You are on page 1of 9

Swiss Journal of Psychology, 74 (2), 2015, 65–73

E. Struys
Sw issJ. Psychol. 74 (2)et©al.: Co gnitive
2015 Co ntrol
Verlag Hans in Bilingual
Huber, Children
Hogrefe AG , Bern

Original Communication

Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children


Disentangling the Effects of Second-Language
Proficiency and Onset Age of Acquisition
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Esli Struys1, Ghazal Mohades1,2, Peggy Bosch3,4, and Maurits van den Noort1,5
1
Centre for Linguistics, Free University of Brussels, Belgium, 2University Hospital Brussels, Belgium,
3
Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands, 4LVR-Klinik Bedburg-Hau, Germany,
5
Kyung Hee University, Republic of Korea

Abstract. Studies comparing the cognitive control of bilingual and monolingual speakers are inconclusive about the nature and underlying
mechanisms of differences in language-related processing. In the present study, in order to disentangle the impact of second-language
onset age of acquisition and bilingualism on cognitive control, we compared a group of bilingual Dutch/French children who had started
acquisition of both languages at birth (simultaneous bilingual group) to a group of children who had started acquisition of their second
language at the age of 3 years (early bilingual group). Both groups had equal proficiency in the two languages. All participants completed
an extensive language test battery in Dutch and French and conducted a linguistic (verbal fluency) and a nonlinguistic cognitive control
task (the color Simon task). We found higher global accuracy rates for the simultaneous bilingual group on the Simon task. Surprisingly,
we did not find any differences in mean reaction time between the two bilingual groups. In conclusion, this study finds no advantage in
terms of verbal fluency, but does reveal that acquiring two languages from birth onward gives simultaneous bilingual children an advan-
tage on the Simon task, even over early bilingual children and when second-language proficiency is held constant.

Keywords: bilingualism, cognitive control, onset age of acquisition, proficiency, Simon task

Cognitive control refers to a number of processes that en- Sa, & Bialystok, 2011; Poarch & van Hell, 2012; Salvatier-
able the human brain to regulate cognitive activity in non- ra & Rosselli, 2012; Schwieter, 2008). The present study
routine situations. These processes include the suppression builds on these studies by examining the relative role of the
of irrelevant information and the selection of the appropri- effect of onset age of L2 acquisition and L2 proficiency on
ate response in the face of other possibilities (Bunge, Ha- Simon task performance in a sample of bilingual children.
zeltine, Scanlon, Rosen, & Gabrieli, 2002).
Previous research found an advantage for bilinguals on
cognitive control tasks (e.g., Garbin et al., 2010; Green,
1998; Hilchey & Klein, 2011). However, controversy re- Bilingualism as a Possible Simon Task
mains concerning the universality and explaining factors of
this so-called “bilingual cognitive control advantage” (e.g., Performance Modulator
Bialystok, 2009; Costa, Hernández, Costa-Faidella, & Se-
bastián-Gallés, 2009; Morton & Harper, 2007, 2009). The Simon task (Simon & Rudell, 1967) is one of the most
In order to further clarify this issue, recent research shift- frequently used experimental tasks in studies on general
ed its focus of attention from comparing a group of bal- cognitive conflict performance in bilingual participants
anced bilinguals to monolingual controls to comparisons (e.g., Bialystok, Craik, Klein, & Viswanathan, 2004; Bialy-
within bilingual and multilingual groups. In these studies, stok et al., 2005; Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008; Morton
various bilingual and multilingual samples are grouped ac- & Harper, 2007). Why would bilingualism modulate Si-
cording to the number of languages spoken, the age of sec- mon task performance? A first reason may be found in the
ond language (L2) acquisition, the attained L2 proficiency, fact that the cognitive mechanisms involved in solving cog-
the measure of equality between first language (L1) and L2 nitive conflict situations, such as those generated by the
proficiency, or by taking an individual differences ap- Simon task, share many characteristics with those being
proach (Linck, Schwieter, & Sunderman, 2012; Luk, De recruited to control language use in the bilingual brain.

DOI 10.1024/1421-0185/a000152 Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
66 E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children

There are two distinct reasons why bilinguals constantly dren with equal proficiency in both languages. However,
have to monitor their language systems: First, many studies there are several reasons to assume that these effects ex-
showed that both the target and nontarget language system ist. One reason is that differences in the recruitment of
are activated even when bilinguals use only one of their cognitive control processes are known to depend on the
languages (Thierry & Wu, 2007; van Heuven & Dijkstra, age of second-language acquisition. Wartenburger et al.
2010). A bilingual thus has to recruit cognitive processes (2003) compared three groups of bilinguals on grammat-
in charge of solving potential interference from the unneed- ical and lexical processing while their brain activity was
ed language. Second, depending on the amount of code being recorded in an MRI scanner. Their results showed
switching within a society or other variables like the pres- that the early bilinguals (who acquired L2 from birth on-
ence of official status for more than one language, a bilin- ward) had more automatic access to their L2 grammar
gual speaker must switch between different language sys- representations. In contrast to late bilinguals (who ac-
tems. Studies investigating these two forms of language quired L2 after the age of 18) with equal proficiency, they
control found a similar network for suppressing a nontarget did not rely on prefrontal regions involved both in lan-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

language system and for executing typically bilingual tasks guage and cognitive control. Many studies replicated the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

like switching, translation, and interpreting (Abutalebi, finding of age-related effects on bilingual language pro-
2008; Price, Green, & von Studnitz, 1999; Rodriguez-For- cessing and language control, showing that early learners
nells et al., 2005). The cognitive mechanism required to have a shared system for both languages, while they are
monitor language use of both origins can be summarized kept separate in late learners (e.g., Silverberg & Samuel,
as the need to inhibit the task-irrelevant language system 2004). A second reason is that very early childhood is an
and to select the task-relevant system. The neural cognitive optimal biological moment for acquiring certain features
processes that underlie these skills are not only specifically of language (Locke, 1997). Bilinguals who acquired a
engaged when dealing with controlling different language second language within this early time frame (age of ac-
systems, but also serve domain-general cognitive control quisition of L2 < 3 years) often outperform equally pro-
purposes (Abutalebi et al., 2011; Alvarez & Emory, 2006). ficient late bilinguals (age of acquisition of L2 > 10
This explains why certain linguistic variables, like onset years) on language control tasks (Isel, Baumgaertner,
age of acquisition and attained proficiency, may also have Thrän, Meisel, & Büchel, 2010). Since automaticity in
an impact on general cognitive control (Luo, Luk, & Bialy- controlling languages leads to (specific) general cogni-
stok, 2010). More specifically, onset age of acquisition and tive advantages for bilinguals (Christoffels, Kroll, &
attained proficiency, of which lexical robustness is a spe- Bajo, 2013), we ask whether performance effects related
cific part (Schwieter & Sunderman, 2008), have an effect to the age of acquisition can also be observed in bilingual
on cognitive processes that underlie language control skills children performing a nonlinguistic conflict task.
(Costa, Santesteban, & Ivanova, 2006). Many studies suggested that bilingualism boosts the de-
A second reason why bilingualism modulates Simon task velopment of cognitive control abilities in children (e.g.,
performance might be that the stimulus/response conflicts Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008). At the same time, how-
generated in the Simon task correspond well with the conflict ever, only few differences in cognitive control abilities are
generated by linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Lan- found in young adults who are at the peak of their atten-
guage conflict most often arises due to the automatic activa- tional abilities (Bialystok, Craik, & Ruocco, 2006; Bialy-
tion of the nontarget language system whenever the target stok, Craik, & Ryan, 2006; Bialystok, Martin, & Viswa-
language is heard and produced. This is comparable to the nathan, 2005). The development of cognitive control abil-
automatic response to the task-irrelevant feature that has to ities in children is well described (Munakata, Snyder, &
be inhibited during incongruent trials in the Simon task. Only Chatham, 2012), and research suggests that three cardinal
rarely does language conflict originate from simultaneous ex- developmental changes take place: During the first pro-
posure to two conflicting language systems. This would cor- gression, children increasingly learn to overcome old rules
respond to a stimulus/stimulus conflict task on a nonlinguistic and behavior in response to signals from their environ-
level, as is the case during Stroop (Stroop, 1935) and Flanker ment. Then, during the second transition, a qualitative
tasks (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). Incongruent trials in the shift takes place in which children have increasing capac-
Simon task do not contain a conflict in the stimuli themselves ity for active maintenance, creating opportunities to start
(Simon & Rudell, 1967). recruiting cognitive control proactively. Finally, during
the third transition, children stop relying on environmental
signals and become increasingly self-directed (for a de-
tailed description, see Munakata et al., 2012). In normal
Potential Differences Related to development, there are specific improvements in the abil-
ity to suppress inappropriate responses between the ages
Second-Language Onset Age of 8 and 12 (Bunge et al., 2002). Hence, one could expect
acquisition-related effects on these abilities in bilingual
To our knowledge, so far no study has focused on poten- groups to manifest themselves most prominently at this
tial differences related to L2 onset age in bilingual chil- age.

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children 67

Hypotheses experiment. Independent two-sample t-tests showed no sig-


nificant differences in age or number of years of parental
education between the two bilingual groups, indicating that
The goal of this study is to disentangle cognitive control
the groups were comparable on these variables.
effects from onset age of L2 acquisition and proficiency by
comparing two groups of Dutch/French bilingual children
who are equally proficient in both languages but who differ
in their onset of L2 acquisition. We expected to observe
Language Test Battery
age-related performance effects, namely, a processing ad-
An extensive language test battery comprised of modified
vantage for simultaneous bilingual children over early bi-
versions of several subtests of the Bilingual Aphasia Test
lingual children. These effects were specifically expected
(Paradis, 1989) was used to obtain an objective measure of
in incongruent trials because of the similarity between the
the children’s language proficiency in L1 and L2. The Bi-
conflict generated during that type of trials and language
lingual Aphasia Test is one of the only available test bat-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

conflicts in the bilingual mind.


teries with culturally and linguistically equivalent tests in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

over 50 languages. Instructions for each task were given by


a Dutch/French bilingual speaker. All tests were conducted
Methods in the participant’s L1 and L2 and the participants were
tested individually. In the simultaneous bilingual group, the
Participants language version used to test L1 corresponded to the L1
identified by the parents. The order of the languages was
A total of 34 bilingual children (17 boys and 17 girls) with counterbalanced across participants.
a mean age of 9 years and 5 months (range: 8 years–10 The following four tests were included in the test bat-
years, 11 months) were recruited from primary schools in tery: 1. Receptive vocabulary. The children heard 18 words
the Brussels region of Belgium. There were two experi- and were asked to choose the drawing that best depicted
mental groups in this study. the meaning of the word from four images. 2. Listening
The first group (hereafter, the “simultaneous bilingual comprehension. The children heard a short story, followed
group”) consisted of 18 children (9 boys and 9 girls) with a by five simple questions about the contents of the story. 3.
mean age of 9 years and 5 months (range: 8 years–10 years, Translation. The children heard 10 words and were in-
11 months). The children’s first languages were either Dutch structed to give the equivalent translation in the other lan-
(n = 12) or French (n = 6). The reported second languages guage. 4. Grammaticality judgments. The children heard
were French (n = 10), Dutch (n = 6), or English (n = 2). All eight sentences and were asked to assess the grammatical-
children in the simultaneous bilingual group had started ac- ity of these sentences and to give the correction.
quiring their two languages from birth onward in line with the In order to assess the extent to which the different lan-
“one parent–one language” approach. They had all attended guage versions of the language tasks were comparable, we
nursery school in one of the two languages from age 3 onward had two monolingual control groups of the same age per-
(12 in L1 and 6 in L2) and had had 2 hours of weekly instruc- form the receptive vocabulary, listening comprehension,
tion in the other language from age 8 onward. and grammaticality judgment tasks. The Dutch monolin-
The second group (hereafter, the “early bilingual gual group consisted of 15 children (6 boys and 9 girls)
group”) consisted of 16 children (8 boys and 8 girls) with with a mean age of 9 years and 7 months (range: 8 years,
a mean age of 9 years and 5 months (range: 8 years, 9 11 months–10 years, 11 months). The French monolingual
months–10 years, 11 months). All children in the early bi- group consisted of 15 children (8 boys and 7 girls) with a
lingual group had started acquiring French at home from mean age of 9 years and 4 months (range: 8 years, 8
birth on and had started learning Dutch in a Dutch-only months–9 years, 11 months). Both groups were recruited
nursery school from age 3 onward. Up to the point of ex- from primary schools in Belgium.
amination, they had only attended a Dutch-speaking SPSS 17.0 (SPSS Inc., 2008) was used for all statistical
school. All of these children had received 2 hours of weekly analyses. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for
instruction in French since the age of 8. each task and each language, with group (simultaneous bi-
To gather information about the participants’ key char- lingual, early bilingual) as the between-subjects factor,
acteristics, we asked the parents to complete an extensive were used to assess equal proficiency across groups and
questionnaire (Mondt et al., 2011) including the children’s across languages.
onset ages of L1 and L2 acquisition, self-ratings of their
children’s language proficiency in listening, speaking,
reading, and writing skills, using a 5-point scale ranging Verbal Fluency
from 0 (not proficient) to 4 (highly proficient), and number
of years of parental education. Only children whose profi- The children were instructed to produce as many words as
ciency in all language skills in both languages was reported possible in one minute starting with a certain letter. For
to be moderate to high were selected to participate in this Dutch, the letters were /p/, /v/, and /k/, for French /b/, /f/, and

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
68 E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children

/l/, and for English /p/, /f/, and /k/. Order was counterbal- stimulus are on the same side, for example, the response is
anced. Even though performance on verbal fluency tasks is to press the left key and the stimulus appears on the left. In
confounded with active vocabulary (Matute, Rosselli, Ardila, incongruent trials, the response key and the stimulus are on
& Morales, 2004), these tasks also tap specific control capac- opposite sides. Reaction times are typically faster and ac-
ities such as clustering words that belong to semantic subcat- curacy higher in congruent trials. These congruency-relat-
egories and switching between these subcategories (e.g., Ma- ed differences are referred to as the Simon effect (Simon &
tute et al., 2004; Snyder & Munakata, 2010). Rudell, 1967) and expressed in milliseconds (reaction time)
For the verbal fluency task, only the mean score for the and percentages (accuracy) (Bialystok et al., 2004). The
three conditions served as input for the analysis. According essential feature of the Simon task, which combines task-
to the CELEX database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Van Rijn, relevant and -irrelevant dimensions and automatic respons-
1993), the frequency of the phoneme as onset letter in each es, is similar to the characteristics of other frequently used
of the three languages was added as a covariate. cognitive control tasks, such as the Stroop task (Stroop,
To assess the comparability of the different language 1935) and the flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

versions of the verbal fluency task, we asked two monolin- Participants completed 156 trials composed of 81
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

gual control groups to perform the task (for background (51.9%) incongruent and 75 (48.1%) congruent trials. The
information about the two control groups, see above under stimuli were applied with a jittered inter-stimulus interval
“Language Test Battery”). (ISI) of 2.2 ± 0.56 s (maximum ISI = 3.18 s, minimum ISI
= 1.19 s) and a total duration of 6.5 min.
One-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests revealed a nor-
mal distribution for accuracy scores and response times for
Simon Task both types of trials (p > .05). Using individual mean accu-
In the Simon task (Simon & Rudell, 1967), a series of red racy and reaction time (RT) scores as input, mixed-model
2 × 2 ANOVAs with group as a between-subjects factor and
and green squares were presented either on the left or on
the right side of the screen. The width of the squares was congruency as a within-subjects factor were used to calcu-
10% of the width of the screen, and their center was posi- late group differences.
tioned vertically on its center line and horizontally at 15%
and 85% of its width. The participants were orally instruct-
ed to look at a computer screen and respond as quickly as Results
possible to the color of the squares, ignoring their location.
In order to make sure that task instructions had been well Language Test Battery and Verbal Fluency
understood, this task was practiced in a short training ses-
sion. During these practice trials, feedback was only given Mean scores on the different language tests and verbal fluen-
on the accuracy of the responses, not on the speed. cy tasks, specified for type of monolingual and bilingual
The Simon task consists of congruent and incongruent group, can be found in Table 1. One-way ANOVAs with
trials, depending on the match between stimulus and re- scores in listening comprehension, receptive vocabulary, and
sponse side. In congruent trials, the response key and the grammaticality judgments in L1 as dependent variables did

Table 1
Means and standard deviations of the different tests of the language test battery and the verbal fluency task in L1 and L2;
specified for type of monolingual and bilingual group
Language test scores Sim.a Bilingual Group Early Bilingual Group Dutch Monolingual Group French Monolingual Group
b
Verbal Fluency (L1) 6.3b (2.0)c 4.5b (1.2) 7.6b (2.3) 4.8b (1.6)
Verbal Fluencyb (L2) 5.5b (1.6) 6.2b (1.6) – –
d
Listening Comprehension (L1) 4.5 (0.6) 4.3 (1.1) 4.5 (0.7) 4.6 (0.6)
Listening Comprehension (L2) 4.3 (0.9) 4.3 (0.5) – –
Receptive Vocabularye (L1) 14.9 (1.3) 15.2 (1.6) 15.4 (1.5) 15.6 (1.1)
Receptive Vocabulary (L2) 14.8 (1.9) 13.9 (1.8) – –
Translationf (L2 > L1) 7.7 (1.4) 7.6 (1.5) – –
Translation (L1 > L2) 7.4 (1.4) 7.4 (1.1) – –
Grammaticality Judgmentsg (L1) 5.5 (1.0) 5.7 (1.4) 6.1 (0.8) 5.9 (0.9)
Grammaticality Judgments (L2) 5.9 (1.1) 5.9 (1.2) – –
Note. aSim. = Simultaneous. bPlease note that for Verbal Fluency, onset phoneme frequency was different for the Dutch and French versions of
the task. cStandard deviations are in parentheses. dThe maximum score on the Listening Comprehension Test = 5. eThe maximum score on the
Receptive Vocabulary Test = 18. fThe maximum score on the Translation Test = 8. gThe maximum score on the Grammaticality Judgments Test
= 8.

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children 69

not reveal any significant differences between the two exper- Dutch/French bilingual children with equal proficiency in
imental groups and the two control groups (p > .05). One-way Dutch and French. Performance on a stimulus/response
ANOVAs with scores in listening comprehension, receptive conflict task was taken as a measure of cognitive control.
vocabulary, and grammaticality judgments in L2 as depend- In order to gain a better understanding of the nature of the
ent variables did not reveal any significant differences be- bilingual children’s advantage on their Simon task perfor-
tween the two experimental groups (p > .05). An analysis of mance and the variables responsible for it, we expected the
covariance (ANCOVA) on the scores of L1 verbal fluency, effect of onset age of L2 acquisition to be seen in the pro-
with onset phoneme frequency as a covariate, did not show cessing of a nonlinguistic conflict task at a young age, even
any significant difference between the two experimental when proficiency is held constant. We expected to observe
groups and the control groups (p > .05). An ANCOVA on the age-related performance effects, that is, a processing ad-
scores of L2 verbal fluency, with onset phoneme frequency vantage for simultaneous bilingual children relative to ear-
as a covariate, did not show any significant difference be- ly bilingual children. These effects were specifically ex-
tween the two experimental groups (p > .05). pected on incongruent trials because of the similarity be-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tween the conflict generated on incongruent trials and


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

language conflicts in the bilingual mind.


Simon Task
The mean response times and accuracy scores of the simul- Language Test Battery and Verbal Fluency
taneous bilingual group and the early bilingual group on
the Simon task are given in Table 2. Unlike previous studies (e.g., Bialystok et al., 2004; Poarch
A mixed-model ANOVA with L2 onset age as a between- & van Hell, 2012), in which only traditional measures of
receptive vocabulary or grammar were taken to establish
Table 2 linguistic proficiency, the present study included four tests
Mean accuracy scores and reaction times for the correct of productive and receptive skills in the two target languag-
trials in the two conditions of the Simon task and for the es (see also Table 1) to ensure equal linguistic proficiency
Simon effect in both languages and between both bilingual groups. We
Simon task Simultaneous Early Bilingual found no differences between the simultaneous bilingual
Condition/Effect Bilingual Group Group group and the early bilingual group in listening comprehen-
Congruent ACCa 96.0 (2.8)b 94.5 (2.7) sion, receptive vocabulary, translation, or grammaticality
Incongruent ACC 94.9 (3.5) 90.3 (5.6) judgments. More importantly for the aim of our study, how-
ever, we found no significant interaction or main effects of
Congruent RTc 667.8 (125.7) 656.0 (93.3)
group, meaning that the two groups did not differ with re-
Incongruent RT 706.0 (135.4) 705.4 (95.8)
spect to language proficiency in Dutch or French. It should
Note. aACC = Accuracy score (in percentage). bStandard deviations be noted, though, that the test scores of both groups were
are in parentheses. cRT = Reaction time (in ms).
quite high, leaving the possibility open that the absence of
differences is partially attributable to ceiling effects. Fur-
subjects factor, congruency as a within-subjects factor, and thermore, the groups did not differ in terms of verbal flu-
accuracy scores as the dependent variable showed a main ency performance, which can be taken as an indicator of
effect of congruency, F(1, 32) = 9.35, p < .01, with higher equal active vocabulary and control capacities such as clus-
accuracy for congruent than for incongruent trials, and a main tering words that belong to specific semantic subcategories
effect of L2 onset age, F(1, 32) = 9.15, p < .01, with higher and switching between these subcategories (Snyder &
accuracy for the simultaneous bilingual group than for the Munakata, 2010).
early bilingual group. The interaction between Congruency
× L2 Onset Age, F(1, 32) = 3.21, p = .08, was only marginally
significant. Simon Task
The same analysis with reaction times as the dependent
variable only revealed a main effect of congruency, Our results for the Simon task revealed differences in ac-
F(1, 32) = 49.28, p < .001, with faster response times for curacy scores between two groups that had comparable
the congruent compared to the incongruent trials, but no proficiency in both languages across different levels of lin-
main effect of L2 onset age and no interaction were found. guistic analysis, thus clearly supporting our hypothesis. In
this study, the simultaneous bilinguals scored significantly
higher than the early bilinguals on global accuracy in the
Simon task (see also Table 2), which confirms the intricate
Discussion link between a linguistic variable like onset age of acqui-
sition and nonlinguistic cognitive control performance. In
This study was designed to disentangle the effects of onset some previous studies, however, not age but equal profi-
age of L2 acquisition and proficiency in two groups of ciency was put forth as the explaining factor behind many

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
70 E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children

of the processing advantages seen in bilingual groups rel- constantly recruited during the Simon task irrespective of
ative to monolingual controls (Bialystok et al., 2004). Our congruency, are influenced not only by frequent exposure
study complements these previous studies by showing that to more than one language system, but also by the onset
processing differences in a nonverbal conflict task can even age of language acquisition, with bilinguals from birth on-
be observed in a population of balanced bilinguals, with ward having the most efficient monitoring processes.
highly proficient bilingual children who acquired their lan- Surprisingly, as can be seen in Table 2, the effects related
guages from birth onward outperforming their equally pro- to onset age of acquisition were observed only in accuracy
ficient peers who started learning a second language slight- scores and not in reaction times. People resolving the sub-
ly later on in life. A first novelty of this finding thus lies in conscious conflict generated by cognitive control tasks
the fact that age-related effects cannot only be observed choose one of the following two trade-off strategies that
between bilinguals traditionally characterized as “early” have an impact on their performance: responding as fast as
and “late” with the age of 10 as a critical cutoff (Luk et al., possible with a lower accuracy rate or trying to avoid errors
2011, p. 588), but even at a much younger age in a sample by responding more slowly (e.g., Meyer, Irwin, Osman, &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of very early bilinguals. Second, these age-related effects Kounois, 1988; Wylie et al., 2009). Children were explic-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

can even be observed when both early bilingual groups itly instructed to respond as quickly as possible to the stim-
have attained equal overall proficiency in their languages, uli. Simultaneous bilingual children managed to maintain
thus minimizing the impact of attained L2 proficiency on a higher accuracy rate than early bilinguals while their re-
cognitive control skills. sponse times were equally fast. The advantage for simulta-
Moreover, we also predicted that the influence of onset neous bilinguals found in our study can thus be defined as
age of L2 acquisition would depend on the congruency of a more efficient speed-accuracy trade-off as this higher ac-
the trials and thus only be observed on incongruent trials. curacy was not compensated for by slower response times.
Because variance analyses yielded a highly significant This combination of higher accuracy and equal reaction
main effect of onset age, but no interaction between onset times was not found in any of the previous studies on the
age and congruency, our results did not support this hypoth- effects of bilingualism on Simon task processing before.
esis. Rather, it seems that the total task with its constant Except for one study (Bialystok et al., 2004), which found
switching between congruent and incongruent trials is a combination of faster responses and higher accuracy for
more difficult for the early bilinguals in terms of the per- bilinguals, most studies reported that a processing advan-
centage of correct responses. tage for bilinguals actually gave evidence of a more effi-
We suggest there are two reasons to explain the lack of cient speed-accuracy trade-off, characterized by the inverse
congruency-related performance differences. A task-relat- combination of faster responding combined with equal ac-
ed explanation might be found in the way our brain deals curacy scores (Martin-Rhee & Bialystok, 2008).
with congruency switching while we are performing the How to interpret these differences? Previous studies
Simon task. Automatic stimulus/response mapping, the showed that the choice between speed and accuracy is con-
driving force behind poorer performance on incongruent trolled by a decision threshold that maximizes a reward rate
trials, attenuates or even disappears when several incongru- (Bogacz, Hu, Holmes, & Cohen, 2010). When accuracy is
ent trials are presented in succession (Hommel, Proctor, & favored, decisions are slower; when speed is favored, more
Vu, 2004; Ridderinkhof, 2002; Stürmer, Leuthold, Soetens, mistakes are made (Pachella, 1974). In our task design, ac-
Schröter, & Sommer, 2002). Obviously, this cannot be curacy was emphasized over speed. During the practice tri-
avoided if the Simon task is presented in an event-related als of the Simon task used in this study, feedback was only
design with randomized order and a (nearly) even distribu- given on the accuracy of the responses but not on speed.
tion of congruent to incongruent trials. A second and task- Therefore, inaccurate responses were more strongly penal-
unrelated explanation for the lack of differences with re- ized than slow responses. Our participants may have cho-
spect to the Simon effect is that bilingualism does not nec- sen to favor accuracy over speed because they were only
essarily boost the conflict resolution processes itself, but rewarded when their response was accurate but not when
the monitoring processing system in charge of evaluating they responded faster. Importantly, the simultaneous bilin-
the need to use conflict resolution processes. Previous stud- guals showed greater accuracy on the Simon task, which
ies showed that the frequent recruitment of processes used might indicate that a more efficient speed-accuracy trade-
to monitor two or more language systems, as is the case in off may also be the result of an earlier onset age of language
bilinguals, influences the efficiency of monitoring process- acquisition in a bilingual population.
es (e.g., Costa et al., 2009). These are in charge of deciding
when to inhibit automatic stimulus/response mapping,
which is measured by global performance during the Simon
task (Abutalebi, 2008; Costa et al., 2009). This means that General Implications and Concluding
bilinguals attain higher accuracy and/or faster responses Remarks
than monolinguals on both congruent and incongruent tri-
als (Bialystok, Barac, Blaye, & Poulin-Dubois, 2010), Our This bilingualism study stresses the importance of onset
results indicate that these monitoring processes, which are age of L2 acquisition in a sample of so-called “early” (Luk

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children 71

et al., 2011, p. 588) bilingual children, even when profi- efficient speed-accuracy trade-off. Moreover, in general,
ciency is held constant. Our results can be explained in light the results of our study stress the importance of onset age
of recent psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic studies in of L2 acquisition in a sample of “early” bilingual children,
which group differences related to the onset age of L2 ac- even if proficiency is held constant, a factor which had not
quisition were found in a wide range of research domains been accounted for in bilingualism studies previously.
(e.g., Kovács & Mehler, 2009; Mohades et al., 2012; War-
tenburger et al., 2003). In line with our results, Kovács and
Mehler (2009) found improved speech discrimination per- Acknowledgments
formance in infants who were simultaneously exposed to
two language systems from birth in comparison to mono- This work was supported by the Research Council (grant
lingual infants. Moreover, in their (functional magnetic res- number: HOA23) of Vrije Universiteit Brussel.
onance imaging) study on grammatical processing in a lan-
guage task, Wartenburger and colleagues (2003) showed
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that early highly proficient bilinguals showed less prefron-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tal brain activation and more automatic processing than late


and equally proficient bilinguals did. Finally, in their (dif- References
fusion tensor imaging) study, Mohades and colleagues
(2012) revealed differences in neural structure between si- Abutalebi, J. (2008). Neural aspects of second language represen-
multaneous and sequential bilingual children. Qualitative tation and language control. Acta Psychologica, 128, 466–478.
doi 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.014
neurostructural differences between highly proficient bilin-
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Green, D. W., Hernandez, M.,
guals who have been simultaneously exposed to two lan-
Scifo, P., Keim, R., . . . Costa, A. (2012). Bilingualism tunes
guage systems at the optimal moment for language learning the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral
and those who acquired an L2 system slightly later in life Cortex, 22, 2076–2086. doi 10.1093/cercor/bhr287
may have an impact on general neurofunctioning that ex- Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the
tends beyond the domain of language processing (Mechelli frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Re-
et al., 2004). Taken together, these psycholinguistic and view, 16, 17–42. doi 10.1007/s11065-006-9002-x
neurolinguistic results may provide further support for our Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Van Rijn, H. (1993). The CE-
finding that differences in cognitive control performance LEX lexical database. Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Con-
can be found in a sample of “balanced” and “early” bilin- sortium.
gual children. Bialystok, E. (2009). Claiming evidence from nonevidence: A re-
Some related issues deserve attention in the future. Lon- ply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12, 499–
gitudinal studies could elucidate whether there is a certain 501. doi 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00868.x
age at which bilinguals lose this processing advantage. Un- Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Blaye, A., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2010).
answered is also the question whether the bilingual chil- Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolin-
gual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Devel-
dren’s advantage in conflict processing is due to the ab-
opment, 11, 485–508. doi 10.1080/15248372.2010.516420
sence of compensating practice effects. These compensat-
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., Chau, W., Ishii, R., Gunji,
ing effects were shown to reduce or eliminate the bilingual A., & Pantev, C. (2005). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive
effect in adults (Bialystok et al., 2004), but the interaction control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage,
between familiarity and language background has not been 24, 40–49. doi 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044
studied in children. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004).
The present study shows the effects of onset age of L2 Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the
acquisition on only one measure of cognitive control. The Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. doi 10.
combination of this effect with the absence of an effect on 1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
verbal fluency seems to indicate that the onset age of L2 Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ruocco, A. C. (2006). Dual-mo-
acquisition specifically affects the control processes re- dality monitoring in a classification task: The effects of bilin-
quired to perform the Simon task. However, there are many gualism and ageing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
other measures of cognitive control that tap into related chology, 59, 1968–1983. doi 10.1080/17470210500482955
processes. Further studies could investigate whether the ef- Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control
in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingual-
fects on the Simon task are also visible in Stroop tasks
ism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
(Stroop, 1935), attentional network tasks (ANT) (Fan, Mc- and Cognition, 32, 1341–1354. doi 10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.
Candliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002), Eriksen flanker 1341
tasks (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974), etc. Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilin-
In conclusion, this study reveals that acquiring two lan- gualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory con-
guages from birth onward gives simultaneous bilingual trol. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119.
children an advantage in cognitive control skills, even over doi 10.1177/13670069050090010701
early bilingual children and even when L2 proficiency is Bogacz, R., Hu, P. T., Holmes, P. J., & Cohen, J. D. (2010). Do
held constant. This advantage can be described as a more humans produce the speed-accuracy trade-off that maximizes

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
72 E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children

reward rate? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol- in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41. doi 10.1016/j.cogni-
ogy, 63, 863–891. doi 10.1080/17470210903091643 tion.2009.08.014
Bunge, S. A., Hazeltine, E., Scanlon, M. D., Rosen, A. C., & Ga- Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of
brieli, J. D. E. (2002). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual
and parietal cortices to response selection. NeuroImage, 17, children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 81–93.
1562–1571. doi 10.1006/nimg.2002.1252 doi 10.1017/S1366728907003227
Christoffels, I. K., Kroll, J. F., & Bajo, M. T. (2013). Introduction Matute, E., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Morales, G. (2004). Verbal
to bilingualism and cognitive control. Frontiers in Psychology, and nonverbal fluency in Spanish-speaking children. Devel-
4, 199. doi 10.3389/fpsyq.2013.00199 opmental Neuropsychology, 26, 647–660. doi 10.1207/
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, s15326942dn2602_7
N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ash-
Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. doi burner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Neurolin-
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 guistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature,
Costa, A., Santesteban, M., & Ivanova, I. (2006). How do highly 431, 757. doi 10.1038/431757a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

proficient bilinguals control their lexicalization process? In- Meyer, D. E., Irwin, D. E., Osman, A. M., & Kounios, J. (1988).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

hibitory and language-specific selection mechanisms are both The dynamics of cognition and action: Mental processes in-
functional. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, ferred from speed-accuracy decomposition. Psychological Re-
Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1057–1074. doi 10.1037/0278- view, 95, 183–237.
7393.32.5.1057 Mohades, S. G., Struys, E., van Schuerbeek, P., Mondt, K., van de
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon Craen, P., & Luypaert, R. (2012). DTI reveals structural differ-
the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception ences in white matter tracts between bilingual and monolingual
and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. doi 10.3758/BF03203267 children. Brain Research, 1435, 72–80. doi 10.1016/j.brain-
Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. res.2011.12.005
(2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional Mondt, K., Struys, E., van den Noort, M., Balériaux, D., Metens,
networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347. T., Paquier, P., . . . Denolin, V. (2011). Neural differences in
doi 10.1162/089892902317361886 bilingual children’s arithmetic processing depending on lan-
Garbin, G., Sanjuan, A., Forn, C., Bustamante, J. C., Rodriguez- guage of instruction. Mind, Brain, and Education, 5, 79–88.
Pujadas, A., Belloch, V., . . . Avila, C. (2010). Bridging lan- doi 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01113.x
guage and attention: Brain basis of the impact of bilingualism Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Re-
on cognitive control. NeuroImage, 53, 1272–1278. doi visiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10,
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.078 719–726. doi 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-se- Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2009). Bilinguals show an advan-
mantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, tage in cognitive control: The question is why. Developmental
67–81. doi 10.1017/S1366728998000133 Science, 12, 502–503. doi 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00867.x
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advan- Munakata, Y., Snyder, H. R., & Chatham, C. H. (2012). Develop-
tages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the ing cognitive control: Three key transitions. Current Di-
plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulle- rections in Psychological Science, 21, 71–77. doi 10.1177/
tin and Review, 18, 625–658. doi 10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7 0963721412436807
Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L (2004). A feature-in- Pachella, R. G. (1974). The interpretation of reaction time in in-
tegration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psy- formation-processing research. In B. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human
chological Research, 68, 1–17. doi 10.1007/s00426-003- information processing: Tutorial in performance and recogni-
0132-y tion (pp. 41–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Isel, F., Baumgaertner, A., Thrän, J., Meisel, J. M., & Büchel, C. Paradis, M. (1989). Bilingual Aphasia Test. Unpublished instru-
(2010). Neural circuitry of the bilingual mental lexicon: Effect ment. Retrieved from http://www.mcgill.ca/linguistics/re-
of age of second language acquisition. Brain and Cognition, search/bat/#langtests
72, 169–180. doi 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.008 Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and
Kovács, Á. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from sec-
speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611–612. ond-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of
doi 10.1126/science.1173947 Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 535–551. doi 10.1016/
Linck, J. A., Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G. (2012). Inhibitory j.jecp.2012.06.013
control predicts language switching performance in trilingual Price, C. J., Green, D. W., & von Studnitz, R. (1999). A functional
speech production. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, imaging study of translation and language switching. Brain,
15, 651–662. doi 10.1017/S136672891100054X 122, 2221–2235. doi 10.1093/brain/122.12.2221
Locke, J. L. (1997). A theory of neurolinguistic development. Brain Ridderinkhof, K. (2002). Activation and suppression in conflict
and Language, 58, 265–326. doi 10.1006/brln.1997.1791 tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses.
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in
between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cogni- perception and action. Attention & Performance, Vol. XIX
tive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, (pp. 494–519). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
588–595. doi 10.1017/S1366728911000010 Rodriguez-Fornells, A., van der Lugt, A., Rotte, M., Britti, B.,
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language pro- Heinze, H. J., & Münte, T. F. (2005). Second language inter-
ficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance feres with word production in fluent bilinguals: Brain potential

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern
E. Struys et al.: Cognitive Control in Bilingual Children 73

and functional imaging evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuro- the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence.
science, 17, 422–433. doi 10.1162/0898929053279559 Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and
Salvatierra, J. L., & Rosselli, M. (2012). The effect of bilingual- Performance, 28, 1345–1363.
ism and age on inhibitory control. The International Journal Thierry, G., & Wu, Y. J. (2007). Brain potentials reveal unconscious
of Bilingualism, 15, 26–37. doi 10.1177/1367006910371021 translation during foreign language comprehension. Proceedings
Schwieter, J. W. (2008). Language attitudes and gender: Descriptors of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Amer-
and nationalistic ideologies. The Buckingham Journal of Lan- ica, 104, 12530–12535. doi 10.1073/pnas.0609927104
guage and Linguistics, 1, 113–127. doi 10.5750%2Fbjll.v1i0.8 van Heuven, W. J., & Dijkstra, T. (2010). Language comprehen-
Schwieter, J. W., & Sunderman, G. (2008). Language switching sion in the bilingual brain: FMRI and ERP support for psycho-
in bilingual speech production: In search of the language-spe- linguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64, 104–122. doi
cific selection mechanism. The Mental Lexicon, 3, 214–238. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.002
doi 10.1075/ml.3.2.06sch Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Vill-
Silverberg, S., & Samuel, A. G. (2004). The effect of age of sec- ringer, A., & Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical
ond language acquisition on the representation and processing processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37, 159–170. doi
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of second language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01150-9


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

51, 381–398. doi 10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.003 Wylie, S. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ba-
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: shore, T. R., Powell, V. D., Manning, C. A., & Wooten, G. F.
The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. The (2009). The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response in-
Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304. doi 10.1037/ terference control in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia,
h0020586 47, 1844–1853. doi 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.025
Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2010). Becoming self-directed:
Abstract representations support endogenous flexibility in
children. Cognition, 116, 155–167. doi 10.1016/j.cogni-
Prof. Dr. Maurits van den Noort
tion.2010.04.007
SPSS, Inc. (2008). SPSS for Windows: Version 17.0. Chicago, IL: Centre for Linguistics
Author. Vrije Universiteit Brussel
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reac- Pleinlaan 2
tions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi 1050 Brussels
10.1037/h0054651 Belgium
Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, Tel. +49 160-96868801
W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in info@mauritsvandennoort.com

Swiss J. Psychol. 74 (2) © 2015 Verlag Hans Huber, Hogrefe AG, Bern

You might also like