You are on page 1of 6

‘We know intolerance of uncertainty is

a transdiagnostic factor but we don’t know


what it looks like in everyday life’:
A systematic review of intolerance
of uncertainty behaviours
Ravi Sankar, Lucy Robinson, Emma Honey & Mark Freeston

This article describes a systematic investigation into intolerance of uncertainty (IU) behaviours. This involved
systematic searches of IU behaviours, developing an expert consensus of the different types of IU behaviour and
classifying behaviours into these categories.

I
T IS ESTIMATED that one in six people suf- 4.4 per cent of the population (MacManus
fer from a common mental health disorder et al., 2009; National Collaborating Centre
during their lifetime (National Collaborat- for Mental Health, 2011b). According to the
ing Centre for Mental Health, 2011a). Gen- Laval model of GAD (Dugas et al., 1998),
eralised anxiety disorder (GAD) is a chronic intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is a central
condition associated with problems in occu- feature of GAD and captures the unhelp-
pational and social domains and affects ful cognitive, emotional and behavioural

10 Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017


A systematic review of intolerance of uncertainty behaviours

responses to uncertain events and situations Method


(Freeston et al., 1994). For example, an indi- Systematic searches
vidual high in IU would perceive the chance Advanced searches were conducted across four
of a negative event happening as unbearable search engines: Scopus, ProQuest, Web of Sci-
regardless of its likelihood of occurrence ence, and Ovid. For each database the phrase
(Carleton et al., 2007). Their appraisal of ‘intolerance of uncertainty’ was searched along-
uncertainty may trigger biological arousal side four studies presenting original versions of
and anxiety, and lead to actions designed to an IU scale (Dugas et al., 2002; Carleton et al.,
reduce or eliminate uncertainty (e.g. escape 2007; Comer et al., 2009; Freeston et al., 1994).
or seek excessive reassurance) (Boswell et al., Inclusion criteria comprised:
2013; Greco & Roger, 2003). (i) Reviews about the IU construct.
Boswell et al. (2013) investigated IU in (ii) Studies with a different IU scale to those
a transdiagnostic cognitive behavioural ther- used in systematic searches.
apy intervention for emotional disorders. (iii) Books with an IU chapter.
They found that decreases in levels of IU,
as measured by the IU scale (Freeston et al., Exclusion criteria included:
1994), were associated with post-treatment (i) Reviews on anxiety disorders that did not
reductions in symptoms of anxiety and depres- focus on IU.
sion. While acknowledging an inability to infer (ii) Studies with no new IU related measure.
causal relationships, Boswell et al. (2013) sug- (iii) Empirical studies with no primary data for
gested IU requires further examination to IU as these studies focused on psychopa-
improve the nature of transdiagnostic anxiety thology rather than IU.
treatments. Interventions may include help-
ing individuals to manage the psychological A total of 885 sources were found and thirteen
effects of uncertainty in order to increase sources met inclusion criteria and surpassed
their tolerance of uncertainty. all exclusion criteria (names of sources availa-
Birrell et al. (2011) argued that the phe- ble from first author). The authors identified
nomenology of IU has not been adequately 103 items specific to the IU construct. It was
scrutinised. This has important implications unknown which items reflected intolerance of
for treatment. For example, examination of uncertainty behaviours.
the features of IU may identify a set of mala-
daptive behaviours that can be addressed in Development of behavioural categories
therapy. Eliciting a description of these behav- The first author had several discussions with
iours could move clinicians into a position of an IU expert researcher (Professor Mark Free-
identifying common IU characteristics. ston) to construct a grid that classified types of
The aim of this review was to develop IU behaviours. This grid was e-mailed to seven
a classification system of unhelpful IU behav- other IU expert researchers (contact authors
iours. Part 1 involved a systematic search that for details). They were asked to rate the clarity,
drew together studies which presented original distinctiveness and clinical relevance of each
versions of an IU scale, as well as books, reviews category on a five-point Likert scale and pro-
and papers that included behavioural manifes- vide descriptive feedback.
tations of IU. All behavioural representations A median score of four or five on the Likert
of IU were extracted and placed into a single scale was taken to indicate very distinct, clear
list. Part 2 included developing a range of IU or clinically relevant categories. A median
behavioural categories based on expert con- score of three or below indicated a need for
sensus. Part 3 scrutinised a coding system by improvement of that category, which was then
calculating inter-rater reliability amongst two modified using descriptive feedback. Catego-
people who independently coded items from ries with a semi-interquartile range score of
the list into categories. one or more were considered to show exces-

Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017 11


Ravi Sankar, Lucy Robinson, Emma Honey & Mark Freeston

Table 1: Intolerance of uncertainty behavioural categories post expert review

Under-engagement
Avoidant behaviours motivated by attempts to disengage or keep away from future situations
with uncertain outcomes, feelings or thoughts. Presentation includes ‘doing any activity
to prevent engaging with uncertainty’. For example, a person procrastinates, escapes from
uncertain social situations and changes the topic of conversation away from that which creates
uncertainty.

Over-engagement
Approach behaviours driven by attempts to attain specific and certain/known outcomes in future
situations. Presentation includes ‘grinding away, going beyond lengths to find certainty’. For
example, a person excessively prepares for talks, excessively seeks reassurance while choosing
a dish from the menu, and plans unnecessarily when travelling to a familiar place.

Impulsive
Behaviours that immediately eliminate uncertainty about outcomes in situations or the distress
caused by uncertainty in situations. Presentation includes ‘doing without considering the
consequences or prior planning’. For example, a person chooses the first dish they see on the
menu, buys the first car they see, and makes snap decisions.

Dither
Behaviours that result in inaction due to hesitancy in choosing between at least two out of
three courses of action, namely under-engagement (avoiding future uncertain situations),
over-engagement (seeking future certainty) and impulsive (immediately reducing feelings of
uncertainty), without really pursuing any of them. Presentation involves ‘rapid deployment
or lack of deployment of these IU behaviours, which can lead to an end state of behavioural
paralysis’.
For example, a person alternates between moving forward slightly and suddenly braking while
driving onto a roundabout, flips between planning to go for a walk and watching television,
and fails to progress on an assignment.

Flip-flop
Behaviours that involve switching between at least two out of three courses of action – namely
under- and over-engagement and impulsive behaviours – over a longer time scale than dithering.
Presentation involves ‘partial deployment of these IU behaviours’. For example, a person spends
months reading reviews and visiting many houses before putting off buying a house for the next
few months, spends years training in a particular career and suddenly changes to another career,
and excessively prepares for an assignment but does not meet the deadline.

sive variation in scores and were also altered ■■ identifying further modifications of cate-
using descriptive feedback. gories;
■■ incorporating feedback to change defini-
Procedure tions;
Eight steps were taken to measure face validity ■■ determining whether issues raised by
of the IU behavioural categories: experts had been addressed;
■■ discussions with supervisor; ■■ having two independent coders use
■■ definitions of categories; expert-reviewed IU behavioural categories
■■ collating feedback from experts and setting to classify items from the IU item list; and
criteria to judge ratings; ■■ measuring inter-rater reliability.

12 Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017


A systematic review of intolerance of uncertainty behaviours

Four behavioural categories were initially Despite high median and low semi-
defined in the grid: interquartile ranges for dither/flip-flop, one
■■ under engagement; consistently raised issue involved separating
■■ over engagement; dither and flip-flop into two categories. Five
■■ impulsive; and categories of IU behaviours were constructed
■■ dither/flip-flop. after expert feedback (see Table 1).

Final definitions of these categories are shown Classification of items


in Table 1. Two independent coders were trained to use
the IU behavioural categories to classify pilot
Results items. Once good inter-rater reliability was
Expert ratings and changes to categories achieved they then classified the 103 items.
based on descriptive feedback are presented. Items were sorted into IU or non-IU behav-
Then the classification of items into IU behav- iours. Inter-rater reliability was determined by
ioural categories by two independent coders Kappa coefficient scores. Both coders agreed
is summarised. that 42 items were IU behaviours and 38 items
were non-IU behaviours.
Expert consensus summary Of 42 observations, there were 27 (64.29
All classes of behaviour were judged to be per cent) observed agreements. The number
clinically salient based on median scores of observed agreements expected by chance
of four or higher. Over-engagement and was 10.60 (25.34 per cent). A Kappa coeffi-
under-engagement required further distinc- cient indicated that the strength of agreement
tion and clarity of definition based on median across categories was considered to be moder-
scores of three or lower. Descriptive feedback ate, k(42) = 0.52.
was consistent with these ratings (e.g. ‘the dis- All categories of IU behaviour had
tinction between over- and under-engagement observed agreements of 83.33 per cent or
needs to be made more clear… it’s not clear higher. Strength of agreement by two coders
what the difference is between avoiding uncer- was considered to be good for under- and
tainty and seeking certainty’. over-engagement, moderate for dither, fair for

Table 2: Number of items categorised into each intolerance of uncertainty category


Coder 2
Under- Over- Impulsive Dither Flip flop Total
engagement engagement

Coder 1 Under- 9* 1 4 1 0 15
engagement
Over- 0 12* 1 0 0 13
engagement
Impulsive 0 0 0* 0 1 1

Dither 0 4 1 5* 1 11

Flip flop 0 1 0 0 1* 2

Total 0 18 6 6 3 42

* = Matched item

Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017 13


Ravi Sankar, Lucy Robinson, Emma Honey & Mark Freeston

flip-flop, and worse than expected by chance after five categories were constructed. Given
for impulsive. Fewest items were categorised that flip-flop and dither were separated into
into impulsive and flip-flop, whilst under and two categories, a second round of feedback
over-engagement had the most items. with median ratings of four or five would have
indicated high utility. This may then increase
Discussion confidence in researchers and clinicians who
The aim of this review was to develop catego- wish to use this classification system.
ries of IU behaviours. Feedback from seven The classification system was developed in
experts in this field contributed to the devel- this review and needs to be validated in future
opment of five categories of IU behaviours: studies. It could also have been helpful to ask
■■ under-engagement; the experts to classify the final 103 items to
■■ over-engagement; provide a robust evaluation of the IU behav-
■■ impulsive; ioural categories.
■■ dither; and
■■ flip-flop. Clinical implications
Noticing behavioural manifestations of IU in
A moderate level of agreement (k = 0.52) was clinical practice could be worthwhile given
found between two coders in classifying items that it is a transdiagnostic factor (Mahoney &
into these categories. McEvoy, 2012). Targeting this construct may
The most plausible reason for a lack of enhance a person’s ability to tolerate the uncer-
strong agreement may involve the item pool. tainty inherent in exposure treatments and
Some categories had very few items allocated standardised cognitive behavioural therapy
to them. Although poor agreement was found packages. For example, a patient who has social
for impulsive IU behaviour, expert consensus anxiety might avoid exposure-based tasks. While
supported the utility of this category. There- a clinician may view their behaviour as disen-
fore, all categories should be retained for fur- gagement, the patient may instead be strug-
ther exploration. gling with the uncertainty of trying something
Items were extracted from well validated new. The patient may benefit from strategies
IU scales and other IU sources. One interpre- to manage under-engagement. An IU-specific
tation is that the literature adds little to our behavioural experiment may best target behav-
understanding of behavioural manifestations iours driven by a fear of uncertainty.
of IU. More work is required to validate these In summary, this review extracted items from
behaviours in analogue and clinical popula- the literature to identify behavioural expressions
tions. It would be interesting to explore the of IU. Most of these expressions reflected two
prevalence of all five categories in populations types of IU behaviour: over- and under-engage.
with GAD, social anxiety and depression. Eliciting behavioural descriptions from people
This review is the first to use a categorical with high levels of intolerance of uncertainty
approach to describe common IU behaviours; could be the next step to determining the clini-
however, the item pool was limited. Other meth- cal relevance of these behaviours.
ods may be used to identify more IU behav-
iours (e.g. card sorting tasks) (McEachan et Authors
al., 2010). Semi-structured interviews may help Dr Ravi Sankar, Clinical Psychologist, Wallsend
discriminate IU from anxiety-related behav- Health Centre, Newcastle upon Tyne;
iours by addressing the functional aspects. r.sankar@newcastle.ac.uk; Dr Lucy Robinson,
Clinical Psychologist, Newcastle University;
Limitations Dr Emma Honey, Clinical Psychologist, New-
There are several flaws in this review that limit castle University; Professor Mark Freeston,
the conclusions which can be drawn. The valid- Professor of Clinical Psychology, Newcastle
ity of categories was not re-evaluated by experts University

14 Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017


A systematic review of intolerance of uncertainty behaviours

References
Birrell, J.A., Meares, K., Wilkinson, A. & Freeston, A preliminary test of a conceptual model. Behaviour
M. (2011). Toward a definition of intolerance of Research and Therapy, 36, 215–226.
uncertainty: A review of factor analytical studies of Freeston, M.H., Rhéaume, J., Letarte, H., Dugas, M.J.
the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Clinical Psy- & Ladouceur, R. (1994). Why do people worry? Per-
chology Review, 31, 1198–1208. sonality and Individual Differences, 17, 791–802.
Boswell, J.F., Thompson-Hollands, J., Farchione, T.J. Greco, V. & Roger, D. (2003). Uncertainty, stress, and
& Barlow, D.H. (2013). Intolerance of uncer- health. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
tainty: A common factor in the treatment of emo- 1057–1068.
tional disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69, Mahoney, A.E.J. & McEvoy, P.M. (2012). Trait versus
630–645. situation-specific intolerance of uncertainty in a
Buhr, K. & Dugas, M.J. (2002). The intolerance of clinical sample with anxiety and depressive disor-
uncertainty scale: Psychometric properties of the ders. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 41, 26–39.
English version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, McEachan, R.R.C., Lawton, R.J. & Conner, M. (2010).
40, 931–945. Classifying health-related behaviours: Exploring
Carleton, R.N., Norton, P.J. & Asmundson, G. (2007). similarities and differences amongst behaviours.
Fearing the unknown: A short version of the Intol- British Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 347–366.
erance of Uncertainty Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disor- National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health
ders, 21, 105–117. (2011a). Common mental health disorders. CG 123.
Comer, J.S., Roy, A.K., Furr, J.M. et al. (2009). The Leicester: British Psychological Society & London:
Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale for Children: A Royal College of Psychiatrists.
psychometric evaluation. Psychological Assessment, National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health
21, 402–411. (2011b). Generalised anxiety disorder in adults.
Dugas, M.J., Gagnon, F., Ladouceur, R. & Freeston, CG 113. Leicester: British Psychological Society &
M.H. (1998). Generalised anxiety disorder: London: Royal College of Psychiatrists.

CPD workshops
­EVENT DATE
Safeguarding: Understanding and responding to child sexual abuse and exploitation 8 Sept

The psychology of inpatient care: Surviving, thriving and leading in institutional settings 15 Sept

Aviation psychology: Clinical skills for working with aircrew 21–22 Sept

Working with complex couples:


29–30 Sept
An introduction to PACT – A psychobiological approach to couple therapy

Co-productive relationships 18 Oct

Psychological therapy for obesity: Using a cognitive behavioural approach to


9 Nov
promote weight loss and manage relapse

www.beta.bps.org.uk/events

Clinical Psychology Forum 296 – August 2017 15

You might also like