You are on page 1of 12

Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior

A synthesis of person- and relational-level factors that influence bullying


and bystanding behaviors: Toward an integrative framework
Idean Ettekal a,⁎, Becky Kochenderfer-Ladd a, Gary W. Ladd a,b
a
T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics, Arizona State University, USA
b
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In this article, we attempt to integrate several theoretical models with the goal of explicating more broadly the
Received 17 March 2015 determinants of bullying and bystanding behaviors. In particular, participant role perspectives (e.g., models in
Accepted 13 May 2015 which bullying is conceptualized in the context of multiple participants) serve as an overarching frame for exam-
Available online 22 May 2015
ining and formulating hypotheses about two additional types of determinants: (a) child attributes—specifically,
the role of children's person-related development (i.e., social-cognitive, emotion, and moral processing), and
Keywords:
Bullying
(b) children's experience with multiple socializing agents (i.e., peers and teachers). Empirical evidence is
Peer victimization reviewed to identify relevant constructs, and critical analyses of evidence within and across conceptual domains
Participant roles are utilized to formulate novel hypotheses about how person- and relational-level (socialization) processes may
Aggression contribute to individual differences in bullying and bystanding behaviors.
Social cognitions © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76
1.1. Overview of synthesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76
1.1.1. Overview of person-level factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76
1.1.2. Overview of relational-level factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
1.1.3. Overview summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
2. Person-level factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
2.1. Social cognitive processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
2.1.1. Overview of social cognitive processing perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
2.1.2. Social cognitive processing models and bullying and bystanding behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
2.1.3. Implications of social cognitive processing perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
2.2. Emotion processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
2.2.1. Overview of emotion processing perspectives. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
2.2.2. Role of emotion processing on bullying and bystanding behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
2.2.3. Synthesis of emotion processing and the SIP framework . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
2.3. Moral processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
2.3.1. Overview of moral processing perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
2.3.2. Role of moral processing on bullying and bystanding behaviors. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
2.3.3. Synthesis of moral processing and the SIP framework . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3. Relational-level factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3.1. Peer influence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3.1.1. Overview of peer influence processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3.1.2. Peer influence processes and bullying and bystanding behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
3.1.3. Implications of peer influence processes for hypothesis generation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
3.2. Teacher influence processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83
3.2.1. Overview of teacher influence processes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83
3.2.2. Role of teacher influence processes on bullying and bystanding behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83

⁎ Corresponding author at: T. Denny Sanford School of Social and Family Dynamics, P.O. Box 873701, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-2502, USA.
E-mail address: Idean.Ettekal@asu.edu (I. Ettekal).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.05.011
1359-1789/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
76 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

3.2.3. Implications of teacher influence processes for hypothesis generation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84


4. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
Acknowledgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84

1. Introduction bullying behaviors which are aimed at supporting or defending victims


and discouraging bullying. Thus, throughout this synthesis, we examine
Over the past two decades, research on bullying has proliferated, and how each of the determinants reviewed may differentiate among pro-
the school context (e.g., classrooms, hallways, playgrounds, cafeterias, bullying and anti-bullying bystanding behaviors and roles.
lavatories) has been the principal place where researchers have studied To organize this large and diverse literature on the factors that con-
this phenomenon. It is now clear that bullying (e.g., repeated aggressive tribute to bullying and bystanding behaviors, we utilize a child (person)
behavior aimed at intentionally harming an individual who is weaker or and environment (relational) framework (see Kochenderfer-Ladd,
in a more vulnerable social position than the perpetrator) is an interna- Ladd, & Kochel, 2009 for similar treatment of risk factors for peer victim-
tional problem that crosses gender, socioeconomic, cultural and racial ization). To identify person-level factors that contribute to bullying and
lines (see Jimerson, Swearer, & Espelage, 2010). Central to this body of bystanding behaviors, we draw from social cognitive theories broadly,
research is the question of what factors encourage, contribute to, and and social information processing (SIP) models in particular. According
sustain bullying behavior in school contexts. Moreover, because it has to SIP models (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2004; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Lemerise
been argued that bullying is a group phenomenon that, in addition to & Arsenio, 2000), it is postulated that three forms of processing operate
bullies and their victims, involves bystanders who take active conjointly within the individual (i.e., cognitive, emotional, and moral) to
participant roles in bullying (e.g., reinforcing or assisting bullies; organize and guide social behavior. Consistent with these perspectives,
defending victims; see Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & we examine research on each of these types of processes as a means of
Kaukiainen, 1996), researchers have also expanded their research to identifying person-level factors that may be implicated in bullying and
examine factors that influence bystanders' behavior. bystanding behaviors. At the relational (environment) level, we consid-
Investigators have worked from a diversity of theoretical assump- er children's experience with multiple socializing agents (i.e., peers and
tions, frameworks and premises to advance our understanding of the teachers) that may account for individual differences in bullying and
bullying phenomenon (e.g., see Espelage & Swearer, 2010; Hong & bystanding behaviors.
Espelage, 2012; Salmivalli, 2010; Schwartz, Kelly, Duong, & Badaly, Using this person and relational framework, the remaining sections
2010; Veenstra et al., 2007). Collectively, this large, and growing, body of this article are used to: (1) identify and critically analyze within
of research has provided valuable insight into the nature of bullying person and relational factors (processes) that may underlie individual
and bystanding behaviors, including the individual characteristics and differences in bullying and bystanding behaviors, and (2) propose
social motivations of the participants, as well as how social context promising avenues for integrating perspectives on bullying and
factors contribute to, and sustain, bullying behavior. However, because bystanding behaviors by generating hypotheses about potential causal
investigators tend to examine these premises independently rather pathways among the identified processes.
than jointly, it is less clear how multiple variables, operating conjointly,
interact with one another to influence bullying and bystanding
behaviors. 1.1.1. Overview of person-level factors
Accordingly, our goal for this article is to review what is known At the person level, models that attempt to explain how children's
about the factors contributing to bullying and bystanding behavior in social-cognitive development impacts their behavioral decisions are rel-
school contexts, and begin to synthesize findings in ways that offer evant to research on bullying and bystanding behaviors. For example,
promising avenues for extending research in novel directions. Although, social information processing (SIP) perspectives (Crick & Dodge, 1994)
individually, theoretical perspectives provide unique insights, we posit have been used to generate hypotheses regarding how children's
that the integration of premises and constructs across perspectives aggressive behavior may stem from poor information processing at
would provide for a more systematic and nuanced understanding of various ‘steps’ in the decision process (i.e., cognitive processing). More
the multifaceted causes of bullying and bystanding behaviors. Thus, it recently, reformulations of the SIP framework have been proposed
is our hope that this synthesis will not only extend investigative which more explicitly integrate children's emotion and moral
agendas, but also encourage greater integration of the theoretical processing with social cognitive processes (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2004;
perspectives that drive research in this area. Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). Applying this reformulated SIP perspective
to the study of bullying allows for a more complex and nuanced set of
hypotheses to be tested. Specifically, the reformulated SIP model
1.1. Overview of synthesis theoretically argues that collective variations in children's social cogni-
tive (e.g., social goals, interpretations of social interactions and outcome
Consistent with the conceptualization of bullying as a group process expectations), emotion (e.g., emotion understanding, affective ties
(Atlas & Pepler, 1998; Craig, Pepler, & Atlas, 2000b), this article is between children, and empathy), and moral (e.g., moral knowledge,
grounded in a participant role perspective (Salmivalli et al., 1996). The reasoning, and disengagement) processing interact in ways that orga-
central proposition of this perspective is that bullying and bystanding nize and motivate children's behavior. Extending this logic, it is reason-
behaviors can be construed as a function of the larger social context in able to hypothesize that the likelihood that children engage in specific
which they occur. In other words, it is argued that the extent to which bullying or bystanding behaviors would depend on person-level factors
bullying arises and is sustained is dependent not only upon the involve- in these three domains. Thus, in this paper, relevant frameworks,
ment of bullies and their victims, but also the behavioral responses of premises, and findings from SIP perspectives which include social
those who witness it (i.e., bystanders; see Salmivalli, 2010; Salmivalli cognition, emotion and moral development perspectives are reviewed.
et al., 1996). For example, some participant roles (i.e., assistants and re- Although important insights have been achieved by investigating
inforcers) are characterized by pro-bullying bystanding behaviors such each of these developmental processes individually, by examining the
as joining in, applauding, or enticing bullying. In contrast, other partici- interactions and functioning of multiple domains simultaneously, new
pant roles (i.e., defenders) may be regarded as demonstrating anti- hypotheses can be formed to better understand how these interrelated
I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86 77

processes collectively predict bullying and bystanding behaviors. Spe- steps that influence individuals' behavioral responses during social in-
cifically, we propose that combinations of social cognitive, emotion, teractions. These steps involve: 1) attending to and encoding social
and moral processes have synergistic effects on bullying and bystanding cues, 2) interpreting cues, 3) selecting a social goal (clarification),
behaviors that better account for individual differences in these 4) generating possible responses (access or construction of possible
behaviors than any one process alone. In the sections that follow, we response), and 5) selecting and enacting a response (decision; outcome
offer specific hypotheses to test the proposition that bullying and evaluation). During the first two steps, individuals attend to a specific
bystanding behaviors (and their corresponding participant roles) may social cue or event and interpret why the event occurred and what
be distinguishable based on unique profiles of social-cognitive, emotion resulted from it. In the third and fourth steps, a social goal is selected
and moral processing. for the event based on desired outcomes, and memories of prior
outcomes and responses to the social cue. In the final step, potential re-
1.1.2. Overview of relational-level factors sponses are evaluated and the one with the most favorable evaluation is
At the relational level, it is proposed that two salient socializa- selected. Although these steps are described in a relatively sequential
tion influences operating within the school context—peers and process, their real-time functioning occurs in parallel, with numerous
teachers—collectively impact bullying and bystanding behaviors. These feedback loops.
socialization agents are theorized to influence bullying and bystanding An integral component of this perspective is that individuals form
behaviors via two process mechanisms: proximal (i.e., direct) and distal cognitive mental representations (i.e., the database; see Crick & Dodge,
(i.e., indirect) influences. With respect to proximal influences, it is argued 1994) from which they can access stored information pertaining to
that by directly intervening in bullying episodes, peers and teachers are their long-term memories, social knowledge, acquired social rules or
able to influence bullying and bystanding behaviors. In comparison, distal norms and relational schemas. Presumably, this stored information (in
mechanisms include factors that indirectly effect bullying and bystanding this database) is the accumulation of individuals' experiences across
behaviors via their influence on children's person-related development their life history and reflective of their collective socialization experi-
(i.e., social cognitive, emotion and moral processing). ences and interactions. Stored information influences online cognitive
It is important to recognize that other socializing agents, besides processing, and over time, these processes result in revisions to the
peers and teachers, also influence children's person-related develop- database. Thus, when children begin formal schooling, their prior
ment. For instance, parents engage in socialization processes socialization experiences with caretakers are likely to influence their
(e.g., parenting styles and parent–child attachment) that also expectations about their interactions with peers, and in turn, children's
contribute to children's social cognitive, emotion, and moral develop- interactions with peers subsequently contribute to revisions in the
ment (e.g., see Dykas & Cassidy, 2011). However, because this review's database.
focus is school-related bullying and bystanding behaviors, peers and
teachers receive primary consideration. Both of these socialization agents 2.1.2. Social cognitive processing models and bullying and bystanding
are present in school environments and, therefore, are most likely to be behaviors
involved in (and affect) bullying and bystanding behaviors. Although Crick and Dodge's (1994, 1996) SIP framework was initial-
ly applied to the study of the development of aggressive behaviors
1.1.3. Overview summary (e.g., proactive and reactive aggression), their social cognitive process-
Undoubtedly, across childhood and adolescence, children's social ing steps can be readily applied to the study of bullying and bystanding
cognitive, emotion and moral processes become more sophisticated behaviors. By differentiating each of the social cognitive steps and iden-
and their socialization experiences with peers and teachers evolve. As tifying the tendencies (or ‘biases’) that children exhibit at each step, it
children strive for greater independence and autonomy and rely may be possible to predict whether or not children will engage in bully-
increasingly on peers for support, it is reasonable to expect that teacher ing or specific bystanding behaviors. For example, it could be theorized
influences wane as peer influences increase, reflecting normative devel- that bullying and pro-bullying bystanding behaviors may be due to spe-
opmental changes. Thus, although we offer integrative hypotheses that cific processing biases at various steps, such that: 1) the child attends to
are broadly applicable for school-aged children and adolescents, it is specific forms of peer behavior which can be 2) interpreted as provoca-
important to note that there most likely are developmental variations tive, annoying, or vulnerable. Their dominance or status goals may be
in the causal mechanisms that predict bullying and bystanding behav- activated (step 3), aggressive or bullying responses are accessed (step
iors as children mature and experience normative changes in their 4), and it is determined that such goals can be achieved with little or
person-related development and in the role and influence of multiple no personal consequence (step 5).
socialization processes. With respect to how children encode and interpret social cues from
In the following sections, each person-level factor (i.e., social cogni- peers (steps 1 and 2), there is evidence that bullies seem to look for, and
tive, emotion and moral processes) and relational influence (i.e., peer attend to, peer behavior that can be interpreted as vulnerable and
and teacher) is reviewed. In particular, each section begins with a offering an easy target. Specifically, investigators have found that bullies
broad theoretical overview and summarizes the central arguments selectively target peers who are vulnerable in the sense that they may
that can be gleaned from each perspective. Next, relevant research be physically weaker, have lower social standing and are disliked by
and empirical evidence specific to bullying and bystanding behaviors other children (i.e., rejected), or are perceived to have fewer or no allies
are reviewed in light of these perspectives. Finally, based on these the- (e.g., friends or classmates) with higher social standing or status
oretical rationales and empirical evidence, each section ends with a (Hodges & Perry, 1999; Salmivalli & Isaacs, 2005; Veenstra et al.,
set of suggestions for integration across perspectives and delineating 2007). Thus, it is plausible that when children with a tendency to
relevant testable hypotheses. bully interpret social cues that indicate a child is defenseless, they are
more likely to consider behaving aggressively.
2. Person-level factors Social goals (step 3) are also critical to the application of SIP perspec-
tives to understanding bullying and bystanding behaviors. For instance,
2.1. Social cognitive processes where some children see a defenseless peer as an opportunity to gain
status or tangible rewards, others may see a peer in need of protection
2.1.1. Overview of social cognitive processing perspectives or help. In acknowledgement of the importance of social goals, investi-
Earlier formulations of the social information processing (SIP) gators have identified two broad types of goals that appear to underlie
perspective (e.g., Crick & Dodge, 1994) focused primarily on social motivations for bullying and various types of bystanding behaviors.
cognitive processes; specifically, they delineated five social cognitive The first type may be conceptualized as goal-directed which
78 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

encompasses instrumental, dominance and agentic goals (related to ac- to pursue these goals when they interpret social cues that discourage
quiring influence and admiration). It has been hypothesized that chil- these behaviors (e.g., a high likelihood of retaliation or bystander
dren who endorse goal-directed social goals would evidence higher intervention) or have negative expectations about whether bullying
rates of bullying behaviors (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Salmivalli, Ojanen, can be used to attain their status goals. These points serve as a theoret-
Haanpää, & Peets, 2005; Sijtsema, Veenstra, Lindenberg, & Salmivalli, ical premise for bystander interventions that attempt to reduce bullying
2009). Support for such hypotheses comes from social dominance behaviors. That is, if bystander interventions can promote anti-bullying
(Pellegrini & Long, 2002) and resource control theories (Hawley, behaviors such as supporting or defending the victim, they may alter
1999), according to which children are inherently motivated to attain bullies' typically positive outcome evaluations about bullying. Without
higher and more powerful social status positions which can hypotheti- such interventions or negative peer feedback, bullying tends to result
cally be attained by bullying. In contrast, the second type of social goals in a favorable outcome thereby reinforcing the child's more stable nor-
are conceptualized as relational goals; children who tend to endorse re- mative beliefs about the use of aggressive behavior, which subsequently
lational and communal goals (related to making friends, and increase the likelihood of bullying in future social interactions.
prosociality) are hypothesized to have lower rates of bullying behaviors Similar hypotheses regarding the interactions among these social
and higher rates of anti-bullying bystanding behaviors such as cognitive processes can be generated for other participant roles and
supporting or defending the victim and intentions to intervene (Rigby may help to differentiate children with anti-bullying attitudes who
& Johnson, 2006; Rigby & Slee, 1993; Salmivalli & Voeten, 2004; actively intervene (i.e., defenders) from those who respond more
Salmivalli et al., 2005). passively when bullying occurs (i.e., outsiders). For example, depending
In addition to attending to social cues, and selecting social goals, that on the nature of their social goals, children who have negative expecta-
make bullying or pro-bullying behaviors appealing to some children, tions about bullying behaviors and express anti-bullying attitudes may
before selecting an aggressive response, they also have to believe they exhibit considerably different response patterns when they observe
are capable of engaging in such types of behavior (step 4) and that social cues that indicate that bullying might occur. On the one hand,
acting aggressively will “pay off” in expected ways (step 5). In other those with relational goals are hypothesized to enact response options
words, before selecting a response to enact, children form outcome such as supporting the victim, intervening (i.e., defending the victim),
expectations about what the likely result will be. For instance, if past or supporting other children who actively intervene (Rigby & Johnson,
experiences behaving aggressively were ineffective or met with serious 2006; Rigby & Slee, 1993; Salmivalli & Voeten, 2004). On the other
consequences, children may not see aggression as a viable option. How- hand, those with an avoidance goal in favor of self-protection and
ever, if prior aggression was rewarded with status, or desired tangible remaining uninvolved (to avoid also being victimized) are more likely
possessions, aggression becomes a real option. Researchers have found to be passive bystanders and those with instrumental or status goals
that children who believe that aggression will lead to desirable out- are more likely to passively (or actively) support the bully so as to not
comes have a tendency to act aggressively (Crick & Dodge, 1996; jeopardize their own social standing (Juvonen & Galván, 2010). In
Perry, Perry, & Rasmussen, 1986). Moreover, not only do some children evaluating each of these goals and response options, children must
associate positive outcome expectations with bullying, but they also access prior experiences and beliefs (stored within their database) in
tend to minimize the potential costs associated with possible negative order to select a response that is associated with the most desirable
outcomes (e.g., getting in trouble, the victim retaliating, the harm goals and favorable outcome evaluations. When accessing this stored
caused to the victim; see Boldizar, Perry, & Perry, 1989). Considering information, emotion and moral processes are also likely to influence
that bullying, by definition, is a behavior that is repeated over time children's decision making. We turn to an examination of these factors
and characterized by a power differential, the likelihood of a positive in the next two sections.
outcome seems inherently favorable for bullies (in terms of their goal
achievement), which in turn would encourage them to engage such
behaviors. 2.2. Emotion processes
Extending this argument beyond bullies, it is plausible that children
who engage in anti-bullying bystanding behaviors are more likely to be- 2.2.1. Overview of emotion processing perspectives
lieve that bullying results in negative outcomes and defending or In addition to social cognitive processing, more recent SIP frame-
supporting the victim in positive outcomes (see Rigby & Johnson, works have clarified and highlighted the role of emotion processes in
2006). Moreover, children's outcome evaluations are likely to be influ- social information processing and subsequent behavioral responses
enced by their self-efficacy for enacting a behavior successfully. Thus, (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). According to these reformulated perspec-
children who have higher self-efficacy beliefs in their abilities to defend tives, emotion processes are theorized to organize and motivate
victims are more likely to actively intervene on behalf of a victim as children's social cognitions and in turn influence their subsequent
opposed to being passive bystanders (i.e., outsiders; Gini, Albiero, goal-directed behaviors (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). Researchers have
Benelli, & Altoè, 2008; Pöyhönen, Juvonen, & Salmivalli, 2010). focused their attention on two aspects of emotion processes believed to
be intricately linked to SIP generally and to bullying and bystanding be-
2.1.3. Implications of social cognitive processing perspectives haviors specifically; namely, emotion understanding and empathy.
Future investigations may benefit from considering that bullying These two emotion processes are themselves so intertwined that before
responses are most likely activated by a combination of three social children can feel and express empathy, they must first recognize and un-
cognitive biases pertaining to how children interpret social cues, select derstand others' emotion cues (Feshbach, 1978). This interconnectedness
social goals, and have positive outcome evaluations about bullying. Con- is reflected in the construal of empathy as a multi-dimensional construct
sidering that these three processes are in a dynamic state of continual that includes both a cognitive and affective component (Davis, 1994)
feedback, it is plausible that when children are motivated by dominance wherein the cognitive component includes the ability to recognize and
and have positive evaluations about bullying, they are more selectively understand other children's emotions whereas the affective component
attending to social information in their environments that will allow refers to the ability to share or experience others' emotions. Thus, within
them to be dominant (i.e., they will look out for easy targets). Consistent the reformulated SIP framework, the cognitive component of empathy is
with Reijntjes et al.'s (2013) argument, it could be hypothesized that reflected in the early stages of SIP when children encode and interpret
bullying is most likely to occur when children recognize opportunities social cues that would also include the emotion cues of their peers
to attain their status goals, and achieving these goals outweigh the po- (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). The extent to which such cues are accurately
tential drawbacks. Thus, even if children are motivated by goals that interpreted or met with empathic responses likely depends on the child's
increase the chances of bullying, it is plausible that they are less likely social goals or processing at later steps.
I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86 79

2.2.2. Role of emotion processing on bullying and bystanding behaviors at least for adolescent boys, is negatively associated with their likeli-
Considering that bullying is an emotionally provocative phenome- hood of engaging in bullying behaviors (Caravita et al., 2009; Jolliffe &
non, the role of emotions in understanding bullying and bystanding Farrington, 2011). However, the evidence base in support of similar as-
behaviors cannot be overstated. It is our contention that any model of sociations among adolescent girls or for younger children of either sex is
the factors that contribute to bullying and bystanding behaviors would sparse (Caravita et al., 2009; Gini, Albiero, Benelli, & Altoè, 2007; Jolliffe
not be complete without consideration of these important processes. In & Farrington, 2011).
other words, children's emotion understanding (i.e., emotion cue detec- While the debate continues, research is clearly needed to help clarify
tion), empathy, and emotional or affective ties with other children are the role emotion processing, especially cognitive and affective compo-
theorized to be important determinants of individual differences in bully- nents of empathy, plays in bullying behavior. Findings thus far have
ing and bystanding behaviors. been sparse and equivocal; in particular, as suggested above, the role
Despite the importance of emotion processes in understanding of empathy in bullying behavior appears to vary according to the com-
bullying-related behaviors, there has been little research done in this ponent of empathy examined, sex, and age of sample.
area—and much speculation, assumption, and debate. For example, In addition to the findings on empathy and bullying, investigators
because SIP perspectives assume bullying stems from deficits or biases have also examined the role of empathy on bystanding behaviors.
in children's processing, there seems to be a tacit assumption that emo- They tend to find that empathy—and in particular, affective empathy—is
tion processing deficits are partly culpable. That is, there has been some positively associated with defending behaviors (Caravita et al., 2009;
debate among researchers as to whether or not bullies have the ability Gini et al., 2007; Pöyhönen et al., 2010). Notably, a study by Gini et al.
to accurately encode or interpret others' emotion cues (cognitive empa- (2008) found that higher empathy was associated with both active
thy) and act empathically (affective empathy). For example, it is reason- defending behaviors and passive bystanding behaviors. Thus, although
able to speculate that bullies do not refrain from hurting others because empathy appears to be an important antecedent to promoting
they do not recognize the distress that is being caused to another child children's active intervention in bullying episodes, examining it inde-
(i.e., lack emotion cue detection). Moreover, their apparent lack of re- pendent of other person-related processes was not sufficient in differ-
morse seems to stem from a lack of realization that their behaviors entiating children who actively intervened on behalf of the victim
have hurt or had damaging consequences for their victims. Thus, it is from children who responded passively (i.e., outsiders). Moreover,
not hard to understand why some may believe that bullying results Pöyhönen et al. (2010) found that affective empathy was more strongly
from emotion processing deficits, especially at the cognitive empathy associated with defending behaviors among children with higher social
level. status, suggesting that empathic children are more likely to engage in
However, what little evidence we have to address this issue indi- defending behaviors (as opposed to passive bystanding) when their
cates otherwise. Specifically, researchers have found that bullies do social position affords them the opportunity (i.e., social power or stand-
not target peers for harassment at random, but rather select targets ing) to more effectively engage in these behaviors with fewer risks of
who appear vulnerable (Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988)—and some aggres- retribution.
sors even feel rewarded when their attacks lead to expressions of fear Another emotion process that can contribute to children's goal-
and distress in their victims (Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates, & Pettit, directed behaviors and subsequently their decision to bully is the
1997; Smithmyer, Hubbard, & Simons, 2000). Moreover, children who emotional or affective tie that characterizes the relationship between
are chronically victimized are more likely to report fearful emotions as two children (Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). In support of this notion,
opposed to anger (Kochenderfer‐Ladd, 2004), suggesting that bullies there is evidence that bullies selectively target victims who they dislike
are detecting this fear and viewing such peers as easy targets for further (Veenstra et al., 2007). In contrast, when there is a strong positive affec-
harassment. tive tie, children are more likely to support or defend a victim who they
Moreover, in their paper addressing the debate on social deficit like or consider a friend (Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999;
models of bullying, Sutton, Smith, and Swettenham (1999a) argue Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997).
that bullies do not suffer from social skills deficits per se, but instead
may be quite socially savvy and skilled manipulators. Most relevant to 2.2.3. Synthesis of emotion processing and the SIP framework
the present synthesis, is that they argue that bullies have the In this section, we attempt to integrate hypotheses about emotion
perspective-taking skills (i.e., theory of mind) to recognize and under- and cognitive processing (SIP premises) in ways that could potentially
stand other children's emotions, but choose to bully anyway (Sutton, extend research on bullying and bystanding behaviors. It is our conten-
Smith, & Swettenham, 1999b). Some support for this position can be tion that emotional and cognitive social-information processes have a
garnered by a study conducted by Caravita, Di Blasio, and Salmivalli synergistic effect on bullying and bystanding responses such that each
(2009). Specifically, they found a moderation effect of empathy and so- of the emotion processes discussed (i.e., emotion cue detection, empa-
cial status such that cognitive (but not affective) empathy was positive- thy, emotional ties) influence children's processing at each SIP step
ly related to bullying for popular girls—but not for unpopular girls. Such (e.g., social goals; outcome expectations), and the combination of
findings suggest that these popular girls were using their emotion these person-level factors have greater explanatory power in predicting
knowledge to select targets for bullying who would further their social bullying and bystanding behaviors than examining them separately.
standing or help them maintain their social status. It is important to Working from a participant role framework, it can be posited that
note that no such findings were found for boys; moreover, findings bullying or bystanding behavior is the result of a unique set (or profile)
did not indicate whether or not such girls evidenced affective empathy of person-level (cognitive and emotional) factors. For example, one hy-
toward their victims, as no correlation was found. pothesis stemming from this work worthy of testing is that a combina-
Nevertheless, while arguing that bullies are skilled at emotion pro- tion of dominance goals, positive expectations about bullying, negative
cesses such as cognitive components of empathy (emotion understand- emotional ties with their targets, lack of empathy, and cue interpreta-
ing), Sutton et al. (1999a) also acknowledge that bullies may lack the tion signifying weakness (e.g., fear or distress) would predict bullying
affective dimension of empathy. Proponents of SIP perspectives counter roles more accurately than any factor alone. In contrast, emotion pro-
argue that the lack of affective empathy is a form of social processing cesses such as positive emotional ties, high empathy, and inattention
deficit. Thus, by understanding their victims are afraid and recognizing to emotion cues that signal vulnerability, are expected to dampen
that their behavior is causing distress, but deciding nonetheless to en- children's motivation or goals to be dominant and thereby reduce the
gage in bullying to attain their goals is evidence of processing deficits likelihood of bullying.
(Arsenio & Lemerise, 2001; Jolliffe & Farrington, 2006). Moreover, In addition to dominance goals, some children appear to be more
there is some preliminary evidence to suggest that affective empathy, motivated by perceived popularity, a related but conceptually distinct
80 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

dimension of children's social standing (Cillessen, Mayeux, Ha, de (i.e., latent mental structures) and retrieve this information during online
Bruyn, & LaFontana, 2014). When bullying is motivated by a desire for SIP. When children access and retrieve moral knowledge related to a
popularity, it may function more as a means of ‘fitting in’ and being specific event during processing, they are more likely to consider the
accepted by other popular children; thus, the emotion cues of moral implications of their behaviors. In contrast, if such knowledge is
bystanders, who can confer or withhold status, become more important not retrieved, then behavioral responses are likely to be enacted with-
than those of potential victims. In this case, children motivated by out consideration of the moral implications.
popularity are more apt to selectively focus their attention on the emo- The integrative perspective proposed by Arsenio and Lemerise
tion cues of bystanders who via their affective and behavioral responses (2004) highlights the potential disconnect between children's moral
provide either support or disapproval of these behaviors. Thus, it may be values and behaviors, but it does not explicitly consider the potential
that some children do not attend to the emotion cues of their victims, role of moral disengagement as an explanatory framework. However,
but instead focus on other social cues that are more pertinent to their Bandura's (1999, 2002) social-cognitive theory of moral agency offers
goals. This rationale is consistent with participant role perspectives a framework for considering moral disengagement as an important
which highlight the role of reinforcers and assistants, children who pro- determinant of bullying and bystanding behaviors. According to his
vide direct reinforcement and support for the bully (Salmivalli et al., theory, children adopt moral standards over time and use these stan-
1996). dards to guide future behaviors. Moreover, his theory delineates several
With respect to other participant roles and forms of bystanding be- mechanisms, collectively described as moral disengagement, which are
haviors, some bystanders may express feelings of anxiety or fear when theorized to mediate the association between children's moral stan-
they witness bullying, similar to victims. These emotional reactions, in dards and their actual behaviors. An examination of such mechanisms
combination with having a negative emotional tie with the victim, are can be useful in understanding how children can believe that bullying
likely to activate avoidance goals and behavioral responses such as is wrong and harmful, and yet engage in these behaviors despite such
staying uninvolved or not doing anything to intervene. Such responses beliefs.
are characteristic of the outsider role in which children respond to bully-
ing in passive ways (Pozzoli & Gini, 2010). In contrast, some children 2.3.2. Role of moral processing on bullying and bystanding behaviors
may experience feelings of excitement, and in combination with instru- Given that bullying is clearly a moral transgression, it is important to
mental goals and a negative affective tie with the victim, these emotion consider the degree to which children are aware of the moral implica-
processes may encourage them to support the bully (consistent with re- tions of their bullying or bystanding behaviors. For example, it is possi-
inforcer and assistant participant roles). Alternatively, children who are ble that children who engage in bullying lack adequate moral
empathic, feel sad or upset when they observe bullying, have a positive knowledge and reasoning skills (e.g., do not have moral knowledge
affective tie with the victim and relational goals are more likely to inter- that can be retrieved during SIP). Alternatively, such children may
vene (consistent with the defender role; Barhight, Hubbard, & Hyde, have the ability to think about the moral implications of their behaviors
2013; Pozzoli & Gini, 2010). Moreover, empathic children are more like- more broadly, but do not activate their moral reasoning when deciding
ly to actively intervene when they have characteristics that are indica- to bully another child. Evidence tends to favor the latter perspective.
tive of having positive outcome evaluations of defending behaviors Specifically, researchers (Dodge et al., 1997; Menesini et al., 2003)
(e.g., they have high social self-efficacy or high social status; see Gini have found that children who engage in bullying are capable of recog-
et al., 2008; Pöyhönen et al., 2010). nizing the harmful intentions of behaviors that are directed at them,
Each of these propositions imply an emotion by social-information but do not consider the harmful intentionality of their behaviors
processing interaction (i.e., a moderated effect), in which the combina- targeted at other children. Thus, it appears that children who bully selec-
tion of, and variations in, person-level factors predict individual differ- tively access moral knowledge (values) when it is to their advantage,
ences in bullying and bystanding behaviors more efficiently and but are less likely to access it when they bully others.
accurately, than examining them in isolation. Future investigations of Consistent with this analysis of moral processes, one possible
such propositions are clearly warranted and could make a significant explanation for why children selectively apply their moral values to
advancement in our understanding of the determinants of bullying self-referent (but not other-referent) events is that when children are
and bystander behavior. in the act of bullying, they do not view their behaviors as a moral
issue and thus, do not consider the moral implications of their behav-
2.3. Moral processes iors. Stated differently, bullies are less likely to judge the wrongness
(i.e., harm) of their own behaviors and are more likely to use personal
2.3.1. Overview of moral processing perspectives or social conventional reasoning rather than moral reasoning when
Drawing upon Arsenio and Lemerise's (2004) reformulation of Crick evaluating aggressive behaviors (Menesini et al., 2003; Murray‐Close,
and Dodge's (1994) SIP model, children's moral processes are viewed as Crick, & Galotti, 2006). Conversely, when they are the targets of bullying,
an integral part of bullying and bystanding behaviors because these ac- it is easier for them to recognize the moral implications of such behav-
tions inherently involve issues of morality; that is bullying is an event iors (e.g., that it is wrong and hurtful).
that raises questions of right and wrong, fairness, human rights, and In light of findings indicating that children know that bullying is
ethical behavior. Moreover, the integration of moral domain perspec- wrong and harmful, an important question becomes “why do some
tives with the SIP framework is a natural extension given that theories still choose to engage in such harmful behavior?” Bandura's (1999,
of moral development assume that children's moral reasoning about so- 2002) mechanisms of moral disengagement offers insights into this
cial events have an influence on their behavioral responses. Using a apparent disconnect between beliefs and action. In particular, moral dis-
moral domain perspective, interpretations of social events and evalua- engagement refers to a set of social-cognitive mechanisms that children
tions of possible behavioral responses involve distinguishing between use to reduce the moral consequences (e.g., blame, guilt, shame) of their
moral and non-moral events. In other words, when a situation is viewed behaviors, and to rationalize using bullying in self-serving ways.
as one of morality, issues of right and wrong are raised. In contrast, if the Bandura (1999, 2002) delineates several social cognitive mechanisms
social situation is not construed as one of morality, behavior is more through which individuals are able to exercise moral disengagement to
likely to be guided by social conventions based on social norms or expec- justify or rationalize immoral behavior. First, cognitive restructuring refers
tations derived from authority figures, traditions and customs regarding to multiple processes including: a) moral justification (e.g., “I was just
appropriate behavior (see Nucci, 1981, 2001). Specifically, according to protecting myself” or “I was teaching him a lesson”), b) euphemistic
Arsenio and Lemerise's (2004) integrative framework, children store labeling (e.g., “I was only joking”), and c) advantageous comparison
knowledge about moral-relevant events in their long-term memory (e.g., comparing one's behavior to others' that are even more immoral
I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86 81

or harmful). Second, displacement or diffusion of responsibility refers to responsiveness functions as a prerequisite for more advanced forms of
minimizing one's role in the harmful behavior to reduce personal moral reasoning and retrieval of morally relevant information. Children
responsibility (e.g., “Everyone does it”). Third, minimizing, ignoring or who lack empathy are less likely to retrieve moral knowledge when bul-
misconstruing negative consequences refers to how individuals distort lying others (Arsenio & Lemerise, 2004; Gini, 2006). Alternatively, chil-
the outcomes of their behaviors (e.g., “No one got hurt”). Fourth, dehu- dren may also lack moral sensitivity (i.e., the ability to recognize
manization and blaming the victim refer to how individuals attribute immoral behaviors and their harmful consequences) which reduces
the behavior to the victim by thinking that the victim deserved to be the tendency for moral reasoning and increases moral disengagement
harmed. Collectively, these cognitive strategies suggest multiple ways (Thornberg & Jungert, 2013).
that children can reduce feelings of guilt and shame when they bully In addition to empathy and moral sensitivity, other aspects of
others and to evaluate their behaviors in a more positive manner. children's SIP—in particular their social goals and outcome
It is conceivable that certain forms of moral disengagement are partic- expectations—are hypothesized to influence whether children access
ularly relevant for distinct bullying and bystanding behaviors. For in- moral knowledge. For example, it is possible that certain social goals
stance, whereas bullies may justify their behaviors by blaming the (i.e., instrumental or dominance goals) discourage children from constru-
victim (e.g., “because they are annoying”), children who take on more ing bullying as a moral issue, thereby reducing the likelihood that they ac-
supportive roles (e.g., assistants, reinforcers) may also engage in other cess moral knowledge (from their database) which, in turn, increases
strategies such euphemistic labeling (e.g., “that they are just joking their likelihood of bullying. Further, children's social goals contribute to
with the victim”) and minimizing their personal role. Similarly, passive how they weigh the advantages gained from bullying against the cost
bystanders (outsiders) may place the responsibility on adults or other of harming another child, such that children with instrumental goals
children to intervene rather than assuming personal responsibility. How- are more likely to favor the personal gains whereas those with relational
ever, research is still needed to determine whether different forms of goals are more likely to be concerned with how their behaviors affect
moral disengagement are role specific; thus far, measurement work sug- others (Crick & Dodge, 1996). Thus, one hypothesis worth examining is
gests that different mechanisms of moral disengagement tend to load that children who endorse instrumental goals and think bullying is easy
onto a single factor. Consequently, the vast majority of extant research and effective for goal attainment are less likely to access morally relevant
has assessed moral disengagement as a unitary construct (Gini, Pozzoli, knowledge, and more likely to exercise moral disengagement, which in
& Hymel, 2014b; Hymel, Schonert-Reichl, Bonanno, Vaillancourt, & Hen- turn increases the chances that they will engage in bullying.
derson, 2010). Similar hypotheses could be generated by considering the conjoint
Considering the different mechanisms of moral disengagement collec- effects of moral, emotion and social-information processes on children's
tively, children who engage in bullying tend to have the highest levels of bystanding behaviors. For example, the combination of low moral
moral disengagement and these effects appear to be more pronounced in disengagement, high moral sensitivity, high empathy, relational goals,
adolescence as compared to childhood (Barchia & Bussey, 2011a; Gini, positive outcome evaluations for defending victims along with greater
Pozzoli, & Bussey, 2014a; Gini et al., 2014b). Moreover, compared to chil- self-efficacy for defending—are proposed to have a synergistic effect
dren who take on more supportive roles (i.e., assistants and reinforcers), on anti-bullying bystanding behaviors.
those who are ringleaders appear to be more morally disengaged (Gini, In sum, a synthesis of research on person-level factors offers a rich
2006). In contrast, those who engage in defending and passive bystanding foundation from which to generate novel hypotheses regarding the
behaviors tend to have lower levels of moral disengagement (Almeida, role of children's social cognitive, emotion and moral processing in bul-
Correia, & Marinho, 2009; Gini, 2006). Notably, although children who lying and bystanding behaviors. By integrating multiple person-level
take on more passive bystanding roles (i.e., outsiders) tend to have factors into single testable models, we can begin to have a more com-
lower levels of moral disengagement, there is some evidence of heteroge- prehensive framework for predicting individual differences in bullying
neity within this participant role such that passive bystanders who are and bystanding behaviors.
unconcerned about the bullying have higher moral disengagement than
passive bystanders who express more guilt (see Obermann, 2011). 3. Relational-level factors
In light of evidence that both defenders and passive bystanders tend
to have lower levels of moral disengagement (compared to children 3.1. Peer influence
with pro-bullying behaviors), this construct alone does not adequately
differentiate children who actively intervene in bullying (i.e., defenders) 3.1.1. Overview of peer influence processes
from those who are passive bystanders (i.e., outsiders). Thus, moral Borrowing from the participant role perspective, it is argued that
engagement appears to be a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for bullying is a social phenomenon that is influenced by the social context
active bystander intervention (Thornberg & Jungert, 2013). Indeed, in which it occurs. One process through which the social context asserts
Thornberg and Jungert (2013) found that although both defending and its influence on bullying and bystanding behaviors is via peer group
passive bystanding (outsider) behaviors were associated with lower norms. Peer group norms reflect the salient and collective beliefs,
moral disengagement, what differentiated these two forms of bystanding attitudes or values of the larger peer group which function as guides
behaviors was the degree to which children felt self-efficacious about for individuals to act in ways that are acceptable to other group mem-
their ability to intervene during bullying episodes. Children with high bers. Whether specific group norms converge or diverge with children's
defender self-efficacy were more likely to engage in defending behaviors personal attitudes, they nevertheless influence their social experiences
and those with low self-efficacy for defending engaged in passive and affect their behaviors. In support of this contention, the theory of
bystanding. reasoned action (see Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980) argues that individuals'
behaviors are the cumulative result of their personal attitudes, their
2.3.3. Synthesis of moral processing and the SIP framework subjective norms (e.g., perceptions of the group's normative or collec-
To incorporate children's moral knowledge and reasoning into tive beliefs about a behavior), and the interaction of the two. Thus,
hypotheses about bullying and bystanding behaviors, it is proposed there is likely a synergistic effect between children's person-level fac-
that children who bully have adequate moral reasoning skills and can tors (e.g., social cognitive, emotion, and moral processes) and their ex-
retrieve moral relevant knowledge, but only consider the harmful intent periences within their peer groups.
of bullying behaviors when they are the victims—not when bullying
others. There are several explanations for why children may exhibit 3.1.2. Peer influence processes and bullying and bystanding behaviors
discrepancies in their self- versus other- referent responses to bullying. There has been a growing body of research on the role of peer
One explanation pertains to empathy such that empathic influence on bullying and bystanding behaviors, and investigators
82 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

have examined multiple processes using varying methodological In addition to the descriptive norms described above, the extent to
approaches. For this synthesis, we focus on how multiple peer group which children collectively practice moral disengagement can also be
norms collectively contribute to the normative relational climate and as- conceived as a group norm that contributes in distinct ways to bullying
sert pressure on these behaviors. More specifically, at least four distinct and bystanding behaviors. In support of this premise, Gini et al. (2014a)
types of peer group norms have been identified (see Gini et al., 2014a; found that, beyond children's own level of moral disengagement, when
Pozzoli, Gini, & Vieno, 2012): 1) collective bullying attitudes which reflect children perceive their classmates to be morally disengaged they are
the group's salient beliefs or judgments about the social acceptability or more likely to engage in aggression and passive bystanding and less
unacceptability of bullying and bystanding behaviors (e.g., pro-bullying likely to engage in defending behaviors. More specifically, findings
and anti-bullying attitudes), 2) injunctive norms which reflect children's revealed that collective moral disengagement amplified the effect of in-
perceptions of what they are expected to do in various situations, 3) de- dividual moral disengagement on aggression such that when collective
scriptive norms which describe children's actual behavioral responses moral disengagement was low, individual moral disengagement was
and the extent to which various bullying-related behaviors exist within not associated with aggression, but when collective moral disengage-
the group, and 4) collective moral disengagement which reflects percep- ment was high, children with high moral disengagement were
tions that the larger peer group is morally disengaged and sanctions bul- considerably more likely than those with low disengagement to act
lying behavior. Findings on each of these group norms are reviewed and aggressively.
support the premise that multiple group norms coexist and collectively Consistent with the theory of reasoned action, one implication of the
influence bullying and bystanding behaviors. above findings is that peer group norms may moderate the effects of
Collective bullying attitudes have been conceptualized as the extent child-level factors on bullying and bystanding behaviors. Support for
to which children in a peer group collectively endorse pro-bullying the premise that the associations between child-levels factors and bul-
(e.g., making fun of classmates, joining in bullying when it occurs) and lying and bystanding behaviors is moderated by relational processes
anti-bullying (e.g., supporting the victim, trying to actively intervene) can also be gleaned from investigations on injunctive norms about by-
beliefs and expectations for group members' behavior. Evidence of the stander intervention. For instance, in peer groups with high perceived
influence of these group norms can be culled from Salmivalli and peer pressure to intervene, adolescents had high levels of defending be-
Voeten's (2004) study in which they found that students were more haviors irrespective of their individual personal responsibility (Pozzoli
likely to defend victims, and less likely to bully others or reinforce & Gini, 2010). However, when they believed peer pressure to intervene
bullying behaviors, if their peer groups (i.e., classrooms) supported in bullying was low, they were significantly less likely to engage in
anti-bullying attitudes more strongly. It is important to note that these defending behaviors if they also had low levels of personal responsibil-
findings were more pronounced among early adolescents (i.e., 6th ity. Collectively, these findings illustrate how the peer context can func-
graders) than younger children (i.e., 4th graders), indicating that per- tion as both a risk and protective factor that either suppresses (buffers)
haps the influence of peer group norms may be stronger in adolescence or amplifies children's individual risks.
as compared to childhood.
In addition to group attitudes about bullying, injunctive 3.1.3. Implications of peer influence processes for hypothesis generation
norms—which reflect peers' expectations about how others should It could be argued that peer group norms impact subsequent
behave when they witness bullying situations—have also been investi- bullying and bystanding behaviors via their influence on children's
gated. Specifically, researchers conceptualize injunctive norms about person-related (i.e., social-cognitive, emotional, and moral) develop-
bullying and bystanding behaviors as perceived peer pressure to inter- ment. For example, children bring their personal characteristics, such
vene in bullying (i.e., class normative pressures to help victims; as need for dominance or social status or the desire for a close friend,
Pozzoli et al., 2012; Rigby, 2005; Rigby & Johnson, 2006). Findings with them into the classroom—and then, based on their social interac-
from these studies indicate that adolescents who do not believe their tions within their peer group, they form cognitive representations
friends expect them to be supportive of victims are more likely to about their perceptions of group norms that guide how they act to
engage in bullying behaviors. In contrast, adolescents who perceive achieve their goals. In this manner, generalized beliefs or attitudes
peer pressure to intervene on behalf of victims and believe that their about the social acceptability of bullying could be expected to influence
friends expected them to provide support for victims are more likely children's evaluations of the effectiveness of engaging in bullying
to engage in defending behaviors and less likely to be passive behaviors to achieve their goals. In contrast, when children and adoles-
bystanders (Pozzoli & Gini, 2010; Rigby & Johnson, 2006). cents associate bullying with peer rejection and social disapproval, they
Consistent with participant role perspectives, multiple descrip- are more likely to believe that their peer group endorses anti-bullying
tive norms may coexist within a peer group and reflect distinct norms (e.g., “If I bully someone, my classmates are not going to like
bystanding behaviors. Therefore, investigators have used various it”). In turn, when they think their peers disapprove of bullying, they
conceptualizations of descriptive norms to assess the distinct behav- are more likely to consider the potential disadvantages of this behavior-
ioral norms that characterize the peer relational climate. For in- al response and select non-aggressive responses with more favorable
stance, Pozzoli et al. (2012) measured two forms of descriptive outcome evaluations. Thus, the influence of peer group norms can be
norms which reflected the peer group's tendencies to engage in conceptualized as being indirect and via their influence on children's
defending or passive bystanding behaviors. They found that in addi- own social cognitive, emotion and moral processes which in turn influ-
tion to group attitudes and injunctive norms, descriptive norms ences their subsequent bullying or bystanding response.
about defending were positively associated with defending and neg- Notably, individual attitudes and group norms about bullying do not
atively with passive bystanding behaviors. In other words, when necessarily match, and discrepancies can be partly attributable to
children and adolescents were in classrooms that made more collec- children's prior person-related development, their social experiences
tive efforts to defend victims, they were themselves more likely to in previous peer groups (e.g., other schools or classes they had previous-
defend victims and less likely to be passive bystanders. In contrast, ly attended), as well as other socializing influences (e.g., parents,
in classrooms in which passive bystanding was the social norm, stu- teachers). In other words, children's person-related development is pre-
dents were more likely to be passive bystanders. Considering that sumably the result of a host of socialization experiences that accumulate
these findings on descriptive norms were in addition to over their lifespan. It is plausible that well before their experiences in
(i.e., controlling for) the effects of group attitudes and injunctive formal schooling where children are exposed to peers (and teachers),
norms, they reflect how children's behavioral responses to bullying parental socialization experiences influence their long-term memories,
depend on the influence of multiple peer group norm processes beliefs, attitudes and relational schemas (contained within their cogni-
(i.e., group attitudes, injunctive and descriptive norms). tive database) which subsequently influence their social-cognitive,
I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86 83

emotion and moral processing and their relationships with other social- within their classrooms, bullying often occurs outside the purview of
izing agents (i.e., peers and teachers). Thus, due to children's cumulative teachers. Moreover, bullying may go unreported for a variety of rea-
socialization experiences, their individual attitudes about bullying and sons, such as when students feel their teachers are not effective in
bystanding behaviors are not necessarily congruent with the values of their intervention efforts, or they feel that they are expected to han-
the collective peer group. dle bullying on their own; thus, bullying often goes unnoticed and
In consideration of these discrepancies, it is hypothesized that group teachers remain unaware of much of the bullying that occurs
norms also function to buffer or amplify (i.e., moderate) the degree to among their students (Atlas & Pepler, 1998; Fekkes, Pijper, &
which children's social-cognitive processing is linked with their subse- Verloove-Vanhorick, 2005; Olweus, 1978). Such findings most likely
quent behavioral responses. That is, when discrepancies arise, group account for discrepancies between teacher and student reports of the
norms are hypothesized to explain inconsistencies between person- prevalence of bullying.
level processes and subsequent behavioral responses. For example, While awareness is a necessary condition for teacher intervention, it
children may endorse dominance goals and have positive expectations does not sufficiently predict whether they actually intervene. Thus,
about bullying, but are less likely to act on these social-cognitive biases several investigators have assessed the conditions in which teacher in-
(i.e., bully) when they are in a social environment in which their peers terventions are most likely to occur. Toward this end, several factors
do not condone bullying (i.e., suppression effect). Thus peer influence have been studied including teachers' characteristics and contextual-
processes appear to have a synergistic effect on (e.g., amplify or suppress) level (i.e., classroom and school) influences. With respect to teachers'
children's person level tendencies to engage in bullying or bystanding characteristics, investigators have assessed the role of: 1) teachers'
behaviors. beliefs about bullying, 2) their preparedness to intervene, and 3) and
The premise that peer groups can function to sanction person-level their self-efficacy for effectively intervening. With respect to contextual
tendencies to engage in bullying behaviors and promote anti-bullying influences on teacher intervention, investigators have assessed the role
behaviors serves as in impetus for intervention efforts that have of the classroom and school climate.
attempted to alter the salient peer group norms or beliefs about the so- More specifically, one line of investigation has been on teachers' be-
cial acceptability of bullying and bystanding behaviors. Based on this liefs and attitudes about whether bullying is normative. On the one
area of research, intervention efforts may be more effective by using a hand, when teachers feel more sympathy and empathy for the victim-
multi-tiered approach that incorporates universal and targeted strate- ized child, they tend to identify bullying, accurately weigh the serious-
gies to alter both the peer group's collective norms about bullying and ness of the event, and intervene in support of the victim (Craig,
bystanding behaviors as well as altering the attitudes of children who Henderson, & Murphy, 2000a). In turn, when teachers report a greater
are most at-risk for bullying. willingness to intervene, their students are also more likely to intervene
Examining the influence of peer group norms from a developmental on behalf of victims (Espelage, Polanin, & Low, 2014). On the other
perspective, it is also important to consider how the role and influence hand, when teachers believe that bullying is a normative behavior and
of the peer context changes as a function of developmental timing. As minimize the severity of bullying, they are less likely to intervene
children get older, they spend increasing amounts of time with peers (Kochenderfer-Ladd & Pelletier, 2008), less likely to reprimand aggres-
and strive to become more autonomous and independent from adult sors, and more likely to use passive response strategies such as telling
figures. Consistent with this rationale it is hypothesized that peer influ- victims to cope with their bullying problems on their own (Troop-
ence processes are more salient during adolescence and have a stronger Gordon & Ladd, 2015).
association with bullying and bystanding behaviors during this devel- Collectively, these findings are indicative of a mediated (indirect)
opmental period than other socializing mechanisms (e.g., teachers; pathway from teachers' beliefs to bullying behaviors. That is, teachers'
see Pozzoli et al., 2012; Rigby, 2005), which are more likely to have beliefs that bullying is normative are associated with more bullying
more pronounced effects during childhood. and peer victimization among students when these beliefs thwarted
teachers from intervening (Hektner & Swenson, 2012). Indeed,
3.2. Teacher influence processes teachers' beliefs about bullying may result in a number of different
response strategies including contacting parents, separating bullies
3.2.1. Overview of teacher influence processes and victims, reprimanding bullies, suggesting victims avoid their bullies,
In addition to peers, teachers are in a unique position to directly advising victims be assertive, and telling victims to handle their bullying
influence children's bullying and bystanding behavior—and there are problems on their own. Exactly how teachers act on their beliefs
several mechanisms through which their influence occurs. Through (i.e., their responses strategies) impacts the nature of children's bullying
their behaviors and beliefs, and social interactions with their students, behaviors (see Troop-Gordon & Ladd, 2015).
teachers foster salient classroom norms and in turn affect the expecta- With respect to teachers' preparedness and self-efficacy to inter-
tions that students have in subsequent interactions with peers. vene, evidence indicates that when teachers are informed by students
about a bullying episode, or observe it for themselves, they are more
3.2.2. Role of teacher influence processes on bullying and bystanding likely to intervene (i.e., coach and support victims) when they feel
behaviors adequately prepared to, but less likely to respond to bullying when
Not surprisingly, because schools have served as the focal setting for they felt unprepared (Novick & Isaacs, 2010). Similarly, Veenstra,
investigating bullying and bystanding behaviors, investigators have ex- Lindenberg, Huitsing, Sainio, and Salmivalli (2014) reported that in
amined both characteristics of teachers and teacher–child interactions classrooms with teachers who have high-self efficacy for intervening,
that are associated with these behaviors (see Holt & Keyes, 2004; Holt, students report lower levels of bullying. Moreover, teacher efficacy
Keyes, & Koenig, 2010; Salmivalli, 2010). Not only are teachers expected buffered the effects of children's intervention efforts on bullying behav-
to monitor and discipline problem behaviors, such as bullying, but they iors. In other words, when children reported low levels of effort in inter-
can also influence salient norms and attitudes about bullying and anti- vening, bullying was less likely when teachers had high self-efficacy and
bullying bystanding behaviors within their classrooms. more probable when teachers had low self-efficacy. Indeed the combina-
Relatedly, one line of investigation has been on teachers' awareness of tion of low student effort and low teacher efficacy for intervening was as-
bullying within the school context. Studies suggest that teachers may not sociated with higher rates of bullying than any other combination of
be as aware of bullying episodes as they believe (Bradshaw, Sawyer, & these two factors (Veenstra et al., 2014). Together, these findings have
O'Brennan, 2007; Rigby, 2005; Rigby & Bagshaw, 2003), and their aware- important implications for intervention efforts that seek to target bully-
ness appears to worsen as children mature (Holt et al., 2010; Schwartz ing behaviors by promoting teachers' competencies (e.g., preparedness
et al., 2010). Although teachers are more aware of bullying when it occurs and self-efficacy) to effectively mitigate these behaviors.
84 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

Similar to earlier propositions about how the peer group environment examining the combined effects of multiple socialization agents may pro-
(e.g., peer group norms) influences children's individual attitudes and be- vide new insights that cannot be assessed by examining these processes
haviors, the classroom—and school climate more broadly—impacts the independently.
likelihood of teacher intervention. Teachers are more likely to intervene
when they feel confident that administrators and other teachers and 4. Conclusion
school staff are supportive of their actions (Mishna, Pepler, & Wiener,
2006). Indeed, when teachers reported greater administrative support, The primary objective of this article was to bring about an integra-
students reported lower levels of bullying (Espelage et al., 2014). These tion of multiple theoretical perspectives on the determinants of bullying
findings speak to the importance of having clear school policies related and bystanding behaviors. Toward this end, a preliminary synthesis was
to bullying and which encourage school administrators to support undertaken as an attempt to incorporate multiple person-level
teachers in their intervention efforts. (i.e., social-cognitive, emotion, and moral processing) and relational-
level (i.e., peer and teacher) influences on bullying and bystanding
3.2.3. Implications of teacher influence processes for hypothesis generation behaviors. Although it was outside the scope of this review to empirical-
It seems likely that theory and research would be enhanced by incor- ly test the tenets that emerged from this synthesis, it is conceivable that
porating hypotheses about teacher influence processes within broader such efforts could spur investigative innovations and generate novel in-
models of children's bullying and bystanding behaviors. It is theorized sights. With respect to children's person-related factors, greater explan-
that teachers are more likely to intervene, and therefore subsequently atory power might be achieved by simultaneously examining person-
reduce bullying behaviors and increase anti-bullying bystanding behav- related factors from multiple domains. Much remains to be learned
iors when they: 1) are aware of when bullying occurs both inside and about how children coordinate social cognitive, emotional, and moral
outside of their classroom, 2) possess personal characteristics and pro- processes, and how these factors are interconnected and associated
cesses that promote intervention (e.g., anti-bullying attitudes, empa- with bullying and bystanding behaviors. Further, even less is known
thy), 3) feel prepared and self-efficacious in their abilities to intervene, about how relational experiences (i.e., socialization contexts and pro-
4) have positive teacher–child interactions characterized by respect cesses) shape the development of person-related factors, and
for all children (e.g., supporting victims, creating a tolerant classroom how—over the course of development—differing combinations of
environment), and 5) perceive their school climate to be supportive of person- and contextual factors influence children's bullying and
their efforts to intervene (e.g., clear school policies against bullying, per- bystanding behaviors.
ceptions that administrators do not tolerate bullying and encourage Expanding the knowledge base on the determinants of bullying and
teacher intervention). bystanding behaviors could also have significant implications for inter-
When teachers intervene, their influence is direct when they stop vention efforts aimed at reducing school related bullying and victimiza-
bullying as it occurs; but teachers can also indirectly influence bullying tion. The factors identified within this preliminary synthesis suggest
and bystanding behaviors via their influence on children's person- that there are multiple potentially malleable personal and relational
related development. In this regard, it is hypothesized that teachers in- processes that would be suitable targets for intervention efforts with
fluence children's bullying attitudes and social-cognitive biases which the most promising chances of reducing bullying and victimization
in turn affects bullying and bystanding behavior. More specifically, and promoting anti-bullying bystanding behaviors.
teachers' reactions and responses to bullying are hypothesized to in-
fluence children's attitudes about whether bullying is normative and
Acknowledgments
socially acceptable, and their outcome expectations related to bully-
ing. In other words, based on observing how teachers respond to bul-
We acknowledge Dr. Larry Dumka and Dr. Sabina Low for their feed-
lying, children form cognitive representations for how their teachers
back on earlier versions of this manuscript.
will likely respond in subsequent bullying episodes. When children
believe that teachers tolerate bullying, it further reinforces their be-
References
liefs that bullying is acceptable and an approved behavioral re-
sponse. In contrast, when children believe that teachers do not Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior.
tolerate bullying, it can influence their outcome evaluations and de- Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Almeida, A., Correia, I., & Marinho, S. (2009). Moral disengagement, normative beliefs of
cisions about bullying and serve as a model for how they should ex-
peer group, and attitudes regarding roles in bullying. Journal of School Violence, 9,
ercise anti-bullying bystanding behaviors. 23–36.
Although teacher and peer socialization are theorized to indepen- Arsenio, W.F., & Lemerise, E.A. (2001). Varieties of childhood bullying: Values, emotion
dently influence children's person-related development and in turn processes, and social competence. Social Development, 10, 59–73.
Arsenio, W.F., & Lemerise, E.A. (2004). Aggression and moral development: Integrating
their bullying and bystanding behaviors, combined or conjoint effects social information processing and moral domain models. Child Development, 75,
should also be considered. It is proposed that peer and teacher socializa- 987–1002.
tion have a synergistic effect on children's person-related development Atlas, R.S., & Pepler, D.J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the classroom. The Journal of
Educational Research, 92, 86–99.
and subsequently their bullying and bystanding behaviors. That is, Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality
when the attitudes and beliefs that children receive via multiple socializ- and Social Psychology Review, 3, 193–209.
ing agents are concordant, they are more likely to internalize these values Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency.
Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101–119.
and apply them in subsequent social interactions. For instance, investiga- Barchia, K., & Bussey, K. (2011a). Individual and collective social cognitive influences on
tors have explored the role of collective efficacy to intervene—construed peer aggression: Exploring the contribution of aggression efficacy, moral disengage-
as individuals' perceptions of the ability of classmates and teachers to ment, and collective efficacy. Aggressive Behavior, 37, 107–120http://dx.doi.org/10.
1002/ab.20375.
work together to intervene effectively in bullying situations—on the oc- Barchia, K., & Bussey, K. (2011b). Predictors of student defenders of peer aggression
currence of bullying and bystanding behavior (Barchia & Bussey, 2011a, victims: Empathy and social cognitive factors. International Journal of Behavioral
2011b). Findings from this line of inquiry suggest that when children be- Development, 35, 1–9http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025410396746.
Barhight, L.R., Hubbard, J.A., & Hyde, C.T. (2013). Children's physiological and emotional
lieve their classmates and teachers are effective at intervening, they are
reactions to witnessing bullying predict bystander intervention. Child Development,
more likely to engage in defending behaviors. In contrast, those with 84, 375–390.
low collective efficacy beliefs were more likely to engage in aggressive Boldizar, J.P., Perry, D.G., & Perry, L.C. (1989). Outcome values and aggression. Child
behaviors (Barchia & Bussey, 2011a, 2011b). Moreover, when the social Development, 60, 571–579.
Bradshaw, C.P., Sawyer, A.L., & O'Brennan, L.M. (2007). Bullying and peer victimization at
messages children receive from socializing agents are discordant, the in- school: Perceptual differences between students and school staff. School Psychology
fluence of one agent may be tempered or suppressed by others. Thus, Review, 36, 361–382.
I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86 85

Caravita, S., Di Blasio, P., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Unique and interactive effects of em- Kochenderfer-Ladd, B.J., Ladd, G.W., & Kochel, K.P. (2009). A child and environment
pathy and social status on involvement in bullying. Social Development, 18, framework for studying risk for peer victimization. In M.J. Harris (Ed.), Bullying,
140–163. rejection, and peer victimization: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective
Cillessen, A.H., Mayeux, L., Ha, T., de Bruyn, E.H., & LaFontana, K.M. (2014). Aggres- (pp. 27–52). New York: Springer.
sive effects of prioritizing popularity in early adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, Kochenderfer‐Ladd, B. (2004). Peer victimization: The role of emotions in adaptive and
40, 204–213. maladaptive coping. Social Development, 13, 329–349.
Craig, W.M., Henderson, K., & Murphy, J.G. (2000a). Prospective teachers' attitudes Kochenderfer-Ladd, B., & Pelletier, M.E. (2008). Teachers' views and beliefs about bully-
toward bullying and victimization. School Psychology International, 21, 5–21. ing: Influences on classroom management strategies and students' coping with
Craig, W.M., Pepler, D.J., & Atlas, R. (2000b). Observations of bullying in the playground peer victimization. Journal of School Psychology, 46, 431–453.
and in the classroom. School Psychology International, 21, 22–36. Lemerise, E.A., & Arsenio, W. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and cog-
Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information nition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107–118.
processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, Menesini, E., Sanchez, V., Fonzi, A., Ortega, R., Costabile, A., & Lo Feudo, G. (2003). Moral
74–101. emotions and bullying: A cross-national comparison of differences between bullies,
Crick, N.R., & Dodge, K.A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive victims and outsiders. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 515–530.
and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 993–1002. Mishna, F., Pepler, D., & Wiener, J. (2006). Factors associated with perceptions of and
Davis, M.H. (1994). Empathy, a social psychological approach. Madison: Brown and Bench- responses to bullying situations by children, parents, teachers and principals.
mark Publishers. Victims and Offenders, 1, 255–288.
Dodge, K.A., Lochman, J.E., Harnish, J.D., Bates, J.E., & Pettit, G.S. (1997). Reactive and pro- Murray‐Close, D., Crick, N.R., & Galotti, K.M. (2006). Children's moral reasoning regarding
active aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired chronically assault- physical and relational aggression. Social Development, 15, 345–372.
ive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37–51. Novick, R.M., & Isaacs, J. (2010). Telling is compelling: The impact of student reports of
Dykas, M.J., & Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information bullying on teacher intervention. Educational Psychology, 30, 283–296http://dx.doi.
across the life span: Theory and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 19–46. org/10.1080/01443410903573123.
Espelage, D.L., Polanin, J.R., & Low, S.K. (2014). Teacher and staff perceptions of school en- Nucci, L. (1981). Conceptions of personal issues: A domain distinct from moral or societal
vironment as predictors of student aggression, victimization, and willingness to inter- concepts. Child Development, 52, 114–121.
vene in bullying situations. School psychology quarterlyhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Nucci, L.P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University
spq0000072 (Advance online publication). Press.
Espelage, D.L., & Swearer, S.M. (2010). A social–ecological model for bullying prevention Obermann, M.L. (2011). Moral disengagement among bystanders to school bullying.
and intervention: Understanding the impact of adults in the social ecology of young- Journal of School Violence, 10, 239–257http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2011.
sters. In S.R. Jimerson, S.M. Swearer, & D.L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in 578276.
schools. An international perspective (pp. 61–72). New York: Routledge. Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools: Bullies and whipping boys. Washington, DC:
Fekkes, M., Pijper, F.I.M., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S.P. (2005). Bullying: Who does what, Hemisphere.
when and where? Involvement of children, teachers and parents in bullying behav- Pellegrini, A.D., & Long, J.D. (2002). A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance, and
ior. Health Education Research, 20, 81–91. victimization during the transition from primary school through secondary school.
Feshbach, N.D. (1978). Studies of empathic behavior in children. In B.A. Maher (Ed.), Prog- British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 259–280.
ress in experimental personality research (pp. 1–47). New York: Academic Press. Perry, D.G., Kusel, S.J., & Perry, L.C. (1988). Victims of peer aggression. Developmental
Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What's wrong? Psychology, 24, 807–814.
Aggressive Behavior, 32, 528–539http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20153. Perry, D.G., Perry, L.C., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social learning mediators of
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoè, G. (2007). Does empathy predict adolescents' bul- aggression. Child Development, 57, 700–711.
lying and defending behavior? Aggressive Behavior, 33, 467–476http://dx.doi.org/10. Pöyhönen, V., Juvonen, J., & Salmivalli, C. (2010). What does it take to defend the
1002/ab.20204. victimized peer? The interplay between personal and social factors. Merrill-Palmer
Gini, G., Albiero, P., Benelli, B., & Altoè, G. (2008). Determinants of adolescents' active Quarterly, 56, 143–163http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0046.
defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying. Journal of Adolescence, 31, Pozzoli, T., & Gini, G. (2010). Active defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying:
93–105http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.05.002. The role of personal characteristics and perceived peer pressure. Journal of Abnormal
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Bussey, K. (2014a). The role of individual and collective moral dis- Child Psychology, 38, 815–827http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9399-9.
engagement in peer aggression and bystanding: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Pozzoli, T., Gini, G., & Vieno, A. (2012). The role of individual correlates and class norms in
Abnormal Child Psychology, 1–12http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9920-7. defending and passive bystanding behavior in bullying: A multilevel analysis. Child
Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hymel, S. (2014b). Moral disengagement among children and Development, 83, 1917–1931.
youth: A meta-analytic review of links to aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M., Olthof, T., Goossens, F.A., van de Schoot, R., Aleva, L., et al.
40, 56–68. (2013). Costs and benefits of bullying in the context of the peer group: A three
Hawley, P.H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary wave longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 1217–1229.
perspective. Developmental Review, 19, 97–132. Rigby, K. (2005). Why do some children bully at school? The contributions of negative
Hektner, J.M., & Swenson, C.A. (2012). Links from teacher beliefs to peer victimization and attitudes towards victims and the perceived expectations of friends, parents and
bystander intervention tests of mediating processes. Journal of Early Adolescence, 32, teachers. School Psychology International, 26, 147–161.
516–536http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431611402502. Rigby, K., & Bagshaw, D. (2003). Prospects of adolescent students collaborating with
Hodges, E.V.E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W.M. (1999). The power of friendship: teachers in addressing issues of bullying and conflict in schools. Educational
Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 23, 535–546.
Psychology, 35, 94–101. Rigby, K., & Johnson, B. (2006). Expressed readiness of Australian schoolchildren to act as
Hodges, E.V.E., Malone, M.J., & Perry, D.G. (1997). Individual risk and social risk as bystanders in support of children who are being bullied. Educational Psychology, 26,
interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental 425–440 (doi:0.1080/01443410500342047).
Psychology, 33, 1032–1039http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.1032. Rigby, K., & Slee, P.T. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relations among Australian
Hodges, E.V.E., & Perry, D.G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and conse- school children and their implications for psychological well-being. Journal of Social
quences of victimization by peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, Psychology, 133, 33–42.
677–685http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.677. Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A review. Aggression and Violent
Holt, M., & Keyes, M. (2004). Teachers' attitudes toward teasing and general school cli- Behavior, 15, 112–120http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2009.08.007.
mate. In D.L. Espelage, & S. Swearer (Eds.), Bullying in American schools: A social–eco- Salmivalli, C., & Isaacs, J. (2005). Prospective relations among victimization, rejection,
logical perspective on prevention and intervention (pp. 121–139). New Jersey: friendlessness, and children's self and peer perceptions. Child Development, 76,
Lawrence Erlbaum. 1161–1171.
Holt, M., Keyes, M., & Koenig, B. (2010). Teacher's attitudes toward bullying. In D.L. Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying
Espelage, & S.M. Swearer (Eds.), Bullying in North American schools (pp. 119–131) as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the
(2nd ed.). New York: Routledge. group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1–15.
Hong, J.S., & Espelage, D.L. (2012). A review of research on bullying and peer victim- Salmivalli, C., Ojanen, T., Haanpää, J., & Peets, K. (2005). “I'm O.K. but you're not” and other
ization in school: An ecological system analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, peer-relational schemas. Explaining individual differences in children's social goals.
17, 311–322. Developmental Psychology, 41, 363–375.
Hymel, S., Schonert-Reichl, K.A., Bonanno, R.A., Vaillancourt, T., & Henderson, N.R. (2010). Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections between attitudes, group norms, and be-
Bullying and morality: Understanding how good kids can behave badly. In S.R. havior in bullying situations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28,
Jimerson, S.M. Swearer, & D.L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in schools: An in- 246–258.
ternational perspective (pp. 101–118). New York: Routledge. Schwartz, D., Kelly, B.M., Duong, M.T., & Badaly, D. (2010). A contextual perspective on in-
Jimerson, S.R., Swearer, S.M., & Espelage, D.L. (2010). Handbook of bullying in schools: An tervention and prevention efforts for bully-victim problems. In E. Vernberg, & B. Biggs
international perspective. New York: NY: Routledge. (Eds.), Preventing and treating bullying and victimization (pp. 17–44). New York: Ox-
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2006). Examining the relationship between low empathy ford University Press.
and bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 540–550. Sijtsema, J.J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., & Salmivalli, C. (2009). Empirical test of bullies'
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D.P. (2011). Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling status goals: Assessing direct goals, aggression, and prestige. Aggressive Behavior, 35,
for individual and social background variables? Journal of Adolescence, 34, 59–71. 57–67.
Juvonen, J., & Galván, A. (2010). Peer contagion in involuntary social groups: Lessons from Smithmyer, C.M., Hubbard, J.A., & Simons, R.F. (2000). Proactive and reactive aggression in
research on bullying. In M. Prinstein, & K. Dodge (Eds.), Understanding peer influence delinquent adolescents: Relations to aggressive outcome expectancies. Journal of
in children and adolescents (pp. 225–244). New York: Guilford Press. Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 86–93.
86 I. Ettekal et al. / Aggression and Violent Behavior 23 (2015) 75–86

Sutton, J., Smith, P.K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and ‘theory of mind’: A critique Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Huitsing, G., Sainio, M., & Salmivalli, C. (2014). The role of
of the ‘social skills deficit’ view of anti-social behavior. Social Development, 8, teachers in bullying: The relation between antibullying attitudes, efficacy, and efforts
117–127. to reduce bullying. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 1135–1143http://dx.doi.
Sutton, J., Smith, P.K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and bullying: Social org/10.1037/a0036110.
inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Zijlstra, B.J.H., De Winter, A.F., Verhulst, F.C., & Ormel, J.
435–450. (2007). The dyadic nature of bullying and victimization: Testing a dual-perspective
Thornberg, R., & Jungert, T. (2013). Bystander behavior in bullying situations: Basic moral theory. Child Development, 78, 1843–1854http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.
sensitivity, moral disengagement and defender self-efficacy. Journal of Adolescence, 2007.01102.x.
36, 475–483http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.02.003.
Troop-Gordon, W., & Ladd, G.W. (2015). Teachers' victimization-related beliefs and strate-
gies: Associations with students' aggressive behavior and peer victimization. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 45–60http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9840-y.

You might also like