You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/11117070

Behavior Problems and Parenting Stress in Families of Three-Year-Old Children


With and Without Developmental Delays

Article  in  American journal of mental retardation: AJMR · December 2002


DOI: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0433:BPAPSI>2.0.CO;2 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

476 8,350

4 authors:

Bruce L. Baker Jan Blacher


University of California, Los Angeles University of California, Riverside
136 PUBLICATIONS   6,729 CITATIONS    187 PUBLICATIONS   6,970 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Keith Crnic Craig Edelbrock


Arizona State University Pennsylvania State University
130 PUBLICATIONS   11,952 CITATIONS    57 PUBLICATIONS   16,117 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Families and Residential Placement View project

Collaborative Family Study View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Bruce L. Baker on 08 November 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior Problems and Parenting Stress in Families


of Three-Year-Old Children With and Without
Developmental Delays
Bruce L. Baker
University of California, Los Angeles
Jan Blacher
University of California, Riverside
Keith A. Crnic and Craig Edelbrock
The Pennsylvania State University

Abstract
Children and adolescents with mental retardation are at heightened risk for mental disor-
der. We examined early evidence of behavior problems in 225 three-year-old children with
or without developmental delays and the relative impact of cognitive delays and problem
behaviors on their parents. Staff-completed Bayley Behavior Scales and parent-completed
Child Behavior Checklists (CBCLs) showed greater problems in children with delays than
in those without delays. Children with delays were 3 to 4 times as likely to have a total
CBCL score within the clinical range. Parenting stress was higher in delayed condition
families. Regression analyses revealed that the extent of child behavior problems was a
much stronger contributor to parenting stress than was the child’s cognitive delay.

Adults with mental retardation are at height- dren with delays are at the same heightened risk
ened risk for mental disorder, a phenomenon as these older children and adolescents.
termed dual diagnosis. The limited literature on Despite high prevalence rates, dually diag-
dual diagnosis in children, derived mainly from nosed children have been among the least under-
the study of adolescents with mental retardation, stood and most underserved of any group with
indicates a higher incidence as well (Gillberg, Pers- disabilities (M. Campbell & Malone, 1991; Pfeif-
son, Grufman, & Themner, 1986; Jacobson, fer & Baker, 1994; Reiss, Levitan, & McNally,
1990). Two recent population-based studies ex- 1982). Dual diagnosis presents unique diagnostic
tended the age limit downward. Stromme and and treatment challenges and leaves children at
Diseth (2000), examining all children ages 8 to 13 particular risk for unfavorable long-term life out-
years born during a 5-year period in a county in comes. Placement is more likely when children
Norway, found that 37% of those with mental with mental retardation have serious maladaptive
retardation met International Classification of Dis- behaviors that challenge the family’s capacity to
eases–10th ed. (ICD-10) criteria for psychiatric dis- manage the child or adolescent effectively (Blach-
order. Steffenburg, Gillberg, and Steffenburg er, 1994; Borthwick-Duffy & Eyman, 1990; Brom-
(1996) found that 59% of children 8 to 16 years ley & Blacher, 1989; McIntyre, Blacher, & Baker,
of age who had mental retardation and epilepsy 2001). Beyond placement considerations, serious
had at least one psychiatric disorder. We do not maladaptive behavior among persons with mental
know yet, however, whether even younger chil- retardation predicts academic problems, failure in

q American Association on Mental Retardation 433


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

community living arrangements, frequent moves, without delays have found more problem behav-
social isolation and rejection, and reduced em- iors in those with delays. Kopp, Baker, and Brown
ployment prospects (Borthwick-Duffy & Eyman, (1992) reported social deficits in 15 children with
1990; Bruininks, Hill, & Morreau, 1988; Pearson delays who were 4 years old (range 5 3 to 5),
et al., 2000). Clearly, dual diagnosis is of serious contrasted with 15 playmates without delays. Play
concern. was observed in triads of 1 child with delays and
The overall aim of our research program is to 2 without delays; the child without delays closest
derive a better understanding of this heightened in age to the child who had delays was chosen for
risk in children with developmental delays. To comparison. Observations showed significantly
track problem development, we have begun as- more ‘‘no-play’’ intervals for children with delays.
sessment of problem behaviors when children are Within intervals where play took place, delay
3 years old. Our focus in this paper is on mothers’ group children engaged in solitary play (rather
and fathers’ reports of problem behaviors exhib- than parallel or social play) 33% of the time versus
ited by young children with and those without 12% for children without delays. Moreover, chil-
delays. Moreover, we have considered the impact dren with delays showed much disruptive entry
that child problem behaviors have on parents’ ex- (53% vs. 0%), and only 53% of children with de-
perience of stress. lays at some time smiled and/or laughed to their
We would not expect to find much diagnos- peers in contrast to 93% of children without de-
able mental disorder per se during the preschool lays.
years. However, longitudinal studies of typically On the Behavioral Screening Questionnaire
developing children have found high continuity (Richman, Stevenson, & Graham, 1982), parents
for behavior problems across the childhood years; of children with delays reported significantly high-
this is especially true for externalizing problems, er behavior problem scores. Merrell and Holland
such as aggression, noncompliance, conduct prob- (1997) found similar differences in a large survey
lems, and hyperactivity (S. Campbell, 1994; S. study of 398 children ages 3 to 5 years. Half of
Campbell, Breaux, Ewing, & Szumowski, 1984; these children were categorized by teachers as de-
Egeland, Kalkoske, Gottesman, & Farrell-Erickson, layed, although no criteria were reported; mea-
1990; Heller, Baker, Henker, & Hinshaw, 1996). sures were completed by ‘‘parents or teachers,’’
Thus, our primary question is: What, if any, prob- without further specification. Social skills deficit
lem behaviors already differentiate young children and behavioral–emotional problem excesses were
with and without cognitive delays? each 4 to 5 times as likely in those children with
There is little known about how early in life delays. The greatest differences were in the social
children with delays evidence heightened problem domain: social interaction and independence
behaviors. Studies of typically developing pre- skills and socially withdrawn behavior patterns.
schoolers have found a modest negative relation- There is some evidence, then, of increased prob-
ship between child cognitive ability and behavior lem behaviors in children with delays by age 3,
problems. Hay and coworkers (1997), studying a although there is a need for further study with
sample of 93 typically developing 4-year-olds in better defined assessment procedures and child
South London, reported negative correlations be- delay status.
tween child IQ and mothers’ and fathers’ Child There has been considerable study of families
Behavior Checklist (CBCL) behavior problem raising a child with mental retardation (Baker,
scores. Similarly, Dietz, Lavigne, Arend, and Ro- Blacher, Kopp, & Kraemer, 1997). At a practical
senbaum (1997), studying typically developing level, these families experience excessive caretak-
children ages 2 to 5, found that lower verbal or ing demands, financial burden, and restrictions on
performance IQ predicted higher externalizing leisure activities and social lives as well as disrup-
and internalizing CBCL scores. On the other tions of family plans (Gunn & Berry, 1987; Rod-
hand, Feldman, Hancock, Reilly, Minnes, and rique, Morgan, & Geffken, 1992). Not surprising-
Cairns (2000a) in their study of 2-year-olds with ly, parents experience increased stress, especially
intellectual delays did not find elevated problems in domains related to childrearing (Baker et al.,
on this measure relative to published norms, al- 1997; Crnic, Friedrich, & Greenberg, 1983; Fidler,
though the delays were mild and in some cases Hodapp, & Dykens, 2000; Orr, Cameron, Dob-
questionable. son, & Day, 1993; Minnes, 1988), Parental stress,
Researchers comparing preschoolers with and in turn, is one domain of family risk and protec-

434 q American Association on Mental Retardation


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

tive factors that may be relevant to the emergence Method


of psychiatric problems in children with mental
retardation (Crnic & Greenberg, 1987; Margalit, Participants
Shulman, & Stuchiner, 1989). A lingering ques- Participants were 225 families with a 3-year-
tion is whether the heightened stress represents a old child. These families had been recruited to
chronic reaction to intellectual disability per se or participate in a 2-year longitudinal study of young
a reaction to the often associated behavioral chal- children from ages 3 to 5 years, with samples
lenges that families face. Our second question, drawn from Central Pennsylvania and Southern
then, is: Do parents of 3-year-olds with delays al- California. The children were classified as having
ready experience heightened child-related stress, delays (n 5 92) or not having delays (n 5 133).
and, if so, what are the relative contributions of Delayed condition families were recruited primar-
cognitive delay versus problem behaviors to pa- ily through community agencies that serve per-
rental stress levels? sons with developmental disabilities. The selec-
Although negative impact has garnered the tion criteria were that the child (a) be between 30
largest share of research attention, there has been and 39 months of age; (b) receive a score on the
a recent trend to also consider the more positive Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (see be-
or growth-promoting effect that a child with men- low) between 30 and 85; (c) be ambulatory, and
tal retardation can have on the family. The poten- (d) not be diagnosed with autism. Nondelayed
tially positive impact on families may operate condition families were recruited primarily
through specific positive contributions of disabil- through preschools and daycare programs. The se-
ity on family members (Turnbull et al., 1993) as lection criteria were that the child (a) be between
well as through promoting positive coping strat- 30 and 39 months of age; (b) receive a score on
egies and adaptation (Crnic et al., 1983; Hawkins, the Bayley Scales of 85 or above; (c) not be born
Singer, & Nixon, 1993). We also examined the prematurely or have a developmental disability.
influence of child behavior problems on parents’ Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics
positive views of childrearing. of this sample by group status (delayed, nonde-
Assessment of child behavior problems relies layed). The intake assessment was conducted, on
heavily on informants’ opinions, usually through average, just before the child’s third birthday (35.2
checklists. Most studies of young children have months; standard deviation [SD] 5 3.0). In the
relied on mothers as informants. Yet, mothers and combined sample, there were more boys than girls
fathers have different relationships with their (57%), and 61% of the children were Caucasian.
child and different opportunities to observe their Recruitment initially focused on intact families, so
child’s behavior; moreover, any informants’ re- most participants (86%) were married (defined
sponses are to some degree influenced by his or here as legally married or living together at least
her own personality (Achenbach, McConaughy, 6 months). The socioeconomic status (SES) was
& Howell, 1987; Hay et al., 1999; Phares, 1996). generally high, with 51% of mothers and 51% of
Studies in which mothers and fathers have both fathers having graduated from college, and 53%
completed CBCLs have reported significant but of families having an annual income of $50,000
modest correlations (Baker & Heller, 1996; Hay or more.
et al., 1999). For example, in the Hay and col- The two conditions did not differ on the
leagues study just noted, although the mean child attributes shown in Table 1. The conditions
scores for mothers and fathers did not differ on differed on the parent and family attributes that
any scale, the correlation between mothers and indicated SES; these were significantly higher in
fathers was .32 for Total score and .42 for Exter- the nondelayed families, with a greater percentage
nalizing; the correlation for Internalizing prob- of mothers and fathers graduating from college
lems did not reach significance. There has been and more families with incomes at $50,000 or
little study of how fathers of children with delays more. We covaried SES indicators in subsequent
perceive problem behaviors or of how these be- analyses where these correlated significantly with
haviors affect their experience of parenting stress. the dependent variable.
Our third questions, then, is: Do fathers and
mothers differ in their perception of problem be- Assessments
haviors in children with and without delays and/ The data considered in this study were ob-
or in their experience of parenting stress? tained in two ways. The measures of child’s de-

q American Association on Mental Retardation 435


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

Table 1. Demographics by Group


Delayed Nondelayed
Variable (n 5 92) (n 5 133) t or chi squared
Child
Age at testing (months) 35.6 35.0 1.59
Gender (% boys) 64.1 51.9 2.85
Race (% Caucasian) 60.9 60.9 0.00
Siblings (% only children) 30.4 29.3 0.00
Bayley Scales: MDIa 57.9 104.6 29.58***
Parent and family
Marital status (% married) 80.4 89.5 2.94
Mother educ. (% college degree) 31.5 61.7 18.57***
Mother employment (%) 52.2 61.7 1.63
Father education (% college degree)b 34.6 58.4 9.94**
Family income (% $50K1)c 43.5 59.8 5.20*
a
Mental Development Index. bN 5 203. cN 5 224. d
The ts are in boldface.
*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

velopmental level and problem behaviors were aminer and an assistant. Only mental develop-
obtained at the home intake assessment session, ment items were administered; the Mental Devel-
conducted when the child was between 30 and 39 opment Index (MDI) is normed with a mean of
months of age. Prior to this session, parents had 100 and an SD of 15. Bayley (1993) reported high
completed a telephone intake interview with our short-term test–retest reliability for the MDI, r 5
staff and had received project descriptions and the .91. Also, with children ages 36 to 42 months, the
informed consent form. Two trained researcher as- MDI related to the Full-Scale IQ of the Wechsler
sistants visited the family for a 2-hour assessment Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Re-
session. After reviewing procedures, answering vised (WPPSI–R), r 5 .73 (Bayley, 1993).
questions, and obtaining informed consent, the Behavior Rating Scale. The examiner completes
staff administered the Bayley Scales of Infant De- this checklist following the testing session. The
velopment to the child. During this testing, moth- scale has three dimensions: Orientation/Engage-
er, and father if present, completed a demographic ment (12 items), Emotion Regulation (10 items),
questionnaire and the CBCL (see below). Mea- and Motor Quality (8 items). We only included
sures that were not completed during the assess- the first two dimensions in the present study. Ex-
ment session were mailed to us soon thereafter. aminers scored collaboratively, discussing any dis-
Immediately following the home assessment ses- agreements and arriving at a joint rating. To de-
sion, the two staff members completed the Bayley termine interjudge reliability, for a subsample of
Behavior Problems Scales together. The measure 25 protocols, the two examiners completed the
of the child’s impact on the family was part of a scale separately and then arrived at consensus rat-
measure packet completed prior to a home obser- ings. For each pair, the examiner with the most
vation, conducted at 36 months or soon after the experience using Bayley Scales was designated as
intake, if the intake was later than 36 months. the standard. There was very high agreement be-
tween raters, r (23) 5 .96, p , .001.
Measures CBCL for Ages 1.5–5 (Achenbach & Rescorla,
Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (Bayley, 2001). This is a new version of the widely used
1993). The Bayley Scales are a widely used assess- CBCL (Achenbach, 1991), aimed at the preschool
ment of mental and motor development of chil- years. It has 99 items that indicate child problems,
dren ages 1 to 42 months. This instrument was listed in alphabetical order (from ‘‘aches and pains
administered in the child’s home, with the mother without medical cause’’ to ‘‘worries’’). The respon-
present. In most cases, there was a primary ex- dent indicates, for each item, whether it is not true

436 q American Association on Mental Retardation


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

(0), somewhat or sometimes true (1), or very true or Table 2. Correlations of Mother and Father
often true (2), now or within the past 2 months. Child Behavior Checklist Scores by Group
This CBCL yields a Total problem score, broad-
Non-
band Externalizing and Internalizing scores, and
Score Delayeda delayedb
narrow-band scales (Emotionally Reactive, De-
pressed/Anxious, Withdrawn, Somatic, Sleep Total Score .75 .57c
Problems, Attention, and Aggression). Scale sum Internalizing .75 .57c
scores were used in analyses. Alphas for the pres- Externalizing .71 .63
ent sample were rs of .94 for both mothers and Emotional Reactivity .61 .38c
fathers. The CBCL also yields T scores, with the Anxious/Depressed .52 .47
mean set at 50 and an SD of 10. We report these
Somatic .73 .58
for the Total scale, where the mean is set at 50
Withdrawal .67 .57
and SD is 10; T scores for the narrow-band scales
are truncated at 50 and, thus, are not as valid for Sleep .71 .61
correlational analyses. Attention .71 .59
Family Impact Questionnaire (Donenberg & Aggression .68 .58
Baker, 1993). This instrument is a 50-item ques- Note. All correlations were significant at the .001 level.
tionnaire that is used to measure the ‘‘child’s im- a
N 5 75. bN 5 126. cDifference between delay and non-
pact on the family compared to the impact other delay correlations significant at p , .05.
children his/her age have on their families’’ (e.g.,
Item 1: ‘‘My child is more stressful’’). Parents en- Internalizing, and Emotionally Reactive scores. In
dorse items on a 4-point scale, ranging from not the delayed condition, mothers’ and fathers’ mean
at all to very much. Five scales are used to measure scores did not differ on any scale. In the nonde-
negative impact on Feelings About Parenting (9 layed condition mothers’ reported significantly
items), Social Relationships (11), Finances (7), higher Attention and lower Aggression scores
and, if applicable, Siblings (9) and Marriage (7). than fathers did, p , .05.
One scale measures impact on positive feelings Child Behavior Scores by Delay Status. We con-
about Parenting (7). A combined score (negative trasted the two status group (delayed and nonde-
impact) is the sum of the first two negative impact layed) on the CBCL and the Bayley Behavior
scales. Alphas in the present sample for negative Scale scores. Analyses were conducted as 2 (status)
impact were .92 for mothers and .90 for fathers; 3 2 (gender) analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Ed-
for positive impact, both alphas were .81. ucation and family income, on which the delay
status groups differed, were related to some of the
Results dependent variables for mothers and fathers and,
thus, were covaried for all analyses. Mothers’
Child Behavior Problems CBCL scores and Bayley Behavior scores are
Mother and Father CBCL scores. In the com- shown in Table 3. On mothers’ CBCL scales, chil-
bined sample there was high agreement between dren in the delayed condition were reported to
mothers’ and fathers’ CBCL scores. Pearson cor- have significantly higher Total and Internalizing
relations for Total, Internalizing, and Externaliz- scores as well as higher scores on Attention and
ing problem scores were .68, .68, and .67, respec- Withdrawal problems. There were several signifi-
tively. Agreement on the narrow-band scales cant gender effects; boys scored higher than did
ranged from .50 (Anxious/Depressed) to .68 (At- girls on problems with Emotional Reactivity and
tention Problems). All correlations were signifi- Aggression as well as the Externalizing and Total
cant at the .001 level. Moreover, mothers’ and fa- scores. Boys with delays scored highest on every
thers’ mean scores were highly similar; mean dif- scale (except Sleep Problems), resulting in several
ferences did not approach significance for any significant Delay 3 Gender interactions. Children
broad-band or narrow-band scale. with delays were scored significantly lower on the
Table 2 shows parents’ agreement by delay Bayley Behavior Scales of Emotional Regulation
status; parents in the delayed condition had high- and Orientation/Engagement; there were no gen-
er agreement on every scale than did parents in der or status by gender effects on the Bayley Be-
the nondelayed condition. The differences be- havior Scales.
tween correlations were significant for the Total, Fathers’ CBCL scores are also shown in Table

q American Association on Mental Retardation 437


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

Table 3. Child Behavior Scores by Group, Gender, and Parent Scores


Delayeda Nondelayedb F
Test score Boys Girls Boys Girls Status Gender S 3 Gc
Mothers’ CBCL
Total Score 50.3 39.6 34.1 34.1 6.73** 5.18* 5.59*
Internalizing 13.2 10.1 7.7 8.4 5.82* 2.63 6.25*
Externalizing 18.5 14.3 13.7 12.6 3.53 7.27** 2.77
Emotional Reactivity 3.6 2.3 2.3 2.3 0.70 5.13* 5.74*
Anxious/Depressed 3.1 2.9 2.2 2.6 0.38 0.67 0.15
Somatic 2.9 1.8 1.5 1.9 1.90 2.11 9.36**
Withdrawal 3.5 3.1 1.6 1.6 23.12*** 0.75 0.90
Sleep 3.7 2.8 3.4 4.1 2.03 0.23 4.60*
Attention 4.4 3.8 2.4 2.1 28.88*** 2.51 0.21
Aggression 14.2 10.5 11.3 10.5 0.31 7.23** 3.55
Bayley Behavior Scales
Emotional Regulation 32.9 34.6 41.9 42.6 59.90*** 1.09 0.07
Orient./Engagement 32.6 31.2 37.7 36.6 27.80*** 1.51 0.06
Fathers’ CBCL
Total Score 45.8 40.7 29.7 34.9 9.43** 0.06 4.39*
Internalizing 11.3 10.5 6.1 9.2 8.80** 0.93 6.04*
Externalizing 17.4 14.4 12.1 12.5 6.28* 1.57 2.84
Emotional Reactivity 2.9 2.5 1.5 2.2 4.80* 0.06 3.67
Anxious/Depressed 2.8 2.9 2.0 2.9 0.52 2.09 3.10
Somatic 2.3 2.0 1.2 2.2 1.65 0.82 8.61**
Withdrawal 3.2 3.2 1.4 1.8 21.69*** 0.35 0.66
Sleep 3.0 3.5 3.1 4.0 0.76 2.43 0.44
Attention 4.0 3.6 2.6 2.6 11.33*** 0.88 0.76
Aggression 13.3 10.8 9.5 9.9 4.02* 1.52 3.15
Note. Analyses were run as 2 (group) 3 2 (gender) ANOVAs, covarying for mother education and family income.
a
Ns 5 59 and 33 for boys and girls, respectively. bNs 5 69 for boys and 63 for girls, respectively. cStatus 3 Gender.
d
Child Behavior Checklist.
*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001.

3. Fathers reported the same status group differ- Total T score was in the clinical range for 5.6%
ences as mothers did, but in addition reported sig- of children without delays and 24% of children
nificantly higher scores for children with delays with delays, a ratio of 1:4.3, continuity corrected
on Externalizing, Aggression, and Emotional Re- x2(1, N 5 201) 5 13.04, p , .001.
activity scales. Fathers did not report gender dif-
ferences, but reported status by gender effects sim- Family Impact
ilar to mothers. For the combined sample, Family Impact
Children meeting criteria for clinical range. We Questionnaire scores were similar for mothers and
determined the number of children in each status fathers. On the Negative Impact Scale, parental
group who were at or above the clinical cut-off, T agreement was r 5 .66; mothers’ mean score was
score 5 64, on the Total CBCL (Achenbach & 13.9 and fathers, 12.8 The only significant differ-
Rescorla, 2001). For mothers, the CBCL Total T ence was on positive impact, where fathers scored
score was in the clinical range for 8.3% of children higher (M 5 16.7) than did mothers (15.7), t 5
without delays and 26.1% of children with delays, 2.54, p , .05.
a ratio of 1:3.1, continuity corrected x2(1, N 5 Family impact and child status groups. We con-
225) 5 11.82, p , .01. For fathers, the CBCL trasted the two status group (delayed and nonde-

438 q American Association on Mental Retardation


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

Table 4. Correlations of IQ and Behavior Problems With Parents’ Stress (FIQ Negative Impact)
Stressa
Test Mothers’ Fathers’
Bayley Mental Development Index 2.34 2.29
Emotional Regulation 2.41 2.37
Orientation-Engagement 2.23 2.29
Child Behavior Checklist
Externalizing scale .70 .65
Internalizing scale .51 .54
Total .69 .65
Note. FIQ 5 Family Impact Questionnaire. All correlations were significant at the .001 level.
a
N 5 225 mothers and 198 fathers.

layed) on the Family Impact Questionnaire scales. relationship another way, we divided CBCL total
Analyses were conducted as 2 (status) 3 2 (gender) scores into thirds, yielding a high, medium, and
ANOVAs, with education covaried. Parents of de- low behavior problem group. We conducted a 2
layed group children reported higher negative (status) 3 3 (behavior problem group) ANOVA
child impact than did parents of nondelayed on mother’s combined Family Impact Question-
group children. Delayed group mothers scored naire Negative Impact score, with mother’s edu-
higher than did nondelayed group mothers on the cation as a covariate. Figure 1 shows mothers’
combined negative impact variable, F(1, 220) 5 Negative Impact scores by status group and be-
13.76, p , .001, as well as negative impact on havior problem group. Status group was signifi-
finances, F(1, 220) 5 21.70, p , .001. There was cant, F(1, 218) 5 11.67, p , .01, but behavior
no status group effect for negative impact on sib- problems group had a considerably stronger ef-
lings or marriage or for positive impact on feelings fect, F(2, 218) 5 43.62, p , .001. For the variables
about parenting. Mothers of delayed boys reported positive impact, negative impact on marriage, and
the greatest negative impact. There was a signifi- negative impact on siblings, status group was not
cant gender effect for negative impact, F(1, 220) significant, but there were significant behavior
5 10.99, p 5 .001, and Status 3 Gender inter- problem group effects. For negative impact on fi-
actions for negative impact, F(1,220) 5 7.66, p , nances, there were significant main effects for sta-
.01, and impact on siblings, F(1,148) 5 6.19, p , tus and behavior problems as well as the only sig-
.05. Fathers’ scores showed a similar pattern, al- nificant interaction, with delayed group high be-
though differences were not as strong. Delayed havior problems associated with highest stress by
group fathers scored higher on negative impact far. We repeated these analyses for fathers’ Family
than did nondelayed group fathers, F(1,188) 5 Impact Questionnaire scores. The behavior prob-
4.76, p , .05, as well as on negative impact on lem group variable was significant for every Fam-
finances, F(1, 189) 5 18.08, p , .001. Fathers of ily Impact Questionnaire scale for our primary
delayed boys also reported the greatest negative im- measure of stress, Negative Impact, F(2, 189) 5
pact; there was a significant Status 3 Gender in-
teraction for negative impact, F(1,188) 5 6.10, p
, .05. No other comparison reached significance.
Parenting stress, child delay, and behavior prob-
lems. Table 4 shows the correlations between
mother and father Negative Impact (stress) scores
and two domains of child functioning: cognitive
functioning (Bayley MDI) and behavior (CBCL
and Bayley Behavior Scales). Although stress
scores related significantly to the child’s cognitive
functioning, the relationships with behavior prob- Figure 1. Mothers stress by delay status and be-
lems were considerably higher. To examine this havior problems (BP) group.

q American Association on Mental Retardation 439


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

42.62, p ,.001. Delay status was associated sig- (Step 2), and status (Step 3). Behavior problems
nificantly only with Negative Impact on Finances. accounted for 52% of the variance, with MDI and
We examined the relationship between child status not accounting for significant variance.
characteristics and parent stress further, using hi- Each of the three behavior problem indicators was
erarchical multiple regression. We considered the significant in the final model.
relative contributions of child cognitive function- Regressions on father negative impact pro-
ing (Bayley MDI) and behavior problems (CBCL duced similar results, with the variables combined
Total score; Bayley Behavior Scales of Emotional accounting for 46% of the variance. With MDI
Regulation and Orientation/Engagement). The entered first, it accounted for 8% and behavior
CBCL total score was modestly related to the Bay- problems accounted for an additional 38%; the
ley Behavior Scale scores for mothers and fathers, CBCL score and the Bayley Emotional Regula-
rs 5 .27 to .34. We also entered the status variable tion score were each significant in the final model.
(delayed, nondelayed) to detect variance in stress When variables were entered in the reverse order,
attributable to having a child with delays over and behavior problems accounted for 46%, with MDI
above the actual level of cognitive functioning. In and status not accounting for significant variance.
the first analysis, we entered MDI (Step 1), status Here too, the CBCL score and the Bayley Emo-
(Step 2), and behavior problems (Step 3). As tion Regulation score were each significant in the
shown in Table 5, these variables explained 52% final model. The thrust of all of the above anal-
of the variance in mothers’ stress, with behavior yses is that child behavior problems contributed
problems accounting for an additional 40% of the to parental stress far more than did the level of
variance after the MDI score was accounted for; cognitive functioning.
status per se did not account for variance. Each
of the three behavior problem indicators was sig-
nificant in the final model. To examine the extent
Discussion
to which cognitive functioning predicted stress af- We examined the extent of behavior prob-
ter accounting for behavior problems, we con- lems in 3-year-old children with and without de-
ducted a further regression analysis with behavior lays and the relative impact on their parents of
problems entered first (Step 1), followed by MDI cognitive delays and problem behaviors. Our first

Table 5. Hierarchical Multiple Regression, Predicting Parental Stress From Child’s Mental Development
and Behavior Problems
Sig F
Negative impact R R3
R change
2
F change change
Mother (n 5 224)
Bayley MDIa .34 .12 .12 29.28 .000
Delay status .34 .12 .00 0.80 .371
Behavior problems .72 .52 .40 61.49 .000
Behavior problems .72 .52 .52 78.42 .000
Bayley MDI .72 .52 .00 2.87 .092
Delay status .72 .52 .00 0.38 .846
Father (n 5 193)
Bayley MDI .28 .08 .08 17.17 .000
Delay Status .30 .09 .01 1.31 .254
Behavior problems .68 .46 .38 44.75 .000
Behavior problems .68 .46 .46 54.12 .000
Bayley MDI .68 .46 .00 0.08 .782
Delay status .68 .46 .01 2.44 .120
Note. Behavior problems variables were: (a) CBCL total score, (b) Bayley Behavior Scales Emotional Regulation score,
and (c) Bayley Behavior Scales Orientation/Engagement score.
a
Mental Development Index.

440 q American Association on Mental Retardation


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

question was, Do children with and without de- as greater negative impact on family finances. Pos-
lays already show differential extent of problem itive impact did not differ by delay status. Thus,
behaviors by age 3? Parents of children with de- parents of children with delays expressed positive
lays reported higher Total CBCL scores and high- feelings similar to those of parents with nondelay-
er broad-band Internalizing Problem scores than ed children, while at the same time acknowledging
did parents of children without delays; indeed, more negative ones. Although many researchers
children with delays were 3 to 4 times as likely to have found heightened stress in families of chil-
have a Total score within the clinical range. On dren with disabilities, the measures commonly
the narrow-band scales, children with delays were used to assess parental stress have a number of
most different from their peers without delays on items tapping the child’s limitations, and these
Social Withdrawal and Attention Problems. Al- items are included in the summary score purport-
though fathers’ scores were higher for children ing to measure the parent’s stress. It is a given,
with delays on Aggression and Emotional Reac- therefore, that stress will appear to increase with
tivity problems, mothers’ scores were not. The increasing disability. The measure employed here,
groups did not differ on either parents’ scores on the Family Impact Questionnaire (Donenberg &
Anxious/Depressed, Somatic, or Sleep Problems. Baker, 1993), does not ask about child function-
The strong difference in social behavior is ing and then make inferences about parental
consistent with the behavioral observations of stress. Rather, it was designed to avoid this prob-
Kopp et al. (1992) and the survey findings of Mer- lem by directly asking parents about their child’s
rell and Holland (1997), discussed earlier. As chil- impact on the family, relative to the impact they
dren with delays grow older, this social domain perceive other children having on their families.
may take on added importance as a moderator of Yet the association of child delay and parental
other types of problems. Children with poor so- stress was still found.
cial interaction skills may be particularly at risk The experience of child-related stress, how-
for problems with anxiety, depression, or aggres- ever, was related much more strongly to the pres-
sion because social demands become more com- ence of behavior problems than to intellectual de-
plex as children with delays become more aware lay. In regression analyses, for mothers and fa-
of their differences and as peers become more re- thers, child behavior problems explained most of
jecting. Kopp et al. (1992) found that 3-year-olds the variance in reports of negative impact. These
without delays were already demonstrating a high- analyses support the conclusion that problem be-
er rate of rejecting responses (80% of children) haviors, much more than the fact or extent of in-
than their playmates with delays. Lang, Baker, and tellectual delay, account for the parents’ stress lev-
Henker (2001) found that among nonaggressive el. We realize, however, that two alternative ex-
preschoolers, poor social skills predicted those planations, where the direction of influence goes
children who subsequently became aggressive by from parent to child, must be entertained. The
third grade. Thus, although CBCL scores for typ- first is that very stressed parents misperceive and
ically developing children have been found to be misreport their children as having more behavior
reasonably stable across several early school years problems, thus creating the apparent relationship
(Achenbach, 1991; Heller et al., 1996), we antici- between these domains. The second is that very
pated that for children with delays, there may be stressed parents, through their parenting, actually
a tendency for behavior problems, especially ex- create more problem behaviors in their children.
ternalizing ones, to increase over time with in- We can address the first hypothesis within
creasing cognitive and social demands, resulting our data set, as the Bayley Behavior Scales were
in an even greater differentiation of the two completed not by parents but by staff members,
groups. following the in-home cognitive assessment. Al-
Our second question was, Do parents of 3- though the Bayley Behavior Scales does not mea-
year-olds with delays experience heightened stress sure the same constructs as does the CBCL, there
relative to parents of children without delays, and, is a conceptual and modest statistical relationship
if so, is this stress primarily related to the chil- between these scores. In the regression analyses,
dren’s cognitive delay or behavioral challenges? although the CBCL scores accounted for consid-
Both mothers and fathers of children with delays erably more variance, the Bayley Behavior Scale
reported greater negative impact of the child on scores also entered significantly. When we ran
the family (the variable we refer to as stress) as well these same regressions with CBCL scores omitted,

q American Association on Mental Retardation 441


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

the pattern of findings was similar, albeit attenu- stress as well as in reduced child risk for later men-
ated; the Behavior Scales, even without the tal disorder.
CBCL, explained more variance in parent stress
than did the MDI. Thus, we are reasonably con-
fident that the relationship between child prob- References
lems and parent stress is not spurious, based on Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the CBCL
stressed parents misreporting their child’s difficul- 4–18. Burlington: University of Vermont, De-
ties because we found it with variables reported partment of Psychiatry.
by others. The second hypothesis, that stressed Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Man-
parents create greater problems in their children, ual for ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles.
cannot be ruled out in this data set and must Burlington: University of Vermont, Depart-
await repeated assessment of these variables over ment of Psychiatry.
time. Even if parental stress has an effect on chil- Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & How-
dren’s behavior and there is evidence that it does ell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral
(Heller et al., 1996) it is not likely that this ac- and emotional problems: Implications of
counts in full for the relationships found here. cross-informant correlations for situation
Mothers and fathers appeared to be viewing specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–
their child’s behavior problems in similar ways. 232.
Their CBCL scores were highly correlated, and Baker, B. L., & Heller, T. L. (1996). Preschool chil-
neither parent scored consistently higher on any dren with externalizing behaviors: Experience
scale. Moreover, mothers and fathers reported of fathers and mothers. Journal of Abnormal
similar degrees of child-related stress, and their Child Psychology, 26, 513–532.
CBCL scores related in a similar way to their Baker, B. L., Blacher, J., Kopp, C. B., & Kraemer,
stress. The parent agreements found here were B. (1997). Parenting children with mental re-
high, especially for parents of children with de- tardation. International Review of Research in
lays. Achenbach (1991) reported mother and fa- Mental Retardation, 20, 1–45.
ther agreement on CBCL Externalizing and Inter- Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Develop-
nalizing scores for boys 4 to 11 to be .86 and .71, ment Second Edition: Manual. San Antonio:
respectively; and for girls, .70 and .57. Researchers Psychological Corp.
focusing on 4-year-olds with no delays, however, Blacher, J. (1994). When there’s no place like home:
have reported considerably lower agreements, for Options for children living apart from their natu-
example, .42 and .18, respectively (Hay et al., ral families. Baltimore: Brookes.
1999) and .55 and .12 (Baker & Heller, 1996). Our Borthwick-Duffy, S. A., & Eyman, R. K. (1990).
agreements between parents of children with de- Who are the dually diagnosed? American Jour-
lays, .71 and .75, were similar to Achenbach’s cor- nal on Mental Retardation, 94, 586–595.
relations with children who were, on average, old- Bromley, B., & Blacher, J. B. (1989). Out-of-home
er. It is possible that the scales on this new 1.5- placement of children with severe handicaps:
to 5-year version of the CBCL will reflect better Factors delaying placement. American Journal
parent agreements and/or that parents of delayed on Mental Retardation, 94, 284–291.
children are especially attuned to their children’s Bruininks, R. H., Hill, B. K., & Morreau, L. E.
behavior and more likely to communicate about (1988). Prevalence and implications of mal-
it. adaptive behaviors and dual diagnosis in res-
It appears that the roots of dual diagnosis are idential and other service programs. In J. A.
taking hold at least by age 3. This finding has two Stark, F. J. Menolascino, M. H. Albarelli, &
clear implications for early intervention programs. V. C. Gray (Eds.), Mental retardation and men-
First, developers of child programs should make tal health: Classification, diagnosis, treatment and
social skill development and behavior problem services. New York: Springer-Verlag.
management a central focus of the curriculum. Campbell, M., & Malone, R. P. (1991). Mental
Second, parent program personnel should share retardation and psychiatric disorders. Hospital
this focus on ameliorating child problem behav- and Community Psychiatry, 42, 374–379.
iors, but also work with families on strategies for Campbell, S. B. (1994). Hard to manage pre-
coping with stress. Successful child behavior prob- school boys: Externalizing behavior, social
lem reduction may result in reduced parenting competence, and family context at two-year

442 q American Association on Mental Retardation


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

follow up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- S. Singer & L. E. Powers (Eds.), Families, dis-
ogy, 22, 147–166. ability and empowerment. Baltimore: Brookes.
Campbell, S. B., Breaux, A. M., Ewing, L., & Szu- Hay, D. F., Pawlby, S., Sharp, D., Schmuker, G.,
mowski, E. (1984). A one-year follow-up study Mills, A., Allen, H., & Kumar, R. (1999). Par-
of parent-referred ‘‘hyperactive’’ preschoolers. ents’ judgements about young children’s
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psy- problems: Why mothers and fathers might
chiatry, 23, 243–249. disagree yet still predict later outcomes. Jour-
Crnic, K. A., Friedrich, W. N., & Greenberg, M. nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40,
T. (1983). Adaptation of families with men- 1249–1258.
tally retarded children: A model of stress, cop- Heller, T. L., Baker, B. L., Henker, B., & Hinshaw,
ing, and family ecology. American Journal of S. (1996). Externalizing behavior and cogni-
Mental Deficiency, 88, 125–138. tive functioning from preschool to first-grade:
Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). Trans- Stability and predictors. Journal of Clinical
actional relationships between perceived fam- Child Psychology, 25, 376–387.
ily style, risk status, and mother–child inter- Jacobson, J. W. (1990). Do some mental disorders
actions in two year olds. Journal of Pediatric occur less frequently among persons with
Psychology, 12, 343–362. mental retardation? American Journal on Men-
Dietz, K. R., Lavigne, J. V., Arend, R., & Rosen- tal Retardation, 94, 596–602.
baum, D. (1997). Relation between intelli- Kopp, C. B., Baker, B. L., & Brown, K. W. (1992).
gence and psychopathology among pre- Social skills and their correlates: Preschoolers
schoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, with developmental delays. American Journal
26, 99–107. of Mental Retardation, 96, 357–366.
Donenberg, G., & Baker, B. L. (1993). The impact Lang, J. M., Baker, B. L., & Henker, B. (2001).
of young children with externalizing behav- Predicting aggression and disruptive behavior dis-
iors on their families. Journal of Abnormal order diagnoses: The role of early social function-
Child Psychology, 21, 179–198. ing. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Egeland, B., Kalkoske, M., Gottesman, N., & Far- California, Los Angeles.
rell-Erickson, M. (1990). Preschool behavior Margalit, M., Shulman, S., & Stuchiner, N.
problems: Stability and factors accounting for (1989). Behavior disorders and mental retar-
change. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- dation: The family system perspective. Re-
atry, 31, 891–909. search in Developmental Disabilities, 10, 315–
Feldman, M. A., Hancock, C. L., Reilly, N., Min- 326.
nes, P., & Cairns, C. (2000). Behavior prob- McIntyre, L. L., Blacher, J., & Baker, B. L. (2002).
lems in young children at risk for develop- Behaviour/mental health problems in young
mental delay. Journal of Child and Family Stud- adults with mental retardation: The impact
ies, 9, 247–261. on families. Journal of Intellectual Disability Re-
Fidler, D. J., Hodapp, R. M., & Dykens, E. M. search, 46, 239–249.
(2000). Stress in families of young children Merrell, K. W., & Holland, M. L. (1997). Social-
with Down syndrome, Williams syndrome, emotional behavior of preschool-age children
and Smith-Magenis syndrome. Early Educa- with and without developmental delays. Re-
tion and Development, 11, 395–406. search in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 393–
Gillberg, C., Persson, E., Grufman, M., & Them- 405.
ner, U. (1986). Psychiatric disorders in mildly Minnes, P. M. (1988). Family stress associated
and severely mentally retarded urban children with a developmentally handicapped child.
and adolescents: Epidemiological aspects. International Review of Research in Mental Re-
British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 68–74. tardation, 15, 195–202.
Gunn, P., & Berry, P. (1987). Some financial costs Orr, R. R., Cameron, S. J., Dobson, L. A., & Day,
of caring for children with Down syndrome D. M. (1993). Age-related changes in stress ex-
at home. Australia and New Zealand Journal of perienced by families with a child who has
Developmental Disabilities, 13, 187–193. developmental delays. Mental Retardation, 31,
Hawkins, N. E., Singer, G. H. S., & Nixon, C. D. 171–176.
(1993). Short-term behavioral counseling for Pearson, D. A., Lachar, D., Loveland, K. A., San-
families of persons with disabilities. In G. H. tos, C. W., Faria, L. P., Azzam, P. N., Hent-

q American Association on Mental Retardation 443


VOLUME 107, NUMBER 6: 433–444 z NOVEMBER 2002 AMERICAN JOURNAL ON MENTAL RETARDATION

Behavior problems and parenting stress B. L. Baker et al.

ges, B. A., & Cleveland, L. A. (2000). Patterns study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-
of behavioral adjustment and maladjustment rology, 42, 266–270.
in mental retardation: Comparison of chil- Turnbull, A. P., Patterson, J. M., Behr, S. K., Mur-
dren with and without ADHD. American Jour- phy, D. L., Marquis, J. G., & Blue-Banning,
nal on Mental Retardation, 105, 236–251. M. J. (Eds.) (1993). Cognitive coping, families,
Pfeiffer, S. I., & Baker, B. L. (1994). Residential and disability. Baltimore: Brookes.
treatment for children with dual diagnosis of Received 5/7/01, accepted 8/14/01.
mental retardation and mental disorder. In J.
Blacher (Ed.), When there’s no place like home: This article is based on activities of the Collab-
Options for children living apart from their natu- orative Family Study, supported by National In-
ral families (pp. 273–298). Baltimore: Brookes. stitute of Child Health and Human Develop-
Phares, V. (1996). Fathers and developmental psycho- ment Grant 34879-1459 (K. A. Crnic, principal
pathology. New York: Wiley. investigator, B. L. Baker, J. Blacher, & C. Edel-
Reiss, S., Levitan, G. W., & McNally, R. J. (1982). brock, co-principal investigators). The Collabo-
Emotionally disturbed mentally retarded peo- rative Family Study is conducted at three sites:
ple: An underserved population. American The Pennsylvania State University, University of
Psychologist, 37, 361–367. California, Los Angeles, and University of Cal-
ifornia, Riverside. We are indebted to our staff
Richman, N., Stevenson, J., & Graham, P. J.
members at these settings and very much ap-
(1982). Preschool to school: A behavioural study.
preciate the assistance of Carolyn Christensen,
London: Academic Press. Kelly Dulan, Catherine Gaze, Juan Gomez, Cas-
Steffenburg, S., Gillberg, C., & Steffenburg, U. ey Hoffman, Christine Low, Christopher Macau-
(1996). Psychiatric disorders in children and ley, Laura Lee McIntyre, Sandra Minassian, Mi-
adolescents with mental retardation and ac- chael Rothman, and Araksia Trmrian. We es-
tive epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 53, 904– pecially acknowledge Lesley Keeler’s early con-
912. tributions to this paper. Requests for reprints
Stromme, P., & Diseth, T. H. (2000). Prevalence should be sent to Bruce L. Baker, Department
of psychiatric diagnoses in children with men- of Psychology, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA 90095.
tal retardation: Data from a population-based E-mail: Baker@psych.ucla.edu

444 q American Association on Mental Retardation

View publication stats

You might also like