You are on page 1of 14

International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-022-10141-2

FULL LENGTH MANUSCRIPT

Early Life SES, Childhood Trauma Exposures, and Cardiovascular


Responses to Daily Life Stressors in Middle‑aged Adults
Kristina D. Dickman1 · Elizabeth Votruba‑Drzal1 · Karen A. Matthews1,2,3 · Thomas W. Kamarck1,2

Accepted: 8 November 2022 / Published online: 22 November 2022


© International Society of Behavioral Medicine 2022

Abstract
Background Dysregulation in physiological responses to stress may provide a mechanism through which low socioeconomic
status (SES) in childhood negatively impacts health. Evidence linking early life SES to physiological stress responses is
inconsistent. Exposure to childhood trauma may be an important source of heterogeneity accounting for mixed findings.
Guided by the adaptive calibration model, we examined whether childhood SES and childhood trauma jointly predict ambu-
latory measures of cardiovascular responses to daily life stressors.
Method A sample of 377 healthy, middle-aged adults (62% female, 80% White, 64% college-educated, Mage = 52.59 ± 7.16)
completed a 4-day ecological momentary assessment protocol that measured task strain, social conflict, and ambulatory
systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP and DBP, respectively) at hourly intervals throughout the day. Average ambulatory
blood pressure responses to stress were calculated by regressing momentary SBP and DBP on momentary measures of stress
within the multilevel models. Early life SES and childhood trauma were measured retrospectively by self-report questionnaire.
Results Multilevel models controlling for momentary influences on blood pressure and age, sex, and race showed no main
effects of early life SES or childhood trauma on ambulatory measures of cardiovascular responses to daily life stress. An
interaction emerged for DBP responses to social conflict, where individuals raised in middle SES families who experienced
trauma showed a blunted response relative to those who did not (𝛾14 =−0.93, 95% CI: [−1.62, −0.24], p = .008). There was
no significant SES-trauma interaction in predicting SBP responses to social conflict or blood pressure responses to task strain.
Conclusion Results do not provide support for our predictions that were derived from the adaptive calibration model, but
do suggest that the impacts of early childhood experiences on cardiovascular responses may vary by type of daily stress
experience.

Keywords Childhood trauma · Socioeconomic status · Early life adversity · Cardiovascular reactivity · Ecological
momentary assessment

Introduction Galobardes and colleagues found that individuals of lower


early life SES were at greater risk of premature mortality
Socioeconomic status (SES) is a salient predictor of health than their higher SES peers, regardless of their SES in adult-
inequalities. Accumulating evidence suggests that chil- hood [2]. Similarly, low SES in childhood places individu-
dren raised in lower SES households show greater physical als at a modestly increased risk for cardiovascular disease
health risk as adults [1]. In a large review of the literature, (CVD), independent of adult SES [3].
Psychosocial factors such as increased stress or stress
* Kristina D. Dickman vulnerability may provide one path through which early life
kdd41@pitt.edu SES may affect health. Low SES environments are associ-
ated with greater risks of familial conflict, harsh and incon-
1
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, sistent parenting, neighborhood crime and violence, and
Pittsburgh, PA, USA
socioeconomic uncertainty and worry [4]. Many physiologi-
2
Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, cal systems are relatively plastic in early life, and exposure to
Pittsburgh, PA, USA
psychosocial stressors during this period may alter nervous,
3
Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, endocrine, and immune systems in persistent and permanent
Pittsburgh, PA, USA

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
802 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

ways [5]. The form that these alterations take may vary as a SES in relation to CVR [11–20]. Of these, five reported an
function of early life environmental exposure. inverse relationship, or exaggerated CVR in relation to lower
Individual differences in the patterning of physiological SES [11–15]. Two studies identified reduced reactivity in
stress responding may form as an adaption to social and relation to lower early life SES [16, 17]. The remaining three
physical conditions present during development. The adap- studies found either null or contradictory effects [18–20].
tive calibration model (ACM) posits, in part, that individu- We hypothesize that these mixed findings may be explained
als adapt optimal stress response systems dependent on the by heterogeneity in sample characteristics — namely, expo-
safety and predictability of their childhood environments [6]. sure to additional forms of childhood adversity. While it is
For an individual reared in a relatively safe and supportive possible that the mixture of results linking SES and car-
environment, a moderately active physiological reactivity diovascular reactivity suggests no overall relationship, it is
profile would allow the individual to respond adequately to also possible that this relationship is moderated by a third
emergencies, while also preserving energy for long-term variable, such as exposure to adverse childhood experiences.
fitness. However, in accordance with this model, a dan- A parallel and growing line of research demonstrates
gerous or unpredictable environment may cause an indi- more consistent associations between childhood adversity,
vidual to adopt a highly reactive stress response profile, as a general term that describes exposure to harmful experi-
this would increase the individual’s ability to respond to ences such interpersonal stress, material deprivation, abuse,
short-term danger. In this environment, an individual may and/or neglect, and CVR to laboratory stressors. With only
be better served placing resources into short-term growth a couple of exceptions [21, 22], the majority of work in this
and maintenance activities, as longer-term reproductive fit- area finds that children with more exposure to adverse life
ness is uncertain. Individuals reared in very traumatic or events demonstrate lower CVR to acute stressors than their
persistently stressful environments, however, may adopt an peers without adversity exposure [23]. Overall, growing evi-
unresponsive approach towards acute stressors, as constant dence suggests an inverse relationship where greater child-
activation of the stress response system may incur greater hood adversity relates with blunted responses to acute stress.
physiological costs with few benefits. In these cases, a pro- Exposure to a materially disadvantaged SES background
file characterized by physiological withdrawal may be the is one type of childhood adversity. While increased child-
optimal strategy because of the frequency and severity of hood trauma, also a form of adversity, is more prevalent
expected stressors and the resource economization associ- among low SES environments, exposure to trauma is not
ated with withdrawal. The ACM asserts that physiological limited to lower SES households [24]. To date, the litera-
stress reactivity develops as a direct consequence of the tures linking early life SES with cardiovascular reactivity
stability and safety of one’s early life environment along and childhood adversity with cardiovascular reactivity have
with one’s own available resources. Both of these provide a remained largely independent of one another. Given the
context for helping the organism predict the likely balance mixed evidence linking early life socioeconomic disadvan-
of demands for both acute adaptation and long-term fitness. tage with elevated [11–15], blunted [16, 17], and equivalent
As alluded to in the above model, one metric of physi- [18–20] cardiovascular reactivity, it might be that differential
ological dysregulation is the level of (either exaggerated or exposure to childhood trauma is an important source of vari-
blunted) cardiovascular response to stress. The reactivity ability causing these two stress-related profiles to emerge.
hypothesis asserts that prolonged or exaggerated cardiovas- As described above, the ACM posits that early environ-
cular reactivity (CVR) to psychological stress promotes the mental influences may predispose the organism to adopt
development of CVD, in part, due to structural and func- different patterns of stress responding, with moderately
tional changes in the coronary arteries that may accompany demanding environments shaping a stress reactive response
frequent or prolonged hemodynamic responses [7]. Empiri- pattern, and extreme conditions eliciting an unemotional,
cally, exaggerated and prolonged CVR to acute cognitive or blunted, stress response profile [6]. We drew from the
or social stressors have been linked with several adverse ACM to generate a priori predictions on how childhood
health outcomes including CVD risk and early mortality [8, trauma and SES may interact. We hypothesized that child-
9]. More recently, blunted CVR to acute stressors has been hood surroundings with few material resources but nontrau-
linked with adverse health outcomes including obesity and matic psychosocial environments may serve as moderately
depression [10]. While early life SES consistently relates demanding and encourage heightened reactivity to stress. At
with disease, the connection between SES and cardiovascu- the same time, the double burden of material disadvantage
lar stress responding is inconclusive. and childhood trauma exposure might create a more persis-
Lower early life SES, typically measured by parental tently stressful developmental context, which promotes the
education and income, has been linked with both exagger- unemotional, or blunted, stress response profile. We hypoth-
ated and blunted cardiovascular reactivity to stress. To our esized that these two domains of early life stress should be
knowledge, at least ten studies have investigated childhood particularly susceptible to this type of synergy, as they might

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 803

be seen to capture heightened demands (i.e., stress/trauma) selected two EMA-based measures of stress — task strain
and decreased psychosocial or material resources during the and social conflict — which reflect diverse thematic content
childhood period. The unbalanced experience of demands and have previously been shown relevant to cardiovascular
and resources may thus increase the early life stress bur- disease risk. Use of EMA in the current study may allow
den and constrain stress coping [25]. Therefore, it may be us to measure stress responsivity while avoiding potential
that links between low early life SES and cardiovascular SES-driven confounds that may arise from participating in
responses to stress depend on exposure to childhood trauma, a laboratory setting.
where low SES individuals with a history of childhood To our knowledge, this interaction between early life SES
trauma demonstrate blunted cardiovascular reactivity and and childhood trauma on cardiovascular reactivity has yet
low SES individuals with no history of childhood trauma to be examined in a single study. Furthermore, while these
demonstrate exaggerated cardiovascular reactivity relative individual associations have been studied in the laboratory,
to their high SES counterparts. the current study is, to our knowledge, the first to examine
A variety of composite measures are utilized to assess the the association between early life SES and cardiovascular
prevalence of adversity. The present study uses the child- stress response in daily life and only the second to do so for
hood trauma questionnaire [26] to retrospectively assess childhood trauma [22]. The present report assesses whether
exposure to experiences of abuse and neglect prior to the childhood SES and the presence or absence of childhood
age of 18. This measure of childhood adversity is specifi- trauma associate with the effect of daily life stress on con-
cally designed to capture trauma experiences, in contrast to currently assessed ambulatory blood pressure (ABP), and
indices such as the Adverse Childhood Experience Ques- whether the two interact in this relationship. Daily stress
tionnaire [27], which concurrently assesses other forms of was operationalized as social conflict and task strain, two
adversity including household dysfunction. areas of stress that relate to risk for cardiovascular disease
Most of the previous work on the topic of childhood in epidemiological studies [31–34]. It was hypothesized
adversity and cardiovascular stress reactivity has centered on that early life SES would not directly relate to cardiovascu-
reactivity defined in the laboratory. In contrast, the current lar responses to stress, but that history of trauma exposure
study uses ecological momentary assessment (EMA), a data would relate with decreased (i.e., blunted) stress responses.
collection method in which information on an individual’s It was predicted that trauma and early life SES would inter-
current state is gathered as it occurs in the natural environ- act to predict stress responses. Specifically, it was expected
ment [28]. Methodologically, there are three advantages of that lower early life SES individuals with a history of child-
using EMA to investigate SES and trauma differences in hood trauma would exhibit blunted ABP stress responses
cardiovascular stress responding. First, momentary meas- when compared to low early life SES individuals without
ures of stress may provide a more representative depiction a trauma history and that low SES individuals with no his-
of daily life experiences than global self-report assessments. tory of childhood trauma would show exaggerated responses
Second, if we assume that low SES individuals may be less relative to those of high early life SES and those without a
comfortable than their more affluent counterparts in a for- trauma history.
mal laboratory setting, EMA may permit us to assess stress
responses in a manner that is more equivalent across the
SES gradient. Research shows that individuals from lower Methods
SES backgrounds perceive greater discrimination in medical
settings compared to their higher SES peers [29], although Participants
no study to our knowledge has explored this phenomenon in
laboratory stress paradigms. Third, psychosocial factors and Participants were drawn from the Study of Health and Inter-
health risks measured through EMA protocols may be more actions in the Natural Environment (SHINE). This study
strongly associated with markers of disease than those meas- recruited 391 healthy adults aged 40 to 65 from the Pitts-
ured using global retrospective self-reports. The Karasek job burgh area between 2014 and 2018. Exclusionary criteria
strain model posits that environments high in psychologi- included a history of cardiovascular disease, schizophrenia
cal demands but low in decision control are associated with or bipolar disease, chronic hepatitis, renal failure, neuro-
elevated cardiovascular risk [30]. Work from this laboratory logical disorder, lung disease requiring drug treatment, or
has shown that EMA measures of job strain prospectively stage 2 hypertension, use of insulin, glucocorticoid, antiar-
predicted intima-medial thickness, a measure of preclinical rhythmic, antihypertensive, lipid-lowering, psychotropic,
vascular disease, while traditional self-report measures of autonomically active, or prescription weight loss medica-
job strain did not [31, 32]. EMA measures of social con- tions, and current pregnancy. The study was approved by
flict have likewise been linked with risk of atherosclerosis the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board.
and coronary heart disease [32–34]. In the current study, we All participants were administered informed consent before

13
804 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

enrollment and were compensated between $350 and $400 into three dummy-coded early life SES groups: low (high
for their participation. school degree or less), middle (some college experience
From the original sample of 391, 8 individuals were but no 4-year degree), and high (at least a college degree).
removed due to missing trauma history data, 5 for incom- A categorical classification strategy was selected to allow
plete parental education data, and 1 for having less than for non-equivalent differences between education groups
two ABP observations. The final analytic sample was and the potential for non-linear associations between early
composed of 377 healthy adults. The final analytical sam- life SES and cardiovascular reactivity. Three groups were
ple was 62% female and 20% BIPOC. Participants were, selected a priori based on examination of sample size dis-
on average, middle-aged (Mage = 52.59 ± 7.16), overweight tributions. Thirty-four percent of the sample grew up in a
(MBMI = 28.0 ± 5.37), and well-educated (64% had received “low SES” household, 29% in a “middle SES” household,
at least a bachelor’s degree). and 37% in a “high SES” household.

Procedure Childhood Trauma During visit 4, participants completed


the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ) to
Individuals completed a total of four laboratory visits, each retrospectively assess the presence and severity of childhood
lasting 3–4 h. In visit 1, participants provided demographic trauma [26]. An example question is “People in my family
information and verified their study eligibility. At visit 2, hit me so hard that it left me with bruises or marks,” rated on
participants completed questionnaires not relevant to this a Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (very often true). The scale
study. At visit 3, participants were trained to participate in provides five subscores: physical abuse, emotional abuse,
ambulatory monitoring procedures. All participants were sexual abuse, emotional neglect, and physical neglect. The
instructed on use of several devices including an automated five CTQ trauma exposure types have been found to have
blood pressure cuff (Oscar 2 oscillometric monitor, Suntech good criterion-related validity [33]. In this study, clinical
Medical Inc, Morrisville, NC) and an electronic diary. Train- cut points as introduced by Walker et al. (1998) were used to
ing visits lasted approximately 4 h and participants demon- designate the presence of each trauma exposure as follows:
strated device comprehension prior to use in daily life. The physical abuse ≥ 8, emotional abuse ≥ 10, sexual abuse ≥ 8,
participants then completed a 7–10-day EMA protocol. Four emotional neglect ≥ 15, physical neglect ≥ 8 [38]. Presence
of these days constituted “full monitoring” days in which of childhood trauma was scored dichotomously, where child-
participants responded to hourly surveys and obtained hourly hood trauma exposure was endorsed when a participant
blood pressure readings. On the “partial monitoring” days, scored at or above clinical threshold on any one subscale
participants responded to morning and evening question- and absence of childhood trauma exposure occurred when
naires. Participants then returned to the laboratory for visit participants scored below all clinical thresholds. These cut
4, where they completed questionnaires including the Child- points have been previously used in middle-aged samples
hood Trauma Questionnaire. similar to ours [39, 40].

Measures Ambulatory Blood Pressure Ambulatory blood pressure


(ABP) was assessed at consistent, hourly intervals across
Socioeconomic Status Participants completed a self-report the waking period of each monitoring day. Both systolic
demographic questionnaire which probed their parents’ edu- (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) readings were gathered. In
cation levels, current household income, and current educa- accordance with the SunTech Oscar 2 user manual, ABP
tion level. Parental education was separately assessed for readings associated with an error code (e.g., leaky cuff, low
each individual’s mother and father, and was scored on a battery) were deleted prior to analyses. In accordance with
12-point scale ranging from 1 = less than eighth grade to criteria put forth by Marler, Jacob, Lehoczky, and Shap-
12 = MD, PhD, JD, PharmD. Early life SES was conceptual- iro (1988), ABP readings were removed as invalid when-
ized as the highest level of education level achieved by the ever SBP < 70 mmHg or > 250 mmHg, DBP < 45 mmHg
most-educated parent. This metric is more easily recalled or > 150 mmHg, or SBP/DBP < [1.065 + (0.00125 × DBP)]
than other childhood SES measures (e.g., parental income) or > 3 [41].
and is recommended for use in retrospective studies with
adult samples [35]. While parental education represents Daily Life Stressors After each ABP measurement, partici-
only one marker of early life socioeconomic disadvantage, pants completed an ambulatory interview on an electronic
it predicts adult health outcomes above and beyond adult diary. The diary presented between 49 and 58 multiple
education levels [36] and explains unique variance in adult choice questions regarding recent stressors and social inter-
health outcomes controlling for family income [37]. Based actions. Using these diary questions, two types of life stress-
on reports of parental education, individuals were divided ors were measured: task strain and social conflict.

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 805

Task Strain On each interview, participants were asked to vs white) and sex (male vs female) were dichotomized. Age
“think about mental and physical activity in the past 10 min,” and BMI were added as continuous variables.
then to indicate the degree of psychological demand asso-
ciated with this activity (i.e., “working hard?”, “working Plan of Analysis
fast?”, juggling “several tasks at once?”), and the relative
control associated with the activity (i.e., “felt in command,” Analyses were performed utilizing R Statistical Software
“handled [themselves] well?”). Of note, this measure of con- [43]. Separate one-way ANOVA and χ2 analyses were used
trol is distinct from and more general than the measure of to test differences between high, middle, and low early life
decision latitude previously used in our laboratory [32, 42]. SES groups for demographic characteristics. Multilevel
Items were scored on a 1–10 Likert scale. Situations rated modeling (MLM) procedures were used to test the study
above the sample median in demand and below or equal to hypotheses. MLM corrects for nested data structures, repre-
the sample median in control were coded “1” as instances sents within-person changes over multiple time points, and
of task strain. All other situations (those low in demand and models how individuals differ in these changes. All analy-
those high in control) were coded “0” as instances absent of ses were run using the “nlme” statistical package for linear
“task strain.” and nonlinear mixed effects models [44] in R software 3.5.1.
using log-likelihood methods, or “ML,” for estimating fixed
Social Conflict Participants documented their most recent and random effects simultaneously. All models were run
social interaction, and whether that interaction was marked with a continuous AR1 autocorrelation structure that cor-
by conflict (“someone was insensitive to you,” “someone rects for correlated error between adjacent timepoints. As
interfered with your efforts,” and “someone made you recommended by the multivariate literature, task strain and
tense”). These three items were designed to capture both social conflict were both person-centered prior to analy-
obvious and subtle instances of social conflict, and were ses [45]. This process created two variables for each stress
scored on a 10-point Likert scale. Only interactions which measure: within-person measures of stress added at level 1,
occurred within the 10 min preceding ABP readings were and between-person measures of stress added at level 2. All
included in the analyses. Social conflict was operationalized independent variables, including covariates, were centered
as the mean of the items. This calculation of a continuous on within-person means (level-1 variables) or on between-
social conflict score is consistent with prior work out of our person means (level-2 variables) prior to statistical modeling.
laboratory [33]. Analyses were performed separately for each measure of
stress and each blood pressure outcome, such that a total
Covariates of four models were fit to assess the impact of early life
SES and childhood trauma on SBP reactivity to task strain,
Time‑Dependent Covariates In the hourly interview, par- DBP reactivity to task strain, SBP reactivity to social con-
ticipants reported their current posture (standing, sitting, or flict, and DBP reactivity to social conflict. For each of the
lying down), temperature comfort (comfortable, too hot, or aforementioned models, analyses followed five hierarchical
too cold), speaking status (speaking or not speaking), self- steps. First, unconditional multilevel models predicting ABP
report physical activity (limited, light, moderate, or vigor- by participants were estimated to inspect the variance in
ous), and recent consumption of food, alcohol, or caffeine ABP accounted for by within and between-person differ-
(yes or no). Participants used the electronic diary to report ences. In step two, time-varying covariates were added as
each time they smoked a cigarette or other nicotine exposure. fixed effects and fixed and random effects of person-centered
Timestamped self-reports of smoking that occurred within stress (either task strain or social conflict) were added to
10 min of a ABP measurement were marked (cigarette used test the acute effect of stress on ABP and whether these
or no cigarette used). These variables have previously been associations vary between people. In step three, all level-2
associated with acute ABP changes [36], and were included predictors and covariates were added as main effects, and
as covariates in the analyses involving stress-related changes early life SES and childhood trauma were added as cross-
in ABP. level interactions to assess the effects of early life SES and
childhood trauma on ABP reactivity. In step four, two-way
Between‑Subject Covariates In addition to the momentary interactions between early life SES and childhood trauma on
covariates, self-reported race, sex, age, and body mass index ABP were added as fixed effects in order to provide a com-
(BMI) were included as between-person covariates. Race, parison to test significance for a fifth model step. In step five,
sex, and age were assessed in the demographic questionnaire the three-way cross-level interaction terms between early
obtained in visit 1. BMI was calculated using measurements life SES, childhood trauma, and the within-person effect of
of height and weight collected by trained research staff at stress were added. The fully identified (step five) models are
visit 1. Race (black, indigenous, people of color (BIPOC) depicted in Electronic Supplementary Materials 1.

13
806 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

Model comparisons occurred between each step using adult SES, we repeated the main analyses controlling for
likelihood ratio tests (L.ratio). The results of the third and adult education (dummy coded into HS degree or less, some
fifth steps were interpreted to test the study hypotheses. college, college degree, and advanced degree) and income
In models where the 3-way interaction terms significantly level.
increased variance explained (significant L.ratio between
steps 4 and 5), simple slopes were investigated by re-cen-
tering the reference groups for early life SES and childhood Results
trauma and examining the relevant 2-way interactions [46].
Table 1 provides descriptive characteristics of the final ana-
Planned Exploratory Analyses For models in which a lytic sample. Overall, the sample was 62% female and 20%
significant three-way interaction appeared, exploratory BIPOC. Sixty-four percent had received at least a bachelor’s
analyses examined whether types of childhood trauma degree and, in total, 42% of the sample reported exposure to
interact differently with early life SES to predict CVR. childhood trauma. Rates of childhood trauma endorsed in the
McLaughlin, Sheridan, and Lambert suggest that threat present sample align with previous reports using the CTQ
(e.g., abuse experiences) and deprivation (e.g., neglect and Walker et al.’s (1998) cutoffs in similar middle-aged
experiences) may represent distinct dimensions of early samples, which range from 38 to 48% exposure to any child-
life trauma and differentially impact downstream physiol- hood trauma [39, 40]. There were no significant differences
ogy [47]. Based off their theory, childhood trauma was in trauma history, biological sex, or adult income between
divided into four distinct groups: history of abuse only, the three early life SES groups. There was a significant dif-
history of neglect only, history of both abuse and neglect, ference in age (F(2,374) = 3.05, p = 0.049), where the middle
or no trauma history. Exploratory analysis involved fitting SES group was marginally younger than the low and high
an MLM almost equivalent to that of the analogous step 5. SES groups; a significant difference in adult educational
obtainment (F(2,371) = 13.29, p < 0.001), where the low and
Sensitivity Analyses Controlling for Adult SES To evaluate middle early life SES groups had less adult education than
whether any observed associations between early life SES, the high SES group; a significant difference in race (χ2 (2,
childhood trauma, and CVR persist after controlling for N = 377) = 9.82, p = 0.007), where the middle SES group had

Table 1  Descriptive characteristics by early life SES (mean ± standard deviation or %)


Variable Low SES (n = 128) Middle SES (n = 108) High SES (n = 141) Total sample (n = 377)

Age* 53.85 ± 6.75 51.85 ± 7.45 52.01 ± 7.19 52.59 ± 7.16


Sex (female) 66% 62% 57% 62%
Race*
White 80% 70% 87% 80%
Black 18% 30% 10% 18%
Asian 2% 2% 4% 3%
American Indian 1% 1% 0% 1%
Childhood trauma
No history 63% 51% 60% 58%
Abuse only 11% 19% 14% 15%
Neglect only 8% 6% 7% 7%
Abuse and neglect 19% 24% 18% 20%
(Adult) education*
High school 9% 6% 4% 6%
Some college 42% 31% 18% 30%
College degree 28% 32% 36% 32%
Advanced degree 21% 31% 42% 32%
(Adult) income 7.83 ± 3.89 7.33 ± 3.49 8.50 ± 4.02 7.94 ± 3.85
Body mass index* 28.3 ± 6.15 28.8 ± 5.09 27.1 ± 4.68 28.0 ± 5.37

An adult household income score of 7 corresponds with $65,000–79,999/year, a score of 8 corresponds with $80,000–94,999/year, and a score
of 9 corresponds with $95,000–109,999/year. Participants were allowed to select multiple racial identities. An * depicts significant (p < .05)
overall difference between low, middle, and high SES groups

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 807

a higher proportion of BIPOC than the low and high SES trauma and within-person task strain ( 𝛾13 =−0.51, 95%
groups; and a significant difference in BMI (F(2,373) = 3.59, CI: [−1.38, 0.35]). There was no increase in variance
p = 0.029), where the middle early life SES group had a explained when three-way interactions were added in step
larger average BMI than the high early life SES group. five (L.ratio = 0.30, p = 0.86).
Individuals completed an average of 46 ambulatory
measurements over the course of the 4 days (range: 9–67). SBP Response to Social Conflict The unconditional model
On average, 27 of these occurred within 10 min of a social demonstrated that 59.6% of the variance in SBP occurs
interaction (range: 2–57), judged to be the average duration at the between-person level. The second step of mod-
over which social interaction effects on ABP might persist. eling explained a significant increase in SBP variance
In total, task strain analyses included 17,263 observations, (L.ratio = 513.90, p < 0.001). A significant fixed effect of
and social conflict analyses included 10,017 observations. within-person social conflict emerged such that, after con-
In-depth output for the hypothesis-testing model steps (steps trolling for time-varying covariates, a 1-point increase in
3 and 5) is depicted in Tables 2 and 3. Relevant parameters social conflict associated with a 0.44-mmHg increase in
for steps 1 through 5 are provided in-text. Full output for all SBP, on average (𝛾10 = 0.44, 95% CI: [0.24, 0.64]). A sig-
steps can be viewed on our OSF page at osf.io/2y376/. nificant random effect of social conflict on SBP (τ11 = 0.46,
95% CI: [0.14,1.52]) indicated that individuals vary in their
SBP Response to Task Strain The unconditional model dem- relationship between social conflict and SBP. The addition
onstrated that 60.0% of the variance in SBP occurs at the of level-2 covariates and two-way cross-level interactions
between-person level. Step two indicated significant influence in step three significantly increased variance explained
of momentary factors on SBP (L.ratio = 1116.46, p < 0.001). (L.ratio = 104.96, p < 0.001). However, no cross-level inter-
A significant fixed effect of within-person task strain emerged actions emerged between early life SES and within-person
such that, after controlling for time-varying covariates, pres- social strain (𝛾11 = 0.40, 95% CI: [−0.09, 0.89]; 𝛾12 =−0.20,
ence of task strain associated with a 1.30 mmHg increase in 95% CI: [−0.65, 0.26], respectively) or between child-
SBP, on average (𝛾10 = 1.30, 95% CI: [0.72, 1.89]). There hood trauma and within-person social strain ( 𝛾13 =−0.07,
was also a significant random effect of task strain on SBP 95% CI: [−0.47, 0.32]). There was no increase in variance
(τ11 = 3.04, 95% CI: [1.14, 8.11]), indicating that people vary in explained when three-way interactions were added in step
SBP response to task strain. The addition of level-2 covariates five (L.ratio = 0.14, p = 0.93).
and two-way cross level interactions in step three explained
additional variance (L.ratio = 114.28, p < 0.001). However, DBP Response to Social Conflict The unconditional model
no significant interaction effects emerged between childhood demonstrated that 54.1% of the variance in DBP occurs
SES and task strain reactivity (𝛾11 = 0.62, 95% CI: [−0.87, at the between-person level. The second step of mod-
2.10]; 𝛾12 = 1.28, 95% CI: [−0.07, 2.63]) or childhood trauma eling explained a significant increase in DBP variance
and task strain reactivity (𝛾13 =−0.91, 95% CI: [−2.07, 0.25]). (L.ratio = 752.96, p < 0.001). A significant fixed effect of
Three-way interactions were added to the model in step five within-person social conflict emerged such that, after con-
and compared to step four. No overall three-way interaction trolling for time-varying covariates, a 1-point increase in
emerged (L.ratio = 1.83, p = 0.40). social conflict associated with a 0.25-mmHg increase in
DBP, on average (𝛾10 = 0.25, 95% CI: [0.11, 0.39]). A sig-
DBP Response to Task Strain The unconditional model nificant random effect of social conflict on DBP (τ11 = 0.21,
demonstrated that 53.9% of the variance in DBP occurs 95% CI: [0.08,0.61]) indicated that individuals vary in their
at the between-person level. Step 2 indicated significant relationship between social conflict and DBP. The addition
influence of momentary factors on DBP (L.ratio = 1633.92, of level-2 covariates and two-way cross-level interactions
p < 0.001). A significant fixed effect of within-person task in step three significantly increased variance explained
strain emerged such that, after controlling for time-varying (L.ratio = 84.94, p < 0.001). However, no significant inter-
covariates, a 1-point increase in task strain was associated actions emerged between early life SES and within-person
with a 0.84-mmHg increase in DBP, on average (𝛾10 = 0.84, social strain (𝛾11 = 0.23, 95% CI: [−0.12, 0.57]; 𝛾12 = 0.03,
95% CI: [0.41, 1.27]). Likewise, there was a significant 95% CI: [−0.30, 0.35], respectively) or between childhood
random effect of task strain on DBP (τ11 = 2.93, 95% CI: trauma and within-person social strain (𝛾13 =−0.18, 95% CI:
[1.56, 5.49]). The addition of level-2 covariates and two- [−0.46, 0.10]). The addition of three-way interactions in step
way cross-level interactions in step three increased vari- five caused a significant increase in variance explained, indi-
ance explained (L.ratio = 85.96, p < 0.001). However, no cating a significant three-way interaction between early life
cross-level interactions emerged between early life SES and SES, childhood trauma, and DBP response to social conflict
within-person task strain (𝛾11 = 0.26, 95% CI: [−0.84, 1.37]; (L.ratio = 6.93, p = 0.031; see Fig. 1). The model showed,
𝛾12 = 0.29, 95% CI: [−0.72, 1.31]) or between childhood specifically, a three-way interaction term between childhood

13
808 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

Table 2  Early life SES, childhood trauma, and blood pressure reactivity to task strain
SBP DBP
Step 3 Step 5 Step 3 Step 5

Fixed effects
Intercept,𝛾00 135.4 (1.66)*** 136.5 (1.88)*** 80.44 (1.08)*** 81.33 (1.18)***
Level-1 variables
Posture standing,𝛾20 3.39 (0.25)*** 3.40 (0.25)*** 3.33 (0.17)*** 3.33 (0.17)***
Posture laying,𝛾30 −7.30 (0.50)*** −7.29 (0.50)*** −7.64 (0.35)*** −7.64 (0.35)***
Talking,𝛾40 2.11 (0.24)*** 2.11 (0.24)*** 1.91 (0.17)*** 1.91 (0.17)***
Physical activity,𝛾50 2.23 (0.21)*** 2.23 (0.21)*** 1.08 (0.15)*** 1.08 (0.15)***
Recent meal,𝛾60 1.15 (0.24)*** 1.15 (0.24)*** −0.22 (0.17) −0.23 (0.17)
Caffeine,𝛾70 0.05 (0.30) 0.05 (0.30) −0.02 (0.21) −0.02 (0.21)
Temperature hot,𝛾80 1.05 (0.49)* 1.04 (0.49)* 0.15 (0.34) 0.15 (0.34)
Temperature cold,𝛾90 3.15 (0.53)*** 3.16 (0.53)*** 1.33 (0.37)*** 1.33 (0.37)***
Alcohol,𝛾100 −0.33 (0.61) −0.34 (0.61) −0.51 (0.42) −0.51 (0.42)
Smoking status,𝛾110 −0.06 (0.69) −0.06 (0.69) 0.14 (0.48) 0.14 (0.48)
Task strain,𝛾10 0.98 (0.56)† 1.39 (0.64)* 0.86 (0.42)* 0.87 (0.48)†
Level-2 variables
Child ­SESmiddle,𝛾01 −0.01 (2.00) −0.97 (2.67) −0.41 (1.25) −1.06 (1.67)
Child ­SEShigh,𝛾02 1.29 (1.88) −0.63 (2.39) 0.63 (1.18) −1.08 (1.48)
Childhood trauma,𝛾03 0.45 (1.60) −2.17 (2.76) −0.36 (1.00) −2.57 (1.73)
Child ­SESmiddle*child trauma,𝛾04 2.62 (4.02) 1.89 (2.52)
­ EShigh*child trauma,𝛾05
Child S 5.01 (3.80) 4.50 (2.38)†
Task strain (person means),𝛾06 6.29 (2.97)* 6.02 (2.97)* 4.88 (1.87)** 4.62 (1.86)**
Race,𝛾07 −6.87 (1.99)*** −6.94 (1.99)*** −5.52 (1.25)*** −5.57 (1.25)***
Age,𝛾08 0.50 (0.11)*** 0.51 (0.11)*** 0.16 (0.07)* 0.16 (0.07)*
Sex,𝛾09 −8.72 (1.61)*** −8.64 (1.62)*** −5.55 (1.02)*** −5.45 (1.02)***
BMI,𝛾010 1.05 (0.15)*** 1.04 (0.15)*** 0.47 (0.09)*** 0.46 (0.09)***
Cross-level interaction variables
Task strain*child ­SESmiddle 𝛾11 0.62 (0.76) −0.08 (0.99) 0.26 (0.57) 0.42 (0.75)
Task strain*child ­SEShigh,𝛾12 1.28 (0.69)† 0.59 (0.86) 0.29 (0.52) 0.19 (0.65)
Task strain*child trauma,𝛾13 −0.91 (0.59) −2.13 (1.10)† −0.51 (0.44) −0.52 (0.82)
Task strain*child ­SESmiddle*child trauma,𝛾14 1.47 (1.54) −0.32 (1.15)
Task strain child ­SEShigh*child trauma,𝛾15 1.90 (1.43) 0.26 (1.07)
Random effects
Intercept,𝜏00 219.60 218.54 86.57 85.74
Task strain slope,𝜏11 2.64 2.36 2.87 2.86
Level 1, Var within person,𝜎 2 184.37 184.42 83.77 83.77
Log-likelihood −69,162.96 −69,161.33 −62,791.57 −62,789.64
L.ratio test (compared to step 2 or 4) 114.28*** 1.83 85.96*** 0.30

Step 3 refers to the model in which level-1 predictors and covariates were added as main effects, and childhood trauma and early life SES were
added as cross-level interactions. Step 5 refers to the model including the three-way cross-level interaction terms between early life SES, child-
hood trauma, and the within-person effect of task strain. An † depicts p < 0.10, an * indicates p < 0.05, an ** indicates p < 0.01, and an *** indi-
cates p < 0.001. All random effects are significantly different from zero

trauma, middle early life SES (compared to low SES), and life SES, where those without a history of childhood trauma
the influence of social conflict on DBP (𝛾14 =−0.93, 95% CI: showed significantly greater DBP response than those
[−1.62, −0.24], p = 0.008). with a history of childhood trauma ( 𝛾13 =−0.66, 95% CI:
[−1.17, −0.15]). There were no childhood trauma differences
Analysis of simple slopes demonstrated significant dif- in DBP response for the individuals from high or low SES
ferences in DBP response for individuals from middle early upbringings (𝛾13 =−0.21, 95% CI: [−0.66, 0.24]; 𝛾13 = 0.23,

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 809

Table 3  Full early life SES, childhood trauma, and blood pressure reactivity to social conflict
SBP DBP
Step 3 Step 5 Step 3 Step 5

Fixed effects
Intercept,𝛾00 137.8 (1.69)*** 139.0 (1.91)*** 81.94 (1.06)*** 82.90 (1.20)***
Level-1 variables
Posture standing,𝛾20 3.33 (0.33)*** 3.33 (0.33)*** 3.27 (0.23)*** 3.27 (0.23)***
Posture laying,𝛾30 −7.43 (0.80)*** −7.43 (0.80)*** −7.85 (0.56)*** −7.83 (0.55)***
Talking,𝛾40 1.83 (0.29)*** 1.83 (0.29)*** 1.72 (0.20)*** 1.72 (0.20)***
Physical activity,𝛾50 1.85 (0.29)*** 1.85 (0.29)*** 0.89 (0.20)*** 0.89 (0.20)***
Recent meal,𝛾60 1.16 (0.32)*** 1.16 (0.32)*** −0.11 (0.23) −0.11 (0.23)
Caffeine,𝛾70 −0.23 (0.40) −0.23 (0.40) 0.06 (0.28) 0.06 (0.28)
Temperature hot,𝛾80 1.00 (0.64) 1.00 (0.64) −0.06 (0.44) −0.04 (0.44)
Temperature cold,𝛾90 3.39 (0.75)*** 3.39 (0.75)*** 1.36 (0.52)** 1.37 (0.52)**
Alcohol,𝛾100 0.76 (0.77) 0.77 (0.77) 0.57 (0.52) 0.60 (0.52)
Smoking status,𝛾110 −0.37 (0.95) −0.37 (0.95) 0.02 (0.66) 0.04 (0.66)
Social conflict,𝛾10 0.42 (0.18)* 0.38 (0.21)† 0.25 (0.13)† 0.10 (0.14)
Level-2 variables
Child ­SESmiddle,𝛾01 −0.58 (2.04) −1.85 (2.72) −0.67 (1.28) −1.32 (1.70)
Child ­SEShigh,𝛾02 1.07 (1.91) −0.98 (2.41) 0.58 (1.20) −1.32 (1.51)
Childhood trauma,𝛾03 0.42 (1.64) −2.54 (2.81) −0.43 (1.03) −2.80 (1.76)
Child ­SESmiddle*child trauma,𝛾04 3.31 (4.09) 1.90 (2.65)
Child ­SEShigh*child trauma,𝛾05 5.38 (3.87) 4.95 (2.42)*
Social conflict (person means),𝛾06 0.57 (0.75) 0.50 (0.75) 0.76 (0.47) 0.68 (0.47)
Race,𝛾07 −7.36 (2.01)*** −7.44 (2.01)*** −6.08 (1.04)*** −6.11 (1.27)***
Age,𝛾08 0.51 (0.11)*** 0.52 (0.11)*** 0.18 (0.07)* 0.18 (0.07)**
Sex,𝛾09 −8.03 (1.64)*** −7.96 (1.65)*** −5.46 (1.04)*** −5.34 (1.04)***
BMI,𝛾010 1.07 (0.15)*** 1.06 (0.15)*** 0.49 (0.09)*** 0.49 (0.09)***
Cross-level interaction variables
Social conflict*child S ­ ESmiddle 𝛾11 0.40 (0.25) 0.47 (0.34) 0.23 (0.18) 0.64 (0.23)**
Social conflict*child S ­ EShigh,𝛾12 −0.20 (0.23) −0.15 (0.29) 0.03 (0.16) 0.17 (0.20)
Social conflict*child trauma,𝛾13 −0.07 (0.20) 0.03 (0.34) −0.18 (0.14) 0.23 (0.24)
Social conflict*child S ­ ESmiddle*child trauma,𝛾14 −0.17 (0.51) −0.93 (0.35)**
Social conflict*child ­SEShigh*child trauma,𝛾15 −0.15 (0.48) −0.39 (0.33)
Random effects
Intercept,𝜏00 221.99 220.86 87.38 86.44
Social conflict slope,𝜏11 0.39 0.38 0.21 0.18
Level 1, Var within person,𝜎 2 188.21 188.23 85.08 85.08
Log-likelihood −40,550.17 −40,549.05 −36,785.98 −36,780.32
L.ratio test (compared to step 4) 104.98*** 0.14 84.94*** 6.93*

Step 3 refers to the model in which level-1 predictors and covariates were added as main effects, and childhood trauma and early life SES were
added as cross-level interactions. Step 5 refers to the model including three-way cross-level interaction terms between early life SES, childhood
trauma, and the within-person effect of social conflict. An † depicts p < 0.10, an * indicates p < 0.05, an ** indicates p < 0.01, and an *** indi-
cates p < 0.001. All random effects are significantly different from zero

95% CI: [−0.23, 0.69], respectively). For individuals without exposed to childhood trauma, there were no significant SES-
a history of childhood trauma, those from a middle early life driven differences in DBP response.
SES showed higher DBP response than those from a low
early life SES (𝛾11= 0.61, 95% CI: [0.15, 1.06]) and margin- Exploratory Analyses Since the middle SES group had a
ally higher response than those from a high early life SES greater proportion of BIPOC than the other two SES groups
(𝛾 = −0.40, 95% CI: [−0.85, 0.06]). Among the individuals (see Table 1), an exploratory analysis was run where race

13
810 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

Fig. 1  DBP reactivity to social


conflict

was substituted in for early life SES in the fully identified our interpretation of the results (see Electronic Supplemen-
model to explore the interaction between race, childhood tary Materials 2 and 3). There were no main effects of early
trauma, and DBP reactivity to social conflict. No signifi- life SES or childhood trauma on ABP response to social
cant interaction emerged (𝛾 = 0.12, 95% CI: [−0.60, 0.84], conflict or task strain. As before, an interaction emerged
p = 0.74), suggesting that the observed effect of early life between early life SES, childhood trauma, and DBP response
SES and childhood trauma on DBP response to social strain to social conflict (L.ratio = 6.46, p = 0.039).
is not driven by racial differences. However, it should be
noted that this analysis may have been underpowered to
detect small interaction effects. Discussion

An additional exploratory analysis probed the heterogene- The present study examined the impact of early life SES
ity within the middle early life SES group, which was com- and childhood trauma on cardiovascular response to daily
prised of parents who had completed technical degrees and life social conflict and task strain in healthy mid-life adults.
those who had initiated but not completed college. The latter Results are in line with expectations and previous literature
may be indicative of contexts (e.g., mental health struggles, demonstrating no effect of early life SES on cardiovascu-
unexpected life events, earlier age of childbirth), which may lar reactivity [18–20], but differ from other studies which
uniquely impact early life environments. The fully identified demonstrate both inverse and positive associations between
model investigating DBP response to social conflict was re- early life SES and cardiovascular reactivity [11–17]. A
run with middle early life SES separated into the two afore- recent meta-analysis probing the relationship between gen-
mentioned sub-categories. The model demonstrated similar eral SES (whether that be adult or child) and cardiovascular
three-way interactions for both middle SES groups, although reactivity does not show clear evidence that SES is related
the latter did not reach statistical significance (𝛾 = −1.01, to laboratory measures of reactivity, although the relation-
p = 0.011; 𝛾 = −0.94, p = 0.068, respectively). ship between SES and cardiovascular reactivity may vary by
As initially proposed, the model for which a three-way stressor type and high SES may be linked with faster recov-
interaction emerged between early life SES, childhood ery to laboratory stress [48]. The present study differed from
trauma, and DBP response to social conflict was broken the majority of previous work [23] in that we found no main
down by type of childhood trauma (neglect only, abuse only, effects of childhood trauma on cardiovascular response to
both neglect and abuse, and none). No significant independ- stress in daily life. Rather, results provide little evidence that
ent three-way interaction terms emerged between any trauma exposures to childhood trauma may interact with early life
group, social conflict, and middle SES. SES to predict stress responsivity. The observed interactions,
however, were inconsistent and not aligned with predictions.
Sensitivity Analysis Controlling for Adult SES The inclusion The present study observed an interaction effect between
of adult education and income as covariates did not alter childhood trauma and early life SES in only one out of the

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 811

four domains tested. In the models investigating cardiovas- due to decreased availability of physical resources [51], thus
cular response to social conflict, the middle childhood SES prompting greater reactivity to social conflict.
group who had not experienced childhood trauma showed Contrary to much of the prior work done in a laboratory
greater diastolic blood pressure response than their lower setting, the present study did not observe an overall effect of
and higher early life SES peers (Fig. 1). Likewise, among the childhood trauma on cardiovascular response to daily stress. To
middle SES individuals, those with a history of childhood our knowledge, the present study is the second empirical work
trauma showed significantly lower DBP reactivity to social to investigate the link between childhood trauma and ambula-
conflict than those without a history of childhood trauma. tory cardiovascular responses to daily stress. The first, which
No significant interactions appeared when investigating SBP investigated the impact of childhood bereavement on SBP
response to social conflict. Unlike the measure of social con- response to daily life stress over the course of 24 h, likewise
flict, neither model investigating ABP response to task strain observed no differences in cardiovascular response by childhood
demonstrated a significant three-way interaction between bereavement, a type of early life adversity [22]. It may be that
childhood trauma, early life SES, and task strain response. the “blunting” effect of early life adversity only manifests when
The fact that the interaction effect was most pronounced in measuring cardiovascular reactivity to acute laboratory stress,
the middle SES group departs from expectations derived from and not in relation to daily life stress in the natural environment.
the adaptive calibration model. We had predicted that the low Daily life stressors differ from those manipulated in a labora-
early life SES group would show marked trauma-dependent tory setting. Laboratory stress paradigms require participants
disparities in reactivity, rather than the middle early life SES to perform relatively atypical tasks, such the preparation and
group. The unexpected observed findings are not explained delivery of a speech or the public performance of mathematical
by either race differences between early life SES groups or operations [52]. Daily life stressors, on the other hand, often
heterogeneity within the middle SES group. A recent p-curve involve routine or more familiar tasks and commonly take place
meta-analysis conducted by Hosseini-Kamkar et al. may help with known individuals, such as coworkers, family, or spouses.
interpret these results [49]. Researchers found evidence for These daily life stressors may be deemed as less foreign, and
an inverted U-shaped relationship between severity of adver- thus less threatening than those manufactured in the laboratory.
sity and cortisol stress reactivity, where exposure to trauma In line with the ACM, it may be that individuals reared in trau-
was related to blunted reactivity while low/medium adversity matic environments develop blunted physiological reactions to
was inconsistently related to hyper-reactivity. It may be that foreign/atypical stressors (such as those in laboratory settings)
growing up in a “middle” SES home without experiences of but not to daily hassles, more predictable, or previously encoun-
trauma prompts hyper-reactivity to stress, while growing up in tered stressors. Our understanding of the relationships between
a “middle” SES home with trauma exposure prompts blunted adverse childhood experiences and physiological reactivity
reactivity. This possibility, however, is speculative. Overall, would be strengthened by further investigation of various types
it is not clear what characteristics of this group made them and domains of stressors.
apparently more vulnerable to the effects of early childhood The present study has several strengths worth mentioning.
trauma in terms of its association with stress reactivity. Future First, as the first investigation to synthesize the literatures link-
research may need to incorporate additional and more sensitive ing childhood trauma and SES to cardiovascular reactivity,
measures of early life SES (e.g., measures of income as well results provide a practical test of the adaptive calibration model
as education) in order to better characterize the early socioeco- as it applies to early life social contextual factors and socioeco-
nomic situation. SES indicators represent unique concepts and nomic disadvantage. Likewise, the present study investigated
predict independent variance in health outcomes [35]. cardiovascular response to two common stressors: social and
Likewise, relationships between childhood socioeco- task strain, allowing us to compare effects across two different
nomic disadvantage and trauma and cardiovascular reactiv- stressor domains. Additionally, the present analysis utilized an
ity in the present study differ slightly by the type of daily EMA design to investigate stress reactivity in the natural envi-
life stressor encountered, with some effects present for ronment. This momentary assessment of stress provided for
responses involving social conflict. It may be that sensitiv- more frequent and real-time depictions of daily life experiences
ity to naturalistic social stressors is influenced by the early than global self-report assessments, and the assessment of stress
life environment, while sensitivity to nonsocial, or task, reactivity in a manner potentially more equivalent across the
stressors is not. Some evidence suggests that individuals SES gradient.
who perceive themselves to be of lower social standing show Despite the strengths of the present study design, key limita-
greater neural activity in the face of negative social evalu- tions are worth noting. First, the present work utilized parental
ation than their peers of higher social standing [50], and education to measure early life SES. Future work should incor-
are more likely to perceive social threat during ambiguous porate additional indicators of early life SES such as family
social situations [11]. It may be that individuals from a lower income or wealth, to better characterize socioeconomic cir-
SES background place greater emphasis on social resources cumstances in childhood. Likewise, the present study grouped

13
812 International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813

individuals whose parents had college degrees and those with Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
advanced degrees into one “high” early life SES group due participants included in the study.
to sample size constraints. There may be differences between
these two groups that could impact their association with car- Conflict of Interest The authors declare no competing interests.
diovascular stress responding. This may be a domain for future
exploration. Second, although inclusion of two distinct domains
of stress (task strain and social conflict) is a key strength of References
the study, not all domains of daily stress are captured by these
indices. Third, given the complexity of the models, the present 1. Ferraro KF, Schafer MH, Wilkinson LR. Childhood disadvantage
analyses may have been underpowered to detect small interac- and health problems in middle and later life: early imprints on
physical health? Am Sociol Rev. 2016;81(1):107–33.
tion effects. Fourth, the present study recruited a sample of 2. Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Smith GD. Is the association between
healthy, middle-aged adults who were higher SES than the childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mor-
national average and primarily white. These factors limit the tality established? Update of a systematic review. J Epidemiol
generalizability of the study and contribute to a body of lit- Community Health. 2008;62(5):387–90.
3. Pollitt RA, Rose KM, Kaufman JS. Evaluating the evidence for
erature normed on white individuals. While use of EMA is a models of life course socioeconomic factors and cardiovascular
key strength of the study, we lack comparable laboratory meas- outcomes: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2005;5(1):7.
ures of stress reactivity. Although we have previously shown 4. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Chen E, Matthews KA. Child-
moderate links between blood pressure responses to laboratory hood socioeconomic status and adult health. Ann N Y Acad Sci.
2010;1186(1):37–55.
stressors and ambulatory measures of the type used here [53], 5. Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Risky families: family social
it is unknown whether SES and trauma associations with stress environments and the mental and physical health of offspring.
reactivity in natural settings differ from those in the laboratory. Psychol Bull. 2002;128(2):330.
Overall, the present work integrates literatures on early 6. Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. The adaptive calibra-
tion model of stress responsivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
life SES and childhood trauma with respect to their joint 2011;35(7):1562–92.
influence on physiological reactivity. Neither early life SES 7. Krantz DS, Manuck SB, Wing RR. Psychological stressors and
nor childhood trauma independently predicted cardiovascu- task variables as elicitors of reactivity. Handbook of stress, reac-
lar responses to daily life stress. Likewise, across most of the tivity, and cardiovascular disease. 1986:85–107.
8. Chida Y, Steptoe A. Greater cardiovascular responses to labora-
measures in this study, we could not confirm that childhood tory mental stress are associated with poor subsequent cardiovas-
SES and trauma interact to predict cardiovascular reactivity cular risk status: a meta-analysis of prospective evidence. Hyper-
in adulthood. Although not consistent with our predictions, tension. 2010;55(4):1026–32.
some of the findings here point to the possibility that social 9. Carroll D, Ginty AT, Der G, Hunt K, Benzeval M, Phillips AC.
Increased blood pressure reactions to acute mental stress are asso-
contextual factors may moderate effects of early life SES ciated with 16-year cardiovascular disease mortality. Psychophysi-
on stress response systems. Future research should continue ology. 2012;49(10):1444–8. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1111/j.​1469-​8986.​
to explore the nature of economic and social influences in 2012.​01463.x.
childhood that trigger changes in stress responding, how 10. Carroll D, Ginty AT, Whittaker AC, Lovallo WR, de Rooij SR.
The behavioural, cognitive, and neural corollaries of blunted car-
stress responding differs by type of stressor and environ- diovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress.
ment in which stressors occur (i.e., laboratory or daily life), Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;77:74–86.
and the development of these effects across the life span. 11. Chen E, Langer DA, Raphaelson YE, Matthews KA. Socioeco-
nomic status and health in adolescents: the role of stress interpre-
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplemen- tations. Child Dev. 2004;75(4):1039–52.
tary material available at https://d​ oi.o​ rg/1​ 0.1​ 007/s​ 12529-0​ 22-1​ 0141-2. 12. Chen E, Matthews KA. Cognitive appraisal biases: an approach to
understanding the relation between socioeconomic status and cardio-
Acknowledgements We would like to express our gratitude to the par- vascular reactivity in children. Ann Behav Med. 2001;23(2):101–11.
ticipants who committed their time and shared their daily life experiences 13. Gump BB, Matthews KA, Räikkönen K. Modeling relationships
with us over the course of this study. We would also like to acknowledge among socioeconomic status, hostility, cardiovascular reactivity,
Dr. Barbara Anderson for her contribution to data collection. and left ventricular mass in African American and White children.
Health Psychol. 1999;18(2):140.
Funding This research is funded by the Heart Lung and Blood Institute 14. Kapuku GK, Treiber FA, Davis HC. Relationships among
(4T32HL007560) and the National Institutes of Aging (AG041778). socioeconomic status, stress induced changes in cortisol, and
blood pressure in African American males. Ann Behav Med.
Declarations 2002;24(4):320–5.
15. Wilson DK, Kliewer W, Plybon L, Sica DA. Socioeconomic sta-
Ethnical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving tus and blood pressure reactivity in healthy black adolescents.
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Hypertension. 2000;35(1):496–500.
the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 16. Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty and health: cumula-
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical tive risk exposure and stress dysregulation. Psychol Sci.
standards. 2007;18(11):953–7.

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine (2023) 30:801–813 813

17. Musante L, Treiber FA, Kapuku G, Moore D, Davis H, Strong among female middle aged community volunteers. Brain Behav
WB. The effects of life events on cardiovascular reactivity to Immun. 2009;23(5):677–83.
behavioral stressors as a function of socioeconomic status, eth- 37. Schmeer KK, Yoon A. Socioeconomic status inequalities in
nicity, and sex. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(6):760–7. low-grade inflammation during childhood. Arch Dis Child.
18. Boylan JM, Jennings JR, Matthews KA. Childhood socioeco- 2016;101(11):1043–7.
nomic status and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery among 38. Walker EA, Unutzer J, Rutter C, et al. Costs of health care use
Black and White men: mitigating effects of psychological by women HMO members with a history of childhood abuse
resources. Health Psychol. 2016;35(9):957. and neglect. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(7):609–13.
19. Evans GW, Exner-Cortens D, Kim P, Bartholomew D. Childhood 39. Matthews KA, Chang Y-F, Thurston RC, Bromberger JT. Child
poverty and late adolescents’ blood pressure reactivity to and recov- abuse is related to inflammation in mid-life women: role of obe-
ery from an acute stressor: the mediating role of family conflict. sity. Brain Behav Immun. 2014;36:29–34.
Psychosom Med. 2013;75(7):691. 40. Semsar N, Mousavi Z, Tran M-LM, Kuhlman KR. Dysregu-
20. Jackson RW, Treiber FA, Turner JR, Davis H, Strong WB. lated arousal as a pathway linking childhood neglect and clini-
Effects of race, sex, and socioeconomic status upon cardiovas- cal sleep disturbances in adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect.
cular stress responsivity and recovery in youth. Int J Psycho- 2021;122:105306.
physiol. 1999;31(2):111–9. 41. Marler MR, Jacob RG, Lehoczky JP, Shapiro AP. The statisti-
21. Heim C, Newport DJ, Heit S, et al. Pituitary-adrenal and auto- cal analysis of treatment effects in 24-hour ambulatory blood
nomic responses to stress in women after sexual and physical pressure recordings. Stat Med. 1988;7(6):697–716.
abuse in childhood. JAMA. 2000;284(5):592–7. 42. Kamarck TW, Shiffman SM, Smithline L, et al. Effects of task
22. Luecken LJ, Kraft A, Appelhans BM, Enders C. Emotional and strain, social conflict, and emotional activation on ambulatory car-
cardiovascular sensitization to daily stress following childhood diovascular activity: daily life consequences of recurring stress in a
parental loss. Dev Psychol. 2009;45(1):296. multiethnic adult sample. Health Psychol. 1998;17(1):17.
23. Brindle RC, Pearson A, Ginty AT. Adverse childhood experi- 43. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical com-
ences (ACEs) relate to blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reac- puting. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
tivity to acute laboratory stress: a systematic review and meta- 2022. https://​www.R-​proje​ct.​org/.
analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2022:104530. 44. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, et al. Package ‘nlme’. Linear and
24. Goodman RD, Miller MD, West-Olatunji CA. Traumatic stress, soci- Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models, version. 2017:3–1.
oeconomic status, and academic achievement among primary school 45. Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-
students. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2012;4(3):252. sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol
25. DeLongis A, Folkman S, Lazarus RS. The impact of daily Methods. 2007;12(2):121.
stress on health and mood: psychological and social resources 46. Aiken LS, Stephen G. Multiple regression: testing and interpreting
as mediators. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(3):486. interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991.
26. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, et al. Development and 47. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood adver-
validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma sity and neural development: deprivation and threat as dis-
Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27(2):169–90. tinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
27. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of child- 2014;47:578–91.
hood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading 48. Boylan JM, Cundiff JM, Matthews KA. Socioeconomic status and
causes of death in adults: the Adverse Childhood Experiences cardiovascular responses to standardized stressors: a systematic
(ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14(4):245–58. review and meta-analysis. Psychosom Med. 2018;80(3):278–93.
28. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary 49. Hosseini-Kamkar N, Lowe C, Morton JB. The differential calibra-
assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:1–32. tion of the HPA axis as a function of trauma versus adversity: a
29. Stepanikova I, Oates GR. Perceived discrimination and privilege systematic review and p-curve meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehav
in health care: the role of socioeconomic status and race. Am J Rev. 2021;127:54–135.
Prev Med. 2017;52(1):S86–94. 50. Muscatell KA, Dedovic K, Slavich GM, et al. Neural mechanisms
30. Karasek Jr RA. Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental linking social status and inflammatory responses to social stress.
strain: implications for job redesign. Administrative science 2016;11(6):915–22.
quarterly. 1979:285–308. 51. Piff PK, Kraus MW, Côté S, Cheng BH, Keltner D. Having less,
31. Kamarck TW, Shiffman S, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Muldoon MF, Tep- giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. J
per P. Daily psychological demands are associated with six- Pers Soc Psychol. 2010;99(5):771.
year progression of carotid artery atherosclerosis: the Pittsburgh 52. Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier
Healthy Heart Project. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(4):432. Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological
32. Kamarck TW, Li X, Wright AG, Muldoon MF, Manuck SB. stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology.
Ambulatory blood pressure reactivity as a moderator in the 1993;28(1–2):76–81.
association between daily life psychosocial stress and carotid 53. Kamarck TW, Schwartz JE, Janicki DL, Shiffman S, Raynor DA.
artery atherosclerosis. Psychosom Med. 2018;80(8):774–82. Correspondence between laboratory and ambulatory measures of
33. Joseph NT, Kamarck TW, Muldoon MF, Manuck SB. Daily mari- cardiovascular reactivity: a multilevel modeling approach. Psy-
tal interaction quality and carotid artery intima medial thickness chophysiology. 2003;40(5):675–83.
in healthy middle aged adults. Psychosom Med. 2014;76(5):347.
34. De Vogli R, Chandola T, Marmot MG. Negative aspects Publisher's Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
of close relationships and heart disease. Arch Intern Med. jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
2007;167(18):1951–7.
35. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, et al. Socioeconomic Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds
status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA. exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the
2005;294(22):2879–88. author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted
36. Phillips JE, Marsland AL, Flory JD, Muldoon MF, Cohen S, manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of
Manuck SB. Parental education is related to C-reactive protein such publishing agreement and applicable law.

13
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine is a copyright of Springer, 2023. All Rights
Reserved.

You might also like