You are on page 1of 12

Emotion © 2015 American Psychological Association

2016, Vol. 16, No. 2, 280 –291 1528-3542/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000142

Maternal Emotion Socialization Differentially Predicts Third-Grade


Children’s Emotion Regulation and Lability

Megan L. Rogers Amy G. Halberstadt


Florida State University North Carolina State University

Vanessa L. Castro Jennifer K. MacCormack and Patricia Garrett-Peters


Northeastern University University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Numerous parental emotion socialization factors have been implicated as direct and indirect contributors
to the development of children’s emotional competence. To date, however, no study has combined
parents’ emotion-related beliefs, behaviors, and regulation strategies in one model to assess their
cumulative—as well as unique— contributions to children’s emotion regulation. We considered the 2
components that have recently been distinguished: emotion regulation and emotional lability. We
predicted that mothers’ beliefs about the value of and contempt for children’s emotions, mothers’
supportive and nonsupportive reactions to their children’s emotions, as well as mothers’ use of cognitive
reappraisal and suppression of their own emotions would each contribute unique variance to their
children’s emotion regulation and lability, as assessed by children’s teachers. The study sample consisted
of an ethnically and socioeconomically diverse group of 165 mothers and their third-grade children.
Different patterns emerged for regulation and lability: Controlling for family income, child gender, and
ethnicity, only mothers’ lack of suppression as a regulatory strategy predicted greater emotion regulation
in children, whereas mothers’ valuing of children’s emotions, mothers’ lack of contempt for children’s
emotions, mothers’ use of cognitive reappraisal to reinterpret events, and mothers’ lack of emotional
suppression predicted less lability in children. These findings support the divergence of emotion
regulation and lability as constructs and indicate that, during middle childhood, children’s lability may
be substantially and uniquely affected by multiple forms of parental socialization.

Keywords: children’s emotion regulation, lability, emotion socialization, emotion beliefs, parent
reactions

A central task in childhood is to develop the skills necessary for experiences and expressions while fostering an optimal level of
approaching emotion-related situations, including skills in main- engagement (Dunsmore, Booker, & Ollendick, 2013; Kim-Spoon,
taining and modulating emotional experience (Eisenberg & Mor- Cicchetti, & Rogosch, 2013; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997), has been
ris, 2002; Halberstadt, Denham, & Dunsmore, 2001). This task can associated with a host of positive outcomes for children, including
be differentiated into two related yet distinct constructs: emotion improved emotional adjustment (e.g., Alessandri et al., 2014;
regulation and lability (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). Emotion reg- Kim-Spoon et al., 2013), friendship quality and social skills (e.g.,
ulation, defined as children’s ability to manage their emotional Blair et al., 2014; Monopoli & Kingston, 2012), and academic
success (e.g., Graziano, Reavis, Keane, & Calkins, 2007; Trenta-
costa & Izard, 2007). In contrast, emotional lability, characterized
as a rapid and intense reactivity to emotional situations, as well as
This article was published Online First December 7, 2015. difficulty recovering from negative reactions (Dunsmore et al.,
Megan L. Rogers, Department of Psychology, Florida State University; 2013; Kim-Spoon et al., 2013), poses many challenges for chil-
Amy G. Halberstadt, Department of Psychology, North Carolina State dren, including increased emotional instability, irritability, and
University; Vanessa L. Castro, Department of Psychology, Northeastern intensity (Oliver & Simons, 2004; Simons, Carey, & Wills, 2009);
University; Jennifer K. MacCormack and Patricia Garrett-Peters, Depart- a variety of attentional and internalizing disorders (Carthy, Horesh,
ment of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Apter, & Gross, 2010; Sobanski et al., 2010); classroom external-
Chapel Hill. izing behaviors and bullying (Dunsmore et al., 2013; Garner &
This work was supported by grants awarded by the National Science Hinton, 2010); and greater risk for substance abuse and risky
Foundation to Amy Halberstadt and Patricia Garrett-Peters (#1023839 &
sexual behaviors later in life (Oshri, Sutton, Clay-Warner, &
#1023977). We thank the Social Science Research Institute at Duke Uni-
versity for the space to conduct our research.
Miller, 2015; Tull, Weiss, Adams, & Gratz, 2012).
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Amy With such starkly different behavioral outcomes, it is under-
G. Halberstadt, Department of Psychology, North Carolina State Uni- standable why parents would wish to scaffold children’s emotion
versity, Poe Hall 755, Box 7650, Raleigh, NC 27695-7650. E-mail: regulation and lability. Although the advantages and disadvantages
amy_halberstadt@ncsu.edu associated with children’s emotion regulation and lability are well

280
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 281

established, parental contributions to the development of these actions (Halberstadt et al., 2013; Meyer, Raikes, Virmani, Waters,
behaviors are unclear. Parental socialization encompasses many & Thompson, 2014). In this way, parents may help children
facets and processes, but two constructs—parents’ beliefs about develop the skills for controlling their emotions. Indeed, parents
children’s emotions and parents’ reactions to children’s emo- who reported that they valued children’s emotions had children
tions— have been theorized to be important contributors to chil- who were better able to regulate their emotions in school (Rams-
dren’s emotion regulation and lability (e.g., Goodnow, 1988; Gott- den & Hubbard, 2002), and who used more adaptive coping
man, Katz, & Hooven, 1997; Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & strategies following a series of terrorist attacks, including problem-
Robinson, 2007). In addition, there is sound theoretical reasoning solving, emotion-oriented, and support-seeking coping (Halber-
to believe that parents’ own emotion regulation skills may be stadt, Thompson, Parker, & Dunsmore, 2008). Similarly, parents
modeled for children, as parents’ emotional profiles and interac- who valued attending to children’s emotions had children who
tions implicitly teach children how to manage the experience of better utilized problem-solving and emotion-focused strategies to
emotions and utilize the emotion regulatory styles most visible to manage emotions (Meyer et al., 2014). Parents who value chil-
them (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998; Morris et al., dren’s emotions might facilitate regulation by being more accept-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2007; Silk, Shaw, Skuban, Oland, & Kovacs, 2006). Thus, the lack ing of, and appropriately attentive to, children’s emotions in ways
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of empirical validation for these pathways is surprising. that scaffold emotion knowledge and skill. Although previous
To remedy this gap in the literature, we examined parents’ own research has not examined relations between parental value of
emotion regulatory skills, as well as parents’ beliefs about and emotion and children’s lability, parents’ acceptance of emotions
reactions to children’s emotions, in relation to children’s emotion (compared with rejection) may also reduce lability by being less
regulation and lability. Because no research has explored how dysregulating and, therefore, might promote quicker emotional
parents’ emotion beliefs, parenting reactions, and regulatory skills recovery in children. As such, we hypothesized that parents’ value
jointly predict children’s emotion regulation or lability, we as- of children’s emotions would relate positively to children’s emo-
sessed the unique and collective contributions of all three social- tion regulation and negatively to their lability.
ization processes to children’s regulatory skills. We assessed both
emotion regulation and lability because recent theory and evidence Contempt for Children’s Emotions
suggest these are distinct constructs. For example, emotion regu-
lation and lability seem to differentially impact children’s adjust- Parents who believe that expressing contempt toward children’s
ment behaviors, ego resiliency, and academic achievement (Eisen- emotions is acceptable are likely to be more rejecting and invali-
berg et al., 2013; Valiente et al., 2013). Regulation may also dating of their children’s emotions (Halberstadt et al., 2013; Line-
modulate components of lability (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), and han, 1993). Ginott (1956) and Gottman, Katz, and Hooven (1996;
may account for relations between lability and internalizing be- Gottman et al, 1997) conceptualized parents’ contempt as a prob-
haviors in children over time (Kim-Spoon et al., 2013). lematic but central component of the dismissing parenting style.
Additionally, although previous research has focused on early Thus, by derogating children’s emotions, parents may inadver-
childhood, middle childhood may be a particularly relevant period tently increase children’s arousal and lability (Linehan, 1993).
for examining these socialization processes. In middle childhood, Although no previous studies examine the impact of parental
children further develop their regulatory skills (Eisenberg & Mor- beliefs about contempt on children’s outcomes, expressing con-
ris, 2002) and emotion knowledge (Pons, Harris, & de Rosnay, tempt within the marital relationship relates positively to internal-
2004), and as a result, parents may need to adjust the socialization izing and externalizing problems in children (Harold, Aitken, &
strategies used in early childhood in response to these maturational Shelton, 2007; Katz & Gottman, 1993). Moreover, studies link
changes. Thus, whether, and in what ways, parents continue to parents’ invalidation of children’s emotional experiences with
socialize emotion regulation in older children remain important children’s and adolescents’ emotion regulation challenges and
empirical questions. heightened lability (Buckholdt, Parra, & Jobe-Shields, 2014; Gott-
In the present study, we tested a socialization model in which man et al., 1996), as well as the development of psychopathology
mothers’ beliefs about children’s emotions, their reactions to chil- and self-injurious behaviors (Linehan, 1993). Therefore, although
dren’s negative emotions, and their own regulatory skills could we had little evidence to associate parents’ beliefs about contempt
each contribute uniquely and collectively to third-grade children’s with emotion regulation, we predicted that parental beliefs about
emotion regulation skill and lability in school. We discuss the contempt as acceptable would be positively associated with chil-
importance of two different instantiations of each of the three dren’s lability.
socialization factors in the subsequent sections.
Parents’ Emotion-Related Behaviors: Supportive and
Nonsupportive Reactions
Parents’ Emotion-Related Beliefs:
Value and Contempt
Supportive Reactions

Valuing Children’s Emotions Parents’ reactions of problem-solving, comforting, and encour-


aging discussion and expression of emotion in response to chil-
Parents who value children’s emotions likely show interest in dren’s negative emotions are thought to foster children’s ability to
and engagement with their children’s emotions, and, in so doing, manage the experience and expression of negative emotion, by
support children’s awareness and acceptance of their own emo- reducing distress and supporting children’s development of mul-
tions as well as the role of emotions in their own and others’ tiple regulatory strategies (Eisenberg et al., 1998; Fabes, Poulin,
282 ROGERS ET AL.

Eisenberg, & Madden-Derdich, 2002), particularly over time technique (Bariola, Hughes, & Gullone, 2012). In their reliance on
(Blair et al., 2014). When parents’ reactions and children’s coping suppression, mothers may fail to provide children with opportuni-
skills are tested concurrently, however, relations between these ties to observe and learn other kinds of emotion regulation skills.
two are less apparent (Perry, Calkins, Nelson, Leerkes, & Marco- Furthermore, by hampering parents’ well-being, interpersonal
vitch, 2012). Parents’ supportive reactions may be negatively functioning, and working memory, while increasing sympathetic
associated with children’s emotional lability; supportive parents nervous system activation (Gross & John, 2003), parents’ emotion
are likely to attend more to children’s labile behaviors through suppression may create a more tumultuous environment in which
problem- and emotion-focused responses. Over time, such interest children struggle to effectively regulate their emotions. Moreover,
and guidance may reduce dysregulation in children. Thus, we if children also suppress emotions, their internal physiological
hypothesized that parents’ supportive reactions will be positively arousal may increase and paradoxically become stronger than
associated with children’s emotion regulation and negatively as- before their emotions were suppressed, thereby increasing lability
sociated with children’s emotional lability. (Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). Thus, we predict that
mothers’ suppression will be negatively related to children’s emo-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Nonsupportive Reactions tion regulation and positively related to children’s lability.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Parents’ reactions of minimizing (devaluing or dismissing) and Additional Characteristics of Potential Influence
punishing (verbally penalizing) children’s expression of negative
emotions are thought to suppress children’s expression of emotion In order to determine whether parents’ emotion-related beliefs,
without providing regulation strategies or guidance (Fabes et al., behaviors, and skills are uniquely related to children’s emotion
2002). The link between parents’ nonsupportive reactions and regulation and lability, it is important to consider additional known
children’s poor emotion regulation and increased lability from sources of influence. Family income has been directly and posi-
preschool through middle childhood is supported, both when emo- tively related to children’s emotion regulation at home and in
tion regulation and lability are combined (Eisenberg & Fabes, school (Lunkenheimer, Shields, & Cortina, 2007; Samuelson,
1994; Eisenberg, Fabes, & Murphy, 1996) and when examined Krueger, & Wilson, 2012). Child gender may be relevant, in that
separately (Blair et al., 2014; Shaffer, Suveg, Thomassin, & Brad- girls are sometimes reported to have greater emotion regulation
bury, 2012). Thus, we hypothesized that nonsupportive reactions and coping skills and lower levels of lability than boys (e.g.,
would be negatively related to children’s emotion regulation and Kim-Spoon et al., 2013; Samuelson et al., 2012; but sometimes
positively related to their lability. not, as in Shaffer et al., 2012). Though less studied, some differ-
ences between European American and African American children
have been noted (Supplee, Skuban, Shaw, & Prout, 2009). Thus,
Parents’ Emotion Regulation Skill: family income, child gender, and child ethnicity were included as
Cognitive Reappraisal and Suppression covariates in our analyses.

Cognitive Reappraisal The Present Study


Reinterpreting an emotionally eliciting situation to modify its To inform and maximize parenting practices aimed at enhancing
affective impact is associated with greater positive and less nega- children’s emotion regulation skills and limiting children’s lability,
tive emotion, better interpersonal functioning, and greater well- we examined pathways by which emotion regulation and lability
being in adults (e.g., Gross, 1998; Gross & John, 2003). Parents’ are likely socialized by parents. We were interested in identifying
use of cognitive reappraisal may facilitate children’s emotion the unique predictive variance associated with specific parental
regulation by modeling an adaptive regulatory style that children socialization factors, as well as the collective contribution of all
can use when confronting their own emotional experiences. In socialization factors in predicting children’s emotion regulation
addition, parents who regularly use cognitive reappraisal to temper and lability, over and above family income, child gender, and child
emotional situations may provide a calmer, more stable environ- ethnicity. We included both positive and negative approaches
ment in which children can effectively regulate their emotions. within each socialization domain in order to gauge their unique
Reappraisal may also reduce children’s lability as they learn that contributions to children’s socioemotional development. Thus, for
problems and emotional situations can be reframed and actively beliefs, we assessed the value of children’s emotions and the
approached, handled, and resolved. Thus, we predict that parents’ acceptability and effectiveness of expressing contempt for chil-
cognitive reappraisal will be positively related to children’s emo- dren’s emotions; for behaviors, we assessed supportive and non-
tion regulation and negatively related to children’s lability. supportive reactions to children’s negative emotions; and for reg-
ulation, we assessed mothers’ use of cognitive reappraisal and
suppression. We hypothesized that mothers’ belief about the value
Suppression
of children’s emotions, their supportive reactions, and their use of
Suppression involves the inhibition of affective expression cognitive reappraisal would be positively associated with chil-
(Gross, 1998; Gross & John, 2003), and, in adults, is associated dren’s emotion regulation, but negatively related to children’s
with less positive and greater negative emotion, worse interper- lability. We also predicted that mothers’ belief about the accept-
sonal functioning and well-being, and increased sympathetic acti- ability of expressing contempt would be positively associated with
vation when experiencing negative emotions (e.g., Gross & John, children’s lability, and that their nonsupportive reactions and use
2003). Consistent with a modeling hypothesis, mothers who sup- of suppression would be negatively related with children’s emo-
press their own emotions have children who also utilize this tion regulation, but positively related to children’s lability.
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 283

We utilized a multimethod and multirespondent approach, with 2011) and with children’s emotion understanding, emotional cop-
mothers’ self-report for beliefs and behaviors, and interviews of ing, and feelings of security (Castro, Halberstadt, Lozada, & Craig,
mothers’ regulatory strategies in three different emotion-eliciting 2015; Halberstadt et al., 2008; Stelter & Halberstadt, 2011). Be-
situations, combined with teachers’ reports of children’s emotion- cause the Positive Emotions Are Valuable and Negative Emotions
regulatory strategies. We note that our study also crosses contexts, Are Valuable subscales are conceptually and empirically related
in that maternal socialization is assessed in the family environment (Castro et al., 2015), we combined them into a single Value
and children’s emotion regulation is assessed in the school context, subscale (␣ ⫽ .86).
which is a highly conservative approach when estimating potential Mothers’ supportive and nonsupportive reactions. The
influence of family socialization. Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES; Fabes,
Eisenberg, & Bernzweig, 1990; Stelter & Halberstadt, 2011) mea-
Method sures parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions. Parents
respond to the hypothetical situations by identifying the degree to
which they would use each of six types of reactions using 7-point
Participants
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Likert scales, ranging from 1 (very unlikely) to 7 (very likely).


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Participants were 165 mother– child dyads participating in a Because the CCNES includes primarily children’s reactions of
study following and supplementing a longitudinal sample that sadness, fear, nervousness, and embarrassment (e.g., “If my child
began with mothers and their infants in a small Southeastern city loses some prized possession and reacts with tears, I would . . .”),
(Garrett-Peters, Castro, & Halberstadt, 2015), and included 95 two of these items were deleted, and six items were added to assess
African American children (53.7% daughters), 67 European Amer- parents’ reactions to children’s frustration and anger (e.g., “If my
ican children (40.3% daughters), and three biracial children child yells and kicks the shopping cart when he/she cannot have a
(33.3% daughters). All children were in third grade and were toy when we are grocery shopping, I would . . .”). Confirmatory
between 7.92 and 9.73 years old (M age ⫽ 8.76, SD ⫽ 0.33). factor analyses following the original scale structure revealed that
Family income ranged from $1,600 to $420,000 (M ⫽ $84,081; one item failed to load significantly onto five of the six subscales
SD ⫽ $63,059). and thus was dropped. These results are consistent with recent tests
of the CCNES factor structure that indicate the structure of sub-
scales may require tailoring to a given sample (Nelson, Leerkes,
Procedure O’Brien, Calkins, & Marcovitch, 2012).
Prior to the lab visit, mothers completed a questionnaire assess- The supportive reactions (problem-focused, emotion-focused,
ing their supportive and nonsupportive reactions to their children’s and expressive encouragement) include reactions such as “help my
negative emotions. During the lab visit, mothers completed a child think of constructive things to do when other children tease
questionnaire on their beliefs about children’s emotions and were him/her,” “distract my child by talking about a fun activity
interviewed about regulatory strategies regarding three emotion- we could do later that day,” and “tell my child that it is okay to
related vignettes. All mothers agreed for teachers to be invited to have strong feelings when you don’t get something you want,” and
fill out questionnaires about their child. Mothers received com- were composited as per Fabes et al. (2002) and others (e.g., Blair
pensation for their time and travel reimbursement, and children et al., 2014; Shaffer et al., 2012; ␣ ⫽ .75). The nonsupportive
received a toy of their choice. reactions (minimizing and punitive) consist of reactions such as
In the spring of the children’s third grade in school, teachers “tell my child that he/she is being immature and that the toy is
received a set of questionnaires in the mail. On average, 3.46 silly” and “tell my child that he/she will be sent to his/her room
months passed between the mother– child sessions and teacher when we get home from the store,” and were also composited (see
return of questionnaire packets. A majority of teachers (82.9% of Blair et al., 2014; Shaffer et al., 2012; ␣ ⫽ .86). The CCNES and
the original sample) returned questionnaire packets and were com- various adaptations have demonstrated good internal consistency,
pensated $25 for their time. reliability over time, and construct validity (Baker, Fenning, &
Crnic, 2011; Fabes et al., 2002; Nelson et al., 2012).
Mothers’ regulation strategies. To assess mothers’ regula-
Measures
tion skill more fully than possible from currently available self-
Mothers’ beliefs. The Parents’ Beliefs about Children’s Emo- report measures, we created a new measure, drawing on vignette
tions Questionnaire (PBACE:;Halberstadt, Dunsmore, Parker, et methodology (e.g., Barrett, Lane, Sechrest, & Schwartz, 2000;
al., 2008) assesses a variety of parental beliefs about children’s Lane, Quinlan, Schwartz, Walker, & Zeitlin, 1990; Subic-Wrana et
emotions. Parents respond using a 6-point Likert scale ranging al., 2014). Mothers were interviewed regarding regulatory strate-
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Of interest are the gies associated with three vignettes (MacCormack, Halberstadt, &
10-item Positive Emotions Are Valuable subscale (e.g., “It is Sibley, 2012). In the first vignette, the mother imagines sitting with
important for children to share their positive emotions with oth- her child in a restaurant when she sees and smells black smoke
ers”; ␣ ⫽ .90), the 12-item Negative Emotions Are Valuable coming from the kitchen. In the second vignette, someone who is
subscale (e.g., “It is useful for children to feel angry sometimes”; usually critical of the mother unexpectedly pays her a compliment.
␣ ⫽ .88), and the eight-item Contempt subscale (e.g., “Making fun In the final vignette, the mother and a friend are applying for the
of children’s feelings is sometimes a good way to get them to same job; the friend gets it and the mother does not. For each
change their behavior”; ␣ ⫽ .82). The PBACE has demonstrated vignette, the mother reported the emotions she would feel, how she
construct validity through relations with parents’ socialization would know that she had those feelings, and, as the key variable,
behaviors (Halberstadt et al., 2008; Perez Rivera & Dunsmore, what she could do to change her emotions. Mothers were prompted
284 ROGERS ET AL.

to keep generating responses until they felt their responses were Table 1
complete. Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges for Maternal
Based on Gross and Thompson’s (2007) process model of Socialization Variables and Children’s Emotion Regulation
emotion regulation, cognitive reappraisal responses included rein- and Lability
terpretation (e.g., “I’d work through it and try to figure out their
intentions”), acceptance (e.g., “accept that I can’t win them all”), Variable Mean SD Range
and seeing the silver lining (e.g., “see it as an opportunity to teach Maternal beliefs
my child what to do in this type of situation”). Emotion suppres- Value 5.06 .47 3.71–6.00
sion responses included ignoring the situation (e.g., “just not think Contempt 2.09 .91 1.00–4.30
about it”) and masking one’s emotional expression (e.g., “be all Maternal behaviors
Supportive 5.76 .80 2.53–6.87
nice and happy to her even though I am so upset”). Responses were Nonsupportive 2.57 .83 1.10–5.40
independently coded by a mixed-ethnicity and mixed-gender team Maternal regulation
of research assistants (Mkappa ⱖ .90). Frequency scores for each Reappraisal 1.25 1.29 .00–6.00
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

variable were calculated as the number of times each strategy was Suppression .21 .46 .00–2.00
Child regulation
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

generated by mothers throughout across the three vignettes. For


Emotion regulation 3.19 .51 1.50–4.00
cognitive reappraisal, scores could theoretically range from 0 to 9 Lability 1.58 .51 1.00–3.40
(three types of reappraisals for three vignettes), but in fact ranged
from 0 to 6. For suppression, scores could theoretically range from
0 to 6, but in fact ranged from 0 to 2. regulatory skills. Model comparisons revealed that the two-factor
Children’s emotion regulation and lability. The Emotion model was a significantly better fit for the data than a one-factor
Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) consists of model (⌬␹2 ⫽ 204.546, ⌬df ⫽ 1, p ⬍ .001), providing empirical
two scales, Emotion Regulation and Lability. Teachers responded support for the distinction of regulation and lability in our sample.
using a 4-point Likert scale (1 ⫽ rarely/never, 2 ⫽ sometimes, 3 ⫽ These constructs were thus examined as separate dependent variables.
often, and 4 ⫽ almost always). The Emotion Regulation subscale To test the collective influence of mothers’ emotion socialization,
(eight items; ␣ ⫽ .82) measures children’s adaptive emotional we conducted two hierarchical regression models in which teachers’
control, sociability, and self-awareness (e.g., “Responds positively reports of children’s emotion regulation (see Table 3) and lability (see
when ‘others’ talk or pay attention to him/her” and “Can say when Table 4) were regressed onto mothers’ emotion-related beliefs (value
s/he is feeling sad, angry or mad, fearful or afraid”). The Lability and contempt), behaviors (supportive reactions), and skills (reap-
subscale (15 items; ␣ ⫽ .92) measures children’s reactivity, inten- praisal and suppression). Family income, child gender, and child
sity, and arousal in experiencing negative emotions, as well as ethnicity were entered as controls in the first model step of both
dysregulated affect (e.g., “Is prone to angry outbursts . . .” and models. Mothers’ beliefs about the value of children’s emotions and
“Displays exuberance that others find intrusive or disruptive”). the acceptability of contempt in response to children’s emotions were
The ERC has shown strong convergence with other established entered into the second model step. Mothers’ supportive reactions to
questionnaires assessing children’s regulatory behavior and good children’s negative emotions were entered into the third model step,
construct validity (e.g., Blair et al., 2014; Dunsmore et al., 2013; and mothers’ cognitive reappraisal and suppression strategies were
Kim-Spoon et al., 2013). entered into the fourth and final step.

Results Regression Model Predicting Children’s


Emotion Regulation
All variables demonstrated univariate and multivariate normal-
ity (see Table 1 for means, standard deviations, and ranges). The overall model predicting children’s emotion regulation (see
Bivariate correlations are presented in Table 2. Total family annual Table 3) was significant, F(8, 134) ⫽ 4.83, p ⬍ .001, R2 ⫽ .22.
income, child gender, and child ethnicity were significantly related The first model step, including family income, child gender, and
to several variables of interest, providing empirical support for child ethnicity, was significant, F(3, 139) ⫽ 10.42, R2 ⫽ .18, p ⬍
controlling for these variables. .001. Income was positively related to children’s emotion regula-
Correlations between mothers’ value and contempt beliefs, re- tion (p ⫽ .022), and remained significant in all subsequent steps
appraisal and suppression strategies, and children’s emotion reg- until the final model step, in which it became marginally signifi-
ulation and lability tended to demonstrate conceptually expected cant (p ⫽ .078). Gender was also related to children’s emotion
patterns, although not always significantly. However, mothers’ regulation (p ⫽ .001), and remained significant in all subsequent
supportive reactions were positively related to children’s lability, steps, with teachers rating girls as better at regulating emotions
and mothers’ nonsupportive reactions were not related to either than boys. Ethnicity was initially a significant predictor (p ⫽
children’s emotion regulation or lability. Because nonsupportive .046), with teachers rating European American children as
reactions were unrelated to both outcome variables, this variable better at regulating emotion than African American children,
was omitted from subsequent analyses. but dropped to marginal significance in subsequent steps (ps ⫽
Before testing the differential influence of mothers’ emotion so- .053 to .086).
cialization factors on children’s emotion regulation versus lability, we The addition of value and contempt beliefs in the second model
first examined whether emotion regulation and lability were suffi- step did not result in a significant change, F(5, 137) ⫽ 6.37, ⌬R2 ⫽
ciently different constructs. To do so, we conducted confirmatory .01, p ⫽ .650. Similarly, the addition of mothers’ supportive
factor analyses to compare one- and two-factor models of children’s reactions to children’s negative emotions in the third model step
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 285

Table 2
Correlations Between Emotion-Related Beliefs, Socialization Behaviors, and Emotion Regulation Skills

Maternal emotion Maternal emotion Maternal emotion Child emotion


Demographics beliefs behaviors regulation regulation
Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

Demographics
1. Family income ⫺.08 .46ⴱⴱⴱ .09 ⫺.18ⴱ .02 ⫺.20ⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱ ⫺.19ⴱ .29ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.32ⴱⴱⴱ
2. Child gendera .12 ⫺.04 ⫺.10 ⫺.03 .08 ⫺.01 .08 ⫺.29ⴱⴱⴱ .23ⴱⴱ
3. Child ethnicityb .05 ⫺.27ⴱⴱⴱ .01 ⫺.16ⴱ .19ⴱⴱ ⫺.12 .22ⴱⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ
Maternal beliefs
4. Value ⫺.01 .21ⴱⴱ ⫺.19ⴱⴱ .03 ⫺.03 .16ⴱ ⫺.22ⴱⴱ
5. Contempt ⫺.08 .40ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.11 .19ⴱ ⫺.02 .19ⴱ
Maternal behaviors
⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.05 ⫺.11 ⫺.07 .19ⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

6. Supportive
⫺.03 .16ⴱ ⫺.13
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

7. Nonsupportive .07
Maternal regulation
8. Reappraisal .13 .11 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ
9. Suppression ⫺.22ⴱⴱ .19ⴱ
Child regulation
10. Emotion regulation ⫺.48ⴱⴱⴱ
11. Lability
a
0 ⫽ girls, 1 ⫽ boys. b
0 ⫽ African American, 1 ⫽ European American.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .001.

did not result in a significant change, F(6, 135) ⫽ 5.59, ⌬R2 ⫽ .01, children’s emotion regulation skill (p ⫽ .285). However, mothers’
p ⫽ .215. In the fourth and final step, the addition of mothers’ suppression was significantly negatively related to children’s emo-
cognitive reappraisal and suppression strategies did not result in a tion regulation, over and above family income, child gender, child
significant change, F(8, 134) ⫽ 4.83, ⌬R2 ⫽ .03, p ⫽ .111. ethnicity, mothers’ value and contempt beliefs, and mothers’ sup-
Mothers’ cognitive reappraisal was not significantly related to portive reactions (p ⫽ .044).

Table 3
Hierarchical Regression Model Predicting Children’s Emotion Regulation

Predictor variables R2 B SE (B) ␤ 95% CI


ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 1 .18
Family income .00 .00 .20ⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gendera ⫺.29 .08 ⫺.28ⴱⴱ [⫺.45, ⫺.13]
Child ethnicityb .19 .09 .18ⴱ [.00, .37]
Step 2 .19ⴱⴱⴱ
Family income .00 .00 .20ⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gender ⫺.28 .08 ⫺.27ⴱⴱ [⫺.44, ⫺.11]
Child ethnicity .18 .09 .17 [⫺.01, .36]
Value belief .09 .09 .07 [⫺.10, .27]
Contempt belief ⫺.00 .05 ⫺.00 [⫺.09, .09]
Step 3 .20ⴱⴱⴱ
Family income .00 .00 .19ⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gender ⫺.28 .08 ⫺.27ⴱⴱ [⫺.44, ⫺.11]
Child ethnicity .18 .09 .17 [⫺.00, .37]
Value belief .11 .10 .10 [⫺.08, .30]
Contempt belief ⫺.01 .05 ⫺.02 [⫺.10, .08]
Supportive reactions ⫺.07 .05 ⫺.10 [⫺.17, .04]
Step 4 .22ⴱⴱⴱ
Family income .00 .00 .16 [.00, .00]
Child gender ⫺.25 .08 ⫺.24ⴱⴱ [⫺.42, ⫺.09]
Child ethnicity .16 .09 .15 [⫺.02, .35]
Value belief .12 .10 .10 [⫺.07, .30]
Contempt belief .01 .05 .02 [⫺.08, .10]
Supportive reactions ⫺.07 .05 ⫺.10 [⫺.18, .04]
Cognitive reappraisal .03 .03 .09 [⫺.03, .10]
Emotional suppression ⫺.20 .10 ⫺.17ⴱ [⫺.39, ⫺.01]
Note. CI ⫽ confidence interval.
a
0 ⫽ girls, 1 ⫽ boys. b 0 ⫽ African American, 1 ⫽ European American.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
286 ROGERS ET AL.

Table 4
Hierarchical Regression Model Predicting Children’s Lability

Predictor variables R2 B SE (B) ␤ 95% CI


ⴱⴱⴱ
Step 1 .16
Family income .00 .00 ⫺.25ⴱⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gendera .23 .08 .23ⴱⴱ [.07, .38]
Child ethnicityb ⫺.10 .09 ⫺.10 [⫺.28, .08]
Step 2 .21ⴱⴱⴱ
Family income .00 .00 ⫺.23ⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gender .22 .08 .22ⴱⴱ [.06, .37]
Child ethnicity ⫺.06 .09 ⫺.06 [⫺.23, .11]
Value belief ⫺.19 .09 ⫺.17ⴱ [⫺.37, ⫺.02]
Contempt belief .10 .04 .18ⴱ [.01, .18]
Step 3 .27ⴱⴱⴱ
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Family income .00 .00 ⫺.22ⴱ [.00, .00]


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Child gender .22 .08 .22ⴱⴱ [.07, .36]


Child ethnicity ⫺.07 .09 ⫺.07 [⫺.24, .10]
Value belief ⫺.25 .09 ⫺.22ⴱⴱ [⫺.43, ⫺.08]
Contempt belief .11 .04 .21ⴱⴱ [.03, .19]
Supportive reactions .15 .05 .24ⴱⴱ [.06, .25]
Step 4 .32ⴱⴱⴱ
Family income .00 .00 ⫺.17ⴱ [.00, .00]
Child gender .19 .07 .19ⴱ [.04, .33]
Child ethnicity ⫺.04 .08 ⫺.04 [⫺.20, .13]
Value belief ⫺.25 .08 ⫺.22ⴱⴱ [⫺.41, ⫺.08]
Contempt belief .09 .04 .17ⴱ [.01, .17]
Supportive reactions .15 .05 .23ⴱⴱ [.05, .24]
Cognitive reappraisal ⫺.08 .03 ⫺.22ⴱⴱ [⫺.13, ⫺.03]
Emotional suppression .19 .09 .17ⴱ [.02, .36]
Note. CI ⫽ confidence interval.
a
0 ⫽ girls, 1 ⫽ boys. b 0 ⫽ African American, 1 ⫽ European American.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

Results from the final model step indicate that child gender and to children’s lability (ps ⫽ .029 and .023, respectively), and
mothers’ suppression skills were uniquely predictive of children’s remained significant at all subsequent steps. The addition of sup-
emotion regulation skill; overall, girls were rated by teachers as portive reactions to children’s negative emotions in the third model
more emotionally regulated than were boys, and mothers who step also resulted in a significant change, F(6, 136) ⫽ 8.22, ⌬R2 ⫽
utilized suppression strategies more frequently had children who .05, p ⫽ .002. Supportive reactions were significantly and
were rated as less well regulated than mothers who utilized sup- uniquely related to children’s lability (p ⫽ .002), indicating that
pression strategies relatively infrequently. These patterns persisted mothers who employ supportive reactions when their children are
even when controlling for the effect of children’s lability on distressed or angry have children with higher levels of lability.
children’s emotion regulation, with only a slight decrease (⌬␤ ⫽ This predictor remained significant in the final model step. The
.061) in predictive value for maternal suppression. The overall addition of mothers’ cognitive reappraisal and suppression strate-
model accounted for approximately 22% of the variance in chil- gies in the fourth and final step resulted in a significant change,
dren’s emotion regulation. F(8, 134) ⫽ 7.92, ⌬R2 ⫽ .06, p ⫽ .006. Cognitive reappraisal was
negatively and suppression was positively related to children’s
Regression Model Predicting Children’s Lability lability, over and above income, child gender, child ethnicity,
The overall model predicting children’s lability (see Table 4) value and contempt beliefs, and supportive reactions (ps ⫽ .005
was significant, F(8, 134) ⫽ 7.92, p ⬍ .001, R2 ⫽ .32. The first and .026, respectively).
model step, containing income, child gender, and child ethnic- This final model step indicated many significant predictors of
ity, was significant, F(3, 139) ⫽ 8.56, R2 ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .001. children’s lability. Specifically, children’s lability was positively
Income was negatively related to children’s lability (p ⫽ .005), associated with children being male, mothers’ belief in the effec-
and remained significant in all subsequent steps. Child gender tiveness and appropriateness of contempt as a response to chil-
was related to children’s lability (p ⫽ .005), and remained dren’s emotions, mothers’ supportive reactions to their children’s
significant in all subsequent steps, with boys rated as signifi- emotions, and mothers’ utilization of suppression as an emotion
cantly more labile than girls. Child ethnicity was not associated regulation strategy, and negatively associated with income, moth-
with children’s lability (p ⫽ .264), and remained nonsignificant ers’ belief about the value of children’s emotions, and mothers’ use
in all following steps. of cognitive reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy. Results
The addition of mothers’ beliefs in the second step resulted in a were maintained when controlling for the effect of children’s
significant change, F(5, 137) ⫽ 7.39, ⌬R2 ⫽ .06, p ⫽ .009. emotion regulation on lability, with the exception of a small
Mothers’ value and contempt beliefs were both negatively related decrease in the predictive strength of maternal suppression (⌬␤ ⫽
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 287

.046). The full model accounted for approximately 32% of the socioemotional confidence and lower emotional reactivity or la-
variance in children’s teacher-reported lability. bility when confronting emotionally challenging situations. To-
gether, these results suggest that when contempt is deemed an
Discussion acceptable response to children’s emotions, and when parents fail
to model appropriate emotional displays and recovery strategies,
We examined associations between mothers’ emotion-related children may behave in a more emotionally labile manner.
beliefs, reactions, and regulatory strategies and their third-grade Contrary to predictions, mothers’ supportive reactions to chil-
children’s emotion regulation and lability, when controlling for dren’s negative emotions were positively associated with chil-
family income, child gender, and child ethnicity. Because recent dren’s emotional lability.1 This finding was initially surprising,
theory and research suggest the importance of differentiating emo- given that supportive parental reactions have been shown to facil-
tion regulation from lability, we studied these two components itate children’s attempts to regulate their emotions and reduce their
separately and discovered different socialization patterns for each. lability in early childhood (Eisenberg & Fabes, 1994; Meyer et al.,
It is noteworthy that children’s emotional lability was predicted 2014; Shaffer et al., 2012). However, in middle childhood, re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

by maternal emotion socialization factors to a greater extent than search is accumulating to suggest that mothers’ supportive reac-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

was children’s emotion regulation, which related only to mothers’ tions may be either causally problematic, by reducing older chil-
suppression of emotion in the face of challenging emotion-related dren’s opportunities to develop their own strategies and emergent
events. We expand on these findings throughout this section and autonomy, or alternatively, an adaptive response to children’s
conclude with a discussion regarding the complexity of assessing lability, with mothers responding more frequently to what they
diverse emotion socialization factors in association with children’s recognize as developmentally inappropriate volatile emotional ex-
emotional skills. periences and expressions (Castro et al., 2015; Dunsmore, Her,
Halberstadt, & Perez-Rivera, 2009; Wray-Lake, Crouter, &
Maternal Socialization of Children’s Lability in McHale, 2010). These findings suggest the importance of a devel-
Middle Childhood opmental perspective and cross-sequential longitudinal designs to
tease apart whether mothers’ supportive socialization practices are
Mothers’ beliefs that children’s emotions are not of value and more likely a cause or an effect of children’s lability.
that expressing contempt for children’s emotions is acceptable, as
well as (surprisingly) mothers’ supportive reactions in response to
children’s negative emotions, and mothers’ reduced use of cogni- Maternal Socialization of Children’s Emotion
tive reappraisal and reliance on emotional suppression were all Regulation in Middle Childhood
uniquely and concurrently predictive of children’s lability. Par-
Although we predicted associations between maternal socializa-
ents’ beliefs about children’s emotions likely reflect and predict
tion and children’s emotion regulation, few significant effects
broad components of the parent– child relationship, such as
emerged. It may be that when children are well-regulated, parents’
warmth and respect for children. Certainly, value and contempt are
emotion-related beliefs and behaviors are not particularly impor-
both intertwined with, and direct, parents’ socialization behaviors
tant, and that socialization processes may matter more when prob-
(e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1998; Gottman et al., 1997; Halberstadt et
lems exist. In addition, ratings of less emotion regulation on the
al., 2008; Lozada, Halberstadt, Craig, Dunsmore, & Dennis, in
ERC measure include being “less cheerful” and “not responding
press), and, as such, tend to have broad impact on children’s
positively to peers or adults”; these are low-affect behaviors rather
behavior. Family environments in which children’s emotions are
than the intense emotional displays associated with lability. That
not valued and/or in which contempt for emotions is expressed
is, children who are not cheerful may not be depressed or sad, but
may convey to children that their emotions are inappropriate or
may be neutral in affect, and children who are not responding
unworthy, leading to maladaptive emotion management. Children
positively to others may be somewhat disengaged, rather than
may then attempt to inhibit their emotion-related thoughts and
disagreeable or unpleasant toward others. Thus, these behaviors
displays (Fabes, Leonard, Kupanoff, & Martin, 2001), which could
may not activate as much parental concern. Regarding cognitive
paradoxically induce a rebound effect, in which thinking more
reappraisal, again, it may be that when children are well regulated,
about the emotional situation may intensify the emotions (Gross &
mothers need not scaffold children’s regulatory skills as much, and
John, 2003; Wegner et al., 1987). Such a process would further
when they are not cheerful or not interacting positively with peers,
challenge children to find ways of engaging with and shifting their
arousal, thus exacerbating emotion lability.
Furthermore, when parents model less successful regulatory 1
Because we were initially surprised by the positive association between
strategies themselves, by suppressing and not reappraising when mothers’ supportive reactions and children’s lability, we ran three regres-
reappraisals are possible, children may have fewer opportunities to sion models with each individual supportive reaction (problem-focused,
develop emotion-related skills. In contrast, by modeling reap- emotion-focused, and expressive encouragement reactions), to attempt to
elucidate the direction of effects. For example, if the association is driven
praisal of emotional events for their children, parents may be by mothers’ problem-solving behaviors, it may be more likely that mothers
supporting children’s reduction in lability by providing them with are responding to children’s high levels of lability rather than promoting
opportunities to directly observe strategies (i.e., reappraisal) for them; if mothers’ supportiveness is driven, however, by mothers’ emotion
reframing and dampening or minimizing the impact of negative encouragement behaviors, it may be more likely that mothers are encour-
aging lability rather than encouraging autonomy or control. Significant
emotional situations. Parental modeling of effective regulatory associations between each type of supportive strategy and children’s labil-
strategies and discussion of emotion regulation strategies may also ity emerged in all steps including the final models, suggesting a pervasive
help children gain a sense of regulatory efficacy, leading to greater effect across strategy types.
288 ROGERS ET AL.

parents may be reappraising those behaviors rather than reacting to children’s lability, as such efforts may, over time, shape children’s
them. developing recruitment of both the brain regions and distributed
Maternal suppression, on the other hand, was the only social- neural networks that support lability.
ization variable that significantly (and inversely) predicted chil- In line with recent suggestions regarding the reconceptualization
dren’s emotion regulation. Mothers who use suppression are likely of emotion regulation in general (Cole, 2014; Eisenberg et al.,
modeling this strategy for their children; such modeling may thus 2013), we also note some divergence between definitions of emo-
be considered a proximal socialization factor, as children are able tion regulation and its operationalization in questionnaire form.
to observe maternal suppression. In contrast, emotion beliefs are The emotion regulation scale on the ERC includes items relating to
internal processes that may not always be visibly modeled in general positivity, sociability, and emotion understanding (e.g.,
mothers’ behaviors. These factors may thus be considered distal “responds positively when adults talk to or pay attention to her/
socialization factors that act to influence children’s emotional him” and “is empathic toward others; shows concern or sadness
skills more globally, perhaps through indirect associations with when others are upset or distressed”), rather than definitional
other socialization factors. Future research would benefit from components of emotion regulation, such as “initiating, maintain-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

considering ways in which proximal and distal processes interact ing, and modulating” emotional experience and expression
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to shape children’s regulatory development over time. (Thompson, 1994) or “strategies used to increase, maintain, or
Given the known negative consequences of suppression, includ- decrease one or more components of an emotional response”
ing the rebound effect mentioned previously (Wegner et al., 1987), (Gross, 1998). Thus, this scale may need further refinement toward
this strategy is likely costly to children. Suppression may halt the specific components of emotion regulation, in a manner similar
children’s opportunities to experiment with and explore the nature, to the emotional lability subscale.
cause, consequences, and appropriate regulation of their emotions.
Finally, mothers who suppress their emotions may be struggling
The Complexity of Emotion Socialization
with their own regulation (and may even be more labile them-
selves), making it more difficult for them to teach their children Finally, the present study demonstrates the complexity of emo-
how to successfully navigate and regulate their emotions. tion socialization. Five different forms of emotion socialization
uniquely predicted children’s lability. This highlights the impor-
tance of including multiple dimensions of emotion socialization
Consideration of the Two Models Together
when predicting children’s socioemotional outcomes. In addition,
The two models tested in this study demonstrate a difference in whereas parents’ beliefs may predict their emotion socialization
the strength of relations between parental socialization variables in behaviors and potentially their own regulatory strategies, our cur-
predicting children’s emotion regulation and lability. Overall, rent findings as well as previous research suggest that parents’
emotional lability seems more amenable to socialization in middle beliefs, reactions, and regulatory strategies each provide unique
childhood compared with emotion regulation; five parental social- contributions to the familial environment and children’s outcomes
ization factors, embedded in three socialization domains, were (Baker et al., 2011; Halberstadt et al., 2013). Furthermore, as
each uniquely predictive of children’s lability, whereas only one mothers’ belief that children’s emotions are less valuable and
factor was predictive of children’s emotion regulation. These re- mothers’ supportive reactions to children’s negative emotions both
sults further highlight the distinction between emotion regulation positively predicted children’s lability, this strange pairing may
and lability, which has recently been differentiated in theory and suggest that parents’ beliefs and behaviors do not always work
research (e.g., Cole, 2014; Dunsmore et al., 2013). This distinction together, contrary to predictions from meta-emotion research,
also implies that children may have different developmental tra- which tends to couple emotion beliefs and behaviors (Gottman et
jectories for regulation and lability. If, as suggested, the difference al., 1996; Katz, Wilson, & Gottman, 1999; Meyer et al., 2014).
between emotion regulation and lability relates to the difference These results accentuate and reaffirm the importance of nesting
between effortful and reactive control, respectively (Eisenberg et children’s abilities within a fuller framework of parental emotion
al., 2013), lability may be particularly challenging for children in socialization factors.
middle childhood who have likely accumulated reactive responses Emotion socialization is complex in part because it is dynamic
over time, and thus must employ greater cognitive effort to inhibit and and transactional. The socialization strategies that parents used
control any involuntary reactive responses. Instead, it may be com- when children were young may no longer be relevant for middle
paratively easier for older children to exert effortful control in select- childhood. As the child changes, so, too, must parents adapt their
ing appropriate regulation behaviors at this age, given advancements parenting style and reactions to that child’s evolving emotions and
in executive function that support effort control (Prencipe et al., behaviors. Parents’ own emotion beliefs and regulation strategies
2011). Parents may thus target scaffolding their children’s reactive may even shift with time as parents learn more about emotions
control rather than effortful control responses, as issues with reactive through socialization behaviors. For example, a mother seeking to
control may persist into middle childhood more so than issues with teach her child how to regulate his or her emotions may realize
effortful control. Moreover, impulsive and uncontrolled behaviors more explicitly what regulatory strategies she uses and how effec-
similar to those demonstrated in lability have been associated with tive they are. This knowledge, gained through the experience of
subcortical brain regions involved in approach and avoidance, and emotion socialization, may improve parents’ emotion expertise.
reward and punishment, systems (Pickering & Gray, 1999). Social- However, the difficulties that a parent with a highly labile child
ization efforts that are approach oriented (such as valuing chil- faces could present unique socialization challenges that may also
dren’s emotions) or punishment oriented (such as expressing con- shift parents’ emotion-related beliefs and behaviors. For example,
tempt at children’s emotions) may thus be particularly relevant to parents of children with higher emotional lability report greater
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 289

parental stress, and research suggests that this stress may impact school, peer settings), that we found associations across contexts—
parents’ socialization behaviors (Graziano, McNamara, Geffken, that is, associations between maternal socialization processes oc-
& Reid, 2011). Recent findings emphasize the potential for chil- curring at home in the family context and children’s behavior
dren to elicit parental socialization responses by way of children’s occurring with peers and teachers in the school context—suggests
regulatory skills (Premo & Kiel, 2014). Future research should some robustness of the effects of maternal socialization. Finally,
explicitly test these transactions to precisely identify how parent- by including an ethnically and socioeconomically diverse sample,
ing beliefs and behaviors change in response to the socialization our results may be more generalizable across populations.
needs of the child, and how, in turn, these parental shifts could In sum, our findings suggest that children’s emotion regulation
benefit or exacerbate children’s regulatory development. and, especially, lability are collectively and uniquely influenced by
a variety of maternal socialization factors. As children’s emotion
regulation and lability are strongly linked to peer social compe-
Limitations and Strengths
tence and problem behaviors (e.g., Blair et al., 2014; Monopoli &
Assessing emotion beliefs with anything other than self-report Kingston, 2012), it is important for research to identify factors
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

would be difficult; however, variables such as socialization behav- within the family context that promote school-age children’s emo-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

iors and regulatory strategies could be assessed with alternative tion regulation and decrease lability. Our findings suggest that
methods. We note, however, that as children get older, observa- emotional lability may be more malleable to socialization practices
tional methods become more intrusive to children as well as than regulatory control, and future research should test this hy-
mothers, as both become aware of the observational process. pothesis, as it has implications for children’s flourishing at both
Furthermore, mothers’ regulation skills were assessed through home and school. Our study also suggests the possibility that
coded vignettes that were specific and reasonably complex, rather children who are struggling with their emotions may have parents
than global self-reports, and children’s emotion regulation and who are also struggling emotionally. Future interventions should
lability were assessed by teachers within the school context, thus thus target both parents and children to help them learn how to
contributing to an overall mixed-methods approach. Although this better navigate their emotional lives together.
provided a rich multirespondent (parent and teacher), cross-
contextual (home and school) assessment, it is important for future
research to replicate our models using additional perspectives, References
including children’s own self-reports and additional nonintrusive Alessandri, G., Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Eisenberg, N., Zuffianò, A., Mil-
observational measures. ioni, M., Vecchione, M., & Caprara, G. V. (2014). Prosociality during
Now that these findings show clear impact of parental social- the transition from late adolescence to young adulthood: The role of
ization upon children’s regulatory abilities, future work should effortful control and ego-resiliency. Personality and Social Psychology
examine the socialization of regulation and lability as directed Bulletin, 40, 1451–1465. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167214549321
toward specific emotions. Parents may have very different social- Baker, J. K., Fenning, R. M., & Crnic, K. A. (2011). Emotion socialization
ization strategies regarding sadness, anger, and fear, for example, by mothers and fathers: Coherence among behaviors and associations
with parent attitudes and children’s social competence. Social Develop-
and their strategies may have different implications for children’s
ment, 20, 412– 430. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00585.x
regulatory abilities when feeling these different emotions. It would Bariola, E., Hughes, E. K., & Gullone, E. (2012). Relationships between
also be useful to study children’s perceptions of their mothers’ parent and child emotion regulation strategy use: A brief report. Journal
socialization practices, because children’s interpretations of their of Child and Family Studies, 21, 443– 448. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
parents’ socialization behaviors may be as important as parents’ s10826-011-9497-5
assessments of their own behavior, particularly as children age. Barrett, L., Lane, R., Sechrest, L., & Schwartz, G. (2000). Sex differences
Perhaps most importantly, although teachers’ reports were col- in emotional awareness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
lected 3.5 months after mothers’ reports, and thus suggest some 1027–1035. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01461672002611001
linkages over time, we cannot determine causal associations be- Blair, B. L., Perry, N. B., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., &
tween maternal factors and children’s emotion regulation skill and Shanahan, L. (2014). The indirect effects of maternal emotion social-
ization on friendship quality in middle childhood. Developmental Psy-
lability. However, our study is an important first step in establish-
chology, 50, 566 –576. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033532
ing that such factors are in fact related at one point in time and Buckholdt, K. E., Parra, G. R., & Jobe-Shields, L. (2014). Intergenerational
serve as a foundation for future longitudinal work in this area. transmission of emotion dysregulation through parental invalidation of
Despite these limitations, our study also demonstrates unique emotions: Implications for adolescent internalizing and externalizing
strengths. First, the factors we tested accounted for nearly one behaviors. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 324 –332. http://dx
fourth of the variance in children’s emotion regulation and approx- .doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9768-4
imately one third of the variance in children’s emotional lability. Carthy, T., Horesh, N., Apter, A., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Patterns of
Collectively, these results support previous claims that multiple emotional reactivity and regulation in children with anxiety disorders.
parental factors contribute to children’s emotion regulation and Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 23–36.
emotional development (Eisenberg et al., 1998; Morris et al., http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10862-009-9167-8
Castro, V. L., Halberstadt, A. G., Lozada, F. T., & Craig, A. B. (2015).
2007). As noted before, our mixed-methods approach (self-report
Parents’ emotion-related beliefs, behaviors, and skills predict children’s
questionnaires, interviews using vignettes, and observer ratings) recognition of emotion. Infant and Child Development, 24, 1–22. http://
helps buffer our results from self-report biases. Although the dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.1868
inclusion of parent data would also have provided a rich assess- Cole, P. M. (2014). Moving ahead in the study of the development of
ment of children’s regulation and lability, given that parents emotion regulation. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
uniquely view their children in a multitude of contexts (e.g., home, 38, 203–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025414522170
290 ROGERS ET AL.

Dunsmore, J. C., Booker, J. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2013). Parental of School Psychology, 45, 3–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09
emotion coaching and child emotion regulation as protective factors for .002
children with oppositional defiant disorder. Social Development, 22, Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integra-
444 – 466. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00652.x tive review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299. http://dx.doi
Dunsmore, J. C., Her, P., Halberstadt, A. G., & Perez-Rivera, M. B. (2009). .org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Parents’ beliefs about emotions and children’s recognition of parents’ Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion
emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33, 121–140. http://dx.doi regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-
.org/10.1007/s10919-008-0066-6 being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348 –362.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental social- http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348
ization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. http://dx.doi.org/ Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual
10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 foundtations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.
Eisenberg, N., Edwards, A., Spinrad, T. L., Sallquist, J., Eggum, N. D., & 3–24). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Reiser, M. (2013). Are effortful and reactive control unique constructs in Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affective
young children? Developmental Psychology, 49, 2082–2094. http://dx social competence. Social Development, 10, 79 –119. http://dx.doi.org/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.doi.org/10.1037/a0031745 10.1111/1467-9507.00150
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1994). Mothers’ reactions to children’s Halberstadt, A. G., Dunsmore, J. C., Bryant, A., Parker, A. E., Beale, K. S.,
negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament. Merrill-Palmer & Thompson, J. A. (2013). Development and validation of the Parents’
Quarterly, 40, 138 –156. Beliefs About Children’s Emotions Questionnaire. Psychological As-
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to sessment, 25, 1195–1210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033695
children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence Halberstadt, A. G., Dunsmore, J. C., Parker, A. E., Beale, K. S., Thompson,
and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67, 2227–2247. http://dx J. A., & Bryant, A., Jr. (2008). Parents’ Beliefs about Children’s
.doi.org/10.2307/1131620 Emotions Questionnaire. Available from authors. Department of Psy-
Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Children’s emotion-related regula- chology, North Carolina State University.
tion. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior Halberstadt, A. G., Thompson, J. A., Parker, A. E., & Dunsmore, J. C.
(Vol. 30, pp. 189 –229). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. (2008). Parents’ emotion-related beliefs and behaviors in relation to
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Bernzweig, J. (1990). The Coping with children’s coping with the 11 September 2001 terrorist attacks. Infant
Children’s Negative Emotions Scale: Procedures and scoring. Available and Child Development, 17, 557–580. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.569
from authors. Department of Family Resources and Human Develop- Harold, G. T., Aitken, J. J., & Shelton, K. H. (2007). Inter-parental conflict
ment, Arizona State University. and children’s academic attainment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of
Fabes, R. A., Leonard, S. A., Kupanoff, K., & Martin, C. L. (2001). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1223–1232. http://dx.doi.org/10
Parental coping with children’s negative emotions: Relations with chil- .1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01793.x
dren’s emotional and social responding. Child Development, 72, 907– Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict
920. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00323 children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Developmental
Fabes, R. A., Poulin, R. E., Eisenberg, N., & Madden-Derdich, D. A. Psychology, 29, 940 –950. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.6
(2002). The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES): .940
Psychometric properties and relations with children’s emotional compe- Katz, L. F., Wilson, B., & Gottman, J. M. (1999). Meta-emotion philoso-
tence. Marriage & Family Review, 34, 285–310. http://dx.doi.org/10 phy and family adjustment: Making an emotional connection. In M. J.
.1300/J002v34n03_05 Cox & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.), Conflict and cohesion in families: Causes
Garner, P., & Hinton, T. S. (2010). Emotional display rules and emotion and consequences (pp. 131–165). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
self-regulation: Associations with bullying and victimization in Kim-Spoon, J., Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2013). A longitudinal
community-based after school programs. Journal of Community & Ap- study of emotion regulation, emotion lability-negativity, and internaliz-
plied Social Psychology, 20, 480 – 496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/casp ing symptomatology in maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Child
.1057 Development, 84, 512–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012
Garrett-Peters, P., Castro, V. L., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2015). Parents’ .01857.x
beliefs about children’s emotions, children’s emotion knowledge, and Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E., Walker, P. A., & Zeitlin,
classroom adjustment in middle childhood. Manuscript submitted for S. B. (1990). The Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale: A cognitive-
publication. developmental measure of emotion. Journal of Personality Assessment,
Ginott, H. G. (1956). Between parent and child. New York, NY: Avon 55, 124 –134. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5501&2_12
Books. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline
Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents’ ideas, actions, and feelings: Models and personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
methods from developmental and social psychology. Child Develop- Lozada, F. T., Halberstadt, A. G., Craig, A. B., Dunsmore, J. C., & Dennis,
ment, 59, 286 –320. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130312 P. A. (in press). Parents’ beliefs about children’s emotions and their
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion conversations with children. Journal of Child and Family Studies.
philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and Lunkenheimer, E. S., Shields, A. M., & Cortina, K. S. (2007). Parental
preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 243–291. http://dx emotion coaching and dismissing in family interaction. Social Develop-
.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.10.3.243 ment, 16, 232–248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00382.x
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How MacCormack, J. K., Halberstadt, A. G., & Sibley, P. (2012). Emotion
families communicate emotionally. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. knowledge and complexity: Mother emotion vignettes coding manual.
Graziano, P. A., McNamara, J. P., Geffken, G. R., & Reid, A. (2011). Unpublished materials available from the authors. Department of Psy-
Severity of children’s ADHD symptoms and parenting stress: A multiple chology, North Carolina State University.
mediation model of self-regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- Meyer, S., Raikes, H., Virmani, E. A., Waters, S., & Thompson, R. A.
ogy, 39, 1073–1083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9528-0 (2014). Parent emotion representations and the socialization of emotion
Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The regulation in the family. International Journal of Behavioral Develop-
role of emotion regulation in children’s early academic success. Journal ment, 38, 164 –173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025413519014
MATERNAL SOCIALIZATION AND CHILD REG/LABILITY 291

Monopoli, W., & Kingston, S. (2012). The relationships among language Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age
ability, emotion regulation and social competence in second-grade stu- children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort
dents. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36, 398 – 405. scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906 –916. http://dx.doi.org/10
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025412446394 .1037/0012-1649.33.6.906
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. Silk, J. S., Shaw, D. S., Skuban, E. M., Oland, A. A., & Kovacs, M. (2006).
(2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion Emotion regulation strategies in offspring of childhood-onset depressed
regulation. Social Development, 16, 361–388. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 69 –78. http://
j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01440.x
Nelson, J. A., Leerkes, E. M., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., & Marcovitch, Simons, J. S., Carey, K. B., & Wills, T. A. (2009). Alcohol abuse and
S. (2012). African American and European American mothers’ beliefs dependence symptoms: A multidimensional model of common and
about negative emotions and emotion socialization practices. Parenting, specific etiology. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 415– 427.
Science and Practice, 12, 22– 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15295192 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016003
.2012.638871 Sobanski, E., Banaschewski, T., Asherson, P., Buitelaar, J., Chen, W.,
Oliver, M. N. I., & Simons, J. S. (2004). The affective lability scales: Franke, B., . . . Faraone, S. V. (2010). Emotional lability in children and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Development of a short form measure. Personality and Individual Dif- adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Clin-
ical correlates and familial prevalence. Journal of Child Psychology and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ferences, 37, 1279 –1288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.12.013


Oshri, A., Sutton, T. E., Clay-Warner, J., & Miller, J. D. (2015). Child Psychiatry, 51, 915–923. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010
maltreatment types and risk behaviors: Associations with attachment .02217.x
style and emotion regulation dimensions. Personality and Individual Stelter, R. L., & Halberstadt, A. G. (2011). The interplay between parental
Differences, 73, 127–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.015 beliefs about children’s emotions and parental stress impacts children’s
Perez Rivera, M., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2011). Mothers’ acculturation and attachment security. Infant and Child Development, 20, 272–287. http://
beliefs about emotions, mother-child emotion discourse, and children’s dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.693
emotion understanding in Latino families. Early Education and Devel- Subic-Wrana, C., Beutel, M. E., Brähler, E., Stöbel-Richter, Y., Knebel,
opment, 22, 324 –354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409281003702000 A., Lane, R. D., & Wiltink, J. (2014). How is emotional awareness
Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Nelson, J. A., Leerkes, E. M., & Marcovitch, related to emotion regulation strategies and self-reported negative affect
S. (2012). Mothers’ responses to children’s negative emotions and child in the general population? PLoS ONE, 9, e91846. http://dx.doi.org/10
emotion regulation: The moderating role of vagal suppression. Devel- .1371/journal.pone.0091846
opmental Psychobiology, 54, 503–513. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev Supplee, L. H., Skuban, E. M., Shaw, D. S., & Prout, J. (2009). Emotion
.20608 regulation strategies and later externalizing behavior among European
Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality. In American and African American children. Development and Psychopa-
L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (pp. 227– thology, 21, 393– 415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000224
299). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of
Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical orga- ment, 59(2–3), 25–52.
nization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127–152. Trentacosta, C. J., & Izard, C. E. (2007). Kindergarten children’s emotion
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17405620344000022 competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade.
Premo, J. E., & Kiel, E. J. (2014). The effect of toddler emotion regulation Emotion, 7, 77– 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.77
on maternal emotion socialization: Moderation by toddler gender. Emo- Tull, M. T., Weiss, N. H., Adams, C. E., & Gratz, K. L. (2012). The
tion, 14, 782–793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036684 contribution of emotion regulation difficulties to risky sexual behavior
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M. D., & Zelazo, within a sample of patients in residential substance abuse treatment.
P. D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the Addictive Behaviors, 37, 1084 –1092. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh
transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, .2012.05.001
108, 621– 637. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.008 Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Haugen, R. G., Thompson,
Ramsden, S. R., & Hubbard, J. A. (2002). Family expressiveness and M. S., & Kupfer, A. (2013). Effortful control and impulsivity as con-
parental emotion coaching: Their role in children’s emotion regulation current and longitudinal predictors of academic achievement. The Jour-
and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 657– 667. nal of Early Adolescence, 33, 946 –972. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1020819915881 0272431613477239
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon, R. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., III, & White, T. L. (1987).
Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and
Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99 –166). Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53
New York, NY: Wiley. .1.5
Samuelson, K. W., Krueger, C. E., & Wilson, C. (2012). Relationships Wray-Lake, L., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (2010). Developmental
between maternal emotion regulation, parenting, and children’s execu- patterns in decision-making autonomy across middle childhood and
tive functioning in families exposed to intimate partner violence. Journal adolescence: European American parents’ perspectives. Child Develop-
of Interpersonal Violence, 27, 3532–3550. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ ment, 81, 636 – 651. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009
0886260512445385 .01420.x
Shaffer, A., Suveg, C., Thomassin, K., & Bradbury, L. L. (2012). Emotion
socialization in the context of family risks: Links to child emotion Received February 10, 2015
regulation. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21, 917–924. http://dx Revision received September 4, 2015
.doi.org/10.1007/s10826-011-9551-3 Accepted October 12, 2015 䡲

You might also like