You are on page 1of 1

Search Wikipedia Search Create account Log in

Somatosensory system 48 languages

Contents hide Article Talk Read Edit View history Tools

(Top) From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

System overview (Redirected from Somatic sensation)

Sensory receptors
"Touch" redirects here. For other uses, see Touch (disambiguation).
Somatosensory cortex
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk [hide]
Structure
page. (Learn how and when to remove these template messages)
General somatosensory pathway
This article needs additional citations for verification. (August 2023)
Tactile feedback This article may be too technical for most readers to understand. (May 2021)

Balance
In physiology, the somatosensory system is the network of neural structures in the brain and body that produce
Fine touch and crude touch the perception of touch (haptic perception), as well as temperature (thermoception), body position
Neural processing of social (proprioception), and pain.[1] It is a subset of the sensory nervous system, which also represents visual, auditory,
touch olfactory, gustatory and vestibular stimuli.

Individual variation
Somatosensation begins when mechano- and thermosensitive structures in the skin or internal organs sense
Clinical significance physical stimuli such as pressure on the skin (see mechanotransduction, nociception). Activation of these

Society and culture structures, or receptors, leads to activation of peripheral sensory neurons that convey signals to the spinal cord
as patterns of action potentials. Sensory information is then processed locally in the spinal cord to drive reflexes,
See also
and is also conveyed to the brain for conscious perception of touch and proprioception. Note, somatosensory
Notes information from the face and head enters the brain through peripheral sensory neurons in the cranial nerves,
References such as the trigeminal nerve.

Further reading The neural pathways that go to the brain are structured such that information about the location of the physical
External links stimulus is preserved. In this way, neighboring neurons in the somatosensory cerebral cortex in the brain
Touch is a crucial means of
represent nearby locations on the skin or in the body, creating a map, also called a cortical homunculus. receiving information. This photo
shows tactile markings identifying
stairs for visually impaired people.
System overview [ edit ]

Sensory receptors [ edit ]

The four mechanoreceptors in the skin each respond to different stimuli for short or long periods.

Merkel cell nerve endings are found in the basal epidermis and hair follicles; they react to low vibrations (5–
15 Hz) and deep static touch such as shapes and edges. Due to having a small receptive field (extremely This diagram linearly (unless
detailed information), they are used in areas like fingertips the most; they are not covered (shelled) and thus otherwise mentioned) tracks the
projections of all known structures that
respond to pressures over long periods.
allow for touch to their relevant
endpoints in the human brain.
Tactile corpuscles react to moderate vibration (10–50 Hz) and light touch. They are located in the dermal
papillae; due to their reactivity, they are primarily located in fingertips and lips. They respond in quick action
potentials, unlike Merkel nerve endings. They are responsible for the ability to read Braille and feel gentle stimuli.

Pacinian corpuscles determine gross touch and distinguish rough and soft substances. They react in quick action potentials, especially to vibrations
around 250 Hz (even up to centimeters away). They are the most sensitive to vibrations and have large receptor fields. Pacinian corpuscles react only to
sudden stimuli so pressures like clothes that are always compressing their shape are quickly ignored. They have also been implicated in detecting the
location of touch sensations on handheld tools.[2]

Bulbous corpuscles react slowly and respond to sustained skin stretch. They are responsible for the feeling of object slippage and play a major role in the
kinesthetic sense and control of finger position and movement. Merkel and bulbous cells - slow-response - are myelinated; the rest - fast-response - are
not. All of these receptors are activated upon pressures that squish their shape causing an action potential.[3][4][5][6]

Somatosensory cortex [ edit ]

The postcentral gyrus includes the primary somatosensory cortex (Brodmann areas 3, 2 and 1) collectively
referred to as S1.

BA3 receives the densest projections from the thalamus. BA3a is involved with the sense of relative position of
neighboring body parts and amount of effort being used during movement. BA3b is responsible for distributing
somatosensory information, it projects texture information to BA1 and shape and size information to BA2.

Region S2 (secondary somatosensory cortex) divides into Area S2 and parietal ventral area. Area S2 is involved
with specific touch perception and is thus integrally linked with the amygdala and hippocampus to encode and
reinforce memories.

Parietal ventral area is the somatosensory relay to the premotor cortex and somatosensory memory hub, BA5.

BA5 is the topographically organized somato memory field and association area.

BA1 processes texture info while BA2 processes size and shape information.

Area S2 processes light touch, pain, visceral sensation, and tactile attention.
Gray's Anatomy, figure 759: the
S1 processes the remaining info (crude touch, pain, temperature).[7][8][9]
sensory tract, showing the pathway
(blue) up the spinal cord, through the
BA7 integrates visual and proprioceptive info to locate objects in space.[10][11] somatosensory thalamus, to S1
(Brodmann areas 3, 1, and 2), S2, and
The insular cortex (insula) plays a role in the sense of bodily-ownership, bodily self-awareness, and perception. BA7
Insula also plays a role in conveying info about sensual touch, pain, temperature, itch, and local oxygen status.
Insula is a highly connected relay and thus is involved in numerous functions.

Structure [ edit ]

The somatosensory system is spread through all major parts of the vertebrate body. It consists both of sensory
receptors and sensory neurons in the periphery (skin, muscle and organs for example), to deeper neurons within
the central nervous system.

General somatosensory pathway [ edit ]

See also: Dorsal column–medial lemniscus pathway

All afferent touch/vibration info ascends the spinal cord via the dorsal column-medial lemniscus pathway via
gracilis (T7 and below) or cuneatus (T6 and above). Cuneatus sends signals to the cochlear nucleus indirectly Gray's Anatomy, figure 717: detail
via spinal grey matter, this info is used in determining if a perceived sound is just villi noise/irritation. All fibers showing path adjacent to the insular
cross (left becomes right) in the medulla. cortex (marked insula in this figure),
adjacent to S1, S2, and BA7
A somatosensory pathway will typically have three neurons:[12] first-order, second-order, and third-order.[13]

1. The first-order neuron is a type of pseudounipolar neuron and always has its cell body in the dorsal root ganglion of the spinal nerve with a
peripheral axon innervating touch mechanoreceptors and a central axon synapsing on the second-order neuron. If the somatosensory pathway is
in parts of the head or neck not covered by the cervical nerves, the first-order neuron will be the trigeminal nerve ganglia or the ganglia of other
sensory cranial nerves).
2. The second-order neuron has its cell body either in the spinal cord or in the brainstem. This neuron's ascending axons will cross (decussate) to
the opposite side either in the spinal cord or in the brainstem.
3. In the case of touch and certain types of pain, the third-order neuron has its cell body in the ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus and ends
in the postcentral gyrus of the parietal lobe in the primary somatosensory cortex (or S1).

Photoreceptors, similar to those found in the retina of the eye, detect potentially damaging ultraviolet radiation
(ultraviolet A specifically), inducing increased production of melanin by melanocytes.[14] Thus tanning potentially
offers the skin rapid protection from DNA damage and sunburn caused by ultraviolet radiation (DNA damage
caused by ultraviolet B). However, whether this offers protection is debatable, because the amount of melanin
released by this process is modest in comparison to the amounts released in response to DNA damage caused
by ultraviolet B radiation.[14]

Touch can result in many different Tactile feedback [ edit ]


physiological reactions. Here, a baby
laughs at being tickled by an older The tactile feedback from proprioception is derived from the proprioceptors in the skin, muscles, and joints.[15]
sister.

Balance [ edit ]

The receptor for the sense of balance resides in the vestibular system in the ear (for the three-dimensional orientation of the head, and by inference, the
rest of the body). Balance is also mediated by the kinesthetic reflex fed by proprioception (which senses the relative location of the rest of the body to the
head).[16] In addition, proprioception estimates the location of objects which are sensed by the visual system (which provides confirmation of the place of
those objects relative to the body), as input to the mechanical reflexes of the body.

Fine touch and crude touch [ edit ]

See also: Two-point discrimination

Fine touch (or discriminative touch) is a sensory modality that allows a subject to sense and localize touch. The
form of touch where localization is not possible is known as crude touch. The posterior column–medial lemniscus
pathway is the pathway responsible for the sending of fine touch information to the cerebral cortex of the brain.

Crude touch (or non-discriminative touch) is a sensory modality that allows the subject to sense that something
has touched them, without being able to localize where they were touched (contrasting "fine touch"). Its fibres
are carried in the spinothalamic tract, unlike the fine touch, which is carried in the dorsal column. [17] As fine
touch normally works in parallel to crude touch, a person will be able to localize touch until fibres carrying fine
touch (Posterior column–medial lemniscus pathway) have been disrupted. Then the subject will feel the touch,
but be unable to identify where they were touched.

Neural processing of social touch [ edit ]

The somatosensory cortex encodes incoming sensory information from receptors all over the body. Affective The cortical homunculus, a map of
touch is a type of sensory information that elicits an emotional reaction and is usually social in nature, such as a somatosensory areas of the brain, was
devised by Wilder Penfield.
physical human touch. This type of information is actually coded differently than other sensory information.
Intensity of affective touch is still encoded in the primary somatosensory cortex and is processed in a similar way
to emotions invoked by sight and sound, as exemplified by the increase of adrenaline caused by the social touch of a loved one, as opposed to the
physical inability to touch someone you do not love.

Meanwhile, the feeling of pleasantness associated with affective touch activates the anterior cingulate cortex more than the primary somatosensory
cortex. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data shows that increased blood-oxygen-level contrast (BOLD) signal in the anterior cingulate
cortex as well as the prefrontal cortex is highly correlated with pleasantness scores of an affective touch. Inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation
(TMS) of the primary somatosensory cortex inhibits the perception of affective touch intensity, but not affective touch pleasantness. Therefore, the S1 is
not directly involved in processing socially affective touch pleasantness, but still plays a role in discriminating touch location and intensity.[17]

Tactile interaction is important amongst some animals. Usually, tactile contact between two animals occurs through stroking, licking, or grooming. These
behaviours are essential for the individual's social healthcare, as in the hypothalamus they induce the release of oxytocin, a hormone that decreases
stress and anxiety and increases social bonding between animals.[18][clarification needed]

More precisely, the consistency of oxytocin neuron activation in rats stroked by humans has been observed, especially in the caudal paraventricular
nucleus.[19] It was found that this affiliative relationship induced by tactile contact is common no matter the relationship between the two individuals
(mother-infant, male-female, human-animal). It has also been discovered that the level of oxytocin release through this behaviour correlates with the time
course of social interaction as longer stroking induced a greater release of the hormone.[20]

The importance of somatosensory stimulation in social animals such as primates has also been observed. Grooming is part of the social interaction
primates exert on their conspecifics. This interaction is required between individuals to maintain the affiliative relationship within the group, avoid internal
conflict and increase group bonding.[21] However, such social interaction requires the recognition of every member in the group. As such, it has been
observed that the size of the neocortex is positively correlated with the size of the group, reflecting a limit to the number of recognizable members
amongst which grooming can occur.[21] Furthermore, the time course of grooming is related to vulnerability due to predation to which animals are
exposed to whilst performing such social interaction. The relationship between tactile interaction, stress reduction and social bonding depends on the
evaluation of risks that occur during conducting such behaviours in the wild life, and further research is required to unveil the connection between tactile
caring and fitness level.

Studies show a correlation between touch a soft or hard object and how a person thinks or even makes decisions[22] and between the firmness of a
touch and the evoking of gender stereotyping.[23]

Individual variation [ edit ]

A variety of studies have measured and investigated the causes for differences between individuals in the sense of fine touch. One well-studied area is
passive tactile spatial acuity, the ability to resolve the fine spatial details of an object pressed against the stationary skin. A variety of methods have been
used to measure passive tactile spatial acuity, perhaps the most rigorous being the grating orientation task.[24] In this task subjects identify the orientation
of a grooved surface presented in two different orientations,[25] which can be applied manually or with automated equipment.[26] Many studies have
shown a decline in passive tactile spatial acuity with age;[27][28][29] the reasons for this decline are unknown, but may include loss of tactile receptors
during normal aging. Remarkably, index finger passive tactile spatial acuity is better among adults with smaller index fingertips;[30] this effect of finger
size has been shown to underlie the better passive tactile spatial acuity of women, on average, compared to men.[30] The density of tactile corpuscles, a
type of mechanoreceptor that detects low-frequency vibrations, is greater in smaller fingers;[31] the same may hold for Merkel cells, which detect the
static indentations important for fine spatial acuity.[30] Among children of the same age, those with smaller fingers also tend to have better tactile
acuity.[32] Many studies have shown that passive tactile spatial acuity is enhanced among blind individuals compared to sighted individuals of the same
age,[29][33][34][35][36] possibly because of cross modal plasticity in the cerebral cortex of blind individuals. Perhaps also due to cortical plasticity, individuals
who have been blind since birth reportedly consolidate tactile information more rapidly than sighted people.[37]

Clinical significance [ edit ]

Main article: Somatosensory disorder

A somatosensory deficiency may be caused by a peripheral neuropathy involving peripheral nerves of the somatosensory system. This may present as
numbness or paresthesia.

Society and culture [ edit ]

Main articles: Haptic technology and Haptic communication

Haptic technology can provide touch sensation in virtual and real environments.[38] In the field of speech therapy, tactile feedback can be used to treat
speech disorders.[citation needed]

Affectionate touch is present in everyday life and can take multiple forms. These actions, however, seem to carry specific functions even though the
evolutionary benefit from such a wide range of behaviours is not entirely understood. Researchers investigated the expression patterns and
characteristics of 8 different affectionate touch actions - embracing, holding, kissing, leaning, petting, squeezing, stroking, and tickling - in a self-report
study.[39] It was found that the affectionate touch has distinct target areas on the body, different associated affect, comfort-value, and expression
frequency based on the type of touch action that is performed.

Besides the rather obvious sensory consequences of touch, it can also affect higher-level aspects of cognition such as social judgements and decision-
making. This effect might arise due to a physical-to-mental scaffolding process in early development, whereby sensorimotor experiences are linked to the
emergence of conceptual knowledge.[40] Such links might be maintained throughout life, and so touching an object may cue the physical sensation to its
related conceptual processing. Indeed, it was found that different physical properties - weight, texture, and hardness - of a touched object can influence
social judgement and decision-making.[41] For example, participants described a passage of a social interaction to be harsher when they touched a hard
wooden block instead of a soft blanket prior to the task. Building on these findings, the ability of touch to have an unconscious influence on such higher-
order thoughts may provide a novel tool for marketing and communication strategies.

See also [ edit ]

Allochiria Muscle spindle Supramarginal gyrus


Cell signalling Molecular cellular cognition Tactile illusion
Golgi tendon organ Phantom limb Vibratese, method of communication through
Haptic communication Physical intimacy touch
Haptic perception Sensory maps Tactile imaging
Interoception Special senses

Notes [ edit ]

References [ edit ]

1. ^ Sherman, Carl (August 12, 2019). "The Senses: The Somatosensory 22. ^ https://www.livescience.com/8360-touch-influence-thoughts-decisions.htm
system" . Dana Foundation. New York. l
2. ^ Sima, Richard (23 December 2019). "The Brain Senses Touch beyond the 23. ^ https://www.livescience.com/9268-firmness-touch-evoke-gender-stereotypi
Body" . Scientific American. Retrieved 16 February 2020. ng.html
3. ^ Paré, Michel, and Catherine Behets. "Paucity of Presumptive Ruffini 24. ^ Van Boven, R. W.; Johnson, K. O. (1 December 1994). "The limit of tactile
Corpuscles in the Index Finger Pad of Humans." Wiley Online Library. 10 spatial resolution in humans: Grating orientation discrimination at the lip,
February 2003. Web. 27 March 2016. tongue, and finger". Neurology. 44 (12): 2361–6.
4. ^ Scheibert J, Leurent S, Prevost A, Debrégeas G (March 2009). "The role doi:10.1212/WNL.44.12.2361 . PMID 7991127 . S2CID 32255147 .
of fingerprints in the coding of tactile information probed with a biomimetic 25. ^ Craig JC (1999). "Grating orientation as a measure of tactile spatial
sensor". Science. 323 (5920): 1503–6. arXiv:0911.4885 . acuity". Somatosensory & Motor Research. 16 (3): 197–206.
Bibcode:2009Sci...323.1503S . doi:10.1126/science.1166467 . doi:10.1080/08990229970456 . PMID 10527368 .
PMID 19179493 . S2CID 14459552 . 26. ^ Goldreich D, Wong M, Peters RM, Kanics IM (June 2009). "A Tactile
5. ^ Biswas A, Manivannan M, Srinivasan MA (2015). "Vibrotactile sensitivity Automated Passive-Finger Stimulator (TAPS)" . Journal of Visualized
threshold: nonlinear stochastic mechanotransduction model of the Pacinian Experiments (28). doi:10.3791/1374 . PMC 2726582 .
Corpuscle" . IEEE Transactions on Haptics. 8 (1): 102–13. PMID 19578327 .
doi:10.1109/TOH.2014.2369422 . PMID 25398183 . S2CID 15326972 . 27. ^ Stevens JC, Alvarez-Reeves M, Dipietro L, Mack GW, Green BG (2003).
6. ^ Paré, Michel, and Robert Elde. "The Meissner Corpuscle Revised: A "Decline of tactile acuity in aging: a study of body site, blood flow, and
Multiafferented Mechanoreceptor with Nociceptor Immunochemical lifetime habits of smoking and physical activity". Somatosensory & Motor
Properties." JNeurosci. 15 September 2001. Web. 27 March 2016. Research. 20 (3–4): 271–9. doi:10.1080/08990220310001622997 .
7. ^ Hashim IH, Kumamoto S, Takemura K, Maeno T, Okuda S, Mori Y PMID 14675966 . S2CID 19729552 .
(November 2017). "Tactile Evaluation Feedback System for Multi-Layered 28. ^ Manning H, Tremblay F (2006). "Age differences in tactile pattern
Structure Inspired by Human Tactile Perception Mechanism" . Sensors recognition at the fingertip" . Somatosensory & Motor Research. 23 (3–4):
(Basel, Switzerland). 17 (11): 2601. Bibcode:2017Senso..17.2601H . 147–55. doi:10.1080/08990220601093460 . PMID 17178550 .
doi:10.3390/s17112601 . PMC 5712818 . PMID 29137128 . S2CID 24407285 .
8. ^ Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Seitz RJ, 29. ^ a b Goldreich D, Kanics IM (April 2003). "Tactile acuity is enhanced in
Zilles K, Rizzolatti G, Freund HJ (January 2001). "Action observation blindness" . The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (8): 3439–45.
activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI doi:10.1523/jneurosci.23-08-03439.2003 . PMC 6742312 .
study". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 13 (2): 400–4. PMID 12716952 .
doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2001.01385.x . PMID 11168545 . 30. ^ a b c Peters RM, Hackeman E, Goldreich D (December 2009). "Diminutive
S2CID 107700 . digits discern delicate details: fingertip size and the sex difference in tactile
9. ^ Seelke AM, Padberg JJ, Disbrow E, Purnell SM, Recanzone G, Krubitzer L spatial acuity" . The Journal of Neuroscience. 29 (50): 15756–61.
(August 2012). "Topographic Maps within Brodmann's Area 5 of macaque doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3684-09.2009 . PMC 3849661 .
monkeys" . Cerebral Cortex. 22 (8): 1834–50. PMID 20016091 .
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr257 . PMC 3388892 . PMID 21955920 . 31. ^ Dillon YK, Haynes J, Henneberg M (November 2001). "The relationship of
10. ^ Geyer S, Schleicher A, Zilles K (July 1999). "Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of the number of Meissner's corpuscles to dermatoglyphic characters and
Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex". NeuroImage. 10 (1): 63–83. finger size" . Journal of Anatomy. 199 (Pt 5): 577–84. doi:10.1046/j.1469-
doi:10.1006/nimg.1999.0440 . PMID 10385582 . S2CID 22498933 . 7580.2001.19950577.x . PMC 1468368 . PMID 11760888 .
11. ^ Disbrow E (June 2002). "Thalamocortical connections of the parietal 32. ^ Peters RM, Goldreich D (2013). "Tactile spatial acuity in childhood: effects
ventral area (PV) and the second somatosensory area (S2) in macaque of age and fingertip size" . PLOS ONE. 8 (12): e84650.
monkeys". Thalamus & Related Systems. 1 (4): 289–302. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...884650P . doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0084650 .
doi:10.1016/S1472-9288(02)00003-1 . PMC 3891499 . PMID 24454612 .
12. ^ Saladin KS. Anatomy and Physiology 3rd edd. 2004. McGraw-Hill, New 33. ^ Stevens, Joseph C.; Foulke, Emerson; Patterson, Matthew Q. (1996).
York. "Tactile acuity, aging, and braille reading in long-term blindness". Journal of
13. ^ "Second Order Neuron" . Encyclopedia of Pain. Springer. 2013. p. 3448. Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2 (2): 91–106. doi:10.1037/1076-
doi:10.1007/978-3-642-28753-4_201964 . ISBN 978-3-642-28752-7. 898X.2.2.91 .
Retrieved 2 December 2022. {{cite book}}: |website= ignored (help) 34. ^ Van Boven RW, Hamilton RH, Kauffman T, Keenan JP, Pascual-Leone A
14. ^ a b Zukerman, Wendy. "Skin 'sees' the light to protect against sunshine" . (June 2000). "Tactile spatial resolution in blind braille readers". Neurology.
newscientist.com. New Scientist. Retrieved 2015-01-22. 54 (12): 2230–6. doi:10.1212/wnl.54.12.2230 . PMID 10881245 .
15. ^ Proske U, Gandevia SC (October 2012). "The proprioceptive senses: their S2CID 12053536 .
roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle 35. ^ Goldreich D, Kanics IM (November 2006). "Performance of blind and
force". Physiological Reviews. 92 (4): 1651–97. sighted humans on a tactile grating detection task" . Perception &
doi:10.1152/physrev.00048.2011 . PMID 23073629 . S2CID 1512089 . Psychophysics. 68 (8): 1363–71. doi:10.3758/bf03193735 .
16. ^ Proske U, Gandevia SC (September 2009). "The kinaesthetic senses" . PMID 17378422 .
The Journal of Physiology. 587 (Pt 17): 4139–46. 36. ^ Wong M, Gnanakumaran V, Goldreich D (May 2011). "Tactile spatial acuity
doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175372 . PMC 2754351 . PMID 19581378 . enhancement in blindness: evidence for experience-dependent
17. ^ a b Case LK, Laubacher CM, Olausson H, Wang B, Spagnolo PA, Bushnell mechanisms" . The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (19): 7028–37.
MC (May 2016). "Encoding of Touch Intensity But Not Pleasantness in doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6461-10.2011 . PMC 6703211 .
Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex" . The Journal of Neuroscience. 36 PMID 21562264 .
(21): 5850–60. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1130-15.2016 . PMC 4879201 . 37. ^ Bhattacharjee A, Ye AJ, Lisak JA, Vargas MG, Goldreich D (October
PMID 27225773 . 2010). "Vibrotactile masking experiments reveal accelerated somatosensory
18. ^ Knobloch, H. Sophie; Grinevich, Valery (2014). "Evolution of oxytocin processing in congenitally blind braille readers" . The Journal of
pathways in the brain of vertebrates" . Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 30 (43): 14288–98. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1447-
Neuroscience. 8: 31. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00031 . ISSN 1662-5153 . 10.2010 . PMC 3449316 . PMID 20980584 .
PMC 3924577 . PMID 24592219 . 38. ^ Gabriel Robles-De-La-Torre. "International Society for Haptics: Haptic
19. ^ Okabe, Shota; Takayanagi, Yuki; Yoshida, Masahide; Onaka, Tatsushi technology, an animated explanation" . Isfh.org. Archived from the
(2020-06-04). "Gentle stroking stimuli induce affiliative responsiveness to original on 2010-03-07. Retrieved 2010-02-26.
humans in male rats" . Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 9135. 39. ^ Schirmer, Annett; Chiu, Man Hey; Croy, Ilona (September 2021). "More
Bibcode:2020NatSR..10.9135O . doi:10.1038/s41598-020-66078-7 . than one kind: Different sensory signatures and functions divide affectionate
ISSN 2045-2322 . PMC 7272613 . PMID 32499488 . touch" . Emotion. 21 (6): 1268–1280. doi:10.1037/emo0000966 .
20. ^ Tang, Yan; Benusiglio, Diego; Lefevre, Arthur; Hilfiger, Louis; Althammer, ISSN 1931-1516 . PMID 34435843 .
Ferdinand; Bludau, Anna; Hagiwara, Daisuke; Baudon, Angel; Darbon, 40. ^ Williams, Lawrence E.; Huang, Julie Y.; Bargh, John A. (2009-12-01). "The
Pascal; Schimmer, Jonas; Kirchner, Matthew K.; Roy, Ranjan K.; Wang, Scaffolded Mind: Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience
Shiyi; Eliava, Marina; Wagner, Shlomo (September 2020). "Social touch of the physical world" . European Journal of Social Psychology. 39 (7):
promotes interfemale communication via activation of parvocellular oxytocin 1257–1267. doi:10.1002/ejsp.665 . ISSN 0046-2772 . PMC 2799930 .
neurons" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 23 (9): 1125–1137. PMID 20046813 .
doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0674-y . ISSN 1546-1726 . PMID 32719563 . 41. ^ Ackerman, Joshua M.; Nocera, Christopher C.; Bargh, John A. (2010-06-
S2CID 220810651 . 25). "Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and
21. ^ a b Lehmann, J.; Korstjens, A. H.; Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007-12-01). "Group decisions" . Science. 328 (5986): 1712–1715.
size, grooming and social cohesion in primates" . Animal Behaviour. 74 Bibcode:2010Sci...328.1712A . doi:10.1126/science.1189993 .
(6): 1617–1629. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.025 . ISSN 0003-3472 . ISSN 1095-9203 . PMC 3005631 . PMID 20576894 .
S2CID 14866172 .

Further reading [ edit ]

Boron WF, Boulpaep EL (2003). Medical Physiology. Saunders. pp. 352–358. ISBN 0-7216-3256-4.
Flanagan, J.R., Lederman, S.J. Neurobiology: Feeling bumps and holes , News and Views, Nature, 2001 July 26;412(6845):389-91.
Hayward V, Astley OR, Cruz-Hernandez M, Grant D, Robles-De-La-Torre G (2004). "Haptic interfaces and devices" (PDF). Sensor Review. 24 (1):
16–29. doi:10.1108/02602280410515770 . S2CID 3136266 .
Purves, Dale (2012). Neuroscience, Fifth Edition. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc. pp. 202–203. ISBN 978-0-87893-695-3.
Robles-De-La-Torre G, Hayward V (July 2001). "Force can overcome object geometry in the perception of shape through active touch" (PDF).
Nature. 412 (6845): 445–8. Bibcode:2001Natur.412..445R . doi:10.1038/35086588 . PMID 11473320 . S2CID 4413295 .
Robles-De-La-Torre, G (2006). "The Importance of the Sense of Touch in Virtual and Real Environments" (PDF). IEEE MultiMedia. 13 (3): 24–30.
doi:10.1109/mmul.2006.69 . S2CID 16153497 .
Grunwald, M. (Ed.) Human Haptic Perception – Basics and Applications. Boston/Basel/Berlin: Birkhäuser, 2008, ISBN 978-3-7643-7611-6
Encyclopedia of Touch Scholarpedia Expert articles

External links [ edit ]

Media related to Somatosensation at Wikimedia Commons


Anatomy of Touch . Factual documentary series by BBC Radio 4.

· · Sensation and perception [show]

· · Sensory receptors [show]

Authority control databases: National Germany

Category: Somatosensory system

This page was last edited on 25 January 2024, at 14:28 (UTC).

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License 4.0; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit
organization.

Privacy policy About Wikipedia Disclaimers Contact Wikipedia Code of Conduct Developers Statistics Cookie statement Mobile view

You might also like