You are on page 1of 18

Articles

https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01215-4

Eye movements during text reading align with the


rate of speech production
Benjamin Gagl 1,2,3 ✉, Klara Gregorova 1, Julius Golch1, Stefan Hawelka 4
, Jona Sassenhagen1,
Alessandro Tavano5, David Poeppel 6,7,8 and Christian J. Fiebach 1,2,9

Across languages, the speech signal is characterized by a predominant modulation of the amplitude spectrum between about
4.3 and 5.5 Hz, reflecting the production and processing of linguistic information chunks (syllables and words) every ~200 ms.
Interestingly, ~200 ms is also the typical duration of eye fixations during reading. Prompted by this observation, we demonstrate
that German readers sample written text at ~5 Hz. A subsequent meta-analysis of 142 studies from 14 languages replicates this
result and shows that sampling frequencies vary across languages between 3.9 Hz and 5.2 Hz. This variation systematically
depends on the complexity of the writing systems (character-based versus alphabetic systems and orthographic transparency).
Finally, we empirically demonstrate a positive correlation between speech spectrum and eye movement sampling in low-skilled
non-native readers, with tentative evidence from post hoc analysis suggesting the same relationship in low-skilled native read-
ers. On the basis of this convergent evidence, we propose that during reading, our brain’s linguistic processing systems imprint
a preferred processing rate—that is, the rate of spoken language production and perception—onto the oculomotor system.

S
peech production and perception form a quasi-rhythmic eye movements during non-linguistic tasks or between different
information processing cycle1. During spoken communica- languages or writing systems.
tion, our brain entrains to the frequency structure of the To address these foundational questions, we first used an empiri-
speech signal2,3, suggesting that the temporal structure of the lin- cal dataset17 to determine eye movement sampling frequencies for
guistic stimulus drives neural processes in auditory and language 50 native speakers of German during sentence reading compared
processing systems4. Across languages, the amplitude modulation with a non-linguistic control task, using two different methodolo-
spectrum of natural speech peaks consistently in a frequency range gies. Next, to determine the generality of these results and to inves-
between 4.3 and 5.5 Hz (refs. 5,6), which reflects the fact that informa- tigate possible cross-linguistic differences in the sampling rate of
tive signals (for example, syllables7,8) are processed by the listeners’ reading, we conducted a meta-analysis of 124 studies from 14 dif-
brains every ~200 ms (ref. 9). Interestingly—and, we hypothesize, ferent languages. To this end, we established a frequency analysis for
not accidentally—a typical eye fixation during reading has a very fixation durations extracted from published eye-tracking studies.
similar duration: between ~200 ms for orthographically transpar- Finally, we acquired two new datasets, one with 48 non-native and
ent writing systems such as German or Finnish10,11 and ~250 ms for one with 86 native speakers of German, to directly investigate the
character-based systems such as Chinese11,12. relationship between the sampling frequency of reading and speech
Abundant research has used eye-movement recordings to study production rates on a subject-by-subject level. The experimental
reading at high temporal resolution, exploring, for example, how and meta-analytic results show (1) that written text is sampled in
reading is influenced by word length, word frequency or word pre- the same frequency range as spoken language; (2) that the sampling
dictability given a sentence context12. Among various measures that rate of reading has an upper limit at ~5 Hz, observable in languages
can be derived from eye-movement recordings, timing measures with transparent orthographies; (3) that this rate can be modulated
such as fixation duration are most frequently examined and consid- depending on the complexity of the writing system (for example,
ered precise markers of reading speed13,14. These temporally highly in character-based as opposed to alphabetic scripts or in alphabetic
resolved measurements have so far been analysed only at the level scripts with opaque grapheme–phoneme mapping); and (4) that a
of individual items—typically words. However, other domains of direct coupling between reading and speech rates is found only in
cognitive research (such as attention15) demonstrate that eye move- persons with lower levels of reading skill.
ments can also be subjected to frequency-based analyses. We here
demonstrate that a frequency-based exploration of how written text Results
is sampled by the eyes can open up new perspectives onto several Estimating the sampling rate of reading. Fifty healthy volun-
fundamental questions related to the process of reading, includ- teers read sentences from the Potsdam Sentence Corpus (144
ing whether reading is related to spoken language processing (as sentences presented as a whole; 1,138 words in total10) while move-
recent investigations of word-per-minute measures suggest16) and ments of their right eye were tracked (resolution, 1,000 Hz). As a
whether the visual system’s sampling of linguistic input differs from non-linguistic control task, the participants scanned ‘z-strings’

1
Department of Psychology, Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 2Center for Individual Development and Adaptive Education
of Children at Risk (IDeA), Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 3Department of Linguistics, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria. 4Centre for Cognitive
Neuroscience, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria. 5Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 6Ernst Struengmann
Institute for Neuroscience, Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 7Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, USA. 8Max-Planck-NYU
Center for Language, Music, and Emotion (CLaME), Frankfurt am Main, Germany. 9Brain Imaging Center, Goethe University Frankfurt,Frankfurt am Main,
Germany. ✉e-mail: benjamin.gagl@univie.ac.at

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 429


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour

that were constructed by replacing all letters of the sentence with these cases. Note that estimating sampling rates from the mean
the letter ‘z’ (for example, ‘Ein berühmter Maler hat sich selbst (rather than the mode) of fixation durations results in lower rates
ein Ohr abgeschnitten’ / ‘A famous painter cut off his own ear’ for reading (4.5 Hz) and scanning (3.7 Hz). This results from an
was transformed to ‘Zzz zzzzzzzzz Zzzzz zzz zzzz zzzzzz zzz Zzz overestimation of the central tendency by the mean in right-skewed
zzzzzzzzzzzzz’; see Methods for the details and ref. 17 for previous distributions (Fig. 1c) and indicates that this procedure would
results from this dataset). Given that fixation numbers do not differ be inadequate.
significantly between sentences and z-strings17–19, similar scan paths Finally, power spectra of reading versus z-string scanning were
are assumed, which qualifies z-strings as valid control stimuli for estimated using canonical frequency analysis. For each task, we cre-
reading experiments (Please find multiple analyses that account for ated a time series (resolution, 1,000 Hz) starting with the first sac-
potential differences related to scan path characteristics in results cade of the first participant and ending with the last fixation of the
section below). last participant, with a ‘1’ at the exact time of saccade onset and a
‘0’ elsewhere. Note that saccade onsets are the appropriate event for
Fixation durations. After preprocessing (leading to the removal of generating this time series, as they are the re-occurring event and can
3.1% of the data), we estimated mean fixation durations separately be measured with high accuracy22. Subsequently, the power spectra
for each participant and experimental condition. Figure 1a shows of these task-specific event time courses were estimated via Fourier
that fixation durations (presented here as subject-specific means) transform to visualize periodic signal components across subjects
are shorter for reading than for scanning (average, 197 ms versus (Methods). Corroborating the results of the first analysis approach,
249 ms, respectively; t(49) = 11.1; P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 1.25; 95% a prominent peak was found at 5 Hz for reading and a somewhat less
confidence interval (CI), 0.9 to 1.6). This has been reported previ- pronounced peak at ~4 Hz for scanning (Fig. 1e). To compare these
ously for this dataset17 and replicates earlier results for German18, estimates between reading and scanning, we next estimated separate
English20 and French19 in which fixation durations increased from power spectra for each participant. Individual peaks were retrieved,
reading to scanning by 38–42 ms. averaged (Fig. 1f) and statistically compared. This analysis repro-
duces the sampling frequencies estimated from the mode of fixation
Saccade probability. As a first characterization of rhythmic durations, with frequencies of 5.0 Hz and 4.4 Hz for reading and
eye-movement patterns during reading, we plotted the probabil- scanning, respectively (t(49) = −7.9; P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = −0.9;
ity that a saccade occurs for each sample point after stimulus onset 95% CI, −1.1 to −0.6). There was a high correlation between the two
(Fig. 1b). This analysis demonstrates distinct peaks visible at regular analysis approaches (reading: t(48) = 9.3; P < 0.001; r = 0.80; 95%
intervals, providing evidence that eye movements follow a rhythmic CI, 0.7 to 0.9; scanning: t(48) = 5.5; P < 0.001; r = 0.62; 95% CI, 0.4
structure in both reading and scanning. Importantly, this rhythmic to 0.8), which underscores the validity of sampling-duration-based
pattern is more pronounced and faster during reading. Dominant frequency estimations.
sampling rates were estimated directly from fixation durations, as To summarize, a quantitative frequency-domain characteriza-
well as using classical frequency analysis. While the former approach tion of eye-tracking data shows that the predominant sampling
is important because fixation durations are also the basis for the frequency during reading in German, across participants, is ~5 Hz.
subsequent meta-analysis, the latter approach allows us to evaluate This frequency representation of the reading process falls squarely
the validity of fixation-duration-based frequency estimation. within the boundaries of the predominant modulation frequencies
of 4.3–5.5 Hz determined for speech signals across languages5,6,
Sampling rates. To estimate sampling rates from fixation durations, which in turn have a clear reflection in the neuronal response to
we first estimated sampling periods T (that is, the time from the speech3. We observed the ~5 Hz peak during reading using two dif-
start of a saccade to the start of the next) by adding to each fixa- ferent analysis strategies—that is, when estimating sampling fre-
tion duration (n = 112,547) the duration of the preceding saccade. quencies from saccade and fixation durations and when analysing
Figure 1c shows the distribution of all sampling periods across par- the sequence of saccade events in the frequency domain. Attentive
ticipants, separately for reading and z-string scanning. Note that scanning of z-strings shows highly similar scan-path character-
due to the ex-Gaussian distribution of fixation durations typical for istics compared to reading18,19 but a significantly lower sampling
fixation duration data21, the mean (dashed line) overestimates the frequency at ~4.2 Hz, convergent with findings from non-linguistic
central tendency, whereas the mode (solid line) by definition is a attentional reorienting tasks15,23.
better representation of the predominant sampling period (Fig. 1c). An analysis of the pupil response in this same dataset had previ-
Next, we estimated an eye movement sampling frequency f for each ously indicated higher cognitive effort during reading than during
participant and condition, by dividing 1 s by the subject-specific z-string scanning17. This finding most likely reflects the additional
mode of the sampling period in seconds. This revealed a higher involvement of reading-specific and linguistic processes, such as
average sampling rate for reading (5.0 Hz) than for the control lexical–semantic access, beyond the oculomotor sampling itself.
task (4.2 Hz; Fig. 1d). This difference was significant (t(49) = −8.2; The specific sampling rate observed for reading is thus unlikely to
P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = −1.27; 95% CI, −1.7 to −0.9), and 45 of 50 be driven exclusively by (perceptual or cognitive) features of the
participants showed a numeric reduction in sampling frequency stimulus. In light of the overlap with the rate of spoken language, we
from reading to scanning (grey lines in Fig. 1d). We find virtually tentatively propose that the observed sampling rate of ~5 Hz may
the same pattern of effects when regressive saccades are removed reflect functional constraints imposed by the interfaced nature that
(that is, when analysing only single fixation cases; 4.9 Hz and 4.2 Hz the process of reading has between visual and linguistic processing
for reading and scanning, respectively; t(49) = −6.9; P < 0.001; (which developed primarily on the basis of spoken language). We
Cohen’s d = −1.13; 95% CI, −1.6 to −0.7) and only slightly higher speculate that the brain’s language systems impose the cortical rate
values when restricting the analyses to inter-word re-fixations (that at which speech is produced and perceived onto oculomotor pro-
is, fixations after regressive saccades; 5.2 Hz versus 4.6 Hz, respec- gramming systems exclusively during reading, possibly to optimize
tively; t(48) = −5.5; P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = −0.8; 95% CI, −1.1 to language-related information processing.
−0.4; note that one participant was excluded due to the absence of This hypothesis predicts that the overlap of reading and speech
regressive saccades in the scanning task). Sampling rates of reading rates should generalize across languages and writing systems.
and scanning are thus highly similar between forward-oriented and However, writing systems differ substantially between languages11,24,
regressive eye movements (t(96) = 6.7; P < 0.001; r = 0.6; 95% CI, 0.4 and even within writing systems, the mapping from orthography to
to 0.7). Therefore, all further analyses do not differentiate between meaning differs between languages25. For example, the letter a in

430 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
a 400
b
0.0100

Fixation duration (ms)


300

Saccade probability
0.0075

200
0.0050 Reading

Scanning
100 0.0025

0 0
Reading Scanning 0 200 400 600 800 1,000
Task Time relative to the first saccade
of a sentence or z-string (ms)

c d
* Mode Fixation-duration-based
0.006 + Mean estimation

* 6

Individual eye movement


sampling frequency (Hz)
0.004
+
Density

Reading
4
Scanning
0.002
2

0 0
0 200 400 600 800 Reading Scanning
Saccade + fixation durations (ms) Task

e Reading Scanning f Power-spectral-based


estimation
Individual eye movement
sampling frequency (Hz)

6
Power spectral

1 × 10–5
density (a.u.)

5 × 10–6 2

0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Reading Scanning
Frequency (Hz) Task

Fig. 1 | Reading-related sampling rates. a, Subject-specific mean fixation durations from 50 participants (dots), the overall means (circle) and CIs
(coloured bars) while reading sentences on the Potsdam sentence corpus10 and scanning z-strings. The lines connect reading with z-string scanning data,
per subject, to visualize effects at the single-subject level. The violin plots show the distributions of individual means (blue indicates scanning, and green
indicates reading; similar in d and f). b, Mean saccade probability (across all participants and stimuli, separated by task) relative to the first saccade of
the sentence, with a nonlinear regression line. c, The sampling period T of one event was defined as the duration of a fixation plus its preceding saccade.
Displayed is the distribution of these sampling periods for sentence reading (green) and z-string scanning (blue), with estimated means (plus symbols and
dashed lines) and modes (asterisks and solid lines). d, Subject-specific mean sampling frequencies f (that is, equal to 1/T) and the overall means (crossed
circles) based on the sampling periods shown in c. e, Power spectra for reading and z-string scanning, estimated across all participants using Fourier
transform analysis. f, Individual peak frequencies estimated from individual power spectra and their means (crossed circles). See Methods for details.

cat versus ball maps onto two different speech sounds in English, Cross-linguistic meta-analysis of reading rates. To investigate
whereas it maps onto the same sound in the German translations the language generality of the alignment between speech and read-
of these words (Katze versus Ball). This letter-to-sound correspon- ing rates, we conducted a meta-analysis of sampling frequencies
dence strongly influences reading acquisition26, so that among the during reading in 14 different languages, based on 1,420 fixation
alphabetic writing systems, opaque orthographies (writing systems duration estimates extracted from 124 original studies published
like English with inconsistent letter-to-sound correspondences) are between 2006 and 2016 (see Methods for the selection criteria). In
associated with lower reading accuracy during the first years of learn- addition to this cross-linguistic comparison, we examined possible
ing to read. These differences would be suggestive of cross-linguistic differences between character-based and alphabetic writing sys-
differences in the frequency at which written text can be sampled, tems, as well as the effect of letter-to-sound correspondence among
and recent experimental evidence (such as the observation of longer alphabetic writing systems. We also explored the cross-linguistic
fixation durations for Chinese than for Finnish or English11) seems correlation between eye movement sampling frequencies and
to support this prediction. language-specific peaks of the speech modulation spectra, and the

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 431


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour

400

300
Fixation duration (ms)

200

100

All Arabic Chinese French Polish Italian English Spanish Thai Japanese Hebrew Korean Finnish German Dutch

+ 1 s.d. range of speech rate


7
Eye movement sampling frequency (Hz)

6
Mean range of speech rate

All Arabic Chinese French Polish Italian English Spanish Thai Hebrew Japanese Korean Finnish German Dutch

Fig. 2 | Meta-analysis of reading-related sampling rates. Fixation durations (top) and corresponding eye movement sampling frequencies (bottom) for
14 different languages. The violin plots (left) represent the respective distributions of all 1,420 duration or frequency values extracted from the included
studies, independent of language. The bars reflect CIs, and the circles reflect the means. In the right panel, each dot reflects one study (mean number of
fixation durations per study, 12.4); the bars reflect CIs, and the circles reflect the mean across studies for each language. In the lower panel, the dashed
lines represent the range of the means of the peak amplitude modulation spectrum that was empirically determined for speech in different languages in
independent work5,6. The dotted lines represent the range between the lowest mean minus one standard deviation and the highest mean plus one standard
deviation for the same data (which was manually read out from figure in ref. 5 and from figure 7 in ref. 6). For Arabic, 1 study and 12 fixation durations
are available; for Chinese, 20 and 205; for Dutch, 5 and 45; for English, 65 and 965; for Finnish, 3 and 21; for French, 2 and 3; for German, 14 and 48; for
Hebrew, 3 and 28; for Italian, 1 and 1; for Japanese, 2 and 12; for Korean, 2 and 39, for Polish, 1 and 1; for Spanish, 4 and 10; and for Thai, 3 and 30.

association between reading rates and information density (linguis- saccade duration from Study 1; 29 ms) to the mean fixation dura-
tic information per syllable27) across languages. tion. Finally, the sampling frequency was calculated as f = 1/T.
All studies selected for inclusion reported mean fixation dura-
tions. However, as discussed above, mean fixation durations are Fixation durations and sampling frequency: descriptive statistics.
not a valid representation of the predominant sampling duration in Figure 2 shows that the majority of mean fixation durations derived
fixation data and accordingly not the preferred basis for calculating from the reading studies were between 200 and 300 ms (upper
the sampling rate of reading. We used 29 full empirical datasets to panel), which transforms to mean sampling frequencies between
develop a transformation function that allowed us to estimate the 3.9 and 5.2 Hz (lower panel). Note that languages with only one
mode from the mean fixation durations reported in the original original study (Arabic, Italian and Polish) were excluded from
publications. In brief, this involved fitting ex-Gaussian distributions descriptive and further statistical analysis. As expected, the major-
to the empirical distributions of these datasets, retrieving distri- ity of studies were conducted in English28. Ten of the 14 languages
butional parameters (including mean and mode) and on this basis in our meta-analysis fall between the minimum (4.3 Hz) and the
optimizing a regression-based transformation that estimates the maximum (5.5 Hz) of previously reported5,6 language-specific peaks
mode from the mean (see Methods and Supplementary Methods for of the speech amplitude modulation spectra (Fig. 2, lower panel,
the details). For the meta-analysis, mean durations were extracted dashed lines). The remaining four languages fell within the range of
from published studies and transformed to the mode. The sampling one standard deviation around the language-specific speech peaks
period T (the interval from saccade onset to the end of the following (Fig. 2, lower panel, dotted lines). Considering the language-specific
fixation; see above) was obtained by adding an estimate (the mode CIs, only for Chinese can we be confident that the sampling rate

432 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
a ference, −0.70 Hz; s.e. = 0.13; see Methods for the details on linear
6 mixed-effects modelling).

Effect of orthographic complexity on sampling frequency. Within


alphabetic languages, one plausible hypothesis is that the ortho-
sampling frequency (Hz)

4 graphic difficulty of writing systems influences the speed of sam-


Eye movement

pling the visual input29. To examine this, we quantified orthographic


difficulty as a continuous predictor representing the number of
grapheme-to-phoneme conversion rules30 as defined by compu-
2
tationally implemented dual-route models of visual word recog-
nition31. Graphemes are letters or letter combinations that map
onto one or multiple speech sounds (phonemes); for illustration,
remember the above example of mapping the grapheme a onto one
0
(Katze/Ball) versus two (cat/ball) phonemes, requiring one ver-
sus two rules. To date, computational implementations are avail-
Alphabet Character able for only five of the nine alphabetic languages included in this
meta-analysis, which restricts this test to English, French, German
b and Dutch (with n = 965, 3, 48 and 45 data points, respectively;
6
Italian, the fifth language, was excluded due to the lack of suffi-
cient data points). A detailed comparison of these language-specific
model implementations can be found in ref. 25. Figure 3b dem-
sampling frequency (Hz)

onstrates that less transparent writing systems (operationalized


4
as more grapheme-to-phoneme rules) elicit significantly lower
Eye movement

sampling frequencies (t = 3.0; Est = −0.10 Hz; s.e. = 0.03). Highly


transparent orthographies (such as German and Dutch) produce
Dutch
relatively fast sampling rates around 5 Hz (Fig. 3b).
2
English
Effects of speech rate and information density. Lastly, we explored
French the effect of cross-linguistic differences in speech rate5,6 and infor-
German mation density (information per syllable27) on the observed eye
0 movement sampling rates (Methods). To control for the strong
100 150 200 250 300 350 effects of orthographic differences on sampling rates (see above),
Number of grapheme-to-phoneme rules
linear mixed models were calculated that also included the fac-
tor alphabetic versus character-based script (t > 4; Est < −0.57 Hz;
Fig. 3 | Comparison of writing systems. a, Character versus alphabetic s.e. < 0.15). Neither the between-language differences in speech fre-
contrast, including 205 fixation durations from 20 Chinese reading quencies (t = 0.8; Est = −0.03; s.e. = 0.03) nor those in information
studies (orange) and 1,215 fixation durations from 97 studies of reading density (t = 0.7; Est = −0.03; s.e. = 0.05) showed significant effects
in alphabetic languages (grey). b, The effect of language transparency/ on the eye movement sampling rate (all analyses included Chinese,
opacity. Only studies from alphabetic languages for which the number Dutch, English, French, Japanese and Spanish).
of grapheme-to-phoneme rules could be quantitatively estimated from We further investigated the relationship between speech and
published computational models (Methods) were used (four languages reading rates within alphabetic languages for which estimates of
with a total of n = 1,025 fixation durations). The dots reflect each study, orthographic complexity (grapheme-to-phoneme rules) could be
and the crossed circles reflect the mean across studies for each language. taken into account (English, French and Dutch). This analysis also
The dashed line in b represents the approximation of the language failed to produce significant effects (t = 1.0; Est = 0.06; s.e. = 0.06).
transparency/opacity effect based on a linear regression. Still, the result indicated a positive relationship between peak
speech modulation rate and eye movement sampling rate. Note that
we report this analysis despite its low statistical power (with only
is lower than the range of the speech amplitude modulation spec- three languages) to motivate future investigations of the relation-
tra5,6. Of the 1,420 individual sampling rate values derived from the ship between speech and reading rates.
studies included in our meta-analysis, only 3.0% fell below and only The meta-analysis (1) replicates the results obtained for German
0.3% were above the range of one standard deviation of the mean in the first section, (2) shows that the eye movement sampling fre-
(violin plot in the lower panel of Fig. 2). Nevertheless, the mean quencies of most languages fall into the range of the peaks of speech
sampling rate of reading observed when averaging across all lan- amplitude modulation spectra (4.3–5.5 Hz)5,6 determined in inde-
guages is at the lower bound of the speech modulation range—that pendent research and (3) shows a systematic modulation of read-
is, at 4.3 Hz (Fig. 2, lower panel). ing rates by the perceptual difficulty of orthographic systems. We
found similar average sampling rates for languages of comparable
Effect of writing system on sampling frequency. The observed orthographic transparency levels (for example, German and Dutch)
cross-linguistic differences are arguably related to different language and the highest reading rates (~5 Hz) in transparent (that is, rela-
characteristics. One plausible hypothesis is that the higher percep- tively easy-to-process) writing systems. Our tentative proposal that
tual complexity of character-based scripts (as opposed to alphabetic the linguistic processing systems underlying speech production and
scripts24) may modulate the rate at which written text is sampled. comprehension provide the temporal frame that ‘drives’ the oculo-
Figure 3a shows that the eye movement sampling frequency is sig- motor machinery during reading would predict a direct relation-
nificantly lower for Chinese (the only character-based language ship between the rate of speech production and the sampling rate
included; n = 205 estimated sampling rates from 20 studies; mean, of reading. In the present meta-analysis, this proposal could only be
3.9 Hz) than for alphabetic languages (n = 1,215 sampling rates from tested cross-linguistically and using highly aggregated data, and we
97 studies; mean, 4.5 Hz; t = −5.2; effect size estimate (Est) of dif- found no robust support for this proposal. However, these analyses

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 433


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour

included small sample sizes, as they were limited by the number of a


languages included. To investigate this proposal in more detail, we 0
next conducted two studies that examine the existence of associa-

Speech modulation index


tions between speech and reading rates at a subject-by-subject level.

(amplitude)
−5
Association of individual speech and reading rates. We tested
the correlation between peaks in the speech modulation spectra of
individual speakers and their eye movement sampling rates during
−10
reading in two experiments. First, we tested 48 learners of German
(Study 3), as we expected to observe higher variabilities in both
measures in non-native language learners than in native speakers32
and a more direct relationship between speaking and reading (simi- 0.1 1.0 10.0
lar to letter-by-letter reading in beginning readers33). We recorded Rate (Hz)
eye movements from each participant while reading German sen-
tences (implemented analogously to the reading task in Study 1) b
and a speech sample based on a ‘small-talk interview’ (22 questions,
on average 18 min of speech per participant; range, 6 to 28 min).

Individual frequency (Hz)


We also statistically controlled for individual differences in read- 4
ing proficiency by adding a standard measure of reading skill34,35
(Methods). The eye movement sampling frequency was esti-
mated on the basis of the fixation durations (that is, as in Study 1),
2
and the speech modulation spectrum was examined analogous to
previous reports6.
Figure 4a shows the average speech modulation spectrum across
participants (black line), with a peak at 4.2 Hz, and individual spectra 0
from all participants (grey lines). As expected for language learners, Reading Speech
the peak of the spectrum was below that of native speakers (com-
pare Fig. 1) and on the lower border of the cross-linguistic range of c d
mean speech rates5,6 (compare Fig. 2). Nevertheless, all participants 5 5
produced peaks between 3 Hz and 6 Hz (Fig. 4b)—that is, within
Individual eye movement

Individual eye movement


4 4
the previously reported5,6 range of the standard deviations around
frequency (Hz)

the language-specific mean peaks (dotted line in Fig. 4c). The peaks

frequency (Hz)
3 3
of individual speech modulation spectra and eye movement sam-
2 2
pling frequencies were in a comparable range (Fig. 4b; confirmed
by a significant equivalence test36: t(47) = −1.8, P = 0.04; equiva- 1 1
lence bounds, ±0.33) and positively correlated (Fig. 4c; t(45) = 2.1;
0 0
P = 0.04; Est = 0.32; s.e. = 0.15). Note that this correlation effect was
3 4 5 20 40 60
estimated while controlling for individual differences in reading
Individual speech Individual reading
proficiency (Fig. 4d; t(45) = 2.1; P = 0.04; Est = 0.032; s.e. = 0.016) by frequency speed (%
calculating a linear model that estimates the individual eye move- (Hz) sentences read)
ment sampling rate with speech modulation rate as a predictor.
In a second, preregistered study (Study 4), we assessed the Fig. 4 | Relationship of speech and reading rates in non-native German
relationship between speech modulation spectrum peaks and eye speakers. a, Speech modulation spectrum from 48 non-native speakers
movement sampling frequencies in a group of 86 native speakers of German. The y axis shows the speech modulation index6; the x axis
of German (Methods; preregistration: https://osf.io/mjhkz). We shows the speech modulation rate. For additional comparison, we present
replicated the finding that the peaks of the speech modulation the mean range (dashed lines) and standard deviations (dotted lines) of
spectra and eye movement sampling frequencies were in the same the speech amplitude modulation spectra across languages, which were
range (Fig. 5a; equivalence tests for left and right eye: t(85) > 3.9; read out from figure 3c in ref. 5 and from figure 7 in ref. 6. b, Eye movement
P < 0.001; equivalence bounds, ±0.33). The preregistered correlation sampling frequency in reading and the mean amplitude modulation
analysis showed a small positive (albeit not significant) relation- spectrum in speech, for each participant. The lines connect the reading
ship between eye movement sampling and speech modulation rates and speech frequencies of each individual, the violin plots represent the
(t(175) = 1.8; P = 0.08; Est = 0.07; s.e. = 0.04). For further explo- distribution of the data, the bars represent the standard error of the mean
ration (that is, non-registered post-hoc analysis), we separately and the circles reflect the mean. c, Positive correlation of the individual peaks
investigated and compared four subgroups created by a 2 × 2 com- of the speech modulation spectrum (x axis), reflecting each participant’s
bination of reading speed and reading accuracy. Specifically, we speech rate, with the eye movement frequency (y axis) from the same
implemented a median split based on reading speed measured with participants. d, Correlation between the eye movement frequency (y axis)
a standardized German reading test (the adult version of the SLS35; and a paper-and-pencil-based reading score (x axis) reflecting a positive
fast versus slow: median, 78% versus 60%) and, orthogonal to this, association of the eye movement sampling rate and reading performance.
divided the sample on the basis of their sentence comprehension In c and d, we present the individual sampling frequencies corrected for
accuracies in the eye-tracking experiment (errors present versus reading skill and speech frequency, respectively, based on predictions from
absent: median, 0% versus 15%). Only readers with the lowest skill the fitted linear regression models used for statistical analysis.
level (that is, slow and low comprehension performance) showed a
robust positive association (n = 21; Fig. 5b, bottom left; t(37) = 3.4;
P = 0.002; Est = 0.33; s.e. = 0.10). None of the other groups showed present when the percentage of regressions, skipping and single fix-
a significant correlation, resulting in a reliable interaction effect ation probabilities (Table 1) were added as covariates to the model.
(t(162) = 2.8; P = 0.005; Est = −0.05; s.e. = 0.02), which was also Note that the low-reading-skill group had a lower reading speed

434 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
a b
Errors No errors
6 6

Individual eye movement frequency (Hz)


4

Fast
Individual frequency (Hz)

2
4

0
6

2
4

Slow
2

0
0
Reading, left eye Reading, right eye Speech 3 4 5 6 3 4 5 6
Individual speech frequency (Hz)

Fig. 5 | Relationship of speech and reading rates in 86 native German speakers. a, Eye movement sampling frequency measured during reading for the
left and right eyes (left and centre plots) and the mean amplitude modulation spectrum of samples of spoken speech (right plot). The grey dots represent
individual data points from all participants; the lines connect the reading and speech frequencies of each participant. The violin plots represent the
distribution of the data, the filled bars represent the standard error of the mean and the circles reflect the mean. b, The correlation between the speech
modulation spectrum (x axis) and the eye movement sampling frequency (y axis). The four panels represent performance subgroups depending on reading
speed (slow versus fast; median split) and whether the participants produced errors in an independent standardized reading test (see Methods for details).

Table 1 | Reading speed, reading comprehension and basic eye tracking measures (fixation durations, skipping probability, single
fixation cases and percentage of regressions) for Study 3 and Study 4
Study 3 Study 4, fast with no Study 4, fast with Study 4, slow with no Study 4, slow with
errors errors errors errors
Reading speed (SLS test; % 26.0 (10.4) 79.0 (6.5) 78.9 (5.3) 58.0 (11.1) 53.6 (9.0)
sentences read)
Reading speed (experiment; in s) 4.8 (2.3) 2.4 (0.7) 2.2 (0.8) 2.4 (0.5) 2.7 (0.6)
Reading comprehension 20 (40) 0 (0) 17 (4.4) 0 (0) 19 (14.5)
(experiment; % errors)
Fixation duration (right eye, in ms) 223 (29) 192 (20) 188 (15) 196 (21) 198 (20)
Fixation duration (left eye, in ms) 225 (30) 192 (20) 189 (15) 197 (21) 199 (20)
Skipping (%) 7.5 (5.8) 19.8 (7.9) 24.0 (10.8) 15.7 (6.9) 14.7 (6.6)
Single fixation cases (%) 35.5 (14.6) 52.3 (9.1) 54.1 (10.2) 56.6 (9.2) 52.7 (9.4)
Regressions (%) 10.9 (4.5) 12.1 (5.3) 9.3 (6.9) 8.6 (4.9) 10.6 (6.0)
For Study 4, the data are presented separately for the four performance-based subgroups. All values reflect means and standard deviations. Statistical comparisons of the four groups from Study 4 (2 × 2,
linear regression models with an error-by-speed-group interaction) yielded a significant main effect of reading speed group (slow versus fast) for SLS-based reading speed (estimate, 0.53; s.e. = 0.22;
t(84) = 2.4; P = 0.017; all other effects, t < 1.7) and an obvious group difference on the percentage of errors (0 versus ~18% errors). For the eye-tracking measures, the only significant effect was a main
effect of reading speed group on the percentage of skipping (estimate, −0.09; s.e. = 0.03; t(84) = 3.4; P < 0.001); no other main effect or interaction effect reached significance (all t < 1.94).

than the slow-only group that produced no errors (t(86) = 18.1; movements sample text at a higher rate than during comparable,
P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 2.7; 95% CI, 2.1 to 3.3) but still had a sub- cognitively less challenging non-linguistic tasks. More impor-
stantially higher reading speed than the non-native readers from tantly, we demonstrate that the sampling frequency of reading lies
Study 3 (t(90) = 26.3; P < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 5.5; 95% CI, 4.6 to 6.4; within the range of previously observed speech rates for one lan-
for general eye-movement characteristics of both experiments, guage, German. Next, by integrating data from 124 empirical stud-
see Table 1). In sum, we could not find the correlation between ies across languages, we show that eye movement sampling varies
reading and speaking rates in the entire sample, but we found tenta- between ~3.9 Hz and ~5.2 Hz, indicating a higher variability than
tive evidence for the correlation when we restricted the group to previously assumed. While it was generally believed that average
native speakers with low reading skills. fixation durations are similar even for very distinct orthographies
such as Chinese and English (Rayner12, p. 1461), our meta-analytic
Discussion results show significantly higher sampling rates for alphabetic than
This frequency-based investigation of eye movements during read- for character-based writing systems. However, average speech rates
ing shows that reading operates in a generally comparable fre- have been shown to vary more narrowly around 5 Hz across lan-
quency domain as the production and perception of natural speech. guages (that is, 4.3–5.4 Hz in ref. 5 and 4.3–5.5 Hz in ref. 6), a range
We first reproduced, in a frequency-domain analysis, previous that would exclude the lower frequencies we observed for reading.
insights based on fixation duration measures17–19—that is, that eye Our meta-analytic findings indicate that this might result from

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 435


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour

differences in the complexity of the underlying orthographies (for reading33, which suggests that low-skilled readers sample written
example, character versus alphabetic), such that more computation- text with a temporal resolution close to the speech processing rate.
ally ‘difficult’ orthographies might slow down reading relative to In contrast, the faster reading rates of fluent readers indicate that
highly transparent alphabetic orthographies. they utilize the static nature of text better by processing the fix-
Subsequently, we demonstrate in two independent empirical ated word and following words (based on parafoveal vision) within
studies that second-language learners (of German) read in a lower one ‘sample’.
frequency range than native readers (~4.3 Hz versus ~4.7 Hz) and The specific characteristics of fixation behaviour and text pre-
that only language learners and low-skilled native readers show a sentation during reading can also provide context for another
positive correlation between individual reading and speech rates. intriguing phenomenon—that is, the significantly lower sampling
Combined, these results suggest that reading (that is, an internally rates in character-based than in alphabetic writing systems, while
controlled visual–perceptual process involving sophisticated ocu- overall reading times for sentences with the same content are com-
lomotor programming) is remarkably well temporally aligned with parable between the writing systems11. In character-based lan-
the rate at which spoken language is produced (and perceived). We guages, fewer fixations per sentence are needed to sample the entire
tentatively suggest that this observed association between speech stimulus, while the increased perceptual complexity and informa-
and reading supports the existence of fundamental perceptual prin- tion density11 lead to longer fixation durations relative to alphabetic
ciples underlying the temporal structure of linguistic information languages. In the non-linguistic control task, the participants were
processing, irrespective of modality16. presented with stimuli consisting of many repetitions of the same
Text is a temporally stable visual stimulus. However, our eye letter. In this case, information density and perceptual complexity
movements impose temporal structure onto the linguistic input are low compared with all real-language stimuli. Nevertheless, we
when sequentially sampling a text. The reading process—includ- observed longer fixation durations in the z-string scanning task,
ing the oculomotor programmes—thus serves as an interface which may indicate the presence of qualitatively different cognitive
between a stable external percept and linguistic processing sys- processes compared with reading.
tems optimized for analysing sequential speech input. The obser- The frequency representation of reading-related eye-tracking
vation of faster sampling rates during reading than during parsing data that we advance here can be construed as nothing but a
non-linguistic letter strings (Study 1) indicates that sampling rates transformation of fixation and saccade duration data. This trans-
are not exclusively driven by the physical layout of the stimulus or formation also comes at the cost of zooming out to a ‘meso-level’
by the cognitive effort of processing the stimulus (in which case representation of the data1, at which we rely on aggregated data
they should have been slower). We tentatively propose that neural (that is, one data point per participant), which is against the trend
processors dedicated to the linguistic analysis of speech impose in eye-movement research of focusing on investigating single words
their preferred timing onto the process of reading. Evidence and using regression methods for detailed analysis of, for example,
for the principled possibility of such internally driven entrain- the influence of word characteristics52. Still, the frequency perspec-
ment of reading comes from the observation that manipulating tive proposed here provides a view of component processes of read-
the speed of ‘inner speech’ during reading has a causal effect on ing as an interface between linguistic and orthographic processing.
reading speed37–39. This approach to reading research opens up several interesting new
Our meta-analysis demonstrates that the sampling rate of read- research questions. For example, it becomes possible to compare
ing varies between languages but falls within the range of speech reading behaviour more directly with evidence from other measure-
rates identified in cross-linguistic studies5,6. The meta-analysis ment modalities (such as oscillatory brain activation data53,54) and
also shows higher sampling rates for transparent than for opaque to other cognitive–psychological domains (such as attention15,55),
orthographies, which converges with transparency effects within which typically do not have the advantage of exact duration mea-
languages29,40 and cross-linguistic studies investigating reading surements for different events of interest (for example, during covert
development26,41,42. Direct associations between reading and speech attention). Maybe most importantly, the frequency perspective on
rates could not be established in the meta-analysis given the small reading offers direct links to several neurodynamic phenomena in
number of languages for which all necessary parameters were speech perception5,6, including the observation that dyslexic chil-
available. Empirical Studies 3 and 4 show this relationship on a dren56,57 and adults58 show altered cortical tracking of speech signals
subject-by-subject level, but only in less-skilled readers. This sug- in the oscillatory domain.
gests that increasing reading expertise makes the tight control of In conclusion, we show that during reading, our eyes ‘sample’
reading by linguistic processors in the brain obsolete. written text in the same frequency range in which speech is pro-
The differential coupling of speech and reading rates in low-skilled duced and perceived, which suggests that extracting information
but not high-skilled readers may also result from other phenomena from linguistic stimuli follows a similar temporal structure irrespec-
well-established in reading research, such as word skipping, parafo- tive of modality. A plausible mechanism is to assume that linguistic
veal preprocessing and re-fixations. Reading is not merely a sequence processing has a preferred cortical rate of information uptake and
of word-to-word fixations. From time to time, we skip words as a thus acts as an internal temporal driver for eye movements elic-
result of parafoveal preprocessing43,44, which describes visual word ited during reading. Eye movements in reading are thus utilized as
recognition based on low-acuity visual information from parafoveal a temporal interface between a stable physical stimulus—written
regions of the retina. Also, words are sometimes fixated multiple text—and brain systems that have evolved to process signals whose
times—for example, to correct perceptual errors after suboptimal temporal structure is constrained by the characteristics of our vocal
landing at the beginning or end of a word14,18,45 or when semantic tract1. However, our empirical data also demonstrate that a direct
inconsistencies must be resolved by re-reading46,47. We14 and others10 coupling between speech and reading rates is present only in per-
have shown that overall probabilities for word skipping and multiple sons with low reading skills, which calls for future work to clarify
fixations on a word are comparable (~20%) when reading the sen- the mediating role of reading expertise in the temporal relation-
tence materials used here. However, low-skilled readers show lower ship between speech processing and reading rates. We suggest that
skipping rates (reflecting reduced parafoveal preprocessing48–50) and the frequency perspective on reading adopted here opens up new
more re-fixations on the same word13. Readers with lower skills thus research paths, such as understanding slow or impaired reading,
focus more on fixated words and their components (letters and syl- understanding second language learning or more directly inves-
lables)51, indicating a greater alignment between the phonological tigating the commonalities and differences between reading and
properties of the words and the eye movements they elicit during other cognitive processes.

436 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
Methods durations shorter than 60 ms and longer than 1,000 ms and saccade durations
Participants in Studies 1, 3 and 4. Fifty (13 male; 18–47 years old; mean = 24 longer than 80 ms were removed from the analysis (3.1% of the data) since they
years; students at the University of Salzburg) native speakers of German probably reflect machine error. On the basis of the remaining fixation durations,
participated in Study 1, 49 (13 male; 18–74 years old; mean = 24 years) non-native each participant’s individual mean fixation duration was calculated (separately
German speakers participated in Study 3 and 86 (36 male; 18–53 years old; for the reading and scanning tasks). To account for the well-known fact that
mean = 25 years; 5 had to be excluded on the basis of preregistered outlier eye fixation data have an ex-Gaussian distribution (Fig. 1c), the data were
correction boundaries for both speech and reading rates; ±3 standard deviations) log-transformed, resulting in a normal distribution (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test not
German speakers participated in Study 4 after giving informed consent according significant; D < 0.14; P > 0.7).
to procedures approved by the respective local ethics committee. For Study 1,
see our original publication of this dataset17 for more details. Note that relative Estimation of the sampling frequency. To estimate the sampling frequency of eye
to the previously published report, one participant was added. Participants with movements in reading, a repetitive event needed to be identified. We defined the
varying mother tongues (Arabic, Azerbaijani, Bulgarian, Chinese, English, Farsi, time between the onset of a saccade and the onset of the following saccade as the
French, Georgian, Hungarian, Indonesian, Italian, Japanese, Persian, Russian, sampling period, which can be transformed into a frequency value. Note that we
Serbo-Croatian, Spanish, Turkish, Ukrainian, Urdu and Uzbek) for Study 3 used the EyeLink eye tracker’s built-in saccade detection algorithm, which in a
and native German participants for Study 4 were recruited on the campus of recent comparative evaluation showed the best detection rates for saccade onsets
Goethe University Frankfurt as part of a more extensive study. Also note that six compared with all other algorithms22. The distribution of the sampling period
participants from Study 3 became literate without the acquisition of an alphabetic is ex-Gaussian, for both reading and z-string scanning (Fig. 1c). Ex-Gaussian
script. The power estimation for Study 4 resulted in a sample size of 90 while distributions are a convolution of a normal distribution and an exponential
assuming the effect size from Study 3 and a power of 0.9 (see the preregistration for distribution reflecting the rightward skew. As Fig. 1c shows, the central tendency
more details: https://osf.io/mjhkz). is best represented by the mode, so that all subsequent calculations were based
on the participant-specific mode of the sampling period (which we denote T).
Procedure of Study 1. Movements of the right eye were tracked with a sampling These subject-specific representations of the predominant sampling period were
rate of 1,000 Hz (EyeLink 1000, tower mount system; SR Research). We used a transformed to an individual eye movement sampling frequency (f = 1/T).
forehead and chin rest to fixate the head of participants at a distance of 60 cm from
a 21-inch CRT screen. In the reading task, we used the Potsdam Sentence Corpus10, Power spectrum. We next performed a canonical frequency analysis by estimating
which consists of 144 sentences and a total of 1,138 words. The participants were a power spectrum for reading and scanning in Study 1. For Fig. 1e, we estimated
instructed to read silently for comprehension, which was controlled by simple the power spectrum on the basis of a time series starting with the first saccade of
comprehension questions after 38 of the 144 sentences. the first participant and ending with the last fixation of the final participant for
As a non-linguistic control task, the participants performed a z-string scanning each task. For Fig. 1f, the time series was cut into participant-specific time series,
task using stimuli in which all letters of the sentence corpus were replaced by so that individual peaks could be recovered for each participant for each task. The
the letter z (preserving letter case, punctuation and word boundaries). The time series was implemented as a sparse sequence of zeros and ones (resolution,
participants were instructed to visually scan the meaningless z-strings as if they 1,000 entries per second), set to one at time points at which a saccade was initiated,
were reading; for obvious reasons, no comprehension questions were administered and zero otherwise. Subsequently, a fast Fourier transform was used to estimate a
in this condition. Z-string scanning has been used as a control task in previous power spectrum (power spectral density; psd_welch function from MNE-Python59;
studies17–20. While it is difficult to find a reasonable control task for reading (see, for 0–100 Hz; length of the fast Fourier transform used, 4,096 samples) for each event
example, the Discussion in ref. 18), z-string scanning has proved to be interesting time course separately.
because participants produce similar scan-path patterns (that is, similar numbers
of fixations) as when reading17–19. Interestingly, while z-string scanning produced Speech amplitude modulation spectrum in Studies 3 and 4. In the first step, all
longer mean fixation durations than reading, the pupil response indicated higher non-participant audio signals were removed from the speech samples (that
cognitive effort in reading, in the dataset used here17. We take this dissociation is, interviewer questions and pauses before answers). To obtain the amplitude
between cognitive effort and reading time as evidence for the operation of modulation spectrum, we adapted the AM_FM_Spectra script6 (https://github.
reading-specific cognitive processes that go beyond mere attentional processes. com/LeoVarnet/AM_FM_Spectra). The first adaptation divided the recording of
In each task, a nine-point standard calibration was performed before the ten each participant into 10 s speech segments, resulting in a mean of 110 segments
practice trials, before the experimental trials and after a break halfway through per participant (range, 35 to 167). In Study 4, we used 20 s segments to increase
the experiment. A calibration was considered accurate when the mean error the efficiency of the analysis (mean number of segments, 55; range, 15 to 81). The
was below 0.5° of visual angle. The visual stimuli were presented in black letters second adaptation was an increase in the resolution of the amplitude modulation
(mono-spaced, bold Courier New font; 14 pt; width, ~0.3°) on a white background spectrum by decreasing the widths of the modulation filters from three to ten per
with a 1,024 × 768 pixel resolution and a refresh rate of 120 Hz, using Experiment octave. After the speech amplitude modulation was estimated for each 10 s speech
Builder software (SR Research). In both tasks, a trial started when an eye fixation segment, we retrieved the frequency at the peak of the modulation spectrum.
was found at a dot presented 100 pixels from the left margin of the monitor, at the Thereafter, we removed outliers by first eliminating unrealistic values lower than
horizontal level of the fixation cross. For this fixation check, real-time analysis 2 and higher than 10 Hz and then removing all values larger and smaller than
of eye-tracking data was used to present the sentence only when a fixation of at two standard deviations from the mean. This procedure removed 3% of the data.
least 100 ms was identified on the position of the dot. If no fixation was registered Finally, we estimated the mean across all segments for each participant. Here we
on the dot for 10 s, a recalibration procedure was initiated. After the fixation found that one participant, in both studies, had a mean amplitude modulation
check, the stimulus (that is, a sentence or z-string) appeared, with the centre of spectrum larger than three standard deviations from the mean of the sample; this
the first word presented at the position of the fixation dot. As a consequence, participant was excluded from the analysis.
the participants always fixated the centre of the first word of the sentence first.
Stimulus presentation was terminated when participants fixated an X in the lower Meta-analysis. We included empirical studies that report eye-tracking results
right corner of the screen after the sentence was read. As noted, in about 25% of from natural reading tasks, published between 2006 and 2016. These studies were
sentences, the presentation was followed by a comprehension question to ensure identified by the search term eye movement in ‘natural reading’ or ‘sentence reading’
that the participants had processed the sentences semantically. This procedure was or ‘text reading’ in the PubMed (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed) and
practiced in ten trials before the main experiment. PsychInfo (https://health.ebsco.com/products/psycinfo) databases. Additionally,
ten studies were manually identified (for example, on the basis of reference lists in
Procedure of Study 3 and Study 4. Eye movement measurements during reading the identified papers). From the resulting sample of 124 articles60–183, we extracted
were acquired using the same stimulus materials and experimental procedures 1,420 fixation durations, including mean fixation durations (all fixations on a word
as in Study 1, with three exceptions: we used a desktop-mount eye tracker and combined; 10% of the meta-analytic dataset), first fixation durations (duration
a horizontal three-point calibration procedure, and we did not implement of the first fixation on a word, which is most often reported in eye-tracking
the z-string scanning task. All other parameters were unchanged. For Study studies; 67% of the meta-analytic dataset) and single fixation durations (fixation
4, we measured not only the right eye but also the left eye (that is, binocular duration when a word was fixated only once, which is the predominant case for
measurement). To acquire a speech sample from each participant, we conducted normal readers10; 23% of the meta-analytic dataset). Note that the results of the
a brief interview in German involving 22 questions about, for example, last above-reported experiment (Study 1) and its previous analysis17 were not included.
weekend’s activities (see the full list of questions in the Supplementary Methods). This meta-analytic dataset encompassed 14 languages, with a range from 1 (Arabic,
Speech was recorded with Audacity software (version 2.1.3; https://www. Italian and Polish) to 65 (English) retrieved papers. Consistent with a general bias
audacityteam.org/) on a standard computer. towards English in reading research28, 68% of the fixation durations in our dataset
were from English.
Data analysis. Fixation durations. The first word of each sentence was excluded
from the analyses, since the first word is known to be contaminated by stimulus Frequency estimation. To estimate the predominant sampling frequency per
onset effects. A total of 994 words were analysed per subject. For each participant, published study, we again have to account for the ex-Gaussian distribution of
all fixation durations from all analysed words were extracted. Words with fixation fixation duration data. Following the general trend in the eye movement reading

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 437


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour
literature, most studies reported only mean fixation durations (except ref. 21, for these data publicly available since these interviews contain potentially personal
which the fitted ex-Gaussian parameters were also reported). For the purposes of matters and therefore pose a threat to person-specific data security. However,
the present meta-analysis, we developed a transformation function that allowed the extracted amplitude modulation spectra, which do not include this critical
us to estimate the mode from the mean fixation durations reported in the original information, are available.
publications. This transformation function was then applied to transform the mean
fixation durations extracted from the published original studies into the mode. In Code availability
the final step, the sampling period was determined as the mode fixation duration The analysis scripts used for all studies are available at https://osf.io/96vp8/
plus the mode saccade duration (as estimated from Study 1) and then converted (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/96VP8).
into a frequency value.
In brief (for the details, see the Supplementary Information), the development
of this function involved fitting ex-Gaussian distributions to the empirical Received: 24 May 2019; Accepted: 10 September 2021;
distributions of fixation durations in 29 original datasets to which we had access Published online: 6 December 2021
(and which were not included in the meta-analysis; Supplementary Fig. 1a) and
retrieving distributional parameters for each fitted distribution (specifically, μ, References
the mean of the normally distributed component; σ, its standard deviation; and 1. Poeppel, D. & Assaneo, M. F. Speech rhythms and their neural foundations.
τ, the parameter reflecting the rightward skew, representing the contribution of Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 322–334 (2020).
the exponential distribution; Supplementary Fig. 1b). This allowed us to apply 2. Ding, N., Melloni, L., Zhang, H., Tian, X. & Poeppel, D. Cortical tracking
a regression analysis to predict the mode (dependent variable) from the mean of hierarchical linguistic structures in connected speech. Nat. Neurosci. 19,
fixation duration (predictor variable). Supplementary Fig. 1d presents the final 158–164 (2016).
generalized mean-to-mode transformation function, applied to all possible fixation 3. Luo, H. & Poeppel, D. Phase patterns of neuronal responses reliably
durations in the range covered by the meta-analysis. Supplementary Fig. 1e shows discriminate speech in human auditory cortex. Neuron 54,
how well the modes of our 29 datasets can be recovered by this function: despite 1001–1010 (2007).
some unsystematic noise, the numeric transformation was nearly perfect (that 4. Martin, A. E. & Doumas, L. A. A. A mechanism for the cortical
is, t(28) = 4.1; β = 0.95; s.e. = 0.23). We then used the transformation function computation of hierarchical linguistic structure. PLoS Biol. 15,
to estimate the respective modes from the 1,420 mean fixation durations of the e2000663 (2017).
meta-analysis dataset. To obtain the sampling period T (that is, the interval from the 5. Ding, N. et al. Temporal modulations in speech and music. Neurosci.
onset of a saccade until the end of the following fixation; see also Study 1, above), a Biobehav. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.011 (2017).
saccade duration estimate of 29 ms (that is, the mode of saccade durations from the 6. Varnet, L., Ortiz-Barajas, M. C., Erra, R. G., Gervain, J. & Lorenzi, C. A
reading dataset used in Study 1) was added to each of the mode fixation durations. cross-linguistic study of speech modulation spectra. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 142,
This is feasible because saccade durations do not differ much between persons 1976–1989 (2017).
during reading (see, for example, ref. 184: range, 20–39 ms; mean, 29 ms). Finally, the 7. Goswami, U. & Leong, V. Speech rhythm and temporal structure:
sampling period values (T) were transformed into frequency values (f = 1/T). converging perspectives? Lab. Phonol. 4, 67–92 (2013).
8. Greenberg, S., Carvey, H., Hitchcock, L. & Chang, S. Temporal properties
Writing system comparisons. To explore whether the sampling frequency of reading of spontaneous speech—a syllable-centric perspective. J. Phon. 31, 465–485
is influenced by global characteristics of writing systems and languages, we (2003).
implemented four tests. First, reading Chinese (205 fixation duration data points) 9. Teng, X., Tian, X., Doelling, K. & Poeppel, D. Theta band oscillations reflect
was compared with all alphabetic writing systems (1,215 fixation durations). Note more than entrainment: behavioral and neural evidence demonstrates an
that the Korean (with an alphabetic syllabary orthography185) and Japanese (using active chunking process. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 2770–2782 (2018).
a mixture of Kana and Kanji) studies in the meta-analysis could not be clearly 10. Kliegl, R., Grabner, E., Rolfs, M. & Engbert, R. Length, frequency, and
assigned to the character or alphabet categories and therefore were not included predictability effects of words on eye movements in reading. Eur. J. Cogn.
in this contrast. Second, among the alphabetic scripts, we examined how the Psychol. 16, 262–284 (2004).
differences in the transparency/opaqueness of the letter-to-sound relationship 11. Liversedge, S. P. et al. Universality in eye movements and reading: a
influence reading rates using a continuous predictor representing the number of trilingual investigation. Cognition 147, 1–20 (2016).
grapheme-to-phoneme rules as defined by computationally implemented dual-route 12. Rayner, K. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and
models25. A low number of grapheme-to-phoneme rules reflects a high transparency, visual search. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 1457–1506 (2009).
meaning that letters more consistently represent only one speech sound. For 13. Hawelka, S., Gagl, B. & Wimmer, H. A dual-route perspective on eye
example, Italian, Dutch and German are considered transparent orthographies, movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition 115, 367–379 (2010).
with 59, 104 and 130 rules, respectively25. English and French, in contrast, are 14. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S. & Hutzler, F. A similar correction mechanism in slow
typically considered non-transparent (opaque), with 226 and 340 rules, respectively, and fluent readers after suboptimal landing positions. Front. Hum. Neurosci.
because letters regularly map to multiple speech sounds. Third, we investigated the 8, 355 (2014).
cross-linguistic relationship between variance in the peak modulation spectra from 15. Hogendoorn, H. Voluntary saccadic eye movements ride the attentional
speech and mean sampling frequencies in reading. To this end, we retrieved the rhythm. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 28, 1625–1635 (2016).
modulation spectra for Chinese, Dutch, English, French, Japanese and Spanish from 16. Brysbaert, M. How many words do we read per minute? A review and
Fig. 3c in ref. 5 and from Fig. 7 in ref. 6. The modulation spectra varied from 4.3 Hz in meta-analysis of reading rate. J. Mem. Lang. 109, 104047 (2019).
English to 5.4 Hz in Dutch. Finally, in the fourth test, we investigated the relationship 17. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S. & Hutzler, F. Systematic influence of gaze position on
between eye movement sampling in reading and the information density of a pupil size measurement: analysis and correction. Behav. Res. Methods 43,
language. This parameter indicates how densely a language codes meaning in texts27. 1171–1181 (2011).
The density is coded from 0 to 1 and could be retrieved for a subgroup of languages 18. Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. The IOVP effect in mindless
in the present meta-analysis dataset (Chinese, English, French, German, Italian, reading: experiment and modeling. Vis. Res. 47, 990–1002 (2007).
Japanese and Spanish) from ref. 27. Density varied from dense languages such as 19. Vitu, F., O’Regan, J. K., Inhoff, A. W. & Topolski, R. Mindless reading:
Chinese (0.94) to less dense languages such as Japanese (0.49). eye-movement characteristics are similar in scanning letter strings and
All four effects were analysed using linear mixed models186. In addition to the reading texts. Percept. Psychophys. 57, 352–364 (1995).
parameters of interest, we accounted for experimental settings (experiment-based 20. Rayner, K. & Fischer, M. H. Mindless reading revisited: eye movements
versus corpus-based studies), for the different eye trackers used (which may also during reading and scanning are different. Percept. Psychophys. 58,
imply the use of different saccade detection algorithms) and for different fixation 734–747 (1996).
measures reported (mean, single or first fixation duration) by introducing these 21. Staub, A., White, S. J., Drieghe, D., Hollway, E. C. & Rayner, K.
parameters into the linear mixed models as fixed effects. For the modulation Distributional effects of word frequency on eye fixation durations.
spectrum and information density comparisons, we also added a factor contrasting J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1280–1293 (2010).
character-based (that is, Chinese) with alphabetic writing systems to account for 22. Friedman, L., Rigas, I., Abdulin, E. & Komogortsev, O. V. A novel
perceptual difficulties, and for all four linear mixed models, we estimated the evaluation of two related and two independent algorithms for eye
random effect on the intercept of the study to account for unspecific differences movement classification during reading. Behav. Res. Methods 50,
between studies. All t values larger than 2 were interpreted as significant187. 1374–1397 (2018).
23. Landau, A. N. & Fries, P. Attention samples stimuli rhythmically. Curr. Biol.
Reporting Summary. Further information on research design is available in the 22, 1000–1004 (2012).
Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. 24. Chang, L.-Y., Chen, Y.-C. & Perfetti, C. A. GraphCom: a multidimensional
measure of graphic complexity applied to 131 written languages. Behav. Res.
Data availability Methods https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0881-y (2017).
The data for all studies are available at https://osf.io/96vp8/. We have one 25. Schmalz, X., Marinus, E., Coltheart, M. & Castles, A. Getting to the bottom
restriction—the raw audio of the interviews in Studies 3 and 4. We will not make of orthographic depth. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 1614–1629 (2015).

438 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
26. Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M. & Erskine, J. M., collaboration with COST 55. Fiebelkorn, I. C., Saalmann, Y. B. & Kastner, S. Rhythmic sampling within
Action A8 network. Foundation literacy acquisition in European and between objects despite sustained attention at a cued location. Curr.
orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 94, 143–174 (2003). Biol. 23, 2553–2558 (2013).
27. Pellegrino, F., Coupé, C. & Marsico, E. A cross-language perspective on 56. Di Liberto, G. M. et al. Atypical cortical entrainment to speech in the right
speech information rate. Language 87, 539–558 (2011). hemisphere underpins phonemic deficits in dyslexia. NeuroImage 175,
28. Share, D. L. On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and 70–79 (2018).
practice: the perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychol. Bull. 57. Archer, K., Pammer, K. & Vidyasagar, T. R. A temporal sampling basis for
134, 584–615 (2008). visual processing in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14,
29. Sereno, S. C. & Rayner, K. Spelling-sound regularity effects on eye fixations 213 (2020).
in reading. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 402–409 (2000). 58. Leong, V. & Goswami, U. Impaired extraction of speech rhythm from
30. Protopapas, A. & Vlahou, E. L. A comparative quantitative analysis temporal modulation patterns in speech in developmental dyslexia. Front.
of Greek orthographic transparency. Behav. Res. Methods 41, Hum. Neurosci. 8, 96 (2014).
991–1008 (2009). 59. Gramfort, A. et al. MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data.
31. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R. & Ziegler, J. DRC: a dual NeuroImage 86, 446–460 (2014).
route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. 60. Amenta, S., Marelli, M. & Crepaldi, D. The fruitless effort of growing a
Psychol. Rev. 108, 204–256 (2001). fruitless tree: Early morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic
32. Chen, H.-C. Lexical processing in a non-native language: effects of language effects in sentence reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41,
proficiency and learning strategy. Mem. Cogn. 18, 279–288 (1990). 1587–1596 (2015).
33. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S. & Wimmer, H. On sources of the word length effect 61. Angele, B., Laishley, A. E., Rayner, K. & Liversedge, S. P. The effect of
in young readers. Sci. Stud. Read. 19, 289–306 (2015). high- and low-frequency previews and sentential fit on word
34. Wimmer, H. Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing skipping during reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40,
system. Appl. Psycholinguist. 14, 1–33 (1993). 1181–1203 (2014).
35. Auer, M., Guber, G., Wimmer, H. & Mayringer, H. Salzburger 62. Ashby, J. Prosody in skilled silent reading: evidence from eye movements.
Lese-Screening für die Klassenstufen 1-4 (Hogrefe, 2005). J. Res. Read. 29, 318–333 (2006).
36. Lakens, D. Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and 63. Blythe, H. I., Johnson, R. L., Liversedge, S. P. & Rayner, K. Reading
meta-analyses. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 355–362 (2017). transposed text: effects of transposed letter distance and consonant-vowel
37. Alexander, J. D. & Nygaard, L. C. Reading voices and hearing text: status on eye movements. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76, 2424–2440 (2014).
talker-specific auditory imagery in reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. 64. Blythe, H. I., Pagán, A. & Dodd, M. Beyond decoding: phonological
Perform. 34, 446–459 (2008). processing during silent reading in beginning readers. J. Exp. Psychol.
38. Yao, B. & Scheepers, C. Contextual modulation of reading rate for direct Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 1244–1252 (2015).
versus indirect speech quotations. Cognition 121, 447–453 (2011). 65. Brocher, A., Foraker, S. & Koenig, J.-P. Processing of irregular polysemes in
39. Stites, M. C., Luke, S. G. & Christianson, K. The psychologist said quickly, sentence reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1798–1813 (2016).
“Dialogue descriptions modulate reading speed!”. Mem. Cogn. 41, 66. Brzezicka, A., Krejtz, I., von Hecker, U. & Laubrock, J. Eye movement
137–151 (2013). evidence for defocused attention in dysphoria—a perceptual span analysis.
40. Inhoff, A. W. & Topolski, R. Use of phonological codes during eye fixations Int. J. Psychophysiol. 85, 129–133 (2012).
in reading and in on-line and delayed naming tasks. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 67. Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P. C. & Swaab, T. Y. The interplay of discourse
689–713 (1994). congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: evidence
41. Landerl, K. et al. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized from ERPs and eye tracking. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 103–128 (2007).
naming as longitudinal predictors of reading in five alphabetic 68. Choi, W., Desai, R. H. & Henderson, J. M. The neural substrates of natural
orthographies with varying degrees of consistency. Sci. Stud. Read. 23, reading: a comparison of normal and nonword text using eyetracking and
220–234 (2019). fMRI. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1024 (2014).
42. Moll, K. et al. Cognitive mechanisms underlying reading and spelling 69. Choi, W. & Gordon, P. C. Word skipping during sentence reading: effects of
development in five European orthographies. Learn. Instr. 29, 65–77 (2014). lexicality on parafoveal processing. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 76,
43. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., Richlan, F., Schuster, S. & Hutzler, F. Parafoveal 201–213 (2014).
preprocessing in reading revisited: evidence from a novel preview 70. Choi, W. & Henderson, J. M. Neural correlates of active vision: An fMRI
manipulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40, 588–595 (2014). comparison of natural reading and scene viewing. Neuropsychologia 75,
44. Schotter, E. R., Jia, A., Ferreira, V. S. & Rayner, K. Preview benefit in 109–118 (2015).
speaking occurs regardless of preview timing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 71. Cop, U., Dirix, N., Drieghe, D. & Duyck, W. Presenting GECO: an
755–762 (2014). eyetracking corpus of monolingual and bilingual sentence reading.
45. Vitu, F., McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. & O’Regan, J. K. Fixation location effects Behav. Res. Methods https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0734-0 (2016).
on fixation durations during reading: an inverted optimal viewing position 72. Cop, U., Keuleers, E., Drieghe, D. & Duyck, W. Frequency effects in
effect. Vis. Res. 41, 3513–3533 (2001). monolingual and bilingual natural reading. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22,
46. Metzner, P., von der Malsburg, T., Vasishth, S. & Rösler, F. Brain responses 1216–1234 (2015).
to world knowledge violations: a comparison of stimulus- and 73. Cui, L. et al. Processing of compound-word characters in reading Chinese:
fixation-triggered event-related potentials and neural oscillations. J. Cogn. An eye-movement-contingent display change study. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66,
Neurosci. 27, 1017–1028 (2015). 527–547 (2013).
47. Schotter, E. R., Tran, R. & Rayner, K. Don’t believe what you read (only 74. Cutter, M. G., Drieghe, D. & Liversedge, S. P. Preview benefit in
once): comprehension is supported by regressions during reading. Psychol. English spaced compounds. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40,
Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614531148 (2014). 1778–1786 (2014).
48. Johnson, R. L., Oehrlein, E. C. & Roche, W. L. Predictability and parafoveal 75. Dambacher, M., Slattery, T. J., Yang, J., Kliegl, R. & Rayner, K. Evidence for
preview effects in the developing reader: evidence from eye movements. J. direct control of eye movements during reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44, 973–991 (2018). Percept. Perform. 39, 1468–1484 (2013).
49. Milledge, S. V., Blythe, H. I. & Liversedge, S. P. Parafoveal pre-processing in 76. Dank, M., Deutsch, A. & Bock, K. Resolving conflicts in natural and
children reading English: the importance of external letters. Psychon. Bull. grammatical gender agreement: evidence from eye movements. J.
Rev. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01806-8 (2020). Psycholinguist. Res. 44, 435–467 (2015).
50. Häikiö, T., Bertram, R. & Hyönä, J. Development of parafoveal processing 77. Davis, C. J., Perea, M. & Acha, J. Re(de)fining the orthographic
within and across words in reading: evidence from the boundary paradigm. neighborhood: The role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical
Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 1982–1998 (2010). decision and reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 35,
51. Blythe, H. I. Developmental changes in eye movements and visual 1550–1570 (2009).
information encoding associated with learning to read. Curr. Dir. Psychol. 78. Desai, R. H., Choi, W., Lai, V. T. & Henderson, J. M. Toward semantics in
Sci. 23, 201–207 (2014). the wild: activation to manipulable nouns in naturalistic reading.
52. Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A. & Engbert, R. Tracking the mind during reading: J. Neurosci. 36, 4050–4055 (2016).
the influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations. J. 79. Deutsch, A., Velan, H. & Michaly, T. Decomposition in a non-concatenated
Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 12–35 (2006). morphological structure involves more than just the roots: evidence from
53. Giraud, A.-L. & Poeppel, D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: fast priming. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1250
emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 788 (2016).
511–517 (2012). 80. Dimigen, O., Sommer, W., Hohlfeld, A., Jacobs, A. M. & Kliegl, R.
54. Gross, J. et al. Speech rhythms and multiplexed oscillatory sensory coding Coregistration of eye movements and EEG in natural reading: analyses and
in the human brain. PLoS Biol. 11, e1001752 (2013). review. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 140, 552–572 (2011).

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 439


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour
81. Dirix, N. & Duyck, W. An eye movement corpus study of the 108. Joseph, H. S. S. L. & Liversedge, S. P. Children’s and adults’ on-line
age-of-acquisition effect. Psychon. Bull. Rev. https://doi.org/10.3758/ processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences during reading. PLoS ONE
s13423-017-1233-8 (2017). 8, e54141 (2013).
82. Drieghe, D. Foveal processing and word skipping during reading. Psychon. 109. Joseph, H. S. S. L. et al. Children’s and adults’ processing of anomaly and
Bull. Rev. 15, 856–860 (2008). implausibility during reading: evidence from eye movements. Q. J. Exp.
83. Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A. & Carreiras, M. NoA’s ark: Influence of the Psychol. 61, 708–723 (2008).
number of associates in visual word recognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 15, 110. Juhasz, B. J. & Berkowitz, R. N. Effects of morphological families
1072–1077 (2008). on English compound word recognition: a multitask investigation.
84. Eiter, B. M. & Inhoff, A. W. Visual word recognition during reading is Lang. Cogn. Process. 26, 653–682 (2011).
followed by subvocal articulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 111. Juhasz, B. J. & Rayner, K. The role of age of acquisition and word
457–470 (2010). frequency in reading: Evidence from eye fixation durations. Vis. Cogn. 13,
85. Farmer, T. A., Yan, S., Bicknell, K. & Tanenhaus, M. K. Form-to-expectation 846–863 (2006).
matching effects on first-pass eye movement measures during reading. 112. Kaakinen, J. K. & Hyönä, J. Task effects on eye movements during reading.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 958–976 (2015). J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 1561–1566 (2010).
86. Foulsham, T., Farley, J. & Kingstone, A. Mind wandering in sentence 113. Kim, Y.-S., Radach, R. & Vorstius, C. Eye movements and parafoveal
reading: Decoupling the link between mind and eye. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. processing during reading in Korean. Read. Writ. 25, 1053–1078 (2012).
Can. Psychol. Expérimentale 67, 51–59 (2013). 114. Kliegl, R., Nuthmann, A. & Engbert, R. Tracking the mind during reading:
87. Frazier, M., Ackerman, L., Baumann, P., Potter, D. & Yoshida, M. the influence of past, present, and future words on fixation durations.
Wh-filler-gap dependency formation guides reflexive antecedent search. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 12–35 (2006).
Front. Psychol. 6, 1504 (2015). 115. Knickerbocker, H., Johnson, R. L. & Altarriba, J. Emotion effects during
88. Frisson, S., Koole, H., Hughes, L., Olson, A. & Wheeldon, L. Competition reading: Influence of an emotion target word on eye movements and
between orthographically and phonologically similar words during sentence processing. Cogn. Emot. 29, 784–806 (2015).
reading: evidence from eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 73, 148–173 (2014). 116. Koornneef, A. & Vanberkum, J. On the use of verb-based implicit causality
89. Gagl, B. Blue hypertext is a good design decision: no perceptual in sentence comprehension: Evidence from self-paced reading and eye
disadvantage in reading and successful highlighting of relevant information. tracking. J. Mem. Lang. 54, 445–465 (2006).
PeerJ 4, e2467 (2016). 117. Kretzschmar, F., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I. & Schlesewsky, M. Parafoveal
90. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S. & Hutzler, F. A similar correction mechanism in slow versus foveal N400s dissociate spreading activation from contextual fit:.
and fluent readers after suboptimal landing positions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. NeuroReport 20, 1613–1618 (2009).
8, 355 (2014). 118. Kuperman, V. & Dyke, Van J. A. Effects of individual differences in verbal
91. Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., Richlan, F., Schuster, S. & Hutzler, F. Parafoveal skills on eye-movement patterns during sentence reading. J. Mem. Lang. 65,
preprocessing in reading revisited: evidence from a novel preview 42–73 (2011).
manipulation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40, 588–595 (2014). 119. Li, X., Liu, P. & Rayner, K. Eye movement guidance in Chinese reading: Is
92. Gollan, T. H. et al. Frequency drives lexical access in reading but not in there a preferred viewing location? Vision Res. 51, 1146–1156 (2011).
speaking: the frequency-lag hypothesis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 140, 120. Libben, M. R. & Titone, D. A. Bilingual lexical access in context: evidence
186–209 (2011). from eye movements during reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35,
93. Gu, J., Li, X. & Liversedge, S. P. Character order processing in 381–390 (2009).
Chinese reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 121. Liu, P. & Li, X. Inserting spaces before and after words affects word
127–137 (2015). processing differently in Chinese: evidence from eye movements. Br. J.
94. Hawelka, S., Gagl, B. & Wimmer, H. A dual-route perspective on eye Psychol. 105, 57–68 (2014).
movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition 115, 367–379 (2010). 122. Liversedge, S. P. et al. Universality in eye movements and reading: a
95. Henderson, J. M., Choi, W. & Luke, S. G. Morphology of primary visual trilingual investigation. Cognition 147, 1–20 (2016).
cortex predicts individual differences in fixation duration during text 123. Lou, Y., Liu, Y., Kaakinen, J. K. & Li, X. Using support vector machines to
reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 2880–2888 (2014). identify literacy skills: evidence from eye movements. Behav. Res. Methods
96. Henderson, J. M., Choi, W., Luke, S. G. & Desai, R. H. Neural correlates of https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0748-7 (2016).
fixation duration in natural reading: evidence from fixation-related fMRI. 124. Lowder, M. W., Choi, W. & Gordon, P. C. Word recognition during reading:
NeuroImage 119, 390–397 (2015). the interaction between lexical repetition and frequency. Mem. Cognit. 41,
97. Henderson, J. M. & Luke, S. G. Stable individual differences in saccadic eye 738–751 (2013).
movements during reading, pseudoreading, scene viewing, and scene 125. Lowder, M. W. & Gordon, P. C. Print exposure modulates the effects of
search. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40, 1390–1400 (2014). repetition priming during sentence reading. Psychon. Bull. Rev. https://doi.
98. Hermena, E. W., Liversedge, S. P. & Drieghe, D. Parafoveal processing of org/10.3758/s13423-017-1248-1 (2017).
Arabic diacritical marks. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 126. Luo, Y., Yan, M., Yan, S., Zhou, X. & Inhoff, A. W. Syllabic tone articulation
2021–2038 (2016). influences the identification and use of words during Chinese sentence
99. Howard, P. L., Liversedge, S. P. & Benson, V. Benchmark eye movement reading: evidence from ERP and eye movement recordings. Cogn. Affect.
effects during natural reading in autism spectrum disorder. J. Exp. Psychol. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 72–92 (2016).
Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 109–127 (2017). 127. Luo, Y., Yan, M. & Zhou, X. Prosodic boundaries delay the processing of
100. Huestegge, L., Kunert, H.-J. & Radach, R. Long-term effects of cannabis on upcoming lexical information during silent sentence reading. J. Exp.
eye movement control in reading. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 209, Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 915–930 (2013).
77–84 (2010). 128. Ma, G., Li, X. & Pollatsek, A. There is no relationship between preferred
101. Inhoff, A. W., Starr, M. S., Solomon, M. & Placke, L. Eye movements during viewing location and word segmentation in Chinese reading. Vis. Cogn. 23,
the reading of compound words and the influence of lexeme meaning. 399–414 (2015).
Mem. Cognit. 36, 675–687 (2008). 129. Ma, G., Pollatsek, A., Li, Y. & Li, X. Chinese readers can perceive a word
102. Jian, Y.-C. & Ko, H.-W. Investigating the effects of background knowledge even when it’s composed of noncontiguous characters. J. Exp. Psychol.
on Chinese word processing during text reading: evidence from eye Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 158–166 (2017).
movements: eye movements on Chinese word processing. J. Res. Read. 37, 130. McDonald, S. A. Effects of number-of-letters on eye movements during
S71–S86 (2014). reading are independent from effects of spatial word length. Vis. Cogn. 13,
103. Jian, Y.-C., Chen, M.-L. & Ko, H. Context effects in processing of Chinese 89–98 (2006).
academic words: an eye-tracking investigation. Read. Res. Q. 48, 131. McDonald, S. A., Spitsyna, G., Shillcock, R. C., Wise, R. J. S. & Leff, A. P.
403–413 (2013). Patients with hemianopic alexia adopt an inefficient eye movement strategy
104. Jincho, N., Feng, G. & Mazuka, R. Development of text reading in Japanese: when reading text. Brain J. Neurol. 129, 158–167 (2006).
an eye movement study. Read. Writ. 27, 1437–1465 (2014). 132. Metzner, P., von der Malsburg, T., Vasishth, S. & Rösler, F. Brain responses
105. Johnson, R. L. & Eisler, M. E. The importance of the first and last to world knowledge violations: a comparison of stimulus- and
letter in words during sentence reading. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 141, fixation-triggered event-related potentials and neural oscillations. J. Cogn.
336–351 (2012). Neurosci. 27, 1017–1028 (2015).
106. Jordan, T. R., McGowan, V. A., Kurtev, S. & Paterson, K. B. A further look 133. Miller, B., Juhasz, B. J. & Rayner, K. The orthographic uniqueness point and
at postview effects in reading: an eye-movements study of influences from eye movements during reading. Br. J. Psychol. 97, 191–216 (2006).
the left of fixation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 42, 296–307 (2016). 134. Nikolova, M., Jainta, S., Blythe, H. I. & Liversedge, S. P. Using a dichoptic
107. Joseph, H. S., Nation, K. & Liversedge, S. P. Using eye movements to moving window presentation technique to investigate binocular
investigate word frequency effects in children’s sentence reading. Sch. advantages during reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 43,
Psychol. Rev. 42, 207 (2013). 265–280 (2017).

440 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Nature Human Behaviour Articles
135. Noh, E.-J., Choo, H. & Koh, S. Processing metalinguistic negation: evidence 162. Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., Duyck, W., Welvaert, M. & Hartsuiker, R. J.
from eye-tracking experiments. J. Pragmat. 57, 1–18 (2013). The influence of semantic constraints on bilingual word recognition during
136. Nuthmann, A., Engbert, R. & Kliegl, R. The IOVP effect in mindless sentence reading. J. Mem. Lang. 64, 88–107 (2011).
reading: Experiment and modeling. Vision Res. 47, 990–1002 (2007). 163. Van der Haegen, L., Cai, Q., Stevens, M. A. & Brysbaert, M.
137. Nuthmann, A. & Henderson, J. M. Using CRISP to model global Interhemispheric communication influences reading behavior. J. Cogn.
characteristics of fixation durations in scene viewing and reading with a Neurosci. 25, 1442–1452 (2013).
common mechanism. Vis. Cogn. 20, 457–494 (2012). 164. Velan, H., Deutsch, A. & Frost, R. The flexibility of letter-position flexibility:
138. Pagán, A., Blythe, H. I. & Liversedge, S. P. Parafoveal preprocessing of word Evidence from eye movements in reading Hebrew. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
initial trigrams during reading in adults and children. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Percept. Perform. 39, 1143–1152 (2013).
Mem. Cogn. 42, 411–432 (2016). 165. Veldre, A. & Andrews, S. Parafoveal lexical activation depends on skilled
139. Perea, M., Giner, L., Marcet, A. & Gomez, P. Does extra interletter reading proficiency. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 586–595 (2015).
spacing help text reading in skilled adult readers? Span. J. Psychol. 19, 166. Veldre, A. & Andrews, S. Lexical quality and eye movements: Individual
E26 (2016). differences in the perceptual span of skilled adult readers. Q. J. Exp. Psychol.
140. Philipp, A. M. & Huestegge, L. Language switching between sentences in 67, 703–727 (2014).
reading: Exogenous and endogenous effects on eye movements and 167. Vernet, M. & Kapoula, Z. Binocular motor coordination during saccades and
comprehension. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 18, 614–625 (2015). fixations while reading: a magnitude and time analysis. J. Vis. 9, 2 (2009).
141. Plummer, P., Perea, M. & Rayner, K. The influence of contextual diversity 168. Vignali, L., Himmelstoss, N. A., Hawelka, S., Richlan, F. & Hutzler, F.
on eye movements in reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 40, Oscillatory brain dynamics during sentence reading: a fixation-related
275–283 (2014). spectral perturbation analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 191 (2016).
142. Pollatsek, A., Juhasz, B. J., Reichle, E. D., Machacek, D. & Rayner, K. 169. Inhoff, A. W., Solomon, M. S., Seymour, B. A. & Radach, R. Eye position
Immediate and delayed effects of word frequency and word length on eye changes during reading fixations are spatially selective. Vision Res. 48,
movements in reading: a reversed delayed effect of word length. J. Exp. 1027–1039 (2008).
Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 726–750 (2008). 170. Wang, A., Zhou, W., Shu, H. & Yan, M. Reading proficiency modulates
143. Rau, A. K. et al. Same same, but different: word and sentence reading in parafoveal processing efficiency: evidence from reading Chinese as a second
German and English. Sci. Stud. Read. 20, 203–219 (2016). language. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 152, 29–33 (2014).
144. Rayner, K., Li, X., Williams, C. C., Cave, K. R. & Well, A. D. Eye 171. Warren, T. & McConnell, K. Investigating effects of selectional restriction
movements during information processing tasks: individual differences and violations and plausibility violation severity on eye-movements in reading.
cultural effects. Vision Res. 47, 2714–2726 (2007). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 14, 770–775 (2007).
145. Rayner, K. & Schotter, E. R. Semantic preview benefit in reading English: 172. White, S. J., Drieghe, D., Liversedge, S. P. & Staub, A. The word frequency
the effect of initial letter capitalization. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. effect during sentence reading: a linear or nonlinear effect of log frequency?
Perform. 40, 1617–1628 (2014). Q. J. Exp. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1240813 (2016).
146. Rayner, K., Slattery, T. J., Drieghe, D. & Liversedge, S. P. Eye movements 173. White, S. J., Johnson, R. L., Liversedge, S. P. & Rayner, K. Eye movements
and word skipping during reading: effects of word length and predictability. when reading transposed text: the importance of word-beginning letters. J.
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 37, 514–528 (2011). Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 1261–1276 (2008).
147. Rayner, K., Yang, J., Schuett, S. & Slattery, T. J. The effect of foveal and 174. White, S. J., Warrington, K. L., McGowan, V. A. & Paterson, K. B. Eye
parafoveal masks on the eye movements of older and younger readers. movements during reading and topic scanning: effects of word frequency. J.
Psychol. Aging 29, 205–212 (2014). Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 233–248 (2015).
148. Reali, C., Esaulova, Y., Öttl, A. & von Stockhausen, L. Role descriptions 175. Winskel, H. Orthographic and phonological parafoveal processing of
induce gender mismatch effects in eye movements during reading. Front. consonants, vowels, and tones when reading Thai. Appl. Psycholinguist. 32,
Psychol. 6, 1607 (2015). 739–759 (2011).
149. Reingold, E. M., Reichle, E. D., Glaholt, M. G. & Sheridan, H. Direct lexical 176. Winskel, H., Perea, M. & Ratitamkul, T. On the flexibility of letter position
control of eye movements in reading: Evidence from a survival analysis of coding during lexical processing: Evidence from eye movements when
fixation durations. Cognit. Psychol. 65, 177–206 (2012). reading Thai. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 1522–1536 (2012).
150. Sainio, M., Hyönä, J., Bingushi, K. & Bertram, R. The role of interword 177. Winskel, H., Radach, R. & Luksaneeyanawin, S. Eye movements when
spacing in reading Japanese: An eye movement study. Vision Res. 47, reading spaced and unspaced Thai and English: a comparison of Thai–
2575–2584 (2007). English bilinguals and English monolinguals. J. Mem. Lang. 61,
151. Schad, D. J., Nuthmann, A. & Engbert, R. Eye movements during reading 339–351 (2009).
of randomly shuffled text. Vision Res. 50, 2600–2616 (2010). 178. Wotschack, C. & Kliegl, R. Reading strategy modulates parafoveal-on-foveal
152. Schad, D. J., Risse, S., Slattery, T. & Rayner, K. Word frequency in fast effects in sentence reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 548–562 (2013).
priming: Evidence for immediate cognitive control of eye-movements 179. Yan, M. & Kliegl, R. CarPrice versus CarpRice: Word boundary ambiguity
during reading. Vis. Cogn. 22, 390–414 (2014). influences saccade target selection during the reading of Chinese sentences.
153. Schotter, E. R., Lee, M., Reiderman, M. & Rayner, K. The effect of J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1832–1838 (2016).
contextual constraint on parafoveal processing in reading. J. Mem. Lang. 83, 180. Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Richter, E. M., Nuthmann, A. & Shu, H. Flexible
118–139 (2015). saccade-target selection in Chinese reading. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 705–725
154. Staub, A. The effect of lexical predictability on distributions of eye fixation (2010).
durations. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 371–376 (2011). 181. Yang, J. Preview effects of plausibility and character order in reading
155. Staub, A., White, S. J., Drieghe, D., Hollway, E. C. & Rayner, K. Chinese transposed words: evidence from eye movements: preview reading
Distributional effects of word frequency on eye fixation durations. J. Exp. of transposed words. J. Res. Read. 36, S18–S34 (2013).
Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1280–1293 (2010). 182. Yen, M.-H., Tsai, J.-L., Tzeng, O. J.-L. & Hung, D. L. Eye movements and
156. Stites, M. C. & Federmeier, K. D. Subsequent to suppression: downstream parafoveal word processing in reading Chinese. Mem. Cognit. 36,
comprehension consequences of noun/verb ambiguity in natural reading. 1033–1045 (2008).
J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 1497–1515 (2015). 183. Yen, M.-H., Radach, R., Tzeng, O. J.-L. & Tsai, J.-L. Usage of statistical cues
157. Stites, M. C., Federmeier, K. D. & Christianson, K. Do morphemes matter for word boundary in reading Chinese sentences. Read. Writ. 25,
when reading compound words with transposed letters? evidence from 1007–1029 (2012).
eye-tracking and event-related potentials. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 184. Rayner, K. Eye movement latencies for parafoveally presented words. Bull.
1299–1319 (2016). Psychon. Soc. 11, 13–16 (1978).
158. Stites, M. C., Federmeier, K. D. & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. Cross-age 185. Taylor, I. in Processing of Visible Language (eds Kolers, P. A. et al.) 67–82
comparisons reveal multiple strategies for lexical ambiguity resolution (Springer, 1980).
during natural reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 186. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects
1823–1841 (2013). models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).
159. Titone, D., Libben, M., Mercier, J., Whitford, V. & Pivneva, I. Bilingual 187. Kliegl, R., Wei, P., Dambacher, M., Yan, M. & Zhou, X. Experimental effects
lexical access during L1 sentence reading: the effects of L2 knowledge, and individual differences in linear mixed models: estimating the
semantic constraint, and L1–L2 intermixing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. relationship between spatial, object, and attraction effects in visual
Cogn. 37, 1412–1431 (2011). attention. Front. Psychol. 1, 238 (2011).
160. Tsai, J.-L., Kliegl, R. & Yan, M. Parafoveal semantic information extraction
in traditional Chinese reading. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 141, 17–23 (2012). Acknowledgements
161. Vainio, S., Hyönä, J. & Pajunen, A. Lexical predictability exerts robust The research leading to these results has received funding from the European
effects on fixation duration, but not on initial landing position during Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2013) under grant agreement no.
reading. Exp. Psychol. 56, 66–74 (2009). 617891 awarded to C.J.F. and from the European Community’s Horizon 2020 Programme

Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav 441


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Articles Nature Human Behaviour
under grant agreement no. 707932 awarded to B.G. The funders had no role in study Additional information
design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. Supplementary information The online version contains supplementary material
We thank J. Müller for helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript. available at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01215-4.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to Benjamin Gagl.
Author contributions Peer review information Nature Human Behaviour thanks Denis Drieghe, Kristen
B.G., D.P. and C.J.F. designed the research. B.G. and S.H. performed Study 1. B.G. and
Pammer and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review
J.G. performed Study 2 (meta-analysis). B.G. and K.G. performed Studies 3 and 4.
of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
B.G., A.T. and J.S. analysed the data. B.G. and C.J.F. wrote the paper. All authors gave
comments on the paper during the process. Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
Competing interests published maps and institutional affiliations.
The authors declare no competing interests. © The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Nature Limited 2021

442 Nature Human Behaviour | VOL 6 | March 2022 | 429–442 | www.nature.com/nathumbehav

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


nature portfolio | reporting summary
Corresponding author(s): Benjamin Gagl
Last updated by author(s): Jul 21, 2021

Reporting Summary
Nature Portfolio wishes to improve the reproducibility of the work that we publish. This form provides structure for consistency and transparency
in reporting. For further information on Nature Portfolio policies, see our Editorial Policies and the Editorial Policy Checklist.

Statistics
For all statistical analyses, confirm that the following items are present in the figure legend, table legend, main text, or Methods section.
n/a Confirmed
The exact sample size (n) for each experimental group/condition, given as a discrete number and unit of measurement
A statement on whether measurements were taken from distinct samples or whether the same sample was measured repeatedly
The statistical test(s) used AND whether they are one- or two-sided
Only common tests should be described solely by name; describe more complex techniques in the Methods section.

A description of all covariates tested


A description of any assumptions or corrections, such as tests of normality and adjustment for multiple comparisons
A full description of the statistical parameters including central tendency (e.g. means) or other basic estimates (e.g. regression coefficient)
AND variation (e.g. standard deviation) or associated estimates of uncertainty (e.g. confidence intervals)

For null hypothesis testing, the test statistic (e.g. F, t, r) with confidence intervals, effect sizes, degrees of freedom and P value noted
Give P values as exact values whenever suitable.

For Bayesian analysis, information on the choice of priors and Markov chain Monte Carlo settings
For hierarchical and complex designs, identification of the appropriate level for tests and full reporting of outcomes
Estimates of effect sizes (e.g. Cohen's d, Pearson's r), indicating how they were calculated
Our web collection on statistics for biologists contains articles on many of the points above.

Software and code


Policy information about availability of computer code
Data collection Experiment Builder software (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) for presentation of sentences and z-strings in eyetracking experiments. Speech
was recorded with the Audacity software (Version 2.1.3; https://www.audacityteam.org/). Both were compiled stand allone software running
on Windows PC.

Data analysis We implemented eye-tracking data preprocessing in Perl on a MAC system, speech data preprocessing in MATLAB and the data preprocessing
for the Fourier transform control analysis in Python both on a Ubuntu system. All further processing steps and analysis were implemented in
R. Please find all scripts here https://osf.io/96vp8/.
For manuscripts utilizing custom algorithms or software that are central to the research but not yet described in published literature, software must be made available to editors and
reviewers. We strongly encourage code deposition in a community repository (e.g. GitHub). See the Nature Portfolio guidelines for submitting code & software for further information.

Data
Policy information about availability of data
March 2021

All manuscripts must include a data availability statement. This statement should provide the following information, where applicable:
- Accession codes, unique identifiers, or web links for publicly available datasets
- A description of any restrictions on data availability
- For clinical datasets or third party data, please ensure that the statement adheres to our policy

Provide your data availability statement here.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


nature portfolio | reporting summary
Field-specific reporting
Please select the one below that is the best fit for your research. If you are not sure, read the appropriate sections before making your selection.
Life sciences Behavioural & social sciences Ecological, evolutionary & environmental sciences
For a reference copy of the document with all sections, see nature.com/documents/nr-reporting-summary-flat.pdf

Behavioural & social sciences study design


All studies must disclose on these points even when the disclosure is negative.
Study description Three quantitative studies (Study 1, 3 & 4) and one meta-analytic study (Study 2) are included.

Research sample Fifty (13 male; 18–47 years old; M=24 years; students at University of Salzburg) native speakers of German participated in Study 1,
forty-nine (13 male; 18–74years old; M=24 years) non-native German speakers participated in Study 3, and eighty-six (36 male; 18–
53years old; M=25years; five had to be excluded based on preregistered outlier correction boundaries for both speech and reading
rates; +-3 standard deviations) German speakers participated in Study 4 after giving informed consent according to procedures
approved by the respective local ethics committee. For Study 1, see Gagl, Hawelka & Hutzler, 2011, BRM for more details. Note that
relative to the previously published report, one participant was added. For Study 3, participants with varying mother tongues (Arabic,
Azerbaijani, Bulgarian, Chinese, English, Farsi, French, Georgian, Indonesian, Italian, Japanese, Persian, Russian, Serbo-Croatian,
Spanish, Turkish, Ukrainian, Hungarian, Urdu, and Uzbek) and, for Study 4, native German participants were recruited on the campus
of Goethe University Frankfurt as part of a more extensive study. Also, note that six participants from Study 3 became literate
without the acquisition of an alphabetic script.

Sampling strategy Datasets for studies 1 and 3 were available before, and the sample size of 49 and 50 participants typically prevent the inflation of
overestimation of effects due to small sample sizes (e.g., see Cremers HR, Wager TD, Yarkoni T (2017) The relation between statistical
power and inference in fMRI. PLoS ONE 12(11): e0184923. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184923 shows stable effects with
N > 30). For the dataset in study 4, we provide a pre-registration, including a power calculation (https://osf.io/mjhkz). We
implemented a Monte-Carlo simulation procedure (i.e., randomly sampling data and re-estimating the general linear model 10,000
times with different participants). With this procedure, we learned that a total of 90 participants must be collected to gain a power
value of .9. After excluding five participants due to a pre-registered exclusion criterion, data from 86 participants were analyzed. For
study 1, student participants from the University of Salzburg were selected, for study 3 non-native German speakers, and for study 4
native German speakers, both from the Frankfurt area were selected.

Data collection In reading and scanning tasks used in Study 1, 3 & 4 all stimuli were presented on computer screens. The researcher presented the
interview questions, and the reading scores were implemented via a paper-pencil test. One researcher was testing one participant,
and researchers testing were blind to the hypothesis of the current study.

Timing For Study 1, we relied on a dataset first presented in Gagl, Hawelka, & Hutzler 2011. For Study 3, all data was collected in 2018. For
Study 4, all data was collected in 2019 and 2020.

Data exclusions In Study 3 we found that one and study 4 five participant had a mean amplitude modulation spectrum, which was larger than three
standard deviations from the mean of the sample; this participant was excluded from the analysis. See analysis section for details.

Non-participation No participants dropped out.

Randomization No allocation into experimental groups.

Reporting for specific materials, systems and methods


We require information from authors about some types of materials, experimental systems and methods used in many studies. Here, indicate whether each material,
system or method listed is relevant to your study. If you are not sure if a list item applies to your research, read the appropriate section before selecting a response.

Materials & experimental systems Methods


n/a Involved in the study n/a Involved in the study
Antibodies ChIP-seq
Eukaryotic cell lines Flow cytometry
March 2021

Palaeontology and archaeology MRI-based neuroimaging


Animals and other organisms
Human research participants
Clinical data
Dual use research of concern

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Human research participants

nature portfolio | reporting summary


Policy information about studies involving human research participants
Population characteristics See above.

Recruitment In booth studies advertisements were used to recruit participants.

Ethics oversight Study 1 was approved by the ethics board of the University of Salzburg and Studies 3/4 was approved by the ethics board of
the Goethe University of Frankfurt
Note that full information on the approval of the study protocol must also be provided in the manuscript.

March 2021

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved


Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:

1. use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
2. use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
3. falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
4. use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
5. override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
6. share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at

onlineservice@springernature.com

You might also like