You are on page 1of 9

Evolutionary Anthropology 22:251–258 (2013)

ARTICLE

Cooperation and Conflict Between Women


in the Family
RUTH MACE

Here I review recent research on reproductive conflict between females in organization that guide the transmis-
families and how it influences their reproductive behaviour. Kin selection can sion and inheritance of wealth and
favor cooperation between parent and offspring, siblings, or unrelated co- power, as well as marriage and
residents who share interests in other family members such as grand-offspring. residence rules. These have been the
However, these are also the individuals most likely to be sharing resources, and traditional preoccupations of an-
so conflict can also emerge. While substantial interest has arisen in evolutionary thropologists since the discipline
anthropology, especially over the last two decades, in the possibility of coopera- emerged.
tive breeding in humans, less attention has been paid to reproductive conflict In another branch of our field,
among female kin. Communal breeding in animals is generally understood as human parenting and alloparenting
emerging from competition over the resources needed to breed. Competition for has been attracting interest among
household resources is a problem that also faces human families. Models sug- evolutionary anthropologists for
gest that in some circumstances, inclusive fitness can be maximized by sharing some time.4,5 Over the last few deca-
reproduction rather than harming relatives by fighting with them, even if the des, it has become apparent that the
shares that emerge are not equal. Thus, competition and cooperation turn out life histories of human females have
to be strongly related to each other. Reproductive competition within and features that are rather different
between families may have underpinned the biological evolution of fertility pat- from those of other apes, including
terns (such as menopause) and the cultural evolution of marriage, residence, long childhoods, rapid reproductive
and inheritance norms (such as late male marriage or primogeniture), which rates after puberty, and then female
can enhance cooperation and minimize the observed incidence of such menopause, followed by a long post-
conflicts. reproductive life. Explanations for
these patterns are varied, but most
are related to the cooperative and
“A time to love and a time to competitive relationships between
hate.” Ecclesiastes 3:1-8 kin.6–8 As the evolution of human
kinship and human life histories are
my two favorite subjects, I am
A central relationship in virtually thrilled that these two large branches
all human social systems is that of anthropology, which often are
Ruth Mace is Professor of Evolutionary
Anthropology at UCL. Her research has between husband and wife. But there considered somewhat separately, are
spanned a variety of areas relevant to
are relatively few social systems in now being more formally linked in
evolution of the human family, life history,
and kinship systems, as well as the evo- which a pair-bonded couple live recent literature. The issue of disper-
lution of cooperation both inside and independently of other kin.1 Hunter- sal may be key to both, for reasons
outside the family. Much of her work has that I will outline.
been done in African populations, but gatherer groups appear to be rela-
she has also worked on data from the tively fluid associations of groups of Sarah Hrdy has worked on both
United Kingdom and is now also working
nuclear families that can be based cooperation and conflict, which makes
on populations in China in collaboration
with the Chinese Academy of Sciences on matrilateral or patrilateral kin- this special issue an ideal format to
in Beijing. In 2008, she was elected a ship, friendship, or convenience.2,3 review how these two topics are related
Fellow of the British Academy. to each other. Her interest in sexual
After the origin of agriculture, our
economies began to rely on farmland conflict dates back to her pioneering
Key words: human behavioural ecology; matriliny; and other heritable resources. That work on infanticide in langurs.9 This
reproductive skew; cultural norms; kinship
made it more difficult for families to work was followed by a sequence of
move freely. More formal rules of path-breaking books on the evolution of
C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V ownership and transmission of human females. In Mother Nature,
DOI: 10.1002/evan.21374 wealth were generated, giving rise to she emphasized the effects of
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com). cultural norms of kinship and social sexual conflict on the evolution of
252 Mace ARTICLE

motherhood in humans.4 I shall not reach sexual maturity, when they the mother underpins short human
delve into infanticide or sexual conflict usually marry, so that only couples interbirth intervals, so that humans
here; as fascinating as these topics are, and their children co-reside. Larger are now often referred to as coopera-
they have been quite widely discussed groups of kin form when some indi- tive breeders.10,19 Grandmothers are
elsewhere. Later, Hrdy’s Mothers and viduals do not disperse. This may sometimes thought to play an essen-
Others helped galvanize interest in occur if there are benefits of staying tial role in provisioning younger
human cooperative breeding.10 What or costs of leaving. The prospect of women and their offspring.17,19
are humans, social cooperators or inheriting a good territory, such as a There is considerable debate about
fierce competitors? We know, of farm, would be an example of the the extent to which human meno-
course, that they are both, but what I benefits of staying. There may be pause can be understood as an adap-
shall outline is how competition itself high costs of leaving if habitats are tation to grandmothering or whether
can be the driver of cooperation. I want saturated and new territories are it emerged due to some kind of phys-
hard to come by, and this may lead iological by-product or constraint.
to focus here on the slightly less-
to delays in dispersal. Important cor- Can infertile grandmothers be con-
trodden path of cooperation and con-
relates of cooperative breeding in sidered on a par with other non-
flict between adult females, not so
animals include long-life span and breeding helpers in a communal or
much over mates,11 but over the
saturated habitats that make disper- cooperatively breeding species?
resources needed to breed.
Implicit in the ‘grandmother hypoth-
Zoologists interested in cooperative esis’ for the evolution of menopause
breeding in animals have used inclu- is the notion that it would not be
sive fitness theory to understand why What are humans, social possible for a grandmother to raise
individuals might stay in groups and her own offspring at the same time
who might win and lose in the compe- cooperators or fierce
as helping her daughter (or son) to
tition for local resources. They also competitors? We know, reproduce; thus, she has to choose
have argued that similar approaches
apply to human families.12 While
of course, that they are between mothering and grandmo-
thering. There is now good evidence
cooperative breeding usually involves both, but what I shall that having a grandmother is associ-
winners and losers in terms of direct
fitness, it may involve a win-win sit-
outline is how competi- ated with enhanced survival and fit-
ness benefits to grandchildren,20–22
uation in terms of inclusive fitness if tion itself can be the but whether it is enough to counter-
individuals can ‘agree’ on a strategy driver of cooperation. balance the loss of late life reproduc-
that, given the constraints, maximizes tion is more controversial.
inclusive fitness for both parties. This
The failure of some to be con-
theoretical framework has been use- vinced by adaptive hypotheses led
ful in explaining reproductive sharing many to place more emphasis on
in communally breeding mammals, sal difficult.14,15 It is likely that coop- menopause as a physiological con-
birds, and insects; these models can erative living and breeding emerge in straint. It was argued that if the
predict when dominants may prefer human groups for similar reasons. length of the fertile period is consid-
to allow kin to breed in the group, at Because humans, in contrast to ered to be constrained to that of a
least a bit, rather than have them the other great apes, are pair-bonded chimpanzee, then only the extension
leave, and when subordinates might and share meat, early socio- of female life span beyond 50 years
choose to stay. Hence, the models ecological models of human families of age is what has been selected for.
help predict the extent of reproduc- placed strong emphasis on “man the It is relatively clear that life spans
tive skew (when some individuals’ hunter,” who helped to support will extend if older females can be
direct fitness is higher than others).13 females and their offspring. While in useful as carers for their kin.23 How-
Here, I describe developments in two some cases males seem to support a ever, a model based on such a con-
of my own areas of interest that are great deal of a woman’s caloric straint is not particularly satisfying
helped by considering cooperation intake,16 in others there are sugges- to those of us that like to believe that
between women in human families in tions that males are not as helpful to evolution is strong and that life-
such terms: human menopause and women and children’s nutrition as history variables like the timing of
human kinship organization. I will might be expected.17 In some cases, reproduction should be relatively
argue that both can be understood as the presence of a male seems to easy to select for. It is also extremely
biological or cultural adaptations to increase rather than decrease wom- hard, if not impossible, to test a
reduce conflict between female kin. en’s workloads.18 An alternative model based on an unspecified con-
model, in which female kin help straint, especially as elephants do
FAMILIES AS COOPERATIVE each other to support the rapid seem to be able to breed after the
reproductive rate of adult females, age of 50.24 If rapidly increasing age-
UNITS specific maternal mortality is
has become very popular, if not pre-
Families are nuclear families if dominant, in recent years.6 It is clear included, along with mother and
individuals disperse as soon as they that food supplied by others besides grandmother effects on both
ARTICLE Cooperation and Conflict Between Women in the Family 253

grandchild survival and daughters’ death occurred.19 Still further, it is They assume that there is likely to
fertility, then models predict little or possible that genetic conflict gener- be reproductive conflict between co-
no fitness advantage to reproducing ated by X-chromosome inheritance residents over resources needed for
after about the age of 50 years.25,26 causes grandmothers to discriminate reproduction. Cant and Johnstone
But accelerating maternal mortality against their sons’ sons.33 All these draw on tug-of-war models of repro-
could itself be considered a ‘physio- factors suggest that matrilineal kin- ductive conflict between kin, in
logical constraint’, possibly emerging ship organization may lead to more which players have partial control
after the evolution of menopause or grandparental solicitude. over the outcome.13 When females
for the same reasons that menopause Unrelated females in the patrilin- customarily disperse, as is probably
evolved, and not the cause of meno- eal Dogon of Mali seems to be in the case in most human groups, the
pause. Still others argue that the out-and-out conflict both across and average relatedness of a female to
female life span is a ‘spandrel’, within generations.29 If the evidence others in her group varies over time.
selected for by selection on male life of cooperative breeding seems This is because daughters disperse
span because males do have oppor- mainly to concern matrilineal kin, and are replaced by unrelated
tunities to reproduce in old age.27 this raises the question of whether females who migrate into the group
Such models are made less persua- ancestral humans had enough con- to marry the sons of the patriline;
sive by the empirical observation grandmothers and daughters-in-law
that females tend to outlive males in are then asymmetrically related to
most human populations. the children that result. The paternal
All these models are compatible There is now good grandmother gains inclusive fitness
with the observation that post- from helping her younger daughter-
reproductive grandmothers are gen-
evidence that having a in-law reproduce because by doing
erally helpful toward their children, grandmother is so she is helping her son to repro-
or at least to their daughters and duce. The reverse is not true. The
grandchildren.20 But whether that
associated with younger woman is related to no one
help drives the evolution of meno- enhanced survival and other than her own offspring, so she
pause or is just occurring to make fitness benefits to gains no inclusive fitness from help-
the best of a bad job is still disputed. ing her mother-in-law (or anyone
Grannies, and sometimes granddads, grandchildren,20–22 but else in the household) to reproduce.
had better make themselves useful if whether it is enough to This asymmetry means that the older
they expect to be given much food or woman has more to lose from a fight
protection from the other members counterbalance the loss than her daughter-in-law does, and
of their community. There are vari- of late life reproduction is destined to lose any reproductive
ous anthropological examples of ger- conflict. After a certain age, the evo-
onticide among hunter-gatherers.28 is more controversial. lutionarily stable strategy (ESS) is
Also, relegation to rather precarious for her to forego reproduction and
and poverty-stricken life styles is a help raise her grandchildren, from
severe risk for old ladies29 or widows whom she gains at least some inclu-
in any culture. sive fitness. This model therefore
suggests that menopause is an adap-
CONFLICT BETWEEN KIN IN THE tact with female kin for cooperative tation to avoid conflict with kin by
breeding to have generated meno- ceasing reproduction and eliminat-
EVOLUTION OF MENOPAUSE
pause. Residence patterns become ing reproductive overlap.37 Here,
The many empirical observations key. There is not much evidence that competition is, almost literally, the
of grandmother effects indicate that ancestral hunter-gatherers were mother of cooperation. If this ‘repro-
maternal grandmothers are almost likely to have lived predominantly ductive conflict’ version of the grand-
always beneficial. In contrast, pater- matrilocally. But then the term make mother hypothesis is correct, the fact
nal grandmothers often are not par- little sense in many more mobile that humans are more likely to have
ticularly helpful, and are sometimes groups with variable residence pat- been characterized by female disper-
even harmful to their grandchildren’s terns.34 Moreover, there are exam- sal than by male dispersal is a bonus
survival,20,30,31 even if they do pro- ples of grandmothers providing rather than a hindrance to adaptive
mote their daughter-in-laws’ fertil- significant help to their daughters explanations for menopause.
ity.32 One possible reason for this is even if they are not permanently co- Older females also increase in
paternity uncertainty; another is that resident with them.18,35,36 average relatedness to the group
the costs of maternal mortality are Cant and Johnstone developed a when neither males nor females dis-
much higher for matrilineal kin, model to evaluate how dispersal pat- perse, but mate outside the group;
whose daughter is irreplaceable, terns influence the evolution of help- this is the case for many cetaceans,
than to the patriline, whose ful and harmful behavior toward which also show menopause.38 In
daughter-in-law could at least par- female kin and the effect of this on the case of whales, which mate out-
tially be replaced by remarriage if the evolution of menopause.37,38 side the group, there is some
254 Mace ARTICLE

Box 1. The Case of Communal Living in Matrilineal Group Households in China

A few matrilineal peasant soci- grandmother, the most important times older men build rooms for
eties resemble the social systems of person in the household. Her room religious worship.
cetaceans, in which mating occurs contains the central fireplace, where Farming and domestic work is all
outside the residential group. In visitors are received; this room is communal, with household mem-
these rare duolocal systems, neither also the bedroom for the grand- bers sharing food, child-care
sex disperses. Generations of broth- mother and her matrilineal grand- responsibilities, and all household
ers and sisters and their matrilineal children. (The offspring of resident resources, and the farm is inherited
offspring co-reside throughout life. males reside elsewhere with their communally by all the matrilineal
One of these societies is the Mosuo mothers.) Adult daughters have family members. Women do most
of southwestern China, also known their own smaller rooms, usually of the domestic and agricultural
as the Na,40 who live around the around the upper level of the court- work.40 There are historical
shores of Lugu Lake. yard, where their male partners visit accounts of mothers having chil-
The social system is built into them at night in what are some- dren from several different
their homes, which are built around times described as ‘walking’ or ‘visit- fathers,55,56 but Mosuo currently
a courtyard. The most important ing’ marriages. Unmarried teenage report that they are mostly
room in the house belongs to the boys usually share a room. Some- monogamous.40
ARTICLE Cooperation and Conflict Between Women in the Family 255

Income from tourism is changing is the communal nature of the house- China, but they tended to dissolve
both the residence and marriage pat- holds that favors a male’s investment with the death of the grandfather,
terns in favor of more nuclear fami- in his natal household rather than since multiple couples within one
lies.53 Both males and females that that of his wife. This is because house led to too much conflict
form neolocal households start to investment in his wife’s communal once the his control was no longer
breed earlier than those that remain household is diluted by sharing there.1 Matrilineal Mosuo house-
within the communal duolocal resources with his wife’s kin, to holds are more stable.
households, indicating the communal whom he is not related.40
living is associated with reproductive Historically, patrilineal group
constraints.40 We have argued that it households were quite common in

indication that older females are par- begin to reproduce. 41–43 In such sys- and marriage practices, combined
ticularly helpful to sons, who, since tems, a delay in the onset of repro- with female dispersal, almost com-
their offspring are elsewhere, are not duction means that reproductive pletely eliminate conflict arising from
in competition with other family overlap between a man’s mother and multiple females breeding on the
members for breeding opportunities. his wife is almost zero. same farm. Whether such costs would
A recent study showed that male My data on over 500 women from apply in a hunter-gatherer context,
orcas tended to experience high mor- communities in rural Gambia, which where there usually is open access to
tality when their mother died,39 so habitually show patrilocal residence, resources and residence patterns are
maybe the rest of the group tolerate illustrate the very different effects more flexible, is unknown. Conse-
their brother until mum dies, then that reproductive overlap has on the quently, it is still an open question
gang up and start taking bites out of reproductive success of daughters whether such conflict led to the evolu-
him. Zoologists have learned to and sons. Only very fertile mothers, tion of menopause.
expect nothing less from ‘coopera- who gave birth young and continued Nevertheless, cultural evolution has
tive’ breeders. Often the more ‘coop- to reproduce late in life, had any generated a wide range of human
erative’ the breeding system, the reproductive overlap with their social systems in far less time than is
more extreme the reproductive skew daughters. When this did occur, the required for the biological evolution
and the more potential for violence daughter did not suffer much cost, of a trait like menopause. The avoid-
to enforce the peace or establish who presumably because female dispersal ance of reproductive conflict is likely
inherits a vacant breeding slot, with means the daughters’ children would to have had a profound effect on the
social insects probably providing the be raised in another household. The cultural evolution of marriage and
most extreme scenarios. There are mother did slow her reproduction kinship systems. Those violating cul-
probably no human social systems upon the arrival of a matrilineal tural norms generally do less well
exactly like that of orcas, but some grandchild, consistent with the than those who follow the locally
matrilineal ‘duolocal’ groups, in notion that she is genuinely helping accepted practice,45 which makes it
which brothers and sisters co-reside her daughter.36 Mother/son repro- complicated to work out whether
throughout life, provide a rare case ductive overlap was very rare. There costs helped generate the norms or
of males breeding outside the resi- were very few cases in which moth- simply emerged as a consequence of
dential group40 (Box 1). ers became paternal grandmothers punishment for violating a norm that
under the age of 50 years, and thus arose for other reasons. Most evolu-
RESOLVING REPRODUCTIVE showed the potential for reproduc- tionary anthropology suffers from
tive overlap with daughters-in-law.36 such complications; although this is
CONFLICT THROUGH KINSHIP
Although we did not have enough annoying, it also is part of what
NORMS cases of mother/son reproductive makes it so interesting.
In humans, reproductive conflict overlap in the Gambia to estimate its Patrilineal kinship is usually asso-
among kin can be avoided not just effects, relevant evidence comes ciated with a subsistence strategy
by menopause, but also by cultural from Finland. In historical Finns, that includes resources that can be
rules.36 I suspect that cultural tradi- only 36 out of 556 women in the monopolized by males and also are
tions such as marriage and kinship sample showed such overlap, but often used to encourage women to
norms may function to reduce repro- when this did occur it had a pro- marry polygynously.46 Hence, men
ductive conflict. In patrilocal resi- nounced negative effect.44 Again, compete, even against their own
dence systems, which are often also among the Finns, mother/daughter brothers, for resources and marriage
polygynous, brothers, and thus their reproductive overlap was more com- partners when fathers control
wives, are in competition for resour- mon and not nearly as costly, if resources. The parents of daughters
ces owned by or inherited from their costly at all.44 can exploit this to demand bride
father, and this may significantly Cultural norms of parental control price. Pastoralist systems typically
affect when sons can marry and over the transmission of resources show all these characteristics:
256 Mace ARTICLE

Livestock are like walking money Chewa in Mali suffer higher mortal- ultimate in reproductive sharing!
and not nearly as hard work to ity when they have more female Younger brothers did badly in terms
maintain as, for example, extensive kin.50 Both findings are suggestive of of direct fitness, but perhaps bene-
crop farming systems. Females in reproductive constraints and compe- fited more from ceding some pater-
patrilineal systems forego the bene- tition among female kin. The fact nity to their elder brother rather
fits of proximity to their female kin that females may be doing worse, in than fighting with the him for the
for the advantages of access to male- terms of direct fitness, when breed- farm or more access to their shared
owned resources. The price could be ing communally should not necessar- wife. When resource constraints
severe female-female conflict, as ily be taken as evidence that they are were lifted by the emergence of jobs
described for the aforementioned not helping each other or benefiting outside farming, younger brothers
Dogon in Mali, who do not seem to from doing so, but rather as evi- quickly left to set up a nuclear fam-
be well-described as ‘cooperative dence that they are experiencing a ily.54 Thus, both the matrilineal
breeders.’29 The evolution of this kin- harsh environment in which they are Mosuo and the polyandrous Tibetans
ship system seems to represent a forced to share the means of repro- show how communally breeding
case of males, who benefit from duction. As with all the systems households can emerge out of lim-
polygyny at the expense of their described, the shares may not be ited opportunities to disperse and
wives’ fitness, winning the conflict of equal; some group members may become unstable when there is relax-
interest with females. Why power have to settle for a reduced share of ation of the ecological constraints.
asymmetries in favor of males are so the resources available for reproduc- The likelihood of helping or harm-
strong in this system and allow such tion. Comparing the reproductive ing siblings may not be set for life;
a male-dominated system to be an success of patrilineal Gambian at times they may harm each other
ESS is more of a puzzle. women living in group compounds. and at other times they may help
A lack of monopolizable resources which normally comprised a father, each other. It is clear that very
is more likely to be associated with brothers, and their families, we young siblings nearly always com-
matrilineal kinship47,48 or with no found no overall benefit to residing a pete for maternal investment, which
particular lineality at all. Matrilineal in a larger compound. But being in is why high birth rates are almost
kinship is almost always associated larger groups did favor older universally associated with higher
with matrilocal residence and would, breeding-age women over younger child mortality.57 But as children get
on the face of it, seem, of all human breeding-age women, suggesting that older they are capable of helping
kinship systems, to provide the best the former gain some help from that their younger siblings and their
opportunity for cooperative or com- latter, perhaps because of their mother. In many societies, elder sis-
munal breeding. When matrilineal higher status within their house- ters in particular provide care for
Khasi are directly compared with holds.36 It is likely that, given the younger siblings.58 However, adult
patrilineal Bengalis who live nearby constraints in the system, energy is siblings may no longer be of assis-
in India, child health appears better better spent cooperating to maximize tance once they have their own off-
in the matrilineal group.45 But co- inclusive fitness of all co-resident spring to attend to. Also, as these
residence does, of course, produce brothers (among the Gambia) or sis- adult siblings’ offspring become their
potential competition between ters (among the Mosuo) rather priority, they may find themselves in
female kin for resources, so matrilo- than fighting relatives over the competition with their siblings. This
cal females may have to forego high resources that they all need to age-dependent change in helping sib-
reproductive rates in the interest of reproduce.13,51,52 lings has been observed, for example,
maximizing inclusive fitness. Among the matrilineal commu- in patrilineal societies in The Gam-
Zoologists make a distinction nally breeding Mosuo, the emergence bia and Senegal.30,60 In societies in
between cooperative breeding sys- of tourism enabled some individuals which inherited wealth matters,
tems, in which nonreproductives to raise extra money independent of there is evidence that fertility is
help kin reproduce, and communal the farm through activities such as reduced when there is competition
breeding systems, in which all work at tourist hotels. These individ- for resources among siblings of
females breed and offspring are com- uals often left their communal reproductive age.59
munally raised. Few human systems households and established neolocal
follow the communal breeding pat- nuclear families.53 Another example
CONCLUSIONS
tern, but duolocal matrilineal groups of communal breeding by brothers
(from which neither sex disperses) comes from polyandrous Sherpa While the anthropological litera-
come close. households in the Himalayas. Here, ture has tended to treat conflict and
We found that females living in farming habitat is limited, restricted cooperation as different subjects,
duolocal communal households to a few river valleys; there are no they are actually related to each
among the matrilineal Mosuo in opportunities to disperse and set up other. The semantic distinction can
China, breed more slowly than those neolocal farms. Competition between even depend to some extent on
living neolocally.40 Also, this group is brothers was intense, and apparently whether one takes a genes’-eye view
not associated with particularly high could be resolved only by all broth- or observes the phenotype (that is,
fertility49 (Box 1). The matrilineal ers marrying the same wife – the behavior).
ARTICLE Cooperation and Conflict Between Women in the Family 257

Cooperatively breeding kin are others at the Theoretical Ecology 20 Sear R, Mace R. 2008. Who keeps children
alive? A review of the effects of kin on child
potentially in reproductive competi- Group of the Chinese Academy of Sci- survival. Evol Hum Behav 29:1–18.
tion in the sense that individuals ences, Beijing, and at HEEG at UCL 21 Sear R, Mace R, McGregor IA. 2000. Mater-
would like to use the same resources for helpful discussion of these issues. nal grandmothers improve the nutritional sta-
Ruth Mace is funded by the ERC. tus and survival of children in rural Gambia.
to reproduce. But potential conflict Proc R Soc Lond B 267:461–467.
can be resolved even if the partners 22 Lahdenpera M, Lummaa V, Helle S, et al.
do not emerge with equal shares of 2004. Fitness benefits of prolonged post-
REFERENCES reproductive lifespan in women. Nature 428:
the cake. Depending on the chances
178–181.
of gaining more control of a resource 1 Harrell S. 1997. Human families. Boulder:
23 Kim PS, Coxworth JE, Hawkes K. 2012.
Westview Press.
and the relatedness of the partners in Increased longevity evolves from grandmother-
2 Hill K, Walker R, Bozičević M, et al. 2011. ing. Proc R Soc Lond B 279:4880–4884.
conflict, individuals may benefit more Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer soci-
by helping their relatives breed than 24 Moss CJ. 2001. The demography of an Afri-
eties show unique human social structure. Sci-
can elephant (Loxodonta africana) population
by fighting with each other for the ence 331:1286–1289.
in Amboseli, Kenya. J Zool 255:145–156.
resources to reproduce. But, almost 3 Apicella CL, Marlowe F, Fowler J, et al. 2012. 25 Shanley DP, Sear R, Mace R, et al. 2007.
Social networks and cooperation in hunter- Testing evolutionary theories of menopause.
by definition, that help will involve a gatherers. Nature 481:497–501. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:2943–2949.
cost in terms of their direct fitness. 4 Hrdy SB. 2000. Mother nature: maternal 26 Pavard S, Metcalf CJE, Heyer E. 2008.
Those individuals who do not breed instincts and the shaping of the species. Lon- Senescence of reproduction may explain adapt-
or do so at a slower rate than others don: Vintage. ive menopause in humans: a test of the
5 Bentley GR, Mace R. 2009. Substitute “mother” hypothesis. Am J Phys Anthropol 136:
may do so because other females are parents: biological and social perspectives on 194–203.
more powerful and punish them, they alloparenting in human societies. New York: 27 Tuljapurkar SD, Puleston CO, Gurven MD.
are less efficient at gaining access to Berghahn Books. 2007. Why men matter: mating patterns drive
resources, or there are other asymme- 6 Hawkes K, O’Connell JF, Blurton Jones NG, evolution of human lifespan. Plos One 2:e785.
et al. 1998. Grandmothering, menopause and 28 Hill K, Hurtado AM. 1996. Ache life history:
tries in the benefits of helping. If the the evolution of human life histories. Proc Natl the ecology and demography of a foraging peo-
evolutionarily stable strategy is to Acad Sci USA 95:1336–1339. ple. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
help co-resident family members 7 Hill K. 1993. Life-history theory and evolu- 29 Strassmann BI. 2011. Cooperation and com-
maximize joint inclusive fitness tionary anthropology. Evol Anthropol 2:78–88. petition in a cliff-dwelling people. Proc Natl
8 Mace R. 2000. The evolutionary ecology of Acad Sci USA 108:10894–10901.
rather than waste energy on family
human life history. Anim Behav 59:1–10. 30 Sear R, Steele F, McGregor IA, et al. 2002.
conflict that would reduce the repro- 9 Hrdy SB. 1990. The langurs of Abu: female The effects of kin on child mortality in rural
ductive success of both partners, and male strategies of reproduction. Cam- Gambia. Demography 39:43–63.
overt aggression may not occur. bridge: Harvard University Press. 31 Leonetti DL, Nath DC, Hemam NS. 2007.
In-law conflict: women’s reproductive lives and
There may be outbreaks of conflict 10 Hrdy SB. 2009. Mothers and others: the evo-
the roles of their mothers and husbands among
lutionary origins of mutual understanding.
when the status quo is disupted, sta- Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
the matrilineal Khasi. Curr Anthropol 48:861–
tus hierarchies are re-established, or 890.
11 Campbell A. 1999. Staying alive: evolution,
32 Sear RS, Mace R, McGregor IA. 2003. The
breeding slots are reassigned. culture, and women’s intrasexual aggression.
effects of kin on female fertility in rural Gam-
Shakespeare’s oeuvre would be con- Behav Brain Sci 22:203–214.
bia. Evol Hum Behav 24:25–42.
siderably reduced if history were not 12 Emlen ST. 1995. An evolutionary theory of 33 Fox M, Sear R, Beise J, et al. 2010.
the family. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:8092– Grandma plays favourites: X-chromosome
full of examples of cultural rules 8099. relatedness and sex-specific childhood mortal-
allowing sufficient ambiguity for 13 Reeve HK, Emlen ST, Keller L. 1998. Repro- ity. Proc R Soc Lond B 277:567–573.
major conflicts to break out. But most ductive sharing in animal societies: reproduc- 34 Marlowe FW. 2005. Hunter-gatherers and
tive incentives or incomplete control by human evolution. Evol Anthropol 14:54–67.
of the time, strict cultural rules of suc- dominant breeders?. Behav Ecol. 9:267–278.
cession may help obviate the need to 35 Gibson MA, Mace R. 2005. Helpful grand-
14 Emlen ST. 1982. The evolution of helping. mothers in rural Ethiopia: a study of the effect
fight with relatives, especially when 1. An ecological constraints model. Am Nat of kin on child survival and growth. Evol Hum
norms resolve conflict in ways that 119:29–39. Behav 26:469–482.
favor those most likely to win anyway 15 Hatchwell BJ, Komdeur J. 2000. Ecological 36 Mace R, Alvergne A. 2012. Female reproduc-
constraints, life history traits and the evolution tive competition within families in rural Gam-
(such as favoring parental choice over of cooperative breeding. Anim Behav 59:1079– bia. Proc R Soc Lond B 279:2219–2227.
children’s choices and older siblings 1086. 37 Cant MA, Johnstone RA. 2008. Reproductive
over younger ones). Thus, human 16 Hill K, Kaplan H. 1999. Life history traits in conflict and the separation of reproductive gen-
humans: theory and empirical studies. Annu erations in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
marriage, residence patterns, and Rev Anthropol 28:397–430. 105:5332–5336.
inheritance norms may evolve by 17 Hawkes K, O’Connell JF, Blurton Jones NG. 38 Johnstone RA, Cant MA. 2010. The evolu-
maximizing the inclusive fitness of 1997. Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring tion of menopause in cetaceans and humans:
parties that are potentially in conflict provisioning and the evolution of long post- the role of demography. Proc R Soc Lond B
menopausal life spans. Curr Anthropol 38:551– 277:3765–3771.
within family units, and thus serve to 578. 39 Foster EA, Franks D, Mazzi S, et al. 2012.
keep family members cooperating, at 18 Meehan CL, Quinlan R, Malcom CD. 2013. Adaptive prolonged postreproductive life span
least most of the time. Cooperative breeding and maternal energy in killer whales. Science 337:1313.
expenditure among aka foragers. Am J Hum 40 Wu JJ, He QQ, Deng LL, et al. 2013. Com-
Biol 25:42–57. munal breeding promotes a matrilineal social
19 Mace R, Sear R. 2005. Are humans coopera- system where husband and wife live apart.
tive breeders? In: Voland E, Chasiotis A, Schie- Proc R Soc Lond B 280:20130010.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS fenhoevel W, editors. Grandmotherhood: the 41 Marlowe FW, Berbesque JC. 2012. The
evolutionary significance of the second half of human operational sex ratio: effects of mar-
Thanks to two anonymous referees life. Piscataway: Rutgers University Press. p riage, concealed ovulation, and menopause on
for useful comments, to Ting Ji and 143–159. mate competition. J Hum Evol 63:834–842.
258 Mace ARTICLE

42 Mace R. 1996. Biased parental investment 49 Shih CK, Jenike MR. 2002. A cultural- 56 Yan RX, Liu XH. 2009. Investigation reports
and reproductive success in Gabbra pastoral- historical perspective on the depressed fertility on matrilineal system and economy of the Naxi
ists. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:75–81. among the matrilineal Moso in Southwest people in Wenquan County, Yongning. In:
43 Skinner EP. 1961. Intergenerational conflict China. Hum Ecol 30:21–47. Board of Yunnan Province in Editorial Com-
among the Mossi: father and son. J Confl Reso- 50 Sear R. 2008. Kin and child survival in rural mittee of Amendments, editors. Investigation
lut 5:55–60. Malawi: are matrilineal kin always beneficial in a reports on the social and matrilineal systems of
matrilineal society? Hum Nat Int Bios 19:277–293. the Naxi people in Yongning, 3rd ed. Ethnic
44 Lahdenpera M, Gillespie D, Lummaa V, Groups Press. p 1–121.
et al. 2012. Severe intergenerational reproduc- 51 Ji T, Xu J, Mace, R. n.d. Intergenerational
tive conflict and the evolution of menopause. and sibling conflict in patrilocal kinship sys- 57 Hobcraft JN, McDonald JW, Rutstein SO.
Ecol Lett 15:1283–1290. tems: a model of reproductive skew shows ben- 1985. Demographic determinants of infant and
efits of monogamy. Hum Nat. Submitted. early child mortality: a comparative analysis.
45 Leonetti DL, Nath DC. 2009. Age at first Popul Stud 39:363–385.
reproduction and economic change in the con- 52 Ji T, Wu J-J, He Q-Q, et al. 2013. Reproduc-
text of differing kinship ecologies. Am J Hum tive competition between females in the matri- 58 Kramer KL. 2011. The evolution of human
Biol 21:438–447. lineal Mosuo of southwestern China. Phil Trans parental care and recruitment of juvenile help.
R Soc B 20130081. Trends Ecol Evol 26:533–540.
46 Hartung J. 1982. Polygyny and the inheri-
tance of wealth. Curr Anthropol 23:1–12. 53 Mattison SM. 2010. Economic impacts of 59 Mace R. 1998. The co-evolution of human
tourism and erosion of the visiting system fertility and wealth inheritance strategies.
47 Aberle DF. 1961. Matrilineal descent in Philos Trans R Soc 353:389–397.
cross-cultural perspective. In: Schneider DM, among the Mosuo of Lugu Lake. Asia Pac J
Gough K, editors. Matrilineal kinship. Berkeley: Anthropol 11:159–176. 60 Nitsch A, Faurie C, Lummaa V. 2013. Are
University of California Press. p 655–727. 54 Haddix KA. 2001. Leaving your wife and elder siblings helpers or competitors? Antago-
your brothers: when polyandrous marriages fall nistic fitness effects of sibling interactions in
48 Holden CJ, Mace R. 2003. Spread of cattle humans. Proc R Soc Lond B 280:20122313.
led to the loss of matrilineal descent in Africa: apart. Evol Hum Behav 22:47–60.
a coevolutionary analysis. Proc R Soc Lond B 55 Hua C. 2001. A society without fathers or
270:2425–2433. husbands. New York: Zone Books. C 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V
Copyright of Evolutionary Anthropology is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like