You are on page 1of 7

Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Sleep Medicine
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep

Original Article

Late bedtimes weaken school performance and predispose adolescents


to health hazards
Ilona Merikanto a,b,⇑, Tuuli Lahti a,c, Riikka Puusniekka d, Timo Partonen a
a
Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse Services, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland
b
Department of Biosciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland
c
Department of Behavioural Sciences and Philosophy, University of Turku, Turku, Finland
d
Department of Children, Young People and Families, National Institute for Health and Welfare, Helsinki, Finland

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Study objectives: Our study explored if bedtimes influenced school performance and motivation, as well
Received 28 January 2013 as the odds ratio (OR) for health-related concerns in adolescents.
Received in revised form 11 June 2013 Methods: The School Health Promotion Study was based on an anonymous self-report questionnaire con-
Accepted 15 June 2013
ducted in 90% of the municipalities in Finland. The study was conducted during 2008 and 2010 in South-
Available online 11 August 2013
ern Finland, Eastern Finland, and Lapland, and during 2009 and 2011 in Western Finland, Northern
Finland, and Åland. Several indicators were used to measure school performance and motivation. Acci-
Keywords:
dents and health-related complaints, such as depressive symptoms, sleep quality, neck or shoulder pains,
Accident
Adolescent
lower back pains, stomachaches, anxiety or nervousness, irritation or tantrums, headaches, and tiredness
Depressive or dizziness were analyzed in relation to the usual bedtime. Our study had a relatively large sample size
Eveningness (N = 384,076), consisting of students in the eighth and the ninth grades of secondary schools and the first
Motivation and the second grades of upper secondary and vocational schools (ages 14–20 years) in Finland.
Sleep Results: All of the various indicators used to assess school performance and motivation suggest that the
later the bedtime of adolescents, the lower their school performance and their motivation. Similarly later
bedtimes increase the OR for depressive symptoms and other negative health consequences in adoles-
cents as well as a tendency towards accidents. All of these problems were emphasized in students with
bedtimes of 11:30 PM and later.
Conclusions: Late bedtimes, especially those after 11:30 PM, indicate poor sleep which deteriorates
school performance and motivation and increases the OR for depressive symptoms and other health-
related issues in adolescents.
Ó 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction increase daytime sleepiness and a risk for accidents, but it also
may deteriorate cognitive test performance and academic perfor-
On average adolescents need 8–9 h of sleep per night to feel rested mance. Sleep also has a profound influence on motivation and mood.
[1–3]. There is a large interindividual variation in sleep duration; for For example, depressed individuals have significantly more sleep
example, the mean sleep duration among 14-year-old adolescents problems compared to nondepressed individuals [15,16]. Previous
is 8.6 h (the range 7.2–10.1 h covers all but the top and bottom 2%) studies with small sample sizes have reported that poor sleep is
[4]. Sufficient sleep is important for learning and memory functions associated with weaker academic performance and working mem-
and for proper attention and performance, especially among ory as well as compromised emotional information processing and
adolescents [5–10]. During sleep the synaptic connections which impaired mood in adolescents [7–10,17–22]. In previous studies,
have been active while awake, especially those related to learning late bedtimes have been associated with poorer diet and larger body
experiences, are strengthened while those that are not as frequently mass index regardless of sleep duration and with a higher risk for
used are weakened [11–14]. Thus insufficient sleep does not just childhood depression [23–25].
In our study, we explored if bedtimes influenced adolescents’
school performance and motivation, as well as their tendency to
have accidents, depressive symptoms, poor sleep quality, neck or
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Department of Mental Health and Substance
shoulder pains, lower back pains, stomachaches, anxiety or ner-
Abuse Services, National Institute for Health and Welfare, FI-00271 Helsinki,
Finland. Tel.: +358 295248213.
vousness, irritation or tantrums, headaches, or tiredness or dizzi-
E-mail addresses: ilona.merikanto@helsinki.fi, ilona.merikanto@thl.fi ness. Our hypothesis is that later bedtimes increase health
(I. Merikanto).

1389-9457/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2013.06.009
1106 I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111

hazards and poorer performance in school, as sleep time is more on the way to school requiring medical attention (scale, 1 = none;
restricted in school days compared to those with earlier bedtimes. 2 = once; 3 = two or several times).
In contrast to previous studies, our sample size was relatively large Depressive symptoms were assessed using a modified 11-item
and we used several indicators for the outcome. version of the Beck Depression Inventory [26,27] (response alter-
natives are given in parentheses): (1) ‘‘How is your mood’’ (quite
good, not gloomy or sad, feeling gloomy or sad, constant feeling
2. Methods of gloominess, or so depressed that cannot take it anymore); (2)
‘‘How do you feel about the future?’’ (optimistic, neutral, future
2.1. Participants seems quite depressing, I feel the future has nothing to offer, or
the future seems hopeless and I do not think it will get better);
The School Health Promotion Study consisted of participants in (3) ‘‘How do you consider your life has gone thus far?’’ (I have suc-
eighth and ninth grades of secondary schools and first and second ceeded quite often, I do not think I have failed, I have failed often,
grades of upper secondary and vocational schools (ages 14– my life has been a series of failings, or I have failed completely as a
20 years). The data collection was based on an anonymous self-re- person); (4) ‘‘How satisfied or unsatisfied do you feel about your-
port paper questionnaire conducted in 90% of the municipalities in self?’’ (I am quite pleased with my life, I am not unsatisfied, I do
Finland, and it took place locally in schools under teacher supervi- not enjoy things like before, I do not get satisfaction from any-
sion during 2008 and 2010 in Southern Finland, Eastern Finland, where, or I am completely unsatisfied in everything); (5) ‘‘How
and Lapland and during 2009 and 2011 in Western Finland, North- do you consider yourself?’’ (I am quite good and not worthless, I
ern Finland, and Åland. Data from 384,076 participants (195,458 feel myself worthless quite often, I feel myself worthless almost
girls and 188,618 boys; total participation rate, 97.9%) were used all the time, or I am altogether worthless); (6) ‘‘Do you have feel-
for the analyses in our study. ings of disappointment?’’ (I am pleased with myself, I am not dis-
appointed with myself, I am disappointed with myself, I am
disgusted with myself, or I hate myself); (7) ‘‘Have you had
2.2. Assessment thoughts of hurting yourself?’’ (I have never had suicidal thoughts,
no such thoughts or no desire to hurt myself, I feel it would be bet-
Bedtimes were categorized into four classes based on an answer ter if I were dead, I have clear plans for suicide, or I would commit
to the question, ‘‘What time do you generally go to sleep on school suicide if I had the opportunity); (8) ‘‘How do you feel about meet-
days?’’(10 response options: 1 = approximately 9:00 PM; and ing strangers?’’ (I like talking to people, I have not lost interest in
10 = approximately 1:30 AM or later): those who went to sleep other people, others do not interest me as much as they used to,
around 10:00 PM or earlier (18.0% of boys and 25.2% of girls); those I have totally lost interest in other people, or I have lost interest
who went to sleep around 10:30 PM (20.2% of boys and 25.9% of in other people and do not care about them); (9) ‘‘How do you feel
girls); those who went to sleep around 11:00 PM (23.9% of boys when making decisions?’’ (making different decisions is easy for
and 23.1% of girls); and those who went to sleep around me, I can make decisions the same as I used to, my self-confidence
11:30 PM to 1:30 AM or later (37.9% of boys and 25.8% of girls). has decreased and I try to avoid making decisions, I have great dif-
School performance was assessed with 11 different items, ficulties in making decisions, or I cannot make decisions at all);
including if the student had difficulties following class learning, (10) ‘‘What do you think about your appearance?’’ (I am quite
with teamwork, with homework, in preparing for examinations, pleased with my appearance, my appearance does not bother me,
finding a suitable study method, initiating or finishing self-directed it concerns me that I look unpleasant, I feel I am ugly, or I am sure
tasks, completing writing tasks, completing reading tasks, com- I am ugly and repulsive); and (11) ‘‘How is your appetite?’’ (I have
pleting practical work tasks, and getting along with teachers or no problems with it, it is the same, it is worse than before, it is
friends (scale, 1 = not at all; 2 = quite little; 3 = quite much; much worse than before, or I have no appetite at all). With this
4 = very much). School motivation was assessed with the following questionnaire, the item on sleep quality was separately assessed.
six items: days of school absence during last 30 days due to skip-
ping, days of school absence during last 30 days due to illness, days
of school absence during last 30 days due to other reasons (scale, 2.3. Statistics
1 = not at all; 2 = one day; 3 = 2–3 days; 4 = over 3 days), feeling
of being overwhelmed with school work, feelings that school had Using v2 tests we judged the statistical significance of the dif-
lost meaning, and feelings of inadequacy in school performance ferences in the distribution of sociodemographic, socioeconomic,
(scale, 1 = hardly ever; 2 = a few times a month; 3 = a few days a and health characteristics between those whose general bedtime
week; 4 = almost daily). was at 10:00 PM or earlier and those whose general bedtime was
Health-related issues were assessed with 16 different items, at 11:30 PM or later (Table 1) and across genders (Supplementary
including the students’ own opinions of their health status (scale, Table 1). The v2 tests also were used to judge the statistical signif-
1 = very good; 2 = quite good; 3 = mediocre; 4 = very bad) and icance of the differences in the distribution of school performance
whether or not a student had had neck or shoulder pains, lower and motivation characteristics (Supplementary Table 2) and health
back pains, stomachaches, anxiety or nervousness, irritation or tan- issues (Supplementary Table 3) across genders.
trums, trouble falling asleep or nightly awakenings, headaches, or Complete data for the indicators of school performance and
tiredness or dizziness during the last 6 months (scale, 1 = rarely motivation and health issues for each general bedtime were avail-
or never; 2 = approximately once a month; 3 = approximately once able for 104,208 participants (51,982 girls and 52,226 boys; 27% of
a week; 4 = almost daily). Quality of sleep was assessed with stu- the total participation rate). Binary logistic regression analyses
dents’ own opinions of their sleep (scale, 1 = no problems with were used to estimate the odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidence
sleeping; 2 = sleeping as well as before; 3 = feeling more tired once interval for the indicators of school performance and motivation
having woken up than before; 4 = insomnia is bothersome; (Table 2) and for health issues (Table 4) for each general bedtime.
5 = insomnia or having difficulties with falling asleep or waking Poisson regression analyses were used to estimate the b values for
up too early or in the middle of sleep). Tendency for accidents the quality of sleep, health status, skipping school, and depressive
was assessed with five items, including accidents during the past symptoms (Table 3). All of the analyses were controlled for gender,
term in recess, in gym class, in other classes, in work training, or class year, working status, circadian problems due to Internet use,
I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111 1107

Table 1
Background sociodemographic, socioeconomic, and health-related characteristics across bedtimes.

General bedtime on school nights


10:00 PM or earlier Approximately 10:30 PM Approximately 11:00 PM Approximately 11:30 PM or later
(n = 83,089) (n = 88,858) (n = 90,337) (n = 121,792)
Gender (%)
Boys 40.8 42.9 50.0 58.6⁄⁄⁄⁄
Girls 59.2 57.1 50.0 41.4⁄⁄⁄⁄
Year in class (%)
Eighth grade 37.0 29.8 23.9 19.2⁄⁄⁄⁄
Ninth grade 26.3 27.9 27.4 26.4
First year upper secondary 10.5 14.3 14.9 13.5⁄⁄
school
Second year upper secondary 7.5 11.7 13.6 14.5⁄⁄⁄⁄
school
First year vocational school 11.2 9.5 11.6 14.5⁄⁄
Second year vocational school 7.5 6.8 8.6 11.9⁄⁄⁄⁄
Working status during the past school term (%)
No 78.7 78.6 78.0 76.8⁄
Yes 0.4 0.4 0.4 0.5
1–5 h 12.1 12.8 12.2 11.0⁄⁄⁄⁄
6–10 h 4.9 4.9 5.2 5.7
>10 h 3.8 3.4 4.2 6.0⁄⁄⁄⁄
Circadian rhythm problems due to time spent using the Internet (%)
Yes 6.4 11.4 18.0 33.9⁄⁄⁄⁄
No 93.6 88.6 82.0 66.1⁄⁄⁄⁄
Friends to open up to (%)
No friends 9.5 7.7 7.5 8.7
One friend 25.1 22.3 21.2 22.3⁄⁄
Several friends 65.4 70.0 71.3 69.0
Current smoking status (%)
Daily 10.8 11.8 17.8 30.5⁄⁄⁄⁄
Once/wk or more 3.2 4.1 5.4 6.2⁄⁄
Less than once/ wk 6.7 9.7 11.4 11.2⁄⁄⁄⁄
Quit 13.2 14.8 16.3 15.6⁄⁄
Nonsmoker 66.0 59.7 49.2 36.6⁄⁄⁄⁄
Alcohol consumption (%)
Once/wk or more 5.7 6.7 10.7 21.1⁄⁄⁄⁄
Once or twice/mo 24.4 33.7 40.9 42.3⁄⁄⁄⁄
Less than once or twice/mo 20.3 22.7 21.6 17.6⁄⁄
Never 49.6 36.8 26.8 19.0⁄⁄⁄⁄
Exercising for at least a half an hour (%)
Several times/d 14.0 12.5 11.6 10.7⁄⁄⁄⁄
4–7 times/wk 45.0 46.1 44.4 38.6
1–3 times/wk 33.6 34.9 35.8 36.5⁄⁄⁄⁄
Less 5.9 5.6 6.9 10.4⁄⁄⁄⁄
Not at all 1.4 0.9 1.3 3.8⁄
Eating proper breakfast during the school week (%)
5 mornings 70.6 54.6 56.2 39.2⁄⁄
3–4 mornings 12.4 14.7 16.8 16.7⁄⁄⁄⁄
1–2 mornings 7.3 8.9 11.6 15.1⁄⁄⁄⁄
Less 9.7 10.8 15.4 29.0⁄⁄⁄⁄

Abbreviations: h, hour; wk, week; mo, month; d, day.



P < .05; ⁄⁄P < .01; ⁄⁄⁄P < .001; ⁄⁄⁄⁄P < .0001 as tested between those who went to sleep the earliest and those who went to sleep the latest.

number of friends to open up to, smoking status, alcohol consump- It is of note that students who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later
tion, exercising habits, and breakfast eating habits. more frequently smoke daily, drank alcohol once a week or more,
and did not eat a proper breakfast during the school week, com-
pared with those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier (Ta-
2.4. Ethics
ble 1). The distribution of the key sociodemographic,
socioeconomic, and health characteristics across genders are pre-
The School Health Promotion Study was approved by the ethical
sented in Supplementary Table 1.
committee of Tampere University Hospital. It was conducted
according to accepted international ethical standards in accor-
dance with the Declaration of Helsinki and its amendments.
3.1. Association of bedtimes with school performance

3. Results Generally, students who went to sleep later than at 10:00 PM


had higher OR for the 12 indicators of negative school performance
The distribution of bedtimes by the key sociodemographic, when compared to those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier;
socioeconomic, and health characteristics are presented in Table 1. only two indicators yielded no difference (Table 2). In other words,
1108 I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111

Table 2
Binary logistic regression predicting school performance by bedtime (odds ratio [95% confidence interval]).a

General bedtime during school nights


Approximately Approximately Approximately 11:30 PM or
10:30 PM 11:00 PM later
Trouble following class education (N = 104,208; no = 33,866, yes = 70,342) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)** 1.4 (1.3–1.5)**
Trouble with teamwork performance (N = 104,150; no = 56,461, yes = 47,689) 0.9 (0.9–1.0) 1.0 (0.9–1.0) 1.0 (1.0–1.1)
Trouble completing homework assignments (N = 104,214; no = 33,790, 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.3 (1.3–1.4)** 1.5 (1.4–1.6)**
yes = 70,424)
Trouble preparing for examinations (N = 104 153; no = 24,075, yes = 80,078) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.3 (1.2–1.3)** 1.4 (1.4–1.5)**
Trouble finding a suitable study method (N = 104,164; no = 30,835, yes = 73,329) 1.1 (1.0–1.1)** 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
Trouble initiating or finishing self-directed tasks (N = 104,161; no = 28,529, 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.3 (1.2–1.3)** 1.4 (1.3–1.4)**
yes = 75,632)
Trouble with writing tasks (N = 104,106; no = 35,276, yes = 68,830) 1.1 (1.0–1.1)* 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
Trouble with reading tasks (N = 104,165; no = 37,169, yes = 66,996) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
Trouble with practical work tasks (N = 39,033; no = 20,879, yes = 18,154)b 1.0 (1.0–1.1) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.2 (1.1–1.2)**
Trouble getting along with teachers (N = 104 166; no = 49,978, yes = 54,188) 1.1 (1.1–1.1)** 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.3 (1.2–1.3)**
Trouble getting along with school friends (N = 104,162; no = 70,521, yes = 33,641) 0.9 (0.9–0.9)** 0.9 (0.9–1.0) 1.0 (0.9–1.0)
Feelings of being overwhelmed with schoolwork (N = 104,213; no = 36,803, 1.1 (1.0–1.1)* 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
yes = 67,410)
Feelings that school has lost its meaning (N = 104,156; no = 557,901, yes = 46,255) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.4 (1.4–1.5)**
Feelings of inadequacy in school performance (N = 103,804; no = 57,886, 1.0 (1.0–1.1) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.3 (1.2–1.3)**
yes = 45,918)
a
Adjusted for gender, year in class, working status, circadian problems due to Internet use, number of friends to open up to, smoking status, alcohol consumption,
exercising habits, and breakfast eating habits. Those subjects whose bedtime generally was 10:00 PM or earlier were used as reference category.
b
Assessed only for vocational school students.
*
P < .001.
**
P < .0001.

Table 3
Poisson regression analyses predicting school performance by bedtime (b values [95% confidence interval]).a

General bedtime during school nights


Approximately 10:30 PM Approximately Approximately 11:30 PM or
11:00 PM later
Absence from school during the last 30 days due to illness (n = 102,226) 0.006 (–0.007 to 0.02) 0.03 (0.02–0.05)** 0.1 (0.1–0.1)**
Absence from school during last 30 days due to skipping class (n = 99,936) 0.03 (0.01–0.04) 0.1 (0.09–0.1)** 0.3 (0.3–0.3)**
Absence from school during last 30 days due to other reasons (n = 100,476) 0.02 (0.003–0.03) 0.04 (0.02–0.05)** 0.1 (0.1–0.1)**
Opinion of one’s own health status (n = 104,092) 0.1  104 ( 0.01 to 0.01) 0.03 (0.01–0.04)* 0.1 (0.09–0.1)**
Quality of sleep (n = 104,201) 0.04 (0.02–0.05)** 0.08 (0.07–0.09)** 0.2 (0.2–0.2)**
Modified version of the Beck Depression Inventory (n = 103,346) 0.1 ( 0.2 to 0.1)** –0.06 ( 0.1 to 0.02) 0.5 (0.5–0.6)**
b
Assessed only for vocational school students.
a
Adjusted for gender, year in class, working status, circadian problems due to Internet use, number of friends to open up to, smoking status, alcohol consumption,
exercising habits, and breakfast eating habits. Those whose bedtimes generally was 10:00 PM or earlier were used as reference category.
*
P < .001.
**
P < .0001.

those with later bedtimes were performing worse. The later the and later also had more absences from school due to skipping or
bedtime, the more difficulties the students had completing home- other reasons than those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier
work assignments, preparing for examinations, following class (Table 3). Students with bedtimes later than at 10:00 PM also had
learning, initiating or finishing self-directed tasks, and getting more feelings of being overwhelmed with schoolwork, and stu-
along with teachers. In regard to practical work tasks and feelings dents who went to sleep 11:00 PM or later also more often felt that
of inadequacy in school performance, only those who went to bed school had lost its meaning (Table 2). Overall, problems with
at 11:30 PM or later had more difficulties than those who went to school performance and motivation were emphasized in students
sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier (Table 2). who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later.
Participants who went to sleep at 10:30 PM or later did not sig-
nificantly differ in teamwork-related tasks from those who went to
3.3. Association of bedtimes with health-related issues
sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier. However, those who went to sleep at
10:30 PM or later got along better with friends than those who
Concerning health-related issues, there was a higher OR for
went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier. There was no significant dif-
tiredness or dizziness in those with later bedtimes (Table 4). In
ference with getting along with school friends when comparing the
addition, the OR for difficulties falling asleep or nightly awaken-
earliest bedtime and bedtimes later than 10:30 PM (Table 2).
ings, headaches, neck or shoulder pains, irritation or tantrums,
stomachaches, lower back pains, or anxiety or nervousness were
3.2. Association of bedtimes with motivation all emphasized in those who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later.
Students who went to sleep at 11:00 PM or later more
Similarly school motivation was weaker in those who went to frequently were absent from school due to illnesses compared to
sleep later than 10:30 PM. Those who went to sleep at 10:30 PM those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier. These students also
I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111 1109

Table 4
Binary logistic regression analyses predicting health issues by bedtime (odds ratios [95% confidence interval]).a

General bedtime during school nights


Approximately Approximately Approximately 11:30 PM
10:30 PM 11:00 PM or later
Neck or shoulder pains during the last 6 mo (N = 103,942; no = 35,114, yes = 68,828) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
Lower back pains during the last 6 mo (N = 103,817; no = 49,795, yes = 54,022) 1.1 (1.0–1.1)* 1.1 (1.1–1.1)** 1.2 (1.2–1.2)**
Stomachaches during the last 6 mo (N = 103,244; no = 45,585, yes = 57,659) 1.1 (1.0–1.1)* 1.1 (1.1–1.1)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)*
Anxiety or nervousness during the last 6 mo (N = 103,960; no = 38,273, yes = 65,687) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.1 (1.0–1.1) 1.1 (1.1–1.1)**
Irritation or tantrums during the last 6 mo (N = 103,977; no = 38,784, yes = 65,193) 1.1 (1.1–1,1)** 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.1–1.2)**
Trouble falling asleep or with nightly awakenings during the last 6 mo (N = 103,956; 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.1 (1.0–1.1)** 1.3 (1.2–1.3)**
no = 49,505, yes = 54,451)
Headaches during the last 6 months (N = 103,988; no = 34,414, yes = 69,574) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.1 (1.1–1.2)** 1.2 (1.2–1.3)**
Tiredness or dizziness during the last 6 mo (N = 104,043; no = 28,417, yes = 75,626) 1.2 (1.1–1.2)** 1.4 (1.3–1.4)** 1.6 (1.5–1.7)**
Accident in recess during the past school term requiring medical attention (N = 103,859; 0.8 (0.8–0.9) 0.7 (0.7–0.8)** 1.0 (0.9–1.1)
no = 99,977, yes = 3882)
Accident in gym class during the past school term requiring medical attention (N = 103,824; 0.9 (0.9–1.0) 0.9 (0.9–1.0) 1.0 (0.9–1.0)
no = 92,700, yes = 11,124)
Accidents in other classes during the past school term requiring medical attention (N = 103,721; 0.9 (0.8–1.0) 0.8 (0.7–0.8)** 0.9 (0.8–1.0)
no = 99,151, yes = 4570)
**
Accidents in work training during the past school term requiring medical attention (N = 38,924; 0.8 (0.7–1.0) 0.7 (0.6–0.8) 0.9 (0.8–1.0)
no = 37,293, yes = 1631b
Accidents on a way to school during the past school term requiring medical attention 0.8 (0.7–0.9)** 0.7 (0.7–0.8)** 0.9 (0.8–1.0)
(N = 103,727; no = 99,162, yes = 4565)

Abbreviation: mo, month.


a
Adjusted for gender, year in school, working status, circadian problems due to Internet use, number of friends to open up to, smoking status, alcohol consumption,
exercising habits, and breakfast eating habits. Those whose bedtimes generally was 10:00 PM or earlier were used as reference category.
b
Assessed only for vocational school students.
*
P < .001.
**
P < .0001.

had a worse opinion of their health status compared to those who depressive symptoms than boys. Furthermore, girls more frequently
went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier. Furthermore, students with complained of neck or shoulder pains, lower back pains, stomach
bedtimes at 10:30 PM and later felt that their sleep quality was ache, anxiety or nervousness, irritation or tantrums, headaches, dif-
worse than those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier ficulties falling asleep or nightly awakenings, and tiredness or dizzi-
(Table 3). ness than boys. In contrast, boys reported more accidents in recess
Moreover, those who went to sleep at 10:30 PM or at 11:00 PM and in other classes than girls (Supplementary Table 3).
experienced less depressive symptoms than those with bedtimes
of 10:00 PM or earlier; yet, depressive symptoms more frequently
occurred among students who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later 4. Discussion
compared to those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier (Table 3).
In addition, the OR for accidents requiring medical attention In our study, weaker school performance and lack of school moti-
that occurred during recess, other classes, or work training were vation were associated with later bedtimes. These issues were
lower among those who went to sleep at 11:00 PM than among emphasized in students who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later. Later
those who went to bed at 10:00 PM or earlier (Table 4); regarding bedtimes also were associated with poor sleep quality, sleep prob-
these accidents, individuals who went to sleep at 10:30 or lems, and tiredness. Similarly the OR for other health-related issues
11:30 PM or later did not significantly differ from those who went was increased in those who went to sleep later than at 10:00 PM.
to sleep at 10:00 PM or earlier. For accidents on the way to school, Thus our results are in line with previous studies indicating that poor
the OR was lower among students with bedtimes of 10:30 or sleep in adolescents might deteriorate academic performance and
11:30 PM than those with bedtimes of 10:00 PM or earlier. predispose individuals to health issues and that the OR for these out-
comes was increased. However, our study was the first to present re-
3.4. Gender differences in school performance and motivation sults using multiple items for school performance and motivation
(e.g., absences) and for different health-related issues and hazards
Girls had more difficulties in preparing for examinations and in (e.g., accidents) from a large nationwide sample covering the whole
finding a suitable study method than boys, whereas boys had more age group of the eighth and ninth grade population.
difficulties with writing tasks than girls (Supplementary Table 2). Based on our results on self-reported sleep, late bedtimes indi-
Girls also got along better with teachers than boys. In general girls cated poor sleep. In our study, students with later bedtimes were
had lower school motivation than boys. First they had had more not satisfied with their sleep quality and reported more sleep prob-
absences from school due to skipping or other reasons than boys. lems (e.g., tiredness, insomnia symptoms, difficulties falling asleep,
Second girls had more feelings of being overwhelmed with school- or awakening too early) compared to students with earlier bed-
work than boys and feelings that school had its lost meaning. Fur- times. Late bedtimes also might indicate a tendency towards eve-
thermore, girls more often felt that their school performance was ningness. In previous studies, eveningness also was related to
inadequate (Supplementary Table 2). sleep problems [28,29]. Furthermore, it is possible that later bed-
times may lead to the accumulation of sleep debt, as students have
3.5. Gender differences in health-related issues to wake up early for school on school days. Compensatory sleep
during weekends may delay circadian rhythms even further, as
Girls had more health-related issues than boys, more absences students with generally later bedtimes are unlikely to advance
from school due to illnesses than boys (Supplementary Table 2), their schedules [30].
and also had a worse opinion of their health status and felt that their In almost all the indicators of school performance and motiva-
sleep quality was worse. They also more frequently reported tion, the trend was toward more difficulties in school and weaker
1110 I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111

motivation as bedtimes were delayed. However, those who went to Furthermore, the tendency towards eveningness predisposes
sleep at 10:30 PM got along better with friends than those who individuals to ‘‘social jetlag,’’ as school and other social schedules
went to sleep at earlier bedtimes. These students also performed interfere with sleep preferences during weekdays, thus resulting
as well as those who went to sleep earlier in practical tasks, while in misalignment of the internal circadian timing system [41,42].
those who went to sleep at 11:30 PM or later had more difficulties. It also has been proposed that the association of smoking with eve-
Similarly depressive symptoms were reported less in students who ningness might be a consequence of social jetlag [41]. The ten-
went to sleep at 10:30 or 11:00 PM compared to those who went to dency towards eveningness is heightened in puberty, especially
bed earlier or later. These students also had more friends to open in girls [43].
up to compared to those who went to bed earlier or later, which Girls had lower school motivation than boys, even though their
might in turn lower depressive symptoms and also improve team- bedtimes generally were earlier. On the other hand, girls were hav-
work performance [31,32]. Interestingly these students also had ing more sleeping problems, such as more insomnia symptoms,
less accidents requiring medical attention; regarding accidents trouble falling asleep or staying asleep, and more feelings of tired-
on the way to school, those who went to sleep at 10:00 PM or ear- ness than boys. Thus it is not surprising that girls had more school
lier had a higher risk for these accidents than those who went to absences due to skipping or illness, more feelings that schoolwork
sleep later. was overwhelming, and more feelings of health-related issues than
It is not surprising that those with later bedtimes had lower boys.
motivation for school, as poor sleep lowers memory and learning It has been suggested that class starting times should be de-
ability, and thus overall performance [5,6,18,29]. For instance, later layed to better cater to the adolescent circadian rhythm. Although
bedtimes were associated with feeling overwhelmed with school- these actions have reportedly improved attention and lessened
work. Absences from school due to skipping also increased, as bed- daytime sleepiness, academic performance has even been reported
times were delayed. This finding may have been due to either a to deteriorate the OR for an increase in alcohol consumption
lack of interest in school, of which these students also reported [38,44,45]. If classes were to start later, they also must end later.
more than those who went to sleep earlier, or due to students Precaution should be taken to prevent this change from excessively
being too tired to go to their morning classes. To our knowledge, delaying other daily activities, as there more frequently would be
our study is the first to report absences from school due to skipping late-opening hours for a broad range of recreational activities to
or other reasons in relation to general bedtimes. meet the increasing demand, which would not help in increasing
Our data on the health-related issues, such as neck or shoulder sleep time. Instead favoring schedules that promote sufficiently
pains, lower back pains, stomachaches, anxiety or nervousness, long nighttime sleep and behaviors that promote early enough
irritation or tantrums, headaches, and tiredness or dizziness, con- bedtimes should be emphasized to avoid social jetlag.
stitute a new addition to the research literature. The later the bed-
time, the more problems students were experiencing in regard to 4.1. Limitations and strengths
all of the named health-related issues. Later bedtimes also in-
creased absences from school due to illness and were associated A limitation to our study is that our data are based on self-re-
with a poorer opinion of their own health status. Because depres- port only. We also lacked exact information regarding sleep dura-
sive symptoms and sleep problems usually are intertwined, it is tion and waking times and separate data regarding weekend
not surprising that those who went to sleep later had more depres- changes. Furthermore, we did not use structured sleep rating scales
sive symptoms [15,29]. For example, depressed individuals often and had no information regarding the stage of puberty or medical
show deficiencies in melatonin secretion, having either lower mel- history of the respondents. However, we were able to use diverse
atonin levels or an earlier timing in their melatonin secretion data indicative of school performance and motivation, depressive
rhythm compared to healthy individuals [15,33–35]. This observa- symptoms, and other health characteristics. A strength of our study
tion also could partly explain why those who went to sleep early was the large sample size derived from students aged 14 to
also had more depressive symptoms compared to those who went 20 years on a nationwide basis.
to sleep at 10:30 PM.
Those with later bedtimes had some lifestyle habits that might 5. Conclusions
delay bedtimes, increase depressive symptoms, and lead to health
hazards (e.g., more alcohol consumption, smoking status, excessive Later bedtimes, especially those after 11:30 PM, are related to
exercising or hardly at all, sleeping problems from Internet usage, poorer school performance and less motivation. They also are asso-
eating less breakfast than those with earlier bedtimes). Likewise, ciated with depressive symptoms and an increased OR for a nega-
previous studies have found an association between insufficient tive health status in adolescents. Schedules that encourage earlier
sleep and an increased risk for smoking and alcohol consumption, bedtimes would be welcome to promote health and well-being
lower physical activity, depressive symptoms, excessive computer among adolescents.
usage, and physical fighting [36–38]. In 2011 Foti et al. [36] sug-
gested that excessive physical activity and computer usage led to
Conflict of interest
insufficient sleep times among adolescents. Similarly working dur-
ing a school term leaves less time for schoolwork and free time, un-
The ICMJE Uniform Disclosure Form for Potential Conflicts of
less one is spared from going to sleep later. Thus it is not surprising
Interest associated with this article can be viewed by clicking on
that those who worked more during a school term had later bed-
the following link: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2013.06.009.
times. On the other hand, it has been reported that adolescents
had a higher tendency towards eveningness than older individuals
[39]. Further, it is possible that later bedtimes might increase the
likelihood of unhealthy lifestyle habits, predispose individuals to References
depression, and deteriorate school performance. For instance,
evening-type ninth graders are more likely to experiment with [1] Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Duke P, Anders TF, Litt IF, Dement WC. Pubertal
smoking and to smoke on a daily basis, to consume more alcohol, changes in daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1980;2:453–60.
[2] Carskadon MA. The second decade. In: Guilleminault C, editor. Sleeping and
and to engage in less physical activity than intermediate-type or waking disorders: indications and techniques. Menlo Park: Addison-Wesley;
morning-type ninth graders [40]. 1982.
I. Merikanto et al. / Sleep Medicine 14 (2013) 1105–1111 1111

[3] Mercer PW, Merritt SL, Cowell JM. Differences in reported sleep need among [25] Lin JD, Tung HJ, Hsieh YH, Lin FG. Interactive effects of delayed bedtime and
adolescents. J Adolesc Health 1998;23:259–63. family-associated factors on depression in elementary school children. Res
[4] Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to Dev Disabil 2011;32:2036–44.
adolescence: reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics [26] Beck AT, Beck RW. Screening depressed patients in family practice: a rapid
2003;111:302–7. technic. Postgrad Med 1972;52:81–5.
[5] Gais S, Lucas B, Born J. Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learn Mem [27] Beck AT, Rial WY, Rickels K. Short form of depression inventory: cross-
2006;13:259–63. validation. Psychol Rep 1974;34:1184–6.
[6] Potkin KT, Bunney Jr WE. Sleep improves memory: the effect of sleep on long [28] Merikanto I, Kronholm E, Peltonen M, Laatikainen T, Lahti T, Partonen T.
term memory in early adolescence. PLoS One 2012;7:e42191. Relation of chronotype to sleep complaints in the general Finnish population.
[7] Mak KK, Lee SL, Ho SY, Lo WS, Lam TH. Sleep and academic performance in Chronobiol Int 2012;29:311–7.
Hong Kong adolescents. J Sch Health 2012;82:522–7. [29] Tzischinsky O, Shochat T. Eveningness, sleep patterns, daytime functioning,
[8] Boschloo A, Krabbendam L, Dekker S, Lee N, de Groot R, Jolles J. Subjective and quality of life in Israeli adolescents. Chronobiol Int 2011;28:338–43.
sleepiness and sleep quality in adolescents are related to objective and [30] Åkerstedt T, Kecklund G, Selén J. Early morning work—prevalence and relation
subjective measures of school performance. Front Psychol 2013;4:38. to sleep/wake problems: a national representative survey. Chronobiol Int
[9] Stroebele N, McNally J, Plog A, Siegfried S, Hill JO. The association of self- 2010;27:975–86.
reported sleep, weight status, and academic performance in fifth-grade [31] Clark MS, Jansen KL, Cloy JA. Treatment of childhood and adolescent
students. J Sch Health 2013;83:77–84. depression. Am Fam Physician 2012;86:442–8.
[10] Rhie S, Lee S, Chae KY. Sleep patterns and school performance of Korean [32] Greydanus DE, Calles Jr J. Suicide in children and adolescents. Prim Care
adolescents assessed using a Korean version of the pediatric daytime 2007;34:259–73.
sleepiness scale. Korean J Pediatr 2011;54:29–35. [33] Beck-Friis J, Kjellman BF, Aperia B, Unden F, von Rosen D, Ljunggren JG, et al.
[11] Cirelli C, Tononi G. Is sleep essential? PLoS Biol 2008;6:e216. Serum melatonin in relation to clinical variables in patients with major
[12] Donlea JM, Shaw PJ. Sleeping together using social interactions to understand depressive disorder and a hypothesis of low melatonin syndrome. Acta
the role of sleep in plasticity. Adv Genet 2009;68:57–81. Psychiatr Scand 1985;71:319–30.
[13] Gilestro GF, Tononi G, Cirelli C. Widespread changes in synaptic markers as a [34] Wetterberg L, Aperia B, Gorelick DA, Gwirtzman HE, McGuire MT, Serafetinides
function of sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Science 2009;324:109–12. EA, et al. Age, alcoholism and depression are associated with low levels of
[14] Vyazovskiy VV, Olcese U, Lazimy YM, Faraguna U, Esser SK, Williams JC, et al. urinary melatonin. J Psychiatry Neurosci 1992;17:215–24.
Cortical firing and sleep homeostasis. Neuron 2009;63:865–78. [35] Monteleone P, Maj M. The circadian basis of mood disorders: recent
[15] Srinivasan V, Pandi-Perumal SR, Trakht I, Spence DW, Hardeland R, Poeggeler developments and treatment implications. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol
B, et al. Pathophysiology of depression: role of sleep and the melatonergic 2008;18:701–11.
system. Psychiatry Res 2009;165:201–14. [36] Foti KE, Eaton DK, Lowry R, McKnight-Ely LR. Sufficient sleep, physical activity,
[16] Merikanto I, Lahti T, Kronholm E, Peltonen M, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, et al. and sedentary behaviors. Am J Prev Med 2011;41:596–602.
Evening-types are prone to depression [published online ahead of print May [37] McKnight-Eily LR, Eaton DK, Lowry R, Croft JB, Presley-Cantrell L, Perry GS.
20, 2013]. Chronobiol Int 2013;30:719–25. Relationships between hours of sleep and health-risk behaviors in US
[17] Gomes AA, Tavares J, de Azevedo MH. Sleep and academic performance in adolescent students. Prev Med 2011;53:271–3.
undergraduates: a multi-measure, multi-predictor approach. Chronobiol Int [38] Onyper SV, Thacher PV, Gilbert JW, Gradess SG. Class start times, sleep, and
2011;28:786–801. academic performance in college: a path analysis. Chronobiol Int
[18] Jiang F, Van Dyke RD, Zhang J, Li F, Gozal D, Shen X. Effect of chronic sleep 2012;29:318–35.
restriction on sleepiness and working memory in adolescents and young [39] Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, et al.
adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2011;33:892–900. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 2007;11:429–38.
[19] Kim SJ, Lee YJ, Cho SJ, Cho IH, Lim W, Lim W. Relationship between weekend [40] Urbán R, Magyaródi T, Rigó A. Morningness-eveningness, chronotypes and
catch-up sleep and poor performance on attention tasks in Korean adolescents. health-impairing behaviors in adolescents. Chronobiol Int 2011;28:238–47.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2011;165:806–12. [41] Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. Social jetlag: misalignment of
[20] Soffer-Dudek N, Sadeh A, Dahl RE, Rosenblat-Stein S. Poor sleep quality biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 2006;23:497–509.
predicts deficient emotion information processing over time in early [42] Touitou Y. Adolescent sleep misalignment: a chronic jet lag and a matter of
adolescence. Sleep 2011;34:1499–508. public health. J Physiol Paris 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
[21] Dagys N, McGlinchey EL, Talbot LS, Kaplan KA, Dahl RE, Harvey AG. Double j.jphysparis.2013.03.008. [Epub ahead of print].
trouble? the effects of sleep deprivation and chronotype on adolescent affect. J [43] Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed
Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012;53:660–7. phase preference. Sleep 1993;16:258–62.
[22] Danielsson NS, Harvey AG, Macdonald S, Jansson-Fröjmark M, Linton SJ. Sleep [44] Lufi D, Tzischinsky O, Hadar S. Delaying school starting time by one hour:
disturbance and depressive symptoms in adolescence: the role of catastrophic some effects on attention levels in adolescents. J Clin Sleep Med
worry. J Youth Adolesc 2013;42:1223–33. 2011;7:137–43.
[23] Olds TS, Maher CA, Matricciani L. Sleep duration or bedtime? Exploring the [45] Wahlstrom K. School Start time and sleepy teens. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
relationship between sleep habits and weight status and activity patterns. 2010;164:676–7.
Sleep 2011;34:1299–307.
[24] Golley RK, Maher CA, Matricciani L, Olds TS. Sleep duration or bedtime?
Exploring the association between sleep timing behaviour, diet and BMI in
children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond) 2013;37:546–51.

You might also like