You are on page 1of 1

Does our Personality Predict Music Preference

Mitch Beavers and Lauren Mutispaugh


Dr. Christopher Buchholz (Faculty Advisor)
Roanoke College

Abstract Hypotheses Results Discussion


As seen in Figure 1, there was a clear preference for complex music From our results there are several tentative conclusions we can make about
Research has shown that various aspects of our personality are related to the In this study we used Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) STOMP which measures
(McomplexMP = 4.75, SD = .81; MsimpleMP = 3.30, SD = .72). After reverse coding the relationship between personality and music preference. Participants who like
types of music that we choose. We used Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) Short an individual’s preference for musical genres; however, we also were interested in
the simple music preference items, nine of the ten original items were found to have complex music tend to be more open to experiences and have a higher need for
Test of Music Preference, which measures an individual’s preference for musical the relationship between cognitive ability and music preference. Several
high intercorrelations, therefore, we created a composite score of complex music cognition. This is to be expected, in that people who seek out new experiences and
genres. We also used Buchholz’s (2006) measures complex/simple music researchers have shown a link between cognitive ability and preference for
preference (α = .80). Demonstrating the link between cognitive ability and music new cognitive problems would be expected to also seek out music that is more
preference and happy/sad music preference. Responses on these scales were aesthetically complex or simple stimuli (Barron, 1955; Frances, 1976; Kammann,
preference, complex music preference was significantly correlated with openness to complex in nature. However, the correlation between complex music preference
correlated with personality measures. Preference for reflective/complex music 1966; as cited in Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003). Therefore, it is possible that measures
experience (r = .396, p=.001) and need for cognition (r = .389, p=.002) (see Table and sad music preference is a little more difficult to explain. It appears that those
was positively correlated with need for cognition. Preference for intense/rebellious of cognitive ability, such as need for cognition, are predictive of an individual’s
1). As hypothesized, complex music preference was positively correlated with the who prefer complex music also like their music to be sad. It is possible that music
music was negatively correlated with sad music preference and positively preference for complex or simple music. In order to assess participant’s preference
global STOMP category reflective & complex (r = .522, p<.001). As shown in Table that has a more melancholy tone may tend to be more complex in structure;
correlated with need for cognition. Energetic/rhythmic was positively correlated for complex music we used Buchholz’s (2006) measures complex/simple music
1, complex music preference was found to positively correlate with a variety of however, future research will have to investigate this more fully.
with extraversion. Finally, complex music preference was positively correlated preference. Finally, we also included Buchholz’s (2006) measure of happy/sad
specific STOMP categories (classical [r = .615, p <.001], blues [r = .358, p=.001], Participants who prefer happy music appear to be more agreeable as well as to
with need for cognition and openness to experiences and happy music music preference in order to test the relationship between preference for different
folk [r = .322, p =.003], jazz [r = .358, p =.001], rock [r = .236, p =.029], and heavy have higher self-esteem. It is unclear the direction of this relationship, but it is
preference was positively correlated with agreeableness. moods of music with personality. We predicted that we would replicate Rentfrow
metal [.234, p=.031]). Complex music preference was found to be negatively nonetheless intriguing. Also, those who like their music happy appear to like music
and Gosling’s (2003) results. Also, we predict that complex music preference will be
correlated with a variety of specific STOMP categories (country [r = -.288, p =.008], that is energetic & rhythmic and possibly upbeat and conventional as well. Both of
correlated with need for cognition. All other variables included in this study were
rap/hip-hop [r = -.369, p=.001], and pop [r = -.255, p =.018]. these categories include music that is happier in its nature, however, happy music
exploratory in nature.
As depicted in Figure 1, participants in our study showed a higher preference preference was negatively correlated with reflective and complex music. In light of
for happy music (M = 4.51, SD = .57) than their preference for sad music (M = 3.72, the relationship between complex music preference and sad music preference, this
SD = 1.04). After reverse coding the happy music preference items, all eight items also makes sense since the reflective/complex category includes music that is more
were found to have fairly high intercorrelations, therefore, we created a composite complex.
score called preference for happy music (α = .69). 1.46). As shown in Table 1, For the remainder of the results, we will discuss the implications in terms of
preference for happy music correlated with both self-esteem (r = .330, p=.009) and what type of personalities are associated with each type of music preference;
Introduction agreeableness (r = .339, p=.007). Interestingly, preference for happy music was
negatively correlated with a preference for complex music (r = -.393, p<.001),
however, most of these relationships are difficult to explain due to the limits of the
data in this study. Individuals who like more religious music appear to have higher
Previous research has shown a link between personality traits and music
suggesting that those who like happy music like simple music and those who like self-esteem and prefer happy music. Those who listen to classical music and
preference (e.g., Catell & Anderson, 1953; McCown, Keiser, Mulhearn, &
Williamson, 1997; North, Hargreaves, & O’Neill, 2000). These studies, however,
Methods sad music like more complex music. Preference for happy music was found to be soundtracks appear to less agreeable and prefer more complex music. In the case
Participants completed various measures of personality and cognitive ability. correlated with several STOMP categories (reflective and complex [r = -.312, p of classical music, these participants also appear to prefer sad music. Those who
have each used different methods of measuring music preference and according
The measures included the Big 5 factors (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swan, 2003), self- =.004], energetic and rhythmic [r = .221, p=.042], and approaching significance for like rap and hip-hop seem to be more agreeable, extraverted, and to prefer music
to Zweigenhaft (2008) music preference may vary with age and location. It is
esteem (Rosenberg, 1965), and need for cognition (Cacioppo, Petty, Feinstein, & upbeat and conventional [r = .212, p =.052]. As shown in Table 1, preference for that is happy and simple in nature. Individuals who listen to heavy metal music
possible that the links between music preference and personality may vary
Jarvis 1996). Participants also completed Renfrow and Gosling’s (2003) Short Test happy music was correlated to several specific genres as well classical (r = -.419, appear to be low in both conscientiousness and emotional stability. This result is
across samples. This study will replicate Rentfrow and Gosling’s 2003 study to
of Music Preference (STOMP). The STOMP test contains questions which indicate p<.001), blues (r = -.289, p=.007), rap/hip-hop (r = .385, p<.001), and religious (r = worth noting in that low scores on these personality measures could indicate other
examine the reliability of their findings. This study will also look at a variety of
14 genres of music: alternative, blue, classical, country, electronica/dance, folk, .252, p=.020). Finally, preference for happy music was also correlated issues in mental health. Country music lovers tend to be low in openness to
other personality and music preference measures to determine their possible
heavy metal, rap/hip-hop, jazz, pop, religious, rock, soul/funk, and soundtracks. agreeableness (r = .331, p =.002). experiences, but high in conscientiousness. They also appear to prefer music that
correlations.
Preferences were rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1-Not at all; 7- A great deal). Concerning the replication of Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) results, the is simpler in nature. As one might expect, those that prefer jazz music are more
Our music preference may be developmental. “Most people acquire their
Buchholz’s (2006) music complexity scale was used assess an individual’s desired majority of the correlations failed to replicate. The only music preference category open to experiences and prefer complex music. Similarly, those who like blues
musical taste during adolescence among friends of the same age, and they carry
level of music complexity. This scale is a 9 item Likert-type scale that included that correlated with any personality traits was energetic/rhythmic. This category was music prefer music that is more complex and sad. Finally, those who prefer folk,
early preferences right through the grave” (Sloboda, 1985, p263). There is also a
questions such as “I prefer music that is more complex in nature”, “Most of the positively correlated to extraversion (r = .263, p = .039). This does, however, rock, and heavy metal tend to prefer more complex music and those who prefer
suggestion that we imprint preferences for music between the ages of ten and
music I listen to has many different instruments and parts”, and “I prefer music that replicate at least one of their findings. If we break down the categories to specific pop, as expected, tend to prefer more simple music
twelve. While these preferences are not set in stone, there does appear to be a
is rhythmic and uncomplicated”. Finally, Buchholz’s (2006) happy-sad music genres, we do find some interesting correlations (see Table 1). Extraverts appear to
stability of preference throughout life (Jourdain, 1997).
preference scale. This scale is a 8 item Likert-type scale that included happy like rap and hip-hop music (r = .282, p = .009). Interestingly, participants high in
There is some indication that these preferences may reflect underlying
statements such as “I prefer music that is upbeat and happy” and “I prefer music agreeableness appear to like rap/hip-hop (r = .297, p = .006) but dislike both
personality traits (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003). One of the earliest attempts to
that is light and uplifting”, while sad-relevant items included statements such as “I classical (r = -.228, p = .036) and soundtracks (r = -.228, p = .036). Conscientious
map music preference onto personality was made by Cattell and Anderson
find that I prefer to listen to sad music even when I am in a good mood” and “I like participants like country (r = .258, p = .017) and dislike heavy metal (r = -.245, p =
(1953). They correlated the IPAT music preference test with Cattell’s 16
music that has a darker feel to it”. .024). Those that are more neurotic appear to like heavy metal (r = .260, p = .016).
Personality Factor Questionnaire. They found that several of the personality
Participants who were more open to experiences seem to like jazz music (r = .237,
factors correlated with music preference. However, because they were mainly
p = .029) but dislike country (r = -.245, p = .024). Finally, those with higher self-
interested in using the IPAT to examine the dimensions of the 16 PFQ, their
esteem appear to show a preference for religious music (r = .307, p = .004).
publication does not provide the specific music preference categories.
There have been several attempts over the years since Cattell and References References
Anderson to examine the relationship between personality and music preference. Buchholz,
Buchholz, C. T., (2006).
C. T., (2006). MusicalMusical complexity,
complexity, mood, andmood, and personality.
personality.
McCown, Keiser, Mulhearn, and Williamson (1997), for example, found a Unpublished
Unpublished manuscript.
manuscript.
connection between extraversion and a musical preference for songs with
exaggerated bass. North, Hargreaves, and O’Neill (2000) indicate that there is
Figure 1 Cattell,
Cattell, R.B. &R.B. & Anderson
Anderson
Personality.
Personality.
J. C. (1953a).
J. C. (1953a).
Champaign,Champaign,
TheMusic
The I.P.A.T. I.P.A.T.
IL: Institute
IL: Institute
Music Preference
Preference
for Personality
for Personality
Test of
and Abilityand Ability
Test of

a link between music preference and ones social identity, in addition to one’s Testing.Testing.
self-views and self-esteem. Gosling,
Gosling, S.D., Rentfrow,
S.D., Rentfrow, P.J., & P.J.,
Swann,& Swann,
W.B., Jr.W.B.,
(2003).Jr.A(2003). A very
very brief briefofmeasure
measure
More recently, Rentfrow and Gosling developed the Short Test of Music of the personality
the Big-Five Big-Five personality domains.
domains. Journal JournalinofPersonality,
of Research Research in
Preferences (STOMP) in 2003 that asked respondents to rate 14 different music Correlation Table Personality, 7, 504–528.
37, 504–528.
categories. These 14 categories clustered into 4 groups after a factor analysis. Jourdain
Jourdain R. (1997).
R. (1997). Music,Music, theand
the brain, brain, andHow
ecstasy: ecstasy:
musicHow music
captures our captures our
They called these groups reflective and complex, upbeat and conventional, imagination.
imagination. New York:NewWilliam
York: William
MorrowMorrow
& Co. & Co.
energetic and rhythmic, and intense and rebellious. Reflective/complex includes Extraversion -- -- -- -- -- -- .28** -- -- -- --
McCown,
McCown, W., Keiser,
W., Keiser, R., Mulhearn,
R., Mulhearn, S., & Williamson,
S., & Williamson, D. (1997). D. The
(1997). The role of
role of
a preference for classical, blues, folk and jazz. Upbeat/conventional includes Agreeableness -- -- -- -.23* -- -- .30** -.23* -- -- --
personality
personality and in
and gender gender in preferences
preferences for exaggeratedfor exaggerated
bass in bass in
country, religious, and soundtracks/themes. Energetic/rhythmic includes Conscientiousness -- -- -- -- .26* -.25* -- -- -- -- -- music.music. Personality
Personality and Individual
and Individual Differences,Differences,
23, 543-547.23, 543-547.
dance/electronica, rap/hip-hop, and soul/funk. Finally, intense/rebellious includes Emotional North,
North, A.C.,A.C., Hargreaves,
Hargreaves, D.J., &D.J., & O’neill,
O’neill, S.A. The
S.A. (2000). (2000). The importance
importance of music toof music
alternative, rock, and heavy metal music. Stability -- -- -- -- -- -.26* -- -- -- -- -- to adolescents.
adolescents. British
British Journal Journal ofPsychology,
of Educational Educational 70,Psychology,
255- 70, 255-
Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found that these categories correlated with Openness -- -- -- -- -.25 -- -- -- -- .24* -- 272. 272.
several personality factors. Extraversion correlated with the upbeat and Need for Cognition -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- P.J., &P.J.,
Rentfrow,
Rentfrow, & Gosling,
Gosling, S.D. The
S.D. (2003). (2003). The
do re doofreeveryday
mi’s mi’s of life:
everyday
The life: The
conventional category, as well as with the energetic and rhythmic category. Self-Esteem -- -- -- -- -- -- -- .31* -- -- -- structure
structure and personality
and personality correlatescorrelates of music preferences.
of music preferences. Journal Journal
Agreeableness was only correlated with the upbeat and conventional category. of Personality
of Personality and
and Social Social Psychology,
Psychology, 84(6), 1236-1256.
84(6), 1236-1256.
Complex MP .24* .36** .32** .62** -.29** .23* -.37** -- -- .36** -.26*
Conscientiousness was found to correlate with the upbeat and conventional Rosenberg,
Rosenberg, M. (1965).
M. (1965). SocietySociety and the Adolescent
and the Adolescent Self-Image. Self-Image.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton, NJ:
Happy MP -- -.29** -- -.42** -- -- .39** .25* -- -- --
category as well. Openness to experience correlated with reflective and complex, Princeton
Princeton University
University Press. Press.
intense and rebellious, and upbeat and conventional categories. None of the Sloboda,
Sloboda, J.A. (1985).
J.A. (1985). The musical
The musical mind: The mind: Thepsychology
cognitive cognitive of psychology
music. of music.

Rap Hip-Hop
Heavy Metal

Soundtracks
Religious
Classical

music preference categories correlated with emotional stability. Finally, self-


Country

New Oxford
New York: York: Oxford University
University Press. Press.
Blues
Rock

Folk

Jazz

Pop
esteem was found to have low, but significant correlations with the upbeat and Zweigenhaft,
Zweigenhaft, R. (2008).
R. (2008). A do reAmi doencore:
re mi encore: A closer
A closer look at thelook at the personality
personality correlates of
conventional category as well as with the energetic and rhythmic category. correlates of
music preferences. musicofpreferences.
Journal Journal of
Individual Differences, Individual
29(1), 45-55. Differences,
*Significant at α = .05 29(1), 45-55.
**Significant at α = .01

You might also like