You are on page 1of 19

Clinical Forum

Central Auditory Processing Disorder:


A Case Study
James Jerger*
Karen Johnson*
Susan Jerger*
Newton Coker*
Francis Pirozzolofi
Lincoln Gray$

Abstract
We carried out extensive audiologic, electrophysiologic, and neuropsychologic testing on
a young woman who complained that she had difficulty hearing in her educational environ-
ment . Conventional audiometric results, including pure-tone, speech, and immittance
audiometry, were all within normal limits . The subject performed normally on tests involv-
ing the processing of rapidly changing temporal information, interaural time and intensity
difference detection, and both absolute and relative sound localization . Early, middle, late
and task-related auditory evoked potentials were essentially normal, although some asym-
metry was observed in the middle latency (MLR) and late (LVR) responses . There was,
however, a consistent left-ear deficit on dichotic sentence identification, on threshold and
suprathreshold speech measures in the left sound field when various types of competition
were delivered in the right sound field, and on cued-target identification in the left sound
field in the presence of multitalker babble. Results suggest a central auditory processing
disorder characterized by an asymmetric problem in the processing of binaural, noncoherent
signals in auditory space. When auditory space was structured such that the target was
directed to the left ear, and the competition to the right ear, unwanted background was
less successfully suppressed than when the physical arrangement was reversed .

Key Words: Central auditory processing disorder, auditory-perceptual disorders, auditory-


diseases-central, auditory evoked potentials

he concept of central auditory process- the teacher in the presence of background noise,
ing disorder (CAPD) in children arose in executing a sequence of auditory instructions
T from observations by teachers and par- and commands, and in sustaining attention to
ents that some children appear to have difficulty auditory input (Cohen, 1980 ; Keith, 1981 ; Lasky,
in hearing even though their pure-tone audio- 1983 ; Willeford, 1985) . The common thread in
grams indicate normal peripheral sensitivity. this "symptom complex" is the consistent
Observers have described problems in hearing description of a child who appears to have a
"hearing loss," especially in the classroom situ-
ation, but whose pure-tone audiogram is invari-
*Department of Otolaryngology and Communicative ably within normal limits (Cohen, 1980 ; Gerber
Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, Texas and Mencher, 1980 ; Jerger et al, 1988; Musiek
TDepartment of Neurology, Baylor College of Medicine, and Guerkink, 1980 ; Rampp, 1979 ; Willeford,
Houston, Texas 1985).
$Department of Otolaryngology, University of Texas
Health Science Center, Houston, Texas The construct that such problems may be
Reprint requests : James Jerger, Baylor College of Medi- related to dysfunction in the central auditory
cine, 6501 Fannin, NA 200, Houston, TX 77030 system follows from the rich literature on the
CAPD: A Case Study/Jerger et al

behavioral manifestations of documented brain tral processing disorder. This young woman
lesions affecting the central auditory pathways complained of hearing and understanding
and centers (cf., Calearo and Antonelli, 1968 ; difficulties in her educational environment. She
Berlin et al, 1972 ; Liden and Korsan-Bengsten, felt that her problems were sufficient to consider
1973 ; Jerger, 1960 ; Jerger and Jerger, 1974, use of a hearing aid. Basic audiologic measures,
1975 ; Lynn et al, 1972 ; Speaks et al, 1975). including pure tone audiometry, acoustic immit-
Patients with demyelinating disease, intra-axial tance measures, and routine speech audiometry
tumor, or cerebrovascular insult typically show were all within normal limits. We noted on rou-
a similar symptom complex if the brain lesion tine examination, however, an abnormality in
affects the central auditory system . Peripheral dichotic listening; specifically, a left ear deficit
hearing sensitivity, as indicated by the pure-tone on the Dichotic Sentence Identification (DSI)
audiogram, is within normal limits, but the abil- test and set out to explore the nature of this ap-
ity to process suprathreshold auditory signals parent deficit in greater detail . We believe that
may be mildly to severely compromised, espe- our findings address many of the persistent is-
cially in the presence of competing background sues surrounding the controversial area of
sounds. This similarity in symptomatology in- CAPD.
vites speculation that at least some of the ap-
parent auditory problems observed in children
without known brain lesions may be manifesta- DESCRIPTION OF SUBJECT
tions of as-yet-undetected dysfunction of the
central auditory system (Barr, 1972; Gerber and History
Mencher, 1980 ; Jerger et al, 1988; Musiek et al,
1984). The subject was an 18-year-old woman who
Complicating the issue, however, is the fact complained that, for the past 2 years, she had
that the combination of apparent auditory experienced difficulty in hearing and under-
processing disorder and academic difficulty has standing her high-school teachers . She com-
also invited speculation, among educators, that plained that she frequently missed the teacher's
at least some of the problems subsumed under verbal assignments. The most difficulty was ex-
the rubric of "learning disorder" or "learning perienced when the classroom was noisy. She did
disability" may be due to such auditory deficits not complain of hearing loss in the sense of
(cf ., Beasley and Freeman, 1977 ; Katz and reduced sensitivity to sound. Rather, she
Illmer, 1972 ; Keith, 1981 ; Knox and Roeser, described her problem as a difficulty in under-
1980 ; Rampp, 1979 ; Sloan, 1980). Similarly, the standing the teacher when there was interfer-
frequently observed association between recep- ence from background noise. The subject also
tive and expressive language function has invit- reported difficulties in group social situations,
ed speculation that at least some of the while watching TV, and in using the telephone.
language disorders of children may be attribut- She had no problem with the telephone on the
ed to CAPD (cf., Lubert, 1981 ; Sloan, 1980 ; Tal- right ear but reported that the sound was "not
lal, 1980 ; Tallal et al, 1985a, b) . In the absence as good" when listening on the left ear. She felt
of substantial experimentally-controlled data that her problems were sufficient to consider use
such speculation has invited counter speculation of a hearing aid.
challenging the reality of the phenomenon of a On the Self Assessment of Communication
specifically-auditory processing disorder (Bloom (SAC) scale she scored 38 percent, a value as-
and Lahey, 1978 ; Lyon, 1977; Rees, 1973, 1981). sociated with mild auditory handicap (Schow
The association, for example, between attention- and Tannahill, 1977). She reported particular
al deficits and performance on auditory tests difficulty on question 3 ("Do you experience
(cf., Campbell and McNeil, 1985 ; Gascon et al, communication difficulties while listening to
1986) has suggested to some investigators that someone speak to a large group?") and question
the presumed auditory problem may, in fact, be 5 ("Do you experience communication difficul-
a manifestation of impaired attention (DeMar- ties when you are in an unfavorable listening en-
co et al, 1989 ; Robin et al, 1989). vironment?") . Nevertheless she had just
In the present paper we present a consider- graduated from high school without obvious
able body of data from a single subject with academic difficulty and was college-bound as a
what we believe to be an auditory-specific cen- music major.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

Physical Findings Right Ear


S:HH
Left Ear
Age: 18

Physical examination revealed an alert, well- [ Pure Tone Audiometry


developed, young adult. Both external auditory 250 1000 4000 dB 250 1000 4000
canals and tympanic membranes were normal. 0
ro
There was no evidence of pathology in the mid- 0 20
z
dle ear clefts . Extraocular muscle movements a 40
were normal . Pupils were equally round and m
v 60
reactive to light and accommodation. Nasal J 80
cavity, oral cavity, pharynx, and larynx were 100
normal . Neck findings were negative . Cranial
nerves III to XII were intact. Speech Audiometry

Routine laboratory studies included a nor-


mal complete blood count with normal sedimen-
tation rate. Serum T4 and T3 levels as well as N
T3 uptake level were normal. Urinalysis was
n
negative. Five-hour glucose tolerance test was
normal . A magnetic resonance image (MRI) of
the brain and posterior fossa was performed
with the adminstration of gadolinium . No ab-
normalities were observed in the brain or in the PB words
Unmasked
0
Masked
0
region of the VIII nerve complex. SSI (O MCR)
SSI (a10 MCR)
0
i

Figure 1 Pure-tone and speech audiometric findings


Basic Audiometric Evaluation in an 18-year-old woman who complained that she had
difficulty hearing her high-school teachers .
Figure 1 shows routine pure-tone and speech
audiometric data . With the exception of the
NEUROPSYCHOLOGIC EXAMINATION
threshold at 250 Hz in the right ear (25 dB HL),

T
all pure-tone thresholds were within normal he subject underwent a neuropsychologic
limits. The PTA (average of pure-tone threshold evaluation, including tests of cognitive, per-
hearing levels at 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz) was ceptual, perceptual-motor, simple and choice
12 dB HL for the right ear and 10 dB HL for reaction times, and other neurobehavioral abil-
the left ear. Results of basic speech audiometry ities. The evaluation, described in previous com-
were also within normal limits. The threshold munications (Jerger et al, 1989), was admin-
for spondee words was 20 dB HL in each ear, istered and interpreted by a neuropsychologist.
the PB score at 80 dB HL was 96 percent in Findings are summarized in Tables 1 and 2.
the left ear, 90 percent in the right ear, and the Table 1 shows scores on the various subtests
maximum of the performance versus intensity of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS),
(PI) function for SSI sentences at 0 dB MCR the Wechsler Memory Scale, the Boston Nam-
function was 90 percent in the right ear and 100 ing lbst, and the Spatial Orientation Memory
percent in the left ear. Immittance audiometry lbst. Table 2 lists simple auditory, simple visual,
showed a normal, type A, tympanogram in each and 4-choice visual reaction times.
ear, and acoustic reflex thresholds, both crossed The profile of neuropsychologic abilities in
and uncrossed, varied from 90-95 dB HL across this patient was remarkable for evidence of:
the frequency range from 500 to 4000 Hz .
The masking level difference (MLD) for a 1. supranormal global intelligence
500-Hz pure tone in the presence of broad-band 2. supranormal speed of mental processing
noise was 16 dB, well within the normal range. 3. supranormal visual-spatial organizational
On the routine DSI test, however, the subject abilities
scored 97 percent on the right ear but only 79 4. supranormal higher cognitive abilities, in-
percent on the left ear. This 18 percent interaural cluding vocabulary and verbal similarities
difference exceeds the normal boundary of 16 5 . discrepancy between global intelligence and
percent established by Fifer et al (1983) . fund of knowledge (information).
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

Table 1 Performance Scores of Experimental EXPERIMENTAL AUDIOLOGIC


Subject on Selected Cognitive Measures EVALUATION

W
Cognitive Measure Score
e administered a series of behavioral
WAIS-R : Verbal Tests' tests selected to evaluate monaural,
Information 8 binaural, and soundfield processing of auditory
Vocabulary 13
Arithmetic 8 signals. Monaural and binaural tasks were ad-
Comprehension 12 ministered via earphones, soundfield tasks via
Similarities 17 loudspeakers . In the following sections we
Digit span 11
present, for the sake of continuity, only enough
WAIS-R : Performance Tests' information on methodology to permit an under-
Picture completion 13 standing of the figures and tables . Expanded
Picture arrangement 12
Block design 13 descriptions of methodology are available to the
Object assembly 13 interested reader as appended notes.
Digit symbol 13

WAIS-R : Summary Monaural Tasks


Verbal IQ 105
Performance IQ 120
Full scale IQ 112 Monaural tasks could be classified into two
groups : (1) tasks involving the controlled study
Wechsler Memory Scalet of rapid temporal analysis, and (2) a task involv-
Passages 9.5
Visual reproduction 13 ing the discrimination of differences in duration .
Associative learning 21 Rapid temporal analysis was studied in two
ways ; (1) variation in the onset of two pure tones,
Boston Naming Testt 57
Spatial Orientation Memory$ 14 and (2) variation in the onset of voicing.
1b measure variation in pure-tone onset we
`age-corrected scores employed a technique modified from a paradigm
tscaled scores
$raw scores originally described by Hirsh (1959) and Hirsh
and Sherrick (1961),1 in which two tones, differ-
ing in frequency and by an onset time difference,
The subject reports "missing" auditory in- At, are presented simultaneously. In our modifi-
formation in school tasks, a condition that may cation the subject judged whether the two tones
account for the relative weakness in fund of in- were "same" or "different:" Figure 2 shows per-
formation in spite of high global intelligence . cent correct "different" judgments for each ear
The arithmetic subtest of the WAIS-R is not as a function of At . The false alarm rate, for tri-
only a test of mathematical abilities, but due to als with no onset difference (.At = 0), was low
its format (auditory presentations of mathemat- in both ears (3 of 225 trials in the left ear ; 0 of
ical problems to be manipulated without benefit
of pencil and paper) it should also be considered
a test of auditory manipulation of arithmetic in- 'A pair of tone bursts was presented to the subject
at a level of 70 dB SPL. The frequency of one tone burst
formation. Parallel tests of attention span and was 800 Hz, the other 1200 Hz. The 800 Hz burst always
memory for visual-spatial information and its had a duration of 500 msec. The onset of the 1200 Hz
manipulation (visual-motor tasks) did not reveal burst could be delayed, relative to the onset of the 800
Hz burst, by a variable amount . Both bursts, however,
similar deficits. In fact, the subject performed ended simultaneously. Thus the only clue to discrimina-
in the supranormal range on these tests . tion was the difference in onset times. Performance was
On the Hand Preference Questionnaire (An- measured over a single block of 300 successive pairs of
tone bursts . The interpair interval was 2 seconds. The
nett, 1970) the subject showed a profile associat- value of At was 25 msec in 25 pairs, 50 msec in 25 pairs,
ed with mixed handedness . She indicated 100 msec in 25 pairs, and 0 msec (no onset difference) in
preference for the right hand on eight items, the the remaining 225 pairs. Thus the a priori probability of
a difference was 0.33. We chose this value, rather than
left hand on two items, and either hand on two the more traditional 0.50, in order to force the subject
items . In view of this finding it is interesting to to adopt a relatively more stringent response criterion.
note, in 7hble 2, that simple auditory and simple For this task the subject judged whether the two mem-
bers of the pair were "same" or "different :' Presumably
visual reaction times were shorter with the left a judgment of "different" was made on the basis of the
hand than with the right hand. perception of differing onset times.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

Table 2 Norms and Performance Scores of iant) were presented monaurally. The intertrial
Experimental Subject on Three Measures of interval was 5 sec. The subject labeled each tar-
Reaction Time
get that she heard as "ba" or "pa." The examiner
Experimental Subject recorded the subject's verbal responses. Signals
Reaction Time Norm . Right Left Difference were presented, without background competi-
Measure 0/0 tion, at 60 dB SPL.
Simple auditory 221 220 .8 190 .1 -13 .9 Figure 3 plots percent correct "pa" judg-
Simple visual 253 223 .0 199 .2 - 10 .7 ments against voice-onset time in msec. Results
Four-choice visual 454 441 .9 459 .2 3 .9 are similar for the two ears and show the rela-
"Reaction time in milliseconds, based on young adults tively sharp boundaries characteristic of
with a mean age of 24 .5 years . Mahurin R, Pirozzolo J . categoric perception . The voice onset time cor-
(1986) . Chronometric analysis : clinical applications in aging responding to the 50 percent point of the iden-
and dementia . Dev Neuropsych 2 : 345-362 .
tification function is about 30 msec, a value in
close agreement with a range of 25 to 31 msec
reported by Jerger et al, (1987) using the same
100 taped tokens on three normal children in the 11
to 12 year age range.
Duration discrimination was measured
80
using a three-tone sequence paradigm suggest-
U
G1
ed by Musiek (1989) .2 Results showed no over-
60
all difficulty with duration discrimination.
0
U Scores were 97 percent correct on the right ear
40
0 and 90 percent correct on the left ear. We did
note, however, that performance was noticeably
20
poorer for one of the six sequences (SLS) than
for any of the other five. For the SLS sequence
0
25 50 performance was 80 percent for the right ear,
75 100
and only 50 percent for the left ear. Figure 4
Onset Difference in msec shows the percentage of errors as a function of
the sequence. Included also, in Figure 4, are nor-
Figure 2 Perception of temporal order . Two tone mative data (mean percent errors and their stan-
bursts, differing in pitch, start at different times but dard errors), obtained from 10 young adults . The
end simultaneously . Percent correct discrimination isolated abnormality for the SLS sequence in
as a function of difference in onset time.
comparison with results or the other five se-
quences, in our subject, is readily apparent . The
fact that our subject's errors are specific to a
225 trials in the right ear) . In general, perfor-
mance on this task was in good agreement with 2 The subject heard a sequence of three tone bursts.
the 20 msec value reported by Hirsh (1959) for The duration of each burst was either 250 msec (short
the 75 percent point of the psychometric func- or S) or 500 msec (long L) . There were 6 possible permu-
tion relating onset difference to judgment of tations of these two durations over a three-burst sequence
(i.e., SLS, SSL, SLL, LSS, LSL, and LLS) . Each permu-
temporal order. There was no evidence of a sig- tation was presented 10 times . The interval between suc-
nificant problem in the appreciation of small cessive tone bursts was 100 msec. The frequency of each
differences in onset times of tonal signals. tone burst was 1000 Hz . 7bnes were presented via ear-
phone at 70 dB SPL. A white noise at the same level (0
'Ib measure variation in voice-onset we em- dB signal-to-noise or S/N ratio) was mixed with the test
ployed a set of consonant-vowel (CV) stimuli signal . The subject was seated before a console contain-
that had been generated on a speech synthesiz- ing two response buttons. Above each button was a ver-
tical bar. One bar was twice the length of the other. The
er at the University of Oregon. The CVs were subject was instructed to listen to a three-burst sequence,
three formant patterns that differed in the then respond by pushing the buttons in the proper ord-
er. She was instructed that the button aligned beneath
acoustic parameter of voice onset time (VOT).
the shorter bar represented the "short" sound, and the
The VOT continuum ranged from 0 to 70 msec button aligned beneath the longer bar represented "long"
in 10 msec increments . Normal listeners perceive sound. She was to press the buttons in the same pattern
as the sequence of durations heard. The 60 sequences were
the series of stimuli in terms of two discrete
presented in a predetermined quasi-random order, with
categories (ba-pa). Tape recorded stimuli (ran- the constraint that each of the 6 permutations was
dom series of 30 examples of each VOT var- presented exactly 10 times .
CAPD: A Case Study/Jerger et al

aural temporal difference detection was studied


100
through two paradigms; (1) a binaural temporal
80 balance task (BTB) and (2) an adaptive inter-
aural temporal discrimination task (ITD).4
60 Table 3 summarizes results on the four
Q
interaural difference detection tasks, along with
0 40 the means and standard deviations for a refer-
20

3 The stimuli for both procedures were gated clicks


0
(100 Wsec on and 100 msec off), presented in triplets at
a rate of one triplet per second . In the ABLB task, click
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
triplets were alternated between the ears. The subject was
instructed to adjust the relative intensities of alternat-
Voice Onset Time in msec ing clicks until she perceived them as equally loud in both
Figure 3 Identification functions for stimuli vary- ears . In the SBLB task, the clicks were presented simul-
ing in voice-onset time . Percent "pa" judgments ver- taneously to both ears. The subject was instructed to ad-
just the relative intensities of clicks until she perceived
sus voice-onset time continuum. them as located in the middle of her head. In both tasks,
the subject controlled the intensity level of the clicks with
a joy stick . When the stick was moved to the right, click
intensity was simultaneously increased on the right and
80 decreased on the left. When the stick was moved to the
left, intensity changed in the reverse direction . Intensi-
Normal ty changes were made in 2-dB increments. Three trials,
60 each with a different starting point, were carried out for
0 Subj-RE
both the ABLB and SBLB tasks. The first trial was be-
O Subj-LE
® gun with equal intensities to both ears . In the second and
w` 40 third trials a 20 dB asymmetry was introduced between
00 the ears ; first in one direction, then in the opposite direc-
tion . The subject's interaural intensity difference, aver-
20 aged over three trials, was 1 .33 dB for each of the loudness
balance tasks.
0 qtr-rte . Win . c'\\rf%i . ,
SSL SLS SLL LLS LSL LSS

Sequence
'The BTB task was analogous to the procedure
described above for the SBLB. The subject was required
Figure 4 Duration discrimination . Stimulus is a to adjust the relative arrival times of simultaneous clicks
three tone-burst sequence . Duration of each tone burst until they were perceived at midline. The stimuli were
is either 250 msec ("short") or 500 msec ("long") . Per- click triplets presented at an overall intensity of 80 dB
cent errors for each possible sequence, compared with peak equivalent SPL. Adjustments in relative arrival time
norms obtained on 12 young adults . Note apparent- to the two ears were made in 20 psec increments via joy
ly selective deficit for short-long-short (SLS) sequence. stick. Again, three trials, with different starting points,
were carried out . The temporal difference in arrival times
between the two ears when perceived at midline, averaged
over three trials, was 33 .33 Wsec .
particular sequence effectively rules out an The interaural temporal discrimination task was car-
attention deficit as an explanation for the ried out using a four-interval, two-alternative, forced-
choice, adaptive-test paradigm. Each trial consisted of
finding.
clicks presented binaurally during each of four listening
intervals, which were labeled sequentially on a computer
terminal. In each trial, an interaural difference in arrival
Binaural Tasks time was introduced during either the second or the third
listening interval (0.50 a priori probability). The subject's
Binaural tasks could be classified into four task was to press a button during the interval in which
groups : (1) a series of tasks involving detection a difference was detected. Differences in arrival times were
initiated at 1000 ,usec, with the right ear leading. There-
of interaural time and intensity differences, (2) after, interaural differences were adjusted according to
a binaural integration task, (3) a serial recall a two-down, one-up rule (Levitt, 1971), wherein the inter-
task, and (4) a series of dichotic listening tasks. aural difference was decreased after two successive cor-
rect responses and increased after every incorrect
Interaural intensity difference detection response. Testing continued until fourteen response rever-
was measured using two loudness balance proce- sals had been obtained . Differences were changed by a
dures: (1) an alternating binaural loudness factor of 2 through the fourth reversal and by a factor
of the square root of 2 for the next 10 reversals . The mean
balance (ABLB) task and (2) a simultaneous
of the last 10 reversals, 58 .20 Wsec, was taken as the sub-
binaural loudness balance (SBLB) task .3 Inter- ject's interaural temporal difference limen .
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

Table 3 Performance Scores of Experimental Subject on Measures of Interaural Difference Detection

Measure Experimental Subject Reference Group

Score Mean (SD)


Alternate binaural loudness balance 1 .33 dB 3 .22 dB (1 .75)
Simultaneous binaural loudness balance 1 .33 dB 1 .66 dB (1 .89)

Binaural temporal balance 33 .33 Asec 45 .93 wsec (14 .02)


Interaural temporal discrimination 58 .20 gsec 56 .37 usec (25.77)
Also shown are means and standard deviations for a reference group of normal hearing subjects (N=12) .

ence group of 12 normal hearing subjects. On for the auditory presentation mode than for the
the ABLB, the subject's scores fell within 2 visual mode, a normal modality effect. The au-
standard deviations of the mean . On the remain- ditory advantage was due to an increase in per-
ing three tasks, the subject's performance was formance for the last one or two items of the
within 1 standard deviation of the mean . Thus, auditory list-again, a normal finding, the au-
there was no evidence of difficulty in detecting ditory recency effect . Figure 6 illustrates the
small time or intensity differences between the magnitude of the recency effect by detailing the
two ears . serial-position curves for the two presentation
The Speech with Alternating Masking In- modalities.
dex (SWAMI ; Jerger et al, 1960) was used to as- Results in the right hand panel of Figure 5
sess the subject's ability to integrate frag- show that recall was reduced, as expected, when
mentary speech information from the two an irrelevant speech suffix was appended to the
ears .' Our subject achieved a maximum of 88 auditory list . As with normal subjects, the
percent correct at both 80 and 100 dB SPL, fall- "suffix effect" did not occur with a noise suffix .
ing within the range of expected performance Thus the normal distinction between speech and
for normal hearing listeners (Jerger et al, 1960). nonspeech suffixes in memory was observed .
The materials and procedures used in the In short, serial recall for lists of digits re-
serial recall task have been detailed previously vealed what are widely regarded as cardinal
(Jerger and Watkins, 1988).6 Figure 5 compares characteristics of normal short-term memory ;
the average percent correct score, across all seri- namely the modality effect, the auditory recen-
al positions, for the visual versus auditory con- cy effect, and the speech suffix effect. As shown
ditions without a suffix, and for the three in Table 1, performance on the digit-span sub-
auditory conditions with and without suffixes. test of the WAIS was also within normal limits .
Comparison of the visual versus auditory con- Dichotic listening was explored using three
ditions in the left-hand panel shows better recall measures: (1) the Staggered Spondee Word
(SSW) test (Katz, 1962), (2) the Competing En-
'The test material consisted of a dual-channel tape-
recording to monosyllabic word lists (PAL-PBs) on which vironmental Sounds (CES) tests (Katz, 1985),
were superimposed 500 msec noise bursts . The noise
bursts, recorded at a level 20 dB more intense than the
words, alternated between the two ears at a rate of one 6 In brief, the task required the subject to reproduce
burst every 0 .5 sec . When this tape is played through a list of 7 digits presented in one of two modes; auditory
either earphone singly the words are virtually unintel- or visual. The lists were comprised of the digits 1 through
ligible; the periodic noise bursts effectively mask all or 6 presented via microcomputer at a rate of one item per
part of most of the words . When listening through both second For the auditory mode, tests items were presented
earphones, however, and provided that the two earphones binaurally at 75 dB SPL . For the visual mode, items were
are phase matched, the listener experiences an illusion flashed in numerical form on the computer screen. For
in which the bursts of noise are localized to the ears, but the auditory mode, the subject was tested under three
the words are localized to the center of the head . The word conditions: no suffix, speech suffix (the digit 9), and noise
fragments from each ear are "fused" into a single uni- suffix (burst of white noise) . For the visual mode, the sub-
tary image . As a result the words are easily understood ject was tested only under the no-suffix condition. Twenty
and the performance score is in the 90 to 100 percent practice items representing the conditions were ad-
range . For the present subject a performance-intensity ministered. Then 10 test trials were gathered in each con-
function was obtained by presenting 25 words at each dition. For all trials, a light on a response panel signaled
of three intensity levels : 60, 80, and 100 dB SPL . The the subject when to begin recall. The subject's task was
subject's task was to repeat each of the words . Perfor- to reproduce the list . The subject responded by pushing
mance was scored as percent correct at each of three in- buttons, labeled 1 through 6 on a response box, in the
tensity levels . correct order.
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

Modality Effect Suffix Effect

U
d
70 0 70
U
d
m
m
60 c 60
W
U
d
a
d
m
50 50
d
a

Visual Auditory None Noise Speech

Conditions Auditory Suffix Conditions

Figure 5 Summary of results of serial-recall paradigm. Average percent correct scores for modality effect (visual
versus auditory) and suffix effect (no suffix versus noise suffix versus speech suffix) .

condition was to ensure that the subject was


100 able to perform the task, in each ear separate-
ly, when there was no competing sentence in the
80
other ear. For the second monotic condition (i .e .,
d
U

60 "double-sequential" condition), pairs of sen-


0
U tences were recorded, one immediately following
m
Ol
40 the other, onto a single channel of cassette tape.
r
The subject's task in this condition was to
U
d 20
a report both sentences of the pair after the se-
0 cond sentence had been presented. This condi-
tion was incorporated to control for memory. If
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
the subject can correctly report both of the se-
Serial Position quential sentences presented monaurally, then
memory, per se, cannot be invoked as a basis for
Figure 6 Serial position curves for auditory and
visual modalities . poor dichotic performance.
The three dichotic conditions added to the
battery differed from the conventional DSI
and the Dichotic Speech Intelligibility (DSI) paradigm (i .e ., "free recall" condition) only in
test (Fifer et al, 1983).' terms of the instructions to the subject. In the
The SSW and CES were both administered
at 60 dB SPL. For the SSW test, only raw scores
were computed. The subject scored 100 percent ' The Dichotic Sentence Identification Test (DSI)
correct for both ears in all noncompeting con- consists of 90 pairs of sentences randomly selected from
the 10 seven-word, third-order sentence approximations
ditions. In the competing conditions, she scored comprising the Synthetic Sentence Identification (SSI)
92 .5 percent and 97 .5 percent for the right and 'Ibst (Jerger et al, 1968) . Each member of the sentence
left ears, respectively. On the CES test, the sub- pair has been temporally aligned in the onset and offset
to within 100 tsec of the other. In the conventional DSI,
ject scored 100 percent in both ears . sentences are presented at the rate of one pair every ten
We expanded the Dichotic Sentence Iden- seconds, via dual-channel cassette tape, simultaneously
tification (DSI) paradigm to include two to the two ears. The 10 sentence targets are listed on a
response panel situated in front of the subject. Beside
monotic control conditions and three addition- each of the sentences is a response button. The subject
al dichotic test conditions. In the first monotic is instructed to identify each member of the sentence pair
condition (i.e., "single" sentence condition), the by pushing the corresponding response buttons on the
panel. Performance is scored as percent correct identifi-
20 sentences were presented sequentially via a cation for each ear according to a free recall format, in
single channel. The subject's task was simply which subjects can report either ear first and need not
to report the sentence heard. The purpose of this designate the ear in which a specific target was heard.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

100

80

60
L
L
G

20

Single Double Free Focused Pre-cued Post-cued


Sequential Recall Recall

Monotic Dichotic
Conditions Conditions
Figure 7 Dichotic sentence identification (DSI) scores in two monotic and four dichotic conditions .

second dichotic condition the subject was in- the subject scored 100 percent correct identifi-
structed to identify the sentences heard in one cation in both ears . She also scored 100 percent
ear only (i .e ., "focused recall") . By comparing for the right ear in all four dichotic conditions .
performance in the "free-recall" versus "focused- However, performance was consistently de-
recall" conditions, the effect of divided versus pressed in the left ear across all dichotic condi-
focused attention could be evaluated. The right tions. Thus, while scoring 100 percent in both
ear was designated the ear-of-report for the first ears in each of the monotic conditions, the sub-
20 items; the left ear for the second 20 . In the ject showed a relatively consistent ear asym-
third and fourth dichotic conditions, the subject metry, of 30 to 40 percent, over all dichotic
was again instructed to report one ear only. In conditions, regardless of attention or memory
both of these conditions, however, the to-be- demands. If such cognitive factors were playing
reported ear was designated by one of two a significant role in the ear asymmetry, we
lights, labeled "right" and "left," located on top might expect, for example, performance on the
of the response panel in front of the subject. In left ear to improve, in the focused recall and
the "pre-cued" condition, the to-be-reported ear pre-cued conditions . These conditions are more
was signaled immediately prior to sentence similar to the monotic conditions where one
delivery . This condition allowed us to evaluate attends to, and reports, only one ear. Such per-
the subject's ability to shift attention rapidly formance shifts did not, however, occur. The
from one channel to the other. In the "post- relatively constant ear asymmetry, in spite of
cued" condition, the ear-to-be-reported was sig- variation in cognitive demand, suggests an
naled immediately following sentence delivery. auditory-specific basis for the performance
Thus, the subject was required to attend to, and deficit on the left ear.
remember, both sentences until the designated
ear-to-be-reported was cued . The ear-of-report
was randomized between the two ears in both Soundfield Tasks
conditions . In all conditions, sentences were
presented at 60 dB SPL. Soundfield studies could be classified into
Figure 7 summarizes results for all DSI test two groups: (1) localization tasks, and (2) tasks
conditions. In both of the monotic conditions, requiring the suppression of background noise.
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

Table 4 Performance Scores of Experimental Subject on Measures of Absolute Sound Localization

Measure Experimental Subject's Reference Group


Score Mean (SD)

Horizontal localization
Facing forward 1 .90° 0.80° (0 .74)
Left ear forward 11 .20° 10 .93° (4.32)
Right ear forward 8 .50° 7.69° (2 .96)

Vertical localization
Facing forward 4 .40° 5 .85° (2 .41)
Left ear forward 4 .80° 6 .22° (1 .61)
Right ear forward 4 .50° 6 .06° (1 .84)

Data are the mean absolute differences (degrees) between the selected and activated speakers. Also shown are mean
absolute differences (± 1 standard deviation) for reference group of twelve normal hearing subjects on the same tasks .

Localization tasks included both the abso- the loudspeakers. These results, along with the
lute localization of sound sources in space and means and standard deviations for a reference
the relative localization of a signal in one field groups of young adults, are listed in Table 4.
with a different signal in the opposite field. Both Results show that the subject performed nor-
studies were carried out in reverberant rooms . mally in the absolute localization of clicks in
For absolute localizations the subject was either the horizontal or vertical plane.
seated facing 12 loudspeakers mounted on the For relative localization9 the subject was
opposite wall at a distance of 2.7 m. The loud- seated facing a semicircular table containing
speakers were arranged in the form of a cross, two loudspeakers, one to the left of the subject's
with the center loudspeaker located directly in midline, the other to the right of midline. One
front of the subject. Three loudspeakers were lo- of the two loudspeakers was arbitrarily defined
cated at 9.8 degrees, 19.1 degrees, and 26 .5 as the "reference" or fixed loudspeaker, the other
degrees to the right, left, and above the center as the variable loudspeaker. Before each trial the
loudspeaker. Two loudspeakers were located at reference loudspeaker was set to an azimuth po-
9.8 degrees and 19 .1 degrees below center. The sition of 45 degrees. The variable loudspeaker
loudspeakers were labeled "1" to "7" from left was then positioned at one of nine azimuths in
to right, and "A" to "F" from top to bottom. the opposite field. These positions varied in five-
In the horizontal condition, the subject's degree steps from 25 degrees to 65 degrees. Our
mean absolute error was 1.9 degrees when fac- subject consistently localized correctly (three
ing the loudspeakers, 11 .2 degrees with the left
ear toward the loudspeakers, and 8.5 degrees
with the right ear toward the loudspeakers . In 9 The reference loudspeaker was positioned in the

the vertical condition, mean absolute errors were right field on 50 percent of trials, in the left field on the
remaining 50 percent of trials . The subject's head was
4.4 degrees when facing the loudspeakers, 4 .8 a constant 2.8 m from each loudspeaker position. The test
degrees with the left ear toward the loudspeak- stimuli were pairs of tone bursts presented alternately
ers, and 4 .5 degrees with the right ear toward to the two loudspeakers at 60 dB SPL. The frequency of
the tone burst from the reference loudspeaker was always
800 Hz, from the variable loudspeaker always 1200 Hz .
The interval between the two test tones was 500 msec .
e Absolute localization was tested separately in the The subject's task was to indicate, after the presentation
horizontal and vertical planes. Stimuli for both of the con- of each pair of tones, whether the angle of azimuth of the
ditions were clicks presented at 84 dB peak equivalent variable loudspeaker agreed with the angle of azimuth
SPL . For each trial, the subject's task was to identify of the reference loudspeaker (i .e ., was the tone from the
the origin of the clicks . She responded by calling out the variable loudspeaker as far to the right of midline as the
corresponding loudspeaker number, in the case of tone from the reference loudspeaker was to the left of mid-
horizontal localization, and loudspeaker letter, in the case line, or vice versa) . The subject responded "same" when
of vertical localization . For both conditions, the subject the variable tone was localized at the same azimuth, in
was oriented in each of three directions : facing loudspeak- its field, as the perceived azimuth of the reference tone
ers, left ear toward the loudspeakers, and right ear in the opposite field. The subject responded "right" when
toward the loudspeakers . Twenty-five trials were carried the variable tone was localized to the right of the target
out for each of the three directions . For each trial, the azimuth in the variable field, and "left" when the vari-
difference in degrees between the selected and the acti- able tone was localized to the left of the target azimuth.
vated loudspeaker was calculated . Accuracy of localiza- Each condition was tested three times. Conditions were
tion was quantified as the mean of the absolute sequentially randomized in such a way that the same con-
differences between selected and activated loudspeakers. dition never occurred twice in succession.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

out of three trials) when the difference between thresholds in the ipsilateral competition (IC) and
azimuths was 15 degrees. Nor were there sig- contralateral competition (CC) modes is rep-
nificant field asymmetries. A normal control, a resented by solid bars, for the right ear, and
23-year-old woman tested on the same appara- striped bars for the left ear. The height of each
tus, did not do as well, requiring a difference of bar represents the effect of moving the compe-
20 degrees to achieve the same level of perfor- tition from the same side to the opposite side
mance. Thus we observed no apparent deficit of the head, that is, the extent to which
in the relative localization of sounds . threshold is improved by having the target and
Background suppression was studied in a competition coming from opposite sides of the
sound-treated room . Figure 8 shows the ex- head rather than from the same side .
perimental arrangement . The subject was seat- Figure 9 shows that, when the target was
ed equidistant between, and 1 .6 meters from, presented in the right auditory field, this advan-
two loudspeakers . A third loudspeaker was tage was typically about 20 dB . When the tar-
mounted on a stand and positioned 0.5 meters get was presented in the left auditory field,
above, and slightly behind, the subject's head .
A pair of signal lights, labeled "right" and
"left", were mounted directly in front of the
subject at eye level. The subject's ability to sup-
press auditory background while attending to
auditory foreground was studied in three ways :
(1) threshold effects; (2) suprathreshold effects;
and (3) a cued target task .
Threshold effects were studied by present-
ing a target signal (auditory foreground) from
either the left or right loudspeakers, while simul-
taneously presenting a competing signal (audi-
tory background) from either the same or the
opposite loudspeaker.'° Figure 9 summarizes
the experimental results. The difference between

'° With the intensity of the competition at a fixed


level the intensity of the target was varied until threshold
for the target had been defined. We employed three differ-
ent targets; (1) 500 Hz tone bursts, (2) monosyllabic (PB)
words, and (3) synthetic sentences (SSI) . Each target was Figure 8 Arrangement of experimental apparatus for
paired with one of three different types of competition ; studies of background suppression.
(1) white noise, (2) multitalker babble, and (3) the continu-
ous discourse of a single talker.
The competing signal was always presented at an 50
SPL of 50 dB . When the target and the competition were
delivered by the same loudspeaker the condition is
referred to as ipsilateral competition (IC). When the com- 40
petition was presented from the loudspeaker in the op-
posite auditory field the condition is referred to as
30
contralateral competition (CC). For purposes of analysis,
data are displayed in the form of the difference between
thresholds in the IC and CC modes . The difference is a 20
measure of the extent to which the subject is able to sup-
press the competition when it comes from the auditory
field opposite the target, as compared with her ability 10
to suppress the competition when it comes from the same
side as the target. All thresholds were measured in 5-dB 0
steps according to a modified method of limits (Carhart
and Jerger, 1959) . lbne bursts had a 10 msec rise-decay
time and a total duration of 500 msec. Monosyllabic
words were taken from the PAL PB lists (Egan, 1948).
Synthetic sentences were taken from the SSI lists (Jerg- Figure 9 Combinations of three targets and three
er et al, 1968). The multitalker babble was taken from types of background competition. Comparison of
the SPIN test (Kalikow et al, 1977), the single-talker con- threshold differences between conditions of ipsilateral
tinuous discourse was taken from the SSI test. competition (IC) and contralateral (CC) for each ear.
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

50 I t the competition was ipsilateral versus the differ-


1
500 Hz Tone Monosyllabic Wo rds 1 t
I
Synthetic Sentences ence between the two sides when the competi-
40 I
t
t
tion was contralateral. When target and com-
e t IC L minus R
petition were presented from the same side (IC
t ® CC L minus R
5
.U 30 mode), threshold asymmetries were slight. Ear
d
I
t t
differences never exceeded 5 dB for any of the
iEi 20 various combinations of target and competition.
0 t t
Larger asymmetries appeared only when the

L
t
10
I
t
target and the competition were presented in op-
t I posite fields (CC mode).
I I
0 t t In summary, when threshold measures were
Noise Babble Disc Noise Babble Disc Noise Babble Disc
used to define ability to suppress auditory back-
Competition ground, our subject showed an ear asymmetry
of about 10 dB . Unwanted background was less
Figure 10 Same data as Figure 9. Combinations of
three targets and three types of background compe- successfully suppressed when the background
tition . Comparison of threshold differences between competition was presented in the right auditory
right and left ears for conditions of ipsilateral (IC) and field and the target was presented in the left
contralateral (CC) competition . auditory field .
1b measure suprathreshold background sup-
pression we presented SSI sentences at various
however, the advantage was consistently message-to-competition ratios (MCRs) . There
smaller, typically about 10 dB . The magnitude were three types of competition; white noise,
of this ear asymmetry did not appear to be relat- multitalker babble, and single-talker continuous
ed to either type of target or type of compe- discourse. When the target was presented via
tition. the right loudspeaker the competition was
It is noteworthy, moreover, that such asym- presented via the left loudspeaker and vice
metries were not observed for performance in versa. Figure 11 summarizes the results of these
any condition of ipsilateral competition. In measures . Percent correct sentence identifica-
Figure 10 the same data are replotted to com- tion is plotted against MCR for each of the three
pare the difference between the two sides when types of background competition. When the

100

e0
U
d 60
O
U
40
0

20
White Noise Babble Continucus
Discourse
0
II
-30 -20 -10 0 +10 -30 -20 -10 0 +10 -30 -20 -10 0 +10

Message-to-Competition Ration (MCR) in dB

Target Right
Target Left

Figure 11 Suprathreshold SSI scores as a function of MCR, in the presence of three types of contralateral
competition .
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

competition was white noise, there was no differ- ELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC STUDIES


ence between sides. But when the competition
was either multitalker babble or single-talker
continuous discourse, performance was better W e studied hearing function in a non-
behavioral context by means of three
different types of electrophysiologic response:
when the target was presented from the right
side than from the left side. In terms of MCR (1) acoustic reflexes, (2) auditory evoked poten-
the difference was about 10 dB . In other words, tials, and (3) otoacoustic emissions.
in order to achieve equivalent performance, the
MCR had to be made more favorable by about Acoustic Reflex
10 dB when the target was presented in the left
field. And, again, such asymmetry did not ap- The morphology of the acoustic reflex was
pear in any of the conditions of ipsilateral com- examined by means of special laboratory ap-
petition . The fact that white noise, as
competition, did not produce the performance
" The subject was seated in the sound-treated room
asymmetries that were observed for the equidistant between the right and left loudspeakers . The
threshold measures may be related to the fact third loudspeaker was mounted on a stand immediately
that the data of Figure 11 represent only the above, and slightly behind the subject's head . Directly
in front of the subject, at eye level, were mounted two
effects of competition on suprathreshold syn- lights, one labeled "left" and the other "right." A stereo
thetic sentence identification scores . Previous tape recording was played to both the right and left loud-
research (Carhart and Tillman, 1970) has speakers . One channel of the tape contained 20 min of
continuous discourse, a male talker with General Ameri-
demonstrated that, in this paradigm, noise is can dialect reading an adventure story written in the first
a less effective source of competition than is ac- person . The second channel contained the same record-
tual speech . ing but offset by about 2 min relative to the first chan-
nel. Thus the identical discourse was recorded on both
For the cued target task we employed the channels, but at any moment in time different parts of
instrumentation already illustrated in Figure 8. the story appeared on the 2 channels. When both loud-
This paradigm" represents, for the subject, a speakers were activated, therefore, the continuous dis-
course was "noncoherent" in the sense that different
relatively difficult case of foreground-back- speech messages were directed to each ear. A trial, or cued
ground differentiation . The subject must attend listen interval was defined by one of the two lights directly
to continuous discourse lateralized to one side in front of the subject. During the time that one or the
other light was illuminated, the subject must attend to
of the head while simultaneously suppressing the discourse from the side indicated by the signal light.
noncoherent continuous discourse lateralized to The subject's task, during this cued interval, was to press
the other side of the head . Then, when a signal a response button each time she heard the personal
pronoun "I" from the cued direction. During each cued
light cues the opposite side, the subject must interval, anywhere from 0 to 12 "I's" were presented from
redefine foreground as the discourse coming the cued side. An error was scored if the subject failed
from the new side, and redefine background as to respond when an "I" was presented, or if she respond-
ed when an "I" had not been presented. In order to
the signal previously defined as foreground . manipulate the difficulty of the task, continuous multi-
Throughout this alternate cueing, the subject talker babble was played from a second tape recorder
must also suppress the additional background through the loudspeaker mounted above the subject's
head . The presentation level of the continuous discourse
represented by the multitalker babble from
containing the "I" targets was 60 dB SPL. The intensi-
above. ty level of the multitalker babble was varied to produce
In spite of the complexity of this task our target-to-babble ratios of -10 dB, -15 dB and -20 dB . In
addition, there was a "no babble" condition.
subject completed it successfully. She com-
The subject's performance was based on responses
plained, however, that it was more difficult when during 48 cued intervals, 12 intervals at each of four com-
the left side was cued than when the right side peting babble conditions. At each condition six intervals
were cued to the loudspeaker on the right side, and six
was cued . She volunteered that, "When the bab-
were cued to the loudspeaker on the left side . The se-
ble comes on, the sound from the left loudspeak- quence in which sides were cued was quasi-random, with
er gets fainter." Figure 12 plots percent error the constraint that the right and left sides be cued an
scores from each side, both without babble and equal number of times. The total number of target presen-
tations for a given competing babble condition varied be-
at various target-to-babble ratios . Whenever the tween 78 and 81 per condition across the four conditions.
babble was present, the left side showed more The total number of targets presented over all conditions
was 319 of which 161 were presented to the right side,
errors than the right side, and the magnitude
and 158 to the left side. From these raw data percent error
of the difference increased as the target-to- scores were computed for right and left sides at each MCR
babble ratio became less favorable. condition .
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

80 ranges for absolute latencies, interpeak laten-


R Speaker cies, and amplitudes . It was the case, however,
that both the N,, - P,, amplitude of the MLR,
® L Speaker
60 and the N, - P2 amplitude of the LVR were
51 % smaller with left ear stimulation than with right
0 43%
L ear stimulation. Figure 14 shows representative
35% waveforms, and their replicates, for ABR, MLR,
and LVR auditory evoked potentials . Figure 15
shows LLTRR waveforms for both right ear and
20
left ear stimulation. Included, also, in Figure 15
12%12%
is an averaged electro-oculographic response
(EOG) from electrodes monitoring eye move-
-20 -15 -10 No ment. In the case of LLTRR the displayed wave-
Babble form is the difference between a baseline
Target/Babble Ratio condition, in which the frequent signal occurred
Figure 12 Cued target task . Percent errors versus 100 percent of the time, and the experimental
target/babble ratio for cueing of right and left loud- condition, in which the frequent signal occurred
speakers. 80 percent of the time and the rare signal oc-
curred 20 percent of the time. This algorithm ef-
fectively cancels out the LVR, which occurs in
paratus described elsewhere (Stach and Jerger, both conditions . Thus the difference waveform
1984). Using dual acoustic probes, both ears are reflects only the response to the rare event. The
measured simultaneously, yielding averaged LLTRR waveform shows the expected positive
reflex waveforms (n=8) for the right crossed and peak in the vicinity of 300 msec (sometimes
uncrossed, and the left crossed and uncrossed referred to as P3 or P3oo), indicating a normal
reflex waveforms. For the present study we task-related potential.
presented 500 msec reflex-eliciting signals at
levels ranging from 90 to 110 dB SPL. Test sig-
Otoacoustic Emissions
nals were either tone bursts at frequencies of
500, 1000, 2000, or 4000 Hz, or burst of white Is it possible that our subject's auditory
noise. problem stems from a subtle cochlear defect not
In general reflex morphology was normal revealed by the conventional pure-tone audio-
for all eliciting signals. All amplitude and laten- gram? Could the left ear deficit revealed by the
cy measures were within normal limits . We did various speech audiometric procedures derive
observe, however, that reflex amplitude growth
functions tended to be smaller for reflexes elicit-
ed by sound to the left ear than for sound to the 'z The active electrode at CZ was referred to the
ipsilateral earlobe, for ABR, MLR, and LVR, and referred
right ear. Figure 13 illustrates such amplitude to linked earlobes for the LLTRR. EEG preamplifiers
growth functions for the 2000-Hz eliciting sig- were Grass P 511, with filters set from 300 to 3000 for
nal, where the ear difference was most promi- ABR, 3 to 300 for MLR, and 1 to 100 for LVR and
LLTRR . Filter slope was 6 dB/octave. For ABR we aver-
nent . In both the crossed and uncrossed modes, aged 2560 signals, for MLR 1280 signals, for LVR 160
reflex amplitude was slightly greater when sig- signals, and for LLTRR 128 signals. For ABR the stimu-
nals were presented to the right ear than to the lus was a 100-microsecond, alternating-polarity click . For
left ear. Similar differences were observed for all MLR the stimulus was a 1000 Hz tone pip (2-1.2 config-
uration) with a total duration of 5 msec. For LVR the sig-
other eliciting signals. nal was a 1000 Hz tone burst with a 5 msec rise-decay
time and a total duration of 500 msec . For LLTRR we
used the "oddball" paradigm The frequent stimulus was
Auditory Evoked Potentials the 1000 Hz tone burst used for LVR; the rare stimulus
was a 500 Hz tone burst of equal duration. Rare stimuli
were randomly interspersed among frequent stimuli, ex-
We measured early (ABR), middle (MLR),
cept that two rare stimuli were not permitted to occur
late (LVR), and long-latency task-related successively. The a prior' probability of a rare stimulus
(LLTRR) auditory evoked potentials by means was 0.20 . Stimulus rate was 24/sec for ABR, 2 .1/sec for
of conventional averaging procedures." MLR, and 0 .5/sec for LVR and LLTRR . For ABR the
epoch was 15 msec, for MLR 100 msec for LVR 800 msec,
In general ABR, MLR, LVR, and LLTRR and for LLTRR 1200 msec following a 100 msec, pre-
waveforms were well-formed and within normal stimulus baseline.
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

Crossed Acoustic Reflex Uncrossed Acoustic Reflex


0.8
RE Q RE
0.6 ® LE UO/000 LE

0.4

0.2

F-1 I I I -T I
0.0

80 90 110 80 90 110

Intensity in dB SPL

Figure 13 Amplitude growth functions for both crossed and uncrossed suprathreshold acoustic reflex waveforms .

Right Ear Left Ear

ABR

MLR

LV R

Figure 14 Early, middle, and late averaged auditory evoked potentials.


CAPD: A Case Study/Jerger et al

Summary of Auditory Findings


LLTRR
Right Left

P3 Pure-tone audiogram 0 0
Monosyllabic word recognition (PB) 0 0
Synthetic Sentence Identification (SSI)
EEG 0 0
Masking level difference (MLD) 0
Tympanogram
LE 0 0

+ EOG
Acoustic reflex thresholds 0 0
Temporal order 0 0
Voice-onset time 0 0
4 ,V L_____~
200 msec
Duration discrimination 0
Figure 15 Long-latency, task-related averaged poten- Interaural intensity difference detection 0 0
tials generated by "oddball" paradigm . Interaural temporal difference detection 0 0
from left-sided hair cell damage which in some
Speech with Alternating Masking (SWAMI) 0
way alters normal speech perception without, at
the same time, impacting pure-tone sensitivity? Serial recall 0
In an attempt to answer this question, we Staggered Spondee Words (SSW)
0 0
studied transiently-evoked and distortion- Competing Environmental Sounds (CES) 0 0
product otoacoustic emissions. These measure- Dichotic Sentence Identification (DSI)
0
ments were carried out in the laboratory of Dr.
Brenda Lonsbury-Martin, Baylor College of
Medicine, to whom we are indebted for the oto-
Absolute azimuth localization 0 0
acoustic emission data and their interpretation .
Relative azimuth localization
0 0
The following summary is condensed from Dr .
Lonsbury-Martin's report : Soundfield thresholds in ipsilateral
vs contralateral competition 0

Soundfield speech with contralateral


ransiently evoked emissions were examined competition 0
Tin both ears. Each ear emitted ahealthy emis- Cued target task
sion, with the left ear exhibiting a slightly larger 0
response. . .A complete test of distortion product
emission growth functions was accomplished for Otoacoustic emissions 0 0
0
both ears . These emissions, in general, tended to
Acoustic reflex amplitudes
be slightly larger than normal, except for the right 0
ear at one test frequency . . . The patient's oto- Auditory brainstem response (ABR) 0 0
acoustic emissions, in general, indicate the Middle latency response (MLR)
0
0
presence of healthy outer hair cell function in both
Late vertex response (LVR)
ears.
Long latency task-related response (P-300) 0 0
Figure 16 summarizes all experimental
results, both behavioral and electrophysiologic, Key to symbols:

on the experimental subject. 0= normal ® = reduced compared to opposite ear

Figure 16 Summary of auditory findings . Compari-


son of interaural performance differences on conven-
DISCUSSION tional and experimental auditory tasks .

here is little reason to doubt that this sub- be explained as an example of a student who at-
T ject has an auditory problem. Her com- tempts to blame poor academic performance on
plaints of inability to hear her high-school a pseudo-hearing problem. She was succeeding
teachers were sufficient for her to seek profes- academically, had just graduated from high
sional help. She was even ready to consider school, and had been accepted at the college of
using a hearing aid. Nor could her complaints her choice. She was seeking help, not to justify
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

past failure, but to forestall any problems that other field, the left ear was consistently poorer
might jeopardize her future academic per- than the right ear. It is difficult to hypothesize
formance. an extra-auditory deficit capable of producing
Could her problems be explained by a cog- such an interaural asymmetry.
nitive deficit in attention, memory, or speed of Is it necessary to invoke a central mechan-
mental processing? Several lines of evidence ar- ism to explain the abnormality? Could the basis
gue against an attentional or memory deficit. for the left ear deficit be a subtle unilateral coch-
First, the relatively robust LLTRR potential in lear defect? Two lines of evidence argue against
the "oddball" paradigm (see Fig. 15) shows that this hypothesis. First there is no audiometric
the subject was able to perform satisfactorily evidence, in either the pure-tone audiograms or
in an auditory-monitoring task requiring sus- the conventional speech audiometric results, of
tained attention. Second, the neuropsychologic a left peripheral disorder. Indeed, pure-tone sen-
examination showed that, on visual-motor tasks sitivity and conventional speech audiometric
requiring attention and memory, our subject scores were, if anything, slightly poorer on the
performed in the supranormal range. Further right ear. Second, examination by otoacoustic
evidence arguing against a memory deficit is the emissions, an exceedingly sensitive measure of
fact that the subject scored normally on the inner ear integrity, failed to disclose asymmetry
Wechsler Memory Scale and on the Digit Span in hair cell function.
subtest on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence These various lines of evidence seem to con-
Scale. Serial recall for lists of digits (see Figs. verge on a central, specifically auditory, deficit
5 and 6) also showed normal modality, recency, involving sound input to the left ear. What can
and speech suffix effects, hallmarks of normal we say about the nature of this central audito-
short-term auditory memory. Finally, efforts to ry deficit? First, it does not appear to be a
manipulate DSI scores by varying cognitive problem in rapid temporal analysis. The subject
demands on memory and attention (see Fig. 7) performed normally on tests involving the tem-
were not successful . The fact that performance poral ordering of pure tones and the perception
was 100 percent on both ears in the double of voice onset time. In addition the subject per-
sequential condition shows that the ability to formed normally on a variety of measures re-
remember two sentences long enough to report quiring the processing of small interaural time
both sentences was not at issue. In addition, fur- and intensity differences. Furthermore, all neu-
ther argument against an attentional problem ropsychologic tests with a time component were
was the fact that the DSI asymmetry was the in the superior range. Second, it does not appear
same whether the subject was asked to report to be a problem in azimuth localization . The sub-
only what was heard from one pre-cued ear, to ject had no difficulty in the absolute localiza-
report what was heard in both ears, or to report tion of clicks or in the relative localization of
what was heard in an ear that was cued only pure tones from either auditory field.
after the paired sentences had been presented. The test procedures in which the subject did
The constancy of the performance deficit, in the have difficulty were; (1) DSI, (2) threshold and
face of variation in attentional and memory de- suprathreshold measures of speech understand-
mands, argues against a cognitive deficit in ing in the presence of various types of contra-
either attention, or memory as a viable expla- lateral competition, and (3) the cued target task
nation for the asymmetry in DSI performance. in the presence of speech competition. Each of
Finally, evidence against a deficit in speed these results seems to suggest a fundamental
of mental processing includes the observations weakness in the processing of targets delivered
that intelligence, visual-spatial organizational to the left ear when an interfering background
abilities, and simple and choice reaction times sound was directed to the right ear. Whenever
(see Table 2) were all either in the normal or the the experimental structure directed a speech tar-
supranormal range. get to one ear, and some form of competition
What, then, is the evidence for an auditory- either to the other ear or to both ears, our sub-
specific, as opposed to an extra-auditory deficit? ject did less well when the left ear was target-
Perhaps the most persuasive argument lies in ed. The deficit does not appear to be a problem
the asymmetry of auditory abnormalities. On in the suppression of background competition,
DSI, and on all soundfield tasks involving a tar- per se, since there was no asymmetry in perfor-
get in one field and speech competition in the mance when the competing message was deli-
CAPD : A Case Study/Jerger et al

vered ipsilaterally (see Fig. 10). Nor does the Gerber S, Mencher G. (1980) . Auditory dysfunction .
Houston : College-Hill Press.
deficit appear to be a processing disorder for all
Gascon G, Johnson R, Burd L . (1986) . Central auditory
inputs to the left ear, since there were many ex-
processing and attention deficit disorders . J Child Neu-
perimental conditions, involving relatively rol 11 :27-33 .
difficult listening tasks, in which the left ear per- Hirsh I . (1959) . Auditory perception of temporal order.
formed as well as the right ear (see Fig. 16). J Acoust Soc Am 31 :759-767.
In summary, the present data suggest that Hirsh 1, Sherrick C. (1961) . Perceived order in different
this subject has an asymmetric disorder in the sense modalities. J Exp Psycho[ 62 :423-432.
processing of binaural, noncoherent signals. Jerger J . (1960) . Audiological manifestations of lesions
When auditory space was structured such that in the auditory nervous system. Laryngoscope
70 :417-425 .
the target was directed to the left ear, and the
competition to the right ear, unwanted back- Jerger J, Jerger S. (1974). Auditory findings in brain stem
disorders . Arch Otolaryngol 99 :342-350 .
ground was less successfully suppressed than
Jerger J, Jerger S. (1975) . Clinical validity of central au-
when the physical arrangement was reserved .
ditory tests . Scand Audio[ 4 :147-163 .
Acknowledgments. We are grateful to Rose Chmiel, Gloria Jerger J, Jerger S, Oliver T, Pirozzolo F (1989). Speech
Delgado-Vilches, Craig Jordan, Henry Lew, Brenda understanding in the elderly. Ear Hear 10:79-89.
Lonsbury-Martin, Terrey Penn, Brad Stach, and Debra Jerger J, Mier M, Boshes B, Canter G . (1960) . Auditory
Wilmington for their assistance in the completion of this behavior in parkinsonism. Acta Otolaryngol 52:541-550 .
project.
Jerger J, Speaks C, Trammel[ J . (1968) . A new approach
to speech audiometry. J Speech Hear Disord 33:318-328.
REFERENCES
Jerger S, Johnson K, Loiselle L . (1988). Pediatric central
Annett M . (1970) . A classification of hand preference by auditory dysfunction: comparison of children with con-
association analysis. Br J Psychol 61 :303-321 . firmed lesions versus suspected processing disorders. Am
J Otol 9 (Suppl):63-71 .
Barr D. (1972) . Auditory Perceptual Disorders. Spring-
field, IL : Charles C Thomas . Jerger S, Martin R, Jerger J. (1987). Specific auditory
perceptual dysfunction in a learning disabled child . Ear
Beasley D, Freeman B. (1977) . Time-altered speech as a Hear 8:78-86.
measure of central auditory processing. In : Keith R, ed .
Central Auditory Dysfunction . New York: Grune & Strat- Jerger S, Watkins M . (1988). Evidence of echoic memory
ton, 129-176. with a multichannel cochlear prosthesis . Ear Hear
9 :231-236.
Berlin C, Lowe-Bell S, Janneta P, Kline D. (1972) . Cen-
tral auditory deficits after temporal lobectomy. Arch Kalikow D, Stevens K, Elliott L. (1977) . Development of
Otolaryngol 96 :4-10 . a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence
materials with controlled word predictability. J Acoust
Bloom L, Lahey M. (1978) . Language Development and Soc Am 61 :1337-1351 .
Language Disorders. New York : John Wiley & Sons .
Katz J . (1962) . The use of staggered spondaic words for
Calearo C Antonelli A. (1968) . Audiometric findings in assessing the integrity of the central auditory nervous
brain stem lesions. Acta Otolaryngol 66 :305-319 . system. J Audit Res 2 :327-337 .
Campbell T, McNeill M. (1985) . Effects of presentation Katz J. (1985) . Competing Environmental Sounds Test.
rate and divided attention on auditory comprehension in Vancouver, WA : Precision Acoustics.
children with acquired language disorder. J Speech Hear
Res 28 :513-520 . Katz J, Illmer R . (1972) . Auditory perception in children
with learning disabilities. In: Katz J, ed. Handbook of
Carhart R, Jerger J . (1959). Preferred method for clini- Clinical Audiology. Baltimore : Williams & Wilkins,
cal determination of pure-tone thresholds . J Speech Hear 540-563 .
Disord 24 :330-354.
Keith R. (1981). Tests of central auditory function. In:
Carhart R, Tillman T (1970) . Interaction of competing Roeser R, Downs M, eds. Auditory Disorders in School
speech signals with hearing losses . Arch Otolaryngol Children . New York: Thieme-Stratton, 159-173.
91 :273-279 .
Knox C Roeser R. (1980). Cerebral dominance in audito-
Cohen R. (1980) . Auditory skills and the communicative ry perceptual asymmetrics in normal and dyslexic chil-
process. Semin Speech Lang Hear 1:107-115 . dren. Semin Speech Lang Hear 1:181-194.
DeMarco S, Harbour A, Hume W, Givens G . (1989) . Per- Lasky E. (1983) . Parameters affecting auditory process-
ception of time-altered monosyllables in a specific group ing. In : Lasky E, Katz K, eds. Central Auditory Process-
of phonologically disordered children . Neuropsychologia ing Disorders: Problems of Speech, Language, and
27:753-757 . Learning . Baltimore: University Park Press, 11-29.
Egan J. (1948). Articulation testing methods. Laryngo- Levitt H. (1971) . Transformed up-down methods in psy-
scope 58 :955-991 . choacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 49 :467-477.
Fifer R, Jerger J, Berlin C, lbbey E, Campbell J . (1983) . Liden G, Korsan-Bengtsen M. (1973) . Audiometric
Development of a dichotic sentence identification test for manifestations of retrocochlear lesions. Scand Audiol
hearing impaired adults . Ear Hear 4 :300-305 . 2 :29-40 .
Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 2, Number 1, January 1991

Lubert N. (1981). Auditory perceptual impairments in Schow R, Tannahill J. (1977) . Hearing handicap scores
children with specific language disorders: a review of the and categories for subjects with normal and impaired
literature. J Speech Hear Disord 46 :3-9. hearing sensitivity. J Am Audiol Soc 3:134-139 .
Lynn G, Benitez J, Eisenbrey A, Gilroy J, Wilner H. Sloan C. (1980). Auditory processing disorders in children .
(1972) . Neuro-audiological correlates in cerebral In : Levinson P, Sloan C, eds. Auditory Processing and
hemisphere lesions. Audiology 11 :115-134 . Language: Clinical and Research Perspectives. New York :
Grune & Stratton, 101-115.
Lyon R . (1977). Auditory perceptual training: the state
of the art. J Learn Disabil 10 :564-572 . Speaks C, Gray G, Miller J, Rubens A. (1975) . Central
auditory deficits and temporal lobe lesions. J Speech Hear
Musiek F (1989) . Personal communication .
Disord 40:192-205 .
Musiek F, Gollegy K, Baran J . (1984). Myelination of the
Stach B, Jerger J. (1984). Acoustic reflex averaging. Ear
corpus callosum and auditory processing problems in chil-
Hear 5:289-296 .
dren: theoretical and clinical correlates. Semin Hear
5 :231-241 . Tallal P (1980). Auditory processing disorders in children .
In : Levinson P, Sloan C, eds. Auditory Processing and
Musiek F, Guerkink N. (1980) . Auditory perceptual
Language: Clinical and Research Perspectives. New York :
problems in children : considerations for the otolaryngol-
Grune & Stratton, 81-100 .
ogist and audiologist. Laryngoscope 90 :962-971 .
Tallal P, Stark R, Mellitus D. (1985a). Identification of
Rampp D. (1979) . Hearing and learning disabilities . In :
Bradford L, Hardy W, eds. Hearing and Hearing Impair- language-impaired children on the basis of rapid percep-
tion and production skills . Brain Lang 25 :314-322 .
ment. New York: Grune & Stratton, 381-389.
Tallal P, Stark R, Mellitus D. (1985b) . The relationship
Rees N. (1973). Auditory processing factors in language
between auditory temporal analysis and receptive lan-
disorders: the view from Procrutes' bed. J Speech Hear
guage development : evidence from studies of developmen-
Disord 38:304-315 .
tal language disorder. Neuropsychologia 23 :314-322 .
Rees N. (1981) . Saying more than we know: is auditory
Willeford J. (1985) . Assessment of central auditory dis-
processing disorder a meaningful concept? In: Keith R,
orders in children . In: Pinheiro M, Musiek F, eds. Assess-
ed . Central Auditory and Language Disorders in Children .
ment of Central Auditory Dysfunction: Foundation and
Houston: College Hill Press, 94-120.
Clinical Correlates. Baltimore : Williams & Wilkins,
Robin D, lbmblin J, Kearney A, Hug L. (1989) . Audito- 239-255.
ry temporal pattern learning in children with speech and
language impairments . Brain Lang 36 :604-613 .

You might also like