You are on page 1of 65

Do people believe they are less predictable than others?

Three replications of Pronin and Kugler (2010)’s Experiment 1

*Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar


Department of Psychology,
Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU)
prasad.chandrashekar@ntnu.no
ORCID: 0000-0002-8599-9241

*Stephanie Permut
Andlinger Center for Energy and the Environment
Princeton University
sepermut@princeton.edu

*Hallgeir Sjåstad
Norwegian School of Economics
hallgeir.sjastad@nhh.no
ORCID: 0000-0002-8730-1038

** Chelsea (Chi Wing) LO


chelsea1@connect.hku.hk / lochiwingchelsea@gmail.com
Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong
ORCID: 0009-0008-6052-7759

**Yong Jun KUEH


u3512000@connect.hku.hk / jean_5086@hotmail.com
Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong

***Emily Sihui ZHONG (ezhong@connect.hku.hk / zhong_emil@bentley.edu),


***Kai Hin WAN (u3538877@connect.hku.hk / dannywan1222@gmail.com),
***Kai Yi Kelly CHOY (u3529138@connect.hku.hk / ckyk97@gmail.com),
***Man Chung WONG (wong1109@connect.hku.hk / manchungwong@gmail.com),
***Stanley Wei Jian HUGH (u3543385@connect.hku.hk / stanleyhugh2017@u.northwestern.edu),
***Khan TAHIRA (u3537184@connect.hku.hk / t.caan786@gmail.com),
Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong

Bo Ley CHENG
boleystudies@gmail.com

^Gilad Feldman
Department of Psychology, University of Hong Kong
University of Hong Kong
gfeldman@hku.hk / giladfel@gmail.com
ORCID: 0000-0003-2812-6599

* shared first author; ** shared third author; *** shared fifth author ;^ corresponding author
Word: abstract – 173, manuscript - 5074
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 2

Author Bios:
Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar is postdoctoral researcher with the Department of
Psychology at the Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU). His research
focuses on moral psychology, lay-beliefs, and judgment and decision-making.

Stephanie Permut is an associate fellow with the Andlinger Center for Energy and the
Environment at Princeton University.
Hallgeir Sjåstad is a professor at the Norwegian School of Economics, Department of Strategy
and Management. His research focuses on judgment and decision-making, and social
psychology.
Chi Wing LO, Yong Jun KUEH, Emily Sihui ZHONG, Kai Hin WAN, Kay Yi Kelly CHOY,
Man Chung WONG, Stanley Wei Jian HUGH, and Khan TAHIRA were students at the
University of Hong Kong in the year 2018.

Bo Ley Cheng was a teaching assistant at the University of Hong Kong psychology department
in the year 2018.
Gilad Feldman is an assistant professor with the University of Hong Kong psychology
department. His research focuses on judgment decision-making.
Declaration of Conflict of Interest:
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or
publication of this article.
Financial disclosure/funding:
The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.
Acknowledgments or Author Notes:
None.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 3

Authorship declaration
A team comprising of Chi Wing LO, Yong Jun KUEH designed and wrote the pre-registrations
for Study 1b. A team comprising of Emily Sihui ZHONG, Kai Hin WAN, Kay Yi Kelly CHOY,
Man Chung WONG, Stanley Wei Jian HUGH, and Khan TAHIRA designed and wrote the pre-
registrations for Study 1a. Bo Ley CHENG and Gilad Feldman guided the two teams. Gilad
conducted the pre-registrations, and ran data collections. The two teams analyzed and wrote
initial reports of the findings.
Hallgeir and Gilad designed and wrote the pre-registration for Study 2. Gilad conducted Study 2
data collection. Prasad and Stephanie verified teams’ work on Studies 1a and 1b, and analyzed
Study 2.
Hallgeir and Stephanie integrated the three projects to a unified manuscript. Gilad reverified the
three studies, added analyses, and edited the first draft. Prasad verified and extended the analyses
and finalized the manuscript for journal submission, with Gilad’s final editing.

Contributor Roles Taxonomy

Role S.P. Hallgeir Sjåstad Stephanie Chi Wing LO, Yong Jun Bo Ley Gilad
Chandrashekar Permut KUEH, Emily Sihui CHENG Feldman
ZHONG, Kai Hin WAN,
Kay Yi Kelly CHOY, Man
Chung WONG, Stanley Wei
Jian HUGH, and Khan
TAHIRA
Conceptualization X
Pre-registrations X X X
Data curation X
Formal analysis X X X X
Funding acquisition X
Investigation X X
Methodology X X
Pre-registration peer X X X
review / verification
Data analysis peer X X X X X
review / verification
Project administration X X
Resources X
Software X X
Supervision X X X
Validation X X X
Visualization X X X
Writing-original draft X X X X
Writing-review and X X X X
editing
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 4

Abstract

Pronin and Kugler (2010) proposed that people believe they have more free will than others. In

their Experiment 1 they showed that American students evaluated their own decisions and life

events as less predictable than similar decisions and life events of close others, presumably

suggesting higher free-will attributions. We conducted three pre-registered replications of this

study, one with undergraduate sample in Hong Kong and two with MTurk participants from the

US using CloudResearch (total N = 1031). In Studies 1a and 1b that mirrored the target article’s

mixed design (self-other between, past-future within), we found support for the original findings

with weaker effects. In Study 2 we contrasted between-subject versus within-subject designs in a

single data collection. We successfully replicated the effects with the between-subject design,

whereas we failed to find support for the effect using the within-subjects design. This suggests

support for the phenomenon in single evaluation mode assessing either the self or the other, but

that people correct for the self-other asymmetry in perceived predictability when the judgment is

made in joint evaluations mode. Materials, data, and code are available on: https://osf.io/ykmqp/

Keywords: Predictability, self-other asymmetry, free will attributions, replications


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 5

Do people believe they are less predictable than others?

Three replications of Pronin and Kugler (2010)’s Experiment 1

Do people believe that they have more free will than others? An influential study by

Pronin and Kugler (2010) provided initial evidence for this hypothesis, in which their

Experiment 1 showed that people believe that their own decisions and life outcomes are less

predictable than similar decisions and life outcomes of others. The link the authors made to free

will, was that higher predictability of past and future events would presumably be at odds with

personal agency, at least in laypersons’ views, thereby leaving less room for freedom of choice.

In the current research, we report three pre-registered replications of this experiment

using different samples and variations of study designs to reproduce, replicate, and test the

generalizability of this effect.

‘Free Will’ Research in Psychology

Free will is often associated with the capacity for making choices between alternatives

without internal or external constraints (Feldman et al., 2014; Monroe et al., 2014; Rigoni et al.,

2017). Most laypersons seem to believe in some form of free will (Alquist et al., 2015; Monroe

& Malle, 2010; Nahmias et al., 2005; Nichols, 2004; Sarkissian et al., 2010; Stillman et al.,

2011). In philosophy, there is extensive debate about the very existence of free will, how it

should be conceptualized in formal terms, whether it is compatible with determinism and

biological evolution, and what the implications of free will might be for ethics and society (e.g.,

Dennett & Caruso, 2021). In social psychology and experimental philosophy, the focus has

shifted towards using empirical research methods to study how ordinary people think and feel
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 6

about free will, and to examine the causes, consequences, and correlates of people’s free-will

beliefs and attributions. The study by Pronin and Kugler has been one of the first to suggest

interesting asymmetries in the way that people tend to think about concepts related to free will

when reflecting on their own comparing to that of others’ life circumstances.

People readily make inferences about the degree of personal agency involved in a

decision, both when reflecting on their own behavior and on the behaviors of others (Gray et al.,

2012). Previous research documented associations between free-will beliefs with motivation and

work performance (Stillman et al., 2010), psychological well-being and adjustment (Crescioni et

al., 2016), and stricter moral judgments of blame and punishment (Martin et al., 2017; Shariff et

al., 2014). It therefore seems that free-will beliefs are associated with a broad range of factors

that might impact people’s lives. We therefore sought to conduct an independent replication of

what we considered to be one of the first central findings in this literature, suggesting that people

believe that they have more free will than other people by being less predictable a priori.

Early replication attempts of free-will research focused on so-called “priming” activation

experiments, seeking to induce a spill-over effect from encouraging disbelief in free will on a

subsequent and unrelated task. For these methods, rigorous studies have concluded that it is more

difficult than previously thought to create such intrinsic variation in free-will beliefs, where

recent studies have failed to replicate the original effects on the dependent variable as well

(Chartier et al., 2020; Nadelhoffer et al., 2020). A more promising approach seems to be

correlational research on associations with individual differences in free-will beliefs (e.g., Martin

et al., 2017), or experiments regarding free-will attributions that vary one aspect of a

hypothetical choice situation before eliciting agency attributions and related judgments (e.g.,

Everett et al., 2021; Feldman et al., 2016, 2018).


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 7

Pronin and Kugler (2010) used a variant of the latter method and randomly assigned

participants to evaluate free-will related indicators regarding their own lives or the lives of

others. In the case of Experiment 1, they tested people’s attributions of predictability regarding

past and future life events, contrasting evaluations of one’s own life compared to that of other

people.

Self-Other Asymmetries in Human Judgment

There are many documented phenomena of self-other asymmetries. One of those is

“unrealistic optimism”, which is the phenomenon that people tend to believe they are more likely

to experience positive events and life outcomes than is warranted, and to underestimate their

personal risks (Shepperd et al., 2015; Weinstein, 1980). Research on the “better-than-average

effect” has documented that people have a broad tendency to view themselves in a favorable

light when comparing themselves to others (Alicke, 1985; Svenson, 1981; see successful

replication by: Koppel et al., 2023; Ziano et al., 2021), though over time identifying some

nuances and moderators, such as an interaction with difficulty (Kruger, 1999).

Pronin and Kugler (2010) found that people attribute more free will to themselves both

for positive and negative outcomes, which suggested a distinct phenomenon from the classic

form of optimism and better-than-average effects. Drawing on the “actor-observer bias” (Jones

& Nisbett, 1971), Pronin and Kugler (2010) proposed that people might perceive the actions of

others as fixed dispositions of their stable and uncontrollable personalities, whereas people

perceive their own actions as intentional unconstrained responses to changing situations. This

latter perspective presumably leaves more room for free will to operate.

Cognitive neglect vs. Self-enhancement motives


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 8

The self-other asymmetry observed in free will attributions may stem from the self-

enhancement motive, wherein individuals tend to perceive themselves more favorably than the

average person across various domains, including intelligence, skill, and positive personality

traits (Alicke, 1985; Alicke et al., 1995; Ziano et al., 2021). Alternatively, this asymmetry could

be partially or entirely influenced by a cognitive bias, where individuals rely on introspective

information, such as feelings and intentions, for self-assessments. Considering these possibilities,

it can be argued that if the self-other asymmetry is primarily driven by self-enhancement

motives, it will persist in within-person comparisons. Conversely, the absence of this asymmetry

in within-person comparisons suggests that cognitive neglect, which individuals can potentially

correct for, plays a significant role in driving the asymmetry. We proposed an extension study to

evaluate this possibility.

Extension: Examining bias, by contrasting within and between subject designs

It remains an open question what research participants themselves would make of their

tendency to claim being less predictable than others. As that experiment was based on a between-

subjects self-other design, where participants either rated the level of predictability in their own

lives or in the lives of close others, the systematic inconsistency between conditions remain

“invisible” to individuals making those ratings. Thus, in addition to examining the replicability

of Pronin and Kugler’s (2010) main finding, in our Study 2 we extended their original design

with a variety of contrasts (1) fully within-subject conditions, 2) a fully between-subject

condition, 3) mixed-design) within a single data collection.

Previous studies suggested that if a given effect is driven by cognitive neglect or

irrational bias, then the effect will tend to be weaker or disappear when tested in a within-

subjects design. This is because the within-subjects structure facilitates direct comparison in joint
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 9

evaluation (Caruso et al., 2008; Hsee et al., 1999; Kahneman & Ritov, 1994). Based on this

logic, we reasoned that if we observe that people in a within-subjects condition do not show the

effect, then we might interpret this as cognitive neglect, and less likely to have a motivational

origin. That is, people can recognize the inconsistency when it is made clear by contrasting

conditions against one another in the same experiment (for a recent example of this method, see

Sjåstad et al., 2020).

Studies overview

In the current research, we report three pre-registered replications of Experiment 1 in

Pronin and Kugler (2010). For the target study, they observed very large effects. Our Studies 1a

and 1b were close replications of the original study design, with initial small samples, which we

considered to be pre-tests. In Study 2, we followed-up with a much larger sample, also aiming to

examine the possible role of cognitive neglect versus motivational self-enhancement as

mechanisms. In Study 2 we included an extension contrasting the fully within-subject condition

with the fully between-subject condition, adjusted from the original mixed design. Study 2 also

included a trait achievement measure as a possible individual-difference moderator of the effect.

We summarized the replication hypotheses in Table 1 and the extension hypotheses in Table 2.

We provided an overview of the findings of Experiment 1 of the target article in Table 2.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 10

Table 1

Pronin and Kugler (2010): Summary of hypotheses

Study Hypothesis

Hypothesis 1 People perceive their own past and future decision as less predictable a priori than
those of a roommate.
Hypothesis 2a People perceive their own past decisions as less predictable than the ones of others

Hypothesis 2b People perceive their own future decisions as less predictable than the ones of others.

Hypothesis 3 People perceive future decisions as less predictable than past decisions.

Table 2

Summary of original findings in Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1

Effect size
df 1 df 2 F p
Cohen’s d with 95% CI
(H1) Across both past
and future outcomes: 1 48 14.46 .0004 -1.08 [-1.67, -0.48]
Self vs. Roommate
(H2a) Across past
outcomes: 1 48 6.40 .010 -0.76 [-1.29, -0.14]
Self vs. Roommate
(H2b) Across future
outcomes: 1 48 8.46 .005 -0.82 [-1.40, -0.25]
Self vs. Roommate

(H3) Future vs. Past 1 48 10.94 .002 -0.94 [-1.52, -0.35]


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 11

Open science, preregistrations, and disclosures

Materials, data, and code are available on the Open Science Framework (project:

https://osf.io/ykmqp/; preregistration Study 1a: https://osf.io/jg94e ; preregistration Study 1b:

https://osf.io/yaepf/ ; preregistration Study 2: https://osf.io/sjb8p). All studies, participants,

measures, manipulations, and exclusions conducted for this investigation are reported, and data

collection was completed before hypothesis testing.

Planned Sample

We calculated effects in Pronin and Kugler (2010) using available F-statistics and sample

sizes for each condition with the help of a guide by Jané et al. (2024). The effect size (Cohen’s d)

of 1.1 for the between-subject factor (self vs. other) used as the basis of power analysis.

In study 1a and 1b, we based our power analysis on having 95% (α =0.05) power to

detect a Cohen’s d = 1.08. This resulted in a suggested sample size of 46 across 2 between-

subject conditions. More detailed descriptions of the effect-size calculations and power analyses

can be found in the supplementary materials.

In Study 2, we used the G*Power software to determine the number of participants

needed to detect an effect size of d = 0.32 at 95% power (α =0.05). The effect size of d = 0.32

was based on the results of Study 1b. Our analysis yielded a sample of 426 participants for

studies involving between-subjects effects and a sample of 108 participants for studies involving

within-subject effects. To ensure that both designs were conducted with adequate statistical

power, aiming for a conservative estimate, we took the largest sample size needed to detect a

moderate between-subjects effect and set this as our target sample.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 12

Deviations from Original Studies

We provided detailed information of designs (type of study, sample, variables, exact

wordings) of the original studies in the Methods and Original articles’ results section of the

Supplementary materials. We note several deviations from the original, summarized in Table 3

and Table 4. We recruited participants on MTurk using CloudResearch rather than recruiting

university students and made several adjustments and added extensions.

Table 3

Comparison of the sample in the original study and in the replications

Pronin and Replication Replication Replication


Kugler (2010), Study 1a Study 1b Study 2
Exp. 1
Sample size 50 47 126 1295
Geographic US American Hong Kong, US American US American
origin SAR
Population Undergraduates Undergraduates MTurk/CloudResearch Prolific
participants participants
Gender Not reported 21 females 63 females 770 females
Median age Not reported 20 24 20
Average age Not reported 20.21 25.83 20.40
Standard Not reported 1.02 6.46 1.48
deviation age
Age range Not reported 3 36 26
Medium Not reported Computer Computer (online) Computer
(location) (online) (online)
Compensation Yes, received Yes Yes Yes
candy for
completing the
survey
Year Probably 2009 2018 2018 2019
or 2010
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 13

Replications Classification

We categorized Studies 1a and 1b as “very close replications” and Study 2 as a “close

replication” based on the criteria by LeBel et al. (2018), with our classification analysis provided

in Table 4.

Table 4

Evaluation of replication closeness (using criteria by LeBel et al., 2018)

Design facet Replication Details


Effect/hypothesis Same
IV construct Same
DV construct Same
IV operationalization Same
DV Same
operationalization
Population (e.g., age) Different The original study was conducted with a sample of
US undergraduates, whereas the replication effort
involved a sample of undergraduate students from
Hong Kong and US participants from
MTurk/CloudResearch and Prolific.
IV stimuli Some There are minor set of variations in experimental
differences for condition allocations.
Study 2; Same
for Studies 1a
and 1b
DV stimuli Same
Procedural details Similar

Physical settings Different Online data collection in our replication vs filling a


questionnaire
in the target article
Contextual variables Different The original authors conducted their studies before
2010, whereas we conducted the replications in the
years 2018-19.
Replication Studies 1a, 1b: Very close replication; Study 2: Close replication
classification:
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 14

Note of alpha tails and p-values interpretations

We note that we did not pre-register whether our alpha is for one-tail or two-tail, with the

pre-registration for Study 1a noting power analysis for two-tail and the pre-registration for Study

1b noting one-tail. In the reported results below we refer to one-tail tests, given that the

hypotheses are well stated and these were replications.

Study 1a: Hong Kong Student sample

Study 1a was our first attempt to replicate the effect, conducted on an undergraduate

student sample in Hong Kong. The experiment was run together with a series of other judgment

and decision-making studies that were designed by the students in the sample and ran together in

randomized order on the designing students. Therefore, given that students were familiar with

other judgment and decision-making phenomena and the relatively small sample, we consider

this study to be a conservative pre-test replication on a well-informed sample.

Method

Participants

We recruited a small sample of 47 participants (16 males, 31 females, Age mean = 20.2,

Age SD = 1.02). A sensitivity analysis indicates that this sample can detect effects of d = 0.97

(one-tail, 95% power), an effect smaller than that in the target article, yet we considered this a

pre-test. Materials were presented in an online survey using the Qualtrics software. We collected

no demographic information in this study.

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions: ‘self’ and ‘other’.

Participants in the ‘self’ condition judged the predictability of past and future events in their own
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 15

lives whereas participants in the ‘other’ condition judged the predictability of past and future

events in the life of a close other. We prompted participants in the ‘other’ condition to provide

the initials of the person they had in mind before proceeding to provide their predictability

ratings.

We ran a 2 target (self versus other; between-subjects) by 2 time (past versus future;

within-subjects) mixed study design. Participants evaluated three past target events (1 = Not at

all predictable, 7 = Extremely predictable): 1) Chosen university for studies, 2) chosen major for

studies, and 3) failure in dating, and three future events: 1) Career choice, 2) person to marry, 3)

place to live, six target events in total.

Exclusions

We pre-registered an exclusion criteria (failing to complete the entire survey, failing to

adhere to pre-test requirements, indicating low English proficiency and reporting low

seriousness), yet no participants were excluded as a result of these exclusion criteria. The

students who designed the study also participated but their answers were excluded from the

analysis.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 16

Results

We summarized descriptives in Table 5 and Figure 1.

Table 5

Study 1:Predictability descriptives

Target [between] Self (n = 23) Other (n = 24)

Time [within] M SD M SD

Past 4.25 1.39 4.68 0.72

Future 3.01 1.39 4.00 1.08

Overall 3.63 1.02 4.34 1.02


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 17

Figure 1

Study 1a: Predictability – Self versus Other

Note. Violin plot of the analysis of Self versus Other in Study 1a. Predictability: 1 = not at all predictable; 7 = extremely predictable. Dependent
variables across the panels: A) Past predictability; B) Future predictability; C) Past and Future predictability.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 18

We conducted an independent t-test and found support for the findings in Pronin and

Kugler (2010). Participants viewed their own decisions as being less predictable than the

decisions of close others (Self: n = 23, M = 3.63, SD = 1.02; other: n = 24, M = 4.34, SD = 0.62;

t(45) = -2.89, p = .003, one-tailed, d = -0.84, 95% CI [-1.46, -0.23]).

Examining past and future separately, future events for self (M = 3.01, SD = 1.39) were

rated as less predictable than others’ (M = 4.00, SD = 1.08; t(45) = -2.72 p = .005, one-tailed, d =

-0.79, 95% CI [-1.41, -0.18]). We did not find support for past events, as self past events (M =

4.25, SD = 1.39) were rated similarly to others’ past events (M = 4.68, SD = 0.72; t(45) = -1.36, p

= .091, one-tailed, d = -0.40, 95% CI [-0.99, 0.20]). Therefore, the effects were stronger for the

future than for the past, although given the small sample we did not detect support for

interaction. The results of the mixed ANOVA analysis are reported in the supplementary section

(see Table S3 and S4).

Consistent with the original findings, there also was a main effect for time, whereby

people perceived the future as less predictable than the past (M = 3.52 (SD = 1.32) vs. 4.47

(1.11); t(92) = -3.78, p < .001, one-tailed, d = -0.78, 95% CI [-1.20, -0.35]).

Study 1b: US Americans on MTurk

Method

Procedure and Exclusions

We recruited an online sample of 136 US American student participants from Amazon

Mechanical Turk using CloudResearch/TurkPrime (Litman et al., 2017). All study materials and

instructions were identical in structure and content to the survey used in Study 1a.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 19

Four participants failed to give consent to partake in the study, and four other participants

indicated that they were not currently American students, and therefore were not allowed to

advance past the consent form of the survey. Two additional participants did not complete the

study. Our final sample consisted of 126 participants (Mage = 25.88, 63 female, 63 male).

Results and discussion

We summarized descriptives in Table 6 and Figure 2.

Table 6

Study 1b: Predictability descriptives

Target [between] Self (n = 64) Other (n = 62)


Time [within] M SD M SD
Past 4.56 0.92 4.81 1.22
Future 3.94 1.36 4.33 1.34
Overall 4.24 0.95 4.58 1.15
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 20

Figure 2

Study 1b: Self vs Other predictability

Note. Violin plot of the analysis of Self versus Other in Study 1b. Predictability: 1 = not predictable at all; 7 = extremely predictable. Dependent
variables across the panels: A) Past predictability; B) Future predictability; C) Past and Future predictability.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 21

We conducted an independent t-tests and effects were in the expected direction, yet we

failed to meet the evidence criteria we set for concluding support for the findings in Pronin and

Kugler (2010). Participants viewed their own decisions as being less predictable than the

decisions of close others (Self: n = 64, M = 4.25, SD = 0.95; other: n = 62, M = 4.57, SD = 1.16;

t(124) = -1.69, p = .046, one-tailed, d = -0.30, 95% CI [-0.66, 0.05]).

Examining past and future separately, future events for self (M = 3.94, SD = 1.36) were

rated as less predictable than others’ (M = 4.33, SD = 1.34; t(124) = -1.63, p = .053, one-tailed, d

= -0.29, 95% CI [-0.64, 0.06]). We did not find support for past events, as self past events (M =

4.56, SD = 0.92) were rated similarly to others’ past events (M = 4.81, SD = 1.22; t(124) = --

1.29, p = 0.099, one-tailed, d = -0.23, 95% CI [-0.58, 0.12]). Therefore, the effects were stronger

for the future than for the past, although given the small sample we did not detect support for

interaction. Additionally, we found support for main effect for time, whereby people perceived

the future as less predictable than the past (M = 4.69 (SD =1.08) vs. 4.13 (1.36); t(125) = -5.14, p

< 0.001, one-tailed, d = -0.45, 95% CI [-0.70, -0.20]). The results of the mixed ANOVA analysis

are reported in the supplementary section (see Table S5 and S6).

The original effect estimates were in the predicted direction, yet caution against

overinterpretation given the weak effects and just-below threshold one-tail p-values, especially

given that we were not clear in advance about whether we will employ one or two-tail analyses,

although our power analyses referred to one-tail. Still, we provide our data as is. We followed

our pre-registered criteria, and tried to avoid any flexibility in analysis, yet recognize the

challenge with p-values just below .05. We see the reporting of one-tail as reasonable given that

this was a replication with clear directional hypotheses.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 22

Across Study 1a and 1b, we found support for the target study’s findings. Yet we caution

against overinterpretation given the weak effects and just-below threshold one-tail p-values1.

Study 2: US Americans on Prolific

We considered Study 2 as an extension with an aim to test replicability of the original

findings with a set of study design variations in a single data collection effort. We aimed to test

whether the effect would be stronger in the fully between-subjects design compared to the other

two conditions, in that participants would correct for the asymmetry if asked to evaluate both self

and other.

The study participants were randomly assigned to one of the following experimental

design conditions1: 1) fully between-subjects design, 2) fully within-subject design, and 3) a

mixed design. All study materials and instructions were identical in structure and content to the

survey used in previous studies. We focused our analyses below on the first two conditions,

given an oversight in the third condition that we felt limited its contribution, and its analyses are

reported in the supplementary materials.

The study participants in fully between-subjects design were randomly assigned to one of

four conditions in a between-subjects design: 2 (Target: self vs. other) × 2 (Time: Past vs

Future). Whereas the participants in fully within-subjects design answers all four combinations

of Target (self, other) and Time (past, future)2.

1
We note that we included two additional exploratory extensions that in hindsight we felt did not contribute to our
understanding of the phenomenon: 1) a mixed-design condition, and 2) an achievement motivation scale as a
potential predictor/moderator. We decided to report the results of these two extensions in the supplementary
materials.
2
We note that in our pre-registration we referred to one-tail in our power analyses yet we felt that we were not
specific enough about whether we planned to employ a one-tail or two-tail analyses. We provide our data as is. We
followed our pre-registered criteria, and tried to avoid any flexibility in analysis, yet recognize the challenge with p-
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 23

Method

Participants

We recruited a total of 1304 online student participants using Prolific participant

recruitment platform. We pre-registered several exclusion criteria for this study: Low English

proficiency, reporting not being serious or if they failed to complete the study. In total, we

excluded 9 participants, which resulted in a sample of 1295 that met our exclusion criteria (525

males, 770 females, mean age = 20.4, mean SD = 1.93).

Procedure

After responding to the consent form, participants completed a 10-item version of the

Achievement Motives Scale (Lang & Fries, 2006). They were then randomly assigned to view

one of three study designs: fully between-subjects (n = 424), fully within-subjects (n = 434) or

the mixed condition (n = 437). We note that we focused our analysis on the fully between and

fully within design, and the analysis of the mixed-subject condition has been moved to the

supplementary materials 3. Materials and measures were exactly same to those used in Studies

1a/1b.

Results

Between-subject Design

We summarized descriptives in Table 7 and Figure 3.

values just below .05. We see the reporting of one-tail as reasonable given that this was a replication with clear
directional hypotheses.
3
The mixed condition was planned (based on the pre-registration) to mirror the original study’s mixed allocation
(i.e., self-other is between-subject, past-future is within-subject). The actual presentation of conditions during the
execution of the study made it impossible to conduct a straightforward test (i.e., ANOVA or t-tests) of the original
predictions. 437 of the 1295 participants in the sample were in the excluded condition. An additional analysis (i.e., a
mixed-effect regression analysis) based on the executed mixed-condition is reported in the supplementary materials.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 24

Table 7

Study 2 between design: Predictability descriptives

Target [between] Self Other


Time [between] n M SD n M SD
Past 106 3.61 0.86 108 3.78 0.81

Future 104 2.93 0.97 106 3.38 0.99

Overall 210 3.27 0.97 214 3.58 0.923


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 25

Figure 3

Study 2 between-subjects design: Self vs Other predictability

Note. Violin plot of the analysis of Self versus Other in Study between-design. Predictability: 1 = not predictable at all; 7 = extremely
predictable. Dependent variables across the panels: A) Past and Future predictability; B) Past predictability; C) Future predictability.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 26

We conducted an independent t-test and effects were in the expected direction.

Participants viewed their own decisions as being less predictable than the decisions of close

others (Self: n = 210, M = 3.27, SD = 0.97; other: n = 214, M = 3.58, SD = 0.92; t(422) = -3.38, p

< .001, one-tailed, d = -0.33, 95% CI [-0.52, -0.14]). Examining past and future separately, future

events for self (M = 2.93, SD = 0.96) were rated as less predictable than others’ (M = 3.38, SD =

0.99; t(208) = -3.33, p < .001, one-tailed, d = -0.46, 95% CI [-0.74, -0.18]). We did not find

support for past events, as self past events (M = 3.61, SD = 0.86) were rated similarly to others’

past events (M = 3.78, SD = 0.81; t(212) = -1.55, p = 0.061, one-tailed, d = -0.21, 95% CI [-0.48,

0.06]). Therefore, the effects were stronger for the future than for the past. Additionally, we

found support for a main effect for time, whereby people perceived the future as less predictable

than the past (M = 3.70 (SD =0.84) vs. 3.15 (1.00); t(422) = -6.04, p < 0.001, one-tailed, d = -

0.59, 95% CI [-0.78, -0.39]). The results of the 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) analysis

are reported in the supplementary section (see Table S7 and S8).

Within-subjects Design

We summarized the descriptives in Table 8 and Figure 4. We conducted paired t-tests as

part of the analyses. In joint evaluation mode, we found no support for participants viewing their

own decisions as being less predictable than the decisions of close others (Self: M = 3.44, SD =

0.96; other: M = 3.44, SD = 0.95; t(433) = -0.01, p = .490, one-tailed, d = -0.00, 95% CI [-0.13,

0.13]). Examining past and future separately, we found no support for differences between future

events for self (M = 3.22, SD = 0.99) and for others’ (M = 3.26, SD = 1.00; t(433) = -0.724, p =

.770, one-tailed, d = 0.04, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.17]). Similarly, we did not find support for past

events, as self past events (M = 3.66, SD = 0.88) were rated similarly to others’ past events (M =

3.62, SD = 0.85; t(433) = -0.821, p = 0.210, one-tailed, d = -0.05, 95% CI [-0.18, 0.09]).
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 27

However, we found support for effect for time, people perceived the future (M = 3.24, SD

=0.99) as less predictable than the past (M = 3.64, SD = 0.87; t(433) = -10.30, p < 0.001, one-

tailed, d = -0.53, 95% CI [-0.67, -0.39]). We summarized the results of the within-subjects

ANOVA analyses in the supplementary section (see Table S9 and S10).

Table 8

Study 2 within design: Predictability descriptives

Target [within] Self Other


Time [within] M SD M SD
Past 3.66 0.89 3.62 0.85
Future 3.22 0.99 3.26 1.00
Overall 3.44 0.96 3.44 0.95
Note. n = 434.
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 28

Figure 4

Study 2 within-subjects design: Self vs Other predictability

Note. Violin plot of the analysis of Self versus Other in Study 2 within-subject design.
Predictability: 1 = not predictable at all; 7 = extremely predictable.

Discussion

Study 2 involved a larger sample and modified experimental designs to test the

robustness of the findings of the Experiment of Pronin and Kugler (2010). The results based on

the between-subjects design supported the predictions of the original study. However, the results

based on the novel within-subjects and mixed designs failed to support and extend the original

findings, which can shed new light on potential mechanisms. Additionally, there was no support

for the moderation effect of achievement motivation as individual-level factor.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 29

Comparing Replication Findings to Original Findings and Extensions

We compiled a comparison between the effects of the original studies and our

replications in Table 9, along with an interpretation of the findings using the evaluation criteria

established by LeBel et al. (2019) for replication results.

The target study’s main prediction (H1) was that people view their own past and future

decision as less predictable a priori than those of others. Our direct replication samples in Studies

1a and 1b supported the prediction, though with weaker effects and just barely below threshold

in Study 1b. We observed similar patterns for H2a and H2b, yet slightly stronger support for the

hypothesis for future than for the past. Also, we found evidence supporting the prediction (H3)

that individuals perceive the future as less predictable than the past.

In Study 2, we juxtaposed two supplementary designs within a single data collection:

fully between-subject, and fully within-subject. The main effect (H1) replicated in the between-

subject condition, whereas we found no support for the effect in a new within-subject condition.

We also conducted the mini-meta-analyses with a random-effects model method using

the meta package in R (Schwarzer, 2007; see Figure S9-S12 for detailed forest plots in the

Supplementary Materials section). The analysis included effects from Study 1a, Study 1b, and

Study 2. Among effect sizes from Study 2 we only considered effects from between-subject

condition. This analysis provided support for the main prediction (H1) that people view their

own past and future life decisions as less predictable a priori than those of a roommate (d = -0.36

[-0.52, -0.20]).
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 30

Table 9

Summary of replication results comparison with the original effect sizes

Sample Predictor Replication Results Original study Replication Interpretation


Study 1a H1: General (Self vs Other) F(1, 45) = 8.33, p = 0.006 -1.08 [-1.67, -0.48] -0.84 [-1.46, -0.23] Signal- consistent
H2a: Past outcomes (Self vs. Other) t(45) = -1.36, p = 0.181 -0.72 [-1.29, -0.14] -0.40 [-0.99, 0.20] No signal- inconsistent
H2b: Future outcomes (Self vs. Other) t(45) = -2.72, p = 0.009 -0.82 [-1.40, -0.25] -0.79 [-1.41, -0.18] Signal- consistent
H3: Future vs Past F(1, 45) = 16.26, p < 0.001 -0.94 [-1.52, -0.35] -0.77 [-1.20, -0.35] Signal- consistent
H1: General (Self vs Other) F(1, 124) = 2.87, p = 0.093 -1.08 [-1.67, -0.48] -0.30 [-0.65, 0.03] No signal- inconsistent
Study 1b H2a: Past outcomes (Self vs Other) t(124) = -1.29, p = 0.198 -0.72 [-1.29, -0.14] -0.23 [-0.58, 0.12] No signal- inconsistent
H2b: Future outcomes (Self vs Other) t(124) = -1.63, p = .106 -0.82 [-1.40, -0.25] -0.29 [-0.64, 0.06] Signal- inconsistent
H3: Future vs Past F (1, 124) = 26.22, p < 0.001 -0.94 [-1.52, -0.35] -0.45 [-0.70, -0.20] Signal- inconsistent (smaller)
Study 2 (between H1: General (Self vs Other) F(1, 420) = 12.59, p < .001 -1.08 [-1.67, -0.48] -0.33 [-0.52, -0.14] Signal- inconsistent (smaller)
condition) H2a: Past outcomes (Self vs Other) t(420) = -1.42, p = .156 -0.72 [-1.29, -0.14] -0.21 [-0.48, 0.06] No signal- inconsistent
H2b: Future outcomes (Self vs Other) t(420) = -3.58, p < .001 -0.82 [-1.40, -0.25] -0.46 [-0.74, -0.18] Signal- inconsistent (smaller)
H3: Future vs Past F(1, 420) = 37.74, p < .001 -0.94 [-1.52, -0.35] -0.59 [-0.78, -0.39] Signal- inconsistent (smaller)
Study 2 (within H1: General (Self vs Other) F(1, 433) = 0.00, p = .988 -1.08 [-1.67, -0.48] 0.00 [-0.13, 0.13] No signal- inconsistent
condition) H2a: Past outcomes (Self vs Other) t(433) = 0.04, p = 0.411 -0.72 [-1.29, -0.14] -0.04 [-0.18, 0.08] No signal- inconsistent
H2b: Future outcomes (Self vs Other) t(433) = -0.04, p = 0.469 -0.82 [-1.40, -0.25] 0.04 [-0.09, 0.17] No signal- inconsistent
H3: Future vs Past F(1, 433) = 105.23, p < .001 -0.94 [-1.52, -0.35] -0.53 [-0.66, -0.39] Signal- inconsistent (smaller)
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 31

General Discussion

We aimed to conduct close direct replications of Experiment 1 in Pronin and Kugler

(2010), finding that people believe that their own decisions and life events are less predictable a

priori than similar decisions and life events for other people. We also went beyond the original

findings by adding extensions examining different study designs.

Across three replications we concluded a mostly successful replication of the self-other

asymmetry in predictability ratings, reported in Pronin and Kugler (2010)’s Experiment 1:

People rate themselves as being less predictable than others. The effect sizes were considerably

smaller than that reported in the original study. We found much weaker to no support for the

effect when we ran a novel within-subject design. This suggests that the tendency to believe that

oneself is less predictable than other people only emerged in separate evaluation mode, in which

the participant makes no direct comparison between herself and others. When the response mode

shifts from separate to joint evaluation, people can directly compare their self and other

judgments, and then seem to correct this bias in their judgments.

Our replications differed from the original study in some expected ways. For example,

stimuli used in the original article were targeted at and tested with American undergraduates

more than a decade ago. We used the same materials in an online survey with a more diverse

population in Studies 1b and 2. Direct close replications are never perfectly exact, yet despite the

differences, we believe that the theoretical grounds of the target paper remain relevant and that

the replications indicate a qualified support for the target article’s primary hypothesis.

It might be possible to make the link between the current findings and the phenomenon

that people view their own judgments as less biased than others’. For example, Pronin et al.

(2002) demonstrated a “bias blind-spot” – that people tend to perceive themselves as less
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 32

susceptible to biases than others. A recent replication based on a larger and more diverse sample

supported these findings, also showing links between free-will agency beliefs and the bias blind-

spot (Chandrashekar et al., 2021). An interesting direction for future research, would be to

examine whether given the self-other asymmetries regarding the bias blind-spot and agency, and

given the links between the agency beliefs and the bias blind-spot, that there might be a common

mechanism underlying these types of effects.

Our Study 2 also offers an important insight given the comparison of the between-subject

and within-subject design. We found support for an effect in a between-subject design but no

support for the effect using the within-subject design. This experimental contrast has been

commonly used in the judgment and decision-making literature in order to demonstrate that

when comparisons are possible people realize the bias and correct it. For example, Hsee (1998)

provided several curious demonstrations of the “Less Is Better” effect when people evaluate

objects or situations in isolation (separately, in a between-subject design) versus the more

rational “More Is Better” effect when people evaluate those in joint evaluations. His findings

were recently successfully replicated by Vonasch et al. (2023). Therefore, it would seem that in

our current replication, like in Hsee (1998) and Vonasch et al. (2023), the effect seems to appear

when evaluations are done in isolation with no reference point for comparison, yet seems to

dissolve when making joint evaluations.

Perhaps one way to tease apart the variation in the findings across between-persons vs.

within-person comparisons is to investigate the normative understanding of free will, in its

different descriptions. A valuable avenue for future research could involve investigating whether

individuals tend to favor or trust others who exhibit less predictability, and under what
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 33

circumstances. Likewise, an intriguing line of inquiry would be to examine whether individuals

express a preference for or trust those who demonstrate a heightened sense of agency.

In replication Study 1a that closely matched the target study, we found strong support for

predictions of the original study (Cohen d = -0.84, 95% CI [-1.46, -0.23]) based on a smaller

student sample (N = 47). In Study 1b with a larger MTurk sample (N = 126) the replication effect

size seemed smaller and just barely detectable signal (Cohen d = -0.30, 95% CI [-0.66, 0.05]),

whereas in Study 2, which had the largest sample, the effect with a between-subject design was

again smaller than the original but this time highly robust, like Study 1a (insert d, CI here). Seen

as a whole, we therefore consider this a mostly successful replication of the main finding in

Pronin and Kugler: Experiment 1 (2010), although the observed effect sizes were smaller. For

future studies aiming to replicate and extend these findings further, for example by looking into

underlying mechanisms and testing potential moderators, we would recommend recruiting a

much larger sample size.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 34

References

Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability

of trait adjectives. Journal of personality and social psychology, 49(6), 1621.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.6.1621

Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., & Vredenburg, D. S. (1995).

Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 68(5), 804–825. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804

Alquist, J. L., Ainsworth, S. E., Baumeister, R. F., Daly, M., & Stillman, T. F. (2015). The

making of might-have-beens: Effects of free will belief on counterfactual

thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(2), 268-283.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214563673

Caruso, E. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). A wrinkle in time: Asymmetric valuation

of past and future events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 796-801.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02159.x

Chandrashekar, S. P., Yeung, S. K., Yau, K. C., Cheung, C. Y., Agarwal, T. K., Wong, C. Y. J.,

Pillai, T., Thirlwell, T. N., Leung, W. N., Tse, C., Li, Y. T., Cheng, B. L., Chan, H. Y. C.

& Feldman, G. (2021). Agency and self-other asymmetries in perceived bias and

shortcomings: Replications of the Bias Blind Spot and link to free will beliefs. Judgment

& Decision Making, 16(6). https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500008470

Chartier, C. R., Arnal, J. D., Arrow, H., Bloxsom, N. G., Bonfiglio, D. B. V., Brumbaugh, C. C.,

Corker, K. S., Ebersole, C. R., Garinther, A., Giessner, S. R., Hughes, S., Inzlicht, M.,

Lin, H., Mercier, B., Metzger, M., Rangel, D., Saunders, B., Schmidt, K., Storage, D., &

Tocco, C. (2020). Many Labs 5: Registered Replication of Albarracín et al.(2008),


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 35

Experiment 5. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(3), 332-

339. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245920945963

Crescioni, A. W., Baumeister, R. F., Ainsworth, S. E., Ent, M., & Lambert, N. M. (2016).

Subjective correlates and consequences of belief in free will. Philosophical

Psychology, 29(1), 41-63. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.996285

Dennett, D. C., & Caruso, G. D. (2021). Just Deserts: Debating Free Will. John Wiley & Sons.

Everett, J. A. C., Clark, C. J., Meindl, P., Luguri, J. B., Earp, B. D., Graham, J., Ditto, P. H., &

Shariff, A. F. (2021). Political differences in free will belief are associated with

differences in moralization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(2), 461–

483. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000286

Feldman, G., Baumeister, R. F., & Wong, K. F. E. (2014). Free will is about choosing: The link

between choice and the belief in free will. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 55, 239-245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.07.012

Feldman, G., Farh, J. L., & Wong, K. F. E. (2018). Agency beliefs over time and across cultures:

Free will beliefs predict higher job satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 44(3), 304-317. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217739261

Feldman, G., Wong, K. F. E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Bad is freer than good: Positive–

negative asymmetry in attributions of free will. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 26-40.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.005

Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of

morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101-124.

https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 36

Hsee, C. K. (1998). Less is better: when low-value options are valued more highly than high-

value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(2), 107–121.

https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199806)11:2%3C107::AID-

BDM292%3E3.0.CO;2-Y

Hsee, C. K., Loewenstein, G. F., Blount, S., & Bazerman, M. H. (1999). Preference reversals

between joint and separate evaluations of options: a review and theoretical

analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(5), 576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-

2909.125.5.576

Jané, M., Xiao, Q., Yeung, S., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Caldwell, A., Cousineau, D., Dunleavy, D. J.,

Elsherif, M., Johnson, B., Moreau, D., Riesthuis, P., Röseler, L., Steele, J., Vieira, F.,

Zloteanu, M., & Feldman, G. (2024). Guide to Effect Sizes and Confidence Intervals.

http://dx.doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/D8C4G

Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1971). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the

causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones et al. (Eds), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of

behavior. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press.

Kahneman, D., & Ritov, I. (1994). Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A

study in the headline method. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 9(1), 5-37.

https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073401

Korbmacher, M., Kwan, C., & Feldman, G. (2022). Both better and worse than others depending

on difficulty: Replication and extensions of Kruger’s (1999) above and below average

effects. Judgment and Decision Making, 17(2), 449–486.

https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500009189
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 37

Koppel, L., Andersson, D., Tinghög, G., Västfjäll, D., & Feldman, G. (2023). We are all less

risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers: Successful replication and extension of

Svenson (1981)‎. Meta-Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.15626/MP.2021.2932

Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The" below-average effect" and the egocentric nature

of comparative ability judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2),

221. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.221

Lang, J. W., & Fries, S. (2006). A revised 10-item version of the Achievement Motives

Scale. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22(3), 216-224.

https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.22.3.216

LeBel, E. P., McCarthy, R. J., Earp, B. D., Elson, M., & Vanpaemel, W. (2018). A unified

framework to quantify the credibility of scientific findings. Advances in Methods and

Practices in Psychological Science, 1(3), 389-402.

https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918787489

LeBel, E. P., Vanpaemel, W., Cheung, I., & Campbell, L. (2019). A brief guide to evaluate

replications. Meta-Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.15626/MP.2018.843

Litman, L., Robinson, J., & Abberbock, T. (2017). TurkPrime. com: A versatile crowdsourcing

data acquisition platform for the behavioral sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 49(2),

433-442. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0727-z

Martin, N. D., Rigoni, D., & Vohs, K. D. (2017). Free will beliefs predict attitudes toward

unethical behavior and criminal punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 114(28), 7325-7330.


Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 38

Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2010). From uncaused will to conscious choice: The need to

study, not speculate about people’s folk concept of free will. Review of Philosophy and

Psychology, 1(2), 211-224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-009-0010-7

Monroe, A. E., Dillon, K. D., & Malle, B. F. (2014). Bringing free will down to Earth: People’s

psychological concept of free will and its role in moral judgment. Consciousness and

Cognition, 27, 100-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.011

Nadelhoffer, T., Shepard, J., Crone, D. L., Everett, J. A., Earp, B. D., & Levy, N. (2020). Does

encouraging a belief in determinism increase cheating? Reconsidering the value of

believing in free will. Cognition, 203, 104342.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104342

Nahmias, E., Morris, S., Nadelhoffer, T., & Turner, J. (2005). Surveying freedom: Folk

intuitions about free will and moral responsibility. Philosophical Psychology, 18(5), 561-

584. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080500264180

Nichols, S. (2004). The folk psychology of free will: Fits and starts. Mind & Language, 19(5),

473-502. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0268-1064.2004.00269.x

Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2010). People believe they have more free will than

others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(52), 22469-22474.

https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1012046108

Pronin, E., Lin, D. Y., & Ross, L. (2002). The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus

others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3), 369-381.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286008
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 39

Rigoni, D., Cleeremans, A., & Brass, M. (2017). Causes and consequences of the belief in free

will. In The science of lay theories (pp. 229-242). Springer, Cham.

https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-57306-9_10

Sarkissian, H., Chatterjee, A., De Brigard, F., Knobe, J., Nichols, S., & Sirker, S. (2010). Is

belief in free will a cultural universal?. Mind & Language, 25(3), 346-358.

https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01393.x

Schwarzer, G. (2007). meta: An R package for meta-analysis. R news, 7(3), 40-45.

Shariff, A. F., Greene, J. D., Karremans, J. C., Luguri, J. B., Clark, C. J., Schooler, J. W.,

Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2014). Free will and punishment: A mechanistic view

of human nature reduces retribution. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1563–

1570. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614534693

Shepperd, J. A., Waters, E. A., Weinstein, N. D., & Klein, W. M. (2015). A primer on unrealistic

optimism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(3), 232-237.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414568341

Sjåstad, H., Baumeister, R. F., & Ent, M. (2020). Greener grass or sour grapes? How people

value future goals after initial failure. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 88,

103965. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103965

Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., & Mele, A. R. (2011). Free will in everyday life:

Autobiographical accounts of free and unfree actions. Philosophical Psychology, 24(3),

381-394. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2011.556607

Stillman, T. F., Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Lambert, N. M., Fincham, F. D., & Brewer, L. E.

(2010). Personal philosophy and personnel achievement: Belief in free will predicts better
Pronin and Kugler (2010): Replications and extensions 40

job performance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(1), 43-50.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550609351600

Svenson, O. (1981). Are we all less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers? Acta

Psychologica, 47(2), 143–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(81)90005-6

Vonasch, A. J., Hung, W. Y., Leung, W. Y., Nguyen, A. T. B., Chan, S., Cheng, B. L., &

Feldman, G. (2023). ‎“Less Is Better” in Separate Evaluations Versus “More Is Better” in

Joint Evaluations: Mostly ‎Successful Close Replication and Extension of Hsee (1998)‎.

Collabra: Psychology, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.77859

Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of personality

and social psychology, 39(5), 806. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.806

Ziano, I., Mok, P. Y., & Feldman, G. (2021). Replication and Extension of Alicke (1985) Better-

Than-Average Effect for Desirable and Controllable Traits. Social Psychological and

Personality Science, 12(6), 1005-1017. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550620948973


Do people believe they are less predictable than others? Three
replications of Pronin and Kugler (2010)’s Experiment 1
Supplementary

Disclosures .............................................................................................................................................. 2
Data collection .................................................................................................................................... 2
Conditions reporting ........................................................................................................................... 2
Variables reporting ............................................................................................................................. 2
Overview of Experimental Design in the Target article ......................................................................... 3
Experimental design........................................................................................................................ 3
Experimental material ..................................................................................................................... 3
Original article’s results .................................................................................................................. 4
Effect size calculations of the target article effects ................................................................................. 4
Power analyses ........................................................................................................................................ 5
Materials and scales related to replication part ....................................................................................... 8
Study Materials: Study 1a & 1b .......................................................................................................... 8
Study Materials: Study 2................................................................................................................... 10
Deviations from Preregistration ............................................................................................................ 13
Figures .............................................................................................................................................. 14
Additional results .................................................................................................................................. 16
Study 1a ............................................................................................................................................ 16
Study 1b ............................................................................................................................................ 16
Study 2 .............................................................................................................................................. 17
Study 2: Mixed design: Past versus future separately for self and for other ................................. 18
Study 2 Extension: Achievement Motives ..................................................................................... 21
Introduction ................................................................................................................................... 21
Results............................................................................................................................................... 21
Summary and Mini-meta analysis .................................................................................................... 23
Mini-meta-analyses including the original study .......................................................................... 23
Mini-meta analyses excluding the original study.......................................................................... 25
Appendix A .......................................................................................................................................... 27
References ............................................................................................................................................ 28
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 2

Disclosures

Data collection
Data collection was completed before analyzing the data.
Conditions reporting
We report all the conditions we collected.
Variables reporting
All variables collected for this study are reported and included in the provided data.
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 3

Overview of Experimental Design in the Target article


Experimental design
Original study authors ran 2(between) x 2 (within) mixed-design ANOVA comparing self-
other difference and past- future difference in the predictability of personal action.
Experimental material
Other/close friend
Please think about one of your roommates (or suitemates) at Princeton. Pick one person
only, and indicate his/her initials here: ____. If you do not have a roommate or suitemate,
choose one you previously had, and indicate his/her initials here: ____.

Looking back…
1. Think about your roommate’s decision to attend Princeton. How easy would it
have been to predict that he/she would end up going to Princeton? (Not at all
predictable = 1; Extremely predictable = 7)
2. Think about a person whom your roommate recently dated (but is not dating any
longer). How easy would it have been to predict that things would not last
between the two of them? (Not at all predictable = 1; Extremely predictable = 7)
3. Think about your roommate’s choice of what to major in. How easy would it have
been to predict that he/she would end up choosing that major? (Not at all
predictable = 1; Extremely predictable = 7)
Looking forward…
1. Think about what your roommate will end up doing for his/her career. How easy
would it be to predict what his/her career will be? (Not at all predictable = 1;
Extremely predictable = 7)
2. Think about the person whom your roommate will marry. How easy would it be to
predict who this person will be? (Not at all predictable = 1; Extremely predictable = 7)
3. Think about your roommate’s decision about where in the country he/she will live
during the decade after he/she graduates. How easy would it be to predict where
he/she will live? (Not at all predictable = 1; Extremely predictable = 7)
Self
Looking back…
1. Think about your decision to attend Princeton. How easy would it have been to
predict that you would end up going to Princeton?
2. Think about a person whom you recently dated (but are not dating any longer).
How easy would it have been to predict that things would not last between the two
of you?
3. Think about your choice of what to major in. How easy would it have been to
predict that you would end up choosing that major?

Looking forward…
1. Think about what you will end up doing for your career. How easy would it be to
predict what your career will be?
2. Think about the person whom you will marry. How easy would it be to predict
who this person will be?
3. Think about your decision about where in the country you will live during the
decade after you graduate. How easy would it be to predict where you will live?
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 4

Original article’s results


We calculated the effect sizes based on the results of the target study. See Appendix A for
sample calculation of Cohen’s d (95% CI intervals).
Prediction 1: Participant perceived their own outcome as less predictable a priori than those
of a roommate (the main effect of a priori predictability of self and others’ past and future
decision)
- Mean = 3.86(self) vs. 4.81(other), (on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all
predictable to 7 = extremely predictable);
- Standard Deviation = Not Reported
- The Statiscal Test: two-way ANOVA: F(1, 48) = 14.46
- The reported degree of freedom = 48
- The reported p-value = 0.0004.
- Cohen’s d = -1.08 [-1.68, -0.47]

Prediction 2: The simple effect for self-other difference for past outcomes
- Mean = 4.32(self) vs. 5.16(other), (on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all
predictable to 7 = extremely predictable)
- Standard Deviation = Not Reported
- The Statistical Test: two-way ANOVA: F(1, 48) = 6.40
- The reported degree of freedom = 48
- The reported p-value = 0.01
- Cohen’s d = -0.72 [-1.30, -0.13]

Prediction 3: The simple effect for self-other difference for future outcomes
- Mean = 3.40(self) vs. 4.47(other), (on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all
predictable to 7 = extremely predictable)
- Standard Deviation = Not Reported
- The Statistical Test: two-way ANOVA: F(1, 48) = 8.46
- The reported degree of freedom = 48
- The reported p-value = 0.005
- Cohen’s d = -0.82 [-1.41, -0.23]

Prediction 3: The differences for future vs past outcome predictabilities


- Mean = 3.93(self) vs. 4.74(other), (on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all
predictable to 7 = extremely predictable)
- Standard Deviation = Not Reported
- The Statistical Test: two-way ANOVA: F(1, 48) = 10.94
- The reported degree of freedom = 48
- The reported p-value = 0.002
- Cohen’s d = -0.94 [-1.53, -0.34]
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 5

Effect size calculations of the target article effects

Effect sizes were not reported in the target article. We thus computed these whenever
possible (when there was sufficient information).
Table S1. Summary of target findings in Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1
Cohen's d with 95% Partial eta-square
df 1 df 2 F p
CI with 95% CI
(H1) Across both past and future
outcomes: 1 48 14.46 0.0004 -1.08 [-1.68, -0.47] 0.23 [0.08, 1.00]
Self vs. Roommate
(H2a) Across both past outcomes:
1 48 6.40 0.010 -0.72 [-1.30, -0.13] 0.12 [0.01, 1.00]
Self vs. Roommate
(H2b) Across both future
outcomes: 1 48 8.46 0.005 -0.82 [-1.41, -0.23] 0.15 [0.03, 1.00]
Self vs. Roommate

(H3) Future vs. Past 1 48 10.94 0.002 -0.94 [-1.53, -0.34] 0.08 [0.00, 1.00]

Power analyses

Steps for power analysis (Pronin & Kugler, 2010):


These are the results reported in the target study:
Participants perceived their own outcomes as less predictable a priori than those of a
roommate (M = 3.86 vs. 4.81, on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 = not at all predictable to 7
= extremely predictable); F (1, 48) = 14.46, P = 0.0004. This self–other difference was
significant for both past outcomes (M = 4.32 vs. 5.16), F (1, 48) = 6.40, P = 0.01 and future
outcomes (M = 3.40 vs. 4.47), F (1, 48) = 8.46, P = 0.005. Consistent with previous
theorizing (1), there also was a main effect for time, whereby people perceived the future as
less predictable than the past (M = 3.93 vs. 4.74); F (1, 48) = 10.94, P = 0.002. There was no
interaction between past/future and self/other (F < 1).

Here is a summarised form of the statistics of the results:


● General: Self vs other (M = 3.86 vs 4.81, SD not provided), F (1, 48) = 14.46, p = 0.0004
● Past outcomes: Self vs other (M = 4.32 vs 5.16, SD not provided), F (1, 48) = 6.40, p = 0.01
● Future outcomes: Self vs Other (M = 3.40 vs 4.47, SD not provided), F (1, 48) = 8.46, p =
0.005
● Future vs Past (M = 3.93 vs 4.74, SD not provided), F (1, 48) = 10.94, p = 0.002

Standard deviations were not provided so we took the F statistic and the overall number of subjects to
extract the effect size in Cohen’s d. As the target study did not state how were the subjects divided
into the two conditions, we assumed that the total number of subjects was divided equally into the
conditions. We used the effect size calculator on http://www.uv.es/~friasnav/TEdatospublicados.xls -
‘How to Calculate Effect Sizes from Published Research: A Simplified Spreadsheet’, by Will
Thalheimer and Samantha Cook.
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 6

For the general main effect (self vs other), the F statistic = 14.46, and the overall number of subjects
was 50 with 25 in each condition, we calculated a Cohen’s d of 1.1, a very large effect.

For the effect on past outcomes (self vs other), the F statistic = 6.40, and the overall number of
subjects was 50 with 25 in each condition, we calculated a Cohen’s d of .73, a medium effect.

For the effect on future outcomes (self vs other), the F statistic = 8.46, and the overall number of
subjects was 50 with 25 in each condition, we calculated a Cohen’s d of .84, a large effect.
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 7

For the effect of time (past vs future), the F statistic = 10.94, and the overall number of subjects was
50 with 25 in each condition, we calculated a Cohen’s d of .95, a large effect.

1. We calculated the effect size (Cohen’s d).


2. Used the Cohen’s d for the power analysis with GPower (Faul et al., 2007).

Gpower captures

G*Power output
Power analysis Analysis: A priori: Compute required sample size
output for Study Input: Tail(s) = Two
1a and Study 1b Effect size d = 1.1
α err prob = 0.05
Power (1-β err prob) = 0.95
Allocation ratio N2/N1 = 1
Output: Noncentrality parameter δ = 3.7302815
Critical t = 2.0153676
Df = 44
Sample size group 1 = 23
Sample size group 2 = 23
Total sample size = 46
Actual power = 0.9542682

Considering the effect size of the general main effect which is the most
crucial to the experiment, d = 1.1. Hence, a sample size of 23
participants in each condition is required for two-tail power .95 alpha
.05. Therefore, a sample of 46 participants is required.

Cohen’s d = 0.32 based on the results of the Study 1b


Power analysis
G*Power output
output for Study
2 t tests - Means: Difference between two independent means (two
groups)
Analysis: A priori: Compute required sample size
Input: Tail(s) = One
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 8

Effect size d = 0.32


α err prob = 0.05
Power (1-β err prob) = 0.95
Allocation ratio N2/N1 = 1
Output: Noncentrality parameter δ = 3.3023628
Critical t = 1.6484553
Df = 424
Sample size group 1 = 213
Sample size group 2 = 213
Total sample size = 426
Actual power = 0.9507565

Materials and scales related to replication part

Study Materials: Study 1a & 1b


OSF Link to the Qualtrics survey file (in word doc format):
Study 1a: https://osf.io/qmhy
Study 1b: https://osf.io/n6gsy

Introduction to evaluations of others


First, please think about your closest friend or your roommate at your university.
Pick one person only and indicate his/her first name initials here (do NOT write full name or
any identifying details, to ensure full anonymity):
Past events in others' lives
Read the following statements about certain past events. Think about how predictable those
events were in your friend/roommate's life and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all
predictable) to 7 (extremely predictable).
1. Think about your friend/roommate’s decision to attend your university. How easy would
it have been to predict that he/she would end up going to this university? (1= Not at all
predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
2. Think about a person whom your friend/roommate recently dated (but is not dating any
longer). How easy would it have been to predict that things would not last between the
two of them? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 9

3. Think about your friend/roommate's choice of what to major in. How easy would it have
been to predict that he/she would end up choosing that major? (1= Not at all predictable;
7 = Extremely predictable)

Future events in others' lives


Read the following statements about certain future events. Think about how predictable those
events are in your friend/roommate's life and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all
predictable) to 7 (extremely predictable).
1. Think about what your friend/roommate will end up doing for his/her career. How easy
would it be to predict what his/her career would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 =
Extremely predictable)
2. Think about the person whom your friend/roommate will marry. How easy would it be to
predict who this person would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
3. Think about your friend/roommate's decision about where he/she will live during the
decade after he/she graduates. How easy would it be to predict where he/she would live?
(1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)

Introduction to evaluations of self


In this section you're asked to evaluate events in your own life.
Read the following statements about past events in your life. Think about how predictable
those events are and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all predictable) to 7 (extremely
predictable).
1. Think about your decision to attend your current university. How easy would it have been
to predict that you would end up going to this university? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 =
Extremely predictable)
2. Think about a person whom you recently dated (but are not dating any longer). How easy
would it have been to predict that things would not last between the two of you? (1= Not
at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
3. Think about your choice of what to major in. How easy would it have been to predict that
you would end up choosing that major? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely
predictable)

Future events in your own life


Read the following statements about future events in your life. Think about how predictable
those events are and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all predictable) to 7 (extremely
predictable).
1. Think about what you will end up doing for your career. How easy would it be to predict
what your career would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
2. Think about the person whom you will marry. How easy would it be to predict who this
person would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 10

3. Think about your decision about where you will live during the decade after you
graduate. How easy would it be to predict where you would live? (1= Not at all
predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)

Study Materials: Study 2

OSF Link to the Qualtrics survey file (in word doc format):
Study 2: https://osf.io/vx4pa
Introduction: Students only
This experiment is only for current American undergraduate students.

This experiment has 6 questions presented in one page asking for you evaluations of
behaviors. There are no right or wrong answers, answer to the best of your understanding.
Are you currently an American undergraduate student?
 Yes, I am currently an undergraduate student (1)
 No, I am not an undergraduate student (2)
What year of your undergraduate studies are you currently at?
 First year (1)
 Second year (2)
 Third year (3)
 Forth year+ (4)
 Not currently enrolled (5)
Achievement motivations

The following questions are meant to understand your motivations for achievement.
Please indicate your agreement with each of the following statements: (1= Strongly
disagree; 4 = Strongly agree)
1. I like situations, in which I can find out how capable I am.
2. When I am confronted with a problem, which I can possibly solve, I am enticed to start
working onit immediately.
3. I enjoy situations, in which I can make use of my abilities.
4. I am appealed by situations allowing me to test my abilities.
5. I am attracted by tasks, in which I can test my abilities.
6. I am afraid of failing in somewhat difficult situations, when a lot depends on me.
7. I feel uneasy to do something if I am not sure of succeeding.
8. Even if nobody would notice my failure, I’m afraid of tasks, which I’m not able to solve.
9. Even if nobody is watching, I feel quite anxious in new situations.
10. If I do not understand a problem immediately I start feeling anxious.
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 11

Introduction to evaluations of others


First, please think about your closest friend or your roommate at your university.
Pick one person only and indicate his/her first name initials here (do NOT write full name or
any identifying details, to ensure full anonymity):
Past events in others' lives
Read the following statements about certain past events. Think about how predictable those
events were in your friend/roommate's life and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all
predictable) to 7 (extremely predictable).
4. Think about your friend/roommate’s decision to attend your university. How easy would
it have been to predict that he/she would end up going to this university? (1= Not at all
predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
5. Think about a person whom your friend/roommate recently dated (but is not dating any
longer). How easy would it have been to predict that things would not last between the
two of them? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
6. Think about your friend/roommate's choice of what to major in. How easy would it have
been to predict that he/she would end up choosing that major? (1= Not at all predictable;
7 = Extremely predictable)

Future events in others' lives


Read the following statements about certain future events. Think about how predictable those
events are in your friend/roommate's life and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all
predictable) to 7 (extremely predictable).
4. Think about what your friend/roommate will end up doing for his/her career. How easy
would it be to predict what his/her career would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 =
Extremely predictable)
5. Think about the person whom your friend/roommate will marry. How easy would it be to
predict who this person would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
6. Think about your friend/roommate's decision about where he/she will live during the
decade after he/she graduates. How easy would it be to predict where he/she would live?
(1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)

Introduction to evaluations of self


In this section you're asked to evaluate events in your own life.
Read the following statements about past events in your life. Think about how predictable
those events are and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all predictable) to 7 (extremely
predictable).
4. Think about your decision to attend your current university. How easy would it have been
to predict that you would end up going to this university? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 =
Extremely predictable)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 12

5. Think about a person whom you recently dated (but are not dating any longer). How easy
would it have been to predict that things would not last between the two of you? (1= Not
at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
6. Think about your choice of what to major in. How easy would it have been to predict that
you would end up choosing that major? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely
predictable)

Future events in your own life


Read the following statements about future events in your life. Think about how predictable
those events are and rate them on a scale from 1 (not at all predictable) to 7 (extremely
predictable).
4. Think about what you will end up doing for your career. How easy would it be to predict
what your career would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
5. Think about the person whom you will marry. How easy would it be to predict who this
person would be? (1= Not at all predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
6. Think about your decision about where you will live during the decade after you
graduate. How easy would it be to predict where you would live? (1= Not at all
predictable; 7 = Extremely predictable)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 13

Deviations from Preregistration


Table S2. Pre-registration plan versus final report

Components in Location of Deviatio Description of deviation Rationale Impact of deviation Date/time of decision for
preregistration preregistered ns for on results deviation + stage
decision/plan deviation

Study design Study 1a: no / / / /


https://osf.io/j5ms4
Measured variables no / / / /
Study 1b:
Exclusion criteria https://osf.io/xwhu3 no / / / /
https://osf.io/tfwrg
IV no / / / /

DV Study 2: no / / / /
https://osf.io/7ucjv
Data analysis Yes We do not deviate in our partial eta- none June 2023
analysis but report the squared is
effect using a partial eta- more apt
squared effect size effect size
measure. Power analysis measure
was based on cohen’d for anova
effect size measure. analysis
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 14

Additional results

Study 1a

Table S3. Study 1a: Results of mixed ANOVA

p-
Effect df MSE F 𝜼𝟐𝒑 (95% CI)
value
Self vs. Other 1, 45 1.42 8.33 .006 0.16 [0.03, 1.00]
Future vs. Past 1, 45 1.32 16.26 <.001 0.27 [0.10, 1.00]
(Self vs. Other) x (Future
1, 45 1.32 1.35 .251 0.03 [0.00, 1.00]
vs. Past)

Table S4. Study 1a: Results of post hoc comparisons based on mixed ANOVA

Contrast estimate SE df T p-value Cohen’s d, 95% CI


Future
Self - Other -0.99 0.36 45 -2.72 0.0092 -0.79 [-1.40, -0.18]
Past
Self - Other -0.43 0.32 45 -1.36 0.1817 -0.40 [-0.99, -0.20]

Study 1b
Table S5. Study 1b: Results of mixed ANOVA

p-
Effect df MSE F 𝜼𝟐𝒑 with (95% CI)
value
Self vs. Other 1, 124 2.25 2.87 .093 0.02 [0.00, 1.00]
Past v. Future 1, 124 0.74 26.22 <.001 0.17 [0.08, 1.00]
(Self vs. Other) x (Past vs.
1, 124 0.74 0.42 .516 0.00 [0.00, 1.00]
Future)

Table S6. Study 1b: Results of post hoc comparisons based on mixed ANOVA
Contrast estimate SE df T p-value Cohen’s d, 95% CI
Future
Self - Other -0.39 0.24 124 -1.626 0.106 -0.29 [-0.64, -0.06]
Past
Self - Other -0.25 0.19 124 -1.29 0.198 -0.23 [-0.58, -0.12]
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 15

Study 2
Table S7. Study 2: Results of ANOVA based on responses from between-subjects design

p-
Effect df MSE F 𝜼𝟐𝒑 with (95% CI)
value
Self vs. Other 1, 420 0.82 12.59 <.001 0.03 [0.01, 1.00]
Past v. Future 1, 420 0.82 37.74 <.001 0.08 [0.05, 1.00]
(Self vs. Other) x (Past vs.
1, 420 0.82 2.40 .122 0.00 [0.00, 1.00]
Future)

Table S8. Study 2: Results of post hoc comparisons based on ANOVA from between-subjects
design

Contrast estimate SE df T p-value Cohen’s d, 95% CI


Future
Self - Other -0.45 0.12 420 -3.59 <.001 -0.46 [-0.74, -0.18]
Past
Self - Other -0.18 0.12 420 -1.42 0.156 -0.21 [-0.48, -0.06]

Table S9. Study 2: Results of ANOVA based on responses from within-subjects design
Effect df MSE F p-value 𝜼𝟐𝒑 with (95% CI)

Self vs. Other 1, 433 0.66 0.00 .988 0.00 [0.00, 1.00]

Past v. Future 1, 433 0.68 105.23 <.001 0.20 [0.14, 1.00]

(Self vs. Other) x (Past vs.


1, 433 0.47 1.43 .233 0.00 [0.00, 1.00]
Future)

Table S10. Study 2: Results of post hoc comparisons based on ANOVA from within-subjects
design
Contrast estimate SE df T p-value Cohen’s d, 95% CI

Future

Self - Other 0.04 0.04 433 0.82 .412 0.05 [-0.09, 0.18]

Past

Self - Other -0.04 0.05 433 -0.72 .469 -0.04 [-0.17, -0.09]
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 16

Study 2: Mixed design: Past versus future separately for self and for other

We also added a condition with a design in which participants rated the predictability

of past or future events across each of the self and other’s reference point. The participants in

the mixed condition were assigned to one of the four scenario combinations: 1) Self-past and

Other-past; 2) Self-past and Other-future; 3) Self-future and Other-past; 4) Self-future and

Other-future. The order of the presentations within the combinations was randomized. We

summarized descriptive in Table 5 and Figure 5. The means were very similar to that of the

within-subject condition, with no differences between self and other.

Table S11. Study 2 mix design: Predictability descriptives


Target [Between] Self Other
Time [within] M SD M SD
Past 3.78 0.75 3.68 0.77
Future 3.22 0.94 3.22 0.95
Note. N = 437.

Figure S4. Study 2 mix design: Self-other predictability separately for past and future
choices
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 17

Given the nature of mixed experimental design, we conducted a mixed effects

regression analysis. We regressed predictability ratings on target frame (self vs. other), time

factor (past vs future), and interaction between reference frame and time factor as fixed

effects predictors, and participants’ ID as the random intercept. We constructed the linear

mixed model (using the lme4 package in R; Bates et al., 2015) in R. The p values were

calculated using the Satterthwaite approximation for the mixed-effects regressions

(Kuznetsova et al., 2017). The results of the analysis are presented in the Table #. There was

no support for target frame as a predictor. However, we found support for the view that future

events (M= 3.22; SD= 0.94) are rated less predictable than the past events (M= 3.73; SD=

0.76; estimate = -0.57, 95% CI [-0.73 – -0.41], p <.001).

Table S12. Study 2: Results of mixed effect regression analysis


DV: Predictability
Predictors Estimates CI p
(Intercept) 3.78 3.67 – 3.90 <0.001
Target frame (Other vs. Self) -0.10 -0.25 – 0.06 0.222
Time (Future vs. Past) -0.57 -0.73 – -0.41 <0.001
Target frame × Time 0.09 -0.13 – 0.32 0.420
Random Effects
σ2 0.65
τ00 ParticipantID 0.08
ICC 0.11
N ParticipantID 437
Observations 874
Marginal R2 / Conditional R2 0.086 / 0.191
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 18

Study 2 Extension: Achievement Motives


Introduction
Our Study 2 also included a second extension in which we measured individual differences in
achievement motivation (Lang & Fries, 2006) as a potential trait moderator. Since a high need for
achievement is positively associated with work ethic and performance, it could well be the case that
the ego-centric effect in predictability judgments is more pronounced for participants with high rather
than low achievement motivation. Such a result would provide initial correlational evidence that the
effect is associated with motivated beliefs.

Table S13
Summary of extension hypotheses
Study Hypothesis
Hypothesis 4 People with high (vs. low) achievement motivation will have a stronger
tendency to think that they have more free will than others (i.e., less
predictable)
Hypothesis 5 People with high (vs. low) achievement motivation will show a greater future
vs. past discrepancy.

Results
Our Study 2 involved experimental designs in which the Target (self vs. other) and Time (past vs
future) varied as a between or within subject factors. Although, we presented the results
independently within each designed, they are less helpful in summarizing the results across the
sample. Therefore, we conducted a linear mixed-effects analysis to draw summary conclusions. We
pooled the responses across experimental conditions to conduct a linear mixed-effects analysis. Linear
mixed-effects models (LMEMs) have several advantages. More importantly, mixed effect analysis is
appropriate for our extension hypothesis that proposed the moderating role of the achievement motive
measured at the individual level (Lang & Fries, 2006). The results of the analysis are presented in
Table 10.
We found no support for differences between self and other. The results of interaction as test of
Hypothesis 4 and 5. The results suggest no support the prediction (H4) that people with high (vs. low)
achievement motivation will have a stronger tendency to think view outcomes are less predictable
than for others (estimate = -0.10, 95% [-0.28, 0.08]), p = .291). Similarly, we found no support for the
prediction (H5) that people with high (vs. low) achievement motivation will show a greater future vs.
past discrepancy (estimate = 0.01, 95% [-0.17, 0.20]), p = .896). Besides, results held support for the
view that future events as more predictable than participants who rated future events.
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 19

Table S14
Study 2: Results of mixed effect regression analysis
Predictability
Predictors Estimates CI p

(Intercept) 3.34 2.96 – 3.72 <0.001


Perspective (Other vs. Self) 0.23 -0.26 – 0.72 0.365
Time (Past vs. Future) -0.54 -1.04 – -0.04 0.034
Achievement Motivation Scale (AMS) 0.13 -0.01 – 0.28 0.067
Perspective × Time 0.48 -0.23 – 1.19 0.183
Perspective × AMS -0.10 -0.28 – 0.08 0.291
Time × AMS 0.01 -0.17 – 0.20 0.896
Perspective × Time × AMS -0.14 -0.40 – 0.12 0.290

Random Effects
σ2 0.61
τ00 ParticipantID 0.22
ICC 0.26
N ParticipantID 1295
Observations 3034
Marginal R2 / Conditional R2 0.062 / 0.308
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 20

Summary and Mini-meta analysis

Mini-meta-analyses including the original study

Forest plots of the mini meta-analyses. CI = confidence interval. ES = effect size (Cohen's d).

Figure S5. Testing H1 (People would view their own past and future decision as less
predictable a priori than those of a roommate.)

Figure S6. Testing H2a (People will consider their own past decisions as less predictable than
the ones of others.)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 21

Figure S7. Testing H2b (People will consider their own future decisions as less predictable
than the ones of others.)

Figure S8. Testing H3 (People would view future decisions as less predictable than past
decisions.)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 22

Mini-meta analyses excluding the original study

Note: Forest plots of the mini meta-analyses. CI = confidence interval. ES = effect size
(Cohen's d).

Figure S9. Testing H1 (People would view their own past and future decision as less
predictable a priori than those of a roommate.)

Figure S10. Testing H2a (People will consider their own past decisions as less predictable
than the ones of others.)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 23

Figure S11. Testing H2b (People will consider their own future decisions as less predictable
than the ones of others.)

Figure S12. Testing H3 (People would view future decisions as less predictable than past
decisions.)
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 24

Appendix A
Effect size and 95 % Confidence Intervals Calculation
Using: http://kylehamilton.net/shiny/MAVIS/
MAVIS: Meta Analysis via Shiny

● Cohen’ d = 1.08
● 95% CI of the main effect of self vs others = [0.47, 1.68]
Pronin and Kugler (2010) Experiment 1: Supplementary 25

References

Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2010). People believe they have more free will than

others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(52), 22469-22474.

LeBel, E. P., McCarthy, R. J., Earp, B. D., Elson, M., & Vanpaemel, W. (2018). A unified

framework to quantify the credibility of scientific findings. Advances in Methods and

Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 389-402.

LeBel, E. P., Vanpaemel, W., Cheung, I., & Campbell, L. (2019). A brief guide to evaluate

replications. Meta-Psychology, 3.

You might also like