You are on page 1of 1

VISUAL ADAPTATION AND RETINAL GAIN CONTROLS 287

size (Biersdorf et al., 1965). This experiment for either rods or cones over much of the visual
indicates that the gain of the retina is affected at range of backgrounds (Rushton, 1962). This can be
the same light levels as the visual threshold for large seen in Figs. 15 and 16. Figure 15 is from Alpern
targets. A further question one might raise is, where and Pugh (1974) and shows the fraction of human
in the retina is the site of this gain change? Much rhodopsin, the rod pigment, bleached as a function
of the physiological work we present below was of a steady background. Even up to rod saturation
motivated by this question. Prior to the (5" 108 quanta(507 nm) (deg 2 s)-~) the rod pigment
physiological investigation, this question of the site has been bleached away less than 2°70, but the visual
in the retina of gain control was investigated threshold would have risen by a factor of more than
psychophysically and this work led to the two 107, a discrepancy in prediction. Figure 16 shows
concepts of gain control by pigment depletion, a similar result for human cones, from Alpern et
which has been shown to be only a minor factor ai. (1971). The cone pigment has been only bleached
in the control of retinal gain, and the adaptation by half at about 104s trolands which is near the top
pool, which still motivates research. of the photopic range. So we can rule out pigment
2.1.6.3. Pigment depletion. Vertebrate visual depletion as playing a major role in human light
pigments are bleached by light into non-receptive adaptation, though it has some effect on photopic
intermediates which must be regenerated to a (cone-driven) sensitivity (Boynton and Whitten,
receptive state enzymatically. It was thought for 1970; Valeton and van Norren, 1983). It is
some time that visual pigment depletion, resulting interesting that photopigment depletion began to be
from bleaching by the background light, was the doubted as an explanation of light adaptation quite
main cause of light adaptation (Hecht, 1924). This a long time ago, from experiments on increment
would put the site of adaptation right at the initial threshold and photopigment concentration in the
transduction stage in vision. However, it is now frog retina conducted by Granit et al. (1939). In
known that the pigment regenerates so quickly that those experiments ERG thresholds were increased
there is a rather small amount of pigment bleached by log units by backgrounds which had no
1.0

0.8 • :,\! '\ \ ; .


r, .-.\.
o
"~
o 0.6 .~ •.°
L-

575 n m
~ 0.4
"!\.

0.2
500 n m
10 sec W h i t e

0 , I I I I I I I
12 13 14 15
Log q u a n t a sec -I c m -~ o f r e t i n a at t h e c o r n e a

FIG. 15. Pigment depletion in rods caused by steady illumination. The amount of the rhodopsin in a 5 deg field located
18 deg peripheral to the fovea in the temporal retina was measured with a retinal densitometer (described in Alpern et al.,
1971 and in Alpern and Pugh, 1974)• Plotted vertically is the fractional amount of rhodopsin left in the steady state at
the background illumination plotted on the horizontal scale. The three curves are for steady lights of 500 nm, 575 nm,
and white light• The 500 and 575 nm lights were obtained with the white source and interference filters• The results to
concentrate on are the 500 nm results, since 500 nm is near the peak wavelength of absorption of rhodopsin and so will
give the greatest pigment depletion at the lowest retinal illumination. The half bleaching point for the 500 nm curve is at
about 13.2 log quanta(cm 2 s)-' which is equivalent to about 10.2 log quanta(deg2 s)-1, the more familiar unit of retinal
illumination [also equivalent to about 4.3 log (scotopic) td]. These values of quanta are referred to the cornea and do not
take into account losses in the eye. From Alpern and Pugh 0974).
PRR3-H*

You might also like