You are on page 1of 16

The Analysis of Verbal Behavior 2011, 27, 45–60

Analyzing Stimulus-Stimulus Pairing Effects on Preferences


for Speech Sounds

Anna Ingeborg Petursdottir, Charlotte L. Carp, and Derek W. Matthies,


Texas Christian University
Barbara E. Esch, Esch Behavior Consultants, Inc.

Several studies have demonstrated effects of stimulus-stimulus pairing (SSP) on children’s vocalizations,
but numerous treatment failures have also been reported. The present study attempted to isolate
procedural variables related to failures of SSP to condition speech sounds as reinforcers. Three boys
diagnosed with autism-spectrum disorders participated. Phase 1 was designed to assess SSP effects on
production of auditory stimuli via button pressing. When SSP failed to produce a preference for the
target stimulus, we instituted a series of procedural manipulations intended to address potential reasons
for failure. One participant preferred the target stimulus when given the opportunity to select preferred
items for pairing prior to each session, but a subsequent reversal attempt produced ambiguous results.
Two participants showed no consistent preference in Phase 1 and underwent a within-session reinforcer
evaluation in Phase 2, in which alternative controlling variables were demonstrated by delivering
preferred stimuli contingent on button pressing.
Key words: autism, conditioned reinforcement, stimulus-stimulus pairing, concurrent operants,
vocalizations

Early behavioral intervention programs for pairing (SSP) procedure to increase the vocal
nonverbal children diagnosed with autism play of children who do not communicate
typically attempt to establish vocal commu- vocally. This procedure involves an adult’s
nication before considering alternative com- repeated presentations of a specific phoneme
munication systems (Lovaas, 2003). Howev- or syllable, each immediately followed by
er, efforts to shape recognizable speech the presentation of a preferred item or
sounds and establish functional speech may activity, without any response requirement
be complicated when children display a low by the child.
frequency and limited variability of free- Early research on SSP (Smith, Michael, &
operant vocalizations. Early intervention Sundberg, 1996; Sundberg, Michael, Parting-
texts based on Skinner’s (1957) analysis of ton, & Sundberg, 1996; Yoon & Bennett,
verbal behavior (Greer & Ross, 2008; Sund- 2000) demonstrated that SSP sessions were
berg and Partington, 1998) have recom- followed by temporary increases in rates of
mended the use of a stimulus-stimulus vocalizations that resembled the paired
auditory stimuli, among children with and
without developmental delays. Yoon and
Derek W. Matthies is now at the Graduate Bennett’s data further suggested that SSP
School of Education and Psychology at Pepper- might be superior to direct reinforcement in
dine University. terms of effects on free-operant vocaliza-
This research was supported in part by a grant
from the Texas Christian University (TCU) tions. Two of the studies (Smith et al; Yoon
Research and Creative Activities Fund, as well & Bennett) included conditions in which the
as an undergraduate research grant awarded to auditory stimulus was presented in the
Derek W. Matthies by the TCU Science and absence of preferred stimuli, demonstrating
Engineering Research Center. We thank the
participants and their families for their time and that mere exposure to the stimulus was not
cooperation, and we thank Anja Peters, Leigh sufficient to produce the effect. In addition,
Carpenter, Chloe Corbell, Andy Kaufman, Eronia Smith et al. provided evidence that pairing an
Melesse, Emma Murphy, and Kristen Secora for auditory stimulus with a presumed aversive
assisting with data collection.
Address correspondence to Anna Ingeborg stimulus might have the opposite effect of
Petursdottir, Department of Psychology, TCU reducing the rate of target vocalizations,
Box 298920, Fort Worth, TX, 76129. further underscoring the role of preferred

45
46 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

stimuli in producing rate-increasing effects. tions, but does not produce this effect
None of the three studies, however, em- reliably. When the procedure works, it may
ployed designs that permitted a rigorous be quite valuable, as it is easy to implement
demonstration of experimental control, and and may quickly increase the levels of
none took steps to prevent adventitious specific vocalizations sufficiently that they
reinforcement of target vocalizations during can be reinforced and brought under stimulus
pairing. In addition, none of the studies control without the use of elaborate shaping
included control conditions in which the procedures. However, if it is to be recom-
auditory and preferred stimuli were presented mended as a clinical procedure, it is neces-
separately in time, to assess the role of sary to identify the conditions under which
temporal contiguity. Finally, an autism- SSP produces an effect. So far, success does
spectrum diagnosis was reported for two of not appear correlated with any obvious
five participants in Sundberg et al., but none participant characteristics, except possibly
of the participants in the other studies. As a young age (for a review, see Stock et al.,
result, these early studies may have provided 2008). Yoon and Feliciano (2007) reported
limited support for clinical implementation that participants who responded to the
with this population. procedure tended to have less functional
Two subsequent studies (Miguel, Carr, & verbal behavior but higher levels of vocal
Michael, 2002; B. E. Esch, Carr, & Grow, play than participants who did not respond.
2009) demonstrated SSP effects on free- However, on the Behavioral Language As-
operant vocalizations of children diagnosed sessment (BLA; Sundberg & Partington,
with autism-spectrum disorders, and ad- 1998), which has been the most commonly
dressed some methodological limitations of reported assessment in the SSP literature,
previous studies. Both studies included profiles of participants who have responded
omission contingencies to rule out adventi- to the procedure are quite similar to those of
tious reinforcement of vocalizations. In nonresponders. The BLA is a simple assess-
addition, both controlled for temporally ment procedure with limited psychometric
noncontiguous presentation of auditory and information, and as a result, it is possible that
preferred stimuli. Miguel et al. included a it may have failed to capture important
condition in which the presentations of participant variables. However, in the ab-
auditory and preferred stimuli were separated sence of other information, procedural vari-
in time, and B. E. Esch et al. interspersed ables should be considered.
presentations of the target auditory stimulus The effects of SSP have typically been
with presentations of a control auditory interpreted in terms of automatic reinforce-
stimulus that was not contiguous with ment (Sundberg et al., 1996; Sundberg &
preferred stimuli. These studies provided Partington, 1998). According to this inter-
more rigorous support for SSP. In spite of pretation, the pairing of an adult-emitted
these positive results, however, numerous speech sound with a primary or other
failures of SSP to increase vocalizations have reinforcer establishes the speech sound as a
also been reported in the literature. In the conditioned reinforcer. Subsequently, when
Miguel et al. study, SSP increased the the child emits a sound that resembles the
vocalizations of only two out of three paired sound, the response-produced auditory
participants. In a direct replication of that stimulus serves to reinforce the vocal re-
study, B. E. Esch, Michael, and Carr (2005) sponse and shape it to match the paired sound
found no effect on three children’s free- more precisely. Similar interpretations have
operant vocalizations after an initial unsuc- long been applied to the shaping of infant
cessful attempt to demonstrate an effect on babbling into speech sounds in the natural
echoic responding. In several other recent environment (Bijou & Baer, 1965; Skinner,
studies, some or all participants have failed 1957). The automatic reinforcement interpre-
to respond to SSP (Carroll & Klatt, 2008; tation suggests that SSP might fail to increase
Normand & Knoll, 2006; Stock, Schulze, & vocalizations for two broad reasons. First,
Mirenda, 2008; Yoon & Feliciano, 2007). SSP may fail to establish the auditory
In sum, the literature to date suggests that stimulus as a conditioned reinforcer. Second,
SSP can increase rates of specific vocaliza- the child may rarely or never emit vocaliza-
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 47

tions that are sufficiently similar to the paired presenting a target speech sound, reaches for
auditory stimulus to reinforce the vocal the preferred item that is to be paired with the
response (i.e., stimulus generalization may speech sound. The sight of a person reaching
be an issue). When the dependent variable is for a preferred item might block the speech
vocalizations, however, it may be difficult to sounds if this stimulus already precedes
separate these two potential reasons for delivery of preferred items reliably. CS pre-
failure. exposure refers to impaired acquisition of a
There are several possible reasons why the conditioned response due to pre-exposure to
pairing of an auditory stimulus with a the neutral stimulus that is subsequently
preferred stimulus might fail to condition paired with the US. In research on stimu-
the auditory stimulus as a reinforcer. First, lus-stimulus pairing, lengthy exposure to the
the preferred stimulus with which the audi- target auditory stimulus during baseline or
tory stimulus is paired might not function as control conditions might similarly impede
a reinforcer. Although several studies have conditioning. Third, the auditory stimulus
reported the use of preference assessments to may not be sufficiently salient to produce an
select preferred stimuli for pairing, only B. E. effect (Dinsmoor, 1995). Only one stimulus-
Esch et al. (2005) directly assessed the stimulus pairing study to date (B. E. Esch et
reinforcing effects of the stimuli following al., 2009) has explicitly used procedures
failures to produce a pairing effect. When designed to enhance observation of the
preferred stimuli were delivered contingent auditory stimulus. Fourth, parameters such
on vocalizations, one child’s target vocaliza- as the number and temporal distribution of
tions failed to increase, suggesting that the pairings, which have varied substantially
stimuli did not function as reinforcers for that across studies, may play a role in the
child’s vocalizations. However, the other two outcome.
participants’ vocalizations increased, sug- In the present study, effects of SSP on
gesting that ineffective identification of preference for a target auditory stimulus over
reinforcers could not account for their failure a control auditory stimulus were evaluated in
to respond to SSP. Second, effects analogous a concurrent-operant button-pressing prepa-
to overshadowing, blocking, and CS pre- ration. This preparation was intended to
exposure, all of which are known to affect separate conditioning failures from failures
respondent conditioning outcomes of stimu- related to the child’s vocal repertoire not
lus pairing procedures (Mazur, 2006), may including vocalizations sufficiently similar to
similarly affect the establishment of condi- the target stimulus. Thus, the purpose of the
tioned reinforcers via similar procedures. In study was not to demonstrate clinically
respondent conditioning, overshadowing oc- relevant behavior change, but rather, to
curs when two neutral stimuli are simulta- identify variables that might influence the
neously paired with an US, but only one outcome of SSP in clinical or research
acquires a CS function. In SSP, when a settings. Variables hypothesized to influence
therapist vocally presents the auditory stim- the conditioning of reinforcers were manip-
ulus, the simultaneous sight of the therapist ulated. These variables included increasing
moving his or her lips might potentially the frequency of preference assessments
overshadow the auditory stimulus. In B. E. (assuming that more frequent preference
Esch et al. (2009), it is possible that the assessment might better serve to identify
interspersal of a control auditory stimulus effective reinforcers, due to potential changes
may have served to prevent blocking by in the reinforcing value of stimuli over time),
eliminating the correlation between lip eliminating pre-exposure to the target audi-
movement and preferred stimuli. Blocking, tory stimulus to prevent potential adverse
in respondent conditioning, occurs when a effects of such pre-exposure, and increasing
neutral stimulus and a stimulus that has the number of pairings per session. An
already acquired a CS function are presented observing prompt (saying the child’s name
simultaneously, followed by the US. The and waiting for eye contact) preceded
neutral stimulus may then fail to acquire a CS stimulus presentation, and presentations of
function. An analogous effect might occur in the target auditory stimulus were interspersed
SSP if the therapist, simultaneously with with presentations of a control stimulus in
48 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

Table 1
Auditory Stimuli

Participant S1 S2 S3 S4
Brandon fluh-ich (flaug) myuht-l (mjöll)
Brennan bruh-ith (brauð) fuk-l (fugl) h-reem (hrı́m) stab-n (stafn)
Dominick ooh ee ai uh
Note. For Brandon and Brennan, anglicized spelling based on pronunciation is presented in italics, and
actual spelling of the foreign word in parentheses. The anglicized spelling may give the impression that
all stimuli used for these participants contained two syllables; however, each of the six words contains
only one syllable when properly pronounced by a native speaker.

order to prevent overshadowing and to frequently manded for many different items
control for stimulus exposure. In addition, and activities and could tact a large number
we recorded the experimenter’s behavior to of items, but had minimal intraverbal and
ensure that the target sound was not present- receptive repertoires. Both twins were ob-
ed simultaneously with the sight of the served to engage in excessive echolalia.
experimenter reaching for a preferred item, Brandon and Brennan’s sessions were con-
in an attempt to prevent blocking by this ducted in their living room at home, while
stimulus. The primary dependent variable they moved freely about the room.
was allocation of button presses to the button Dominick was 3.5 years old. His BLA
that produced the target auditory stimulus. (based on information from parents) indicat-
However, vocalizations were also recorded to ed infrequent vocal play, no echoic behavior
assess the possibility that the participants and no other verbal operants. Dominick’s
might prefer vocalizing the target sound to sessions were conducted in a preschool
pressing buttons to produce it. If no prefer- classroom that was otherwise not in use
ence was observed in any condition, a within- during sessions. During sessions, Dominick
session reinforcer assessment was conducted was seated at a child-sized table, facing an
to test whether allocation of button presses to experimenter who was seated on the other
the target stimulus could be altered via side. A second experimenter was usually
contingent presentation of preferred stimuli. seated behind Dominick.
This assessment tested whether the absence
of a preference for the target stimulus could Stimuli and Apparatus
be attributed to failure to pair it with an
effective reinforcer. Auditory stimuli consisted of prerecorded
speech syllables in an adult, female voice,
GENERAL METHOD each 800 ms in duration. For Brandon and
Brennan, the stimuli were one-syllable words
Participants and Setting from an unfamiliar foreign language (Icelan-
dic). Each syllable contained at least one
The participants were three boys who had vowel or consonant-consonant combination
received diagnoses of autism-spectrum dis- that is nonexistent in English. The syllables
orders by qualified professionals, according used for Dominick were English vowel
to their parents. For each participant, 1–3 sounds. Table 1 shows the specific stimuli
sessions were conducted per day, 1–2 days used for each participant. Stimuli 1 (S1) and
per week. 2 (S2) were used in baseline and in most of
Brandon and Brennan were 4-year-old the subsequent conditions. For Dominick and
twins. The Behavioral Language Assessment Brennan, S1 and S2 were replaced with
form (BLA; Sundberg & Partington, 1998) stimuli 3 (S3) and 4 (S4) in their final
indicated (based on information from their condition.
mother and informal observation) that both The experimental manipulandum consisted
twins had high levels of vocal play, could of two identical plastic microswitch response
echo many different words and phrases, buttons (Big RedH) with an activation area
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 49

that measured 12.7 cm in diameter. The two sions was 94.5% (range, 83.3%–100.0%) for
buttons were identical in color (red for Brandon, 89.8% (range, 66.7%–100.0%) for
Dominick and green for Brandon and Bren- Brennan, and 91.6% (range, 83.3%–100.0%)
nan). Each button required 85 g of pressure to for Dominick.
activate. The buttons were connected via a
USB interface to a laptop computer running Procedure
E-Prime software (Schneider, Eschman, &
Zuccolotto, 2002) that was programmed to Paired-stimulus preference assessment.
record button presses and to control the Prior to the experiment, paired-stimulus pre-
presentation of auditory stimuli. Activation ference assessments (Fisher et al., 1992) were
of a response button resulted in immediate conducted to identify preferred toys and food
presentation of an auditory stimulus through items. Preferences for toys and food items
the built-in computer speakers. When the were assessed separately. Eight items were
response buttons were disconnected from the selected for each assessment based on infor-
computer, the experimenter could present mation from parents. The five items with the
auditory stimuli by pressing keys on the highest selection percentages were used in
keyboard. Brandon and Brennan’s baseline and initial
pairing phases (see Table 2). For Dominick,
PHASE 1: PAIRING one of the five highest-ranking items was
wooden blocks that repeatedly occasioned
Measurement problem behavior during the preference
assessment when he was asked to return
Dependent variables. The primary depen- them to the experimenter. As a result, the
dent variable was the rate of button pressing sixth highest-ranking item was used in place
to produce auditory stimuli during 2-min test of blocks during baseline and the initial
periods. These data were recorded automat- pairing phase.
ically by the experiment-running software. Experimental sessions. Each session con-
Data were also collected on vocalizations sisted of (a) a stimulus presentation period in
during test periods. Observers recorded these which a predetermined number of stimuli
data from video using real-time recording on were presented to the participant, and (b) a 2-
a laptop computer running the Behavioral min test period that immediately followed the
Evaluation Strategy and Taxonomy (BEST) stimulus presentation period. Experimental
software application (Sharpe & Koperwas, manipulations affected only the stimulus
1999). For Dominick, the observer recorded presentation period, whereas test period
all vocalizations that matched the vowel procedures remained constant throughout all
sounds listed in Table 1. Brandon and conditions.
Brennan could not precisely articulate the Baseline. During baseline sessions, a
syllables listed in Table 1. As a result, vocal stimulus presentation period consisted of 10
approximations were recorded when these presentations of S1, 10 presentations of S2,
participants emitted one-syllable vocaliza- and 10 presentations of a preferred item. Five
tions in which the vowel and at least one preferred items were used, with two presen-
consonant matched one of the syllables tations of each, except that if the participant
shown in Table 1. failed to consume one or more items, only
Interobserver agreement. A second ob- the remaining items were used for the
server independently scored vocalizations remainder of that session.
from video in at least 25% of each partici- During the stimulus presentation period,
pant’s test periods. To calculate exact count- the response buttons were out of the
per-interval (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, participant’s sight, and preferred items were
2007) agreement, each session was divided out of reach except when they were presented
into 10-s bins. An agreement was scored to the participant. The experimenter present-
when the two observers recorded identical ed the stimuli when prompted by the
data on the frequency and type of vocaliza- software program. Presentation order was
tions within a bin; otherwise, a disagreement randomized in blocks of three. The intertrial
was scored. Average agreement across ses- interval (ITI) was 10 s, and its timing began
50

Table 2
Preferred Stimuli in Experimental Conditions

Pre-session Pre-session selection +


Baseline and Pre-session Pre-session selection + pairing without
Participant pairing selection conditiona selection reversala extended pairinga pre-exposurea Phase 2
Brandon Police truck, cell Keyboard (x2), music Finger paint (x3),
phone, plain box (x1), shortbread Picture book (x2),
potato chips, cookies (x4) police truck (x2),
RufflesH, music box (x2),
chocolate teddy keyboard (x1), cell
grahams phone (x1),
shortbread cookies
(x11)
Brennan Police truck, finger Music box (x3), police Music box (x2), picture Finger paintb,
paint, picture truck (x1), markers book (x1), chocolate cheese
book, chocolate (x1), keyboard (x1), chip cookies (x2), crackers
chip cookies, chocolate chip cookies cheese crackers (x1)
cheese crackers (x4), cheese crackers
(x1), graham crackers
(x1)
Dominick Playdough, spinning Blocks (x6), rubber ball Blocks (x4), Blocks (x2), spaghetti Blocks, fruit
rod, spaghetti (x1) fruit snacks (x5) spaghetti ball ball (x1), fruit snacks
ball, goldfish saltine crackers (x2) (x1), playdough snacks (x3)
crackers, saltine (x1), moon dust
ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

crackers (x1), saltine


crackers (x2),
fruit snacks (x5)
a
Times selected in parentheses
b
Replaced with a picture book in minute 17
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 51

at the conclusion of the presentation of an session. Each of the two items was thus
auditory stimulus, or following the consump- scheduled for presentation five times in the
tion of a preferred item. session, but if a participant failed to consume
Before presenting a stimulus, the experi- one of the items, only the remaining item was
menter said the participant’s name to get his used during the remainder of the session (no
attention. Once the participant made eye participant ever failed to consume both of the
contact with the experimenter, the experi- items). The items selected by each partici-
menter presented the stimulus. An auditory pant during this and subsequent conditions
stimulus was presented by pressing a key on are shown in Table 2.
the keyboard, which resulted in the auditory Extended pairing. During this condition,
stimulus being played three times in succes- which was conducted with Dominick only,
sion, for a total stimulus duration of approx- each session contained 20 pairings of S1 with
imately 3000 ms. The stimuli were presented preferred items, and 20 presentations of S2.
through the built-in computer speakers posi- Pre-session selection of preferred items
tioned directly in front of the experimenter. continued during this condition.
The presentation of a preferred item consist- Pairing without pre-exposure. This condi-
ed of a small piece of food, or 10–20 s of tion was conducted with Dominick and
access to a toy. The experimenter presented Brennan. The procedures were the same as
the preferred item by placing it in front of the before, except that S3 (target) and S4
participant. (control), to which the participant had no
Pairing. Pairing sessions were identical to exposure in prior conditions, were substituted
baseline, except that during the stimulus for S1 and S2. Pre-session selection of
presentation period, presentations of S1 (the preferred items also continued during this
target stimulus) and preferred items were condition.
temporally contiguous. After saying the Testing. A test period consisted of 2 min of
participant’s name and establishing eye free access to the response buttons that
contact, the experimenter first presented S1 occurred immediately following each stimu-
by pressing the key on the keyboard, and lus presentation period. For Dominick, the
waited until the stimulus had been presented response buttons were placed 23 cm from the
twice. Upon the third presentation of the edge of the table, 140 cm apart, and
stimulus, the experimenter reached for and equidistant from Dominick’s midline. If
delivered a preferred item. The session Dominick got up from his seat during the
contained 10 pairings of S1 and a preferred session, he was redirected to sit. For Brandon
item, and 10 presentations of S2 (the control and Brennan, the experimenter positioned
stimulus) alone. herself in front of the participant, held each
Pre-session selection of preferred items. button at an arm’s length, one to the left and
When no preference for the target stimulus one to the right, and looked down. If the
was observed in the initial pairing condition, participant moved around the room, the
a pre-session stimulus selection procedure experimenter followed with the buttons. If
was implemented and included in all subse- the participant left the experimental area, he
quent conditions. The procedure was imple- was redirected into the area by a second
mented immediately before the beginning of experimenter.
each session and included the eight food Activation of each button produced one of
items and eight toys that were included in the the two auditory stimuli that were used in the
original paired-stimulus assessment. First, all preceding stimulus presentation period. The
eight food items were lined up in front of the left-right position of the target and the
participant. The participant was instructed to control stimuli alternated across sessions,
scan the array, and told to ‘‘Pick one.’’ The such that the same stimulus was presented in
participant received a small bite of the food the same location more in no more than two
he selected. Next, the procedure was repeated consecutive sessions. Before the beginning of
with toys, followed by approximately 20 s of each test period, the participant was prompt-
access to the selected toy. The selected food ed to press each button once, using a verbal
item and toy were paired with the target (‘‘press this one’’) and pointing prompt,
auditory stimulus in the subsequent pairing followed by a physical prompt if the
52 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

participant did not press the button. The item (defined as the beginning of any visible
purpose of prompting was to ensure that the movement that resulted in handing over a
participant contacted the auditory conse- preferred item) no earlier than the offset of
quences of pressing each button. The exper- the second presentation of the target stimu-
imenter then instructed the participant to lus, and no later than 1 s after the offset of the
‘‘press the buttons’’ while pointing to both third presentation. The average percentage of
buttons, and started the session timer. The correct stimulus presentations was 95.6% for
experimenter did not interact with the Brandon, 98.3% for Brennan, and 97.9% for
participant during the test period, unless it Dominick.
was necessary to redirect the participant to
the experimental area (this happened rarely). PHASE 2: REINFORCER ASSESSMENT
Each participant underwent brief button
training prior to baseline. During training, the Dominick and Brennan participated in a
placement of the buttons was the same as brief within-session reinforcer assessment in
during test sessions, but button activation Phase 2. Its purpose was to test whether
produced WindowsH jingles instead of preferred items, selected via the pre-session
speech sounds. The participant was prompted selection procedure from the pool of items
to press each button, and prompts gradually used in Phase 1, would serve as reinforcers
withdrawn until the participant made at least for pressing the response buttons.
one unprompted response on each button
during a 2-min period. Measurement
Experimental design. Following baseline,
different pairing conditions were presented The primary dependent variable was the
sequentially to the participants and a reversal cumulative number of unprompted button
design used to evaluate effects of pairing. A presses. Two observers independently re-
preference for the target sound was defined corded prompted and unprompted presses on
as four consecutive sessions in which rates of each response button from video, using a 1-
pressing for the target sound were higher min frequency-within-interval paper-and-
than for the control sound. If a participant pencil recording system. In each interval,
demonstrated preference for the target sound percentage agreement for each button was
in one of the pairing conditions, it was calculated by dividing the lower number of
followed by a reversal condition. The responses by the higher number of responses
reversal condition was identical to the and multiplying by 100. Average agreement
preceding condition except that the previous was 91.2% for Brennan and 98.4% for
control sound (e.g., S2) was paired with Dominick.
preferred items, while the previous target
sound (e.g., S1) served as the unpaired Procedure
control sound. In addition, a multiple-base-
line design (concurrent) across participants Pre-session selection of preferred items. A
was used for Brandon and Brennan’s obser- pre-session selection procedure identical to
vations. that used in Phase 1 was conducted to
Procedural fidelity. An observer scored at identify one food item and one toy for use
least 20% of all stimulus presentation periods in Phase 2. Brennan selected finger paint and
with respect to the timing of the experiment- cheese crackers and Dominick selected
er’s stimulus presentations. In baseline, the blocks and fruit snacks.
presentation of an auditory stimulus was Baseline. During baseline, response but-
scored as correct if it was not followed by the tons were arranged as in Experiment 1 test
presentation of another stimulus (auditory or periods. Responses on the right button
preferred item) within 10 s. Presentations of produced S1 and responses on the left button
control stimuli were scored in the same produced S2. At the beginning of baseline,
manner during all conditions that involved the participant was instructed to ‘‘Press the
pairing. Presentations of a target auditory buttons.’’ Every 20 s, the experimenter
stimulus were scored as correct if the prompted a response on the right button, by
experimenter initiated delivery of a preferred pointing (Brennan) or physically (Dominick).
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 53

An unprompted response delayed the presen- intervention program and became unavail-
tation of the next prompt by 20 s. Responses able for additional sessions. Brennan contin-
on either button did not produce any ued to press at similar rates as before during
consequences other than the auditory stimuli. the pre-session selection condition (M 5 8.08
FR1. The FR1 condition was identical to for S1 and M 5 7.50 for S2). When S3 and
baseline except that all prompted and un- S4 were introduced to the pairing sessions
prompted responses for S1 produced a bite of without prior exposure, no immediate pre-
food or 10-s access to a toy in an alternating ference was observed for either stimulus
sequence. The timing of the 20-s prompt (M 5 9.17 for S3 and M 5 8.50 for S4).
delay began at the conclusion of preferred The bottom panel of Figure 1 shows rates
item consumption. Dominick underwent two of button pressing for Dominick in Phase I.
FR1 phases, and in the second, the left-right Dominick made only one unprompted button
location of the auditory stimuli was reversed phase in baseline and none in the initial
and prompts occurred on the left button. pairing phase. The introduction of pre-
Extinction. Extinction was identical to session selection coincided with a temporary
baseline, except the left-right location of the increase in response rates, but button presses
auditory stimuli was reversed from the for the target and the control sound were
previous phase, and prompts occurred on the overall undifferentiated (M 5 .57 for S1 and
left button. The purpose of switching locations M 5 .50 for S2). During the extended pairing
was to use the extinction burst as an indicator condition, response rates decreased on both
of whether increased responding for the target buttons (M 5 .36 for S1 and M 5 .21 for S2),
sound in the previous phase reflected rein- and Dominick did not make any button
forcement of button pressing in a particular presses for S3 or S4 in the final condition.
location, or reinforcement of the production of Figure 2 shows the rate of vocalizations
a particular auditory stimulus. matching the target and control stimuli
during test periods. Brandon (top panel) and
RESULTS Dominick (bottom panel) engaged in low
rates of vocalizations throughout the exper-
The upper two panels of Figure 1 show iment, with the exception of Dominick’s
rates of button pressing for Brandon and Session 5 in which he matched S2 at a high
Brennan in Phase I. In baseline, both rate. Brandon and Dominick did not show
participants pressed the buttons at moderate, any changes in vocalization rates that
variable rates (Brandon: M 5 5.57 for S1 and coincided with phase changes, and they did
M 5 6.79 for S2; Brennan: M 5 7.45 for S1 not match target stimuli at higher rates than
and M 5 7.80 for S2). Both participants control stimuli. Brennan initially engaged in
alternated frequently between buttons, and low rates of vocalizations approximating S1
did not consistently press one button more and S2, but did not approximate S3 or S4. At
than the other. During the initial pairing the end of baseline, S1 and S2 approxima-
phase, both participants continued to press tions began to increase and continued at a
the buttons at similar rates (Brandon: M 5 higher level until the end of the pre-session
6.0 for S1 and M 5 6.16 for S2; Brennan: selection condition. His vocalizations were
M 5 7.50 for S1 and M 5 6.70 for S2). When mostly undifferentiated; however, he en-
the pre-session selection procedure was gaged in higher levels of S1 than S2
introduced, Brandon’s response rates in- approximations for six consecutive sessions
creased on both buttons, and he consistently starting with the last data point in pairing
produced the target sound at greater rates without pre-session selection. In the last
than the control sound (M 510.38 for S1 and condition, when S3 and S4 were introduced,
M 5 7.75 for S2). However, this preference Brennan’s vocalizations began to approxi-
was not successfully reversed in the follow- mate those stimuli and no longer approxi-
ing reversal condition; instead, response rates mated S1 or S2.
on both buttons returned to baseline levels Figure 3 shows the data from Phase 2 for
(M 5 5.45 for S1 and M 5 5.18 for S2). Brennan (upper panel) and Dominick (lower
Further analysis could not be conducted as panel). Brennan emitted unprompted re-
Brandon was enrolled in a full-time early sponses on both buttons during baseline,
54 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

Figure 1. Participants’ button pressing in Phase 1. Phase labels indicate (within parentheses) which
stimulus was paired with a preferred item in each condition. PSP 5 Pre-session Selection of Preferred
Items.

but during FR 1, a majority of his responses ing with the finger paint when it was
were on the button that produced S1. The per presented, and made repeated attempts to
minute rate of unprompted responses on the access a picture book. As a result, the picture
S1 button was higher during baseline (M 5 book replaced the finger paint in minute 17
6.75) than during FR 1 (M 5 2.27), but this and was used for the remainder of the phase.
was likely due to preferred item consumption During extinction, when the left-right loca-
time. During FR 1, Brennan stopped engag- tion of S1 and S2 was reversed, Brennan
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 55

Figure 2. Participants’ vocalizations in Phase 1. In the upper two panels, data from session 5 and 14
(both Brennan) are missing due to technical issues.

responded on both buttons. Brennan contin- Dominick did not make any unprompted
ued to respond for 3 minutes before respond- responses during baseline in Phase 2. During
ing decreased to zero. Following extinction, FR 1, he gradually began to make unprompt-
the session was terminated, as Brennan ed responses on the S1 button, but not on the
refused to continue cooperating with the S2 button. During extinction, most of Dom-
experimenters. He was unavailable for a inick’s responses were allocated to the S2
second session due to full-time enrollment button in the previously reinforced location.
in an early intervention program. FR 1 was re-instituted when responding on
56 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

Figure 3. Cumulative button presses for Brennan and Dominick in Phase 2.

the S2 button had slowed considerably. of the toys or food items used in the previous
During the second FR 1 phase, Dominick conditions. Thus, it is possible that the effect
immediately began to allocate all responses observed in this condition reflected a shift in
to S1 in the new location, and made no Brandon’s preferences from the pre-experi-
responses on the S2 button. mental preference assessment. Unfortunately,
an attempt at reversing this effect in the
DISCUSSION subsequent condition was only partially
successful, because although preference for
SSP did not reliably alter preferences for S1 was eliminated, preference did not shift to
speech sounds, as assessed with a concurrent- S2. It is possible that in the reversal
operant procedure. Brandon was the only condition, the prior history of S2 being
participant who showed evidence that the correlated with the absence of reinforcement
pairing procedure produced a consistent impeded its acquisition of reinforcing value.
preference for a target stimulus. Following Another possibility is that by this time, none
introduction of the pre-session selection of the items in the stimulus pool were
procedure, Brandon reliably allocated more effective reinforcers for Brandon’s behavior.
responding to the button that produced S1 However, because the absence of a prefer-
than the button that produced S2. His ence reversal precluded a convincing dem-
selections during this condition differed onstration of experimental control, an alter-
substantially from the preferred items pre- native explanation is that the pattern of
sented in the previous conditions (see responding observed in the first pre-session
Table 2); specifically, he never chose any selection condition was simply due to
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 57

extraneous factors and not functionally tions than the other participants. Most of his
related to the pre-session selection proce- vocalizations, as well as most of Brandon’s,
dure. Indeed, the pre-session selection effect occurred in close temporal proximity with
was not replicated convincingly with the button pressing and thus may have been
other two participants. Dominick pressed under echoic control. His higher rates of
the S1 button at a higher rate than the S2 vocalizations approximating S1 than S2 in
button in the first two pre-session selection sessions 31 through 40, which were not
sessions, and in addition, his overall rate of accompanied by consistently higher rates of
responding increased. However, this pattern button-pressing, might suggest that approxi-
did not persist, and thus, it is unknown if it mations of S1 were more reinforcing than
was related to the experimental manipula- approximations of S2 during this time.
tion. However, the difference was small and
Brennan and Dominick never showed a transient, and it was not observed with S3
consistent preference for a target stimulus, in and S4 in the subsequent condition.
spite of a number of procedural manipula- Given the numerous failures of SSP to
tions. Brennan’s mean level of responding affect vocalization rates that have been
for the target stimulus was slightly higher reported in the literature (Carroll & Klatt,
than for the control stimulus in all conditions 2008; B. E. Esch et al., 2005; Miguel et al.,
that involved SSP, but preference for the 2002; Normand & Knoll, 2006; Stock et al.,
target stimulus was not observed consistently 2008; Yoon & Feliciano, 2007), these
across sessions. The results of Phase 2 results are perhaps not surprising. In the
suggest that in Phase 1, the auditory stimuli present study, we assessed the hypothesized
had been paired with effective reinforcers, conditioning effect of stimulus-stimulus
but they still failed to acquire sufficient pairing in isolation from its hypothesized
reinforcing value to alter the distribution of reinforcing effects on vocalizations. Thus, a
button presses by themselves. However, piece of information that it contributes is
Brennan’s data may be limited in this regard, the suggestion that failure of SSP to
as responding for S1 was already higher than increase vocalizations may, at least in some
responding for S2 in baseline, and it was not cases, be attributed to failure of the
possible to conduct a second FR1 phase procedure to condition auditory stimuli as
following extinction. For Dominick, button reinforcers. However, the results are dis-
location appeared to be a more salient couraging in that they do not shed much
stimulus than the auditory stimuli that the light on procedural variables that reliably
buttons produced, as he continued to respond moderate or enhance the effects of SSP on
in the previously reinforced location during the reinforcing value of auditory stimuli.
extinction, as opposed to responding on the While they tentatively suggest that frequent
button that produced the sound previously preference assessments may be beneficial,
correlated with reinforcer delivery. This the effects of this variable were not reliable.
pattern may suggest that the failure to Eliminating pre-exposure to the target
observe a preference during Phase 1 was stimulus did not result in preference for
not likely due to a problem with discrimi- the target stimulus, and neither did an
nating which button location produced a increase in the number of pairings. In
more favorable consequence. For Brennan, addition, careful attempts were made to
this was less clear, as he made almost as prevent blocking and overshadowing at the
many responses for S1 in the new location as procedural level. By presenting auditory
for S2 in the previously reinforced location stimuli through the computer speakers and
during extinction. including a control stimulus in all condi-
No clear effects were observed on vocal- tions, we attempted to ensure that no
izations resembling the target auditory stim- stimulus other than the target stimulus was
uli, suggesting that the absence of an effect uniquely correlated with reinforcer delivery.
on button-pressing was not likely due to In addition, the experimenter reliably waited
displacement of button-pressing by a vocal- until the end of the target sound presenta-
ization that produced a similar stimulus. tion to visibly reach for a preferred item to
Brennan engaged in more frequent vocaliza- deliver to the participants. Thus, it seems
58 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

unlikely that any of these variables were clinically relevant behavior, the inclusion of
related to the failure to observe an effect other nonvocal participants might have
with Brennan and Dominick. strengthened external validity. Second, we
Some aspect of the concurrent-operants recorded vocalizations only during the 2-min
procedure may have contributed to the test periods that followed stimulus-presenta-
absence of an effect on button-pressing for tion periods, and not during the stimulus-
Brennan and Dominick and the absence of a presentation periods themselves. Most stud-
reversal effect for Brandon. For example, it is ies on SSP, including the majority of studies
possible that a concurrent-chains procedure that have successfully reported effects on
would have been more sensitive to prefer- vocalizations (Miguel et al., 2002; Sundberg
ence, or that an intermittent schedule of et al., 1996; Yoon & Bennett, 2000), have
stimulus presentation on the buttons would similarly assessed the dependent variable
have resulted in response differentiation. It is during brief periods immediately following
also possible that the presence of the SSP, in the absence of any stimulus presen-
response buttons during test periods made tation by the experimenters, in order to rule
them too easily discriminable from stimulus out echoic control over vocalizations. How-
presentation periods. Thus, it is possible that ever, in the most successful SSP demonstra-
the procedure did alter the reinforcing value tion to date with participants diagnosed with
of the target stimuli, but that our test autism, B. E. Esch et al. (2009) departed
procedures failed to detect it. Nevertheless, from this convention by recording vocaliza-
the absence of an effect, not only on button- tions during stimulus-presentation periods,
pressing but also on vocalizations, is in after pilot data indicated that an effect was
agreement with prior research suggesting more likely to be detected at this time. It is
that SSP effects on vocalizations are not possible that in the present study, an effect on
obtained reliably. vocalizations that could have been observed
Future research might explore the contri- during stimulus presentation periods dissi-
bution of additional procedural variables to pated quickly when pairing was discontin-
SSP effects on the conditioned reinforcing ued. This possibility was not evaluated due to
value of stimuli. Based on a review of the inconsistent video quality for Brandon and
existing SSP literature, Stock et al. (2008) Brennan, who moved frequently around the
tentatively concluded that SSP effects might experimental area during stimulus presenta-
be more likely with procedures that included tion periods. Third, we used a fixed rather
a greater number of pairings per minute, than a variable ITI. In B. E. Esch et al.
fewer presentations of the target sound per (2009), it is possible that a variable ITI
pairing, and social reinforcers, such as contributed to the successful demonstration
tickles. In the present study, the density of of an effect, and the potential contributions
pairings, number of presentations of the of ITI and pairing density should be evalu-
target sound per pairing, and the selection ated further. Fourth, a minimal number of
of preferred stimuli were highly similar to a sessions were conducted in Brennan and
study by B. E. Esch et al. (2009), in which Dominick’s final condition with stimuli S3
SSP successfully increased vocalizations. and S4, as well as in Dominick’s extended
However, none of these variables were pairing condition. It is possible that an effect
manipulated, and as a result, it is unknown would have been observed with prolonged
if they would have made a difference. exposure to these conditions. Finally, in
Additional limitations of the study should Phase 2, Brennan’s data would have benefit-
be noted. First, Dominick was the only ed from a more extended baseline, as well as
participant who was representative of indi- greater exposure to FR 1.
viduals for whom SSP might be considered SSP is easy to implement and requires
clinically. Brandon and Brennan had exten- little therapist training compared to other
sive vocal repertoires and were not in need of procedures for increasing specific vocaliza-
intervention for enhancing their level of tions, such as shaping. However, given its
vocal play or establishing prerequisites for unreliable effects in the literature to date and
echoic responding. While the purpose of the the lack of information on participant or
study was not to demonstrate effects on procedural variables that contribute to its
SPEECH SOUND PREFERENCES 59

effects, it may be premature to recommend are observed or attended to. In an SSP


the procedure clinically. Future research procedure, by contrast, there is no way of
might explore other procedures that might verifying that the stimuli are observed or that
increase the frequency or variability of vocal they are sufficiently discriminable for the
play without a need for shaping, for example, participant. Future research might evaluate
lag schedules of reinforcement (see J. W. the effects on vocalizations of speech stimu-
Esch, Esch, & Love, 2009). With regard to li’s acquisition of discriminative function.
SSP, the present results suggest that more
research may be needed specifically on the REFERENCES
conditions under which the auditory stimulus
may acquire a reinforcing function. In Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1965). Child
addition, future research should further development II: Universal stage of infan-
develop methods for assessing the reinforc- cy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
ing value of auditory stimuli in isolation from
Carroll, R. A., & Klatt, K. P. (2008). Using
their effects on vocalizations.
stimulus-stimulus pairing and direct rein-
In general, there is a paucity of research on forcement to teach vocal verbal behavior
conditioned reinforcement in children diag-
to young children with autism. The
nosed with autism. This is surprising, given
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 135–146.
that a common suggestion in the early
Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Haymes, L. K.
intervention literature is that social stimuli
(1998). Using objects of obsession as
(e.g., praise) may fail to function as rein-
token reinforcers for children with autism.
forcers for children with autism and must be
Journal of Autism and Developmental
established as such by association with
Disorders, 28, 189–198.
primary reinforcers (e.g., Greer & Ross,
2008; Lovaas, 2003). A few studies have Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L.
investigated token reinforcement (e.g., Char- (2007). Applied Behavior Analysis (2nd
lop-Christy & Haymes, 1998; Moher, Gould, ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Hegg, & Mahoney, 2008; Tarbox, Ghezzi, & Dinsmoor, J. A. (1995). Stimulus control:
Wilson, 2006) or other nonsocial conditioned Part II. The Behavior Analyst, 18, 253–
reinforcers (e.g., Greer & Singer-Dudek, 269.
2008; Holth, Vandbakk, Finstad, Gronnerud, Esch, B. E., Carr, J. E., & Grow, L. L.
& Sorensen, 2009) in this population. (2009). Evaluation of an enhanced stim-
However, we are aware of only two studies ulus-stimulus pairing procedure to in-
(Greer, Singer-Dudek, Longano, & Zrinzo, crease early vocalizations of children with
2008; Isaksen & Holth, 2009) that have autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
successfully demonstrated establishment of Analysis, 42, 225–241.
verbal or social reinforcers in children with Esch, B. E., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. (2005).
autism diagnoses. Interestingly, neither study Evaluating stimulus-stimulus pairing and
employed a contiguous pairing procedure direct reinforcement in the establishment
similar to those employed in the SSP of an echoic repertoire of children diag-
literature; Greer et al. employed an observa- nosed with autism. The Analysis of Verbal
tion learning protocol, whereas Isaksen and Behavior, 21, 43–58.
Holth employed an operant discrimination Esch, J. W., Esch, B. E., & Love, J. R.
training procedure. Indeed, Holth et al. (2009). Increasing vocal variability in
(2009) found that discrimination training children with autism using a lag schedule
was more effective than contiguous pairing of reinforcement. The Analysis of Verbal
with preferred items at establishing arbitrary Behavior, 25, 73–78.
neutral stimuli as conditioned reinforcers for Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L.G.,
children with and without autism diagnoses. Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I.
The authors offered no particular explanation (1992). A comparison of two approaches
for this finding; however, it may be specu- for identifying reinforcers for persons
lated that an operant response requirement with severe and profound disabilities.
offers the advantage of providing the thera- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
pist with evidence that the relevant stimuli 25, 491–498.
60 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al.

Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto,
behavior analysis: Inducing and expanding A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pitts-
new verbal capabilities in children with burgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
language delays. Boston, MA: Pearson. Sharpe, T., & Koperwas, J. (1999). BEST:
Greer, R. D., Singer-Dudek, J., Longano, J., Behavioral evaluation strategy and taxon-
& Zrinzo, M. (2008). The emergence of omy software [Technical Manual]. Thou-
praise as conditioned reinforcement as a sand Oaks, CA: Sage/Scolari.
function of observation in preschool and Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior.
school age children. Revista Mexicana de Acton, MA: Copley.
Psicologı́a, 25, 5–26. Smith, R., Michael, J., & Sundberg, M. L.
Greer, R. D., & Singer-Dudek, J. (2008). The (1996). Automatic reinforcement and au-
emergence of conditioned reinforcement tomatic punishment in infant vocal be-
from observation. Journal of the Experi- havior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
mental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 15–29. 13, 39–48.
Holth, P., Vandbakk, M., Finstad, J., Gron- Stock, R. A., Schulze, K. A., & Mirenda, P.
nerud, E. M., & Sorensen, J. M. A. (2009). (2008). A comparison of stimulus-stimu-
An operant analysis of joint attention and lus pairing, standard echoic training, and
the establishment of conditioned social control procedures on the vocal behavior
reinforcers. European Journal of Behavior of children with autism. The Analysis of
Analysis, 10, 143–158. Verbal Behavior, 24, 123–133.
Isaksen, J., & Holth, P. (2009). An operant
Sundberg, M. L., Michael, J., Partington, J.
approach to teaching joint attention skills
W., & Sundberg, C. A. (1996). The role of
to children with autism. Behavioral Inter-
automatic reinforcement in early language
ventions, 24, 215–236.
acquisition. The Analysis of Verbal Be-
Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals
havior, 13, 21–37.
with developmental delays: Basic inter-
vention techniques. Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998).
Mazur, J. E. (2006). Learning and behavior Teaching language to children with au-
(6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pren- tism or other developmental disabilities.
tice Hall. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts.
Miguel, C. F., Carr, J. E., & Michael, J. Tarbox, R. S. F., Ghezzi, P. M., & Wilson, G.
(2002). The effects of a stimulus-stimulus (2006). The effects of token reinforce-
pairing procedure on the vocal behavior of ment on attending in a young child with
children diagnosed with autism. The autism. Behavioral Interventions, 21,
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 18, 3–13. 155–164.
Moher, C. A., Gould, D. D., Hegg, E., & Yoon, S., & Bennett, G. M. (2000). Effects of
Mahoney, A. M. (2008). Non-generalized a stimulus-stimulus pairing procedure on
and generalized conditioned reinforcers: conditioning vocal sounds as reinforcers.
Establishment and validation. Behavioral The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 17, 75–
Interventions, 23, 13–38. 88.
Normand, M. P., & Knoll, M. L. (2006). The Yoon, S., & Feliciano, G. M. (2007).
effects of a stimulus-stimulus pairing pro- Stimulus-stimulus pairing and subsequent
cedure on the unprompted vocalizations of a mand acquisition of children with various
young child diagnosed with autism. The levels of verbal repertoires. The Analysis
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 81–85. of Verbal Behavior, 23, 3–16.

You might also like