You are on page 1of 12

Fry's Readability Graph: Clarifications, Validity, and Extension to Level 17

Author(s): Edward Fry


Source: Journal of Reading, Vol. 21, No. 3 (Dec., 1977), pp. 242-252
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the International Reading Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40018802 .
Accessed: 18/06/2014 10:31

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and International Reading Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend
access to Journal of Reading.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Fry's Readability Graph:
Clarifications, Validity,
and Extension to Level 17
EDWARD FRY I find myself in the somewhat
untenable position of having several
Fry is currently director books in print (Fry, 1963, 1972, 1977)
of the reading center that give differing instructions re-
at Rutgers University, garding the inclusion of proper
New Brunswick, New Jersey. nouns when using the Fryreadability
graph. The latest book, a version of
the graph in slide rule form (Fry,
1976), suggests that proper nouns
should be included in the word count,
and this article will discuss brieflythe
reasons for including them. In
addition, I would like to take up some
other areas related to readability
estimates and the use of my graph.
Specifically, a number of questions
have been presented to me regarding
somewhat detailed but very real
problems such as what a syllable is
and what a word is- for example, is
stopped a two syllable word, and is
1945 or IRA a word? A third area
concerns the problems of validityand
reliability of readability scores, and
recent work being done on new ways
to establish this. Finally,an extension
of my graph upward into the college
levels will be presented.
I must confess that when I first
developed the readability graph in
242 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Africa in about 1961, I had no idea training. Next, I added some
that anyone would take it very Americanization (grade levels) and
seriously, or that so many thousands validation of the graph at the
of people would start using it. If I had, secondary level (Fry, 1968) and the
I might have put more care into its primary level (Fry, 1969). It was after
development, but on the other hand, the appearance of these two articles
if it was to be a large research project that American educators began to
with a proper statistical design, it use the graph, first in teacher training
might have never seen the light of classes, then in textbooks.
day. Its original purpose, and its Certainly, readability had been
present purpose, is to aid teachers around for a number of years,
and editors to help children or adults possibly formally beginning with
read better by giving them material Lively and Pressey in 1923, but few
on the proper difficulty level. people outside of reading specialists
At the time of the graph's origin, I and researchers used it. Then
had a Fulbright lectureship at readability began getting great
Makerere College in Uganda, and my surges of momentum from other
purpose was to help a group of formula builders, such as Lorge in
African teachers on a UNESCO 1939, Flesch in 1943, Dale and Chall
training project who were teaching in 1948, and Spache in 1953. When
English as a second language. It was teachers began asking publishers
first published in a British journal about the readability of their books,
(1964) and as an appendix in my the publishers began to take a greater
book, Teaching Faster Reading interest in readability.
(1963), which was also originally The Readability Graph's con-
written for those same Unesco tribution seems to be in simplicity of
teachers. As near as I know, for years use without sacrificing much, if any,
nobody ever used the graph; it was accuracy, and its wide and con-
not reprinted or cited, nor did I get tinuous range from grade one up
any informal feedback about usage. through college. That it was not
From this, I can possibly conclude copyrighted and could be repro-
that American educators do not read duced on one sheet of paper might
British journals, or that its essentially have helped also.
British designations (1000 word level, Surprisingly, few people ask me
2000 word level, etc., and the Oxford what the curved line in the graph
English Readers series book levels) represents (it has little to do with the
were too parochial. It might also be graph's use). Not deterred by this
that readability was not a terribly lack of curiosity, I will tell you
popular topic in the early 1960s; anyway. It is the smoothed mean of
certainly it doesn't use up much the plots of sample passages. If you
space in teacher training books of plot a large number of passages with
that period. a wide range, they will tend to fall
However, under the principle of somewhere near the line. In short, it is
"never throw out your old good ideas, an "eye ball" job. However, my
just dust them off occasionally and friends in higher mathematics tell me
see if they will fly," I started doing a that "smoothing a curve" in this
bit more with readability in teacher manner is just about as accurate as

FRY: . . . Readability Graph 243

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
doing it by complicated formula. the graph (1963 and 1964) said
The grade areas were assigned the nothing about omitting proper
same way, only this time the grade nouns. Somehow or other, possibly
level for each plot was necessary to because of influence of other
delineate concentrations. The grade formulas which had specific instruc-
lines were made perpendicularto the tions about not counting proper
curved line (which is one reason the nouns, Iomitted them in 1968. 1would
curve was made in the first place), like to reverse this decision now and
and they were adjusted a little when say that they should be included.
correlation studies were done with The reasons for doing this are
more material and other formulas, simultaneously subjective, logical,
such as the Spache and the Dale- and empirical. Proper nouns do
Chall. It is of moderate consternation contribute to the difficulty of the
that the grade level areas came out material. It is easier for a child to read
unequal size, but Ichose to follow the "Joe"than "Joseph,"and children or
old researcher's maxim: "When in adults certainly do not skip proper
doubt, believe the data." nouns in most reading. Complaints
Fortunately, time and other re- about using proper nouns do not
search studies have continued to come from teachers, but most often
show the efficacy of the two inputs of from editors of texts who have
the graph, syllables and sentence difficulty in getting the readability
length. Klare (1974-1975), a widely low enough for the grade level at
recognized bibliographer of read- which they hope to sell the book.
ability studies, has summarized: Empirically, both the 1968 article
Unless the user is interested in doing and a recent thesis by Zingman
research, there is littleto be gained from (1977) show that the grade level
choosing a highly complex formula. A designations of the graph are a little
simple 2-variable formula should be on the low side, compared with other
sufficient, especially if one of the vari- formulas
ables is a word or semantic variableand (about a year, in many
the other is a sentence or syntactic instances), when the graph is used
variable... If the count is to be made without proper nouns.
by hand, counting syllables in some A recent study by Britton and
fashion ... is somewhat faster than us- Lumpkin(1977) using a large number
ing most word lists. of samples and comparing the Fry
Zipf s (1965) principle- that higher formula (with proper nouns) and five
frequency words are shorter- seems other formulas plus publisher
intact. designations, also tends to support
the inclusion of proper nouns. The
Include Proper Nouns data in the table show almost perfect
It is always embarrassing to admit agreement in ranking and close
mistakes, especially if they are in agreement in grade level designa-
print and thousands of people know tions.
about them, but I think I made a Britton and Lumpkin,incidentally,
mistake in the 1968 article in which I used my formula slightly differently
included the sentence in the graph from the published directions; they
directions. "Don't count proper averaged a large number of grade
nouns." The first two publications of level designations and thus obtained
244 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A Comparison of Publishers' Designations with Six Readability Formulas
for the Ginn Reading 720 Series (1976)

No. of 100
Publisher's word
Book Level samples Readability
Farr-
Harris- Dale- Jenkins-
Fry8 Jacobson Spache Chall Flesch Patterson
(1-13)b (PP-8+)b (1-3)b (4-16)b (5-17)b (5-17)b

Preprimer 8 1.0 .5 1.3


Primer 8 1.0 1.0 1.6
1 23 1.1 1.2 1.7
21 26 2.2 1.8 1.9
22 26 2.7 2.5 2.3
3, 26 2.7 2.6 2.5
32 20 4.0 3.1 2.7
A, 28 4.2 3.7 5.3
42 29 4.4 3.8 5.3
5 26 5.8 5.3 6.7 6.8 8.7
6 37 6.6 5.6 7.5 7.2 8.9
Source: Abstracted from A Consumer's Guide on Readability by Gwyneth Britton and Margaret
Lumpkin. G. Britton and Associates, Corvallis, Oregon, 1977.
a
The Fry word and syllable count uses proper nouns.

The numbers in parentheses are the range of the formula in grade levels. This also explains why all
formulas are not used at all levels.

grade level designations with a nothing but increase the child's


decimal point. The regulardirections comprehension, pleasure, and in-
call for the averaging of the syllables clination to keep reading.
and sentence length, then entering
the graph to get a whole grade level Syllables and Words
designation. I do not find their Graph users sometimes have a little
procedure objectionable since they bit of trouble in determining syl-
did it with a large number of samples lables. Much of this is caused by a
(on a computer). However, users dissonance between phonetic and
must continually be aware that graphic considerations. To illustrate,
readability scores are estimates and wanted is a two-syllable word but
that individual samples jump around stopped is one-syllable. The quick
a mean score, as was demonstrated answer is, believe what you hear, not
by Coke and Rothkopf (1970) (see what you see. In other words, when
figure). counting syllables, go by speech
By including proper nouns in the sounds. Fortunately, most people,
count, if we err, we err on the side of including children, can fairly ac-
the angels, or perhaps you might say curately determine the number of
on the side of the children, if you per- phonetic syllables in a passage.
ceive the two not to be synonymous. Children don't have any trouble
Causing the teacher to select easier syllabifying Sall-y-is-a-scard-y-cat.
books for the child to read will do The problem comes with literate
FRY: . . . Readability Graph 245

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
teachers who know that many affixes or book on a sample population for
form separate syllables. They do have whom the book is intended.
a leg to stand upon, because
morphology influences syllabifica- Reliability and Validity
tion, but an overriding principle is A readability formula is in many
that every syllable has a separate respects like a reading test, except
vowel sound. instead of testing children, it tests
In most prose counts, there is no written material.Hence, it is properto
problem in defining what a word is, assume that many testing concepts
but for those graph users who should apply. Readability formulas
request a more precise statement, I are not strong in reporting either
suggest the computer definition: A reliabilityor validity.
word is a symbol or group of symbols We can assume that the formulas
bounded by a blank space on either have at least a modest amount of
side. Thus, 1945, &, and IRA are all reliability because they consistently
words. correlate fairly well with each other,
A problem now arises as to how but direct measures and useful
many syllables you allot for these statistics like Standard Error of
strange words. In an effort to keep it Measurement are usually not given.
simple but logical, Isuggest that each As a notable exception, George
symbol receive a syllable count of Spache (1966) reported a probable
one. Thus, 1945 is four syllables, &is error of 3.3 months.
one, and IRA is three. When trying to find the reliabilityof
In the case of initialisms like IRA, a formula, we encounter the problem
where each letter is spoken, the rule that written prose samples contain a
follows the general phonetic syllable good deal of variability.When Coke
principle, and US is easier to read and Rothkopf (1970) programmed a
than USOE. Numbers are similar- computer to continuously sample
there is surface validity to the idea every hundred words for a 20,000
that 43,172 is harder to read than 72, word passage, they found that the
which is harder to read than 2. This readability scores tended to follow a
suggestion might aid those who normal distribution curve. We could
tackle the difficult problem of expect that writers have different
readability of mathematics texts. amounts of variabilityor consistency
Incidentally, I have been, asked in writing on grade level; hence, if
many times about how to use unreliability is found, it could be the
readability formujas on mathematics formula, or it could be the variability
textbooks. There is no simple answer. in writing.
Readability formulas were made for Validity of formulas is approached
prose, not numerical formulas or in a numberof differentways, such as
poetry. The new policy stated above correlations between formulas or
will help in the prose parts of the math correlations with comprehension
text, but the parts that have many scores, with cloze scores, with oral
numbers or mathematical formulas reading errors, with observer
must rely on another type of difficulty judgment, and with written passages
evaluation. I suggest that there is no of known difficulty. My graph has
substitute for trying out the passage been validated by interformula and
246 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Figure 1

Flesch ReadingEase Score


This graph shows an approximately normal distribution of readabilitydifficulty levels
when 200 continuous 100-word samples were rated for readability using the Flesch
Reading Ease Scores on a passage about modern physics. It illustrateswhy it is neces-
sary to take multiple samples from a passage in order to arrive at a true mean score.
Graph taken from Coke and Rothkopf (1970).

comprehension scores (Fry, 1968) erage discrepancy in readability levels


and oral reading errors (Fry, 1969; established by the SEER technique
Paolo, 1977). Indirectly, it is validated and those computed by readability
by studies such as Zingman's (1977) formulas (Spache and Dale-ChaU)
and those by Dulin (1969), who did was less than one grade level.
readability on a number of national Moreover, the SEER technique was
news stories, and by Britton and as valid as the Fry graphed
Lumpkin (1977), who found that the procedure, but took much less time,
graph produced scores similar to an average of only two minutes per
those from other formulas (see table). paragraph." I might point out that if
Another method of estimating you wish to save all this time and get
readability is by using judges. the same degree of validity reported
Developing the SEER (Singer Eyeball by Singer, you have to have thirty-two
Estimate of Readability), Singer judges working for two minutes, then
(1975) had thirty-two college stu- average their findings.
dents judge eight paragraphs rang- Carver (1975-1976) also compared
ing from grades one to seven. He the graph with his Rauding tech-
concluded, "Results revealed the av- nique, which used trained judges for
FRY: . . . Readability Graph 247

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
comparing prose samples against a readability formulas. Ernest Roth-
standard. He also compared the kopf at Bell Laboratories is currently
graph with cloze rankings (Bormuth experimenting with something called
levels). The graph correlated .85 with "functional chaining." In simplified
Rauding technique and .81 with terms, a functional chain is the
Bormuth level. Incidentally, it number of words a typist can con-
correlated .95 with Flesch and .85 tinue typing after the copy has been
with Dale-Chall. removed from sight. This was found
However, both Singer and Carver to be related to the Flesch reading
have demonstrated that it is possible ease index, syntactic complexity,
to judge the difficulty levels of familiarity with topical content, and
unknown writing samples subjec- eye movement patterns during learn-
tively, and this can be seen as a ing. This is similar to the work of
contribution to the validity of Holgerson (1977) who compared eye-
readability formulas. voice span on passages of differing
It might also be noted that Singer, difficultyand with readers at different
and many others, use the graph in a levels.
manner contrary to directions, Hardyck and Petrinovich (1970)
namely, they take the estimated found that when students are asked
grade level based on only one to read easy and hard passages
sample. The directions state that silently while sensitive measures of
three or more samples should be muscle activity in the oral area are
averaged. The graph will yield a recorded, subvocalization increases
grade level score for a 100 word as reading difficulty of material
sample, but the user should be aware increases. In an extension of this
that there is necessarily a sacrifice in work at the Rutgers Reading Center,
both reliabilityand validity. Leo Campbell is using myographsto
A very interesting validation of record oral muscle activity during
readabilityformulas has been done in reading of passages that increase in
journalistic studies. I am mentioning syntactic difficulty and of passages
it here, not because it is new but that increase in vocabulary difficulty.
because it is in literature not always In summary, readability formulas
searched by reading researchers. can be validated by a wide variety of
Journalists tend to use such measures, and their reliability is
techniques as split runs, in which half attested to by intercorrelations.
the papers carry an article written at However, this does not mean that
one grade level and half carry it there is not plenty of work left to do in
written at a lower grade level. They the way of refinements as well as
then sample the readership utilizing basic understandings.
such dependent variables as amount
of people reading the story and Graph Modifications
number of paragraphs read. Writing Several investigators have at-
to lower readability often substan- tempted to refine my readability
tially increases readership (Lyman, graph. Maginnis (1969) extended the
1949; Murphy,1947; Swanson, 1948). graph downward into the preprimer
Researchers are continually look- levels and used it with shorter
ing for new methods of validating passages. Considering the general
248 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Figure 2
GRAPH FOR ESTIMATINGREADABILITY-EXTENDED
bv Edward Fry, Rutgers University Reading Center, New Brunswick, N.J. 08904

Average number of syllables per 100 words

108- 112 116 120 124 128 132 136 140 144 148 152 156 160 164 168 172 176 180 182+
- - - - - .- - - r- . - - - .- - i . ..................... .,,... ,.,. ,.,..,....„,.
25.0+I | | | | 1 | | 1 | | | ^ |
., , ,. ..„.,... ,. 25.0+
20.0 ' ' '**"*" ~~~ - - . - «.««. 20.0
^-^ ~S**'
16.7 - _ JL_^^: - _^___^ ,- 167

(/) g q - ^> r^ \f^ l , , , jr * »j( .-^ __ ___ ^ , . ,iarj.jjl. ,f._. nr.irLf.


i.lii.j;j^m}i rrj. i: ur_ .j. .. . irYit.ifl o o

^- y 7
I 7;7jh- 3j^r\^ //_ 77

i :iEE2i|||E?||s
H

#lEEEEE^
p
- I- LJ- ' t ,i i I I I 1- I- I- I- i- 1- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I- I 1- I- I- I- I- I 1- I- J 2.0
2.0-L
10t-K_ 1lf 120 124 128 132 136 140 144 148 152 156 160 164 168 172 176 180 182 +
;:

Expanded Directions for Working Readability Graph

1. Randomly select three (3) sample passages and count out exactly 100
words each, beginning with the beginning of a sentence. Do count proper
nouns, initializations, and numerals.
2. Count the number of sentences in the hundred words, estimating length of
the fraction of the last sentence to the nearest one-tenth.
3. Count the total number of syllables in the 100-word passage. If you don't
have a hand counter available, an easy way is to simply put a markabove
every syllable over one in each word, then when you get to the end of the
passage, count the number of marks and add 100. Small calculators can
also be used as counters by pushing numeral 1, then push the + sign for
each word or syllable when counting.
4. Entergraph with average sentence length and average numberof syllables;
plot dot where the two lines intersect. Areawhere dot is plotted willgive you
the approximate grade level.
5. If a great deal of variabilityis found in syllable count or sentence count,
putting more samples into the average is desirable.
6. A word is defined as a group of symbols with a space on either side; thus,
Joe, IRA, 1945, and <&are each one word.
7. A syllable is defined as a phonetic syllable. Generally, there are as many
syllables as vowel sounds. Forexample, stopped is one syllable and wanted
is two syllables. When counting syllables for numerals and initializations,
count one syllable for each symbol. Forexample, 1945 is four syllables, IRA
is three syllables, and <&is one syllable.

Note: This "extended graph" does not outmode or render the earlier (1968) version inoperative or
inaccurate; it is an extension. (REPRODUCTION PERMITTED- NO COPYRIGHT)

FRY: . . . Readability Graph 249

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
lack of pinpoint reliability of the noun (subject), verb (predicate),
formulas, this distinction within and sometimes an object. Distance
grade levels is not warranted. I have applies to any word or phrase not a
no reason to think that my graph is part of the kernel. The theory then
any more accurate than Spache's, for states that distance between the
example, and he only knows where a noun and the verb makes the sen-
book is within .6 of a year and 50 tence harder than does distance
percent of the time. Also, on a logical outside the kernel, as in the example
level, a beginning reader is so above.
dependent on the particular basal This part of the theory was
series that he has used in the first half confirmed in a dissertation by
of the year that there seems to be little DePierro (1976), who presented pairs
benefit in determining a generalized of sentences to fifth and sixth graders
readability for that level. Kretschmer and to college undergraduates and
(1976), on the other hand, tried to asked them to recall the sentences;
improve the accuracy of the graph by he also noted theirsilent reading time
adding a set of vocabulary words to and response time after presentation.
be consulted. This tends to It was also confirmed in a master's
complicate the graph's use, and thesis by Weber (1977), who used
unless he or somebody can demon- subjective judgment of junior college
strate that there is a significant students.
improvement in accuracy, we should Two other parts of the theory were
hold this modification in abeyance. not confirmed: that distance before
There have been numerous the kernel caused more difficulty
attempts to improve readability than distance after the kernel, and
determination through studying that distance between noun and verb
syntax variations. Two of these have caused more difficulty than distance
been closely related to the graph and between verb and object.
some work that I have done. At the This research has more implication
twenty-fourth annual meeting of the for writers than for construction of
National Reading Conference, I readability formulas. It suggests that
proposed the Kernel Distance one way writers can lower readability
Theory, which tried to explain why levels is to avoid splitting the kernel of
two sentences containing essentially a sentence; however, this will not
equal words, hence equal length and affect the readability score on most
equal syllables and the same or formulas.
nearly the same meaning, can have Another way of looking at gram-
unequal difficulty (Fry, 1975a). For matical complexity was investigated
example: by Pearson (1974-1975), who pointed
No belief, if injustices and evils are to out that in some specific instances,
be eradicated, can be regarded as longer sentences produced better
infallible. comprehension than short sen-
No belief can be regarded as infallibleif tences. For example, when two short
injustices and evils are to be eradi- sentences were put together and
cated. a "because" was added, questions
The Kernel Distance Theory about the relationship between
defines the kernel of a sentence as the sentences were easier. Pearson

250 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
is undoubtedly correct in specific student and vice versa. These
instances, as is the Kernel Distance variables are all in addition to the
Theory in the noun-verb split in- readability principle that "High
stance, but these specific conditions motivation overcomes high
are statistically not common, and I readability level, but low motivation
believe that Klare's general state- demands a low readabilitylevel"(Fry,
ment about sentence length in- 1975b).
creasing difficulty still holds for This article is intended to update
general use. readers on some background and
some activity in the area of readability
Graph Extension with particular reference to my
With considerable trepidation, I readability graph. Readability
have extended the graph through the continues to be an active area of
college years by simple extrap- research and fortunately an actively
olation. The college year areas are used tool for practicing teachers and
based on the average areas for the curriculum material developers. I
preceding three years. It is known hope that some of the new rules on
that vocabularycontinues to increase word count and the extension to
throughout the college years; how- college level material will prove
ever, I openly confess to not having helpful and that some of the
any data about the difference be- discussion of recent research will
tween thirteenth through sixteenth stimulate others to work in this
grade material. I do hope someone interesting area of the reading field.
will gather some for validation.
In the meantime, I have had References
requests for some kind of objective Bormuth, John R. "Readability: A New Approach."
Reading Research Quarterly, vol. 1 (Fall, 1966), pp.
measurement of materialdifficulty in 79-132.
Gwyneth and Margaret Lumpkin. A Consumer's
the college areas. I am, therefore, Britton, Guide on Readability: Ginn and Company, Ginn
proposing this extension as a relative Reading 720, 1976. Corvallis, Ore.: G. Britton and
Associates, 1977.
difficulty differentiation ratherthan a Carver, Ronald P. "Measuring Prose Difficulty Using the
Rauding Scale." Reading Research Quarterly, vol.
normed score. In other words, I 11, no. 4(1975-1976), pp. 660-85.
Esther U. and Ernst Z. Rothkopf. Note on a
believe that it is somewhat defensible Coke, Simple Algorithm for a Computer Produced Reading
to state that a book scoring at level Ease Score." Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 54
(1970), pp. 208-10.
sixteen is more difficult than a book Dale, Edgar and Jeanne Chall. "A Formula for Predicting
Readability." Educational Research Bulletin, vol. 27
scoring at level fourteen, but it is not (January, 1948), pp. 11-20.
appropriateto say that one is suitable DePierro, Joseph. Some Effects of Sentence Structure
Variables on Reading Ease. Unpublished doctoral
for college seniors and the other for dissertation, Rutgers University, New Brunswick,
N. J., June 1976.
college sophomores. Dulin, K. "Readability Levels of Adult Magazine Ma-
Part of the difficulty in determining terial." The Psychology of Reading Behavior, George
Schick and Merrill May, Eds., pp. 176-80. 18th
college norms is that college Yearbook, National Reading Conference, Clemson
University, Clemson, S.C., 1969.
populations have wide divergences in Flesch, Rudolf F. "A New Readability Yardstick." Journal
of Applied Psychology, vol. 32 (June, 1948), pp. 221-
academic qualifications of students. 33.
College reading ability also tends to Flesch, Rudolf. Marks of Readable Style: A Study in
Adult Education. New York, N.Y.: Bureau of
become very "subject specific." That Publications, Teachers College, Columbia
1943.
means that what may be normal Fry,University, Edward B. Teaching Faster Reading. London:
reading for a physics student could Fry,Cambridge University Press, 1963.
Edward B. A Readability Estimate Graph for Any
be quite difficult for a philosophy English Language Material." Teacher Education.

FRY: . . . Readability Graph 251

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
London: Oxford University Press, May 1964. Psychology, vol. 33 (1949), pp. 78-80.
Fry, Edward B. A Readability Formula That Saves Maginnis, George H. "The Readability Graph and In-
Time." Journal of Reading, vol. 11 , no. 7 (Aprii, 1968), formal Reading Inventories." The Reading Teacher,
pp. 513-16, 575-78. vol. 22, no. 6 (March 1969), pp. 516-18, 559.
Fry, Edward B. "The Readability Graph Validated at Murphy, D. Test Proves Short Words and Sentences
Primary Levels." The Reading Teacher, vol. 22, no. 6 Get Best Readership." Printer's Ink, no. 218 (January
(March, 1969), pp. 534-38. 10, 1947), pp. 61-64.
Fry, Edward B. Reading Instruction for Classroom and Murphy, D. "How Plain Talk Increases Readership 45%
Clinic. New York, N.Y.: McGraw-Hill, 1972. to 66%." Printer's Ink, no. 220 (September 19, 1947),
Fry, Edward B. "A Kernel Distance Theory for Read- pp. 35-37.
ability." Reading: Convention and Inquiry, George Paolo, Margaret F. A Comparison of Readability
McNinch and Wallace D. Miller, Eds. 24th Yearbook, Graph Scores and Oral Reading Errors on Trade
National Reading Conference, Clemson University, Books for Beginning Reading. Unpublished master's
Clemson, S.C., 1975a. thesis, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, N.J.,
Fry, Edward B. "The Readability Principle." Language January 1977. (Available through ERIC.)
Arts, vol. 52, no. 6 (September 1975b), pp. 847-51. Pearson, David P. "The Effects of Grammatical Com-
Fry, Edward B. Fry Readability Scale. Providence, R.I.: plexity on Children's Comprehension, Recall, and
Jamestown Publishers, 1976. Conceptions of Certain Semantic Relations."
Fry, Edward B. Elementary Reading Instruction. New Reading Research Quarterly, vol. 10, no. 2 (1974-
York, N.Y.: McGraw-Hill, 1977. 1975), pp. 155-92.
Hardyck, CD. and L.F. Petrinovich. "Subvocal Speech Rothkopf, Ernst Z. Text Difficulty Predicted from
and Comprehension Level as a Function of the Functional Chaining in Short Term Memory."
Difficulty Level of Reading Material." Journal of Abstracts of Papers and Symposia. Annual Meeting,
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, vol. 9 (1970), American Educational Research Association, 1977.
pp. 647-52. Singer, Harry. "The SEER Technique: A Non-Computa-
Holgerson, Arnold S. The Relationship of Eye-Voice tional Procedure for Quickly Estimating Read-
Span to Reading Ability and Readability. Un- ability Level." Journal of Reading Behavior, vol. 7, no.
published master's thesis, Rutgers University, New 3 (1975), pp. 255-67.
Brunswick, N.J., June, 1977. (Available through Spache, George. "A New Readability Formula for
ERIC.) Primary Grades Reading Materials." Elementary
Klare, George R. "Assessing Readability." Reading English, vol. 53 (March 1953), pp. 410-13.
Research Quarterly, vol. 10, no. 1 (1974-1975), pp.
Spache, George. Good Books for Poor Readers. Cham-
62-102.
paign, III.:Garrard Publishing Co., 1966.
Kretschmer, Joseph C. "Updating the Fry Readability Swanson, C. "Readability and Readership: A Controlled
Formula." The Reading Teacher, vol. 29, no. 6 (March
Experiment." Journalism Quarterly, vol. 25 (1948),
1976), pp. 555-58. pp. 339-45.
Lively, Bertha A. and S. L. Pressey, "A Method for Weber, Jane E. The Kernel Distance Theory- Evalua-
Measuring the 'Vocabulary Burden' of Text Books." tion by Student Judgment of Sentence Difficulty.
Educational Administration and Supervision, vol. 9 Unpublished master's thesis, Rutgers University,
(Oct. 1923), pp. 389-98. New Brunswick, N.J., June 1977. (Available through
Lorge, Irving I. The Lorge Formula for Estimating ERIC.)
Difficulty of Reading Materials. New York, N.Y.: Zingman, Doris E. Readability and Mass Political Litera-
Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia ture: The 1976 Presidential Election Campaign.
University, 1959. Unpublished master's thesis, Rutgers University,
Lorge, Irving I. "Predicting Reading Difficulty of Selec- New Brunswick, N.J., June 1977. (Available through
tions for Children." The Elementary English Review, ERIC.)
vol. 16 (October 1939), pp. 229-33. Zipf, G.K. The Psycho-Biology of Language. Houghton
Lyman, H. Flesch Count and the Readership of Articles Mifflin, Boston, 1935. (Reprinted, Cambridge, Mass.:
in a Midwestern Farm Paper." Journal of Applied MIT Press, 1965.)

If Your State Needs Legislation on Reading


Call your legislator COLLECT- he'll remember you better next time.
Always introduce yourself, "I'mlegislative chairperson of IRAfrom _,
we represent more than 16,000 people" (or whatever impressive
number).
Cultivate a legislator who'll carry your bill.
Cultivate the secretaries of the legislators-they'll help you.
Don'tassume that a legislator who was in education is now pro-education.
Suggestions from Barbara Valdez, Sacramento, California, made at the
state and local councils meeting on legislation at the Anaheimconvention,
May 1976.

Hall of Fame Members Named


Dr. GertrudeH. Hildrethand Dr. Russell G. Stauffer have been elected by
members of the Reading Hallof Fame to membership in that organization.

252 Journal of Reading December 1977

This content downloaded from 185.44.78.190 on Wed, 18 Jun 2014 10:31:43 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like