You are on page 1of 13

Developmental Psychology

© 2018 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 55, No. 2, 377–389


0012-1649/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000612

The Role of Fathers’ Versus Mothers’ Parenting in Emotion-Regulation


Development From Mid–Late Adolescence: Disentangling Between-Family
Differences From Within-Family Effects

Caspar J. Van Lissa Renske Keizer


Utrecht University Erasmus University

Pol A. C. Van Lier Wim H. J. Meeus


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Utrecht University and Tilburg University


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Susan Branje
Utrecht University

This 4-year, multi-informant longitudinal study (N ! 480, initial age: 15) investigated the interplay between
parental support, behavioral and psychological control, and adolescents’ emotion regulation development. We
examined reciprocal effects between parents and children, mothers’ versus fathers’ unique roles in emotion
regulation development, and sex differences. Multi-informant data allowed us to compare effects of
adolescent-perceived and parent-reported parenting. Finally, innovative analyses allowed us to disentangle
between-family differences from within-family predictive processes. Parenting and emotion regulation were
associated at the between-family and within-family levels, especially according to adolescent reports. Support
primarily played a role between mothers and adolescents, and perceived behavioral control between fathers
and adolescents. Sex moderation revealed that support played a more prominent role in mother– daughter than
mother–son relationships, and that daughters experienced greater behavioral control. Child effects outnum-
bered parent effects, which might reflect the increasing equality of adolescent–parent relationships. Finally,
adolescent-perceived parenting was a stronger correlate of emotion regulation than parent-reports, suggesting
that adolescents’ perceptions are a relevant source of information for research and practice. Consistent with
the self-determination theory perspective on parenting, emotion regulation flourished when adolescents felt
like mothers provided support, and fathers loosened behavioral control. These results are in line with the
notion that mother– child relationships are supportive attachment relationships, whereas fathers provide
“activation” relationships, challenging adolescents to regulate emotions autonomously by providing less
explicit structure.

Keywords: emotion regulation, socialization, adolescence, random intercept cross lagged panel model,
longitudinal

Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000612.supp

Emotion regulation refers to the processes involved in modulat- tional demands, despite their emotional states (Gratz & Roemer,
ing, understanding, and accepting emotional responses, which 2004). This ability is essential for positive psychosocial adjustment
enable individuals to act in goal-oriented ways, and meet situa- and mental health (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010).
Recently, there has been a call for research to examine parents’
role in emotion regulation development in adolescence (Bariola,
Gullone, & Hughes, 2011); a developmentally sensitive period for
This article was published Online First December 20, 2018. emotion regulation (Van Lissa et al., 2014; Zimmermann & Iwan-
Caspar J. Van Lissa, Department of Methodology & Statistics, Utrecht ski, 2014). The present study aimed to answer this call and con-
University; Renske Keizer, Department of Rotterdam, Family Sociology, tribute to the current understanding of parents’ socialization of
Erasmus University; Pol A. C. Van Lier, Department of Clinical Devel- emotion-regulation in several ways. First, we examined reciprocal
opmental Psychopathology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam; Wim H. J.
effects between parents and children, in line with the interactional
Meeus, Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University, and Depart-
view of parenting (Kerr, Stattin, & Özdemir, 2012). Second, emo-
ment of Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University; Susan Branje,
Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University. tion socialization is hypothesized to be a domain in which fathers
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Caspar J. play a unique role (Paquette, 2004). We therefore examined moth-
Van Lissa, Department of Methodology & Statistics, Utrecht University, ers’ and fathers’ unique influences, moving beyond prior research
Padualaan 14, 3584 CH Utrecht, the Netherlands. E-mail: c.j.vanlissa@uu.nl that focused on mothers. We also explored sex differences, be-

377
378 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE

cause boys and girls are raised differently, and might be differen- of these parenting dimensions will likely have different associa-
tially affected by parenting behaviors (Keizer, Lucassen, Jaddoe, tions with adolescents’ emotion regulation development.
& Tiemeier, 2014; Seiffge-Krenke & Pakalniskiene, 2011). Third, There is a strong theoretical link between support and emotion
we compared patterns of results for adolescent perceptions of regulation development. Baumrind (1991, p. 62) defined respon-
parenting, and parental self-reports. Although parents’ and chil- siveness (akin to support) as actions that “intentionally foster
dren’s perspectives on their relationship tend to diverge in adoles- individuality, self-regulation and self-assertion by being attuned,
cence (Van Lissa et al., 2015), most studies focus on single supportive and acquiescent to the child’s special needs and de-
informants, or aggregate dual-informant reports (De Los Reyes & mands”. Research has validated this conceptual association (for
Kazdin, 2005). Finally, we disentangled stable differences between reviews, see Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998; Morris et
families from predictive links between parenting and children’s al., 2007). For example, adolescent-reported support predicted
outcomes within families (Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman, 2015). overtime increases in coping (Seiffge-Krenke & Pakalniskiene,
2011), and decreased internalizing symptoms (Van der Giessen,
Branje, & Meeus, 2014). This suggests that support might predict
Parents’ Role in Adolescents’ Emotion
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

increased emotion regulation, as well.


Regulation Development
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Behavioral control reflects the standards parents set, which


According to the self-determination theory of parenting (Jousse- require adolescents’ self- and emotion regulation to live up to
met, Landry, & Koestner, 2008), parents face the challenge of (Baumrind, 1991; Stattin & Kerr, 2000). The structure provided by
teaching children how to function effectively within the family and parental control benefits young children’s emotion regulation de-
society at large, while at the same time nurturing children’s fun- velopment (Morris et al., 2007). According to social domain the-
damental need for autonomy. This challenge is particularly rele- ory, however, parental control might come to frustrate adolescents’
vant in adolescence, when parent– child relationships become less autonomy needs over time, as their concept of the personal domain
hierarchical (Branje, Laursen, & Collins, 2013). Young children expands (Smetana & Daddis, 2002). Indeed, adolescents’ percep-
require parental guidance in emotion regulation (Morris, Silk, tions of being controlled have been linked with both internalizing
Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007; Thompson & Meyer, 2007), and externalizing problems (Meeus, 2016; Stattin & Kerr, 2000),
but adolescents benefit more when parents support autonomous suggesting they might also negatively predict emotion regulation
management of emotional difficulties (Sarıtaş, Grusec, & Gençöz, development. Conversely, adolescents’ emotion regulation might
2013). However, parents are often still concerned with instilling increase when parents gradually relinquish control.
proper values in their children, even when it comes to issues that Whereas behavioral control provides a clear structure for ado-
adolescents view as personal (Deković, Noom, & Meeus, 1997). lescents’ appropriate behavior, psychological control is more in-
Parents’ desire to raise children well might thus motivate them to trusive (Barber & Harmon, 2002; Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2010).
respond proactively to adolescents’ emotional dysregulation, just It involves internally controlling strategies, which frustrate ado-
like they would do in childhood. Adolescents, in turn, can perceive lescents’ autonomy needs—invalidating children’s perspectives,
parental involvement as a threat to their growing autonomy needs love withdrawal, guilt-induction, shaming, and anxiety induction
(Allen & Manning, 2007). Consequently, distressed adolescents (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2010). Psychological control is asso-
might pull away from parents, rather than turning to them for ciated with internalizing problems (Barber & Harmon, 2002; Mor-
support. If parents do exercise control in domains that adolescents ris et al., 2002; Soenens & Vansteenkiste, 2010), which suggests
perceive to be personal, such as adolescents’ emotional experi- that it would also be linked with emotion dysregulation.
ences, parental efforts are likely to backfire, leading to further
emotional distress (Meeus, 2016; Smetana & Daddis, 2002). The
Child Effects on Parenting
literature thus suggests that adolescents’ increasing autonomy
needs can sometimes be at odds with parents’ desire to raise According to the transactional view of family relationships,
children well. families are dynamic systems in which members exert reciprocal
influences (Kerr et al., 2012). In adolescence, parent– child rela-
tionships become more egalitarian (Branje et al., 2013), and par-
Mechanisms of Parental Influence
ents face the challenge of balancing control and autonomy granting
One way in which parents contribute to emotion regulation (Smetana & Daddis, 2002). Average developmental trajectories
development is through specific parenting behaviors (Morris et al., show that parental control and support decrease, and conflict first
2007). Three behaviors that are especially relevant for emotion peaks and then decreases, as families adjust to this increasing
regulation socialization are support, behavioral control, and psy- equality (Barber, Maughan, & Olsen, 2005; Meeus, 2016). Within
chological control (e.g., Kerr et al., 2012). These constitute the individual families, however, such changes in parenting are likely
underpinnings of the parenting styles taxonomy (Barber & Har- to be contingent on children’s age-appropriate adjustment. Parents
mon, 2002; Baumrind, 1991; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Support expect maturing children to regulate their emotions increasingly
refers to parental warmth and responsiveness to the adolescents’ autonomously (Dix, 1991). If adolescents meet this expectation,
needs. Behavioral control refers to the structure parents provide parents might grant autonomy by providing support and reducing
through rule setting, and attempts to regulate children’s behaviors control. If children fall short of this expectation, parents might
to conform to social or family norms. Psychological control, on the shift toward more controlling parenting. Cross-sectional research
other hand, refers to intrusive, passive-aggressive parenting behav- has indeed found that parents were less supportive, and more
iors, such as invalidating the child’s feelings and opinions, guilt punitive, toward older adolescents’ negative emotion displays
induction, and blaming the child (Barber & Harmon, 2002). Each (e.g., Klimes-Dougan et al., 2007). Longitudinal research has
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 379

similarly found that adolescents’ adaptive emotion regulation pre- girls are more sensitive to the affective family climate (Denham,
dicted overtime increasing support, diminishing psychological Mitchell-Copeland, Strandberg, Auerbach, & Blair, 1997). We
control (Otterpohl & Wild, 2015), and diminishing maternal be- might thus expect mothers to provide greater support to girls, and
havioral control (Van der Giessen, Branje, Keijsers, et al., 2014). girls to be more influenced by this than boys.

Mothers’ and Fathers’ Roles in Emotion Adolescent Perceptions Versus Parental Self-Reports
Regulation Development Although multi-informant data are widely encouraged, reporter
The notion that fathers might play a unique role in emotion discrepancies are rarely considered (De Los Reyes & Kazdin,
socialization hails back to attachment theory (see Lamb & Lewis, 2005). There is substantial evidence that adolescents’ perceptions
2013). According to Paquette (2004), the “attachment relation- of parenting are a valid and important source of information, which
ship,” based on comfort in stressful situations, best characterizes might have implications for adolescents’ adjustment beyond par-
mother– child relationships. Relationships with fathers can be con- ents’ self-reports. In adolescence, parents’ and children’s perspec-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

strued as an “activation relationship”: Fathers encourage children tives on their relationship diverge temporarily (Branje et al., 2013;
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to take risks and overcome obstacles, which opens children to the Van Lissa et al., 2015). The intergenerational stake hypothesis
world and helps develop their socioemotional skills (Majdandžić, posits that parents are more emotionally invested in this relation-
Möller, de Vente, Bögels, & van den Boom, 2014; Paquette, ship, and project more positive feelings, whereas children are
2004). Through rough-and-tumble play, for example, fathers pro- motivated to enhance differences, because they desire indepen-
vide unpredictable emotional stimuli, expanding children’s emo- dence (Bengtson & Kuypers, 1971). Indeed, research has shown
tional repertoire (Lamb & Lewis, 2013). Whether mothers and that parents view their own child rearing behaviors overly opti-
fathers continue to contribute in unique ways to emotion regulation mistically (Bögels & van Melick, 2004). Interestingly, adolescent-
development in adolescence remains to be examined (Bariola et perceived parenting correlated more strongly with observers’ re-
al., 2011). ports than with mothers’ self-reports (Gonzales, Cauce, & Mason,
Studies on middle and late childhood suggest that mothers and 1996). Parent-reported parenting also correlates less strongly with
fathers continue to play unique roles in emotion regulation devel- covert aspects of well-being, such as emotional adjustment, than
opment beyond early childhood. For example, mothers are more with overt aspects, such as academic achievement (Cheung, Po-
likely to respond constructively to children’s negative emotion merantz, Wang, & Qu, 2016). This might be because teens are
expressions, whereas fathers are more likely to minimize them, or more inclined to share emotional troubles with friends than with
encourage inhibition (Cassano, Perry-Parrish, & Zeman, 2007). parents (Ackard, Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & Perry, 2006). Con-
Compared to mothers, fathers also respond more punitively to sequently, adolescents’ perceptions of parenting might hold pre-
children’s displays of vulnerable emotions (e.g., Eisenberg et al., dictive power for their emotional adjustment, above and beyond
1999). Fathers’ controlling behavior was also a more consistent parents’ self-reports, and researchers should examine both, rather
predictor of children’s socioemotional outcomes than supportive than aggregating them (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005). In support
behavior, whereas this pattern was reversed for mothers (McDow- of this, effects from parent-reported control to adolescents’ mal-
ell, Parke, & Wang, 2003). Finally, adolescents’ relationships with adjustment were mediated by adolescent-perceived control (Kaki-
mothers tend to be closer, whereas fathers are viewed as authority hara, Tilton-Weaver, Kerr, & Stattin, 2010).
figures (Branje et al., 2013; Klimes-Dougan et al., 2007; Lamb &
Lewis, 2013). In line with the notions of the attachment and Between-Family Differences Versus
activation relationship (Paquette, 2004), these findings suggest that
Within-Family Processes
support is a more salient socialization mechanism for mothers, and
control for fathers. Developmental studies often aim to provide insight into within-
family (causal) processes. However, cross-lagged panel models
have come under considerable scrutiny, because their time-lagged
Differences Between Sons and Daughters
effects can be substantially biased if stable between-family differ-
Some sex differences may be relevant to the interplay between ences are unaccounted for (Hamaker et al., 2015; Keijsers, 2016).
parenting and emotion regulation development. First of all, ado- A stronger case can be made about effects parents might have on
lescent girls typically report greater emotion regulation difficulties their own children (or vice versa) if one shows that within-family
than boys (Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003). Second, parents raise changes in parenting precede changes in children’s outcomes. This
boys and girls differently (Seiffge-Krenke & Pakalniskiene, 2011), is accomplished using the random-intercept cross-lagged panel
exercising greater control over girls than boys (e.g., Pomerantz & model (Hamaker et al., 2015), which splits the variance in indi-
Ruble, 1998; Smetana & Daddis, 2002). Third, boys and girls viduals’ responses into (a) stable between-family differences, and
might be differently affected by parenting (Keizer et al., 2014). For (b) within-family fluctuations. Correlations at the between-family
example, according to social learning theory, the same-sex parent level can reflect, for example, the “crystallized” effects of parent-
plays a stronger role in a child’s development (Bussey & Bandura, ing at an earlier age, or third-variable effects on parenting and
1999). However, other studies suggest that children’s relationships children’s outcomes. The within-family predictive effects address
with mothers are closer than with fathers (Klimes-Dougan et al., questions such as “If fathers display relatively more behavioral
2007) and that mother– daughter relationships are closest of all control than they usually do in one year, do their children display
(Branje et al., 2013). Mothers express greater support toward relatively lower emotion regulation in the next year?” Traditional
daughters than sons (Leaper, Anderson, & Sanders, 1998), and approaches conflate these two levels, and disentangling them pro-
380 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE

vides a more nuanced understanding of the links between parent- most of adolescents’ families were classified as medium- to high-
ing and emotion regulation. SES (10% low-SES).

The Present Study Procedure and Design


This 4-year longitudinal study set out to investigate links be- RADAR was approved by the medical ethical committee of the
tween parenting behaviors and emotion regulation from mid to late University Medical Center at Utrecht University. Participants were
adolescence. On the basis of the literature, we formulated the recruited from randomly selected schools in the province of
following hypotheses. Utrecht, and four main cities in the Netherlands. Of 1,081 families
contacted, 470 refused and 114 failed to produce informed con-
Support sent. From 2008 to 2012, trained interviewers conducted four
annual home visits to collect data on adolescents’ self-reported
We expected parental support to be positively associated with difficulties in emotion regulation and perceived parental support,
adolescents’ emotion regulation, both at the between- and within-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavioral control, and psychological control. Families received


family level, and in terms of parenting effects and child effects.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

financial compensation for their participation in annual measure-


We hypothesized these links to be stronger for mothers than for ments (€100).
fathers. We further expected to find greater support, and stronger
associations between support and emotion regulation in mothers
and daughters than in mothers and sons. Measures
Reliability was estimated separately for each variable at each
Behavioral Control time point. We report ranges of reliabilities here; all scale descrip-
tives and intercorrelations are reported in Table S3 in the supple-
We hypothesized that behavioral control would be negatively mental materials. We also report intraclass correlations (ICC)
associated with emotion regulation at both the between- and across measurement waves, which reflect the proportion of vari-
within-families level, and in terms of parenting effects and child ance due to stable between-individual differences.
effects. We hypothesized that these links would be stronger for Emotion regulation. Emotion regulation was reported by ad-
fathers than for mothers. We expected behavioral control to be olescents, using the reverse-coded 32-item difficulty in emotion
higher for girls than boys. regulation scale (e.g., “When I’m upset, I have difficulty thinking
about anything else,” Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Responses ranged
Psychological Control from 1 (Almost never) to 5 (Almost always). This scale distin-
guishes six aspects of difficulties in emotion regulation, including
We hypothesized that psychological control would be nega- lack of emotional awareness, lack of emotional clarity, impulse
tively associated with emotion regulation at the between- and behavioral control difficulties, difficulties engaging in goal-
within-families levels, and in terms of both parenting effects and directed behavior, nonacceptance of emotional responses, and lim-
child effects. We predicted finding greater psychological control ited access to emotion regulation strategies. Reliability analyses
for girls than boys. (reported on https://osf.io/su9n6/) indicated that, in each wave, the
As both adolescents and parents reported on support and behav- emotional awareness items correlated low or negatively with the
ioral control, we were able to examine and compare how total scale and diminished reliability. Similarly, exploratory factor
adolescent-perceived and parent-reported parenting related to analyses with Oblimin rotation indicated that, in each wave, two
emotion regulation. Psychological control was reported by adoles- factors explained most of the item variance, with the first factor
cents only. We hypothesized that links between parenting and containing all items except those related to lack of emotional
emotion regulation would be stronger for adolescent-perceived awareness, and the second factor containing all items related to
parenting, than for parent-reported parenting. lack of emotional awareness. These results are in line with a prior
validation study on the RADAR population (Neumann, Van Lier,
Method Gratz, & Koot, 2010), which also showed that the emotional
awareness subscale correlated negatively with most other sub-
Participants scales of the DERS. We therefore omitted the emotional awareness
subscale from further analyses. Together, the remaining items had
Participants were 480 adolescents (273 boys; age at T1: M ! excellent reliability, "s .94 –.95. The ICC was moderately high;
15.04, SD ! 0.46) and their parents (data were available for 475 ICC ! .53.
mothers and 436 fathers), enrolled in the longitudinal RADAR Support. Adolescents rated perceived maternal and paternal
study (Van Lier et al., 2018). At T1, 99% of adolescents reported support, and parents provided self-reports, using the 8-item Sup-
living with their biological or adoptive mother (two reported living port subscale of the Network of Relationships Inventory (e.g.,
with a step- or foster mother, and two without a mother); 90% “Does your mother/father admire and respect you?”, Furman &
reported living with the biological or adoptive father (6% with a Buhrmester, 1985). Responses ranged from 1 (Not at all) to 5
step- or foster father, 4% without a father, and one with two (Very much). Reliability ranged from good to excellent for
mothers). All adolescents were Dutch nationals, although a minor- adolescent-perceived support, "s .83–.89, and ranged from accept-
ity (4.28%, one missing) indicated having a different ethnic back- able to good for parent-reported support, "s .72 –.80. All ICCs
ground. On the basis of parents’ reports of employment status, were moderately high, ICCs .62–.68.
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 381

Behavioral control. Adolescents rated perceived maternal and Jalal’s nonparametric MCAR test, p ! .39. As covariance
and paternal behavioral control, and parents provided self-reports, coverage exceeded the minimum of .10 (range: .76 –.95), full
using the 5-item Parental Behavioral control scale of the Parenting information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) was war-
Practices questionnaire (e.g., “Do you need permission from your ranted to make use of all available information without estimating
mother/father to come home late on a weekday?” Stattin & Kerr, missing data. Thus, families could be included, as long as data
2000). Responses ranged from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always). Reliability from least one family member were available. To evaluate model
ranged from good to excellent for both adolescent-perceived con- fit, we considered Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
trol, "s .85 –.91, and parent-reported control, "s .85–.89. All ICCs (RMSEA) # .05 and comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-
were low, ICCs .41–.44. Lewis Index $ .95, to indicate good fit (Little, 2013). We also
Psychological control. Adolescents rated both parents’ psy- provide two comparative fit indices, for which lower values indi-
chological control using the 8-item Psychological Control scale cate better fit: The Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) and
(e.g., “My mother/father always tries to change my thoughts and sample-size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (aBIC).
feelings,” Barber & Harmon, 2002). Responses ranged from 1 (Not Model building. We used the random intercept cross-lagged
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

applicable at all) to 5 (Very applicable). Reliability ranged from panel model (Hamaker et al., 2015) to investigate associations
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

good to excellent, "s .85–.90. ICCs were moderately high for between parenting and emotion regulation at the between- and
maternal (ICC ! .57) and paternal (ICC ! .66) psychological
within-family levels. The difference from a traditional cross-
control.
lagged panel model is that the variance of the measured variables
is partitioned into a random intercept, which captures stable
Strategy of Analyses between-individual differences across all time points, and within-
Syntax for all analyses are available at https://osf.io/su9n6/. person residuals, which capture individual deviations from a per-
Mean scores were calculated for each scale. Descriptive statistics son’s stable level within each time point. These latent variables are
are presented in Table 1. Analyses were conducted in Mplus centered, and the means structure of the measured variables is
Version 7.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 –2012). Per the developers’ freely estimated. A two-variable, four-wave version of this model
recommendation, we used robust maximum-likelihood estimation. is presented visually in Figure 1. In the present study, this model
Attrition ranged from 9.26% in the first wave to 14.26% in the last was extended to three variables. To maintain an acceptable
wave. There was no evidence for a relationship between missing- parameter-to-N ratio (Little, 2013), we conducted analyses sepa-
ness and the values of observed data, as indicated by Jamshidian rately for the three parenting behaviors.

Table 1
Variable Means and Standard Deviations, by Wave and Adolescent Sex

Emotion
Support Behavioral control Psychological control regulation
Mother Father Mother Father Mother Father
Wave Sex M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Adolescent reports
1 Boys 3.65 .56 3.48 .58 3.29 1.02 2.97 1.04 1.72 .68 1.85 .71 4.15 .56
Girls 3.78 .64 3.39 .67 3.51 1.03 3.08 1.04 1.85 .76 1.96 .83 3.83 .78
Total 3.70 .60 3.44 .62 3.39 1.03 3.02 1.04 1.77 .72 1.90 .77 4.01 .68
2 Boys 3.52 .62 3.40 .62 3.09 1.02 2.75 1.00 1.79 .70 1.89 .72 4.16 .57
Girls 3.78 .61 3.35 .67 3.52 1.13 3.07 1.08 1.96 .78 1.96 .78 3.80 .79
Total 3.63 .63 3.38 .64 3.27 1.09 2.89 1.05 1.86 .74 1.92 .74 4.00 .70
3 Boys 3.53 .59 3.35 .66 2.67 1.05 2.53 .97 1.73 .69 1.82 .70 4.13 .57
Girls 3.80 .65 3.36 .75 3.21 1.17 2.77 1.13 1.89 .79 1.92 .80 3.82 .80
Total 3.65 .63 3.36 .70 2.91 1.13 2.64 1.05 1.80 .74 1.87 .75 4.00 .69
4 Boys 3.47 .63 3.34 .63 2.44 1.06 2.22 .91 1.74 .75 1.81 .71 4.04 .66
Girls 3.77 .61 3.32 .78 2.78 1.23 2.36 1.11 1.89 .77 1.90 .81 3.83 .81
Total 3.60 .64 3.33 .70 2.58 1.15 2.28 1.00 1.80 .76 1.85 .75 3.95 .73
Parent reports
1 Boys 3.41 .44 3.25 .47 4.13 .97 3.92 .95
Girls 3.48 .45 3.23 .47 4.21 .92 4.00 .92
Total 3.44 .44 3.24 .47 4.16 .95 3.96 .94
2 Boys 3.37 .44 3.17 .50 3.80 1.11 3.62 1.03
Girls 3.52 .46 3.23 .44 3.88 1.00 3.75 1.00
Total 3.44 .45 3.20 .48 3.83 1.06 3.67 1.02
3 Boys 3.34 .46 3.19 .48 3.30 1.13 3.20 1.07
Girls 3.49 .48 3.19 .47 3.33 1.11 3.24 1.04
Total 3.40 .47 3.19 .48 3.31 1.12 3.22 1.06
4 Boys 3.38 .48 3.17 .55 2.80 1.08 2.65 1.01
Girls 3.51 .52 3.20 .49 2.91 1.16 2.93 1.03
Total 3.44 .50 3.19 .52 2.84 1.11 2.77 1.03
382 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 1. Accurate representation of a two-variable random-intercept cross-lagged panel model. Four-wave


RI-CLPM of maternal support (MS) and emotion regulation (ER). The variance of observed indicators is
partialized into a between-family level random intercept and within-family level deviations (dev.). Numbered
parameters are estimated freely; unnumbered parameters are constrained over time. For the present study, this
model has been extended by adding a third variable. Simplified illustrations of the final models are presented in
Figures 2 and 3.

Model building consisted of five steps, summarized in Table 2 cross-lagged panel models. Improved model fit indicated that
for adolescent-perceived parenting and in Table 3 for parent- accounting for stable between-family differences was defensible.
reported parenting. First, we estimated traditional cross-lagged Third, we constrained all autoregressive and cross-lagged regres-
panel models. Second, we extended these to random-intercept sion coefficients and within-time correlations over time, to in-

Table 2
Adolescent-Report Model Fit Indices

&%2
Model %2 df scf AIC aBIC RMSEA CFI TLI p value

Support
1. Unconstrained CLPM 115.92 27 1.45 7430.35 7493.35 .08 .95 .89 —
2. Unconstrained RI-CLPM 14.11 21 1.30 7292.66 7361.65 .00 1.00 1.01 .00
3. Constrained RI-CLPM 35.95 45 1.32 7273.63 7318.63 .00 1.00 1.01 .59
4. Sex mod., all free RI-CLPM 160.20 114 1.27 7191.91 7257.90 .04 .98 .97 .00
5. Sex mod., constrained RI-CLPM 174.06 130 1.30 7183.99 7233.99 .04 .98 .98 .50
Behavioral Control
1. Unconstrained CLPM 90.19 27 1.27 11121.53 11184.52 .07 .97 .92 —
2. Unconstrained RI-CLPM 17.20 21 1.20 11039.22 11108.21 .00 1.00 1.01 .00
3. Constrained RI-CLPM 42.43 45 1.22 11022.33 11067.32 .00 1.00 1.00 .40
4. Sex mod., all free RI-CLPM 167.44 114 1.11 10965.61 11031.60 .04 .97 .97 .00
5. Sex mod., constrained RI-CLPM 184.84 129 1.13 10959.44 11010.43 .04 .97 .97 .26
Psychological control
1. Unconstrained CLPM 119.97 27 1.36 8414.78 8477.77 .08 .95 .89 —
2. Unconstrained RI-CLPM 8.23 21 1.31 8274.73 8343.72 .00 1.00 1.02 .00
3. Constrained RI-CLPM 49.69 45 1.37 8283.90 8328.90 .01 1.00 1.00 .02
4. Sex mod., all free RI-CLPM 151.80 114 1.29 8259.99 8325.98 .04 .98 .98 .00
5. Sex mod., constrained RI-CLPM 177.94 135 1.34 8260.52 8305.51 .04 .98 .98 .19
Note. Sex mod. ! moderated by adolescent sex. Constrained ! parameters not significantly different between boys and girls have been constrained.
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 383

Table 3
Parent-Report Model Fit Indices

&%2
Model %2 df scf AIC aBIC RMSEA CFI TLI p value

Support
1. Unconstrained CLPM 176.16 27 1.17 5435.13 5498.38 .11 .94 .85 —
2. Unconstrained RI-CLPM 17.34 21 1.02 5259.11 5328.39 .00 1.00 1.00 .00
3. Constrained RI-CLPM 52.49 45 1.05 5248.36 5293.54 .02 1.00 1.00 .07
4. Sex mod., all free RI-CLPM 198.12 114 1.06 5222.86 5289.12 .06 .97 .96 .00
5. Sex mod., constrained RI-CLPM 216.35 133 1.07 5206.89 5254.08 .05 .97 .97 .44
Behavioral Control
1. Unconstrained CLPM 136.71 27 1.04 11040.16 11103.42 .09 .95 .88 —
2. Unconstrained RI-CLPM 32.35 21 .94 10940.25 11009.53 .03 .99 .98 .00
3. Constrained RI-CLPM 61.31 45 1.00 10923.39 10968.57 .03 .99 .99 .21
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

4. Sex mod., all free RI-CLPM 180.74 114 1.00 10913.72 10979.99 .05 .97 .96 .00
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

5. Sex mod., constrained RI-CLPM 197.63 133 1.03 10898.46 10945.65 .04 .97 .97 .46
Note. Sex mod. ! moderated by adolescent sex. Constrained ! parameters not significantly different between boys and girls have been constrained.

crease degrees of freedom and aid interpretability. These time 8.60, p ! .003. This means that, when girls perceive more maternal
constraints did not significantly worsen fit for most of the models, support than usual, they report increased emotion regulation one year
although fit indices were inconclusive for adolescent-reported psy- later. Regarding child effects, emotion regulation positively predicted
chological control. For this model, we deemed the misfit intro- maternal support for both sexes.
duced by time constraints to be acceptable when weighed against Parent-reported support. At the between-family level, only
the added parsimony. Fourth, we used multigroup models to test maternal support was significantly positively correlated with emo-
sex moderation, and freely estimated all structural model param- tion regulation (see Figure 3). Moreover, in line with predicted sex
eters for boys and girls. Fifth, in order to obtain more parsimonious differences, the mean intercept of maternal support was higher for
final models, we constrained all parameters that did not differ girls than for boys, indicating that mothers provided more support
significantly between boys and girls to be equal (based on Wald %2 to daughters, %2(1) ! 11.48, p # .001.
tests). All final models showed good fit according to all indices, Within families, there were no significant predictive effects between
and AIC and aBIC showed a steady decline from each model to the parent-reported support and emotion regulation. There was, however, a
next. We used Wald tests to evaluate our hypotheses about differ- bidirectional interplay between parents, indicating that they followed each
ences between mothers and fathers. other’s lead in terms of the support they provided to children.

Results Behavioral Control


Standardized model coefficients are graphically depicted in Fig- Adolescent-perceived behavioral control. At the between-
ure 2 (adolescent-perceived parenting), and Figure 3 (parent- family level, we found no evidence for the hypothesized negative
reported parenting), and clarified in the text below. associations between adolescent-perceived control and emotion reg-
ulation. However, in line with predicted sex differences, the mean
intercepts of behavioral control from both mothers and fathers were
Support
significantly greater for girls than for boys, %2(1) ! 15.09, p # .001,
Adolescent-perceived support. At the between-family level, and %2(1) ! 4.39, p ! .036, respectively.
the intercepts of adolescent-perceived maternal and paternal support Within families, effects were in line with hypotheses. There
were both positively correlated with the intercept of emotion regula- were negative effects from behavioral control to emotion regula-
tion (see Figure 3). This means that, as hypothesized, adolescents who tion for fathers, but not for mothers. Adolescents’ emotion regu-
reported higher levels of parental support also reported higher levels lation, in turn, negatively predicted paternal behavioral control.
of emotion regulation than adolescents who perceived lower levels of Unexpectedly, we found that fathers’ behavioral control positively
parental support. This correlation was significantly stronger for fa- predicted mothers’ behavioral control for parents of boys, but not
thers’ support than mothers’ support, %2(1) ! 3.87, p ! .049. Finally, for parents of girls, %2(1) ! 12.72, p # .001.
in line with hypothesized sex differences, the mean intercept of Parent-reported behavioral control. At the between-family
maternal support was significantly higher for girls than boys, indicat- level, we found no evidence for the hypothesized negative
ing that girls reported receiving more maternal support than boys, associations between parent-reported control and emotion reg-
%2(1) ! 40.90, p # .001. Between-family level correlations did not ulation. However, in line with predicted sex differences, the
differ between boys and girls. mean random intercept of father-reported behavioral control
Within families, we found predictive effects between emotion was significantly greater for girls than for boys, indicating that
regulation and adolescent-perceived maternal support, but not pater- fathers reported controlling girls more than boys, %2(1) ! 4.56,
nal support. In line with our hypothesis that these effects would be p ! .033.
strongest between mothers and daughters, perceived maternal support Within families, there were no significant predictive effects
positively predicted girls’, but not boys’, emotion regulation %2(1) ! between parent-reported behavioral control and emotion regula-
384 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Simplified representation of models for adolescent-perceived parenting. Standardized coefficients


are displayed. Fluctuations in parameters constrained over time are due to differences in the time-specific
residual variances. For all parameters, see Table S2 in the supplemental materials. Parameters significantly
different between boys and girls are presented as Boys/Girls (e.g., .04/.27!!!). ! p ! .05. !! p # .01. !!! p # .001.

tion. There was, however, a significant bidirectional interplay predicted. The strength of these associations did not differ signifi-
between maternal and paternal control, indicating that parents cantly between mothers and fathers. We found no support for the
followed each other’s lead in terms of the control they exerted on hypothesized sex differences in the mean level of psychological
their children. control between boys and girls.
Within families, we found no evidence for the hypothesized
Adolescent-Perceived Psychological Control effects of psychological control on adolescents’ emotion regula-
tion, but we did find significant child effects. Children’s emotion
At the between-family level, intercepts of adolescent-perceived regulation negatively predicted both parents’ psychological con-
maternal and paternal psychological control were both negatively trol. These effects did not differ between mothers and fathers,
correlated with the intercept of children’s emotion regulation, as %2(1) ! 0.02, p ! .885.
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 385
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 3. Simplified representation of models for parent-reported parenting. Standardized coefficients are
displayed. Fluctuations in parameters constrained over time are due to differences in the time-specific residual
variances. For all parameters, see Table S2 in the supplemental materials. Parameters significantly different
between boys and girls are presented as Boys/Girls (e.g., .04/.27!!!). ! p ! .05. !! p # .01. !!! p # .001.

Finally, across all models, the mean intercept of emotion regulation known about the role of parents in later stages of development.
was significantly lower for girls than boys, indicating that, as pre- Our findings demonstrate that, in mid- to late adolescence,
dicted, girls reported lower emotion regulation than boys. This dif- children’s perceptions of their parents’ socialization practices
ference was significant, with %2s(1) between 23.06 and 27.62, ps # are a significant predictor of emotion regulation development.
0.001. Specifically, daughters’ perceived maternal support predicts
increasing emotion regulation, and decreasing perceived pater-
Discussion nal control predicts increasing emotion regulation for all ado-
lescents. These findings are in line with the self-determination
The aim of the present study was to examine the role of
theory perspective on parenting (Joussemet et al., 2008), which
parenting in emotion regulation development from mid- to late
implies that adolescents flourish when parents support their
adolescence. Substantial evidence was found for the hypothesized
autonomy needs. Our findings differ from socialization effects
links between parenting and emotion regulation, but primarily
in early childhood: At a younger age, support plays a prominent
when examining adolescent-perceived parenting. Support played a
role for both sexes (Morris et al., 2007; Thompson & Meyer,
greater role in adolescent–mother relationships, especially for
2007). We found that support still plays a role between mothers
girls, and behavioral control played a greater role in adolescent–
and adolescent daughters, but not sons. Moreover, behavioral
father relationships. Across all models, child effects substantially
outnumbered parent effects. control benefits young children’s emotion regulation (Morris et
al., 2007). We found no evidence for such positive effects in
adolescence, but instead, found that emotion regulation in-
Parent Effects
creased when youngsters perceived that fathers relinquished
Parents are known to play an important role in emotion behavioral control. These findings suggest that the same par-
regulation socialization in infancy and early childhood (Eisen- enting behaviors have different connotations for adolescents
berg et al., 1998; Morris et al., 2007), but relatively little was than for younger children.
386 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE

Child Effects 2000). Any such factors that independently predict fathers’ parenting
and children’s outcomes would cause them to be correlated. Surpris-
Our results suggest that emotion regulation predicts ingly, father-reported support was not significantly correlated with
adolescent-perceived parenting. When emotion regulation in- emotion regulation, whereas mother-reported support was. As support
creases, adolescents feel like they are supported more and is thought to be an especially important socialization mechanism for
controlled less. The downside is that adolescents with emotion
mothers (Lamb & Lewis, 2013), mothers’ perceptions of support
regulation difficulties feel as though mothers reduce support,
provided to their children might be more accurate.
and fathers increase behavioral control. It is noteworthy that
We found no between-family level associations between be-
child effects substantially outnumbered parent effects. This may
havioral control and emotion regulation. The fact that we did
be explained, in part, by the fact that parent– child relationships
find negative predictive within-family effects from adolescent-
become more egalitarian in adolescence (Meeus, 2016). Evi-
perceived paternal control to emotion regulation raises the
dence for parent effects on emotion regulation has been found
question whether these negative links first come to the fore in
in younger children (e.g., see Eisenberg et al., 1998; Morris et
mid-to-late adolescence. If they had already been present at an
al., 2007), whereas work with early adolescents also revealed
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

earlier age, we should have observed a between-family level


mostly child effects (Otterpohl & Wild, 2015). An important
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

correlation. Perhaps adolescents’ increasing autonomy leads


caveat is that these child effects were based on adolescent-
them to question the legitimacy of behavioral control, and
perceived parenting. Thus, one explanation is that shifts in
perceive it as intrusive and as an impediment to autonomous
emotion regulation abilities might color adolescents’ percep-
emotion regulation (Smetana, 1995). We further found that
tions of parents’ behavior. Adolescents who experience diffi-
children who perceived their parents to be more psychologically
culties coping with negative emotions might, for example,
controlling had lower levels of emotion regulation. Substantial
perceive their mothers as less supportive, because their need for
evidence links psychological control to younger children’s
support has increased— even if mothers’ behavior did not
emotion regulation difficulties (Barber & Harmon, 2002; Mor-
change. Similarly, adolescents’ tolerance for behavioral control
ris et al., 2002). Thus, the between-family level correlation we
might decrease when they are experiencing emotion dysregu-
found might reflect accumulated effects of psychological con-
lation, leading them to perceive parents as more controlling.
trol on children’s emotion regulation throughout childhood.
An alternative explanation is that adolescents are accurately
reporting changes in parents’ behaviors that parents themselves
do not notice (see Gonzales et al., 1996). Prior research has Differences Between Mothers and Fathers
found that parents indeed disengage in response to children’s
problem behaviors, by reducing support and control (Huh, In line with the distinction between “attachment” and “acti-
Tristan, Wade, & Stice, 2006; Kerr et al., 2012). Similarly, our vation” relationships (Majdandžić et al., 2014; Paquette, 2004),
findings suggest that adolescents feel like mothers respond to we found that perceived support played a role in mother–
difficulties in emotion regulation by decreasing support— but adolescent relationships, and perceived behavioral control in
they also report increases in fathers’ behavioral control and father–adolescent relationships. The finding that emotion reg-
both parents’ psychological control. This suggests that adoles- ulation increased when adolescents felt like fathers relinquished
cents feel like their parents respond to emotion dysregulation by behavioral control can be construed as an age-appropriate man-
attempting to step in and “fix” them (Barber & Harmon, 2002). ifestation of the activation relationship: Fathers challenge ado-
lescents to live up to family and societal norms autonomously
Correlations at the Between-Families Level by decreasing explicit demands and structure for adolescents.
Conversely, if fathers do not gradually reduce control, this
For both adolescent-perceived and parent-reported parenting, we
found that children of parents who provided greater support had might frustrate adolescents’ autonomy needs, tax their emotion
higher levels of emotion regulation. Interestingly, for adolescent- regulation, and curb its development (Smetana, 1995).
perceived support, these links were stronger for fathers than for
mothers, even though maternal support played a larger role at the Differences Between Sons and Daughters
within-family level. One potential explanation is that adolescents’
perceptions of fathers’ support may have shaped emotion regulation In line with prior research, we found that girls reported
development at an earlier age, as others have found (e.g., Sarkadi, greater difficulties in emotion regulation than boys. We also
Kristiansson, Oberklaid, & Bremberg, 2008). Another explanation replicated findings that, according to both adolescent and parent
might be found in different base-rates of maternal versus paternal reports, girls received greater support from mothers, and greater
support: On average, mothers provide high support, whereas fathers’ behavioral control from parents, than boys (e.g., Leaper et al.,
support is more variable (Laible & Carlo, 2004). Because greater 1998; Pomerantz & Ruble, 1998). Finally, perceived maternal
variability implies a potentially larger correlation, differences in fa- support positively predicted girls’, but not boys’, emotion reg-
thers’ support might matter more. Finally, another explanation might ulation. These results repudiate social learning theory’s propo-
be that third variables predict both paternal support and children’s sition that links between parents and the same-sex child are
emotion regulation. For example, sociologists have argued that pater- stronger (Bussey & Bandura, 1999). Instead, our results were in
nal involvement is increasingly becoming a privilege of the highly line with the notion that mother– daughter relationships are
educated (Perelli-Harris et al., 2010). Highly educated parents are also particularly close and that support plays a more prominent role
able to afford houses in better neighborhoods, and more nutritious in mother– daughter relationships (Branje et al., 2013; Denham
food, which predicts desirable childhood outcomes (Cabrera & Peters, et al., 1997).
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 387

Reporter Discrepancies sirable to investigate these dimensions in isolation. Second, the


random-intercept cross-lagged panel model is not amenable to cate-
We predicted that links between parenting and emotion reg- gorical predictors, so it would be impossible to tease apart between-
ulation would be weaker for parent-reported than for and within-family variance in parenting styles. Another limitation is
adolescent-perceived parenting, because parent-reported par- that we measured general parenting practices, not emotion socializa-
enting is known to be a poor predictor of children’s emotional tion specifically (cf. Klimes-Dougan et al., 2007). Future research
adjustment (Ackard et al., 2006; Cheung et al., 2016). In line might use dyadic interactions to investigate whether parents’ support-
with this prediction, we found substantial evidence for the ive and controlling responses to adolescents’ situational emotion
hypothesized links between parenting and emotion regulation regulation have similar effects to the ones we found using self-report
when examining adolescents’ perceptions of parenting, but measures. A final consideration is that, in addition to the between-
parent-reported parenting was mostly uncorrelated with emo- family differences in levels of parenting behavior and emotion dys-
tion regulation. An important implication for research is that regulation we examined, families probably differ in the extent to
adolescents’ perceptions of parenting appear to be a legitimate which parents and children influence each other. The random-
source of information in relation to covert aspects of their
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

intercept cross-lagged panel model only yields an average estimate of


adjustment. Regarding interventions, our findings suggest that
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

within-family predictive effects. Recently, dynamic structural equa-


it might be at least as important to change adolescents’ percep- tion modeling methods have become available, which capture
tions of parenting as it is to change parents’ actual behaviors. between-family variation in within-family effects (Asparouhov, Ha-
Adolescents’ growing capacity for perspective taking (Van maker, & Muthén, 2018). We were unable to apply these methods,
Lissa et al., 2014) might help them to consider parents’ point of however, as they require a larger sample with more measurement
view. Moreover, recent experimental research showed that in- occasions. An important caveat is thus that we focused on differences
citing adolescents to take parents’ perspective motivates them between fathers and mothers in the average cross-lagged effects
to work constructively toward mutual benefit in conflict dis- across families. Average patterns do not apply to all families, and
cussions (Van Lissa, Hawk, & Meeus, 2017). Future research further research is required to explore between-family heterogeneity
should investigate whether reflecting on the ways in which in cross-lagged effects, and within-family differences between mater-
mothers support them, and fathers relinquish behavioral con- nal and paternal roles.
trol, might benefit adolescents’ emotion regulation develop-
ment, and whether any effects are enhanced when such refram- Conclusions
ing is accompanied by actual parental behavior change.
The present paper demonstrated that parents continue to play a role
Strengths and Limitations in emotion regulation development in adolescence—at least when it
comes to adolescent-perceived parenting. We examined the role of
One strength of this study is that the longitudinal design allowed us
fathers, who, despite being theorized to play a unique role in emotion
to answer the call for research on parents’ role in emotion regulation
socialization, have rarely been studied. We found that perceived
development from mid- to late adolescence (Bariola et al., 2011). By
support primarily played a role between mothers and adolescents,
obtaining multi-informant reports of parents’ child rearing behaviors,
whereas perceived behavioral control played a role between fathers
we were able to compare mothers’ and fathers’ unique influences and
and adolescents. In line with the transactional perspective, we exam-
examine differences in the pattern of results for adolescent-perceived
ined bidirectional influences between parenting and emotion regula-
versus parent-reported parenting. The most important contribution,
tion. Child effects substantially outnumbered parent effects, which
however, is the use of innovative methods that account for stable
might reflect the increasing equality of adolescent–parent relation-
differences between families and provide more accurate estimates of
ships (Branje et al., 2013). We also contrasted adolescent-perceived
overtime predictive effects within families. Future research should be
and parent-reported parenting. As the aforementioned child effects
mindful of this distinction, and the pitfalls of failing to account for
were found for adolescent-perceived parenting behavior, it is possible
between-family differences when examining within-family processes.
that shifts in emotion regulation color adolescents’ perceptions of
The present study also had several caveats. Relying on adolescent
parents’ behavior. Overall, our results suggest that, in adolescence,
reports for both the parenting variables and emotion regulation might
perceptions of parenting are a stronger correlate of emotion regulation
introduce common method bias. However, a typical assumption is
development than parents’ self-reports. In clinical settings, it might
that this bias affects all measurement occasions equally (Podsakoff,
thus be at least as important to change adolescents’ perceptions of
MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). Under this assumption, the
parenting as it is to change parents’ actual behaviors. Finally, we used
random-intercept correlations capture this bias, and within-family
cutting-edge statistical techniques to control for stable differences
effects are controlled for its influence. Common method bias also
between families, and obtain more accurate estimates of within-family
cannot explain the differentiated pattern of findings, in line with
(potentially causal) processes. In line with the self-determination
hypotheses derived from prior research that used different methods
perspective on adolescent development, our main finding is that
and sources of information. Another limitation is that we distin-
adolescents’ emotion regulation benefits most when they feel like
guished individual parenting dimensions, whereas some have argued
parents are meeting their increasing need for autonomy by providing
that these dimensions should aggregated into parenting styles (Mac-
support and relinquishing control.
coby & Martin, 1983). Both approaches have yielded valuable com-
plementary insights (see Kerr et al., 2012). In the present study, it was
unfeasible to use parenting styles for two reasons: First, since the References
literature suggested an asymmetry in the role of support and control in Ackard, D. M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Perry, C. (2006).
mothers’ versus fathers’ emotion regulation socialization, it was de- Parent– child connectedness and behavioral and emotional health among
388 VAN LISSA, KEIZER, VAN LIER, MEEUS, AND BRANJE

adolescents. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 30, 59 – 66. petence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 65– 86.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2005.09.013 http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024426431247
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion- Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and
regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25. http://dx.doi
Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j .org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.3
.cpr.2009.11.004 Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental social-
Allen, J. P., & Manning, N. (2007). From safety to affect regulation: ization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. http://dx.doi.org/
Attachment from the vantage point of adolescence. New Directions for 10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1
Child and Adolescent Development, 2007, 23–39. http://dx.doi.org/10 Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C.,
.1002/cd.192 & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions:
Asparouhov, T., Hamaker, E. L., & Muthén, B. (2018). Dynamic structural Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child
equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 25, 359 –388. http://dx Development, 70, 513–534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00037
.doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2017.1406803 Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the
Barber, B. K., & Harmon, E. L. (2002). Violating the self: Parental personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychol-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

psychological control of children and adolescents. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), ogy, 21, 1016 –1024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children and Gonzales, N. A., Cauce, A. M., & Mason, C. A. (1996). Interobserver
adolescents (pp. 15–52). Washington, DC: American Psychological agreement in the assessment of parental behavior and parent–adolescent
Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10422-002 conflict: African American mothers, daughters, and independent observ-
Barber, B. K., Maughan, S. L., & Olsen, J. A. (2005). Patterns of parenting ers. Child Development, 67, 1483–1498. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
across adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Develop- 1131713
ment, 2005, 5–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cd.124 Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emo-
Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and adolescent tion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and
emotion regulation: The role of parental emotion regulation and expres- initial validation of the difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal
sion. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 198 –212. of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54. http://dx.doi
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0092-5 .org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the
competence and substance use. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102–116. http://
56 –95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0272431691111004 dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038889
Bengtson, V. L., & Kuypers, J. A. (1971). Generational difference and the Huh, D., Tristan, J., Wade, E., & Stice, E. (2006). Does problem behavior
developmental stake. Aging & Human Development, 2, 249 –260. http:// elicit poor parenting? A prospective study of adolescent girls. Journal of
dx.doi.org/10.2190/AG.2.4.b Adolescent Research, 21, 185–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Bögels, S. M., & van Melick, M. (2004). The relationship between child- 0743558405285462
report, parent self-report, and partner report of perceived parental rearing Joussemet, M., Landry, R., & Koestner, R. (2008). A self-determination
behaviors and anxiety in children and parents. Personality and Individ- theory perspective on parenting. Canadian Psychology, 49, 194 –200.
ual Differences, 37, 1583–1596. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012754
.02.014 Kakihara, F., Tilton-Weaver, L., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2010). The
Branje, S. J. T., Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (2013). Parent– child relationship of parental control to youth adjustment: Do youths’ feelings
communication during adolescence. In A. L. Vangelisti (Ed.), Routledge about their parents play a role? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39,
handbook of family communication (Vol. 2, pp. 271–286). New York, 1442–1456. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9479-8
NY: Routledge. Keijsers, L. (2016). Parental monitoring and adolescent problem behaviors:
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender How much do we really know? International Journal of Behavioral Devel-
development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676 –713. opment, 40, 271–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025415592515
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676 Keizer, R., Lucassen, N., Jaddoe, V., & Tiemeier, H. (2014). A prospective
Cabrera, N. J., & Peters, H. E. (2000). Public policies and father involve- study on father involvement and toddlers’ behavioral and emotional
ment. Marriage & Family Review, 29, 295–314. http://dx.doi.org/10 problems: Are sons and daughters differentially affected? Fathering, 12,
.1300/J002v29n04_04 38 –51. http://dx.doi.org/10.3149/fth.1201.38
Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Özdemir, M. (2012). Perceived parenting style and
parental socialization of children’s sadness regulation. Social Develop- adolescent adjustment: Revisiting directions of effects and the role of
ment, 16, 210 –231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00381.x parental knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1540 –1553. http://
Cheung, C. S., Pomerantz, E. M., Wang, M., & Qu, Y. (2016). Controlling dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027720
and autonomy-supportive parenting in the United States and China: Klimes-Dougan, B., Brand, A. E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Usher, B., Hastings,
Beyond children’s reports. Child Development, 87, 1992–2007. http:// P. D., Kendziora, K., & Garside, R. B. (2007). Parental emotion social-
dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12567 ization in adolescence: Differences in sex, age and problem status.
Deković, M., Noom, M. J., & Meeus, W. (1997). Expectations regarding Social Development, 16, 326 –342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
development during adolescence: Parental and adolescent perceptions. 9507.2007.00387.x
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 253–272. http://dx.doi.org/10 Laible, D. J., & Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of maternal and
.1007/s10964-005-0001-7 paternal support and control to adolescent social competence, self-worth,
De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the and sympathy. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 759 –782. http://dx
assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical .doi.org/10.1177/0743558403260094
framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bul- Lamb, M. E., & Lewis, C. (2013). Father– child relationships. In N. J.
letin, 131, 483–509. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483 Cabrera & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), Handbook of father involve-
Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & ment: Multidisciplinary perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 119 –135). New York,
Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional com- NY: Routledge.
EMOTION REGULATION ADOLESCENCE 389

Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender tematic review of longitudinal studies. Acta Paediatrica, 97, 153–158.
effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmen- http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00572.x
tal Psychology, 34, 3–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3 Seiffge-Krenke, I., & Pakalniskiene, V. (2011). Who shapes whom in the
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York, family: Reciprocal links between autonomy support in the family and
NY: Guilford Press. parents’ and adolescents’ coping behaviors. Journal of Youth and Ado-
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the lescence, 40, 983–995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10964-010-9603-9
family: Parent– child interaction. In P. H. Mussen. (Ed.), Handbook of Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion
child psychology: Formerly Carmichael’s manual of child psychology. regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem
New York, NY: Wiley. behavior. Child Development, 74, 1869 –1880. http://dx.doi.org/10
Majdandžić, M., Möller, E. L., de Vente, W., Bögels, S. M., & van den .1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x
Boom, D. C. (2014). Fathers’ challenging parenting behavior prevents Smetana, J. G. (1995). Parenting styles and conceptions of parental au-
social anxiety development in their 4-year-old children: A longitudinal thority during adolescence. Child Development, 66, 299 –316. http://dx
observational study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 301– .doi.org/10.2307/1131579
310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9774-4 Smetana, J. G., & Daddis, C. (2002). Domain-specific antecedents of
McDowell, D. J., Parke, R. D., & Wang, S. J. (2003). Differences between parental psychological control and monitoring: The role of parenting
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mothers’ and fathers’ advice-giving style and content: Relations with beliefs and practices. Child Development, 73, 563–580. http://dx.doi.org/
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

social competence and psychological functioning behavior in middle 10.1111/1467-8624.00424


childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 55–76. http://dx.doi.org/10 Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the
.1353/mpq.2003.0004 concept of parental psychological control: Proposing new insights on the
Meeus, W. (2016). Adolescent psychosocial development: A review of basis of self-determination theory. Developmental Review, 30, 74 –99.
longitudinal models and research. Developmental Psychology, 52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.11.001
1969 –1993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000243 Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation.
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. Child Development, 71, 1072–1085. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-
(2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion 8624.00210
regulation. Social Development, 16, 361–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation
j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x in the family. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Sessa, F. M., Avenevoli, S., & Essex, 249 –268). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
M. J. (2002). Temperamental vulnerability and negative parenting as inter- Van der Giessen, D., Branje, S., Keijsers, L., Van Lier, P. A. C., Koot,
acting predictors of child adjustment. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, H. M., & Meeus, W. (2014). Emotional variability during mother–
461– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00461.x adolescent conflict interactions: Longitudinal links to adolescent disclo-
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los sure and maternal control. Journal of Adolescence, 37, 23–31. http://dx
Angeles, CA: Author. .doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.10.007
Neumann, A., van Lier, P. A. C., Gratz, K. L., & Koot, H. M. (2010). Van der Giessen, D., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2014). Perceived autonomy
Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation difficulties in ado- support from parents and best friends: Longitudinal associations with
lescents using the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Assessment, adolescents’ depressive symptoms. Social Development, 23, 537–555.
17, 138 –149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191109349579 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sode.12061
Otterpohl, N., & Wild, E. (2015). Cross-lagged relations among parenting, Van Lier, P. A. C., Frijns, T., Neumann, A., Den Exter Blokland, E., Koot,
children’s emotion regulation, and psychosocial adjustment in early H. M., & Meeus, W. (2018). The RADAR young study: Design, descrip-
adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, tion of sample, and validation of cohort assignment. Unpublished man-
93–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.862802 uscript.
Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the father– child relationship: Mechanisms Van Lissa, C. J., Hawk, S. T., Branje, S. J., Koot, H. M., Van Lier, P. A.,
and developmental outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193–219. http:// & Meeus, W. H. (2015). Divergence between adolescent and parental
dx.doi.org/10.1159/000078723 perceptions of conflict in relationship to adolescent empathy develop-
Perelli-Harris, B., Sigle-Rushton, W., Kreyenfeld, M., Lappegård, T., ment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 48 – 61.
Keizer, R., & Berghammer, C. (2010). The educational gradient of van Lissa, C. J., Hawk, S. T., de Wied, M., Koot, H. M., van Lier, P., &
childbearing within cohabitation in Europe. Population and Develop- Meeus, W. (2014). The longitudinal interplay of affective and cognitive
ment Review, 36, 775– 801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1728-4457.2010 empathy within and between adolescents and mothers. Developmental
.00357.x Psychology, 50, 1219 –1225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035050
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Van Lissa, C. J., Hawk, S. T., & Meeus, W. H. J. (2017). The effects of
Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the affective and cognitive empathy on adolescents’ behavior and outcomes
literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, in conflicts with mothers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
88, 879 –903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 158, 32– 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.002
Pomerantz, E. M., & Ruble, D. N. (1998). The role of maternal control in Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early
the development of sex differences in child self-evaluative factors. Child adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differ-
Development, 69, 458 – 478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998 ences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental varia-
.tb06202.x tions. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 182–194.
Sarıtaş, D., Grusec, J. E., & Gençöz, T. (2013). Warm and harsh parenting http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515405
as mediators of the relation between maternal and adolescent emotion
regulation. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 1093–1101. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1016/j.adolescence.2013.08.015 Received July 12, 2017
Sarkadi, A., Kristiansson, R., Oberklaid, F., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Revision received May 14, 2018
Fathers’ involvement and children’s developmental outcomes: A sys- Accepted July 19, 2018 !

You might also like