You are on page 1of 36

Accepted Manuscript

Morningness−eveningness correlates with sleep time, quality, and hygiene in


secondary school students: A multilevel analysis

Christian Vollmer, Konrad S. Jankowski, Juan F. Díaz-Morales, Heike Itzek-Greulich,


Visiting researcher, Peter Wüst-Ackermann, Christoph Randler

PII: S1389-9457(16)30264-7
DOI: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.09.022
Reference: SLEEP 3223

To appear in: Sleep Medicine

Received Date: 1 July 2016


Revised Date: 16 September 2016
Accepted Date: 17 September 2016

Please cite this article as: Vollmer C, Jankowski KS, Díaz-Morales JF, Itzek-Greulich H, Wüst-
Ackermann P, Randler C, Morningness−eveningness correlates with sleep time, quality, and
hygiene in secondary school students: A multilevel analysis, Sleep Medicine (2016), doi: 10.1016/
j.sleep.2016.09.022.

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to
our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo
copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please
note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all
legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Running head: SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 1
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Morningness−eveningness correlates with sleep time, quality, and hygiene in secondary

2 school students: A multilevel analysis

3
4 Christian Vollmer a,*, Konrad S. Jankowski b, Juan F. Díaz-Moralesc, Heike Itzek-Greulich,

5 Peter Wüst-Ackermann e, Christoph Randler d,e

PT
6
a
7 Institute of Psychology, University of Education Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

RI
b
8 Faculty of Psychology, University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

SC
c
9 Faculty of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
d
10 Institute of Science, Technology & Geography, University of Education

U
11 Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
AN
e
12 Department of Biology, University of Tuebingen, Tuebingen, Germany
M

13

14 ARTICLE INFO
D

15
TE

16 Article history:

17 Received
EP

18 Received in revised form


C

19 Accepted
AC

20

21 Keywords:

22 Adolescents

23 Chronotype

24 Morningness–eveningness

25 Circadian rhythm
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 2
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Sleep hygiene

2 Sleep quality

3 School type

4 School start time

PT
6 *Corresponding author at: Institute of Psychology, University of Education

7 Heidelberg, Keplerstr. 87, D-69120 Heidelberg, Germany. Tel.: 49 6221 477176; fax: 49

RI
8 6221 477432.

SC
9 E-mail address: vollmer@ph-heidelberg.de (C. Vollmer).

10

U
11
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 3
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 ABSTRACT

2 At the onset of puberty, students shift their sleep to later hours, but school starts early.

3 It is suggested that evening orientation and early school start times do not go well together.

4 Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate sleep problems in adolescence, and it was

5 expected that the adolescents’ eveningness orientation is associated with many sleep-related

PT
6 problems. Students of secondary education (n = 3201; mean = 13.8 ± 1.8 years) filled out a

7 self-report questionnaire containing measures of morningness–eveningness, sleep time

RI
8 (midpoint of sleep, social “jetlag”), sleep length (on schooldays and on weekends), sleep

SC
9 quality, and sleep hygiene as well as questions on electronic screen media use. The impact of

10 circadian preference on sleep time (midpoint of sleep and social jetlag), sleep length (on

U
11 schooldays and on weekends), sleep quality, and sleep hygiene of adolescents was tested via
AN
12 multilevel analyses while controlling for covariates on the student level (age, sex, screen

13 media use, and time leaving home) and on the class level (school type, grade level, and school
M

14 start time). Morningness–eveningness was a significant predictor of all dependent variables


D

15 and associations were highest (β > 0.40) for midpoint of sleep, social jetlag, problems in
TE

16 going to bed, problems in falling asleep, and problems in returning to wakefulness. Providing

17 guidance for parents on sleep hygiene behavior routines for their child, an educational
EP

18 program in sleep hygiene, and later school start times could help to synchronize adolescents’

19 circadian rhythms to daily educational and social demands.


C

20
AC
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 4
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 1. Introduction

2 1.1. Sleep problems in adolescence

3 Adolescents are familiar with late bedtime, short sleep, irregular sleep pattern, low

4 sleep quality, and insufficient sleep hygiene, and they accumulate a dramatically high

5 prevalence of such sleep-related problems [1,2]. However, sufficient sleep length and good

PT
6 sleep quality are critical for health [3] and everyday performance. In fact, the poor sleep of

7 adolescents leads to daytime impairments that may result in poorer quality diet [4], aggressive

RI
8 and antisocial behavior [5−7], common psychiatric disorders [8], and poor academic

SC
9 achievement [9−11]. Adequate sleep hygiene could be considered as a protective factor [12],

10 but adolescents do not follow sleep hygiene recommendations [13] and thereby aggravate

U
11 their sleep-related problems.
AN
12 Although adolescents’ sleep habits and the negative consequences for health and

13 academic achievement are recognized, the causes of these sleep problems are not well known.
M

14 This study investigates morningness–eveningness, age, sex, screen media use, school start
D

15 time, and school type as predictors of sleep time, sleep quality, and sleep hygiene.
TE

16

17 1.2. Predictors of sleep problems in adolescence


EP

18 1.2.1. Morningness–eveningness

19 Morningness–eveningness, circadian preference, or chronotype is an individual’s preference


C

20 for early or late bedtimes and rise times. A shift toward eveningness during puberty is
AC

21 observed, and physicians label the adolescent sleep latency as a clinical condition, delayed

22 sleep-phase disorder [14], requiring medical treatment. The insufficient sleep, irregular sleep

23 pattern, reduced sleep quality, and daytime fatigue on weekdays [15] is due to a conflict

24 between biological and social clocks that has been coined “social jetlag” [16] and which

25 arises in this developmental stage.

26
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 5
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 1.2.2. Electronic screen media

2 Adolescents often engage in evening leisure activities that interfere with sleep, such as

3 prolonged electronic screen media use [17−19]. Electronic screen media use is correlated with

4 shorter sleep length [20], late midpoint of sleep [21], and poor sleep quality [22], because the

5 blue light emitted by these media delays the onset of melatonin secretion [23]. Adolescents’

PT
6 screen media use has increased in recent decades, and therefore this major disruptor of

7 circadian rhythmicity should be targeted when studying the sleep of adolescents [24].

RI
8

SC
9 1.2.3. School start time

10 Contributing to inadequate sleep is the fact that class times are early and the

U
11 commuting time to school often increases with the transition to secondary school and thus
AN
12 reduces the sleep duration of adolescents even further [25]. Social norms prescribe a

13 normative and strict 24-hour timetable with specified times for withdrawal into sleep and
M

14 social life and do not account for adolescent developmental changes in the sleep−wake
D

15 rhythm.
TE

16

17 1.2.4. School type


EP

18 Sleep problems are also reinforced by increasing demands and opportunities from

19 familiar, educational, job-related, leisure, and peer backgrounds [26]. In the present study, the
C

20 school type was used as an indicator of different social and educational backgrounds.
AC

21

22 1.3. Present study

23 None of the previous studies have analyzed all listed factors in conjunction with

24 limiting their ability to detect or overestimating effects of morningness–eveningness on sleep-

25 related variables. The present study sampled a large number of classes, collected information

26 on morningness–eveningness, sleep time, sleep length, sleep quality, and sleep hygiene, and
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 6
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 used multilevel analyses to address the clustering of the data. This allowed us to check for

2 differential effects of morningness–eveningness on several sleep-related variables in

3 adolescents while taking into account the nested data structure in classes from different grade

4 levels and school types with diverging school start times.

5 Although most previous studies have indicated negative effects of morningness–

PT
6 eveningness on sleep time, sleep quality, and sleep hygiene, these studies have not

7 investigated the causes of these sleep-related problems of adolescents in one coherent study.

RI
8 The present study aimed to estimate adolescents’ differences in sleep time, sleep length, sleep

SC
9 quality, and sleep hygiene by morningness–eveningness. Besides age and sex, electronic

10 screen media use was also addressed as a predictor of sleep problems, because previous

U
11 studies have reported their harmful effects on adolescents’ night sleep [21,27]. Moreover,
AN
12 because German school starts early in the morning between 07:30 and 08:10, the time when

13 students leave their homes in the morning was assessed. Also, because the students are
M

14 clustered into classes and schools, and because different schools start at different times, the
D

15 start time of the first lesson at the various German school types (special needs, lower track,
TE

16 intermediate track, and upper track) was operationalized.

17 We formulated the following hypotheses: Eveningness is related to late sleep time and
EP

18 to short sleep length (hypothesis 1). Morningness is associated with better sleep quality and

19 better sleep hygiene (hypothesis 2). Electronic screen media use, age, sex, school start time,
C

20 grade level, and school type were used as covariates because it is suggested that these
AC

21 variables also contribute to the variance in the sleep-related outcome variables.

22

23 2. Methods

24 2.1. Sample

25 Participants were 3201 students from 169 school classes of grades 5 to 10 from 28

26 schools of secondary education in three adjacent German administrative districts (Heidelberg,


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 7
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Mannheim, and Rhein-Neckar-Kreis). Classes were selected by the principals in reasoned

2 consideration of the class schedule. Participation was voluntary and anonymous. The students

3 completed the self-administered questionnaire in normal classroom settings in the presence of

4 a researcher and a teacher. Informed consent was obtained from the authority

5 (Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe), school principals, parents, and students. Written consent

PT
6 was obtained from the authority and the parents. The research method met the ethical

7 principles and standards of human research proclaimed by the University of Education

RI
8 Heidelberg. The field work took place from November 13, 2009, to March 22, 2010.

SC
9

10 2.2. Measurement instruments

U
11 2.2.1. Morningness–eveningness
AN
12 Morningness–eveningness was measured with the Composite Scale of Morningness

13 (CSM) [28], a Likert-type scale. The psychometric properties of the CSM have been validated
M

14 with cortisol levels (Randler & Schaal, 2010) [29], and higher scores have been found to
D

15 correlate with an earlier midpoint of sleep (r = −0.46 [30] and r = −0.52 [31,32]). The CSM
TE

16 has been adapted to German children and adolescents by making the wording a bit easier,

17 which was done by considering some aspects of the Basic Language Morningness Scale
EP

18 (BALM) [33]. The German CSM is available in different versions (adults, adolescents, and

19 parent report) [34]. In detail, some of the items were adapted to match the school background
C

20 of adolescents (items 1, 6, 8, and 10), and some items were rephrased to suit the language of
AC

21 the younger adolescents and students from the special needs track (items 1, 2, 3, 6, and 12)

22 [34,35]. Furthermore, Tonetti et al. [32] reported some evidence for the validity of the

23 German scale. A short version of the scale (containing items 1, 2, 5, 6, 9, 10, and 12) was

24 given to the students of the special needs track. The scores were normally distributed (Fig. 1)

25 and CSM’s Cronbach’s α of the present study indicated good reliability of the scale

26 (Cronbach’s α = 0.85).
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 8
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

2 2.2.2. Electronic screen media use

3 Two questions about time use of electronic screen media (item 1: “How much time do

4 you spend on a regular schoolday at the computer/game console?” and item 2: “How much

5 time do you spend on a regular schoolday watching TV and videos?”) on a six-point Likert-

PT
6 type scale (0 = “no time at all”; 1 = “less than ½ hour”; 2 = “½ hour, up to 1 hour”; 3 = “more

7 than 1 hour, up to 2 hours”; 4 = “more than 2 hours, up to 3 hours”; and 5 “more than 3

RI
8 hours”) were combined to an index of screen media use (Cronbach’s α = 0.54).

SC
9

10 2.2.3. Sleep time and sleep length

U
11 Students reported their usual bedtimes and rise times on schooldays and on weekends. Four
AN
12 variables were calculated from these four clock times: midpoint of sleep, social jetlag, sleep

13 length (time spent in bed) on schooldays, and sleep length (time spent in bed) on weekends.
M

14 Because the data are not based on actual sleep data, we used “time in bed” as an
D

15 approximation of sleep length. The midpoint of sleep was adjusted for individual sleep debt
TE

16 on schooldays (MSFsc) [36] and, in the present study, was not based on actual sleep data

17 (sleep onset/awakening) but on self-report measures on bedtimes and rise times. Therefore,
EP

18 bedtimes and rise times were used as approximations of actual sleep length. Social jetlag is

19 considered as the absolute difference between mid-sleep on schooldays and mid-sleep on


C

20 weekends [16].
AC

21

22 2.2.4. Sleep quality

23 The Adolescent Sleep−Wake Scale (ASWS) [37] measures sleep quality on five

24 subscales (going to bed, falling asleep, maintaining sleep, re-initiating sleep, and returning to

25 wakefulness; 28 items total). A short version of the scale, containing 11 items, was given to

26 the students in the special needs track.


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 9
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

2 2.2.5. Sleep hygiene

3 The Adolescent Sleep Hygiene Scale (ASHS) [37] measures behaviors that may inhibit sleep

4 onset in five subscales (physiological problems, cognitive problems, emotional problems,

5 sleep environment, and use of substances; 28 items total). A short version of the scale,

PT
6 containing 6 items, was given to the students in the special needs track.

RI
8 2.3. Data analyses

SC
9 Items on the ASWS and ASHS were analyzed with confirmatory factor analysis in

10 Mplus 7 [38] to confirm the model fit of the data to the latent structure proposed by

U
11 LeBourgeois et al. [37]. Factor scores were used for subsequent analyses (ASWS: five-factor
AN
12 model, χ2 = 5821, df = 340, χ2/df = 17.1, p < 0.001, root mean square error of approximation

13 (RMSEA) = 0.07, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.88, TLI = 0.87; ASHS: five-factor model,
M

14 χ2 = 4276, df = 160, χ2/df = 26.7, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.09, CFI = 0.83, TLI = 0.80).
D

15 After the investigation of descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations in SPSS 22


TE

16 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), multilevel regression analyses were calculated in Mplus 7

17 with sleep time (MSFsc, social jetlag), time in bed (on schooldays, on weekends), sleep
EP

18 quality (ASWS, five subscales), and sleep hygiene (ASHS, five subscales) as dependent

19 variables. Morningness–eveningness, age, sex, screen media use, and time leaving home were
C

20 used as predictors at the student level. Moreover, because students were nested in classrooms
AC

21 and schools, class was introduced as a cluster variable, and school type (dummy variables

22 with the reference group upper track), grade level, and school start time (start of the first

23 lesson) were used as predictors at the class level.

24

25 2.4. Missing data


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 10
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Before the analyses, 161 students from 20 classes with fewer than 10 participants were

2 removed from the dataset, because a class size of at least 10 students is recommended for

3 multilevel analysis. The resulting dataset contained 3040 students in 149 classes (mean class

4 size = 20.40, SD = 6.09). The amount of missing data for each variable in these 149 classes is

5 due to questionnaire design (a shorter questionnaire was used for the special needs track) and

PT
6 nonresponse to single items or scales (Table 1). In the multilevel analyses, full information

7 maximum likelihood (FIML) [39] was used to take the missing data into account. In addition

RI
8 to morningness–eveningness, the other predictors were included in the multilevel regression

SC
9 analyses to make the assumption of missing-at-random more plausible.

10

U
11 3. Results
AN
12 3.1. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations

13 Descriptives of all study variables are shown in Table 1. The mean value for
M

14 morningness–eveningness was 34, and values were normally distributed around the scale’s
D

15 dead center (Fig. 1). On average, the students left home 36 minutes before the start of the first
TE

16 lesson. Time in bed (proxy for sleep length) on schooldays was 1 hour 20 minutes shorter than

17 on weekends. Problems with the sleep−wake rhythm were highest in returning to wakefulness
EP

18 (mean = 2.97) and going to bed (mean = 2.50). The sleep hygiene problems were highest on

19 the cognitive subscale (mean = 2.96).


C

20 Concerning the predictors, morningness–eveningness did not differ by sex (boys,


AC

21 mean = 33.89 ± 7.11; girls, mean = 34.09 ± 7.14; p = 0.431). Morningness–eveningness was

22 negatively associated with age, screen media use, and time leaving home. Age was positively

23 correlated with screen media use but not with time leaving home. Screen media use was

24 positively correlated with time leaving home (Table 2).

25 Concerning the dependent variables, MSFsc was negatively correlated with time in

26 bed on schooldays and weekends, but social jetlag was not correlated with time in bed on
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 11
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 weekends. Correlations of MSFsc and social jetlag, respectively, were stronger for the

2 subscales of the ASHS than for the ASWS, indicating that late sleep was rather a matter of

3 lacking sleep hygiene (ASHS) than of lacking sleep quality (ASWS). Time in bed on

4 schooldays and on weekends was negatively associated with all subscales of the ASWS and

5 the ASHS with higher correlation coefficients for time in bed on schooldays, except for

PT
6 problems in re-initiating sleep on schooldays. The subscales of the ASWS and the ASHS were

7 positively correlated with each other (r = 0.20−0.88) (Table 2).

RI
8 Inspection of the multilevel structure of the data showed that the intraclass correlations

SC
9 (ICCs) (Table 1) were highest for time in bed on schooldays (0.35), MSFsc (0.22), and

10 substances (0.24), indicating more differences between classes at mean levels for these

U
11 variables than for the other sleep-related variables.
AN
12

13 3.2. Effects of morningness−eveningness on sleep time and sleep length


M

14 The effects of morningness–eveningness on sleep time and time in bed (proxy for
D

15 sleep length) were tested via multilevel analyses (Table 3). Morningness was negatively
TE

16 associated with late sleep time (MSFsc and social jetlag) and time in bed on weekends, but

17 was positively associated with time in bed on schooldays. These associations were more
EP

18 pronounced for sleep time than for time in bed.

19 Among the other covariates, notably (with β > 0.30), older age and school type
C

20 (special needs and lower track) (Fig. 2) were positively associated with late sleep (MSFsc and
AC

21 social jetlag). Although age was negatively associated with time in bed (proxy for sleep

22 length) on schooldays (β = −0.38) and, to a lesser extent, with time in bed on weekends (β =

23 −0.20), on the class level there were few differences in time in bed on schooldays, and no

24 differences between classes for the weekend time in bed (Fig. 2).

25

26 3.3. Effects of morningness−eveningness on sleep quality


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 12
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 The effects of morningness−eveningness on sleep quality were also tested via

2 multilevel analyses (Table 4). Generally, morningness was positively associated with sleep

3 quality; that is, there were negative associations with all five subscales of problems in sleep

4 quality. There was a highly negative association between morningness–eveningness and

5 problems in returning to wakefulness. The associations of eveningness with problems in going

PT
6 to bed and problems in falling asleep were of medium strength, whereas problems in

7 maintaining sleep and problems in re-initiating sleep were weakly associated with

RI
8 eveningness. Girls reported lower sleep quality than boys on all five subscales.

SC
9

10 3.4. Effects of morningness−eveningness on sleep hygiene

U
11 The effects of morningness–eveningness on sleep hygiene were also tested via
AN
12 multilevel analyses (Table 5). Morningness was positively associated with sleep hygiene;

13 thus, eveningness was related to problematic sleep hygiene behavior (the five subscales of the
M

14 ASHS). The standardized coefficients were equally low (−0.21 to −0.26).


D

15 Girls reported notably more emotional sleep hygiene problems than boys (β = 0.37).
TE

16 The special needs and lower track school types were associated with notably more

17 physiological problems and problematic sleep environment than the other school types (Fig.
EP

18 2).

19 Effect sizes (percentage of explained variance) were highest for MSFsc (42%), social
C

20 jetlag (37%), time in bed on schooldays (44%), problems in going to bed (35%), problems in
AC

21 returning to wakefulness (56%), and substance use (32%). On the other hand, the multilevel

22 models did not explain that much variance in time in bed on weekends (11%), problems in

23 maintaining sleep (10%), problems in re-initiating sleep (8%), and in emotional sleep hygiene

24 problems (14%).

25

26 4. Discussion
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 13
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1

2 The aim of this study was to investigate predictors of sleep problems in adolescence.

3 The impact of morningness–eveningness on sleep time (MSFsc and social jetlag), time in bed

4 (on schooldays and on weekends), sleep quality, and sleep hygiene of adolescents was tested

5 via multilevel analyses while controlling for covariates on the student level (age, sex, screen

PT
6 media use, and time leaving home) and on the class level (school type, grade level, and school

7 start time).

RI
8 Morningness–eveningness was a significant predictor of all dependent variables, and

SC
9 associations were highest (β > 0.40) for MSFsc, social jetlag, problems in going to bed,

10 problems in falling asleep, and problems in returning to wakefulness. Because the first lesson

U
11 starts early in the morning, eveningness was related to shorter time in bed on school days,
AN
12 which is in line with findings by Arrona-Palacios et al. [40] and Carissimi et al. [41]. These

13 authors showed that lessons starting in the afternoon are best for optimal sleep duration of
M

14 adolescents, regardless of their circadian preference [42]. Since another study also found
D

15 longer sleep duration and lower sleepiness in afternoon schedule students but with distinct
TE

16 results for morning and evening types [43], the benefits of later schedules for academic

17 achievement are still unclear.


EP

18 Morningness–eveningness is less valuable in predicting problems of sleep hygiene and

19 sleep quality. The lesser interrelation of morningness–eveningness with aspects of sleep


C

20 quality (maintaining and re-initiating sleep) and time in bed confirm that morningness–
AC

21 eveningness is rather a matter of sleep timing than of sleep quality or time in bed. Problems in

22 the sleep−wake rhythm become more severe with increasing age and, more crucially, with

23 increasing drift to eveningness. Morningness–eveningness was a strong predictor of problems

24 in returning to wakefulness: every student has to get up at the same time in the morning

25 because school starts early in the morning, regardless of whether the student prefers a later

26 waking time. This underlines that the early school start times are inappropriate [44]. In
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 14
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 contrast, in the evening, adolescents are much more able to influence their own best time of

2 going to bed and, as a consequence, report fewer problems in the evening. The high

3 correlation of morningness–eveningness and problems in returning to wakefulness may partly

4 reflect that morning items play a greater role in the construction of the CSM: eight of 13 items

5 on the CSM are “morning items” (items 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 10, 11, and 12) and two are “evening

PT
6 items” (items 2 and 7). On the other hand, the morning items on the CSM are verified as

7 being more suitable to measure morningness–eveningness [28], and researchers have

RI
8 advocated using only these as predictors of individual circadian preferences, referring to

SC
9 them as “morning affect factor” [45].

10 The consistently significant correlations of morningness–eveningness and sleep

U
11 hygiene subscales indicate that good sleep hygiene practice could lessen the drift to
AN
12 eveningness in adolescence. Girls reported more sleep related problems than boys, in line with

13 findings by Lazaratou et al. [46]. Both sexes were evening oriented, and morningness–
M

14 eveningness was unrelated to sex. In contrast to this, some research found that boys had a
D

15 greater number of incorrect sleep beliefs than girls [47]. Although there is no consensus
TE

16 regarding sex differences in sleep parameters in adolescents, when sleep habits are

17 considered, girls generally reported better sleep hygiene than boys [48]. In the present study,
EP

18 girls reported more problems than boys; thus it is likely that girls with similar morningness–

19 eveningness experience more sleep problems.


C

20 Regarding age differences, results corroborate previous study findings. As adolescents


AC

21 become older, they progressively delay their rise time and bedtime; time in bed decreases

22 [14], and sleep irregularity increases [49]. Thus, the present results strengthen the finding that

23 older adolescents exhibit more sleep problems than younger ones. Adolescents’ individual

24 rhythm desynchronizes more and more from socially acceptable waking times and bedtimes

25 as they shift to evening types [50]. In consequence, they accumulate an ever-increasing sleep

26 loss on schooldays, and returning to wakefulness becomes constantly more challenging,


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 15
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 especially for evening types. This becomes even more severe because, on schooldays,

2 adolescent evening types can only partly recover from sleep debt by taking long naps in the

3 afternoon, which perpetuates their late bedtimes.

4 Electronic screen media are consumed during leisure time, that is, in the late afternoon

5 and evening hours before bedtime [27], when they are most harmful for the night sleep. The

PT
6 results confirmed that screen media hinder adolescents from going to bed in a timely manner

7 and are considered as a problematic sleep environment. Screen media emit blue light, which is

RI
8 harmful for an early sleep onset because it delays melatonin secretion [51].

SC
9 Concerning the class level variables, results have shown that the prevalence of sleep-

10 related problems was higher in the lower and intermediate tracks of secondary education and

U
11 that the upper track copes best with the early school start times. This result suggests that the
AN
12 social and educational background plays a vital role, and that parents can effectively ensure

13 early and consistent bedtimes, give structure with mealtimes to reduce physiological
M

14 problems, and give their child the opportunity for a quiet bedroom and adequate sleep
D

15 environment.
TE

16

17 4.1. Study limitations


EP

18 A methodological limitation is the use of self-administered questionnaires. However,

19 we collected the data with reliable and valid measures. In addition, validation studies as well
C

20 as Cronbach’s α values in our sample have shown that these measures are valid and reliable
AC

21 [52], and thus we assume no bias by our self-report data. Collection of objective data on sleep

22 variables (eg, via actigraphy) is expensive and was not feasible for such a large sample as

23 ours. Another limitation of the self-report nature of the data lies in voluntariness of

24 participation, and thus in an inevitable self-selection of schools, classes, and individuals.

25 However, every class in three administrative districts had an equal chance to participate.

26 Moreover, the correlative nature of the analyses could not determine causes and effects.
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 16
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Furthermore, in the questionnaire, we did not ask for sleep length, but used the time spent in

2 bed as an approximation, which calls for a cautious interpretation of the sleep measures

3 MSFsc and social jetlag, which are calculated from the time in bed measure and are not based

4 on real sleep data.

5 Furthermore, there are more correlates of sleep problems in adolescents that were not

PT
6 targeted by the present study. For example, increased stress perception in everyday life

7 [25,53], anxiety [54], and depression [55] lead to elevated arousal before falling asleep and

RI
8 give way to a vicious circle [56,57].

SC
9

10 4.2. Practical implications

U
11 Later school start time had a positive effect on sleep length in a comparison of US and
AN
12 Australian adolescents [58]. Moreover, a 45-minute delay of class start times improved

13 tardiness, disciplinary issues, and sleep length half a year later, but not academic performance
M

14 [59]. As sleep timing is more important than sleep length and quality for academic
D

15 performance [60], and as morningness–eveningness has a biological underpinning, a later


TE

16 school start time would be beneficial for adolescent health and might lead to better academic

17 performance; however, but this is a hypothesis that still needs to be investigated further.
EP

18 Inappropriate sleep environment showed the highest correlations with problems in

19 going to bed. This issue could be mitigated to some extent if parents removed electronic
C

20 devices from the adolescents’ bedroom and gave them the opportunity to sleep in an aired and
AC

21 dark room. However, this may be unrealistic, because adolescent development implies

22 increased autonomy with less interference from parents. Physiological problems could be

23 addressed by regular family dinner times not past 19:00, along with abstinence from later food

24 intake and no use of substances. Furthermore, parents could solve psychological problems

25 that hinder adolescents’ sleep onset by taking the time to talk with their children about their

26 day and encourage them to face the next day’s events. As adolescents report extreme
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 17
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 difficulties in returning to wakefulness, adequate sleep hygiene behavior in the hour before

2 going to bed and an adequate sleep environment are the most important steps to promote early

3 bedtimes and to cushion adolescents’ drift to eveningness. Also, a program on sleep education

4 could promote healthy sleep beliefs [47]. Behavioral change, however, is very difficult to

5 achieve, especially because late sleep onset is caused by a daily endogenous rhythm of

PT
6 hormones [61].

RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 18
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 Conflict of interest

2 The authors have no conflicts of interest in regard to this work.

4 Acknowledgements

5 This study was supported by grants from the University of Education Heidelberg. The

PT
6 work of the second author was supported by a grant 2011/03/D/HS6/05760 from the National

7 Science Centre in Poland. The work was carried out at the University of Education

RI
8 Heidelberg. We would like to thank all principals, teachers, and parents who supported the

SC
9 study, and especially all students who participated; Farina Pötsch for visiting participating

10 schools; and Caroline Buhmann, Farina Pötsch, and Lena Saliger for feeding the computer.

U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 19
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 References

3 [1] Díaz-Morales JF, Escribano C. Consequences of adolescent’s evening preference on

4 school achievement: A review. Anales de Psicología 2014;30:1096–1104.

5 [2] Russo PM, Bruni O, Lucidi F, Ferri R, Violani C. Sleep habits and circadian preference in

PT
6 Italian children and adolescents. J Sleep Res 2007;16:163–9.

RI
7 [3] Jarrin DC, McGrath JJ, Drake CL. Beyond sleep duration: Distinct sleep dimensions are

8 associated with obesity in children and adolescents. Int J Obes 2013;37:552–8.

SC
9 [4] Golley RK, Maher CA, Matricciani L, Olds TS. Sleep duration or bedtime? Exploring the

10 association between sleep timing behaviour, diet and BMI in children and adolescents. Int

11 J Obes 2013;37:546–51.
U
AN
12 [5] Lange L, Randler C. Morningness–eveningness and behavioural problems in adolescents.
M

13 Sleep Biol Rhythms 2011;9:12–18.

14 [6] Schlarb AA, Sopp R, Ambiel D, Grunwald J. Chronotype-related differences in childhood


D

15 and adolescent aggression and antisocial behavior─a review of the literature. Chronobiol
TE

16 Int 2014;31:1–16.
EP

17 [7] Simor P, Zavecz Z, Palosi V, Torok C, Koteles F. The influence of sleep complaints on

18 the association between chronotype and negative emotionality in young adults. Chronobiol
C

19 Int 2015;32:1–10.
AC

20 [8] Haregu A, Gelaye B, Pensuksan WC, Lohsoonthorn V, Lertmaharit S, Rattananupong, et

21 al. Circadian rhythm characteristics, poor sleep quality, daytime sleepiness and common

22 psychiatric disorders among Thai college students. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry 2015;7:182–9.

23 [9] Randler C, Frech D. Young people's time-of-day preferences affect their school

24 performance. J Youth Stud 2009;12:653–67.


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 20
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [10] Tonetti L, Natale V, Randler C. Association between circadian preference and academic

2 achievement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Chronobiol Int 2015;32:792–801.

3 [11] Vollmer C, Pötsch F, Randler C. Morningness is associated with better gradings and

4 higher attention in class. Learn Indiv Diff 2013;27:167–73.

5 [12] Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during

PT
6 adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9:947–57.

RI
7 [13] Vollmer C, Hammer J, Keller C, Maxand AK, Díaz Morales JF, Randler C. Development

8 and evaluation of a sleep education program in middle school pupils based on self-

SC
9 determination theory. Int J Biol Educ 2014;3:12–23.

10 [14] Gradisar M, Gardner G, Dohnt H. Recent worldwide sleep patterns and problems during

11
U
adolescence: A review and meta-analysis of age, region, and sleep. Sleep Med
AN
12 2011;12:110–8.
M

13 [15] Laberge L, Petit D, Simard C, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir J. Development of

14 sleep patterns in early adolescence. J Sleep Res 2001;10:59–67.


D

15 [16] Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. Social jetlag: Misalignment of


TE

16 biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 2006;23:497–509.


EP

17 [17] Arora T, Broglia E, Thomas GN, Taheri S. Associations between specific technologies

18 and adolescent sleep quantity, sleep quality, and parasomnias. Sleep Med 2014;15:240–7.
C

19 [18] Demirhan E, Randler C, Horzum MB. Is problematic mobile phone use explained by
AC

20 chronotype and personality? Chronobiol Int 2016;1–11.

21 [19] Randler C, Horzum MB, Vollmer C. Internet addiction and its relationship to chronotype

22 and personality in a Turkish university student sample. Social Sci Comput Rev

23 2014;32:484–95.
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 21
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [20] Harbard E, Allen NB, Trinder J, Bei B. What's keeping teenagers up? Prebedtime

2 behaviors and actigraphy-assessed sleep over school and vacation. J Adolesc Health

3 2016;58:426–32.

4 [21] Vollmer C, Michel U, Randler C. Outdoor light at night (LAN) is correlated with

5 eveningness in adolescents. Chronobiol Int 2012;29:502–508.

PT
6 [22] Bruni O, Sette S, Fontanesi L, Baiocco R, Laghi F, Baumgartner E. Technology use and

7 sleep quality in preadolescence and adolescence. J Clin Sleep Med 2015;11:1433–41.

RI
8 [23] Higuchi S, Lee SI, Kozaki T, Harada T, Tanaka I. Late circadian phase in adults and

SC
9 children is correlated with use of high color temperature light at home at night. Chronobiol

10 Int 2016;33:448–52.

11
U
[24] Eugenia T, Paulina S, Justyna M. Disruption of circadian rhythmicity─an exploratory
AN
12 study in Poland. Personal Indiv Diff 2014;66:98–101.
M

13 [25] Pereira EF, Moreno C, Louzada FM. Increased commuting to school time reduces sleep

14 duration in adolescents. Chronobiol Int 2014;31:87–94.


D

15 [26] Jenni OG. Children's sleep: An interplay between culture and biology. Pediatrics
TE

16 2005;115:204–16.
EP

17 [27] Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and

18 adolescents: A review. Sleep Med 2010;11:735–42.


C

19 [28] Smith CS, Reilly C, Midkiff K. Evaluation of three circadian rhythm questionnaires with
AC

20 suggestions for an improved measure of morningness. J Appl Psychol 1989;74:728–38.

21 [29] Randler C, Schaal S. Morningness–eveningness, habitual sleep-wake variables and

22 cortisol level. Biol Psychol 2010;85:14–18.

23 [30] Önder I, Besoluk S, Horzum MB. Psychometric properties of the Turkish version of the

24 Composite Scale of Morningness. Spanish J Psychol 2013;16:E67.


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 22
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [31] Jankowski KS. Composite Scale of Morningness: Psychometric properties, validity with

2 Munich ChronoType Questionnaire and age/sex differences in Poland. Eur Psychiatry

3 2015;30:166–71.

4 [32] Tonetti L, Adan A, Di Milia L, Randler C, Natale V. Measures of circadian preference in

5 childhood and adolescence: A review. Eur Psychiatry 2015;30:576–82.

PT
6 [33] Brown FM. Psychometric equivalence of an improved Basic Language Morningness

7 (BALM) Scale using industrial population within comparisons. Ergonomics 1993;36:191–

RI
8 7.

SC
9 [34] Randler C. CSM. Composite Scale of Morningness─Deutsche Fassung [German

10 version]. (PSYNDEX Tests-Nr. 9006790). In: Leibniz-Zentrum für Psychologische

11
U
Information und Dokumentation (ZPID), Elektronisches Testarchiv. 2014. Available at:
AN
12 https://www.zpid.de/retrieval/PSYNDEXTests.php?id=9006790.
M

13 [35] Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase

14 preference. Sleep 1993;16:258–62.


D

15 [36] Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller P, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, et al. A marker for
TE

16 the end of adolescence. Curr Biol 2004;14:R1038-9.


EP

17 [37] LeBourgeois MK, Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Wolfson AR, Harsh J. The relationship

18 between reported sleep quality and sleep hygiene in Italian and American adolescents.
C

19 Pediatrics 2005;115:257–65.
AC

20 [38] Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Seventh edition. Los Angeles, CA:

21 Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2012.

22 [39] Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. Methodology in the social sciences. New

23 York: Guilford Press; 2010.

24 [40] Arrona-Palacios A, Garcia A, Valdez P. Sleep-wake habits and circadian preference in

25 Mexican secondary school. Sleep Med 2015;16:1259–64.


SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 23
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [41] Carissimi A, Dresch F, Martins AC, Levandovski RM, Adan A, Natale V, et al The

2 influence of school time on sleep patterns of children and adolescents. Sleep Med

3 2016;19:33–9.

4 [42] Koscec A, Radosevic-Vidacek B, Bakotic M. Morningness–eveningness and sleep

5 patterns of adolescents attending school in two rotating shifts. Chronobiol Int 2014;31:52–

PT
6 63.

7 [43] Martin JS, Gaudreault MM, Perron M, Laberge L. Chronotype, light exposure, sleep, and

RI
8 daytime functioning in high school students attending morning or afternoon school shifts:

SC
9 An actigraphic study. J Biol Rhythms 2016;31:205–17.

10 [44] Carskadon MA. Factors influencing sleep pattern of adolescents. In: Carskadon MA (ed),

11
U
Adolescent sleep patterns. Biological, social, and psychological influences. Cambridge:
AN
12 Cambridge University Press; 2002, p. 4–26.
M

13 [45] Caci H, Deschaux O, Adan A, Natale V. Comparing three morningness scales: Age and

14 gender effects, structure and cut-off criteria. Sleep Med 2009;10:240–5.


D

15 [46] Lazaratou H, Dikeos DG, Anagnostopoulos DC, Sbokou O, Soldatos CR. Sleep
TE

16 problems in adolescence: A study of senior high school students in Greece. Eur Child

17 Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;14:237–43.


EP

18 [47] Díaz-Morales JF, Prieto PD, Escribano C, Collado MJ, Randler C. Sleep beliefs and
C

19 chronotype among adolescents: The effect of a sleep education program. Biol Rhythm Res
AC

20 2012;43:397–412.

21 [48] Sun WQ, Chen WJ, Jiang YR, Li SH, Yan CH, Jin XM, et al. The association of sleep

22 hygiene and sleep quality among school-age children. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi

23 [Chinese J Prev Med 2012;46:713–7.

24 [49] Yang C-K. Age-related changes in sleep/wake patterns among Korean teenagers.

25 Pediatrics 2005;115:250–6.
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 24
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [50] Carskadon MA, Wolfson AR, Acebo C, Tzischinsky O, Seifer R. Adolescent sleep

2 patterns, circadian timing, and sleepiness at a transition to early school days. Sleep

3 1998;21:871–81.

4 [51] West KE, Jablonski MR, Warfield B, Cecil KS, James M, Ayers MA, et al. Blue light

5 from light-emitting diodes elicits a dose-dependent suppression of melatonin in humans. J

PT
6 Applied Physiol 2011;110:619–26.

7 [52] Tonetti L. Validity of the Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire for adolescents.

RI
8 Sleep Hypnosis 2007;9:47–51.

SC
9 [53] Vollmer C, Schaal S, Hummel E, Randler C. Association among schoolrelated,

10 parental and selfrelated problems and morningness–eveningness in adolescents. Stress

11 Health 2011;27:413–9.
U
AN
12 [54] Talbot LS, McGlinchey EL, Kaplan KA, Dahl RE, Harvey AG. Sleep deprivation in
M

13 adolescents and adults: Changes in affect. Emotion 2010;10:831–41.

14 [55] de Souza CM, Hidalgo MP. Midpoint of sleep on school days is associated with
D

15 depression among adolescents. Chronobiol Int 2014;31:199–205.


TE

16 [56] Hiller RM, Lovato N, Gradisar M, Oliver M, Slater A. Trying to fall asleep while
EP

17 catastrophising: What sleep-disordered adolescents think and feel. Sleep Med 2014;15:96–

18 103.
C

19 [57] Sarchiapone M, Mandelli L, Carli V, Iosue M, Wasserman C, Hadlaczky, G et al. Hours


AC

20 of sleep in adolescents and its association with anxiety, emotional concerns, and suicidal

21 ideation. Sleep Med 2014;15:248–54.

22 [58] Short MA, Gradisar M, Lack LC, Wright HR, Dewald JF, Wolfson AR, et al. A cross-

23 cultural comparison of sleep duration between U.S. and Australian adolescents: The effect

24 of school start time, parent-set bedtimes, and extracurricular load. Health Educ Behav

25 2013;40:323–30.
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 25
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
1 [59] Thacher PV, Onyper SV. Longitudinal outcomes of start time delay on sleep, behavior,

2 and achievement in high school. Sleep 2016;39:271–81.

3 [60] Genzel L, Ahrberg K, Roselli C, Niedermaier S, Steiger A, Dresler M, et al. Sleep timing

4 is more important than sleep length or quality for medical school performance.

5 Chronobiology Int 2013;30:766–71.

PT
6 [61] Dijk D-J, von Schantz M. Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and

7 performance by a symphony of oscillators. J Biol Rhythms 2005;20:279–90.

RI
8 [62] Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Modeled variance in two-level models. Sociol Methods Res

SC
9 1994;22:342–63.

U
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 26
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 1

Descriptive characteristics of the sample.

Covariates on the student level n Mean SD Range ICC


Morningness–eveningness 3007 33.99 7.13 13–55
Age 3014 13.76 1.80 9.7–19.6
Sex 3035
Male 1576
Female 1459
Screen media use 2937 2.67 1.17 0–5

PT
Time leaving home 2951 07:11 23 min 05:40–08:05
Covariates on the class level
School type 3040
Special needs (Förderschule) 114

RI
Lower track (Hauptschule) 723
Intermediate track (Realschule) 1263
Upper track (Gymnasium) 940

SC
Grade level 3040
N, fifth: 451, sixth: 498, seventh: 558, eighth: 589, ninth: 494,
tenth: 450
School start time 3040 07:47 12 min 07:30–8:10

U
Sleep variables
MSFsc 2962 04:22 1 h 28 min 01:07–10:17 0.22
AN
Social jetlag 2962 2 h 42 min 1 h 14 min 0 h 0 min–8 h 37 min 0.16
Time in bed on schooldays 3005 8 h 35 min 1 h 10 min 2h 0 min - 12 h 0 min 0.35
0 h 0 min–14 h 45
Time in bed on weekends 2974 9 h 55 min 1 h 37 min 0.04
min
ASWS
M

Problems in going to bed 2885 2.50 0.72 1–5 0.05


Problems in falling asleep 2885 2.35 0.74 1–5 0.03
Problems in maintaining sleep 2709 2.26 0.79 1–5 0.03
D

Problems in re-initiating sleep 2874 1.92 0.70 1–5 0.03


Problems in returning to
2880 2.97 0.96 1–5 0.06
wakefulness
TE

ASHS
Physiological problems 2923 2.28 0.62 1–5 0.12
Cognitive problems 3028 2.96 0.83 1–5 0.09
Emotional problems 2907 2.23 0.87 1–5 0.05
EP

Problematic sleep environment 3026 1.86 0.77 1–5 0.11


Substances 2898 1.35 0.76 1–5 0.24

ASHS, Adolescent Sleep Hygiene Scale (1 = never to 5 = always); ASWS, Adolescent Sleep−Wake Scale (1 =
C

never to 5 = always); ICC, intraclass correlation (with larger values indicating greater differences between
classes); MSFsc,
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 27

Table 2
Correlations.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
1 Morningness–eveningness

PT
– **
2 Age
.31 *
– ** **

RI
3 Screen media use .25
.36 * *
– ** **
4 Time leaving home .02 .07
.14 * *

SC
Sleep variables
– ** ** ** **
5 MSFsc .39 .38 .17
.53 * * * *

U
– ** ** ** – **
6 Social jetlag .33 .30 ** .86
.53 * * * .06 *

AN
** – ** – ** ** – ** – **
7 Time in bed on schooldays .39 .13
* .57 * .33 * * .46 * .39 *
– – ** – ** – ** – **
8 Time in bed on weekends * .02 .30
.05 .17 * .17 * .42 * .02 *

M
Adolescent Sleep−Wake Scale
– ** ** ** ** ** – ** – **
9 Problems in going to bed .13 .30 .05 ** .32 .28
.57 * * * * * .28 * .11 *

D
1 – ** ** ** ** – ** – **
.8 **
Problems in falling asleep .06 ** .19 .04 * .21 .17
0 .40 * * * * .20 * .11 8* *

TE
1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – **
.5 ** .7 **
Problems in maintaining sleep .08 .09 .03 .16 .12
1 .28 * * * * * .14 * .10 8* * 7 *
1 – ** – – – .5 ** .7 ** .6 **
Problems in re-initiating sleep ** .06 ** .05 ** .05 * .02 † **
2 .19 * .05 .03 .06 2 * 4 * 4 *
EP
1 Problems in returning to – ** ** ** ** ** – ** ** .6 ** .5 ** .3 ** .3 **
.19 .22 .05 ** .27 .30 .08
3 wakefulness .73 * * * * * .21 * * 8 * 3 * 9 * 3 *
Adolescent Sleep Hygiene Scale
C

1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – ** .5 ** .5 ** .4 ** .3 ** .3 **
Physiological problems .31 .33 .05 ** .40 .34
4 .40 * * * * * .39 * .18 * 1 * 1 * 6 * 8 * 8 *
AC

1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – ** .5 ** .5 ** .4 ** .3 ** .3 ** .8 **
Cognitive problems .28 .29 .01 .33 .29
5 .37 * * * * * .35 * .14 * 2 * 1 * 6 * 5 * 8 * 3 *
1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – ** .4 ** .5 ** .4 ** .4 ** .3 ** .8 ** .8 **
Emotional problems .22 .16 .02 .20 .18
6 .30 * * * * * .24 * .08 * 5 * 0 * 7 * 1 * 7 * 1 * 1 *
1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – ** .4 ** .4 ** .3 ** .3 ** .3 ** .8 ** .7 ** .5 **
Problematic sleep environment .25 .36 .06 ** .40 .33
7 .35 * * * * * .36 * .20 * 6 * 4 * 9 * 2 * 1 * 8 * 2 * 9 *
1 – ** ** ** ** ** – ** – ** .3 ** .3 ** .2 ** .2 ** .3 ** .7 ** .5 ** .5 ** .6 **
Substances .48 .31 .02 .48 .42
8 .39 * * * * * .46 * .22 * 5 * 2 * 9 * 0 * 0 * 6 * 4 * 0 * 8 *
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

SLEEP – TIME, LENGTH, QUALITY, AND HYGIENE 28

Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used. MSFsc, .


† p < .10. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
EP
C
AC
ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 29
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 3

Effects on sleep time and sleep length.


Time in bed Time in bed
MSFsc Social jetlag
on schooldays on weekends
β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p
Within classes
Morningness–eveningness –0.40 (0.02) *** –0.48 (0.02) *** 0.21 (0.02) *** –0.16 (0.02) ***
Age 0.39 (0.05) *** 0.35 (0.05) *** –0.38 (0.04) *** –0.20 (0.06) **

PT
Sex (female) –0.12 (0.03) *** 0.06 (0.03) † –0.11 (0.03) *** 0.22 (0.04) ***
Screen media use 0.13 (0.02) *** 0.07 (0.02) *** –0.15 (0.02) *** –0.15 (0.02) ***
Time leaving home 0.05 (0.02) * –0.18 (0.02) *** 0.23 (0.02) *** 0.03 (0.02)
Between classes

RI
Special needs 0.44 (0.09) *** 0.49 (0.09) *** –0.18 (0.11) † 0.11 (0.13)
Lower track 0.36 (0.05) *** 0.33 (0.06) *** –0.10 (0.05) † –0.09 (0.06)
Intermediate track 0.14 (0.04) ** 0.12 (0.05) ** –0.08 (0.04) * –0.06 (0.05)

SC
Grade level –0.10 (0.03) ** –0.12 (0.03) *** –0.07 (0.03) * 0.01 (0.04)
School start time 0.02 (0.02) 0.01 (0.03) –0.06 (0.02) ** –0.03 (0.03)
Effect size R2 R2 R2 R2
Within classes 0.26 0.28 0.17 0.06

U
Between classes 0.94 0.83 0.95 0.61
Sniders and Bosker 0.42 0.37 0.44 0.11
N 2761 2761 2794 2769
AN
Note: Multilevel regression analysis calculated in Mplus 7. All continuous variables were z standardized beforehand
(morningness–eveningness with higher values indicating morningness, age, screen media use with higher values indicating
more screen time, time leaving home, and school start time are clock times. Grade levels are from fifth grade to tenth grade
of secondary school. Dummy variables (coded 0, 1) are sex, special needs, lower track, and intermediate track. The reference
M

group among the school types is the upper track (gymnasium). β are standardized regression coefficients. MSFsc,; SE,
standard error; Snijders and Bosker: explained variance (R2) following Snijders and Bosker [62].
† p < .10. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.
D
TE
C EP
AC
ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 30
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 4

Effects on Sleep Quality (ASWS).


Problems
Problems in Problems in Problems in Problems in
in returning
going to bed falling asleep maintaining sleep Re-initiating sleep
to wakefulness
β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p
Within classes
Morningness–eveningness –0.55 (0.02) *** –0.41 (0.02) *** –0.27 (0.02) *** –0.22 (0.02) *** –0.76 (0.02) ***

PT
Age –0.03 (0.05) 0.02 (0.05) 0.05 (0.05) –0.02 (0.05) –0.10 (0.04) *
Sex (female) 0.09 (0.03) ** 0.21 (0.04) *** 0.31 (0.04) *** 0.27 (0.04) *** 0.14 (0.03) ***
Screen media use 0.13 (0.02) *** 0.07 (0.02) *** 0.02 (0.02) 0.02 (0.02) –0.02 (0.02)
Time leaving home –0.03 (0.02) –0.01 (0.03) –0.03 (0.03) 0.01 (0.03) –0.02 (0.02)

RI
Between classes
Special needs 0.07 (0.13) –0.03 (0.10) –0.05 (0.09) –0.15 (0.10) –0.06 (0.12)
Lower track –0.04 (0.06) –0.10 (0.06) † –0.08 (0.06) –0.13 (0.06) * –0.07 (0.04)

SC
Intermediate track –0.03 (0.04) –0.05 (0.04) –0.03 (0.05) –0.06 (0.05) –0.06 (0.04) †
Grade level –0.03 (0.03) –0.07 (0.03) † –0.04 (0.03) –0.07 (0.03) * 0.03 (0.03)
School start time –0.01 (0.03) 0.00 (0.03) 0.04 (0.03) 0.03 (0.03) –0.04 (0.02) †
Effect size R2 R2 R2 R2 R2
Within classes 0.32 0.18 0.09 0.06 0.53

U
Between classes 0.73 0.50 0.36 0.35 0.85
Sniders and Bosker 0.35 0.19 0.10 0.08 0.56
AN
N 2809 2809 2809 2809 2809
Note: See footnote to Table 2. ASWS, Adolescent Sleep Wake Scale; SE, standard error.
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 31
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Table 5

Effects on Sleep Hygiene (ASHS).


Physiological Cognitive Emotional Problematic sleep
Substances
problems Problems Problems environment
β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p β (SE) p
Within classes
Morningness–eveningness –0.26 (0.02) *** –0.26 (0.02) *** –0.23 (0.02) *** –0.21 (0.02) *** –0.22 (0.02) ***
Age 0.23 (0.05) *** 0.12 (0.05) ** 0.14 (0.05) ** 0.20 (0.06) *** 0.40 (0.05) ***

PT
Sex (female) 0.20 (0.04) *** 0.29 (0.03) *** 0.37 (0.04) *** 0.13 (0.04) *** 0.05 (0.04)
Screen media use 0.18 (0.02) *** 0.18 (0.02) *** 0.07 (0.02) *** 0.23 (0.02) *** 0.12 (0.02) ***
Time leaving home –0.05 (0.02) * –0.07 (0.02) ** –0.03 (0.02) –0.07 (0.02) ** –0.07 (0.02) **
Between classes

RI
Special needs 0.26 (0.08) ** 0.23 (0.12) † –0.02 (0.10) 0.39 (0.08) *** 0.08 (0.09)
Lower track 0.21 (0.05) *** –0.02 (0.05) –0.06 (0.06) 0.37 (0.05) *** 0.22 (0.05) ***
Intermediate track 0.08 (0.04) * –0.06 (0.04) –0.09 (0.05) † 0.15 (0.04) *** 0.13 (0.05) **

SC
Grade level –0.02 (0.03) 0.02 (0.03) 0.00 (0.03) –0.05 (0.03) –0.01 (0.03)
School start time 0.03 (0.02) 0.01 (0.02) 0.01 (0.03) 0.06 (0.02) ** 0.04 (0.03)
Effect size R2 R2 R2 R2 R2
Within classes 0.14 0.14 0.09 0.13 0.12

U
Between classes 0.98 0.90 0.81 0.98 0.94
Sniders and Bosker 0.25 0.22 0.14 0.24 0.32
N 2809 2809 2809 2809 2809
AN
Note: See footnote to Table 2. ASHS = Adolescent Sleep Hygiene Scale; SE, standard error.
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 32

Fig. 1. Distribution of morningness–eveningness scores. CSM, Composite Scale of Morningness.

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 33

Fig. 2. Differences in sleep time, sleep length, sleep quality, and sleep hygiene by school type.

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

ADOLESCENTS’ SLEEP-WAKE PROBLEMS 34

Note: Four school types are shown: special needs, lower, intermediate, and higher track, which refer to the cognitive abilities of the pupils. The words above and below the respective scales illustrate
the coding of the scales (eg, in MSFsc, it refers to late MSFsc and early MSFsc). Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. MSFsc,

Significant differences (* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001) were tested via multilevel analyses while controlling for all covariates (see Tables 3─5).

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC
ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT
Highlights

• Multilevel analyses of morningness−eveningness and sleep-related habits of students


from secondary education were conducted.
• Morningness was a significant predictor of sleep time, quality, and hygiene.
• School type explained variance in sleep time and hygiene, but not sleep quality.

PT
RI
U SC
AN
M
D
TE
C EP
AC

You might also like