You are on page 1of 8

PAPERS | JANUARY 01 2024

Using Math in Physics: 7.Telling the story 


Edward F. Redish

Phys. Teach. 62, 5–11 (2024)


https://doi.org/10.1119/5.0159037

CrossMark

 
View Export
Online Citation

02 March 2024 16:32:17


Using Math in Physics:
7.Telling the story
Edward F. Redish, University of Maryland—emeritus, College Park, MD

T
o develop complex problem-solving skills, students for these different views of what to do to learn—how an indi-
need to learn to develop a coherent story about what vidual interprets the nature of the knowledge being learned
the situation in the problem is, what the mechanism is, and what knowledge is needed to deal with a particular situ-
and what physics principles are appropriate to apply. This can ation.3
be challenging for students who have been successful in science What can we as instructors do to be explicit and help stu-
through memorizing answers and simply plugging numbers into dents reframe what it means to use math in science?
equations. And it can be challenging for instructors who get a In earlier papers in this series, I’ve suggested that a useful
class full of students who “just want to be given the answers.” But way is to identify tools to help students build the blend. The
taking the easy way out—matching students’ desires rather than tools I’ve covered in the previous papers are important and
their needs—does them and the scientific community a serious useful. But to solve complex problems, they’ll need to learn
disservice. the essential element: telling the story.
As discussed in the earlier papers in this series,1 we use math
in physics differently than it’s used in math classes.2 In math The key element is knowing how to tell the
classes, students manipulate equations with abstract symbols story
that usually have no physical meaning. In physics, we blend con- We all know the importance and the stability of stories
ceptual physics knowledge with mathematical symbology. This from our everyday experiences. Well into adulthood, we can
changes the way that we use math and what we can do with it. easily recount stories we learned as children, whether from
Even if students can make the blend—interpret physics cor- Aesop’s fables or Disney movies. The moral messages from
rectly in mathematical symbology and graphs—they still need those stories help build how we function and who we are.
to be able to apply that knowledge in productive and coherent Storytelling is a fundamental resource we all have to or-
ways. As instructors, we can show our solutions to complex ganize our long-term memories. We also generate stories to

02 March 2024 16:32:17


problems in class. We can give complex problems to students as make sense of what’s happening in our everyday lives—what’s
homework. But our students are likely to still have trouble be- going on, what something means.4
cause they are missing a key element of making sense of how we Solving a complex physics problem by making sense of it
think about physics: How to tell the story of what’s happening. means building a new story, adapted from previously learned
Physicists use these blended mental structures to create sto- stories and drawing on blended concepts and fundamental
ries about what’s happening (mechanism) and stabilize them physical principles.
with fundamental physical laws (synthesis). Figure 1 shows the How can we as instructors help students use their existing
importance of synthesis and mechanism in making sense of the storytelling resources to learn to build appropriate stories
physical world and how all the steps are tied together with equa- when solving physics problems?
tions (with math).
Storytelling is a fundamental tool in the
mathematical toolbelt
Case studies in the research literature suggest that being
explicit about epistemological issues can have a powerful
impact, even crossing disciplinary boundaries5 and lasting
beyond the individual physics class.6
My approach is to identify telling the story as part of the
mathematical toolbelt I’ve been writing about in this series of
papers. As with the other tools, I help students see storytelling
as a useful tool by being explicit about its use and expanding
the kind of tasks I assign in order to focus on learning this
Fig. 1. Building coherent physics knowledge. skill.
As with the other tools, I represent storytelling by an icon:
Students too often focus only on the bottom boxes of Fig. 1, a reading terrapin (the University of Maryland’s mascot) that
memorizing facts and process steps (algorithms). We want them you see next to the title of this article. I use it on slides, text,
to be able to tell a coherent story about the problem. We want and problem solutions whenever the students and I are telling
them to not just know the answer. We want them to understand a story to set up a problem, solve it, or decide that we’ve done
what’s going on: What’s the mechanism? How does this fit with it correctly.
fundamental physics principles (synthesis)? In the rest of this paper, I first explain what the cognitive
The research literature uses the term epistemological framing science research literature tells us about how memory for

DOI: 10.1119/5.0159037 THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024 5


stories works—illustrated with some of my own physics
(a)
stories. Understanding this research can help you see what
our students are going through as they develop and use their
storytelling skills for the physics blend. Then, I give you five
specific techniques for helping your students learn to tell and
value stories as a way to understand mechanism and synthesis
in physics.

Stories are central to our creation of stable (b)

long-term memories
In the early 1990s, when I was just learning about physics
education research (PER), I invited Ron Thornton, one of the
developers of active learning supported by computer-assisted
measurement devices, to give a seminar to my research group
on using those tools. He demonstrated the sonic ranger and Fig. 2. Walking a velocity graph—wrong!
claimed that students had serious difficulties reading a veloci-
ty graph. He said it was harder than it looks. much more than the ones his first-semester professor had
To show this, he called for a “volunteer,” pointing at me done. When I asked, “Why?” he responded, “Well, he just
(the senior member of the research group). He showed a ve- did demos that showed us that what we knew was going to
locity graph [Fig. 2(a)] and asked me to walk to match it with happen, did happen.” I asked for an example. Calvin replied,
the sonic ranger taking data on my position but displaying “Like with the racing billiard balls on the straight and dipped
my velocity. I had no hesitation in doing this, having taught tracks” (see Fig. 3). “We knew that since the ball on the
the concept in introductory physics classes for more than 20 dipped track sped up but slowed back down, they would get
years. to end at the same time, and that’s what happened.”
When he started collecting data, I confidently strode back- Well! The story he told himself was good enough to help
ward, my velocity rising to match the graph. But … when I him remember the result into the next semester. The only
got the value of my velocity to match, instead of continuing at problem is that’s not what happens! The ball on the dipped

02 March 2024 16:32:17


that velocity as shown by the graph, I stopped! And produced track speeds up as it falls and returns to its original speed as
something like the red graph in Fig. 2(b). it rises back to the straight part of the track, so the horizontal
“Wait! Wait!” I said. “I have to put my brain in velocity component of that ball’s speed is always equal to or greater
mode!” The second time, I was able to walk the correct graph than that of the ball on the undipped track. As a result, the
with no difficulty, making Ron’s point perfectly, to the amuse- one on the dipped track reaches the end first. This had to be
ment of my grad students. what Calvin saw. But it was not what he remembered. Calvin
But the point of this story is not the story itself, but rather reconstructed the story so that it made sense to him, consis-
a “metastory”—a story about the story. tent with his (mis)conception of what was going on.
Soon after this, I began to put this story into the many
seminars I gave introducing PER to physics departments
around the country. Some years later, I gave an invited talk at
an AAPT meeting.7 Someone came up to me after the talk,
thanked me for it, and commented that they had heard me
lecture about PER many years ago—and they most enjoyed
the story about the sonic ranger. They were able to recount it
correctly more than a decade later!
But … Fig. 3. Two spring-launched billiard balls race along a straight
and a dipped track.
The stories we recall are not necessarily accurate
Another story, this time from my personal experience Stories are reconstructed rather than simply
teaching introductory physics to engineers, illustrates this. recorded
I was one of a number of instructors teaching large lecture A lot of anthropological9 and cognitive10 research sup-
classes in introductory physics for engineers in the late 1990s. ports the idea that memories are not just replays of what
Between the first and second semesters, some number of stu- happened but are put together from knowledge bits and sto-
dents would switch from one instructor to another, typically rylines. This actually makes good sense.
to get lecture times that fit with the rest of their schedule. Memory has powerful evolutionary implications. It helps
One day, I had done a demonstration in class with a sur- find food, avoid predators, and win a mate to pass on our
prising result: a demonstration of Lenz’s law (sort of 8) pro- genes to descendants. These tasks require predicting the fu-
ducing a ring jumping 10 feet in the air. After class, Calvin ture by creating stories about what will happen from what
came up to me and said that he liked my demonstrations you know about what did happen. Grandmothers’ memories

6 THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024


of relevant stories about food, water, and medicinal herbs are your calculation and find
documented to play a role in successful survival for other spe- it.” Not. What. Happened.
cies as well as our own.11 Merry immediately folded
We create these future stories by putting together elements their hand and copied Pippin’s
in our memory and making a plausible story about what will solution.
happen from what we’ve experienced. So when we tell a story This shows both the value
about a past event, we’re not bringing up a stored accurate of having a good story tied to
video. We’re “predicting the past.”12,13 fundamental physics and the
In helping our students learn to tell stories about physics need to value that story. Stu-
problems, we have to figure out how to help them not just dents need to have and use the
come up with a story, which will often tie to an inappropriate epistemological resource that
but easy-to-recall fact or generalization (as in Calvin’s story), a simple story based on a solid
but create a correct story that ties to fundamental physics physical principle is a good
principles. We have to help them to not just tell a qualitative way to check a complex calcu-
hand-waving story, but, when we’re dealing with a physics lation.20 Vector integrals are Fig. 4. In what direction is
problem, tie it to the symbolic math, blending the math with tricky, and it’s easy to make a the magnetic force on a cur-
physics meaning, creating a disciplinary coherence appropri- sign error! rent-carrying loop of wire?
ate to physics. Putting together Calvin’s story with the dipped track with
But first, they have to decide to try to create a story. Merry and Pippin’s story with the current-carrying loop, we
see that success requires not just valuing stories.21 They have
Students have to choose to tell and value a story to be tied to fundamental principles, done with a careful con-
At the introductory level, students, especially those who sideration of context and the applicability of the principle,
have succeeded in earlier science classes by memorizing and connected to calculations.
answers, often get things wrong by quick associations. “Oh, So, what can we as instructors do to help students tell
I remember this. It’s …” I call this “one-step thinking”14 (for accurate stories, value their stories, and use storytelling to
students, or “p-primming”7 for researchers). develop an appreciation for and understanding of physical
If a calculation is required, students may simply reach for mechanism and synthesis?

02 March 2024 16:32:17


an equation and put in numbers inappropriately (“recursive
plug-and-chug”15), even putting in inappropriate quantities, You can help your students learn to tell useful
like a velocity for a volume, if they haven’t learned to blend stories in class
concepts and symbols. Students’ belief that they need to (on- We want
ly) get answers in a science class often blocks their building an • students to tell a story about what’s happening and not
appropriate story. (Why do they do this? Read the two stories just reach for an equation or a remembered answer
at the beginning of the Supplementary Materials!16) • the story they tell to be physically correct (that is, appro-
Even if they build a story, they might not know when to priately based on fundamental physical principles)
value it.
• that story to be “mathematizable”—tied to blended
My research group observed a dramatic event as we
symbology so as to be able to guide setting up a calcu-
watched (and videotaped17) two physics majors, Merry and
lation, moving to a solution, and evaluating its correct-
Pippin, working together on a homework problem in an E&M
ness
class from the Griffiths textbook:18
A square loop (Fig. 4, top) in the x–y plane is As professional physicists, we learn that the most import-
carrying a constant current. There is a constant ant first step in approaching a complex problem is to ask,
B-field pointing in the z-direction that varies with “What’s the physics?” By this, we mean, “What story can we
y as B =(B0/L)y k (Fig. 4, bottom). Is there a net tell that will help us set up an appropriate reasoning chain or
magnetic force on the loop? If so, in what direction calculation, based on fundamental physics principles?”
does it point?19 We also want to help students learn to do this. However,
because of all the many ways remembered stories can fail,
They each finished a solution. Merry had a story. “This we don’t want to just help students “learn to tell the story of a
looks pretty trivial—like a Physics 2 problem. You just use the physics problem.” We want them to “learn to tell it in a physi-
right-hand rule. The forces on the sides cancel but the forces cally reliable way.”
on the top and bottom both point up so the answer is up.” But In this section, I give you five techniques that I have found
Pippin had a calculation. “We’re learning about line integrals help students build their storytelling tool. It’s particularly use-
of vectors, so I think they want us to do the line integral. I ful to rely on variation—changing situations and perspectives
worked it out and got 0.” to see how the results change. This helps students focus on
I would have expected Merry to respond something like process and what they’re doing rather than just the answers.22
this: “Well, I can’t see how the simple physics could be wrong. The five techniques are:
You must have dropped a sign somewhere. Let’s go through

THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024 7


• Scaffold building a story tied to blended symbols and A block, A, is sitting on a table.
based on principles. On top of that is a lighter block, B. Block
• Foster coherence by telling a story in graphs with multi- A is resting on a table, T, and being
ple variables. pushed by a finger, F. Identify the direction
of forces (A)–(F) in this system under the
• Remove the scaffold by presenting problems that require 3 circumstances given. The labels indi-
students to both create a story and use fundamental cate: N = normal force, f = friction force.
principles. In each case, explain your reasoning.
• Use essay questions to get students to think about their (A) NA B (D) NF A
thinking. (B) fT A (E) fB A
• Blend two independent stories—reconcile personal ex- (C) fA B (F) NB A
perience (often with one-step thinking)—and physical
1. You start pushing on block A as shown, but it is too heavy and
reasoning (based on fundamental principles). does not move.
2. Now you push a little harder and the block begins to move.
Learning to tell the story also is a part of learning to use the Block B moves with it without slipping. The blocks are speeding
other tools in the mathematical toolbelt. Examples in previous up.
papers of this series rely on constructing a coherent story. 3. Now you push so that the blocks continue to move but at a
• In “Anchor equations”: Making up a lap1(d) constant velocity. Block B moves with A without slipping.

• In “Toy models”: The pellet, cart, and spring1(e) Fig. 5. A qualitative problem helping students build the story of
what’s happening in a situation that looks simple, but isn’t.
• In “Reading the physics in a graph”: Cart on a tilted air
track with spring1(g) You can also ask students to explicitly tell the story of
what’s happening to related variables in a problem, as in The
Scaffold building a story tied to blended symbols rocket problem in the Supplementary Materials.16 Those ma-
and based on principles terials also contain additional examples of the next two types
One way that I particularly like to help students learn to of problems with detailed solutions.
build the physical story is with problem triples: a qualitative

02 March 2024 16:32:17


problem, a symbolic problem, and a computational problem. Foster coherence by telling a story in graphs with
The qualitative problem is tied to symbolic quantities to help multiple variables
them build the blend and to call on fundamental principles. In building physically reliable stories tied to blended vari-
The symbolic problem shows how to use equations in qualita- ables, it can help to tell the story using different lenses. One
tive reasoning and how one can build new equations from the way to do this is to generate graphs for multiple variables for a
combination. Once the mathematized story is built, setting up single situation [as described in Ref. 1(g)]. I show an example
the computation is straightforward and makes sense. in Fig. 6. Many of the examples in Ref. 1(g) are of this type.
To solve these problems, you have to apply Newton’s first
and second laws qualitatively to the conceptually blended Remove the scaffold by presenting problems that
variables and tell the story of what is happening to each object require both a story and the use of principles
in the problem. Figure 5 shows the parts of the qualitative Figure 7 shows a multiple-choice problem that requires
problem. (See the Supplementary Materials for the answers to blending, a good development of the storyline, and applica-
all the problems in the rest of the paper.16)
A young father is pushing a baby carriage
In the second (symbolic) version of the problem, students
at a constant velocity along a level street.
are asked to find relations among the various forces in the A friend comes by to chat, and he lets go
problem and to decide which forces are easily measured (force of the carriage. It rolls on for a bit, slows,
of the finger and weight of the boxes), and which are “invisible and comes to a stop. At t = 0, he is moving
forces” that have to be inferred in each case from applying with a constant velocity. At t1, he releases
Newton’s laws (normal and frictional forces). the carriage. At t2, the carriage comes to rest. Sketch qualitatively
In the third (computational) version, students are asked accurate graphs of each of the indicated variables.
to calculate various forces given numbers for the masses and • Position of the carriage
coefficients of friction. • Velocity of the carriage
One part of the third version is typically the only one stu- • Acceleration of the carriage
dents are given in traditional classes, assuming that they can • Net force felt by the carriage
build the necessary story and the resulting relations without • Force the man exerts on the carriage
guidance, and missing the enlightening comparison of the • Force of friction felt by the carriage
different situations. (The second and third versions of the Discuss the relation among the different graphs and why you
problem are in the Supplementary Materials,16 with detailed drew them as you did.
answers displaying how the story is used to support the calcu-
lation.) Fig. 6. Telling a story with multiple graphs (image from https://
www.123rf.com).

8 THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024


The figure below shows the first frame of a video of a juggler Analogous to a story, a physics problem has various
throwing a ball upward. The graph shows the vertical acceleration components. Depending on the context, it’ll have dif-
of the ball calculated from the video data. In the frame shown, ferent relevant characters who will interact through
he has caught the central ball and holds it momentarily at rest. the story’s timeline. For example, if a physics problem
This corresponds to t = 0 on the graph. He then throws the ball involves a model rocket, a fuse, and the earth, we can
upwards and it goes up, then comes most of the way down to his think of them as our story’s characters, each behaving
hand, but the video stops before he catches it. & interacting with one another in a particular way…
We could see the fuse behaving so that it influences
the rocket, changes its behavior, and resisting the
influence of the earth. This could be considered con-
flict, which climaxes and resolves as the rocket rises,
falls, and lands back down.
Six points are labeled on the graph. (The positive direction of the
At the end of the Supplementary Materials,16 I provide an-
y-axis is up.)
For each of the descriptive statements below, choose the letter
other essay question, Is the momentum of the train conserved?
or letters on the graph that match the description. If none match, asking students to think about how the story of a particular
put N. problem is tied to fundamental principle.
1. At this instant, the ball is moving upward with its maximum
Students often ask how to answer essay questions. I don’t
speed. want them treating it as either a memorized or an algorithmic
2. At this instant, his hand releases the ball. activity, so I simply respond, “Be sure you answer every part
3. At this instant, the ball has velocity 0. of the question” (if it has multiple parts) and “Don’t just do
4. At this instant, the ball is traveling upward. a brain dump, writing everything you know about the topic.
Look at the question and think about it.”
Fig. 7. A multiple-choice question that requires building a story
based on fundamental principles (juggler image © P. Laws,
used with permission).
Blend two independent stories—reconcile personal
experience and physical reasoning
tion of fundamental physics principles without providing The erroneous stories that students spontaneously build

02 March 2024 16:32:17


much scaffolding. (There is some scaffolding in the choice in response to a physics question are often built from quick
and ordering of the questions.) associations with everyday experience. While these are often
To get the full value of this problem, there needs to be a referred to as “misconceptions,” their stability often comes
class discussion drawing out how the students solved the from the ease of bringing them up,14 not necessarily from
problem and what they used to decide on the answers. coherent incorrect theories or mental models. If the question
is phrased differently, a different story can be activated and a
Use essay questions to get students to think about more correct one built and stabilized.
their thinking A useful tool for doing this is the paired-question tech-
It’s important to not just use stories tied to fundamental nique developed by Elby.25 In this approach, instead of offer-
physics principles but call out explicitly that you are doing so. ing students introductory questions that research has shown
In class discussions, in asking students to explain their rea- most will get wrong, a pair of matched questions are created
soning, and in lecture, explain how you are using stories both so that most students will get one right and one wrong, pro-
to approach creating a solution and to evaluate your solution. ducing a conflict.26 An example, shown in Fig. 9, helps rec-
It’s also valuable to ask your students to think about and oncile the automatically generated story that a smaller object
write about the use of stories explicitly. Figure 8 shows an ex- hitting a larger must feel a larger force (the p-prim “more
am question that I posed for my class one year.23 means more”) with Newton’s third law that says the two forces
More than half of the students provided satisfactory an- must be equal.27
swers, even under the pressure of an in-person 50-minute Elaboration and analysis of the pair of answers show stu-
handwritten exam. Here’s one answer that I was particularly dents that their intuitions are leading them into a conflict.
happy with.24 They are then guided to find their (essentially correct) “raw
Emma and Ryan were studying for their Physics exam. Ryan says, (a) A truck rams into a parked car. Intuitively, which is larger: the
"Prof. Redish says that one of the tools we're supposed to have force exerted by the truck on the car or by the car on the truck?
available for solving problems is telling the story. How can I 'tell a
(b) Suppose the truck has mass 1000 kg and the car has mass
story' of a physics problem?" Emma had a good explanation and
500 kg. During the collision, the truck slows by 5 m/s. How much
an example. What do you think she might have said? Note: This
speed does the car gain?
is an essay question. Your answer will be judged not solely on its
correctness, but for its depth, coherence, and clarity. Fig. 9. An “Elby pair” question to help students reconcile sponta-
neous with physics-accurate stories.
Fig. 8. An essay question to get students to think about the value
of telling the story.

THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024 9


advice on clear writing, and editorial suggestions through-
out the creation of this entire series. The work has been sup-
ported in part by a grant from the Howard Hughes Medical
Institute and NSF grants DUE-1504366 and DUE-1624478.

References
1. E. Redish, “Using math in physics,” Phys. Teach., (a) “Over-
view,” 59, 314–318 (2021); (b) “1. Dimensional analysis,” 59,
397–400 (2021); (c) “2. Estimation,” 59, 525–528 (2021); (d)
“3. Anchor equations,” 59, 599–604 (2021); (e) “4. Toy models,”
59, 683–688 (2021); (f) “5. Functional dependence,” 60, 18–21
(2022); (g) “6. Reading the physics in a graph,” 61, 651–656
Fig. 10. Reconciling the conflicting stories generated by Elby pair
questions.
(2023).
2. E. Redish and E. Kuo, “Language of physics, language of math:
intuition” that underlies both their answers and are guided to Disciplinary culture and dynamic epistemology,” Sci. & Educ.
maintain that intuition and to refine it in a way that leads to 24, 561–590 (2015).
results that are consistent with the physics they are learning. 3. D. Hammer, A. Elby, R. E. Scherr, and E. F. Redish, “Resources,
In this way, we hope to convince students that their intu- framing, and transfer,” in Transfer of Learning: Research and
itions about the physical world are valuable and, when prop- Perspectives, edited by J. Mestre (Information Age Publishing,
erly refined, support the physics knowledge they are learning 2004), ISBN: 978-1593111656.
by tying the story to a fundamental principle. Figure 10 shows 4. R. Schank, Tell Me a Story (Scribners, 1990), ISBN:
an example—an unbalanced force goes with acceleration, not 0684190494.
velocity, by the fundamental principle of Newton’s second 5. J. Gouvea, V. Sawtelle, and A. Nair, “Epistemological progress
law. in physics and its impact on biology,” Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ.
You can see more examples of all these types of questions Res. 15, 010107 (2019); V. Sawtelle and C. Turpen, “Leveraging
in the Open Source Tutorials available on PhysPort.28 a relationship with biology to expand a relationship with phys-
ics,” Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res 12, 010136 (2016).
Digging deeper 6. B. Geller, J. Rubien, S. Hiebert, and C. Crouch, “Impact of
introductory physics for the life sciences in a senior biology

02 March 2024 16:32:17


Many researchers have studied the issues I’ve explored in capstone course,” Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 18, 010120 (2022).
this paper. For broader, more complete research-based frame- 7. E. Redish, “Oersted Lecture 2013: How should we think about
works, see Kaldaras and Wieman,29 Odden and Russ,30 Spiers how our students think?” Am. J. Phys. 82, 537–551 (2014).
et al.,31 and Reiser et al.32 Others provide tools for studying 8. E. Churchill and J. Noble, “A demonstration of Lenz’s law?”
student success in telling stories (though they use different Am. J. Phys. 39, 285–287 (1971).
terms such as sensemaking with mathematical equations,33,34 9. F. Bartlett, Remembering (Cambridge University Press,
following and creating reasoning chains,35 or developing 1932/1995), ISBN: 978-0521483568.
mechanistic reasoning36,37). 10. J. Kotre, White Gloves (Norton, 1998), ISBN: 978-0393315257.
11. P. Lee, V. Fishlock, C. Webber, and C. Moss, “The reproductive
Instructional suggestions and resources advantages of a long life: Longevity and senescence in wild
With all of these problems, it is useful to re-present the female African elephants,” Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 70, 337–345
same situation used in homework in a quiz and/or exam— (2016); L. Brent et al., “Ecological knowledge, leadership, and
but with variations, so that the students can learn to use the the evolution of menopause in killer whales,” Curr. Biol. 25,
learned story as a template to vary from, applying the same 746–750 (2016); V. Caballero-Serrano et al., “Traditional eco-
principles to similar-looking problems but getting different logical knowledge and medicinal plant diversity in Ecuadorian
Amazon home gardens,” Global Ecol. Conserv. 17, e00524
results. This helps break their learned tendency to rely on
(2019).
memorization and one-step thinking.
12. R. L. Buckner and D. C. Carroll, “Self-projection and the
In addition to the problems in the Supplementary Materi- brain,” Trends Cognit. Sci. 11, 49–57 (2006).
als, there are many more “tell-the-story” problems from the 13. D. Schachter, Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and
NEXUS/Physics collection with solutions at the Living Physics Remembers, Updated edition (Mariner Books, 2002/2021),
Portal.38 ISBN: 978-0358325680.
A great example of using storytelling is Energy Theater, 14. D. Kahnemann, Thinking Fast and Slow (Farrar, Strauss, and
developed for helping pre- and in-service teachers build the Giroux, 2013), ISBN: 978-0374533557.
blend for the complex concept of energy.39 15. J. Tuminaro and E. F. Redish, “Elements of a cognitive model
of physics problem solving: Epistemic games,” Phys. Rev. Spec.
Acknowledgments Top. Phys. Educ. Res. 3, 020101 (2007).
I thank the members of the UMd PERG over the last two 16. Readers can access these materials at TPT Online at https://doi.
decades for discussion on these issues. I also thank two anon- org/10.1119/5.0159037, in the “Supplementary Material” sec-
ymous referees for useful comments. And finally, my heartfelt tion.
thanks go to Ginny Redish for encouragement, invaluable 17. R. Hodges, private communication, spring 2006.

10 THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, J anuary 2024


18. D. Griffiths, Introduction to Electrodynamics, 4th ed. (Prentice 18 (2023).
Hall, 1999), ISBN: 978-0321856562. 30. O. Odden and R. Russ, “Defining sensemaking: Bringing clari-
19. While this looks like a standard context-free problem, in an ty to a fragmented theoretical concept,” Sci. Educ. 103, 197–205
introductory physics class that covers the basics of electromag- (2019).
netism, it can be made authentic by framing it as a toy model 31. J. Spiers, M. Stetzer, B Lindsey, and M. Kryjevskala, “Exploring
that explains why it is dangerous for people with metal im- and supporting student reasoning in physics by leveraging
plants to have MRI exams. The rapidly changing magnetic field dual-process theories of reasoning and decision making,” Phys.
in the MRI induces current loops in the implant, and its strong Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 17, 020137 (2021).
magnetic field gradient can produce large forces on those cur- 32. B. Reiser et al., “Storyline units: An instructional model to
rents. support coherence from the students’ perspective,” J. Sci. Teach.
20. As is taking a limiting case, where the story makes the answer Educ. 32, 805–829 (2021).
obvious. G. White, T.-R. Sikorski, J. Landay, and M. Ahmed, 33. S. White Brahmia et al., “Physics inventory of quantitative liter-
“Limiting case analysis in an electricity and magnetism course,” acy: A tool for assessing mathematical reasoning in introducto-
Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 19, 010125 (2023). ry physics,” Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 17, 020129 (2021).
21. B. Lindsay et al., “Investigating student ability to follow reason- 34 . F. F. Zhao, L. Chau, and A Schuchardt, “Blended and more:
ing chains: The role of conceptual understanding,” Phys. Rev. Instructors organize sensemaking opportunities for mathe-
Phys. Educ. Res. 19, 010128 (2023). matical equations in different ways when teaching the same
22. J. Airey and C. Linder, “Social semiotics in university physics scientific phenomenon,” Int. J. STEM Educ. 8, 26 (2021).
education,” in Multiple Representations in Physics Education, 35. B. Lindsay et al., “Investigating student ability to follow reason-
edited by D. Treagust et al. (Springer, 2017), pp. 95–122. DOI: ing chains: The role of conceptual understanding,” Phys. Rev.
10.1007/978-3-319-58914-5_5. Phys. Educ. Res. 19, 010128 (2023).
23. My hour-long exams in introductory physics always include a 36. R. Russ, R. Scherr, D. Hammer, and J. Mikeska, “Recognizing
10-point (out of 100 max) essay question, hand graded by the mechanistic reasoning in student scientific inquiry: A frame-
instructor. I found these gave me feedback and insights into work for discourse analysis developed from philosophy of sci-
what students were learning (and where they were confused) ence,” Sci. Educ. 92, 499–525 (2008).
that was hard to get in any other way. 37. D. Hammer and E. vanZee, Seeing the Science in Children’s
24. Gabe Almario, with permission, private communication, Apr. Thinking: Case Studies of Student Inquiry in Physical Science
18, 2023. (Heinemann, 2006), ISBN:978-0325009483.
25. A. Elby, “Helping physics students learn how to learn,” Am. J. 38. E. Redish and the NEXUS Physics Team, “Using math in phys-

02 March 2024 16:32:17


Phys. 69, S54–S64 (2001); E. F. Redish and D. Hammer, “Rein- ics – Telling the story,” The Living Physics Portal, https://www.
venting college physics for biologists: Explicating an epistemo- livingphysicsportal.org/details/750da34c-52d5-4dad-9fb6-
logical curriculum,” Am. J. Phys. 77, 629–642 (2009). 0084202d257c.
26. The more traditional approach—elicit, confront, resolve—tends 39. A. Daane, L. Wells, and R. Scherr, “Energy theater,” Phys. Teach.
to put students down, undermining their trust in their physical 52, 291–294 (2014); H. Close et al., “Energy project,” PhysPort,
intuition rather than helping them build it. https://www.physport.org/methods/method.cfm?G=Energy_
27. T. Smith and M. Wittmann, “Comparing three methods for Project.
teaching Newton’s 3rd law,” Phys. Rev. Spec. Top. Phys. Educ.
Res. 3, 20105 (2007). E. F. (Joe) Redish is professor emeritus of physics at the University of
28. R. E. Scherr and A. Elby, “Open source tutorials,” PhysPort, Maryland. He is an AAPT Millikan and Oersted Award winner and an NSF
https://www.physport.org/methods/method.cfm?G=OST_Tu- Distinguished Teacher Scholar. His current research is in physics edu-
cation and focuses on building cognitive models of student thinking in
torials.
physics and on the use of math in physics problem solving.
29. L. Kaldaras and C. Wieman, “Cognitive framework for blended redish@umd.edu
mathematical sensemaking in science,” Int. J. STEM Educ. 10,

THE PHYSICS TEACHER t Vol. 62, January 2024 11

You might also like