boston College

In this study, the prevailing view that girls are pervasively more skilled in their friendships than boys was
challenged by examining whether girls respond more negatively than boys when a friend violates core friend-
ship expectations. Fourth- and fifth-grade children (n = 267) responded to vignettes depicting transgressions
involving a friend’s betrayal, unreliability, or failure to provide support or help. Results indicated that girls
were more troubled by the transgressions, more strongly endorsed various types of negative relationship
interpretations of the friend’s actions, and reported more anger and sadness than did boys. Girls also
endorsed revenge goals and aggressive strategies just as much as boys. These findings lead to a more complex
view of boys’ and girls’ friendship competencies.

Within the growing literature on children’s friend- Kupersmidt, & Griesler, 1990), and their friendships
ships (Bukowski, Newcomb, & Hartup, 1996; New- are not any less stable than those of girls (Benenson
comb & Bagwell, 1995; Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, & Christakos, 2003; also see Rose & Rudolph, 2006).
2006), a general theme has emerged that girls make In this article, we propose that one reason for the
better friends than do boys (see Rose & Rudolph, gender paradox in children’s friendships is that
2006, for a review of gender differences in chil- girls have greater difficulty with a particular
dren’s peer relations). Specifically, research sug- ‘‘friendship task’’ that has received little attention
gests that girls are more competent at conflict in the empirical literature, namely, responding to a
resolution, helping, and intimate disclosure (Buhr- situation in which a friend violates a core friend-
mester & Furman, 1987; Lansford & Parker, 1999; ship expectation. It is possible that girls are no
Parker & Asher, 1993; Rose & Asher, 1999, 2004). more competent than boys at this task and, in fact,
There appears to be a paradox, however, with it is possible that girls may respond even more
regard to gender differences in children’s friend- problematically to this task than do boys. The pre-
ships (see Rose & Asher, 2011). The paradox is that sent study examined whether boys and girls differ
even though boys look worse on a variety of friend- in their responses to situations in which a friend
ship tasks than do girls, boys are not more lonely has violated a core expectation of friendship. That
than girls (Parker & Asher, 1993; Renshaw & is, we examined whether there are gender differ-
Brown, 1993), they report being just as satisfied ences in how children react when a friend disap-
with their friendships as do girls (Furman & Buhr- points them by committing a significant
mester, 1985; Parker & Asher, 1993; Patterson, transgression within the friendship. Specifically, we
were interested in how children interpret the
This article is based on an unpublished doctoral dissertation actions of their friend, their emotional reactions to
submitted to Duke University by the first author. An earlier ver- the situation, the kinds of goals and strategies they
sion of this article was presented at the March 2007 meeting of would pursue, how severe they perceive the trans-
the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, Massa-
chusetts. The research was supported by fellowships awarded to gressions to be, and how much they would be
the first author from the Spencer Foundation and from Duke thinking about the transgressions afterward.
University. We gratefully acknowledge the students and staff at Previous research has consistently demonstrated
the elementary schools in Granville County, NC and Providence,
RI who took part in this research. We also thank Daniel Blalock, that, by middle childhood, children come to have
Julie Buddensick, Keith Chan, Melanie Dirks, Kristina McDon- well-defined expectations of their friends and
ald, Alison Papadakis, and Elizabeth Van Hooser for their help
with data collection, data entry, or consultation on data analysis.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Julie Paquette MacEvoy, Department of Counseling, Develop-  2011 The Authors
mental, and Educational Psychology, Boston College, Campion Child Development  2011 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
Hall 320, 140 Commonwealth Ave., Chestnut Hill, MA 02467. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2012/8301-0009
Electronic mail may be sent to julie.macevoy.1@bc.edu. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01685.x
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 105

expect them to not only be good companions but (Parker et al., 2005) has suggested, higher levels of
also to help them, share confidences, and be trust- friendship jealousy may mean that girls have
worthy and emotionally supportive (Bigelow, 1977; higher expectations than do boys for exclusivity
Bigelow & La Gaipa, 1975; Furman & Bierman, within their friendships. Taken together, these stud-
1984; Zarbatany, Ghesquiere, & Mohr, 1992). How- ies suggest that perceived violations of core friend-
ever, having well-developed expectations for ship expectations may represent an area of
friends means that children open themselves up to particular vulnerability in girls’ friendships.
a certain kind of emotional vulnerability simply by The overarching goal of the current study was to
participating in friendships. Friends are not perfect, compare boys’ and girls’ responses to transgres-
and over time it is likely that even good friends will sions of their friendship expectations. In previous
violate one another’s expectations. For example, a research in which children have been asked about
friend may fail to show up for a play date, betray a their responses to hypothetical vignettes depicting
confidence, neglect to give validation or emotional minor conflicts with friends (e.g., an argument over
support when it is needed, or not provide instru- which game to play next), boys were found to
mental aid. Although research identifies reliability, endorse higher levels of maladaptive goals, such as
loyalty, emotional support, and the provision of trying to get even with their friend, and higher
help as core expectations of both boys and girls, it levels of aggressive strategies (Rose & Asher, 1999).
appears that there are gender differences in how In contrast, girls were found to more strongly
high children ‘‘set the bar’’ for their friends. For endorse relationship-maintenance goals and strate-
example, Clark and Ayers (1993) found that both gies involving accommodation and compromise.
boys and girls expect their friends to be loyal and Importantly, this prior research focused on the
empathic, but girls expect their friends to be even kinds of normative conflicts of interest that can
more loyal and empathic than do boys. It could be arise when two friends have different needs or
that although boys and girls both expect their desires in a particular situation rather than on
friends to be loyal and emotionally supportive, girls transgressions of core friendship expectations. In
may demand or desire more from their friends in the present study, because we assessed how chil-
order to feel that their friends have met their stan- dren respond to the violation of core friendship
dards for loyalty and emotional supportiveness. expectations, we anticipated that our findings
If girls have higher expectations of their friends would not be consistent with previous research that
with regard to certain relationship provisions, they has found boys to be more hostile and aggressive
may respond particularly negatively when one of than girls in their goals and strategies. Specifically,
their expectations has been violated. Support for we hypothesized that girls would endorse mal-
this hypothesis comes from research on how chil- adaptive goals and aggressive strategies just as
dren respond to verbal aggression and to social and strongly, and perhaps even more strongly, than
relational aggression. For example, Whitesell and would boys in response to situations in which a
Harter (1996) found that girls perceive being called friend violates a core expectation of friendship.
mean names by a friend to be more of a violation The methodology employed in this study
of their expectations than do boys and that girls involved asking children to respond to a series of
report more emotional distress in this situation than hypothetical situations depicting violations of vari-
do boys. In another study, Paquette and Under- ous kinds of friendship expectations. In selecting
wood (1999) found that following real-life incidents the specific types of friendship expectation trans-
in which children were the targets of socially gressions to be depicted in the vignettes, we drew
aggressive behaviors such as negative gossip, back- from research on children’s friendship expectations
stabbing, and friendship manipulation, girls (Bigelow, 1977; Bigelow & La Gaipa, 1975; Furman
reported more negative emotion and reported & Bierman, 1984; Kahn & Turiel, 1988; Zarbatany
thinking more about the events than did boys. et al., 1992) and from a framework for conceptualiz-
Additional support for the hypothesis comes from ing the social tasks of friendship (Asher, Parker, &
research on how children respond when a friend Walker, 1996). Based upon this literature, we
spends time with others. Parker and colleagues designed a set of vignettes in which a friend fails to
(Lavallee & Parker, 2009; Parker, Low, Walker, & provide validation or emotional support, fails to
Gamm, 2005) have found that girls report more provide instrumental help, commits an act of
jealousy in their friendships than do boys and betrayal, or is an unreliable partner. Hypothetical
engage in more jealousy-driven surveillance of their vignettes methodology has been extensively used
friendships than do boys. As this research group in previous research (e.g., Dodge, 1980; Erdley &
106 MacEvoy and Asher

Asher, 1996; Hopmeyer & Asher, 1997; Jones, New- causes (Crick & Ladd, 1993; see also Sandstrom &
man, & Bautista, 2005; Murphy & Eisenberg, 1996; Zakriski, 2004). However, when a child experiences
Parker et al., 2001; Rabiner & Gordon, 1992; Rose & a transgression of a friendship expectation, it is
Asher, 1999, 2004; Sandstrom, Cillessen, & Eisen- likely that in addition to trying to figure out whether
hower, 2003; Slaby & Guerra, 1988; Whitesell & the behavior was intentional and who is at fault, the
Harter, 1996). There is evidence that children’s child makes other interpretations of the experience.
responses to hypothetical vignettes are positively Specifically, a friendship transgression may be con-
correlated with their real-life social behavior and strued as having sent a message about how the
predict children’s real-life success in their relation- friend views the child and the relationship (see
ships (e.g., Chung & Asher, 1996; Crick, Grotpeter, Watzlawick, Beavin, & Jackson, 1967, for an early
& Bigbee, 2002; Dodge & Frame, 1982; Rose & discussion of the ‘‘relationship statements’’ implic-
Asher, 1999, 2004; Wichmann, Coplan, & Daniels, itly conveyed by the behavior of social partners).
2004). For example, a transgression could be seen as a sign
The selection of goals and strategies utilized in the that the friend does not care about the friendship,
present study was guided by previous research on does not respect the child, or was trying to push the
children’s goals and strategies in specific social situa- child around. In contrast, though, the transgression
tions (e.g., Chung & Asher, 1996; Delveaux & Dan- could be interpreted as the friend simply being busy
iels, 2000; Erdley & Asher, 1996; Rose & Asher, 1999; and distracted rather than a reflection of the friend’s
Troop-Gordon & Asher, 2005) as well as by pilot view of the child or the relationship.
testing in which children were asked open-ended In the current study, then, we extend previous
questions about their responses to friendship trans- research on children’s interpretations of their social
gressions that had actually happened to them. interactions by examining more closely the content
Although a number of possible goals and strategies of their interpretations. Consistent with our pre-
are plausible responses for children to have, we mise that friendship expectation transgressions are
selected goals and strategies that were most repre- particularly threatening to girls, we hypothesized
sentative based on past research and the pilot testing. that girls would be more likely than boys to inter-
We included goals aimed at trying to get revenge on pret their friends’ behavior as indicative of lack of
the friend, trying to keep the friend from being con- caring, lack of respect, or as a desire to control
trolling, trying to maintain the relationship, trying to them. Our selection of these specific types of nega-
figure out why the transgression occurred, and try- tive interpretations was informed by Watzlawick
ing to keep from getting upset. Strategies included et al.’s (1967) discussion of the meta-level messages
verbally aggressing, terminating the friendship, that are communicated in relationship interactions
denial (i.e., ‘‘forgetting about it or pretending it did as well as by previous work on rejection sensitivity,
not happen’’), and problem solving. perceptions of disrespect, and interpretations of an
In addition to examining boys’ and girls’ goals inequitable balance of power (Downey & Feldman,
and strategies in response to transgressions of their 1996; McDonald & Asher, 2007; Nisbett & Cohen,
friendship expectations, we explored children’s 1996).
interpretations of the transgressions. Human beings We also hypothesized in this study that girls
are motivated to find explanations for their social would report more anger than would boys in
partners’ behavior, especially for incidents that are response to friendship transgressions. Research on
negative and relevant to self (Weiner, 1985). Inter- children’s observed and self-reported anger in
preting the meaning of another person’s behavior response to conflicts with a peer tends to show that
not only allows individuals to try to anticipate how either boys experience more anger than girls or that
that person will treat them in the future (Kelley & there are no gender differences in anger (Hubbard,
Thibaut, 1978), but also shapes how individuals 2001; Murphy & Eisenberg, 2002; Underwood,
treat their social partners (Heider, 1958). To date, Hurley, Johanson, & Mosley, 1999). Importantly,
research on children’s interpretations of their peer though, this research has not assessed children’s
interactions has generally focused on whether chil- anger specifically in the context of friendship, par-
dren perceive the behavior of peers to be intentional ticularly in situations in which a friend has violated
(Dodge, 1980; see also Gifford-Smith & Rabiner, a core friendship expectation. It is possible that
2004; Orobio de Castro, Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & when a friend violates a core expectation of friend-
Monshouwer, 2002) and whether they perceive the ship, girls may experience more anger than do boys.
causes of their own and others’ social failures and There is also a need to assess feelings of sadness
successes to be due to external versus internal as well as anger. Research on children’s sadness in
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 107

response to conflicts with a peer tends to show that would be associated with certain emotional reac-
either girls experience more sadness than boys or tions to transgressions of friendship expectations.
that there are no gender differences in sadness This proposed link is consistent with notions put
(Murphy & Eisenberg, 2002; Underwood et al., forth in the emotion literature that different apprais-
1999). Importantly, when presented with hypotheti- als result in varying emotional responses (see Saar-
cal vignettes involving conflicts with a friend, girls ni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006). We
reported significantly more sadness than did boys anticipated that the more children endorsed nega-
(Whitesell & Harter, 1996). In the present study, we tive interpretations of the transgressions, the more
hypothesized that, compared with boys, transgres- likely they would be to experience anger and sad-
sions of friendship expectations would lead girls to ness. Past research on friendship-related processes
endorse higher levels of sadness as well as higher has shown no gender differences in the linkages
levels of anger. between variables (Parker & Asher, 1993; Rose &
Finally, we included two measures aimed Asher, 1999, 2004). Therefore, we did not expect
directly at learning how bothered boys and girls there to be any differences between boys and girls
would be by the friendship transgressions with with regard to the linkages between any of the inter-
which they were presented. First, we asked chil- pretations and emotions.
dren to rate the severity of the transgressions. Sec- Second, we were interested in how children’s
ond, similar to what has been done in previous emotional reactions connect to the goals they would
work (Paquette & Underwood, 1999), we asked pursue and the response strategies they would
children how much they would be thinking about select. Given previous research on the association
the transgressions 1 week after the episode took between anger and aggression (see Berkowitz, 1993),
place. We anticipated that girls would perceive the we hypothesized that the more angry children
transgressions to be more severe than would boys reported they would be, the more strongly they
and that they would report ruminating more about would endorse relationship-damaging goals and
the transgressions a week later (cf. Nolen-Hoek- strategies. In contrast, consistent with previous
sema, Wisco, & Lyubomirksy, 2008). research by Murphy and Eisenberg (2002) on chil-
In addition to being interested in mean-level dren’s emotional reactions to their peer experiences,
gender differences, we were also interested in we anticipated that sadness would not be associated
whether there would be gender differences in the with relationship-damaging goals and strategies,
associations between children’s interpretations, but instead would be associated with more prosocial
emotions, goals, and strategies. Several researchers goals and strategies. Here, too, based on past
have recently emphasized the importance of exam- research on friendship-related processes (Parker &
ining whether boys and girls differ in the linkages Asher, 1993; Rose & Asher, 1999, 2004), we did not
between variables (e.g., see Rose & Rudolph, 2006; expect there to be differences between boys and girls
Underwood, 2003). These researchers have with regard to these linkages between emotions and
described two ways of exploring for potential gen- goals, or between emotions and strategies.
der differences. The first is the more traditional
method of examining mean-level gender differ-
ences. The second, which has been done less often, Method
involves exploring possible gender differences in
Participants
the associations between variables. Gender differ-
ences in the associations between variables would Participants were fourth- and fifth-grade chil-
suggest differences in the function of certain social dren (approximate age 9- to 11-years-old) recruited
processes for boys versus girls. from 12 classrooms in two public elementary
Two specific linkages were of interest to us in this schools. One of the elementary schools was located
study. First, we were interested in how the interpre- in a rural area of the Southeastern United States
tations children made of the friend’s actions related and the other school was located in a medium-
to the emotions children reported. Scholars have sized city in the Northeast. Children in both schools
suggested ways in which children’s interpretations came from lower-middle-class to middle-class back-
of peer interactions and their emotions might be grounds. Of the 296 children who were invited to
related (see Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000, for a relevant participate, a total of 270 children (91.2%) were
discussion). However, relatively little empirical granted parental permission. Three children moved
work exists on the topic. It seems plausible that chil- during the course of the study resulting in a final
dren’s interpretations of their friend’s behavior sample size of 267 (134 boys and 133 girls). The
108 MacEvoy and Asher

sample was ethnically diverse with 49.3% European transgression. There were four vignettes depicting
American, 26.6% Latino, 21.5% African American, each of four friendship transgressions: (a) betrayal,
and 2.6% Other. Preliminary analyses indicated that (b) failing to provide validation or emotional sup-
there were no differences between children in the port, (c) failing to provide instrumental help, and
two schools on any of the variables of interest. (d) being an unreliable partner. Within each trans-
gression category, an attempt was made to vary the
severity of the transgressions across vignettes.
Procedure
In designing the final set of vignettes, pilot test-
Data collection was conducted in two phases with ing was conducted with 20 children (10 boys and
approximately 1 week between sessions. The first 10 girls). In the pilot testing, participants were first
half of the friendship transgression vignettes was asked to describe an incident in which their feelings
administered during the first session and the second were hurt by a friend and how they responded
half of the vignettes was administered in the second when this happened. They were then presented
session. The vignettes were group administered and with several vignettes depicting friendship trans-
all items were read aloud to the class while students gressions and were asked how they would feel if
followed along. The order in which the vignettes the incidents actually happened to them. Results of
were presented was randomized across classrooms. this pilot testing helped to ensure that the vignettes
In response to each vignette, children were asked to we finally selected were representative of children’s
first rate how they would feel, then to rate their actual experiences of friendship transgressions and
strategies, then their goals, and then their interpreta- that the vignettes elicited at least some negative
tions. Our decision to order the administration of emotion. Pilot testing also indicated that the vign-
the constructs in this manner was influenced by ettes used in the final measure were successful in
Crick and Dodge’s (1994) social information pro- eliciting an adequate level of variability in the
cessing model, which suggests that children inter- degree of negative emotion children reported in
pret a social event, then decide upon a goal to response to the transgressions. See Table 1 for an
pursue, and then select a strategy to enact. Although example of a vignette from each of the four catego-
previous research in which similar vignette-based ries.
constructs have been assessed has shown that the When administering the vignettes for the actual
order in which these constructs are assessed does study, children were instructed to imagine that the
not appear to influence children’s responses (e.g., situation in the vignette really happened to them
Chung & Asher, 1996; Rose & Asher, 1999), we still and that their friend in the story was a same-
wanted to avoid any potential priming effects of gender peer. A practice vignette was included in
interpretations upon goals and of goals upon strate- the measure to ensure that children understood the
gies. Therefore, we assessed these constructs in a questions as well as how to correctly use the rating
manner inconsistent with the order in which they scales.
likely happen according to social information pro- Emotions.. After being read each vignette, chil-
cessing theory. Accordingly, children were always dren were first asked, ‘‘How would you feel if this
asked first about their emotions, then about their really happened to you?’’ They then rated how
strategies, followed by their goals, and then their angry, sad, and ‘‘okay’’ they would feel on three
interpretations. Within each construct, the specific separate 1 (not at all) to 5 (a lot) scales. (It should be
order of the items (e.g., the various goals) was pre- noted that children were also asked to indicate how
sented in a different random order in different class- hurt their feelings would be in response to each
rooms. (Of note, two additional items were vignette. The correlation between hurt feelings and
administered. These items asked children how sadness was very high [.91] and the pattern and
much they would forgive their friend and how bad strength of associations between hurt feelings and
they would feel about themselves following the the other variables of interest was the same as those
transgressions. These results are not presented in for sadness. Therefore, the results for hurt feelings
this article for space reasons but may be obtained are not presented; however, these results can be
from the authors.) obtained from the authors.)
Strategies.. Children’s strategies were assessed
next by presenting the question, ‘‘What would you
Friendship Expectation Transgression Vignettes
say or do in this situation?’’ This question, which
Children were presented with 16 hypothetical has been used in previous research (Chung &
vignettes each depicting a friendship expectation Asher, 1996; Erdley & Asher, 1996; Rose & Asher,
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 109

Table 1
Sample Vignettes From Each of the Four Friendship Expectation Transgression Categories

Transgression category Sample vignette

Betrayal One day you are really upset because you got a bad grade on your social studies
test. You tell your friend how upset you are about your bad grade. You also tell
your friend that you don’t want the other kids in your class to find out how
poorly you did. Later that day, though, you find out that your friend told a bunch
of kids in your class about your bad grade.
Lack of validation or emotional support You’re really upset because your pet is sick. You tell your friend about how sick
your pet is but your friend won’t listen to you talk about your pet and instead
says, ‘‘It’s no big deal—it’s just a pet.’’
Lack of instrumental help It’s lunchtime and you are so hungry that your stomach is growling. You sit down
at the lunch table in the school cafeteria and realize that you forgot your lunch at
home. You don’t have any money with you so you can’t buy any food. Your
friend is sitting next to you and has a really big lunch. So, you ask your friend if
you can have some food but your friend says ‘‘no.’’
Being an unreliable partner You and your friend are working on a school project together. You work really
hard on your part of the project and think you did a really good job. On the day
the project is due, though, you find out that your friend didn’t do the part of the
project that your friend was supposed to do. Because your friend didn’t do part of
the project, you both end up getting a bad grade on the project.

1999, 2004), was followed by four strategy options: friend did this?’’ Four possible interpretations of
(a) verbal aggression (‘‘I would tell my friend that the friend’s behavior were offered: (a) lack of care
he ⁄ she is a jerk’’ or ‘‘I would tell my friend to shut about the friendship (‘‘My friend doesn’t care about
up’’), (b) friendship termination (‘‘I would tell my our friendship’’), (b) lack of respect (‘‘My friend
friend that I don’t want to be friends anymore’’), (c) doesn’t respect or value me’’), (c) control (‘‘My
denial (‘‘I would forget about it or pretend it didn’t friend was trying to push me around’’), and (d)
happen’’), and (d) problem solving (‘‘I would find neutral (‘‘My friend didn’t mean to do anything
a way to work things out with my friend’’). Chil- wrong’’). Children rated the degree to which they
dren responded to each strategy using a 5-point would make each of the interpretations on a 1–5
Likert scale ranging from really disagree to really scale ranging from not at all to a lot.
agree to indicate how much they agreed that they Perceived severity.. After rating each potential
would carry out each strategy. interpretation, children’s perceptions of the severity
Goals.. To assess children’s social goals in of the transgressions was assessed with the item,
response to each vignette, participants were asked, ‘‘How bad of a thing is this for a friend to do?’’
‘‘What would your goal be in this situation?’’ As in Participants were asked to rate this item from 1 (not
previous research (Chung & Asher, 1996; Erdley & bad at all) to 5 (really bad).
Asher, 1996; Rose & Asher, 1999, 2004), children Rumination.. Once all vignettes were presented,
were instructed that a goal is ‘‘something you are an additional item was administered in the second
trying to do or accomplish.’’ Using a 5-point scale session to assess the extent to which children
ranging from really disagree to really agree, children would ruminate about the transgressions in each
rated five goal options: (a) revenge (‘‘I would be category afterward. First, children were explicitly
trying to get back at my friend’’), (b) control (‘‘I reminded of the kinds of vignettes they had read in
would be trying to keep my friend from pushing each transgression category. For example, to
me around’’), (c) friendship maintenance (‘‘I would remind children of the betrayal vignettes, they were
be trying to stay friends’’), (d) reasoning (‘‘I would told, ‘‘In some stories, your friend told one of your
be trying to figure out why this happened’’), and secrets, talked about you behind your back, told
(e) tension reduction (‘‘I would be trying to keep other kids about your bad grade, and told other
from getting upset’’). kids that you got pizza on your clothes.’’ Partici-
Interpretations.. To assess children’s interpreta- pants were then asked to rate how much they
tions of why the transgression occurred, partici- would be thinking about this type of transgression
pants were asked, ‘‘Why would you think your 1 week later if it really happened. Ratings were
110 MacEvoy and Asher

made on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 10 (a lot). This to the transgressions were pooled across the trans-
procedure was repeated for each of the four catego- gression categories for all analyses.
ries of transgressions. We next examined the correlations among chil-
dren’s responses with data pooled across the four
transgression categories (the authors can be con-
tacted for a complete table containing the raw cor-
Results
relations between variables). There were several
First, analyses aimed at data reduction and at the high correlations among the different interpreta-
assessment of the psychometric properties of the tions, among the different goals, and among the
friendship transgression measure are described. different strategies. Therefore, we conducted
Next, we examine whether there were mean-level agglomerative hierarchical cluster analyses sepa-
gender differences in children’s responses to the rately for the four interpretations, five goals, and
friendship transgressions, including their interpre- four strategies. As has been done in previous
tations, emotions, goals, and strategies as well as research (e.g., Rose & Asher, 1999), we conducted
how severe they perceived the transgressions to be cluster analysis rather than factor analysis because
and the extent to which they would ruminate about it can be difficult to obtain stable factors from a
the transgressions. Following this, we present a small number of items (e.g., only four interpreta-
series of hierarchical regression analyses designed tions).
to address our research questions regarding the Cluster analyses suggested a three-cluster solu-
linkages between variables. The first regression tion for the interpretations. One cluster was the
analyses assess whether children’s interpretations Neutral Interpretation cluster, which consisted of
of why the transgressions occurred predict to their the neutral interpretation only. Another cluster was
emotional responses. The second set of regression the Control Interpretation cluster, which consisted
analyses examine whether children’s emotions pre- only of the interpretation that the friend was trying
dict to their goals and strategies. For each regres- to push you around. The third cluster was the
sion analysis, we examine whether the pattern of Devalued Interpretation cluster that included the
linkages differ for boys versus girls. interpretation that the friend does not care about
the friendship and the interpretation that the friend
does not respect you.
Data Reduction and Psychometric Properties
A three-cluster solution was also suggested for
Preliminary analyses were conducted to deter- the goals. One of these clusters was a Revenge
mine whether or not children’s responses to the Goal cluster, which was composed of only the
friendship transgression vignettes should be pooled goal of trying to get even with the friend.
across the four transgression categories. First, Cron- Another cluster was the Control Goal cluster,
bach’s alpha scores were computed to examine the which was composed only of the goal of trying to
internal reliability of the measure. The internal get the friend to stop pushing you around. The
reliability of each interpretation, emotion, goal, and third goals cluster was a Relationship Mainte-
strategy was calculated separately for each trans- nance Goal cluster and included the goals of try-
gression category and then again across the four ing to stay friends, trying to figure out why the
transgression categories. As can be seen in Table 2, transgression occurred, and trying to keep oneself
although the internal reliability scores were gener- from getting upset.
ally adequate when examined within each trans- Lastly, cluster analysis of the strategies sug-
gression category, they were considerably higher gested a three-cluster solution. One cluster was a
when examined across categories. As an additional Problem Solving Strategy cluster that consisted
check of whether we should pool the data across only of the strategy of finding a way to work
the four transgression categories, for each trans- things out with the friend. The second cluster
gression category we separately examined the cor- was a Denial Strategy that consisted only of the
relations between the variables assessed in the strategy of forgetting about the transgression or
friendship transgressions measure (i.e., interpreta- pretending that it did not happen. The third clus-
tions, emotions, goals, strategies, perceived sever- ter was an Aggressive Strategy cluster that
ity, and rumination). The pattern of findings was included the strategies of verbally aggressing and
similar for all four of the transgression categories. terminating the friendship.
Accordingly, for ease of presentation as well as To summarize, based on the cluster analyses,
increased internal reliability, children’s responses each child was given three interpretation scores,
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 111

Table 2
Cronbach’s Alphas for Children’s Interpretations, Emotions, Goals, Strategies, and Perceived Severity

Lack of Lack of All types of


validation or instrumental Unreliable transgressions
Betrayal emotional support help partner combined

Interpretations
Lack of care about friendship .75 .77 .75 .78 .93
Lack of respect .75 .75 .75 .77 .93
Control .72 .75 .73 .76 .92
Neutral .69 .56 .66 .61 .88
Emotions
Angry .69 .63 .64 .61 .88
Sad .80 .74 .80 .78 .94
Okay .66 .56 .58 .60 .86
Goals
Revenge .81 .75 .77 .77 .94
Control .74 .73 .71 .72 .92
Friendship maintenance .84 .77 .75 .80 .94
Reasoning .76 .75 .77 .74 .93
Tension reduction .73 .63 .71 .72 .91
Strategies
Verbal aggression .86 .78 .82 .82 .95
Friendship termination .76 .74 .70 .78 .92
Denial .69 .67 .68 .66 .90
Problem solving .78 .73 .76 .75 .93
Perceived severity .76 .74 .69 .72 .91

three goal scores, and three strategy scores. Each of and .27, respectively). There was no significant gen-
these scores was the mean of the responses to all 16 der difference in children’s endorsement of the
of the hypothetical vignettes for a given cluster Neutral Interpretation.
(e.g., the Aggressive Strategy score was the com- Emotions.. Next, a MANOVA was conducted to
bined mean of a child’s ratings for verbal aggres- learn whether boys and girls differed in their emo-
sion and friendship termination strategies across tional responses to the transgressions. A significant
the 16 vignettes). Cronbach’s alpha scores for the main effect for gender was found, F(3, 266) = 17.05,
final set of variables ranged from .88 to .96, with a p < .0001. Girls endorsed a significantly higher level
mean of .93. of anger than did boys (d = .43). Girls also endorsed
a significantly higher level of sadness than did boys
(d = .86). In contrast, boys reported feeling ‘‘okay’’
Boys’ and Girls’ Responses to Friendship Transgressions
about the transgressions significantly more than
A series of one-way multivariate analyses of did girls (d = .36).
variance (MANOVAs) were performed to test for Goals.. A MANOVA was then performed to
potential gender differences in children’s mean- examine possible gender differences in the types
level endorsement of the interpretations, emotions, of social goals that children would pursue in
goals, and strategies. The results of these response to the friendship transgressions. This
MANOVAs are reported below and F ratios from analysis revealed a significant main effect for gen-
follow-up univariate analyses of variance are der, F(3, 266) = 4.18, p < .01. Girls endorsed the
presented in Table 3. Relationship Maintenance Goal more than did
Interpretations.. First, a MANOVA was conducted boys (d = .31). There was also a nonsignificant
to learn whether boys and girls differed in the trend for girls to endorse the Control Goal more
interpretations they made of the friendship trans- than did boys. There was no significant gender
gressions. A significant main effect for gender was difference in children’s endorsement of the
found, F(3, 266) = 12.44, p < .0001. Girls endorsed Revenge Goal.
the Devalued Interpretation and the Control Inter- Strategies.. Finally, a MANOVA was conducted
pretation significantly more than did boys (d = .61 to learn whether boys and girls differed in the
112 MacEvoy and Asher

Table 3 friendship expectations. To examine this, a series


Mean Responses to Transgressions of Friendship Expectations as a of six separate regression analyses were per-
Function of Gender formed (i.e., a regression analysis predicting from
Girls Boys
each of the three interpretations to each of the
two emotions). In each regression analysis, grade
n = 133 n = 134 and ethnicity were controlled and interactions
between the variable of interest and gender were
M SD M SD F value
examined. Results are presented in Table 4. To
Interpretations avoid a redundant description of gender differ-
Devalued 3.20 1.02 2.57 1.06 24.49*** ences described earlier, we will not discuss sig-
Control 3.00 1.06 2.71 1.09 4.75* nificant effects for gender, although these are
Neutral 2.77 0.96 2.80 0.95 0.04 presented in the table. The Devalued Interpreta-
Emotions tion and the Control Interpretation both posi-
Angry 3.92 0.73 3.56 0.94 11.96** tively predicted anger as well as sadness in
Sad 3.65 0.97 2.73 1.15 50.30*** response to the transgressions. The Neutral Inter-
Okay 1.86 0.68 2.14 0.85 8.69** pretation that the friend did not mean to do any-
Goals
thing wrong negatively predicted anger but was
Revenge 2.93 1.17 2.69 1.24 2.50
not a significant predictor of sadness. Out of the
Control 3.82 0.95 3.60 1.09 3.25
Relationship maintenance 3.63 0.82 3.36 0.94 6.06*
six regression analyses performed, only one sig-
Strategies nificant interaction emerged (for the Control
Aggression 2.59 1.07 2.46 1.12 0.96 Interpretation). This interaction was not probed
Denial 2.64 1.01 2.65 1.04 0.01 because it was the only significant interaction out
Problem solving 3.34 1.17 3.21 1.10 0.93 of 18 possible interactions examined in this study
that was found to be significant, which suggests
p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
that this was likely to be a chance finding.

response strategies they would select when faced


Do Children’s Emotional Reactions to Friendship
with the transgressions. A nonsignificant trend for
Transgressions Predict Their Goals and Strategies?
a main effect for gender was found, F(3,
266) = 2.11, p < .10. However, comparison of the Regression analyses were next performed to
means revealed that there were no significant gen- examine whether children’s emotions predict the
der differences in children’s endorsement of any of goals children indicated that they would pursue
the strategies. and the strategies they selected in response to
Perceived severity and rumination.. Next, two t tests transgressions of friendship expectations. In all, 12
were performed to examine potential gender differ- separate regression analyses were conducted (i.e., a
ences in children’s perceived severity of the trans- regression analysis predicting from each of the two
gressions and in the extent to which they would be emotions to each of the three goals and each of
thinking about the transgressions afterward. Signifi- three strategies). Again, in each regression analysis,
cant gender differences were found in perceived grade and ethnicity were controlled. In addition, in
severity of the transgressions, t(265) = 3.56, p < .001 all analyses in which anger was the predictor vari-
and in rumination, t(265) = 2.51, p < .05. Girls able, sadness was also controlled for. In analyses in
(M = 3.85, SD = 0.63) perceived the transgressions which sadness was the predictor variable, anger
to be significantly more severe than did boys was also controlled for.
(M = 3.54, SD = 0.75, d = .45) and girls (M = 6.26, The results of the regression analyses predicting
SD = 2.62) reported that 1 week later they would be from emotions to goals are presented in Table 5
thinking about the incidents more than did boys (again, to avoid redundancy, effects for gender are
(M = 5.44, SD = 2.68, d = .31). not discussed here but are presented in the table).
As hypothesized, anger positively predicted both
Revenge and Control Goals and negatively predicted
Do Children’s Interpretations Predict Their Emotional
Relationship Maintenance Goals. Also as hypothe-
Reactions to Friendship Transgressions?
sized, sadness negatively predicted Revenge Goals
The next set of analyses was performed to and positively predicted Relationship Maintenance
examine whether children’s interpretations predict Goals. In addition, sadness positively predicted Con-
their emotions in response to transgressions of trol Goals. Importantly, there were no significant
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 113

Table 4
Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Children’s Emotions From Their Interpretations of the Friendship Transgressions

Anger Sadness

b t p Adjusted R2 b t p Adjusted R2

Devalued interpretation
Gender .19 3.20 .02 .44 .39 6.98 .00 .23
Devalued interpretation .71 10.62 .00 .26 3.35 .00
Gender · Devalued interpretation ).17 1.08 .28 .03 .16 .87
Control interpretation
Gender .45 3.15 .00 .35 .42 2.69 .01 .22
Control interpretation .67 9.53 .00 .27 3.57 .00
Gender · Control interpretation ).37 2.41 .02 ).06 .38 .71
Neutral interpretation
Gender .36 2.01 .05 .12 .43 2.46 .02 .16
Neutral interpretation ).23 2.85 .01 .07 .82 .41
Gender · Neutral interpretation ).19 1.01 .32 ).04 .21 .84

Note. Controlling for grade and ethnicity. Boys = 0; Girls = 1.

Table 5
Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Children’s Goals From Their Emotional Reactions to the Friendship Transgressions

Revenge Control Relationship maintenance

b t p Adjusted R2 b t p Adjusted R2 b t p Adjusted R2

Anger
Gender .08 1.23 .22 .30 ).02 .32 .75 .16 .41 1.53 .00 .17
Anger .65 9.22 .00 .33 4.27 .00 ).26 3.41 .00
Gender · Anger ).30 1.14 .26 .06 .22 .82 ).39 1.38 .17
Sadness
Gender ).02 .30 .76 .30 .12 .67 .49 .17 .19 3.04 .00 .16
Sadness ).23 2.96 .00 .21 2.46 .02 .41 4.81 .00
Gender · Sadness .01 .05 .96 ).19 .89 .38 ).01 .03 .97

Note. Controlling for grade and ethnicity. Sadness was also controlled for in the analyses in which anger was the predictor. Anger was
also controlled for in the analyses in which sadness was the predictor. Boys = 0; girls = 1.

interactions with gender for any of the regression children’s friendships. The paradox is that
analyses involving anger or sadness. although girls are often thought of as being bet-
The results of the regression analyses predicting ter at a variety of friendship tasks than boys,
from emotions to strategies are presented in Table 6. tasks such as conflict resolution, intimacy, and
Consistent with expectations, anger positively pre- helping, boys report being just as satisfied with
dicted Aggressive Strategies and negatively pre- their friendships, they are no more lonely than
dicted the Denial and Problem-Solving Strategies. In girls, and their friendships are just as stable as
contrast, and again consistent with expectations, those of girls. In this article, we proposed that
sadness negatively predicted Aggressive Strategies one important factor that may help to explain
and positively predicted Problem Solving. Again, this paradox is that girls have particular diffi-
there were no significant interactions with gender culty coping with transgressions of core friend-
for any of the analyses involving anger or sadness. ship expectations, including expectations for
reliability, loyalty, emotional support, and help-
ing. A psychometrically strong measure of chil-
dren’s reactions to transgressions of their
Discussion
friendship expectations was created in this study.
The overarching purpose of this study was to Using this new measure, we examined potential
shed light on an apparent gender paradox in gender differences in children’s cognitive,
114 MacEvoy and Asher

Table 6
Summary of Regression Analyses Predicting Children’s Strategies From Their Emotional Reactions to the Friendship Transgressions

Aggression Denial Problem solving

b t p Adjusted R2 b t p Adjusted R2 b t p Adjusted R2

Anger
Gender ).26 1.10 .27 .37 .12 .46 .65 .18 .49 1.84 .07 .21
Anger .61 9.14 .00 ).47 6.17 .00 ).34 4.48 .00
Gender · Anger .26 1.04 .30 ).12 .44 .66 ).53 1.91 .06
Sadness
Gender ).02 .14 .89 .37 ).20 1.10 .27 .19 ).08 .45 .65 .18
Sadness ).15 2.08 .04 .13 1.55 .12 .35 4.21 .00
Gender · Sadness .00 .02 .98 .25 1.22 .22 .09 .46 .65

Note. Controlling for grade and ethnicity. Sadness was also controlled for in the analyses in which anger was the predictor. Anger was
also controlled for in the analyses in which sadness was the predictor. Boys = 0; girls = 1.

emotional, and behavioral responses to such Although girls were clearly more bothered by the
transgressions. transgressions than boys, it should be pointed out
We found a number of significant gender differ- that girls still endorsed higher levels of relationship
ences that support our hypothesis that trans- maintenance goals than did boys. It could be that
gressions of core friendship expectations are the priority for girls to have friends leads them to
particularly troubling for girls. First, it is clear that try to maintain their friendships even when they are
girls were much more bothered by the transgres- very angry at their friend and believe that their
sions than were boys in that they perceived the friend does not respect or care about them. This
transgressions to be more severe and they reported could also explain why girls did not endorse aggres-
that they would think more about the transgres- sive strategies significantly more than boys—
sions afterward than did boys. Second, compared perhaps girls are aware that if they act upon their
to boys, girls more strongly endorsed interpre- anger, they risk permanently damaging or losing the
tations that their friend devalued them and was friendship. On the flipside, it is interesting to note
trying to control them. Third, with regard to emo- that boys endorsed revenge goals and aggressive
tional responses, girls reported both more anger strategies just as much as girls did, even though they
and more sadness than did boys. Lastly, girls more did not perceive the transgressions to be as severe,
strongly endorsed the goal of trying to keep their did not interpret the transgressions as negatively,
friend from pushing them around. According to a and did not feel as angry as girls. There are various
number of indices, therefore, girls appear to be possible explanations for this finding. For example,
more sensitive to transgressions of core friendship boys may be more likely to propose aggressive strat-
expectations than are boys. egies because they believe more in the legitimacy
In addition to these significant gender differences, of aggression (Erdley & Asher, 1998; Huesmann &
it is also important to note that girls endorsed Guerra, 1997). Boys may also hold stronger negative
revenge goals and aggressive strategies just as much reciprocity beliefs (Eisenberger, Lynch, Aselage, &
as boys did. Our findings stand in contrast to a large Rohdieck, 2004; McDonald, 2008) in that they think
body of previous research that has shown boys to be that if they have been wronged in some way by a
more angry than girls and to pursue more maladap- friend, they are justified in responding in kind
tive goals and aggressive strategies in their relation- toward their friend. In addition, norms for the dis-
ships (e.g., Delveaux & Daniels, 2000; Murphy & play of anger and aggression (i.e., ‘‘display rules’’;
Eisenberg, 2002; Rose & Asher, 1999; see also Mac- Saarni, 1979) may keep girls from acting upon their
coby, 1998; Rose & Rudolph, 2006). Importantly, thoughts and feelings, but not prevent boys from
these previous findings have emerged from studies doing so. Previous research has shown girls to be
that have generally focused on conflicts involving more likely than boys to employ display rules for
limited resources or on social norm violations while anger (Underwood, Coie, & Herbsman, 1992).
playing a game (Hubbard, 2001; Rose & Asher, 1999; Another important issue needing attention is
Underwood et al., 1999) rather than on transgres- why girls were more bothered by violations of core
sions of core friendship expectations. friendship expectations than were boys. Research is
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 115

needed to better understand why this is the case. they shed new light on the associations between
Consistent with previous research (e.g., Clark & children’s interpretations, emotions, goals, and
Ayers, 1993), we do not think that boys and girls strategies. In the first set of linkages that we exam-
necessarily have different expectations of their ined, we found that children’s interpretations of
friends. Just as girls do, boys likely expect their why the friend committed the transgressions pre-
friends to be reliable and trustworthy and to help dicted to their emotional responses to the transgres-
them when they need help. It may be, though, that sions. The more strongly children endorsed
boys and girls use different criteria for deciding interpretations that the friend devalued them or
whether a friend’s behavior meets expectations. was trying to control them, the more anger and
Consider, for example, a boy who is very worried sadness they felt. That linkages between specific
about whether his parents will divorce. He confides interpretations and emotions are similar for boys
in his friend, who says that he ‘‘guesses things will and girls speaks to the commonality of their core
be okay’’ and to ‘‘hang in there.’’ The boy may feel needs to be valued, respected, and treated as auton-
supported by these brief but meaningful state- omous in relationships (see Asher, MacEvoy, &
ments. In contrast, for a girl to feel supported in the McDonald, 2008; Asher & McDonald, 2009). Fur-
same situation she might expect her friend to thermore, given the finding that negative interpre-
repeatedly check in with her on how things are tations (e.g., lack of caring, lack of respect, etc.)
going and even perhaps to ‘‘drop everything’’ if were strongly associated with anger for both boys
she were to call and want to talk to her friend. In and girls, our study suggests that a key component
this situation, both boys and girls might expect in anger management programs for children (Loch-
their friend to provide emotional support, but boys man, Burch, Curry, & Lampron, 1984) should be to
and girls may simply set the bar for their friends at focus on how children interpret their friend’s
different levels. Consistent with this idea, Benenson behavior when things go awry.
et al. (2009) recently found that college-aged In addition to examining how interpretations are
females reported being more bothered by the associated with children’s emotions, we also sought
behavior of their same-sex roommates than did to learn whether children’s emotions predict to the
males. These authors suggest that, compared to kinds of social goals and strategies they pursue in
males, females may have higher standards for their response to transgressions of their friendship
same-sex social partners. Of note, the Benenson expectations. Results indicated that the more angry
et al. research was conducted with young adults children reported that they would feel, the more
whereas the present study was with children. It strongly they endorsed revenge goals and aggres-
will be important to study possible developmental sive strategies and the less strongly they endorsed
differences in the standards that males and females relationship maintenance goals and problem-
set for their friends and in how they respond when solving strategies. It is interesting that although
a friend disappoints them. children’s negative interpretations of the friendship
Importantly, the present findings also provide transgressions were related to both feelings of
support for the point that gender differences in anger and feelings of sadness, sadness was found
the mean level of variables do not imply gender to function quite differently from anger with regard
differences in the linkages between variables. to children’s goals and strategies in response to the
Although there were mean-level gender differ- transgressions. Whereas anger predicted to goals
ences in children’s perceived severity of the and strategies that would most likely result in the
friendship transgressions and in their interpreta- deterioration of friendships, sadness predicted to
tions, emotions, and goals in response to the goals and strategies that would be more apt to lead
transgressions, we found very little evidence of to the maintenance and even strengthening of
gender differences in the linkages between chil- friendships for both boys and girls. In particular,
dren’s interpretations and emotions, or the link- the more sadness children reported that they would
ages between children’s emotions and their goals feel, the more strongly they endorsed relationship
and strategies. Indeed, of 18 regression analyses, a maintenance goals and the more strongly they
significant gender interaction emerged for only endorsed problem-solving strategies. These find-
one analysis. These findings suggest that chil- ings suggest that sadness can function as part of
dren’s interpretations and emotions function in the ‘‘social glue’’ that helps hold friendships
similar ways for boys and girls. together.
Across gender, however, a number of interesting Our results regarding the associations between
linkages emerged that merit discussion because emotions and goals and strategies are consistent
116 MacEvoy and Asher

with those of Murphy and Eisenberg (2002) who relationship can be fixed, though, there might be
found anger to be associated with unfriendly goals greater motivation to take steps toward reconcilia-
and destructiveness of behavior, and sadness to be tion. Research is needed to examine the role of
associated with friendly goals. However, our chronicity in how children respond to transgres-
results are only partially consistent with theories sions of their friendship expectations.
and findings in the emotion literature on the func- Another important direction for future research
tion of different emotions. Building on Frijda’s will be to examine the causal relationship between
(1986) notion of ‘‘action tendencies,’’ Saarni et al. children’s interpretations, emotions, goals, and
(2006) have described how various emotions result strategies in response to friendship transgressions.
in tendencies to respond in different ways when In this study, we conceptualized interpretations as
obstacles are presented to the attainment of either influencing emotions and emotions as in turn influ-
social or instrumental goals. Whereas anger is said encing goals and strategies. Our conceptualization
to be associated with the tendency to take steps to was based on discussions in the literature of how
eliminate the obstacle, sadness is said to be associ- interpretations and emotions may be related (e.g.,
ated with the tendency to disengage or passively Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000; Saarni et al., 2006), but
withdraw. tests of these associations are needed. For example,
However, in this study sadness was positively the direction of effect between these variables could
related to prosocial goals and strategies rather than be examined using path analyses on large sample
to withdrawal. There are a few potential explana- sizes or by conducting studies in which children’s
tions for this finding. First, the transgressions we responses to friendship transgressions are experi-
presented to children occurred specifically in the mentally manipulated.
context of friendship and it may be that when sad- To summarize, the results of this study have the
ness occurs within friendships versus within rela- potential to significantly qualify our understanding
tionships that do not have a close, shared history, of gender differences in relationships. The general-
children are more likely to want to try to salvage ization that girls are more prosocial and empathic
their relationship than to withdraw. Second, it may in relationships (see Maccoby, 1998; Rose &
be that sadness does lead to withdrawal when Rudolph, 2006) may not apply to situations in
offenses repeatedly occur within a relationship. In which a friend has violated a core expectation of
our study, only two of the 16 transgressions made friendship. Indeed, the pattern of gender differ-
any reference to the transgression being a repeat ences in response to friendship transgressions may
occurrence. Follow-up analyses did not reveal chil- help to explain why it is that even though girls
dren to perceive these two transgressions to be exhibit a large number of strengths in their friend-
more severe than the other transgressions. The ships (e.g., shared intimacy, emotional support),
repeat nature of the offenses was only mentioned girls’ friendships are not found to be more stable
briefly in these two transgressions, though, and than boys’ friendships nor are girls more satisfied
was not emphasized. Chronic transgressions for than boys with their friendships (see Rose & Asher,
which the repetitive nature of the offenses is obvi- 2011). How children respond when a friend disap-
ous might lead children to conclude that trying to points them may be a critical part of the story.
maintain their friendship is not worth the effort
because transgressions will likely occur again. In
this case, sadness may result in disengagement
References
when a person has no hope that the relationship
can be saved (see Richman & Leary, 2009, for a Asher, S. R., MacEvoy, J. P., & McDonald, K. L. (2008).
recently proposed model of the association between Children’s peer relations, social competence and school
emotional reactions to social rejection, cognitive adjustment: A social tasks and social goals perspective.
appraisals of such experiences, and behavioral In M. L. Maehr, S. Karabenick, & T. Urdan (Eds.),
Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 15, pp. 357–
responses). This hypothesis about the potential
390). Amsterdam: Emerald Press.
effects of chronic violations of relationship expecta-
Asher, S. R., & McDonald, K. L. (2009). The behavioral
tions is consistent with research from the adult basis of acceptance, rejection, and perceived popularity.
marital literature in which sadness has been found In K. H. Rubin, W. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), The
to predict the dissolution of marriages (see handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp.
Gottman, 1994). Perhaps in the face of chronic 232–248). New York: Guilford.
difficulties, people lose hope about their relation- Asher, S. R., Parker, J. G., & Walker, D. L. (1996). Distin-
ship and give up. If an individual has hope that the guishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 117

intervention and assessment. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. ences in negative reciprocity norm endorsement.


Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 787–799.
keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 366– Erdley, C. A., & Asher, S. R. (1996). Children’s social
405). New York: Cambridge University Press. goals and self-efficacy perceptions as influences on
Benenson, J. F., & Christakos, A. (2003). The greater their responses to ambiguous provocation. Child Devel-
fragility of females’ versus males’ closest same-sex opment, 67, 1329–1344.
friendships. Child Development, 74, 1123–1129. Erdley, C. A., & Asher, S. R. (1998). Linkages between
Benenson, J. F., Markovitz, H., Fitzgerald, C., Geoffrey, children’s beliefs about the legitimacy of aggression
D., Flemming, J., Kahlenberg, S. M., et al. (2009). Males’ and their behavior. Social Development, 7, 321–339.
greater tolerance of same-sex peers. Psychological Sci- Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge
ence, 20, 184–190. University Press.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, Furman, W., & Bierman, K. (1984). Children’s conceptions
and control. New York: McGraw-Hill. of friendship: A multimethod study of developmental
Bigelow, B. J. (1977). Children’s friendship expectations: changes. Developmental Psychology, 20, 925–931.
A cognitive-developmental study. Child Development, Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s percep-
48, 246–253. tions of the personal relationships in their social net-
Bigelow, B. J., & La Gaipa, J. J. (1975). Children’s written works. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024.
descriptions of friendship: A multidimensional analy- Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Social infor-
sis. Developmental Psychology, 11, 857–858. mation processing and children’s social adjustment. In
Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development K. A. Dodge & J. B. Kupersmidt (Eds.), Children’s peer
of companionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58, relations: From development to intervention (pp. 61–79).
1101–1113. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F., & Hartup, W. W. Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relation-
(1996). The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and ship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hills-
adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press. dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chung, T., & Asher, S. R. (1996). Children’s goals and Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations.
strategies in peer conflict situations. Merrill-Palmer New York: Wiley.
Quarterly, 42, 125–147. Hopmeyer, A., & Asher, S. R. (1997). Children’s
Clark, M. L., & Ayers, M. (1993). Friendship expectations responses to peer conflicts involving a rights infraction.
and friendship evaluations: Reciprocity and gender Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 43, 235–254.
effects. Youth & Society, 24, 299–313. Hubbard, J. A. (2001). Emotion expression processes in
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and refor- children’s peer interactions: The role of peer rejection,
mulation of social information-processing mechanisms aggression, and gender. Child Development, 72, 1426–1438.
in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s
115, 74–101. normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive
Crick, N. R., Grotpeter, J. K., & Bigbee, M. A. (2002). Rela- behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
tionally and physically aggressive children’s intent 408–419.
attributions and feelings of distress for relational and Jones, D. C., Newman, J. B., & Bautista, S. (2005). A three-
instrumental peer provocations. Child Development, 73, factor model of teasing: The influence of friendship,
1134–1142. gender, and topic on expected emotional reactions to
Crick, N. R., & Ladd, G. W. (1993). Children’s perceptions teasing during early adolescence. Social Development,
of their peer experiences: Attributions, loneliness, social 14, 421–439.
anxiety, and social avoidance. Developmental Psychology, Kahn, P. H., & Turiel, E. (1988). Children’s conceptions of
29, 244–254. trust in the context of social expectations. Merrill-Palmer
Delveaux, K. D., & Daniels, T. (2000). Children’s social Quarterly, 34, 403–419.
cognitions: Physically and relationally aggressive strat- Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. (1978). Interpersonal relations:
egies and children’s goals in peer conflict situations. A theory of interdependence. New York: Wiley.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 672–692. Lansford, J. E., & Parker, J. G. (1999). Children’s interac-
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s tions in triads: Behavioral profiles and effects of gender
aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162–170. and patterns of friendships among members. Develop-
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive mental Psychology, 35, 80–93.
biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Develop- Lavallee, K. L., & Parker, J. G. (2009). The role of inflexi-
ment, 53, 620–635. ble friendship beliefs, rumination, and low self-worth
Downey, G., & Feldman, S. I. (1996). Implications of rejec- in early adolescents’ friendship jealousy and adjust-
tion sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Per- ment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 873–885.
sonality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343. Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated
Eisenberger, R., Lynch, P., Aselage, J., & Rohdieck, S. model of emotion processes and cognition in social
(2004). Who takes the most revenge? Individual differ- information processing. Child Development, 71, 107–118.
118 MacEvoy and Asher

Lochman, J. E., Burch, P. R., Curry, J. F., & Lampron, L. Patterson, C. J., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Griesler, P. C. (1990).
B. (1984). Treatment and generalization effects of cogni- Children’s perceptions of self and of relationships with
tive-behavioral and goal-setting interventions with others as a function of sociometric status. Child Develop-
aggressive boys. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy- ment, 61, 1335–1349.
chology, 52, 915–916. Rabiner, D. L., & Gordon, L. V. (1992). The coordination
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, of conflicting social goals: Differences between rejected
coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Har- and nonrejected boys. Child Development, 63, 1344–
vard University Press. 1350.
McDonald, K. L. (2008). Interpretations and beliefs associated Renshaw, P. D., & Brown, P. J. (1993). Loneliness in mid-
with children’s revenge goals in conflicts. Unpublished dle childhood: Concurrent and longitudinal predictors.
doctoral dissertation, Duke University, Durham, NC. Child Development, 64, 1271–1284.
McDonald, K. L., & Asher, S. R. (2007, May). Vengeful Richman, L. S., & Leary, M. R. (2009). Reactions to dis-
reactions to minor conflicts of interest with friends, crimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms
roommates, and romantic partners. In S. R. Asher of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model. Psy-
(Chair), Overreacting to interpersonal events: Why people chological Review, 116, 365–383.
make mountains out of molehills. Symposium conducted Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (1999). Children’s goals and
at the annual meeting of the Association for Psycholog- strategies in response to conflicts within a friendship.
ical Science, Washington, DC. Developmental Psychology, 35, 69–79.
Murphy, B. C., & Eisenberg, N. (1996). Provoked by a Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (2004). Children’s strategies
peer: Children’s anger-related responses and their rela- and goals in response to help-giving and help-seeking
tions to social functioning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, tasks within a friendship. Child Development, 75, 749–
103–124. 763.
Murphy, B. C., & Eisenberg, N. (2002). An integrative Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (2011). Boys’ friendships: What’s
examination of peer conflict: Children’s reported goals, good about being male? Manuscript submitted for publi-
emotions, and behaviors. Social Development, 11, 534– cation.
557. Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex
Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s differences in peer relationship processes: Potential
friendship relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychologi- trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development
cal Bulletin, 117, 306–347. of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131.
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (2006).
psychology of violence in the South. Boulder, CO: West- Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In
view. N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirksy, S. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 571–
(2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psycholog- 645). New York: Wiley.
ical Science, 3, 400–424. Saarni, C. (1979). Children’s understanding of display
Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. rules for expressive behavior. Developmental Psychology,
D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of 15, 424–429.
intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington,
Development, 73, 916–934. D. (2006). Emotional development: Action, communi-
Paquette, J. A., & Underwood, M. K. (1999). Gender cation, and understanding. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
differences in young adolescents’ experiences of peer Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional,
victimization: Social and physical aggression. Merrill- and personality development (pp. 226–299). New York:
Palmer Quarterly, 45, 242–266. Wiley.
Parker, E. H., Hubbard, J. A., Ramsden, S. R., Relyea, Sandstrom, M. J., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Eisenhower, A.
N., Dearing, K. F., Smithmyer, C. M., et al. (2001). (2003). Children’s appraisal of peer rejection experi-
Children’s use and knowledge of display rules for ences: Impact on social and emotional adjustment.
anger following hypothetical vignettes versus follow- Social Development, 12, 530–550.
ing live peer interaction. Social Development, 10, 528– Sandstrom, M. J., & Zakriski, A. L. (2004). Understanding
557. the experience of peer rejection. In J. B. Kupersmidt &
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friend- K. A. Dodge (Eds.), Children’s peer relations: From devel-
ship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer opment to intervention (pp. 101–118). Washington, DC:
group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social American Psychological Association.
dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611–621. Slaby, R. G., & Guerra, N. G. (1988). Cognitive mediators
Parker, J. G., Low, C. M., Walker, A. R., & Gamm, B. K. of aggression in adolescent offenders: I. Assessment.
(2005). Friendship jealousy in young adolescents: Indi- Developmental Psychology, 24, 580–588.
vidual differences and links to sex, aggression, and Troop-Gordon, W., & Asher, S. R. (2005). Modifications in
social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 41, 235– children’s goals when encountering obstacles to conflict
250. resolution. Child Development, 76, 568–582.
Transgressions of Friendship Expectations 119

Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls. Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
New York: Guilford. motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 548–
Underwood, M. K., Coie, J. D., & Herbsman, C. R. (1992). 573.
Display rules for anger and aggression in school-age Whitesell, N. R., & Harter, S. (1996). The interpersonal
children. Child Development, 63, 366–380. context of emotion: Anger with close friends and class-
Underwood, M. K., Hurley, J. C., Johanson, C. A., & mates. Child Development, 67, 1345–1359.
Mosley, J. E. (1999). An experimental, observational Wichmann, C., Coplan, R. J., & Daniels, T. (2004). The
investigation of children’s responses to peer provoca- social cognitions of socially withdrawn children. Social
tion: Developmental and gender differences in middle Development, 13, 377–392.
childhood. Child Development, 70, 1428–1446. Zarbatany, L., Ghesquiere, K., & Mohr, K. (1992). A con-
Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J. H., & Jackson, D. D. (1967). text perspective on early adolescents’ friendship expec-
Pragmatics of human communication: A study of inter- tations. Journal of Early Adolescence, 12, 111–126.
actional patterns, pathologies, and paradoxes. New York:
W.W. Norton.

You might also like