You are on page 1of 3

letters to nature

21. Zahavi, A. The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle). J. Theor. Biol. 67, 603–605
(1977).
22. Hamilton, W. D. & Zuk, M. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science 218,
384–387 (1982).
23. Møller, A. P. Female swallow preference for symmetrical male sexual ornaments. Nature 357, 238–240
(1992).
24. Slagsvold, T. & Lifjeld, J. T. Plumage colour is a condition-dependent sexual trait in male pied
flycatchers. Evolution 46, 825–828 (1992).
25. Sætre, G.-P., Fossnes, T. & Slagsvold, T. Food provisioning in the pied flycatcher: do females gain direct
benefits from choosing bright-coloured males? J. Anim. Ecol. 64, 21–30 (1995).
26. Sætre, G.-P., Dale, S. & Slagsvold, T. Female pied flycatchers prefer brightly coloured males. Anim.
Behav. 48, 1407–1416 (1994).
27. Andersson, M. Sexual Selection (Princeton Univ. Press, NJ, 1994).
28. Felsenstein, J. PHYLIP: Phylogeny Inference Package, Version 3.5c (Univ. Washington, Seattle, 1993).
29. Sætre, G.-P., Král, M. & Bureš, S. Differential species recognition abilities of males and females in a
flycatcher hybrid zone. J. Avian Biol. (in the press).
30. Gustafsson, L., Qvarnström, A. & Sheldon, B. Trade-offs between life history traits and a sexually
selected character in male collared flycatchers. Nature 375, 813–815 (1995).

Acknowledgements. We thank V. Bičı́k, O. H. Brekke, C. Chappuis, K. Clausen, E. Curio, B. Hallmann, K.


Horáčková, K. Jakobsen, H. Källander, F. Krause, J. T. Lifjeld, J. Ormerod, M. Panayiotopoulou, T.
Slagsvold and Y. Tsougrakis. Financial support was provided by the Norwegian Research Council.

Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to G.P.S. (e-mail: glennp@darwin. Figure 1 Priming effects across patients (D.E., J.G., T.S.) and control subjects for
uio.no).
regular and irregular past tense primes and for semantic primes. Effects are
expressed as response proportions (mean priming effect for each condition as a
proportion of mean control reaction-time for that condition) to normalize for
differences in base reaction-time between subjects.
Dissociating types of mental
computation inflectional affix: as in forms such as ‘jumps’ or ‘smiling’ (decom-
posable into {jump} þ {−s} and {smile} þ {−ing})8,9. Such patients
William D. Marslen-Wilson* & Lorraine K. Tyler*
should also have problems in the access and interpretation of
Centre for Speech and Language, Birkbeck College, University of London, regular past tense forms such as ‘jumped’ or ‘smiled’ which again
Malet Street, London WC1E 7HY, UK involve the combination of a stem with a regular inflectional affix.
* Both authors contributed equally to this work. The crucial question is whether they will show the same deficit for
.........................................................................................................................
irregular forms such as ‘gave’ or ‘taught’, where the morphological
A fundamental issue in the study of cognition and the brain is the relationship between prime and target does not involve the same
nature of mental computation. How far does this depend on types of combinatorial operation.
internally represented systems of rules, expressed as strings of To test this required appropriate experimental tasks, using
symbols with a syntax, as opposed to more distributed neural auditory rather than visual materials, and avoiding spoken
systems, operating subsymbolically and without syntax? The responses, because both patients have difficulty in reading text
mental representation of the regular and irregular past tense of and in producing speech11,12. Instead, we used an auditory priming
the English verb has become a crucial test case for this debate. task, where a spoken target word is immediately preceded by a
Single-mechanism approaches argue that current multilayer con- spoken prime word, and the listener makes a timed lexical decision
nectionist networks can account for the learning and representa- response to the target (‘is this a word or not?’). For unimpaired
tion both of regular and of irregular forms1,2. Dual-mechanism subjects, responses to a target word speed up when it is preceded by
approaches, although accepting connectionist accounts for the a morphologically related prime (as in the prime/target sequence
irregular forms, argue that a symbolic, rule-based system is ‘jumped/jump’ or by a semantically related prime (as in ‘swan/
required to explain the properties of the regular past tense and, goose’), but not when the relationship is purely phonological (as in
by extension, the properties of language and cognition in gen- ‘gravy/grave’). Earlier studies confirmed that these patients could
eral3–5. We show here that the regular and irregular past tense are perform this task and that they showed normal levels of semantic
supported by different neural systems, which can become dis- priming9,10,13.
sociated by damage to the brain6,7. This is evidence for functional The two patients and six age-matched normal controls were
and neurological distinctions in the types of mental computation tested in an auditory priming experiment that contrasted priming
that support these different aspects of linguistic and cognitive effects for regular and irregular past tenses (Table 1). For the regular
performance. past tense condition, the target word (for example ‘jump’) shares its
This research brings new evidence to bear on the mental repre- stem morpheme with the prime, always a regular inflected past tense
sentation of the regular and irregular past tense in English, by form (for example ‘jumped’), and would normally show priming
examining the performance of aphasic patients with acquired relative to a control prime (for example ‘locked’) that has no
neurological damage. If regular and irregular forms are mentally morphological or semantic relation with the target. Similarly, for
represented and processed in fundamentally the same way, then the irregular past tense conditions, the target (such as ‘give’) shares
both should be affected in similar ways by damage to the brain that an underlying morpheme with the prime, always an irregular
disrupts morphological processing systems. If there are two separate inflected form (such as ‘gave’), and this should also produce
underlying systems engaged, respectively, by the regular and irreg- priming relative to an unrelated prime (such as ‘shows’). In each
ular morphology, then it should be possible to find dissociations in case, we assume that priming reflects the repeated activation, by the
performance between these two morphological domains. prime and the target, of the same underlying lexical representations.
We selected two aphasic patients (J.G. and D.E.) with well For comparison, we included a semantic priming condition
documented difficulties in the comprehension and production of (Table 1), where the prime word (such as ‘swan’) is preceded by a
inflected forms in English8. Both patients have typical ‘agrammatic’ semantically (but not associatively) related prime word (‘goose’),
speech, which is hesitant and rarely contains inflected words8–10. In which should elicit priming for both controls and patients, and a
tests of their ability to interpret morphologically complex words, phonological condition where the prime and target were only
they can access the stems of such words, but have consistent phonologically related (as in pairs such as ‘gravy/grave’).
difficulties in interpreting the combination of the stem with an Control subjects, consistent with results for other groups of
Nature © Macmillan Publishers Ltd 1997
592 NATURE | VOL 387 | 5 JUNE 1997
letters to nature
Table 1 Sample stimulus set is entered as a factor. In each analysis there is a three-way interaction
Condition Test prime Control prime Target word
between patient, regularity and priming: for the comparison
............................................................................................................................................................................. between D.E. and T.S., F½1; 26ÿ ¼ 4:63, P ¼ 0:041; for J.G. and
Regular past jumped locked jump
............................................................................................................................................................................. T.S., F½1; 27ÿ ¼ 4:99, P ¼ 0:034.
Irregular past found shows find
............................................................................................................................................................................. These results make two important points about types of mental
Semantic swan hay goose computation and their neural substrate. First, they demonstrate that
.............................................................................................................................................................................
Phonological gravy sherry grave the regular and irregular past tenses are neurologically dissociable,
.............................................................................................................................................................................
with the appropriate on-line tasks revealing deficits in the access of
regular but not irregular inflected forms for D.E. and J.G., and the
Table 2 Mean lexical decision response times and error rate converse effect for T.S. Second, they suggest that the two morpho-
Controls D.E. J.G. T.S. logical categories ally themselves with different types of mental
computation. The deficits of D.E. and J.G. for the regular past tense
Condition Test Control Test Control Test Control Test Control
............................................................................................................................................................................. are consistent with earlier evidence that these patients have marked
Regular past 809 852 1,115 1,021 1,150 976 998 1,119
(0) (2) (10) (15) (15) (15) (0) (0)
deficits in combinatorial operations involving morphologically
............................................................................................................................................................................. complex words. The irregular past tenses, which have been relatively
Irregular past 805 892 1,047 1,269 1,041 1,086 1,056 1,106
(0) (2) (10) (10) (10) (15) (0) (0) spared for D.E. and J.G., seem to fit with the pattern of evidence that
.............................................................................................................................................................................
Semantic 707 809 772 871 848 943 1,250 1,203 suggests these patients can still access the semantic properties of
(2) (5) (0) (5) (5) (15) (0) (0)
.............................................................................................................................................................................
morphologically simple words. T.S., in contrast, who shows no
Phonological 867 897 950 1,156 1,095 1,146 1,085 1,126 semantic priming, also shows no priming for the irregular past tense
(0) (0) (10) (25) (10) (25) (0) (0) relative to the normal levels of priming he achieves for the regular
.............................................................................................................................................................................
Times are in ms, error rate is in percent (in parentheses). past tense.
The specific challenge this poses for single-mechanism connec-
tionist accounts is to show how a neural network, exposed to the
unimpaired adults in auditory priming tasks, showed faster same training input as a child learning English, can learn to
responses both for regular and irregular past tense forms (Table partition its representations of regular and irregular morphology
2), with no interaction between regularity and priming in such a way that (1) its end state is functionally separable into
(MinF9½1; 39ÿ ¼ 1:56, P . 0:20). This pattern held for each control apparently combinatorial and non-combinatorial operations, and
subject individually, none of whom showed an interaction between (2) these two aspects of its function are doubly dissociable by
regularity and priming. It is also the pattern shown by a group of 25 different types of damage to the network. M
.........................................................................................................................
young adults tested on the same materials. J.G. and D.E., in contrast,
produced distinctively different responses to the two types of past Methods
tense prime. They showed positive priming effects for the irregular Subjects. The 6 control subjects ranged in age from 58 to 66 years and had no
past tense, but significant interference for the regular past tense. history of neurological impairment.
This was reflected in a significant interaction between regularity D.E. (aged 41 years at testing) is a right-handed man who suffered a cardio-
and priming for both J.G. (F½1; 27ÿ ¼ 6:33, P , 0:01) and D.E. vascular accident (CVA) in 1970. A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan
(F½1; 27ÿ ¼ 4:68, P , 0:05). Consistent with our earlier work with taken in 1996 revealed a large left-hemisphere (LH) lesion affecting the middle
these patients, they show impairments for the regular inflectional and posterior parts of the frontal lobe and most of the temporal lobe. There was
morphology, but this does not extend to the irregular morphology no damage to the parietal lobe and no abnormality in the right hemisphere
as well. (RH).
This cannot be interpreted as a phonological priming effect, J.G. (aged 66 at testing) is a right-handed man who suffered a left tempero-
based on the phonetic similarity between prime and target word, parietal CVA in 1980. A positron-emission tomography (PET) scan carried out
because the regular past tense primes, with their unchanged stems in 1992 showed no active metabolism in his LH.
(as in ‘jumped/jump’ or ‘danced/dance’), are phonologically more T.S. (aged 50 at testing) is a right-handed man with a 16-year history of
similar to their targets than the irregular past tense primes, which vascular problems culminating in a RH middle cerebral haemorrhage in
typically undergo some form of stem change (as in ‘gave/give’, 1995. A computerized tomography (CT) scan in October 1996 revealed RH
‘crept/creep’). Nonetheless, it is the less similar irregular past tenses inferior parietal and temporal damage and changes to frontal and occipital
that prime, and not the regulars. Furthermore, purely phonological lobes, as well as patchy ischaemic damage to LH frontal, parietal and
primes did not produce significant effects, either for the controls temporal lobes.
(MinF9½1; 26ÿ ¼ 1:22, not significant), or for J.G. (F , 1) or D.E. We assessed T.S.’s language comprehension and production using standard
(F½1; 13ÿ ¼ 3:23, P ¼ 0:96). tests. On the BDAE14 he showed the profile typical of a Broca’s aphasic with a
Turning to the semantic primes, these produced consistent effects mild comprehension deficit. His speech output was non-fluent with minimal
throughout, with significant facilitation both for the controls syntax and few complex words. Describing the ‘Cookie theft’ picture14 he said:
(MinF9½1; 22ÿ ¼ 16:99, P , 0:001), and for J.G. (F½1; 13ÿ ¼ 10:18, kitchen room.. lady washing.. accident sink flow.. water.. splash, splash, splash..
P , 0:001) and D.E. (F½1; 15ÿ ¼ 4:32, P , 0:05). This suggests a um.. play dry.. boy stool.. cupboard.. cookie jar.. friends.. hands’’. In additional
closer relationship, across subject groups, between semantic primes tests he could repeat single words (56/60 correct) but not sentences (0/36
and irregular past tense primes than between semantic and regular correct)15. His many errors (16/34) on a 3-picture sentence-picture matching
primes. task were all reverse role distractors16. He was poor at making grammatical
This relationship was sustained in subsequent testing of a third judgements, scoring 46/60 correct17. This is an overall pattern of performance
patient (T.S.), also classified as ‘agrammatic’, but with right as well as typical of ‘agrammatic’ aphasics.
left hemisphere damage. T.S. produces the opposite pattern to D.E. Materials and design. Twenty pairs of each prime-target type (Table 1), with
and J.G., with normal performance for the regular past tense targets matched for frequency over conditions, and combined with real word
(F½1; 18ÿ ¼ 4:21, P ¼ 0:05) and no priming for the irregular past and nonword filler pairs, were assigned to two experimental versions. These
tense (F , 1), accompanied by no phonological or semantic prim- were presented to patients and controls in two testing sessions 4 weeks apart,
ing (F , 1). This double dissociation for the regular and irregular such that each target word was heard only once in each session, preceded by
morphology is plotted in Fig. 1, together with the pattern of either the test or control prime. There was a 250 ms interval between prime and
semantic priming effects. Statistical confirmation for this dissocia- target, and a 3 s interval between trials. Lexical decision responses were made by
tion comes from two additional analyses of variance, where patient pressing response keys labelled either yes or no.

Nature © Macmillan Publishers Ltd 1997


NATURE | VOL 387 | 5 JUNE 1997 593
letters to nature
Received 10 October 1996; accepted 1 April 1997. orientation in cat V1. Optical images of orientation columns
1. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. in Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Micro- (Fig. 1a) show a characteristic pattern, with fields of constant or
structure of Cognition Vol. 2 (eds McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E.) 216–271 (MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA, 1986).
slowly changing orientation preference interrupted by local dis-
2. Plunkett, K. & Marchman, V. From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in continuities (‘fractures’ or ‘singularities’)2–4. Multi-unit RFs
children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48, 21–69 (1993). recorded near fractures show a distinctly non-uniform movement
3. Pinker, S. Rules of language. Science 253, 530–535 (1991).
4. Pinker, S. & Prince, A. On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed model of through space, with rapid jumps across fractures and high overlap
language acquisition. Cognition 28, 73–193 (1988). in regions of uniform orientation (Fig. 1a–c). A longer sequence of
5. Prasada, S. & Pinker, S. Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Lang. Cogn.
Processes 8, 1–56 (1993).
roughly uniformly spaced recording sites traversing a singularity
6. Shallice, T. From Neuropyschology to Mental Structure (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1988). shows the same pattern, of RFs clustered within an orientation zone
7. Teuber, H.-L. Physiological psychology. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 9, 267–296 (1955). and no overlap between zones (Fig. 1d). This gives the correspond-
8. Tyler, L. K. Spoken Language Comphension (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1992).
9. Ostrin, R. & Tyler, L. K. Dissociations of lexical function: semantics, syntax and morphology. Cogn. ing region of visual space the appearance of being tiled by abutting
Neuropsychol. 12, 345–389 (1995). clusters of RFs, as opposed to being covered by a smooth progres-
10. Tyler, L. K., Ostrin, R. K., Cooke, M. & Moss, H. E. Automatic access of lexical information in Broca’s
aphasia: against the automaticity hypothesis. Brain Lang. 48, 131–162 (1995).
sion of RFs. Note that at site 2, in the fracture, we recorded distinct
11. Funnell, E. Morphological errors in acquired dyslexia: a case of mistaken identity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. units that responded at two orthogonal orientations at the same
39A, 497–539 (1987). electrode position. Their receptive fields (marked 2 and 29) over-
12. Patterson, K. & Marcel, A. Q. Aphasia, dyslexia and the phonological coding of written words. J. Exp.
Psychol. 29, 307–318 (1977). lapped with RFs of corresponding orientation preference obtained
13. Tyler, L. K. & Ostrin, R. The processing of simple and complex words in an agrammatic patient: at other recording positions. This extends the idea that some
evidence from priming. Neuropsychologia 32, 1001–1013 (1994).
14. Goodglass, H. & Kaplan, E. The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders 2nd edn (Lea & Febinger,
orientation singularities are infinitesimal in extent, with neurons
Philadelphia, 1982). of one orientation preference adjacent to neurons of orthogonal
15. Kay, J., Lesser, R. & Coltheart, M. The Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language in Aphasia (Lawrence orientation preferences1,4. Recordings across cortical regions of
Erlbaum Associates, Hove, 1992).
16. Black, M., Nickels, L. & Byng, S. Patterns of sentence processing deficit: processing simple sentences smoothly changing orientation show that the saltatory RF move-
can be a complex matter. J. Neuroling. 6, 79–101 (1991). ment illustrated in Fig. 1a–d defines the extremes of a more general
17. Linebarger, M., Schwartz, M. & Saffran, E. Sensitivity to grammatical structure in so-called
agrammatic aphasics. Cognition 13, 361–392 (1983).
linear relation between rates of RF movement and orientation
change (Fig. 1e). At the same time, vertical penetrations through
Acknowledgements. We thank M. Cooke, Y. Griffiths, F. Jennings, J. Morris and K. Voice for their
assistance with the studies reported here and R. Wise and the Wellcome Department of Cognitive
the upper cortical layers at any recording site produce multi-unit
Neurology for the MRI scan of D.E. This research was supported by the MRC and the ESRC. RFs that are largely superimposed on each other, with little scatter in
either size or position (Fig. 1f). This suggests that inhomogeneities
in retinotopy are not diluted significantly by RF scatter and that the
relation between retinotopy and the orientation map is maintained
Distortions of visuotopic map faithfully down cortical columns.
We quantified the relation between RF movement and orienta-
match orientation singularities tion shift by measuring changes in RF position as a function of
changes in orientation preference between pairs of neighbouring
in primary visual cortex recording sites (Fig. 2a; also see Methods). The values thus obtained
were then divided by the corresponding intersite distance on the
Aniruddha Das & Charles D. Gilbert cortex to give the rate of RF movement across the cortex as a
The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, New York 10021-6399, USA function of the rate of change of orientation (Fig. 2b). Two features
.........................................................................................................................
of these pooled data (taken from 143 electrode penetrations, in 20
The map of orientation columns in primary visual cortex (V1) is series of recording sites in 13 animals) are noteworthy. We see that
known to show strong local distortions, with a generally smooth the local rate of RF movement is proportional to the local rate of
progression of orientation preference across extended regions of change of orientation (Fig. 2b; Table 1). This would be expected to
cortex, interrupted by sharp jumps (fractures) and point singula- lead to strong and systematic modulations in the overall visuotopic
rities1–4. The map of visual space on V1, in contrast, has been map in register with the varying rates of change of orientation over
assumed to be locally smooth and isotropic. We find, on the the cortical surface (Fig. 3). With RF movement normalized by RF
contrary, that the map of visual space on cat V1 shows strong and dimensions (see Methods and Fig. 2 legend) the linear relation
systematic local distortions in register with inhomogeneities in between rates of RF movement and orientation change holds out to
the orientation map, with the rate of receptive field movement all eccentricities of V1 tested, scaling appropriately with RF size and
across cortex being largely proportional to the local rate of change cortical magnification7. The next noteworthy feature is the slope of
of orientation. This suggests possible systematic local variations the regression line in Fig. 2a relating shifts of RF centre to changes in
in the functional connectivity of short-range lateral connections orientation between neighbouring recording sites. The value of
that underlie local cortical processing. 0.0106 RF diameters per degree of change in orientation (Table 1)
A detailed knowledge of the map of visuotopy on V1 is critical to translates to a shift of 1.9 RF diameters over a 1808 cycle of
understanding the cortical representation and processing of visual orientation preferences. This means that if we were to map the RF
input. Early electrophysiological recordings of visual field position from a cortical site of any particular orientation preference and then
in V1 suggesting that the map of visual space is smooth and locally move one cycle of orientation in any arbitrary direction on cortex to
isotropic5–7. A systematic change in cortical magnification on another site of the same orientation preference, we would have
moving from the centre to the periphery of visual space was believed moved about 2 RF diameters, that is, to a non-overlapping region of
to be the only inhomogeneity present in this map; any deviations visual space.
from smoothness were attributed to random scatter in the positions A number of precautions were taken to minimize, and then check
of receptive fields (RFs). The advent of optical imaging has made it for, bias in our data and to check the robustness of our results.
possible to examine whether the apparent scatter in RF positions Electrode recordings were made ‘blind’ so as not be influenced by
was random or showed a systematic relation to the distribution of knowledge of the orientation map. We checked that there were no
orientation columns. systematic errors of measurement (Fig. 2c legend) by confirming the
We have used a combination of optical imaging of intrinsic lack of correlations in scatter plots of RF shift versus intersite
signals (that is, changes in cortical reflectance linked to metabolic distance (R2 ¼ 0:06 (see supplementary information); Fig. 2c)
changes resulting from local neural activity8,9) and extracellular or of orientation shift versus intersite distance (R2 ¼ 0:09 (see
recording of RFs to explore the relation between maps of space and supplementary information)) in our pooled data. Moreover, these

Nature © Macmillan Publishers Ltd 1997


594 NATURE | VOL 387 | 5 JUNE 1997

You might also like