You are on page 1of 5

Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, Vol. 41, No.

1, 2011

Characteristics of Cognitive Functions in Patients


with Chronic Spinal Pain

K. A. Melkumova, E. V. Podchufarova,
and N. N. Yakhno

Translated from Zhurnal Nevrologii i Psikhiatrii imeni S. S. Korsakova, Vol. 109, No. 11, pp. 20–24,
November, 2009.

A total of 64 patients with musculoskeletal pain syndromes in the lumbosacral area for more than three
months were studied. Patients were divided into age groups: 30–50 years (41 patients) and 51–60 years
(23 patients). The reference group consisted of 20 healthy volunteers comparable in terms of gender, age,
and level of education. Patients underwent neurological, neuro-orthopedic, clinical-pathopsychological,
and neuropsychological investigations. Complaints of difficulty with mental concentration were present in
17.3% of patients and problems with remembering information in 20.2%. Both groups of patients with
chronic pain showed mild neurodynamic impairments. As compared with healthy subjects, they had sig-
nificantly worse performance in tests assessing memory (delayed reproduction in the 12-word test), atten-
tion, mental flexibility, and visuomotor coordination (the sequential number-letter combination test, digit
symbol substitution test, and, in younger patients, the forward and backward number series repetition test).
Cognitive functions in younger patients were affected by the sensory-discriminant (intensity) and affec-
tive-motivational (negative emotions, particularly anxiety) characteristics of pain. Cognitive functions in
older patients were affected by the affective-motivational (anxiety, level of psychoemotional distress) and
cognitive (level of catastrophization) components of pain. Treatment directed at correcting the peripheral
sources of pain and emotional disorders (rational psychotherapy, tranquilizers, antidepressants) could
potentially have positive effects on cognitive functions in patients with chronic pain.

KEY WORDS: chronic spinal pain, cognitive functions.

WHO data indicate that pain syndromes represent one pain is the most common cause of temporary disability and
of the leading causes of medical consultations in the prima- restriction of activity among people of working age [26].
ry healthcare system (from 11.3% to 40% of cases) [17]. In Russia, lower spinal pain accounts for from 20% to 80%
Among neurological conditions, patients with chronic pain of cases of temporary disability in the active social group
(CP) account for up to 52.5% of cases [4]. Epidemiological and 20.4% of all disability due to degenerative diseases of
studies of chronic pain syndromes in the adult population of the musculoskeletal system [10].
Russia show that their incidence ranges from 13.8% to Around 80% of people experience lumbosacral pain at
56.7%, averaging 34.3 per 100 questioned [9]. some time in their lives; in 80–90% of patients, the pain
CP is more common among women and people with regresses over six weeks, though 10–20% of patients develop
low income, and its incidence increases with age. A rela- CP and disability. This group of patients has an unfavorable
tionship between CP and depression and emotionally stress- prognosis for recovery and represent a major medical-social
ful factors has been demonstrated. CP is dominated by pain problem, as they account for up to 80% of healthcare costs
of musculoskeletal origin and headache [1, 2, 8]. Spinal [11, 15, 34]. The incidence of disability and spinal pain does
not increase with age and does not correlate with age-relat-
Department of Nervous Diseases, Therapeutic Faculty, ed degenerative change in the spine. The peak incidence of
I. M. Sechenov Moscow Medical Academy; CP occurs at working age (up to 40 years) [25]. The severi-
e-mail: kar-mel@mail.ru. ty of pain sensations in the spine, disability, and pain-related
42
0097-0549/11/4101-0042 ©2011 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
Characteristics of Cognitive Functions in Patients with Chronic Spinal Pain 43

behavior in CP do not correspond to objective lesions, which which identifies the extent to which spinal pain affects the
can be minimal. CP and disability are more associated with patient’s daily activity [7]. The University of Alabama Pain
the presence of emotional distress, depression, failed treat- Behavior Scale was used to assess the degree of pain-related
ment, and adoption of the “patient role” [34]. behavior [24]. Catastrophization (the patient having extreme-
Results obtained from numerous studies have demon- ly negative views of his/her status) was assessed using the
strated that CP (regardless of the presence of traumatic brain Chronic Pain Catastrophizing Scale [31]. Anxiety levels were
damage or other neurological diseases in the history) has assessed using the Spielberger–Hanin self-assessment anxi-
adverse influences on cognitive functions. In addition, it is ety scale [5]. Depression was detected using the Hamilton
often accompanied by anxiety, depression, and restricted scale [1]: results from 0 to 6 were evaluated as showing the
daily activities, resulting in significant decreases in patients’ absence of depression, while patients with scores of 7 and
quality of life [2, 13, 18, 23, 29]. Cognitive disorders in above were excluded from the study. Psychological changes
patients with CP consist mainly of impairments to memory, and levels of psychological distress were assessed using the
attention, information processing speed, and the flexibility Symptom Check List 90 (SCL-90) [5].
of thought. Cognitive impairments in CP and their underly- Neuropsychological investigations included analysis of
ing mechanisms are currently being actively studied [6]. complaints of impairments to memory and attention, as well
Normal aging involves moderate reductions in informa- as evaluation of cognitive functions using neuropsychologi-
tion processing speed, memory, and regulatory functions [12, cal methods. Visuomotor coordination and mental flexibility
27, 28]. It has been suggested that with age, patients with CP (the ability to adapt and develop individual intellectual
can show increasingly severe cognitive disorders due to the schemes when working with new information) were
negative effects of pain on attention and memory [21]. assessed using the sequential number-letter combination test
The aims of the present work were to obtain more data [30]. Information processing speed, visuomotor coordina-
on the nature of impairments to cognitive functions in CP tion, and selective attention were assessed using the digit
and to identify the factors influencing them, with the aim of symbol substitution test [22]. Attention-switching ability
optimizing the treatment of this group of patients. and left frontal lobe function were tested using the Stroop
test (University of Victoria version) [30]. The ability to solve
tasks in response to changes in stimulus and the ability to
MATERIALS AND METHODS switch and maintain a defined program using feedback were
assessed using the Wisconsin card sorting test, which allows
A total of 64 patients (22 men and 42 women) with impairments to the dorsolateral areas of the prefrontal cortex
chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes in the lumbosacral to be evaluated [30]. Verbal fluency and semantic memory
area for more than three months were studied. were assessed using the verbal associations test (literal and
Patients were divided into two age groups with no sig- categorial) [30]. The 12-word memorization and reproduc-
nificant differences in gender composition or levels of educa- tion test with the modification of Grober and Buschke (the
tion. Group 1 consisted of 41 patients (29 women, 12 men) 12-word test) was used (immediate and delayed reproduc-
aged from 30 to 50 years (mean age 44.4 ± 5.7 years). tion) to assess memory [30]. Short-term memory and atten-
Group 2 included 23 patients (13 women, 10 men) aged from tion were evaluated using the number sequence repetition
51 to 60 years (mean age 53.8 ± 2.5 years). The reference test in the forward and reverse directions by the Wechsler
group consisted of 20 healthy volunteers and was compara- method [22]. Exaggeration and simulation of reduced mem-
ble in terms of gender, age, and level of education with the ory were monitored in all patients and the reference group
study groups. using the Rey test for memory assessment [30].
Patients with neurological and somatic diseases Data were analyzed statistically using EpiInfo 5.0,
(hypothyroidism, marked impairments of liver and kidney which is freely distributed and recommended by the WHO.
function, severe systemic and metabolic abnormalities, etc.),
which can be accompanied by cognitive function impair-
ments were excluded from the study, as were physician- RESULTS
dependent patients, and those with exacerbation of pain
syndrome and cognitive impairments, and patients taking Comparative analysis of the various characteristics of
agents with the potential to influence cognitive functions. pain in the two study groups showed that the mean dura-
Clinical investigations included analysis of the devel- tions of lumbar pain were 10.83 ± 6.92 years in group 1 and
opment of the condition, neurological and neuro-orthope- 10.09 ± 7.57 years in group 2; the mean durations of current
dic, clinical-psychopathological, and neuropsychological exacerbations were 4.46 ± 3.54 and 5.78 ± 4.45 months; pain
investigations. intensities on the VAS were 5.07 ± 1.71 and 5.17 ± 1.59
Pain syndrome was studied using visual analog scales points, respectively. Assessment of pain using the McGill
(VAS) and the McGill pain questionnaire [3]. The level of pain questionnaire showed that in patients of group 1, the
disability was evaluated using the Oswestry Disability Scale, index of the number of descriptors identified (INDI), a mea-
44 Melkumova, Podchufarova, and Yakhno

TABLE 1. Results Obtained from Neuropsychological Tests Showing Significant Differences between Groups of Patients with CP and the
Reference Groups

Note. *Significant differences compared with reference group (p < 0.05).

sure of the number of pain characteristic descriptors identi- to express emotional problems through physical symptoms)
fied on the sensory and affective scales, and the rank pain and psychological distress scales. The value on the anxiety
index (RPI), reflecting the severity of the pain characteris- and depression scale for women of group 1 was greater than
tic descriptors on the sensory, affective, and evaluative the population mean. In group 2, there were also increases
scales, were somewhat higher than those in patients of in values on the somatization, psychological distress, and
group 2 (sensory INDI: 5.46 ± 3.67 and 3.83 ± 2.15 points, anxiety scales as compared with population means, while in
affective INDI: 3.22 ± 1.62 and 2.74 ± 1.21 points, sensory men, there was also an increase on the depression scale.
RPI: 12.41 ± 10.65 and 7.17 ± 5.06 points, affective RPI: Analysis of complaints of cognitive impairments
5.24 ± 3.24 and 5.0 ± 3.06 points, evaluative RPI: 3.1 ± 1.46 showed that complaints of difficulty with mental concentra-
and 2.43 ± 0.51 points, respectively). Invalidity levels on tion were made by 17.3% of patients. These were present
the Oswestry scale showed no difference, with values of significantly more often (31.7%) among patients of group 1
35.17 ± 15.67% in group 1 and 35.22 ± 13.41% in group 2, than among patients of group 2 (21.7%) (p = 0.03). Com-
which corresponded to moderate decreases in functionality. plaints of difficulties with remembering information were
The level of catastrophization was somewhat higher in made by 20.2% of patients. Complaints of memory impair-
group 1, at 18.8 ± 10.94 points, compared with 15.83 ± 6.15 ments were more common in patients of group 1 (39%) than
points in group 2. Significant differences were seen on eval- in patients of group 2 (21.7%), though this difference was
uation of pain behavior: 4.38 ± 2.01 points in group 1 and not statistically significant (p = 0.15).
2.78 ± 1.62 points in group 2 (p = 0.023). Patients complaining of memory impairments per-
Analysis of neuro-orthopedic impairments showed that formed significantly less well on the 12-word test with
dysfunction of the sacroiliac joint was present in 90.6% of delayed reproduction than patients without these com-
cases (90.2% in group 1 and 91.3% in group 2), facet joint plaints, scoring 11.0 ± 1.3 and 11.68 ± 0.69 words, respec-
syndrome in 78.1% (73.2% and 87% in groups 1 and 2, tively (p = 0.003). Patients complaining of impaired atten-
respectively), myofascial pain syndrome in 23.4% (24.4% tion performed the digit symbol substitution test
and 21.7%, respectively), and myotonic syndrome in 76.6% significantly worse than patients lacking these complaints,
(75.6% and 78.3%, respectively). There were no statistical- with scores of 43.27 ± 5.73 and 51.04 ± 10.53 symbols,
ly significant differences in the frequencies of neuro-ortho- respectively (p = 0.004).
pedic derangements between the two groups. Comparison of the results obtained from neuropsycho-
Assessment of reactive and endogenous anxiety using logical tests of the two study groups revealed significant
the Spielberger–Hanin scale revealed moderate reactive differences (p < 0.05) only on the Stroop test (assessment of
anxiety in both groups: 45.54 ± 10.45 points in group 1 and the switching of attention). Patients of group 2 took more
41.87 ± 6.17 points in group 2. In patients of group 1, the time to perform the test with the “words” (18.17 ± 3.58 sec)
level of endogenous anxiety was higher (47.27 ± 11.64 and “colors” (31.09 ± 6.32 sec) cards than patients of group
points) than that in group 2 (42.4 ± 7.37 points), though the 1 (16.13 ± 3.08 and 26.03 ± 6.12 sec, respectively). In terms
differences were not significant. of the remaining neuropsychological methods, there were
Comparative evaluation of mean values on the SCL-90 no significant differences between the two study groups.
scale (the somatization, endogenous anxiety, depression, Spearman correlation analysis revealed a statistically
and anxiety scales, and measures reflecting the overall significant interaction (p < 0.001) between the results
index of anxiety, which is a measure of psychological dis- obtained on the Stroop test (“words” and “colors” cards)
tress) showed that there were no significant differences and patients’ age – increases in age were associated with
between patients of groups 1 and 2. Both men and women increases in the time taken to perform the test.
of the two groups showed differences from population Patients of both groups differed from healthy volun-
mean values on the somatization (the ability of the patient teers in performance on memory assessment (delayed
Characteristics of Cognitive Functions in Patients with Chronic Spinal Pain 45

reproduction in the 12-word test) and attention and regula- Cognitive functions in patients of group 1 were affect-
tory functions tests (the sequential number-letter combina- ed by the sensory-discriminative (intensity) and affective-
tion test, the digit symbol substitution test). Patients of motivational (negative emotions, particularly anxiety) char-
group 1 also showed significant differences from healthy acteristics of pain.
subjects in terms of the digit repetition test in the forward In patients of group 2, cognitive functions were affect-
and reverse directions. ed by the affective-motivational (anxiety, level of psycho-
On performance of the 12-word test, patients of both logical distress) and cognitive (level of catastrophization)
age groups named significantly fewer words on delayed characteristics. These data are consistent with results
reproduction as compared with healthy subjects (p = 0.0027 obtained in other studies.
and p = 0.0035, respectively). The sequential digit-letter Published data [20] indicate that elderly patients with
combination test were performed significantly worse by CP in the spine showed an interaction between the severity
patients of group 1 (p = 0.0004) and group 2 (p = 0.0011); of pain syndrome and impairments to regulatory functions,
patients of both groups required more time than healthy particularly mental flexibility. Pain intensity and the severi-
subjects to perform this test, pointing to impairments to ty of anxiety (but not depression) had negative influences
attention functions, mental flexibility, and visuomotor coor- on memory and information processing speed [16]. A num-
dination in patients of both age groups. Decreases in infor- ber of studies [32] have demonstrated that negative attitudes
mation processing speed and impairments to attention and to pain, pain behavior, and the severity of emotional distress
visuomotor coordination were identified by results obtained on cognitive functions (impairments to attention) have
on the digit symbol substitution test. On performance of this influences. Patients with high levels of emotional distress
test, CP patients of both groups substituted fewer symbols showed impairments to attention and psychomotor process-
in the 90-sec test period than healthy subjects (p = 0.0015). ing speed (in the Stroop test), memory and regulatory func-
In the digit repetition test in the forward and reverse direc- tions as compared with the results obtained from these tests
tions (patients were told to repeat series of digits after the by patients with low levels of emotional distress [19].
investigator first in the forward direction then in the reverse Eccleston et al. [14] demonstrated that psychological dis-
direction), the total points score of patients of group 1 was tress and increased concentration on bodily sensations
significantly lower than that in the control group (p = 0.0091) (somatization) affected the severity of cognitive impair-
(see Table 1). ments in patients with CP. Thus, cognitive functions in
Spearman correlation analysis revealed a significant patients with CP affected the sensory-discriminative, emo-
(p < 0.05) negative relationship between the results obtained tional-motivational, and cognitive components of CP.
from the 12-word test (delayed reproduction) and pain The present study did not identify any effects of the level
intensity in the VAS, the results on the evaluative scale of of somatization or the severity of pain behavior on cognitive
the McGill pain questionnaire, and the level of invalidity on functions: these were was greater than the population mean
the Oswestry scale. There was a positive correlational rela- but within the ranges typical of patients with chronic diseases,
tionship between pain INDI on the sensory scale of the especially in-patients. The present study did not include any
McGill pain questionnaire and results obtained from patients with marked depressive disorders which could influ-
the sequential digit-letter combination test assessing mental ence measures of cognitive functions. In the absence of
flexibility and visuomotor coordination. There was a nega- depression, cognitive functions were more influenced by the
tive correlational relationship between the level of endoge- sensory-discriminative characteristics of pain (pain intensity
nous anxiety on the SCL-90 scale and results obtained from on the VAS, descriptive characteristics on the McGill pain
the digit symbol substitution test addressing information questionnaire). Increased anxiety, psychological distress, and
processing speed, visuomotor coordination, and selective catastrophization could also be seen to have a role in the
attention. appearance of mild neurodynamic cognitive impairments.
Patients of group 2 showed a negative correlational Data obtained by various authors [33] indicate that the pres-
relationship between the results obtained from the 12-word ence of anxiety disorders is typical of patients with chronic
test (delayed reproduction) and the level of psychological pain syndromes. Among patients with CP in the spine, more
distress on the SCL-90 scale and a positive relationship than 22% had marked anxiety disorders and more than 23%
between the level of catastrophization and results obtained had depression [13]. Anxiety as an indicator of psychological
from the sequential digit-letter combination test. stress is known to influence cognitive functions to a signifi-
cant extent [12]. Investigation of the influence of anxiety on
cognitive functions performed on healthy volunteers have
DISCUSSION shown that it has negative effects on working memory, infor-
mation processing speed, learning, the abilities to perform
Thus, both younger and older patients with CP in the abstraction and solve tasks, and response suppression [35].
spine had mild neurodynamic impairments to attention, men- Treatment directed to correcting peripheral sources of
tal flexibility, information processing speed, and memory. pain and emotional disorders (rational psychotherapy, tran-
46 Melkumova, Podchufarova, and Yakhno

quilizers, and antidepressants) may potentially have posi- 16. G. M. Grace, W. R. Nielson, M. Hopkins, et al., “Concentration and
tive influences on cognitive functions in patients with CP. memory deficits,” Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol., 21, 477–487 (1999).
17. O. Gureje, G. E. Simon, and M. Von Korff, “A cross-national study
Thus, the need to optimize the treatment of CP with of the course of persistent pain in primary care,” Pain, 92, 195–200
the aim of decreasing its negative influences on cognitive (2001).
functions is evident. 18. R. P. Hart, M. F. Martelli, and N. D. Zasler, “Chronic pain and neu-
ropsychological functioning,” Neuropsychol. Rev., 10, No. 3,
131–149 (2000).
REFERENCES 19. T. Iezzi, Y. Archibald, P. Barnett, et al., “Neurocognitive perfor-
mance and emotional status in chronic pain patients,” J. Behav.
1. A. M. Vein, T. G. Voznesenskaya, V. L. Golubev, and G. M. Dyukova, Med., 22, 205–216 (1999).
Depression in Neurological Practice (clinical features, diagnosis, 20. J. F. Karp, C. F. Reynolds, M. A. Butter, et al., “The relationship
treatment) [in Russian], Meditsinskoe Informatsionnoe Agentstvo, between pain and mental flexibility in older adult pain clinic
Moscow (2007), 3rd Edition, Revised and updated. patients,” Pain med., 7, 5 (2006).
2. V. N. Grigoriev, Psychosomatic Aspects of Neurorehabilitation. 21. D. G. Kewman, N. Vaishampayan, D. Zald, et al., “Cognitive impair-
Chronic Pain [in Russian], NGMA Press, Nizhnii Novgorod (2004). ment in musculoskeletal pain patients,” Int. J. Psychiatr. Med., 21,
3. V. V. Kuzmenko, V. A. Fokin, E. R. Mattis, et al., “Psychological 253–262 (1991).
methods for quantitative assessment of pain,” Sov. Med., 10, 44–48 22. M. D. Lezak, Neuropsychology Assessment, New York University
(1986). Press (1983).
4. L. Ya. Livshits, Yu. Ya. Lubzin, V. V. Usin, and L. V. Nikanorov, 23. L. M. McCracken and G. L. Iversen, “Predicting complaints of
“Experience in the study of distributed chronic pain syndromes,” in: impaired cognitive functioning in patients with chronic pain,” J. Pain
Organization of Medical Services for Patients with Pain Syndromes: Symptom Manage., 21, No. 5, 392–396 (2001).
Proc. Russ. Sci.-Appl. Conf. [in Russian], Novosibirsk (1997), 24. J. S. Richards, C. Nepomuceno, M. Riles, and Z. Suer, “Assessing
pp. 34–35. pain behavior: the UAB Pain Behavior Scale,” Pain, 14, No. 4,
5. S. A. Lytaev, B. V. Ovchinnikov, and I. F. Dyakonov, Basic Clinical 393–398 (1982).
Psychology and Medical Psychodiagnostics [in Russian], ELBI, 25. L. J. Rowe, “Imaging of mechanical and degenerative syndromes of
St. Petersburg (2008). the lumbar spine,” in: Clinical Anatomy and Management of Low
6. K. A. Melkumova, “Chronic pain and cognitive functions,” Nevrol. Back Pain, L. G. F. Giles (ed.), Butterworth-Heinemann, Oxford
Zh., 14, No. 2, 41–48 (2009). (1997), pp. 275–313.
7. N. G. Minaeva, Initiatives in Spinal Pain, World Health Organization, 26. D. I. Rubin, “Epidemiology and risk factors for spine pain,” Neurol.
Department of Noninfectious Diseases, G. E. Ehrlich and N. G. Clin., 25, 353–371 (2007).
Khataev (eds.) (1999); Nevrol. Zh., 6, No. 3, 53–57 (2001). 27. T. A. Salthouse, “The processing-speed theory of adult age differ-
8. S. S. Pavlenko and N. L. Tov, “Studies of the incidence of the main ences in cognition,” Psychol. Rev., 103, No. 3, 403–428 (1996).
forms of chronic pain syndromes among the population of 28. T. A. Salthouse, “Memory aging from 18 to 80,” Alzheimer’s Dis.
Novosibirsk,” Bol, 1, 13–16 (2003). Assoc. Dis., 17, No. 3, 172–167 (2003).
9. S. D. Pavlenko, “The situation and problems in epidemiological 29. R. Severeijns, J. W. Vlaeyen, M. A. van den Hout, et al., “Pain catas-
studies of pain syndromes,” Bol, 4, 2–7 (2006). trophizing predicts pain intensity, disability, and psychological dis-
10. S. S. Pavlenko, Pain in the Lower Spine (Epidemiology, Clinical tress independent of the level of physical impairment,” Clin. J. Pain,
Diagnostic Classification, Current Trends in Diagnosis, Treatment, 17, 165–172 (2001).
and Standardization of Medical Services): A Handbook [in Russian], 30. E. Strauss, E. M. S. Sherman, and O. Spreen, A Compendium of
Sibmedizdat NGMU, Novosibirsk (2007). Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms and Commentary,
11. G. B. Andersson, “Epidemiology of low back pain,” Acta Orthop. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2006), pp. 477–499, 499–522,
Scand., Suppl. 281, 28–31 (1998). 655–672, 1166–1171.
12. H. Ben-Zur, B. Rappaport, R. Ammar, and G. Uretzky, “Coping 31. M. J. L. Sullivan, S. Bishop, and J. Pivik, “The pain catastrophizing
strategies, life style changes and pessimism after open heart scale: development and validation,” Psychol. Assess., 7, 524–532
surgery,” Health and Social Work, 25, No. 3, 201–210 (2000). (1995).
13. R. Chakraverty and C. Parsons, “An acute spinal pain clinic run in 32. J. B. Wade and R. P. Hart, “Attention and the stage of pain process-
accordance with CSAG guidelines. A nine month report,” J. Orth. ing,” Pain Med., 3, No. 1, 30–38 (2002).
Med., 21, 7–12, (1999). 33. M. Von Korff and G. Simon, “The relationship between pain and
14. C. Eccleston, G. Crombez, S. Aldrich, et al., “Attention and somatic depression,” Br. J. Psychiat., Suppl. 30, 101–108 (1997).
awareness in chronic pain,” Pain, 72, 209–215 (1997). 34. G. Waddell, “A new clinical model for the treatment of low-back
15. R. J. Gatchel and M. A. Gardea, “Lower back pain: psychological pain,” Spine, 12, 632–644 (1987).
issues. Their importance in predicting disability, response to treat- 35. J. L. Wetherill, C. A. Reynolds, M. Gatz, et al., “Anxiety, cognitive
ment and search for compensation,” Neurologic Clinics, 17, performance, and cognitive decline in normal aging,” J. Gerontol. B.
149–166 (1999). Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci., 3, 246–255 (2002).

You might also like