You are on page 1of 11

Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Hans Eysenck: A research evaluation


Michael W. Eysenck
Department of Psychology, Roehampton University, Whitelands College, Holybourne Avenue, London SW15 4JD, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Hans Eysenck made outstanding contributions to the description of human personality with his identification of
Received 21 December 2015 three orthogonal personality dimensions although his approach was less exhaustive than that of subsequent re-
Accepted 6 April 2016 searchers. He also proposed an ambitious agenda for developing comprehensive theoretical explanations based
on the experimental approach and the biological underpinnings of major personality dimensions. Subsequent
Keywords:
theories have followed his blueprint. Hans Eysenck's higher-level theoretical assumptions have stood the test
Hans Eysenck
Personality
of time better than his lower-level ones. However, a general limitation was his de-emphasis of cognitive process-
Intelligence es and structures. He was less successful at implementation and interpretation than theory generation. This oc-
Behavior therapy curred in part because of his preference for a lawyer-like approach to research rather than a more scientific and
Psychopathology objective one.
© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

1. Introduction cause in principle it should come closer to capturing the richness of


human personality. In the words of Cattell, Eber and Tatsouka (1970,
Hans Eysenck (my father) had a very long and phenomenally pro- pp. 111-112):
ductive career. It would be absurd of me to pretend to have a detailed
“The primary factors give one most information, and we would ad-
familiarity with all this work. In any case, it would be impossible to dis-
vocate higher-strata [second-order] contributors only as supple-
cuss it all in a single article. Instead, I have focused on his major contri-
mentary concepts.”
butions as well as his approach to research. My relationship with my
father is discussed in M. Eysenck (2013). In practice, this putative advantage of the primary-factor approach
was not manifest for two main reasons. First, it proved very difficult to
1.1. Dimensions of personality: historical context replicate the primary factors identified with any given personality test.
For example, Barrett and Kline (1982) conducted several different factor
It is important to consider Hans Eysenck's taxonomic approach to in- analyses on the 16PF. They found between seven and nine factors in
dividual differences in personality within the relevant historical context. these analyses, and these factors were tenuously linked to Cattell's 16
American psychologists typically assumed the optimal approach was to factors. Second, it proved impossible to achieve consensus on the num-
identify a fairly large number of correlated or primary factors. For exam- ber and nature of the primary factors of personality.
ple, Guilford (1939) argued that there were probably approximately 20 It was also typically assumed (sometimes implicitly) that individual
important primary traits. differences in these primary factors depended almost entirely on envi-
In practice, Guildford identified 11 personality traits or factors. In ronmental influences. This assumption derived in part from the exces-
similar fashion, Cattell developed his Sixteen Personality Factor (16PF) sively environmentalist behaviorist approach prevalent at the time,
Questionnaire that identified 16 traits or factors, many dissimilar to and was subscribed to by later social learning theorists such as Rotter
those identified by Guildford. and Bandura. Intriguingly, Guilford (1934, p. 337) argued that, “most
This approach can be contrasted with Hans Eysenck's emphasis on writers have regarded I–E [introversion–extraversion] as primarily a
identifying a small number of orthogonal or second-order factors. It matter of heredity”, however, he failed to test this hypothesis.
was plausible a priori to prefer the former, primary-factor approach be- Many American personality researchers in the 1930s and 1940s re-
lied heavily on factor analysis in their studies of individual differences
in personality. This technique provides suggestive evidence concerning
E-mail address: michael.eysenck@roehampton.ac.uk. the number and nature of personality factors. However, the personality

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.039
0191-8869/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
210 M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

factors identified by factor analysis are not necessarily the most impor- the distribution) imply that the P dimension is not directly relevant to
tant ones. Much additional research was required (but rarely carried much of the healthy population.
out) to establish their importance and validity and to provide explana- In spite of the above doubts about whether psychoticism qualifies as
tory accounts. one of the main personality dimensions, it is clearly important in many
contexts. For example, Corr (2010) discussed much evidence of com-
2. Dimensions of personality monality between psychoticism and psychopathy. More specifically,
he argues persuasively that psychoticism and psychopathy have similar
Hans Eysenck's approach to personality was distinctively different deficits involving the behavioral approach system, the behavioral inhi-
from previous ones. He advocated focusing on large, second-order fac- bition system, and the fight–flight–freeze system.
tors. His early contention that extraversion and neuroticism are the
most important and readily replicable personality factors has stood 3. Explanations of personality
the test of time. Similar factors had previously been identified on both
sides of the Atlantic Ocean. However, nearly all American personality Hans Eysenck adopted a more ambitious and systematic approach to
theorists (and Freud) assumed extraversion was negatively correlated human personality than any previous theorist. The essence of his ap-
with neuroticism/anxiety and so often failed to distinguish clearly be- proach (Eysenck, 1994, pp. 7–8) was as follows:
tween the two dimensions. Of interest, Jung (1923) argued that the
two dimensions were independent of each other. In fact, there is a mod- “In order to qualify as a major dimension of personality, any concept
erate negative correlation between extraversion and trait anxiety (Gray, should be based on a general theory which has its antecedents in
1981) but the neuroticism dimension was designed to be orthogonal to DNA (genetic analysis), and advances through biological intermedi-
extraversion. aries (psychophysiological, hormonal, etc.) to the dimensional and
In England, Burt (1915) identified a factor of general emotionality factorial analysis of behavior and trait patterns. It should from there
resembling neuroticism. Burt (1940) replicated his earlier findings and proceed to the experimental testing of the general theory, making
also identified “a bipolar factor making for aggressive behavior when deductions from the theory as to how people at various positions
positive and for inhibited or introverted behavior when negative” on the relevant personality dimensions would behave in carefully
(p. 374). However, Burt's research was limited in scope and designed experimental situations. And finally one would expect that
methodology. the theory, and the knowledge acquired through experimental
Evidence supporting the notion that extraversion and neuroticism study, should enable us to make predictions as to the general social
are of special importance has come from re-analyzing data from person- behavior of people in carefully defined situations.”
ality questionnaires not explicitly assessing these dimensions
Below some of these aspects of Hans Eysenck's approach are
(e.g., Saville & Blinkhorn, 1976, 1981). There is a thorough discussion
discussed. For reasons of space, it is not feasible to address all of them.
of this research in Eysenck and M. Eysenck (1985) and it would be oti-
ose to repeat that discussion. Of particular importance is subsequent re-
search based on the fundamental lexical hypothesis (the notion that all 3.1. Heritability of personality
salient individual differences in personality are represented by single
words in language). Researchers (e.g., Goldberg, 1990) using this hy- One of Hans Eysenck's key contributions to personality theory was
pothesis to discover the main personality factors have consistently iden- his emphasis on the major role played by genetic factors in accounting
tified factors resembling extraversion and neuroticism even though for individual differences in personality. He argued (Eysenck, 1979,
their approach differs radically from Hans Eysenck's. p. 525), “Using measures of the major personality dimensions P, E, and
Support for the special importance of extraversion and neuroticism N, and calculating heritabilities along the lines of modern biometrical
has come from studies comparing the structure of personality and of genetical analysis, we get figures in the band from 60% to 80%, when
mood. Meyer and Shack (1989) compared the two-dimensional model test unreliability has been allowed for.”
of mood (positive affect and negative affect) with the two- Vukasović and Bratko (2015) recently reported a comprehensive
dimensional model of personality (extraversion and neuroticism). The meta-analysis of twin, family, and adoption studies of personality.
two models shared a structural identity: extraversion aligned with pos- They assessed the percentage of individual differences in each
itive affect and neuroticism aligned with negative affect. Eysenckian dimension due to genetic factors: for extraversion, it was
There is much less consensus that Hans Eysenck's third orthogonal 39%, for neuroticism it was 42%, and for psychoticism it was 30%.
factor, psychoticism, is of major importance (e.g., van Kampen, 2009). The above findings indicate genetic factors are less important than
The dominant approach to personality is the five-factor model (e.g., assumed by Hans Eysenck. However, an early study by Eysenck and
Costa & McCrae, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 1985). The Big Five factors iden- Prell (1951) is inconsistent with that conclusion. They reported a very
tified within this model do not include psychoticism. Two of the five fac- high heritability estimate of 81% in a twin study on neuroticism assessed
tors (agreeableness and conscientiousness) correlate negatively with by a gallimaufry of unreliable tests including the body-sway and Ror-
psychoticism although the correlation between conscientiousness and schach tests. An attempted replication of Eysenck and Prell's study by
psychoticism is relatively modest. Blewett (1953) was notably unsuccessful: several tests failed to inter-
The above findings suggest agreeableness and conscientiousness correlate as predicted, there was no clear neuroticism factor, and there
might be primary factors associated with the higher-order factor of was scant evidence of any hereditary determination (Shields, 1954).
psychoticism (Eysenck, 1992a,b). However, there is no compelling sup-
port for this interpretation. It is noteworthy that essentially the same 3.2. Biological underpinnings
five factors have been found using several very different methodologies
and across numerous cultures (McCrae & Costa, 1997). As mentioned earlier, most early American personality researchers
Another reason for doubting the fundamental importance of focused on using factor analysis applied to questionnaire data to provide
psychoticism (P) is the fact that its nature changed considerably over descriptive accounts of human personality. This approach is intrinsically
the years: Claridge, Robinson and Birchall (1983) found the correlation limited. Hans Eysenck pointed out that explanatory accounts of person-
between the original P scale and that in the Eysenck Personality Ques- ality could be developed by relating individual differences in personality
tionnaire (EPQ) was only + .19. Finally, the mean scores on the 25- to their biological underpinnings. This approach was a substantial ad-
item psychoticism scale on the EPQ were very low: 2.63 for females vance, and has had a major impact (e.g., Cloninger, Svrakic, &
and 3.78 for males. These low means (and a strong positive skew in Przybeck, 1993; Gray, 1981; Pickering, Cooper, Smillie, & Corr, 2013).
M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219 211

3.2.1. Extraversion because introverts have a strongly functioning higher-level control


Eysenck (1967) explained individual differences in extraversion by mechanism that reduces the impact of arousal changes on performance.
assuming that the threshold for arousal in the ascending reticular Even more speculatively, the greater ARAS activation often found in in-
activating system (ARAS) was lower for introverts than for extraverts. troverts may in part reflect the functioning of this control mechanism.
This theoretical approach (owing much to Corcoran, 1965, and Gray,
1964) proved extremely productive and led to a substantial amount of
research. 3.2.2. Neuroticism
In spite of many anomalous findings, the evidence overall indicates Eysenck's (1967) explanation of neuroticism in terms of individual
that there is typically a negative relationship between extraversion differences in activation within the visceral system (hippocampus,
and arousal (Eysenck, 1982; Gale, 1983). The positive findings include amygdala, cingulum, septum and hypothalamus) was also influential.
numerous studies in which manipulations such as sleep deprivation, However, it de-emphasized the importance of brain networks associat-
noise, and caffeine were used to increase or decrease arousal levels. ed with cognitive functioning. For example, the dorsolateral prefrontal
Very strong support for the arousal model of extraversion was pro- cortex and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex play major roles in emotion
vided by Howarth and Eysenck (1968). Their study was based on a the- regulation via downregulation of the amygdala (Kohn et al., 2014).
ory of memory consolidation assuming that high arousal produces a Such downregulation is most effective in individuals having the stron-
strong consolidation process interfering with short-term recall but facil- gest links between prefrontal areas and the amygdala (Lee, Suchday, &
itating long-term recall. Kleinsmith and Kaplan (1963) used galvanic Wylie-Rosett, 2012). Individuals high in neuroticism have much weaker
skin responses (GSRs) to assess the arousal associated with each item. functional connections between brain areas than low scorers (Servaas
There was a large cross-over interaction between item arousal and re- et al., 2015).
tention interval in memory performance: low-arousal items were well According to attentional control theory (M. Eysenck, Derakshan,
recalled at short retention intervals but poorly at long ones; in contrast, Santos, & Calvo, 2007; M. Eysenck, Moser, Derakshan, & Allen,
high-arousal items showed reminiscence (memory improved over submitted for publication), individuals high in trait anxiety have im-
time). paired top-down attentional control combined with greater use of com-
Howarth and Eysenck (1968) predicted a similar cross-over interac- pensatory strategies to minimize the adverse effects of impaired
tion should be obtained comparing low- and high-arousal individuals control. As predicted, high- and low-anxious individuals differ in the
(extraverts and introverts, respectively). There was no overall effect functioning of brain networks of direct relevance to the processes iden-
of retention interval (between 0 min and 24 h). However, there tified within attentional control theory (Sylvester et al., 2012). These
was a dramatic cross-over interaction between personality and brain networks include the fronto-parietal network, the ventral atten-
retention interval with a significance level considerably greater than tion network, the default mode network, and the cingulo-opercular
p b .0000000000000000001. In this interaction (one of the most signif- network.
icant in the whole of memory research), the memory performance of in-
troverts was approximately 55% greater after 24 h than 0 min.
There are three issues with this research on consolidation theory. 3.2.3. Gray's biological model
First, there was almost certainly a confounding between item arousal Jeffrey Gray (e.g., 1981; Pickering & Gray, 1999) provided an im-
and intra-list position in the study by Kleinsmith and Kaplan (1963). proved biological model based on the notion that trait anxiety
Schürer-Necker (1990) found high-arousal items came predominantly (consisting mainly of neuroticism but also including some introversion)
from the start of the list whereas low-arousal items came mostly from involves susceptibility to punishment and is linked to the behavioral ap-
the end of the list. When Schürer-Necker unconfounded these variables, proach system (BAS). As predicted, individual differences in neuroti-
there was no interaction in memory performance between item arousal cism are associated with volume variations in brain areas involved in
and retention interval. threat, punishment, and negative affect (DeYoung et al., 2010) [this is
Second, there have been several failures to replicate Howarth and Jeremy Gray rather than Jeffrey Gray].
Eysenck's (1968) findings even when using a very similar methodology Gray also assumed there are individual differences in susceptibility
(e.g., Fuller, 1978, Knezović, 1985; Knezović & Bauer, 2003; McLaughlin, to reward linked to the BAS and are plausibly associated with extraver-
1968; Schneller & Garske, 1976). No subsequent study has obtained sion and impulsivity. As predicted, there is a positive association be-
findings remotely as strong as the original ones of Howarth and tween extraversion and volume of medial orbitofrontal cortex, a brain
Eysenck. area involved in coding the reward value of stimuli (DeYoung et al.,
Third, small reminiscence effects have occasionally been reported in 2010). Other research has shown that brain areas associated with re-
the literature. However, as Keppel (2014, p. 159) concluded in a review ward processing are more activated in extraverts than introverts (e.g.,
of the research: “Reminiscence for high-arousal pairs and for high- Canli et al., 2001).
arousal subjects has been reported, but not by later researchers who Gray's (1981) biological approach based on independent behavioral
attempted to replicate the impressive findings.” inhibition system (BIS) and the BAS linked to trait anxiety/neuroticism
In sum, it has often been found that introverted individuals have and impulsivity/extraversion, respectively, accounts for the existence
higher levels of arousal than extraverted ones. A crucial (but unre- of the two orthogonal factors of extraversion (impulsivity) and neurot-
solved) issue concerns the centrality of high arousal to introversion. icism (trait anxiety). Research of relevance to Gray's approach has been
The ARAS is responsible for alertness (Young & Pigott, 1999) and high obtained in studies focusing on mood state. Two major orthogonal di-
ARAS activation is associated with feelings such as ‘energized’, ‘coura- mensions of mood state (positive affect and negative affect) have been
geous’, and ‘alerted’, whereas low ARAS activation is associated with identified (Watson & Tellegen, 1985). There is a moderately strong rela-
feelings such as ‘tired’, ‘weary’, ‘drowsy’, and ‘powerless’ (Siebel, tionship between extraversion and positive affect and between neurot-
Winkler, Seitz-Bernhard, & Noosomatik, 1990). However, it is implausi- icism and negative affect (Clark & Watson, 1999; Hepburn & M. Eysenck,
ble that you need to be very alert, courageous and energized to be an in- 1989; Meyer & Shack, 1989).
trovert or tired, powerless, and weary to be an extravert. It is more likely Other related research has indicated that subjective well-being is as-
that high arousal occurs as a consequence (rather than cause) of sociated with high extraversion and low neuroticism (Steel, Schmidt, &
introversion. Shultz, 2008). It is typically assumed that such findings reflect a direct
Another theoretical issue concerns the common finding that arousal influence of personality on subjective well-being. However, recent evi-
manipulations have more influence on the performance of extraverts dence indicates there are reciprocal influences of personality and well-
than of introverts. M. Eysenck (1982) speculated that this occurs being on each other (Soto, 2015).
212 M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

3.2.4. Conclusions agreeableness and openness). However, this research was limited by
In spite of previous theorizing on the biological substrate of person- being cross-sectional.
ality by Russian psychologists (e.g., Pavlov and Teplov), Hans Eysenck Third, other evidence reported by Kotov et al. (2010) is less consis-
made a substantial contribution with his biological approach. The gen- tent with Hans Eysenck's approach. Of the six personality traits consid-
eral biological approach has become increasingly influential over the ered, conscientiousness was the second most strongly associated with
several decades since Hans Eysenck first advocated it. His arousal theory anxiety disorders and depression, always in the negative direction. Ex-
of extraversion has proved very successful based on the findings from traversion was the third most strongly associated with anxiety disor-
empirical research. At the theoretical level, however, the causal rela- ders and depression, again always in the negative direction.
tionships between extraversion and arousal remain somewhat unclear. Fourth, there is evidence (e.g., Hettema, Neale, Myers, Prescott, &
More generally, the extent to which we can understand the differences Kendler, 2006) that genetic factors shared with neuroticism account
between introverts and extraverts in terms of arousal differences has for up to 50% of the genetic risk associated with anxiety and depressive
not been established. disorders. However, much of the overlap between neuroticism and the
Gray's biological model is an advance on Hans Eysenck's. Of most im- various anxiety disorders may simply reflect ‘criterion overlap’ (Jang,
portance, his model provides a coherent theoretical rationale for the Wolf, & Larstone, 2006): the personality factor and the mental disorders
dominance of orthogonal dimensions of extraversion and neuroticism are based on rather similar descriptive categories.
in the personality domain. These two dimensions relate to independent Another complication is the finding (Spinhoven, Penelo, de Rooij,
brain mechanisms associated with susceptibility to reward and to pun- Penninx, & Ormel, 2014) that changes in the symptoms of anxiety disor-
ishment, respectively. In addition, there are two orthogonal mood di- ders or depression were followed by changes in neuroticism. Thus, there
mensions (positive and negative affect) that are closely related to are reciprocal effects of personality and psychopathology rather than
extraversion and neuroticism. the typically assumed unidirectional impact of personality on
psychopathology.
4. Continuity between personality and psychopathology Fifth, the links between psychoticism and psychosis are typically
weaker than those between psychoticism and criminality and antisocial
The assumption of continuity between personality and psychopa- behavior. Chapman, Chapman and Kwapil (1994) found in a 10-year
thology has been advocated by numerous theorists (including Freud, longitudinal study that individuals initially scoring high in psychoticism
Jung and Kretschmer). However, Hans Eysenck made more use of the were no more likely to develop a psychosis than those scoring lower on
continuity assumption than previous researchers. At the risk of over- psychoticism. Lonnqvist et al. (2009) found in a longitudinal study that
simplification, his key assumptions were as follows: the onset of schizophrenia was predicted by high neuroticism and low
extraversion.
(1) The various neuroses or anxiety disorders are all related. Sixth, the associations between personality and psychopathology
(2) High scorers on neuroticism are more susceptible than low are often more complex than implied by the above assumptions. For ex-
scorers to develop neuroses. ample, Andersen and Bienvenu (2011) identified five possible explana-
(3) There is a continuum from low levels of neuroticism through to tions for links between personality and psychopathology: (1) the
neurosis with no precise dividing line between normality and ab- vulnerability hypothesis (personality predisposes to mental disorders);
normality. (2) the scar hypothesis (illness adversely affects personality); (3) the
(4) “All functional psychoses…are related and do not form indepen- pathoplasty hypothesis (no direct causal relationship between person-
dent categories” (Eysenck, 1992a, p. 670) ality and mental disorder; (4) the common cause hypothesis (shared
(5) High scorers on psychoticism within the healthy population are environmental or genetic causes); (5) criterion overlap hypothesis
more susceptible than low scorers to develop psychoses. (similar descriptive categories for personality and mental disorder).
(6) There is a continuum from low levels of psychoticism through to There is some empirical support for all these explanations and Hans
psychosis with no precise dividing line between normality and Eysenck often did not distinguish clearly among them.
abnormality.
5. Personality and mortality

4.1. Evidence and evaluation It is very important to show that individual differences in personality
have strong predictive power for major real-world outcomes. The most
Space limitations preclude a detailed discussion of the evidence re- dramatic such evidence emerged from Hans Eysenck's collaborative re-
lating to all six theoretical assumptions. However, a few points can be search with Grossarth-Maticek. Grossarth-Maticek, Eysenck and Vetter
made. First, there are similar genetic risk factors across the anxiety dis- (1988) argued that individuals with Type 1 personalities (hopelessness
orders. Tambs et al. (2009) conducted a twin study across the anxiety and depression) are susceptible to cancer, Type 2 personalities (arousal
disorders and concluded, “The latent liability to all anxiety disorders and aggression) are susceptible to coronary heart disease, and Type 4
was substantially more heritable (54%) than the individual disorders personalities (personal autonomy or control) are the healthiest. The rel-
(23% to 40%)” (p. 301). However, note that these findings were mostly ative risk of Type 1s dying of cancer compared to Type 4s was 55.6 and
based on individuals possessing sub-clinical levels of the disorders. for Type 2s the relative risk of dying of heart disease was 58.8 (Lee,
Other research suggests that the picture is more complex. Hettema, 1991).
Prescott, Myers, Neale and Kendler (2005) found in a twin study that The above relative risks are almost 20 times greater than those of
two genetic factors were shared across the anxiety disorders. One factor smoking. The mystery deepens when we consider questionnaire mea-
was most strongly associated with generalized anxiety disorder, panic sures of Type 1 and Type 2 personalities correlate very highly (+.81)
disorder, and agoraphobia, whereas the other factor was mostly associ- (Amelang & Schmidt-Rathjens, 1992). This makes it hard to understand
ated with specific phobia. Social phobia was moderately associated with how Type 1 personality could be strongly related to death from cancer
both factors. but not coronary heart disease whereas the opposite pattern was
Second, there is convincing evidence that high neuroticism is associ- found for Type 2s.
ated with all the major anxiety disorders and with depression (Kotov, Grossarth-Maticek and Eysenck (1995) assessed mortality over a
Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, 2010). Indeed, neuroticism was more 15-year period in individuals initially between 45 and 68 years of age.
strongly associated with these disorders than were any other personal- The correlation between scores on the Self-Regulation Inventory (on
ity trait considered (extraversion, disinhibition, conscientiousness, which high scorers have low neuroticism) and mortality was −.79
M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219 213

suggesting self-regulation accounts for 64% of the variance in mortality. of mental disorders is in danger of omitting important traits having little
If account is taken of the unreliability of the questionnaire, the correla- direct relevance to mental disorder (e.g., agreeableness and openness:
tion becomes −.84 (accounting for 70% of the variance). Even that is Kotov et al., 2010). As discussed above, the preponderance of very low
an underestimate given the wide age range (23 years) within the scores on psychoticism suggests this dimension may lack major rele-
sample. vance within healthy populations.
In contrast, it has been argued that the fundamental lexical hypoth-
5.1. Explanations esis provides a principled basis for describing all major aspects of
human personality. However, this hypothesis has been criticized on sev-
How can we explain these various findings (which are hugely more eral grounds (e.g., Uher, 2013) and it is debatable whether all aspects of
impressive than those reported by other researchers)? Perhaps the personality are expressible in one word. Despite that, findings obtained
questionnaires used were markedly superior. However, consider their using the lexical hypothesis have typically produced factors overlapping
items. The following representative 84-word item does not adhere to substantially with those from the five-factor model (e.g., Goldberg,
the universal recommendation to use short, single-barreled, unambigu- 1990). The loadings of personality terms on the Big Five were excellent
ous items”. predictors of those terms' relational impact (rated importance of each
personality term when responding to another person) (Wood, 2015) in-
“Do you change your behavior according to consequences of previ- dicating the social importance of all Big Five factors.
ous behavior, i.e., do you repeat ways of acting which have in the
past led to positive results, such as contentment, well-being, self- 6.2. Biological approach
reliance, etc., and to stop acting in ways which lead to negative con-
sequences, i.e., to feelings of anxiety, hopelessness, depression, ex- A key assumption of Hans Eysenck's biological approach to personal-
citement, annoyance, etc.? In other words, have you learned to ity was that individual differences in personality depend to a large ex-
give up ways of acting which have negative consequences, and to re- tent (60%–80%) on genetic factors. This assumption has received
ly more and more on ways of acting which have positive conse- partial support. As discussed earlier, a recent meta-analysis (Vukasović
quences?” & Bratko, 2015) indicated that the mean heritability of the three
Eysenckian personality factors was 37%, which is substantially lower
Attempts to interpret the unique findings of Hans Eysenck and
than assumed by Hans Eysenck.
Grossarth-Maticek were make harder by their reluctance (or refusal)
Of course, genetic factors are important even if they account for
to provide full information about their methodology and question-
under 40% of individual differences in a given personality factor. How-
naires. For example, Grossarth-Maticek, Eysenck and Barrett (1993) re-
ever, the fact that individual differences in personality depend more
ported that a large-scale study of theirs starting in 1973 had shown that
on environmental than genetic factors means it is of great importance
a personality questionnaire predicted future cancer and coronary heart
to study in detail environmental factors influencing personality
disease accurately only when the interviewer established trust with the
development.
participants and explained the questions. This crucial aspect of the pro-
cedure was not mentioned in any of their previous research publica-
6.3. Personality and cognition
tions. In any case, the question is so complex and convoluted that it
would seem impossible to provide simple explanations of them that
In spite of the very broad and comprehensive nature of Hans
captured all the numerous nuances of meaning they contain.
Eysenck's theorizing about personality, he failed to give proper consid-
No unequivocal interpretation of Eysenck and Grossarth-Maticek's
eration to individual differences in cognitive processes and associated
findings is possible. However, an article by van der Ploeg (1991), if accu-
brain networks. For space reasons, I will discuss these issues primarily
rate, may clarify some issues.
with respect to neuroticism.
Consider the typical finding that individuals high in trait anxiety re-
6. Personality research: overall assessment port spending far more time in the cognitive activity of worrying than
do those low in trait anxiety (M. Eysenck & van Berkum, 1992). Anxious
What is most impressive about Hans Eysenck's entire approach to members of other species probably spend little time worrying about
human personality is its sheer ambitiousness. Below I evaluate his ap- possible future negative events.
proach with respect to the research topics discussed above. Other research indicates an important role for cognitive factors in in-
dividual differences in neuroticism or trait anxiety. There is consider-
6.1. Personality factors able evidence (M. Eysenck, 1992; M. Eysenck, 1997) indicating that
only those high in neuroticism or trait anxiety exhibit several cognitive
A potential advantage of Hans Eysenck's ambitious approach with biases (e.g., attentional bias: excessive attention to threat-related stim-
respect to identifying personality factors is that it is arguably the opti- uli; and interpretive bias: a tendency to interpret ambiguous stimuli and
mal method to ensure that those identified are of substantial impor- situations in a threatening fashion). Cognitive biases are causally related
tance. In contrast, it is possible that some of the personality factors to individual differences in anxiety. Cognitive bias modification tech-
identified by use of the fundamental lexical hypothesis are only of niques designed to reduce or eliminate these biases are effective in re-
minor importance. ducing anxiety (MacLeod & Clarke, 2013).
Hans Eysenck typically argued that his entire approach was
theoretically-driven but this seems an exaggeration. For example, as 6.4. Personality and psychopathology
Goldberg pointed out in a letter to Hans Eysenck (February 6, 1995;
quoted in van Kampen, 2009, p. 13): “It is not clear to the world at Another major feature of Hans Eysenck's ambitious research pro-
large how your PEN model is not well described as an example of the gram was his systematic attempt to integrate theory and research on
‘heuristic’ school, given that you explicitly adopted ‘some psychiatric personality and psychopathology. This proved only partially successful.
system of classification’ for P and N, and you used your own ‘notion of The findings with respect to neuroticism and the anxiety disorders have
what traits might be important’ to select E [extraversion].” mostly supported his predictions: high neuroticism is associated with
Hans Eysenck's approach may fail to provide a truly comprehensive all anxiety disorders and with depression; there are common genetic
description of human personality. It is arguable that the emphasis on risk factors found across anxiety disorders; there are commonalities in
finding personality traits or factors meaningfully related to categories the genetic factors underlying high neuroticism and the anxiety
214 M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

disorders. However, there are some concerns about criterion overlap factors?; (4) are anxiety disorders independent of each other or relat-
between neuroticism and the anxiety disorders. The most important ed?; (5) are psychoses independent of each other or related?; (6) is a di-
unresolved issue concerns causality: cross-sectional studies cannot clar- mensional or categorical approach preferable to an understanding of
ify the extent to which any overlap between high neuroticism and the mental disorder?
anxiety disorders reflects an influence of the former on the latter. It is arguable that Hans Eysenck consistently selected the more ap-
propriate answer to these dichotomies. However, it is essential at
6.5. Validation of personality factors some point to move away from simple dichotomies in order to provide
more nuanced accounts of human personality.
The value of a personality measure can be assessed via concurrent or
predictive validity. All Hans Eysenck's personality scales demonstrated 7. Other contributions
reasonable or good validity in these senses. However, his research
with Grossarth-Maticek produced (to my knowledge) the strongest Hans Eysenck produced numerous articles and books on topics not
predictive validity in the entire history of personality research. Their re- directly associated with personality. Here I will consider a few of his
search findings have not been replicated (Amelang, Schmidt-Rathjens, best-known contributions outside personality research.
& Matthews, 1996). Amelang and Schmidt-Rathjens (2003, p. 12)
reviewing findings on personality, cancer and coronary heart disease, 7.1. Behavior therapy
concluded:
“The literature on etiological factors of cancer and coronary heart Hans Eysenck provided an early comprehensive theoretical rationale
disease (CHD) indicates that personality factors are of little impor- for behavior therapy (Eysenck, 1959) based squarely on Pavlovian
tance. Not more than 1–2% of the heart-disease variance is explained learning theory. For example, he assumed symptoms are maladaptive
by personality variables.” conditioned responses, effective treatment involves extinguishing mal-
adaptive autonomic and skeletal conditioned responses and establish-
ing desirable conditioned responses.
Hans Eysenck was a very effective proselytizer for behavior therapy
6.6. Lumpers vs. splitters
in the late 1950s and early 1960s, in part through his founding of the
journal Behavior Research and Therapy. However, his influence waned
Charles Darwin in a letter to J. D. Hooke in 1857 introduced the dis-
over the years because his views changed remarkably little. Here is an
tinction between lumpers and splitters. The former use broad categories
example:
because they emphasize similarities and de-emphasize differences
whereas the latter use numerous relatively narrow categories because “The different types of neurotic illness arise through a process of
they emphasize differences rather than similarities. Hans Eysenck was Pavlovian conditioning, and can be eliminated through a process of
obviously a lumper rather than a splitter: he identified only three per- Pavlovian extinction...all cures of neurotic disorders are based on
sonality factors, the term ‘neurosis’ was applied to all anxiety disorders, Pavlovian extinction.” (Eysenck, 1987, pp. 6-7)
the term ‘psychosis’ was applied to schizophrenia and bipolar disorder,
and so on. It would be supererogatory to document all the counter-evidence to
Is it better for theorists to be lumpers or splitters? There is no these views. For example, the etiology of the anxiety disorders depends
straightforward answer to that question. However, there are major ad- on vicarious and informational pathways as well as Pavlovian condi-
vantages in being a lumper early in the development of a research area. tioning (Rachman, 2015). In addition, there is increased differentiation
Lumpers can provide a comprehensive framework that is very produc- of effective forms of therapy for the various anxiety disorders.
tive in generating research. The dramatic impact of cognitive and cognitive-behavioral therapy
The disadvantages of being a lumper are obvious. First, lumpers' the- on the treatment of anxiety disorders has revealed the limitations of
ories are inevitably oversimplified because entities that are clearly dif- an excessive emphasis on behavior therapy and the changing of behav-
ferent are nevertheless categorized together. For example, the use of a ioral patterns. However, this neglect of cognitive processes and struc-
category such as ‘neurosis’ ignores the differences among the various tures is entirely consistent with Hans Eysenck's neglect of the
anxiety disorders, and the same is true of the category ‘psychosis’. In ad- cognitive system when accounting for individual differences in
dition, it is improbable that the richness of human personality can be personality.
captured by three orthogonal factors.
7.2. Social attitudes
6.7. Broad vs. narrow theorizing
Eysenck (1954) identified two orthogonal dimensions of social atti-
The distinction between broad versus narrow theories is related to tudes: radicalism and toughmindedness. Of major importance, a subse-
that between lumpers and splitters. Theorizing in psychology is typical- quent twin study by Eaves and Eysenck (1974) reported that individual
ly most productive when it proceeds from the broad or general to the differences in social attitudes have a fairly strong genetic influence:
narrow or specific as we can see by considering cognitive psychology. the heritability for conservatism was .65 and it was .54 for
Several classic studies in cognitive psychology involve oversimplified toughmindedness. They also found the relationship between social atti-
dichotomies (M. Eysenck & Groome, 2015). Examples include the dis- tudes and personality was due primarily to latent shared genetic influ-
tinction between episodic and semantic memory; between the dorsal ences. Subsequent research has replicated this finding and indicated
and ventral streams in perceptual processing; and between analytical an absence of causal influences of personality on social attitudes
and heuristic processing when making judgments. In all these cases (e.g., Hatemi & Verhulst, 2015).
(and others), the postulation of an extremely broad dichotomy led These findings are of major importance because they identify strong
over time to richer and more complex theoretical accounts. influences on social attitudes typically ignored by social psychologists.
The importance of dichotomies within Hans Eysenck's theorizing For example, Tesser and Shaffer (1990) in a review of research on atti-
can easily be seen. Theorists may choose between several dichotomies: tudes reported no references at all to heritability in the social psycholo-
(1) do individual differences in personality depend in part on genetic gy literature.
factors?; (2) does factor analysis provide an adequate account of per- Hogg and Vaughan (2005) failed to mention genetic influences in a
sonality; (3) is it preferable to identify orthogonal or oblique personality chapter on attitudes in their major textbook on social psychology.
M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219 215

Research subsequent to the study by Eaves and Eysenck (1974) indi- Striking disproof of the above assumptions came in a study by
cated a genetic influence on 26 social attitudes (Olson, Vernon, Harris, & Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov and Duncan (1996) on black and white
Jang, 2001). The exciting (and to many psychologists very surprising) American 5-year-olds. The mean IQ of the black children was 1 standard
implication is that individual differences in a very large variety of atti- deviation lower than that of the white children. However, “Adjustments
tudes and behavior may depend in part on genetic factors although pre- for economic and social differences in the lives of black and white chil-
cisely how these effects are mediated remains to be determined. dren all but eliminate differences in the IQ sores between the two
groups” (Brooks-Gunn et al., p. 396).
Further evidence disconfirming Hans Eysenck's assumptions was re-
7.3. Intelligence: event-related potentials (ERPs)
ported by Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D'Onofrio and Gottesman
(2003) in a study on American children. Within affluent families of
Hendrickson and Hendrickson (1980), working in Hans Eysenck's
high socioeconomic status, genetic factors accounted for 72% of individ-
Department, developed a ‘string’ measure based on event-related po-
ual differences in intelligence. In contrast, genetic factors accounted for
tentials (ERPs) which involved fitting a piece of string to the waveform
only 10% of individual differences in intelligence among impoverished
of each ERP record for the first 250 to 500 ms after stimulus presenta-
families of low socioeconomic status. The relevance of these findings
tion. Using this measure, Elaine Hendrickson (1982) reported a correla-
is that Afro-American families are disproportionately of low socioeco-
tion of −.72 between IQ and string length. This finding was interpreted
nomic status.
as supporting the hypothesis that more intelligent individuals make
In sum, many arguments adduced by Hans Eysenck in the book Race,
fewer errors in processing and process information more efficiently.
Intelligence and Education are fallacious. This seems especially irrespon-
Eysenck (in Eysenck & M. Eysenck, 1989, p. 112) was very confident
sible given the very socially sensitive nature of the issues discussed in it.
about the value of ERPs: “There seems to be no doubt that we can now
measure intelligence physiologically with an accuracy that compares fa-
8. The research enterprise: scientists vs. lawyers
vorably with the best IQ tests currently available.” The phrase “no
doubt” is perhaps debatable: Rust (1975) and Shagass, Roemer,
How should researchers conduct themselves? A consensual view
Straumanis and Josiassen (1981) had previously reported non-
was expressed by Cournand (1977). First, he argued scientists must pos-
significant findings; Vetterli and Furedy (1985) reported a correlation
sess intellectual integrity and objectivity, “They must prevent their de-
between the string measure and IQ in the wrong direction; Vogel,
sires and aversions from penetrating their observations of the
Kruger, Schalt, Schnobel and Hassling (1987) in the largest study in
phenomena that they study and their analyses of these observations.”
this area of research found that none of the numerous correlations
(p. 700).
they calculated between various ERP measures and IQ scores even
Second, scientists should have a “doubt of certitude” including a
approached statistical significance.
willingness to acknowledge and admit to errors. The same basic point
Barrett and Eysenck (1992a) reported 8 correlations between ERP
was made eloquently by Feynman (1985, p. 341): “If you're doing an ex-
string length and IQ ranging from −.20 to −.44 with every correlation
periment, you should report everything that you think might make it in-
in the opposite direction to prediction. These findings prompted
valid — not only what you think is right about it: other causes that could
Barrett and Eysenck (1992b) into a dramatic volte face from the total
possibly explain your results … if you make a theory, … then you must
confidence of Eysenck in Eysenck and M. Eysenck (1989): “With regard
also put down all the facts that disagree with it.”
to the various studies of AEP [average evoked potential] correlates of IQ,
Third, scientists should display unselfish engagement with respect
…for every study that claims a ‘significant’ result, there is another study
to the growth of scientific knowledge. Their focus should be on increas-
that claims the opposite … no confidence in correlations between AEP
ing understanding rather than on self-aggrandizement.
parameters and IQ can be expressed.”
The above idealized version of the scientist's approach can be
It is unclear theoretically why we would expect positive findings
contrasted with a caricatured version of the lawyer's approach (my
from tasks imposing practically no cognitive demands. A superior strat-
apologies to any lawyers who happen to read this article!). In essence,
egy is to focus on brain areas (especially within the prefrontal cortex)
the stereotypical view is that lawyers present information very selec-
most closely associated with complex cognition. For example, Basten,
tively (perfectly acceptable within our adversarial legal system).
Hilger and Fiebach (2015) reported a meta-analysis of functional
When defending a client, they emphasize all the evidence pointing in
brain-imaging studies in which participants performed cognitive tasks.
the direction of his/her innocence while ignoring, de-emphasizing,
The findings supported the parieto-frontal integration theory of intelli-
distorting, or dismissing evidence suggestive of guilt.
gence: there were generally positive associations between intelligence
and activation levels in frontal and parietal brain regions. There was lit-
8.1. Hans Eysenck: scientist or lawyer?
tle support for the neural efficiency hypothesis underlying Hans
Eysenck's ERP research program – the majority of brain regions showed
Hans Eysenck consistently claimed he followed a rigorous scientific
a positive (rather than a negative) association between intelligence and
approach to research. Much of relevance on the issue of whether Hans
activation level.
Eysenck adopted a scientist's or a lawyer's approach was discussed by
Buchanan (2010) in his biography. His account (so accurate that no un-
7.4. Race and intelligence biased person could fail to share most of his views) strongly suggests
Hans Eysenck's approach was considerably closer to that of a lawyer.
Hans Eysenck's best-known contribution to the study of intelligence Hans Eysenck often discussed findings in a way lacking objectivity.
was his 1971 book, Race, Intelligence and Education, in which it was ar- For example, Eysenck (1952) claimed that patients with neurotic disor-
gued that some of the observed mean IQ differences between white ders were much more likely to recover if they received no systematic
and black individuals are probably due to genetic factors. The book con- treatment (spontaneous recovery) than if they received psychoanalysis.
tains even more very incorrect assumptions than generally assumed. However, the median length of treatment for the psychotherapy groups
Here are three: “The environmentalist theory is…too vague to be tested was approximately 15 weeks (unscientifically, this information is miss-
at all” (p. 119); American whites and Afro-Americans “share a common ing in the original article). If we compare improvement over a standard
cultural heritage and a common educational system…different racial length of time, psychoanalysis was moderately effective (McNeilly &
populations growing up within the same culture are being measured” Howard, 1991).
(p. 77); and “Interaction effects [between genetic and environmental Another distortion of the evidence occurred in an article (Eysenck,
factors] are rather unimportant” (p. 117). 1991b, p. 330) describing the findings of research on vitamins and IQ
216 M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

by Schoenthaler, Amos, Eysenck, Peritz and Yudkin (1991): “A reason- he was not a genius on the grounds of being insufficiently high in
ably random selection of school children…contains a sizable number psychoticism. However, he repeatedly claimed to be the most cited living
whose IQ can be raise through [vitamin] supplementation, compared psychologist (e.g., Eysenck, 1997) so perhaps his modest claim should not
to a control group, by some 11 points of IQ.” In fact, three IQ measures be taken at face value.
were obtained before and after vitamin administration and there were Third, it is perhaps unfair to view Hans Eysenck's approach from
three different vitamin doses. Only one of the nine vitamin-control com- our contemporary perspective. As Hartley (1953, p. 1) pointed out:
parisons was statistically significant. This finding involved an IQ gain av- “The past is a foreign country: they do things differently there.”
eraging 3.7 points greater than the control-group gain and involved Perhaps Hans Eysenck's approach was more acceptable decades
only non-verbal IQ. ago when theory and research in psychology were at a primitive
There are several distorted interpretations of the evidence on the stage.
dangers of smoking in Eysenck (1991a). Here are three. First, he The past also differs from the present in that various countercul-
claimed that there was very little evidence that smoking by itself is ture movements were very influential during the 1960s. For
associated with cancer or coronary heart disease. In fact, there was example, Paul Feyerabend, a philosopher of science, advocated
pre-existing compelling evidence indicating that chronic smokers epistemological anarchism. He summarized his approach as follows:
on average lose 10 years of life through smoking (see Doll, Peto, “Science is an essentially anarchistic enterprise…The only principle
Boreham, & Sutherland, 2004). that does not inhibit progress is anything goes” (Feyerabend, 1975,
Second, Eysenck (1991a) argued that it was wholly inappropriate p. 1). According to epistemological anarchism, science advances by
to use the word ‘addiction’ when describing smoking behavior. This a “who-shouts-the-loudest” strategy in which publicity and visibility
argument makes it hard to explain the behavior of patients with count for more than the intrinsic quality of the research. A central
Buerger's disease – “If a patient with the condition continues to problem with Feyerabend's views is that they imply there is no
smoke, gangrene may eventually set in. First a few toes may have clear dividing line between science and non-science.
to be amputated, then the foot at the ankle, then the leg at the
knee, and ultimately at the hip…Patients are strongly advised that 9. Summary and conclusions
if only they will stop smoking,…the otherwise inexorable march of
gangrene up the legs will be curbed…it is not at all uncommon to How can we sum up the overall impact of Hans Eysenck's enormous
find a patient with Buerger's disease vigorously puffing away in his research output on numerous diverse topics? Perhaps the most striking
hospital bed following a second or third operation” (Brecher, 1972). overall characteristic is that the variance in the quality of his research
Third, Eysenck (1991a) argued that exposure to environmental to- was greater than that of anyone else in the history of psychology
bacco smoke was probably not a factor in cancer or coronary heart dis- (with the possible intriguing exception of Sigmund Freud).
ease. Contrary evidence was available at the time (Peto & Doll, 1986). On the positive side, Hans Eysenck made key contributions including
Compelling evidence was reported by Jee, Ohrr and Kim (1999) in a the following:
large-scale study of non-smoking Korean married women. If the
husband had smoked for more than 30 years, they had a 230% increased (1) His emphasis on a few major orthogonal factors rather than nu-
probability of developing lung cancer relative to the wives of non- merous oblique ones.
smokers. (2) The identification of the two most important personality factors
(extraversion and neuroticism) together with extensive research
8.2. Does it matter? to demonstrate their importance.
(3) His argument that factor analysis is intrinsically limited in that it
Does it matter that Hans Eysenck typically adopted the lawyer's provides a descriptive but not an explanatory account of person-
approach to research? At least three arguments can be adduced to ality.
suggest it does not. First, there is a continuum between scientists' (4) Arguing for (and helping to demonstrate) an important role for
and lawyers' approaches and most scientists are not totally objective genetic factors in accounting for individual differences in person-
and unbiased when interpreting their own and other researchers' ality and social attitudes.
data. The location of the dividing line between the acceptable and (5) Developing theories to explain individual differences in person-
unacceptable is a matter of personal judgment. ality in terms of underlying biological/physiological mechanisms.
Second, Hans Eysenck came up with an ingenious defense of the (6) His emphasis on the continuity hypothesis that there is no firm
lawyer's approach: as he said, “Scientists' … finest and most original dis- dividing line between normality and psychopathology.
coveries are rejected by the vulgar mediocrities filling the ranks of or- (7) His emphasis on the hypothesis that high levels of neuroticism
thodoxy … The figures do not quite fit, so why not fudge them a little and psychoticism are risk factors for neuroses and psychoses,
bit? Usually, the genius is right, of course…, and we may in retrospect respectively.
excuse the childish games, but clearly this cannot be regarded as a li-
cense for non-geniuses to foist their absurd beliefs on us. Such, then,
are the conditions in which leading scientists may be led to falsify In my opinion, Hans Eysenck probably contributed more than anyone
their data, or invent them. Convinced (rightly as it happens) that they else to the development of personality psychology. Decades ago he set out
have made a stupendous discovery, …they see that discovery threat- a paradigm or agenda for a comprehensive approach to theory and re-
ened by enemies or hostile people…The first findings in trying to sub- search in personality (represented by the major contributions identified
stantiate a theoretical discovery are never clear cut … But enemies above). This paradigm remains current to this day. In other words, Hans
would seize upon these anomalies to destroy his theory. Obviously the Eysenck largely asked the most important questions concerning individu-
way to overcome this problem is simply to make sure the data fit the al differences in personality. As Einstein pointed out: “The formulation of
theory!” (Eysenck, 1995a, p. 126). a problem is often more essential than its solution…To raise new
Perhaps the above quotation is intended as a joke although the notion questions, new problems, to regard old problems from a new angle,
that geniuses can (and perhaps should) suit themselves was reiterated requires creative imagination and makes real advances in science.”
elsewhere (Eysenck, 1995b). However, the context is a lengthy discussion What are the limitations of Hans Eysenck's contribution? First, several
of evidence indicating that Newton manipulated some of his data and of his publications (and those of his collaborators) reported dramatic and
theoretical assumptions. The crucial point is that Hans Eysenck seems to counterintuitive findings of potentially great theoretical importance that
endorse Newton's deception. It is true that Hans Eysenck claimed that subsequently failed to replicate. For example, Eysenck's (1952) findings
M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219 217

claiming that psychotherapy reduces the probability of recovery from Blewett, D. B. (1953). An experimental study of the inheritance of neuroticism. (Ph.D. thesis)
University of London.
mental illness are very different from those of most other researchers. Brecher, E. M. (1972). The case of dr. Sigmund Freud. The consumers union report on licit
Other examples include the claim that genetic factors account for 80% of and illicit drugs (http://www.druglibrary.org/schaffer/library/studies/cu/cu24.html).
individual differences in neuroticism (Eysenck, 1952), that longevity Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K., & Duncan, G. J. (1996). Ethnic differences in children's intel-
ligence test scores: Role of economic deprivation, home environment, and maternal
and the disease from which you will die depend almost totally on person- characteristics. Child Development, 67, 396–408.
ality (Grossarth-Maticek & Eysenck, 1995; Grossarth-Maticek et al., Buchanan, R. D. (2010). Playing with fire: The controversial career of Hans J. Eysenck.
1988), that intelligence can be assessed very accurately by ERPs Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Burt, C. (1915). The general and specific factors underlying the primary emotions. Report to
(Hendrickson, 1982), and that introverts show a very strong reminiscence the British Association for the Advancement of Science, Vol. 69. (pp. 45).
effect in memory whereas extraverts have a typical forgetting effect Burt, C. (1940). Factors of the mind: An introduction to factor-analysis in psychology.
(Howarth & Eysenck, 1968). London: University of London Press.
Canli, t., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E. J., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study
Another limitation is that Hans Eysenck's interpretations of his find-
of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral
ings cannot always be taken at face value because he omitted crucially Neuroscience, 115, 33–42.
important information. Here are two examples (referred to earlier). Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsouka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for the sixteen personality
First, when Eysenck (1952) compared the effectiveness of spontaneous factor questionnaire (16 PF). Champaign, Ill: Institute for Personality and Ability
Testing.
recovery and psychotherapy, he failed to indicate that the time period Chapman, J. P., Chapman, L. T., & Kwapil, T. R. (1994). Does the Eysenck psychoticism scale
within which recovery could occur was 700% greater for patients in predict psychosis? A ten-year longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences,
the former group than those in the latter group. 17, 369–375.
Claridge, G., Robinson, D. L., & Birchall, P. (1983). Characteristics of schizophrenics' and
Second, much of the research by Hans Eysenck and Grossart-Maticek neurotics' relatives. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 651–664.
on personality and longevity (e.g., Grossarth-Maticek et al., 1988) failed Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy for anxiety disorders. Review of
to mention that the findings depended critically on the person adminis- Psychiatry, 16, 1–9.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament: A new paradigm for trait anxiety. In L. A.
tering the questionnaire generating trust and providing detailed explana- Pervin, & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 265–286)
tions of the questions. The importance of the factor was only revealed (3rd. ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
several years later (Grossarth-Maticek et al., 1993). Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temper-
ament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.
Hans Eysenck sometimes provided partisan interpretations of the Corcoran, D. W. J. (1965). Personality and the inverted-U relation. British Journal of
findings of others. Examples discussed earlier include his rejection of Psychology, 56, 267–273.
the evidence indicating clearly the dangers of smoking (Eysenck, Corr, P. J. (2010). The psychoticism–psychopathy continuum: A neuropsychological
model. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 695–703.
1991a), his endorsement of the notion that race differences in IQ owe
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO
much to genetic factors (Eysenck, 1971), and the assertion that all cures Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assess-
of anxiety disorders are based on Pavlovian conditioning. ment Resources.
Third, Hans Eysenck systematically de-emphasized the importance of Cournand, A. (1977). The code of the scientist and its relationship to ethics. Science, 198,
699–705.
cognitive processes and structures. Since I am a cognitive psychologist, it DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010).
is certainly possible that my views here are biased. However, there is Testing predictions from personality neuroscience: Brain structure and the Big Five.
compelling evidence that individual differences in neuroticism depend Psychological Science, 21, 820–828.
Doll, R., Peto, R., Boreham, J., & Sutherland, I. (2004). Mortality in relation to
importantly on various cognitive biases (M. Eysenck, 1992; M. Eysenck, smoking: 50 years' observations on male British doctors. British Medical
1997). There is also strong evidence that cognitive therapy is an effective Journal, 328, 1519–1528.
form of treatment for all the anxiety disorders (Clark & Wells, 1997) Eaves, L. J., & Eysenck, H. J. (1974). Genetics and the development of social attitudes.
Nature, 249, 288–289.
which is inconsistent with Hans Eysenck's emphasis on changing behav- Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: An evaluation. Journal of Consulting
ior through conditioning. Psychology, 16, 319–324.
Finally, Hans Eysenck differed from the overwhelming majority of Eysenck, H. J. (1954). The psychology of politics. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Eysenck, H. J. (1959). Learning theory and behavior therapy. Journal of Mental Science, 105,
researchers and theorists in lacking any ‘doubt of certitude’. One of my 61–75.
main recurring thoughts while writing this article echoes a famous Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas.
quotation by William Lamb (2nd Viscount Melbourne) about the poet, Eysenck, H. J. (1971). Race, intelligence and education. London: Temple Smith.
Eysenck, H. J. (1979). Genetic model, theory of personality and the unification of psychology.
historian, and politician Thomas Macaulay:
In J. R. Royce, & J. P. Mos (Eds.), Theoretical advances in behavior genetics (pp. 517–540).
Alphen aan den Rijn: Sitjhoff and Noordhoff.
“I wish I were as cocksure of anything as Tom Macaulay is of every-
Eysenck, H. J. (1987). Behavior therapy. In H. J. Eysenck, & I. Martin (Eds.), Theoretical
thing.” foundations of behavior therapy (pp. 3–35). New York: Plenum.
Eysenck, H. J. (1991a). Smoking, personality, and stress: Psychosocial factors in the preven-
tion of cancer and coronary heart disease. Berlin, Germany: Springer Verlag.
Eysenck, H. J. (1991b). Raising IQ through vitamin and mineral supplementation: An intro-
duction. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 329–333.
References Eysenck, H. J. (1992a). 4 ways 5 factors are not basic. Personality and Individual Differences,
13, 667–673.
Amelang, M., & Schmidt-Rathjens, C. (1992). Psychometric properties of modified Eysenck, H. J. (1992b). The definition and measurement of psychoticism. Personality and
Grossarth-Maticek and Eysenck inventories. Psychological Reports, 71, 1251–1263. Individual Differences, 13, 757–785.
Amelang, M., & Schmidt-Rathjens, C. (2003). Personality, cancer and coronary heart dis- Eysenck, H. J. (1994). The importance of theory in the taxonomy of personality. In B. De
ease: Fictions and facts in the etiological research. Psychologische Rundschau, 54, Raad B, W. K. B. Hofstee, & G. L. Van Heck (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe,
12–23. Vol. 5. (pp. 6–13). Tilburg: Tilburg University Press.
Amelang, M., Schmidt-Rathjens, C., & Matthews, G. (1996). Personality, cancer and coro- Eysenck, H. J. (1995a). Burt as hero and anti-hero: A Greek tragedy. In N. J. Mackintosh
nary heart disease: Further evidence on a controversial issue. British Journal of Health (Ed.), Burt: Fraud or framed? (pp. 111–129). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychology, 1, 191–205. Eysenck, H. J. (1995b). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
Andersen, A. M., & Bienvenu, O. J. (2011). Personality and psychopathology. International versity Press.
Review of Psychiatry, 23, 234–247. Eysenck, H. J. (1997). Rebel with a cause: The autobiography of Hans Eysenck (2nd ed.).
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1992a). Brain evoked potentials and intelligence: The Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
Hendrickson paradigm. Intelligence, 16, 361–382. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural sci-
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1992b). Brain evoked potentials and intelligence. In A. Gale, ence approach. New York: Plenum.
& M. W. Eysenck (Eds.), The handbook of individual differences: Biological perspectives, Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1989). Mindwatching (2nd ed.). London: Prion/
Vol. 3. (pp. 255–285). Chichester: Wiley. Multimedia.
Barrett, P. T., & Kline, P. (1982). An item and radial parcel analysis of the 16PF question- Eysenck, H. J., & Prell, D. B. (1951). The inheritance of neuroticism: An experimental study.
naire. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 259–270. Journal of Mental Science, 97, 441–465.
Basten, U., Hilger, K., & Fiebach, C. J. (2015). Where smart brains are different: A quantitative Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and arousal: Cognition and performance. Berlin: Springer
meta-analysis of functional and structural brain-imaging studies on intelligence. Verlag.
Intelligence, 51, 10–27. Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
218 M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219

Eysenck, M. W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove, UK: Psychology Lee, Y. S. C., Suchday, S., & Wylie-Rosett, J. (2012). Perceived social support, coping styles,
Press. and Chinese immigrants' cardiovascular responses to stress. International Journal of
Eysenck, M. W. (2013). Lost in shadows? In B. D. Kirkcaldy (Ed.), Chimes of time: Wounded Behavioral Medicine, 19, 174–185.
health professionals. Essays in recovery. Leiden: Sidestone Press. Lonnqvist, J., Verkasalo, M., Haukka, J., Nyman, K., Tiihonen, J., Laaksonen, I., et al.
Eysenck, M. W., & Groome, D. (Eds.). (2015). Cognitive psychology: Revisiting the classics. (2009). Premorbid personality factors in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder:
London: Sage. Results from a large cohort study of male conscripts. Journal of Abnormal
Eysenck, M. W., & van Berkum, J. (1992). Trait anxiety, defensiveness, and the structure of Psychology, 118, 418–423.
worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 1285–1290. MacLeod, C., & Clarke, P. J. F. (2013). Cognitive bias modification: A new frontier in
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive cognition and emotion research. In M. D. Robinson, E. Watkins, & E. Harmon-
performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. Jones (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 540–562). New York:
Eysenck, M. W., Moser, J. S., Derakshan, N., & Allen, P. (2016). Trait anxiety, processing Guilford Publications.
efficiency and performance effectiveness: Developing attentional control theory McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1985). Updating Norman's “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligence
(submitted for publication) . and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of
Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against method: Outline of an anarchist theory of knowledge. Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.
London: New Left Books. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal.
Feynman, R. P. (1985). Surely you're joking, Mr. Feynman: Adventures of a curious character. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.
New York: W. W. Norton. McLaughlin, R. J. (1968). Retention in paired-associate learning related to extroversion
Fuller, A. R. (1978). Personality and paired-associate learning. International Journal of and neuroticism. Psychonomic Science, 13, 333–334.
Psychology, 13, 123–128. McNeilly, C. L., & Howard, K. I. (1991). The effects of psychotherapy: A re-evaluation
Gale, A. (1983). Electroencephalographic studies of extraversion–introversion: A case based on dosage. Psychotherapy Research, 1, 74–78.
study in the psychophysiology of individual differences. Personality and Individual Meyer, G. J., & Shack, J. R. (1989). Structural convergence of mood and personality: Evi-
Differences, 4, 371–380. dence for old and new directions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five 691–706.
factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229. Olson, J. M., Vernon, P. A., Harris, J. A., & Jang, K. L. (2001). The heritability of
Gray, J. A. (1964). Strength of the nervous system and levels of arousal: A reinterpretation. attitudes: A study of twins. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
In J. A Gray (Ed.), Pavlov's typology. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 845–860.
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A Peto, J., & Doll, R. (1986). Passive smoking. British Journal of Cancer, 54, 381–383.
model for personality (pp. 246–279). Berlin: Springer Verlag. Pickering, A. D., & Gray, J. A. A. (1999). The neuroscience of personality. In L. Pervin, & O.
Grossarth-Maticek, R., & Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Self-regulation and mortality from cancer, John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (pp. 277–299) (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
coronary heart disease and other causes: A prospective study. Personality and Press.
Individual Differences, 19, 781–795. Pickering, A. D., Cooper, A. J., Smillie, L. D., & Corr, P. J. (2013). On the shoulders of giants.
Grossarth-Maticek, R., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1993). Prediction of cancer and coro- The Psychologist, 26, 22–25.
nary heart disease as a function of method of questionnaire administration. van der Ploeg, H. (1991). What a wonderful life it would be: A re-analysis of some of the
Psychological Reports, 73, 943–959. work of Grossarth-Maticek. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 280–285.
Grossarth-Maticek, R., Eysenck, H. J., & Vetter, H. (1988). Personality type, smoking habit Rachman, S. (2015). The evolution of behavior therapy and cognitive behavior therapy.
and their interaction as predictors of cancer and coronary heart disease. Personality Behaviour Research and Therapy, 64, 1–8.
and Individual Differences, 9, 479–495. Rust, J. (1975). Cortical evoked potential, personality, and intelligence. Journal of
Guilford, J. P. (1934). Introversion–extraversion. Psychological Bulletin, 31, 331–354. Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 1220–1226.
Guilford, J. P. (1939). General psychology. New York, NY: Van Nostrand. Saville, P., & Blinkhorn, S. (1976). Undergraduate personality by factored scores. London:
Hartley, L. P. (1953). The go-between. London: Hamish Hamilton. NFER Publishing Company.
Hatemi, P. K., & Verhulst, B. (2015). Political attitudes develop independently of personal- Saville, P., & Blinkhorn, S. (1981). Reliability, homogeneity and the construct validity of
ity traits. PloS One, 10(3). Cattell's 16PF. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 325–333.
Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence: 2. Measurement. In H. J. Schneller, W. F., & Garske, J. P. (1976). Effects of extroversion and neuroticism on learning
Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 197–228). New York: Springer Verlag. and memory: A test of Eysenck's theory of individual differences in arousal. Journal of
Hendrickson, D. E., & Hendrickson, A. E. (1980). The biological basis of individual differ- Psychology, 94, 27–32.
ences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 3–33. Schoenthaler, S. J., Amos, S. P., Eysenck, H. J., Peritz, E., & Yudkin, J. (1991). Controlled trial
Hepburn, L., & Eysenck, M. W. (1989). Personality, average mood and mood variability. of vitamin-mineral supplementation: Effects on intelligence and performance.
Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 975–983. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 351–362.
Hettema, J. M., Neale, M. C., Myers, J. M., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2006). A Schürer-Necker, E. (1990). Arousal and paired-associate learning: Evidence refuting the
population-based study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing action decrements theory of Walker and Tarte (1963). Pavlovian Journal of
disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 857–864. Biological Science, 25, 195–200.
Hettema, J. M., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J. M., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2005). The struc- Servaas, M. N., Geerligs, L., Renken, R. J., Marsman, J. -B. C., Ormel, J., Riese, H., & Aleman, A.
ture of genetic and environmental risk factors for anxiety disorders in men and (2015). Connectomics and neuroticism: An altered functional network organization.
women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 1987–1995. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 296–304.
Hogg, M. A., & Vaughan, G. M. (2005). Social psychology (4th ed.). Harlow, UK: Pearson Shagass, C., Roemer, R. A., Straumanis, J. J., & Josiassen, R. C. (1981). Intelligence as a factor
Education Limited. in evoked-potential studies in psychopathology. 1. Comparison of low and high IQ
Howarth, E., & Eysenck, H. J. (1968). Extraversion, arousal, and paired-associate learning. subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 11, 1007–1029.
Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 3, 114–116. Shields, J. (1954). Personality differences and neurotic traits in normal twin
Jang, K., Wolf, H., & Larstone, R. (2006). What is the role of personality in psychopatholo- schoolchildren: A study in psychiatric genetics. The Eugenics Review, 45,
gy? A view from behavior genetics. In R. F. Krueger, & J. L. Tackett (Eds.), Personality 213–246.
and psychopathology (pp. 166–186). New York: Guilford Press. Siebel, W. A., Winkler, T., Seitz-Bernhard, B., & Noosomatik, I. (1990). Theoretische
Jee, S. H., Ohrr, H., & Kim, I. S. (1999). Effects of husbands' smoking on the incidence Grundlegung [theoretical foundations]. Langwedel: Glaser & Wohlschlegel.
of lung cancer in Korean women. International Journal of Epidemiology, 28, Soto, C. J. (2015). Is happiness good for your personality? Concurrent and
824–828. prospective relations of the Big Five with subjective well-being. Journal of
Jung, C. G. (1923). Psychological types. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Personality, 83, 45–55.
van Kampen, D. (2009). Personality and psychopathology: A theory-based revision Spinhoven, P., Penelo, E., de Rooij, M., Penninx, B. W., & Ormel, J. (2014). Reciprocal
of Eysenck's PEN model. Clinical Practice & Epidemiology in Mental Health, 5, effects of stable and temporary components of neuroticism and affective disor-
9–21. ders: Results of a longitudinal cohort study. Psychological Medicine, 44,
Keppel, G. (2014). Consolidation and forgetting. In H. Weingartner, & E. S. Parker (Eds.), 337–348.
Memory consolidation: Psychobiology of cognition (pp. 149–162). New York: Psychology Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between personality and
Press. subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 138–161.
Kleinsmith, L. J., & Kaplan, S. (1963). Paired-associate learning as a function of arousal and Sylvester, C. M., Corbetta, M., Raichle, M. E., Rodebaugh, T. L., Schlaggar, B. L., Sheline, Y. I.,
interpolated interval. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 190–193. et al. (2012). Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends
Knezović, Z. (1985). Multivariate and classical-experimental test of unitarity of Eysenck's in Neurosciences, 35, 527–535.
dimensions of introversion–extraversion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Tambs, K., Czajkowsky, N., Roysamb, E., Neale, M. C., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.,
University of Zagreb, Croatia. Aggen, S. H., et al. (2009). Structure of genetic and environmental risk factors
Knezović, Z., & Bauer, K. (2003). Extraversion and paired-associate recall. Review of for dimensional representations of DSM-IV anxiety disorders. British Journal
Psychology, 10, 125–130. of Psychiatry, 195, 301–307.
Kohn, N., Eickhoff, S. B., Scheller, M., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., & Habel, U. (2014). Neural net- Tesser, A., & Shaffer, D. (1990). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of
work of cognitive emotion regulation: An ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. Psychology, 41, 479–523 (Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews).
NeuroImage, 87, 345–355. Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeco-
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to nomic status modifies heritability of Q in young children. Psychological Science, 14,
anxiety, depression, and substance abuse disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological 623–628.
Bulletin, 136, 768–821. Uher, J. (2013). Personality psychology: Lexical approaches, assessment methods, and
Lee, P. N. (1991). Personality and disease: A call for replication. Psychological Inquiry, 2, trait concepts reveal only half the story — Why it is time for a paradigm shift?
251–253. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 47, 1–55.
M.W. Eysenck / Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 209–219 219

Vetterli, C. F., & Furedy, J. J. (1985). Evoked potential correlates of intelligence: Some prob- Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of mood. PsychoIogical
lems with Hendrickson's string measure of evoked potential complexity and error Bulletin, 98, 219–235.
theory of intelligence. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 1–3. Wood, D. (2015). Testing the lexical hypothesis: Are socially important traits more dense-
Vogel, F., Kruger, J., Schalt, E., Schnobel, R., & Hassling, L. (1987). No consistent relation- ly reflected in the English lexicon? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108,
ships between oscillations and latencies or visually and auditorily evoked EEG poten- 317–335.
tials and measures of mental performance. Human Neurobiology, 6, 173–182. Young, G. B., & Pigott, S. E. (1999). Neurobiological basis of consciousness. Archives of
Vukasović, T., & Bratko, D. (2015). Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior Neurology, 56, 153–157.
genetic studies. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 769–785.

You might also like