You are on page 1of 8

Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Full length article

Celebrity's self-disclosure on Twitter and parasocial relationships: A


mediating role of social presence
Jihyun Kim a, *, 1, Hayeon Song b, **, 2
a
School of Communication Studies, Kent State University, Kent, OH 44242, USA
b
College of Business, Global Business Track, Gachon University, 1342 Seongnamdaueo, Sujeong-gu, Seongnam-si, Gyeonggi-do, Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study investigated how celebrities' self-disclosure on personal social media accounts, particularly
Received 1 August 2015 Twitter, affects fans' perceptions. An online survey was utilized among a sample of 429 celebrity fol-
Received in revised form lowers on Twitter. Results demonstrated that celebrities' professional self-disclosure (e.g., sharing their
8 March 2016
work-related life), personal self-disclosure (e.g., sharing their personal life such as friends and family),
Accepted 31 March 2016
and fans' retweeting behavior, enhanced fans’ feeling of social presence, thereby positively affecting
parasocial interaction with celebrities. Further, the study found that the effects of self-disclosure and
retweeting on parasocial interaction were mediated by social presence. Implications and future research
Keywords:
Celebrities
directions are provided.
Parasocial interaction © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Self-disclosure
Social media
Social presence
Twitter

1. Introduction As Ms. Silverman's story indicates, celebrities share a variety of


information about themselves on personal social media. For
In September of 2013, Ms. Sarah Silverman's (best known as a example, celebrities share personal life stories about friends and
comedian) tweet gained much attention from fans when she family as well as professional life to advertise upcoming events
shared sad news about passing of her dog, Duck. Ms. Silverman (e.g., see Hambrick, Simmons, Greenhalgh, & Greenwell, 2010;
later posted a long and emotional tribute to Duck, an obituary type Kassing & Sanderson, 2010; Stever & Lawson, 2013). With easy
story on her blog. By referring Duck as “my best friend”, she shared access to direct and interactive communication methods with ce-
her personal life story with millions of fans via her personal social lebrities, fans following celebrities through social media may feel
media. When Ms. Silverman posted this tweet, she has received like they “know” the celebrity and experience increased intimacy
countless responses such as “… I don't know you personally, of and a strong parasocial relationship.
course, but I send you a virtual {{hug}}.” Ms. Silverman might not There has been some research exploring the use of social media
have a real, personal relationship with most of those people who between celebrities and fans. Thus far, the focus has been mostly on
responded to her tweet. However, her disclosure of aspects of her either the content of celebrities' social media (e.g., Frederick, Lim,
personal life on her social media influenced her fans to relate their Clavio, & Walsh, 2012; Kassing & Sanderson, 2010; Stever &
emotions to Ms. Silverman. Lawson, 2013) or fans' motivations and reasons for following ce-
lebrities' social media from uses and gratification perspectives (e.g.,
Hargittai & Litt, 2011; Sanderson, 2011). However, almost little is
* Corresponding author. School of Communication Studies, Kent State University,
known about how celebrities' various self-disclosure styles on so-
P.O. Box 5190, Kent, OH 44242, USA.
** Corresponding author. College of Business, Gachon University, 1342 Seong- cial media affect fans’ perceptions about those celebrities. Thus, this
namdaueo, Sujeong-gu, Seongnam-si, Gyeonggi-do, Republic of Korea. study begins to fill this research gap.
E-mail addresses: jihyunkim218@gmail.com (J. Kim), hayeon.song@gmail.com Specifically, the current study examines how celebrities' self-
(H. Song). disclosure, particularly professional self-disclosure (revealing in-
1
Jihyun Kim (Ph.D., University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, 2012) is an Assistant
Professor in the School of Communication Studies at Kent State University.
formation about their professional life) and personal self-disclosure
2
Hayeon Song (Ph.D., University of Southern California, 2008) is an Associate (revealing information about their personal life), on personal social
Professor in the College of Business at Gachon University. media, particularly Twitter, influence fans' parasocial relationship

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.083
0747-5632/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577 571

with celebrities. Further, given that one of the unique features of concerning family-, friends-related issues, or personal beliefs might
Twitter is the function of ‘retweeting’, the study also explores how be viewed as personal self-disclosure. Depth refers to the degree of
fans' act of retweeting celebrities' tweets would affect fans' per- disclosure in a specific area of an individual's life. That is, depending
ceptions of parasocial relationship with celebrities. Lastly, a on how deep an individual discloses him/herself about a certain
considerable amount of research has revealed that social presence topic, depth of self-disclosure could vary from a superficial to
is one of the key underlying mechanisms that facilitate people's intimate level (Altman & Taylor, 1973; Derlega et al., 1993; Jourard,
experiences in a mediated-communication environment (e.g., Jin, 1971).
2009, 2011; Jin & Park, 2009; Lee & Nass, 2005; Lee, Park, & Through social media, many contemporary celebrities share
Song, 2005; Lee, Peng, Yan, & Jin, 2006; Song, Kim, & Lee, 2014). their life with their fans (Marshall, 2010), and topics of stories that
Therefore, the study examines the role of social presence, particu- celebrities share with fans might appear in various forms. Given
larly as a mediator that links celebrities' self-disclosure and fans' act that celebrities need fans' support for their career success, some
of retweeting celebrities' tweets to fans' perceptions of parasocial celebrities might disclose their professional life more than other
relationship. parts of their life to promote their work (e.g., new album, new
movie). Stever and Lawson (2013) investigated a number of famous
2. Literature review celebrities' on Twitter. One of the main themes found in the study
was professional self-disclosure. Specifically, the study found that
2.1. Celebrities’ use of personal social media and self-disclosure Katy Perry, an American singer and songwriter, was mostly focused
on sharing her work-related information on her Twitter. In a similar
2.1.1. Celebrities’ social media use vein, another study (Hambrick et al., 2010) also reported that one of
“Social media are inherently designed to facilitate human the main themes found on sport celebrities’ tweets was concerned
connection” (Sanderson, 2011, p. 494). Because communicating with their work (e.g., information sharing or providing insights
with fans on social media does not necessarily require a special about their team or game).
permission to access, social media have become an increasingly Some celebrities might mainly reveal personal life stories on
popular option for celebrities who want to communicate directly social media (e.g., Click et al., 2013; Hambrick et al., 2010; Marshall,
with their fans (Click, Lee, & Holladay, 2013; Stever & Lawson, 2010; Stever & Lawson, 2013). Stever and Lawson noted that Kristen
2013). Chenoweth, an American actress and singer, was found to share a
Traditionally, celebrities delivered their message to fans through significant portion of her personal life on Twitter. Similarly, some
traditional media outlets (e.g., television, news paper) indirectly via sport celebrities also disclose non-sport-related life stories such as
reporters that might have filtered the original messages. However, dinner menu, favorite restaurants, and movies they want to see
social media platforms provide a personalized and unfiltered (Hambrick et al., 2010). Politicians have also been found to disclose
method of communication. Consequently, these platforms have personal life events on social media (Jackson & Lilleker, 2011; Park,
significantly influenced communication patterns between celeb- 2010; Small, 2010).
rities and fans. Many celebrities are establishing their own news
channel through their personal social media such as Twitter (Brau, 2.2. Social presence
2013). A good number of celebrities use such platforms for
expression of dissent (Sanderson, 2009) and self-presentation With its capacity of direct and interactive communication, social
(Sanderson, 2008). media may provide fans with a strong feeling of ‘being together’ or
Similarly, Twitter has been used to provide athletes and their ‘being connected’ with the celebrity (Stever & Lawson, 2013). That
fans with an opportunity to communicate more interpersonally and is, through mediated communication, fans ‘meet’ celebrities and
to enhance the overall sport experiences (Frederick et al., 2012; learn about their daily life, whether it is one-way or two-way
Kassing & Sanderson, 2010). Kassing and Sanderson found that communication. Through this interaction, fans might feel as if
athletes used Twitter to share opinions and comments about the those celebrities are socially present in their life.
sport event, to foster interactive communication with fans, and to Described as ‘being aware of the other person in online environ-
cultivate perspectives about the event among fans. ments’ (Short, Williams, & Christie, 1976), social presence is one of
the important concepts to the understanding of mediated-
2.1.2. Celebrities’ self-disclosure communication experiences (e.g., Biocca, Harms, & Burgoon,
Social Penetration Theory (SPT) explains that human relation- 2003; Short et al., 1976). Numerous scholars have explored this
ships develop through revealing the self, and this process primarily notion since Short et al.’s research (e.g., Biocca et al., 2003; Biocca &
occurs through self-disclosure; the voluntary sharing of personally Nowak, 2001; Caspi & Blau, 2008; Edwards, Edwards, Spence, &
relevant information such as feelings, thoughts, values, and beliefs Westerman, 2015; Lee, 2013; Lee et al., 2005; Song et al., 2014;
with another (Altman & Taylor, 1973). SPT holds a perspective that Spence, Westerman, Edwards, & Edwards, 2014; Tu, 2000).
the self consists of several layers from peripheral layers (i.e., de- Although a definition of social presence has not been agreed upon
mographic information) to central layers (i.e., personal values, self- yet, social presence is generally understood as perceptual experi-
concept). And, revealing each layer of the self to others, the act of ence of being psychologically involved in the interaction with
self-disclosure, is an important step in building a relationship another in a mediated environment (Biocca et al., 2003; Durlach &
(Altman & Taylor, 1973). Given that self-disclosure is a complex Slater, 2000; Schroeder, 2006).
multidimensional concept explained by a variety of features and
characteristics of individuals, researchers have suggested and 2.2.1. Self-disclosure and social presence
investigated various types of self-disclosure. A majority of the SPT Several studies have examined what makes people to feel social
scholars highlight two dimensions of self-disclosure, breadth and presence or related concepts such as presence (a broader category
depth (e.g., Altman & Taylor, 1973; Derlega, Metts, Petronio, & of social presence; see Lee, 2004) (e.g., IJsselsteijn, Ridder, Freeman,
Margulis, 1993; Jourard, 1971). Breadth refers to the variety of & Avons, 2000; Lee & Nass, 2005). Research identifies a few cate-
topics of self-disclosure, thus a wide range of topics or a narrow gories of causal factors for social presence: media/technology-
range may be disclosed. For example, topics related to profession or related factor, user factors (Lombard & Ditton, 1997), and social
work might be seen as professional self-disclosure, and topics factors (Lee & Nass, 2005). Media/Technology-related factors are
572 J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577

concerned with media/technology features such as image size, following hypothesis.


quality, and vividness. User factors include media user character-
H2: The act of retweeting celebritie's tweets increases fans'
istics such as demographics, familiarity with media, and media use
feeling of social presence.
patterns. Social factors, a particular interest of this investigation,
refer to users’ social response toward media.
Social factors are an important factor for an extended under-
standing of social presence research (Lee & Nass, 2005). Lee and 2.2.2. Social presence and parasocial interaction experiences
Nass’ theory driven research found that individuals hearing a Confirming the argument that social presence affects the nature
computer voice manifesting a personality similar to theirs experi- of the interaction (Short et al., 1976), social presence has been
enced stronger social presence compared to others hearing a voice identified to facilitate individuals’ psychological and physiological
manifesting a dissimilar personality. Their study also found that responses in a mediated environment (Lombard & Ditton, 1997). In
people felt stronger social presence when they heard an extrovert particular, a great deal of empirical research reveals a positive
voice than an introvert voice (Lee & Nass, 2005). relationship between social presence and various relationship-
Moreover, the importance of social factors in fostering social related outcomes such as relationship satisfaction (e.g., Moore &
presence has been documented in the online education research. In Kearsley, 2005; Richardson & Swan, 2003) and perceived trust-
particular, Tu (2000) argues that social factors such as social and worthiness (Bente, Rüggenberg, Kra €mer, & Eschenburg, 2008).
informal relationships and trust influence a feeling of social pres- In a particular context of the current study, given that celebrities
ence among online students. Additionally, humor (Eggins & Slade, use social media to make their relationships with fans more real
1997), warmth (Rourke, Anderson, Garrison, & Archer, 2001), and (Stever & Lawson, 2013), it seems necessary to explore how social
perceived self-disclosure (Rourke et al., 2001; Swan & Shih, 2005) presence is related to parasocial interaction (PSI) experiences.
have been also addressed as important social factors that induce Generally, PSI is defined as an imaginary relationship to a person
strong social presence in online learning environments. whom one knows well but who barely knows the other (Horton &
Compared to other causal factors for social presence, social Wohl, 1956). That is, it is the relationship that individuals might
factors have received relatively less attention. Particularly, at the develop with media figures including celebrities, news hosts, and
time of writing this report, none has investigated how self- even fictional characters (e.g., Horton & Wohl, 1956; Reeves & Nass,
disclosure, one of the social factors, influences social presence in 1996; Rubin & McHugh, 1987). Because social media such as Twitter
a context of celebrity-fan relationships. As discussed earlier, a sig- allow people to respond to celebrities' messages, some people
nificant number of celebrities use personal social media and might feel and act like they were in a typical interpersonal rela-
communicate with fans on a variety of topics (e.g., Frederick et al., tionship. However, “users can follow one another's content without
2012; Kassing & Sanderson, 2010; Stever & Lawson, 2013). reciprocal obligation” (Hargittai & Litt, 2011, p. 825), which is
Acknowledging popular use of disclosing themselves on social especially common in the celebrity's social media use context. That
media and importance of social factors in fostering a feeling of is, it would still be perceived as one-sided communication, as the
social presence, the current investigation examines the effect of one party (celebrity) mostly controls their communication mes-
self-disclosure on fans’ feeling of social presence. Given that each sages (Cohen & Perse, 2003). Thus, it still remains as a parasocial
social media platform (e.g., Twitter, Facebook, Instagram) has its relationship rather than a typical interpersonal relationship.
own unique features, which might influence communication pat- Previous research has indicated the association between social
terns, the current study limits its scope to Twitter only. presence and PSI (e.g., Lee, 2013; Lombard & Ditton, 1997; Rubin,
Perse, & Powell, 1985). In particular, research found that social
H1a-b: Celebrities' (a) professional self-disclosure and (b) per-
presence fosters a feeling of imagined intimacy, which is part of PSI
sonal self-disclosure on Twitter increase fans' feeling of social
experience (Rubin et al., 1985). Directly germane to the current
presence.
study, Lee (2013) examined how people respond to a politician's
Twitter has capacity to provide various communication modes messages on media and found that heightened social presence
between celebrities and fans. In most cases, a majority of fans are enhanced positive PSI experiences and overall evaluation of the
likely to be observers of what celebrities have posted or/and what politician.
others have responded to celebrities' tweets. However, some fans Although the celebrity-fan culture is pervasive, the under-
engage in ‘retweeting,’ an act of posting or forwarding someone's standing of the PSI in the social media domain and social presence
tweets to others. That is, some fans actively distribute celebrities' is still limited. Considering the importance of social presence in a
self-disclosed messages (tweets) to others in their personal mediated environment (Short et al., 1976) and a strong need for
network (e.g., their own followers) and share and discuss their more understanding of PSI on the social media platform (Stever,
opinions about the disclosed messages. 2009), it is important to investigate the association of these two.
Retweeting is an active communication behavior in a sense that Therefore, the study proposes the following hypothesis.
the act of retweeting represents an active reaction to someone's
H3: Feeling of social presence enhances fans' parasocial interac-
communication messages by voluntarily reposting the message to
tion experiences with celebrities.
share with users' own followers. In addition, the act of retweeting
reflects users' active way of showing psychological and emotional A body of research has identified a variety of factors related to
involvement with the content. This active form of communication PSI experiences (e.g., Rubin et al., 1985; Rubin & McHugh, 1987;
is well represented with celebrity Ellen DeGeneres's (a talk show Rubin & Perse, 1987; Savage & Spence, 2014). A meta-analysis
host) tweet. As of 2015, Ms. DeGeneres's tweet hit the record of the (Schiappa, Allen, & Gregg, 2007) reported that TV viewing con-
most famous tweet to date with 3,344,002 retweets and 2,021,170 sumption, desire for watching TV, perceived similarity with TV
favorites (Bennett, 2015). characters, and communication apprehension are positively related
Previous studies indicate that involvement and active partici- to PSI experiences. A study (Savage & Spence, 2014) found that
pation of media use induce strong social presence (McGreevy, 1993; radio listeners tend to have favorable PSI experiences when a radio
Witmer & Singer, 1998). That is, when media users are deeply host reveals personal stories during a show. However, what is not
involved in an activity in the media, they tend to experience strong so clear is how and why such factors are leading to favorable PSI
social presence. In this regard, the current study proposes the experiences.
J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577 573

As discussed earlier, the current study predicts that celebrities' party, participants were asked not to think of political figures for
self-disclosure and fans' involvement in retweeting lead to fans' the purpose of this study.
feeling of social presence of celebrities (H1a-b & H2 of this inves- When participants finished completing the survey, they were
tigation). Then, the study also predicts that the feeling of social redirected to a separate website, independent from the original
presence would facilitate positive PSI experiences (H3). In this re- survey, where they could provide their name and class information.
gard, the current investigation argues that social presence mediates This information was obtained only to provide participants with
the relationship between a celebrity-fan Twitter use (in particular, research participation credit for their course. The use of separate
celebrities' self-disclosure and fans’ retweeting) and PSI websites allowed the researcher to guarantee anonymity.
experiences.
Although specific contexts are different, a mediating role of 3.3. Measures
social presence (or presence, a broader category of social presence;
see Lee, 2004) has been well documented in the current body of The survey questionnaire included a set of measures. Profes-
research (e.g., Jin, 2009, 2011; Jin & Park, 2009; Lee & Nass, 2005; sional self-disclosure (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.73) was measured with
Lee et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2006; Song et al., 2014). Germane to three items, developed for the study (e.g., “This celebrity talks
the context of the current investigation, a study (Lee, 2013) found a about her/his work schedule (e.g., concert, shows),” “…. talks about
conditional mediating role of social presence in a PSI context. Lee joy, passion or enthusiasm for work,” “…. talks about work in
found that social presence mediated the relationship between general”). Personal self-disclosure (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.82) was
seeing a politician's TV interview (vs. Twitter messages) and measured with nine items (e.g., “This celebrity often discloses
favorable PSI experiences among those with a low need for personal things about herself/himself on Twitter,” “This celebrity
cognition. In this regard, the current research proposes the talks about her/his romantic partners,” “… talks about personal
following hypothesis. habits,” “… talks about personal life history”). Two items were
adopted from Wheeless (1978) and seven items were developed for
H4a-c: Social presence mediates the relationship between a ce-
the study. Responses for both professional and personal self-
lebrity-fan Twitter use e in particular, (a) professional self-disclo-
disclosure were obtained on a 7-point Likert-type scale
sure, (b) personal self-disclosure, (c) fans' retweeting behavior e
(1 ¼ Never, 7 ¼ Always).
and parasocial interaction experiences.
Retweeting was measured by asking the following question,
“How often do you retweet this celebrity's tweet in an average
3. Methods week?” Response was obtained on a 5-point scale (1 ¼ None,
5 ¼ Very often). Social presence (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.87) was measured
3.1. Participants with five items using a semantic differential scale adopted from
Lombard and Ditton (2000) and Short et al. (1976) (e.g., “I feel that
A total of 572 undergraduate students from a large introductory this celebrity is remote—immediate,” “… unemotional—
communication course at a public university in the United States emotional,” “… impersonal—personal”). Responses for social pres-
were contacted for this study. For the purpose of the current study, ence were obtained on a 7-point semantic differential scale.
celebrity followers on Twitter were only included to the sample of Parasocial interaction (Cronbach's a ¼ 0.82) was assessed with
the study. This created a sample of 429 participants. There were six items selected from Rubin et al. (1985) and slightly modified for
more females (n ¼ 242: 56.4%) than males (n ¼ 172: 40.1%), and 15 the particular context of this research (e.g., “I feel like this celebrity
individuals did not identify their biological sex (3.5%). The average is my friend,” “This celebrity seems to understand the kinds of
age was 19.24 years (SD ¼ 2.42). A majority of the participants were things I want to know about her/himself,” “I like to share some of
Caucasian (n ¼ 355: 82.8%) followed by African American (n ¼ 27: my life (e.g., thoughts, opinions, hobbies) with this celebrity”).
6.3%), Asian (n ¼ 22: 5.1%), and other ethnic groups (n ¼ 25: 5.8%). Responses were obtained on a 7-point Likert-type scale
(1 ¼ Strongly Disagree; 7 ¼ Strongly Agree).
3.2. Procedure
4. Results
An online survey was conducted with university-licensed sur-
vey tools (www.qualtrics.com). The primary researcher contacted a 4.1. Descriptive analyses
course director of the large introductory communication course
and asked whether it would be possible to provide students with A set of simple descriptive analyses was conducted to assess
this research participation opportunity. Upon the director's overall patterns of Twitter use in the sample (n ¼ 429). A majority
approval, a recruitment message that included a link to the survey of participants (n ¼ 275; 64.1%) have used Twitter between 2 and 5
was sent to potential participants through university email. Once years. The rest of the participants reported Twitter use as following:
participants accessed the survey, they were asked to read and 17 participants (4.0%) under 6 month; 34 participants (7.9%) be-
acknowledge the informed consent prior to completing the survey. tween 6 months and 1 year; 72 participants (16.8%) between 1 and
At the start, participants were given with a description of ce- 2 years; and 31 participants (7.2%) over 5 years. On average, par-
lebrities as following: “In general, ‘celebrity’ refers to a person or a ticipants reported spending 46.05 min (SD ¼ 35.08) for reading
group of people who receive popular fame and public attention. For tweets on Twitter and 11.74 min (SD ¼ 15.24) for writing tweets on
example, celebrities would include actors, actresses, singers, co- a daily basis. Regarding the particular celebrity that participants
medians, models, and athletes.” Then, participants were asked to identified for the current study, the following provides a variety of
think of one particular celebrity and to provide the celebrity's name primary job categories of the celebrity: actor/actress (n ¼ 77;
and primary occupation. These questions were asked to trigger 17.9%); singer (n ¼ 164; 38.2%); comedian (n ¼ 24; 5.6%); model
participants' experiences with the particular celebrity's tweets. (n ¼ 17; 4.0%); athlete (n ¼ 84; 19.6%); and other (n ¼ 60; 14%).
Then, participants were asked to answer the rest of the survey
questions based on the particular celebrity. Although political fig- 4.2. Primary analyses
ures can be viewed as celebrities, in order to avoid any potential
effects of favoritism (or anti-favoritism) for a particular political Before conducting hypothesis testing, correlation matrix was
574 J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577

first produced (Table 1). The highest correlation among the five As to H4b, a sobel test indicated that social presence mediates
variables (professional self-disclosure, personal self-disclosure, the relationship between professional self-disclosure and PSI ex-
retweeting, social presence, and parasocial interaction) was 0.42 periences (b ¼ 0.97; z ¼ 3.95; p < 0.001; CI ¼ [0.05, 0.14]). A
(correlation between social presence and parasocial interaction). bootstrap test also confirmed the finding with a mean score for the
Thus, no multicollinearity problem was observed because the score indirect effect of 0.97 (SE ¼ 0.03; CI ¼ [0.04, 0.16]). Subsequent
is lower than the recommended threshold of 0.7 (Tabachnick & evaluation of regression analysis indicated a significant relationship
Fidell, 2001). between personal self-disclosure and PSI experiences (b ¼ 0.18,
To test H1eH3, a path analysis was conducted using Lisrel 9.1 t(404) ¼ 3.25, p < 0.01). However, when controlling for social
program. H1a-b predicted that celebrities' (a) professional and (b) presence, the relationship was no longer significant (b ¼ 0.08,
personal self-disclosure increase fans’ feeling of social presence. t(404) ¼ 1.58, p > 0.05). Thus, full mediation of social presence
Two types of self-disclosure were included in the model. Both occurs between personal self-disclosure and PSI experiences. Data
professional (b ¼ 0.22, t ¼ 4.15, p < 0.05) and personal self- supported H4b.
disclosure (b ¼ 0.11, t ¼ 2.17, p < 0.05) were found to have signifi- With regard to H4c, a sobel test indicated social presence as a
cantly positive associations with social presence. Thus, H1a and mediator between retweeting and PSI experiences (b ¼ 0.96;
H1b were supported. z ¼ 2.94; p < 0.01; CI ¼ [0.03, 0.16]). A bootstrap test confirmed the
H2 was concerned with retweeting and social presence. Results finding with a mean score for the indirect effect of 0.97 (SE ¼ 0.03;
found that the act of retweeting a celebrity's tweets significantly CI ¼ [0.03, 0.17]). Evaluation of regression analysis indicated that
induced fans' feeling of social presence (b ¼ 0.09, t ¼ 1.80, p ¼ 0.05). there is a significant relationship between retweeting and PSI ex-
Thus, H2 was supported. periences (b ¼ 0.53, t(418) ¼ 6.85, p < 0.001). When controlling for
Next, H3 predicted a relationship between social presence and social presence, although the relationship remained significant, it
parasocial interaction (PSI) experiences. Data confirmed that social decreased (b ¼ 0.43, t(418) ¼ 6.03, p < 0.001). Overall, findings
presence is positively related to PSI experiences with the celebrity indicated that partial mediation of social presence occurs between
(b ¼ 0.42, t ¼ 9.02, p < 0.05). H3 was supported. See Fig. 1 for retweeting and PSI experiences. H4c was supported. See Table 2 for
H1eH3. all steps of regression coefficients for H4a-c.
To test H4a-c concerning a mediating role of social presence, a
SPSS Macro, which produces sobel tests, bootstrap tests, and 5. Discussion
regression tests, was used (see Preacher & Hayes, 2004, 2008).
While a sobel test assumes normal distribution, a bootstrap pro- 5.1. Primary findings
cedure imposes no distributional assumption. For a safe approach
to results, both tests were assessed. Unstandardized indirect effects The current investigation discovered new findings in the area of
were computed with 3000 bootstrapped samples. Results for both a research on celebrities' self-disclosure on Twitter, retweeting, social
sobel test and bootstrapping were interpreted with the 95% confi- presence, and parasocial interaction (PSI) experiences. First, find-
dence interval (CI). Further, regression coefficients were evaluated ings demonstrated that celebrities' self-disclosure on Twitter en-
to determine whether it is full or partial mediation. Full mediation hances fans' feeling of social presence. That is, when celebrities
occurs when the effect of IV on DV becomes statistically non- share their life and directly communicate about theses experiences,
significant when controlling for a mediator. And, partial media- fans tend to feel as if those celebrities were socially present in their
tion occurs when the effect of IV on DV decreases although it still life. Although the context is different, this finding is consistent to
remains significant when controlling for a mediator. previous research that examined self-disclosure and social pres-
Regarding H4a, a sobel test indicated that social presence me- ence in a virtual learning environment between a teacher and
diates the relationship between professional self-disclosure and PSI students (e.g., Rourke et al., 2001; Swan & Shih, 2005). Additionally,
experiences (b ¼ 0.10; z ¼ 4.67; p < 0.001; CI ¼ [0.06, 0.14]). A a retweeting act is found to positively influence social presence.
bootstrap test confirmed social presence as a mediator with a mean That is, actively involved fans in sharing celebrities’ self-disclosed
score for the indirect effect of 0.10 (SE ¼ 0.03; CI ¼ [0.06, 0.16]). The messages tend to experience strong social presence of celebrities.
results of subsequent evaluation of regression analysis indicated a Further, social presence is found to facilitate positive PSI expe-
significant relationship between professional self-disclosure and riences. In the specific context of this investigation, results found
PSI experiences (b ¼ 0.28, t(415) ¼ 5.93, p < 0.001). When con- that when fans feel celebrities to be socially present, they tend to
trolling for social presence, although the relationship remained develop imaginary interpersonal relationships with celebrities as if
significant, the coefficient decreased (b ¼ 0.18, t(415) ¼ 3.93, they were friends in their real life. This finding on social presence
p < 0.001). These findings indicate that partial mediation of social and PSI is consistent to previous research (e.g., Lee, 2013; Lombard
presence occurs between professional self-disclosure and PSI ex- & Ditton, 1997; Rubin et al., 1985).
periences. Overall, H4a was supported. The most meaningful highlight of this investigation is a signif-
icant mediating role of social presence. In addition to the above-
mentioned associations with social presence, the study revealed
Table 1 that social presence mediates the relationship between celebrities'
Zero-order correlations, means, and standard deviations. Twitter use and fans' PSI experiences. Importantly, personal self-
1 2 3 4 5
disclosure is found to be a full mediator. If merely the association
between celebrities’ self-disclosure and PSI experiences were
1 Professional SD 1
examined, the argument might simply observe and conclude that
2 Personal SD 0.37** 1
3 Retweeting 0.18** 0.20** 1 the way celebrities disclose themselves might be a direct cause of
4 Social Presence 0.28** 0.21** 0.15** 1 PSI experiences. However, this study revealed that it is because of
5 Parasocial Interaction 0.28** 0.17** 0.32** 0.42** 1 social presence. That is, when taking social presence into the
M 4.46 3.64 1.64 4.9 4.40 equation, the relationship between personal self-disclosure and PSI
SD 1.22 1.10 0.73 1.16 1.23 experience no longer exists. Further, although it is partial media-
Note 1. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. tion, social presence also mediates the relationship between pro-
Note 2. SD refers to self-disclosure. fessional self-disclosure and PSI, and the relationship between
J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577 575

Professional
SD 0.22*
0.11* 0.42*
Social Parasocial
Personal SD
Presence Interaction

Retweeting 0.09*

Note1. *p < .05

Note2. SD refers to self-Disclosure


Fig. 1. Path model (H1a-b, H2, and H3).

Table 2 explored how celebrities' self-disclosure on personal social media


Regression analyses for mediation tests (H4a-c). affects fans’ perceptions toward the celebrities, particularly in
b t
relation to social presence. In this regard, this research finding
provides a new understanding of communication phenomena by
Professional SD (X) YX 0.28 5.93*** Partial mediation
unveiling the understudied area of research.
MX 0.26 5.77***
YM.X 0.39 8.08*** Furthermore, the study makes meaningful contributions to so-
YX.M 0.18 3.93*** cial presence research. While media/technology factors have been
Personal SD (X) YX 0.18 3.25** Full mediation widely and extensively studied since Short et al.’s (1976) research,
MX 0.23 4.48***
social factors as a theoretical causal factor have not received much
YM.X 0.42 8.59***
YX.M 0.08 1.58
attention until Lee and Nass’ (2005) theoretical argument. By
Retweeting (X) YX 0.53 6.85*** Partial mediation investigating self-disclosure in relation to social presence, the
MX 0.24 3.14** current study strengthens the importance of social factors for
YM.X 0.40 8.76*** fostering social presence. The study makes a further contribution to
YX.M 0.43 6.03***
the importance of user factors. As found in this investigation, highly
Note 1. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. involved social media users, in particular, those who engage in
Note 2. SD refers to self-disclosure.
retweeting, tend to feel stronger social presence. This finding pro-
Note 3. X (Independent variable); Y (Dependent variable) ¼ PSI (Parasocial inter-
action experience); M (Mediating variable) ¼ Social presence.
vides useful implications for user characteristics and social pres-
ence research.
The current study also makes contributions to self-disclosure
retweeting and PSI experiences. That is, the way professional self- research. The theoretical notion of self-disclosure research was
disclosure and retweeting influence PSI experience becomes first developed in an interaction setting where reciprocity is ex-
weak when considering the effect of social presence. Overall, these pected (Altman & Taylor, 1973). Thus, most self-disclosure research
findings indicate the importance of social presence in a context of has been done in contexts of interpersonal relationships in which
PSI experiences. reciprocity is expected such as romantic relationships and teacher-
student relationships. This investigation implies that self-
5.2. Implications and contributions disclosure needs to be further studied in a celebrity-fan relation-
ship where reciprocity might not be always expected or
This investigation reveals several contributions and implications guaranteed.
for research. First, the current research contributes to further The current study also provides practical implications and
expanding social media research to the PSI context. Since the rise of contributions for celebrities. First, celebrities may consider
social media, numerous scholars have examined the role of social communicating with their fans through social media platforms.
media in diverse communication contexts such as interpersonal Given that celebrities' success is largely based on how fans respond,
communication (e.g., Fox, Osborn, & Warber, 2014), communication it is important to consider what facilitates fans' favorable percep-
education (e.g., Evans, 2014), health communication (e.g., tions toward celebrities. The current study's findings suggest that
Antheunis, Tates, & Nieboer, 2013), and crisis communication (e.g., celebrities can benefit from appearing to self-disclose to fans,
Lachlan, Spence, Lin, Najarian, & Del Greco, 2016; Spence, Lachlan, whether it is about professional or personal life.
Lin, & del Greco, 2015; Spence, Lachlan, Edwards, & Edwards, 2016). Second, for celebrities who are reluctant to sharing their life on
However, social media use in PSI has received much less attention personal social media, the current study suggests that they directly
from scholars although celebrity culture is widely embedded in focus on fostering social presence. By helping fans feel like “they are
everyday life. In this regard, the current study contributes to the in my life; I feel so connected,” the study's finding indicates that
understanding of social media research in PSI. fans can enjoy positive PSI experiences, which would be important
Next, the study provides a new perspective for social media and to creating strong fan base.
celebrity research. In the limited extant literature of social media Lastly, the positive effect of professional self-disclosure found in
use among celebrities, much of the research has been focused in this investigation implies that celebrity's personal social media can
areas such as what celebrities share on their social media (e.g., be used as a cost-effective marketing tool. Typically, celebrities and
Frederick et al., 2012; Hambrick et al., 2010; Kassing & Sanderson, their agents employ a variety of media outlets when they promote
2010) and why fans follow celebrities on social media (e.g., events (e.g., movies, concerts, TV shows). In addition to using
Hargittai & Litt, 2011; Sanderson, 2011). However, little has traditional media outlets, which can be expensive, the study's
576 J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577

finding indicates that simple disclosure of celebrities' work or up- Bennett, S. (January, 2015). Ellen, 1D and the president e the 3 most retweeted
Tweets in history. Social Times. Retrieved on May 2015 from http://www.
coming events can attract fans' attention and create increased
adweek.com/socialtimes/most-retweets-twitter/612320.
support. Bente, G., Rüggenberg, S., Kr€ amer, N. C., & Eschenburg, F. (2008). Avatar-mediated
networking: increasing social presence and interpersonal trust in net-based
collaborations. Human Communication Research, 34, 287e318. http://
5.3. Limitations and future research directions dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00322.x.
Biocca, F., Harms, C., & Burgoon, J. K. (2003). Towards a more robust theory and
The directions for future research should be addressed with measure of social presence: review and suggested criteria. Presence: Tele-
operators and Virtual Environments, 12, 456e480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/
limitations identified in this investigation. First, future researchers
105474603322761270.
may explore other aspects of self-disclosure. The current study Biocca, F., & Nowak, K. (2001). Plugging your body into the telecommunication
tested breadth of self-disclosure only, particularly topics related to system: mediated embodiment, media interfaces, and social virtual environ-
ments. Communication Technology and Society, 407e447.
professional and personal life. Given that depth is also another
Brau, M. (2013). Twitter kills the publicity star?: How social media is influencing the
important core concept of self-disclosure (Altman & Taylor, 1973), business of celebrity PR. Germany: Grin Verlag.
future researchers may test effects of depth in each professional Caspi, A., & Blau, I. (2008). Social presence in online discussion groups: testing three
and personal self-disclosure on fans’ perceptions about celebrities. conceptions and their relations to perceived learning. Social Psychology Edu-
cation, 11, 323e334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11218-008-9054-2.
Next, future researchers might explore how celebrities' Click, M. A., Lee, H., & Holladay, H. W. (2013). Making monsters: lady Gaga, fan
emotional (evaluative) versus factual (descriptive) self-disclosure identification, and social media. Popular Music and Society, 36(3), 360e379.
influence fans' perceptions and attitudes. Emotional self- http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03007766.2013.798546.
Cohen, J., & Perse, E. M. (2003, May). Different strokes for different folks: an
disclosure is focused on revealing personal feelings and emotions empirical search for different modes of viewer-character relationships. In Paper
(e.g., “My last break up hurts me so much”) rather than a simple presented at the annual meeting of the international communication association,
factual description of life events (e.g., “He broke up with me”) san Diego, CA.
Derlega, V. J., Metts, S., Petronio, S., & Margulis, S. T. (1993). Self-disclosure. Newbury
(Morton, 1978; Reis & Patrick, 1996). Research has found that Park, CA: Sage.
emotional self-disclosure is more strongly related to relationship Durlach, N., & Slater, M. (2000). Presence in shared virtual environments and virtual
closeness and satisfaction compared to merely sharing self-relevant togetherness. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 9(2), 214e217.
Edwards, C., Edwards, A., Spence, P. R., & Westerman, D. (2015). Initial interaction
facts (e.g., Morton, 1978). That is, although the act of sharing self-
expectations with robots: testing the human-to-human interaction script.
relevant information itself could contribute to fostering intimacy, Communication Studies, 1e12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10510974.2015.1121899.
the particular aspect of emotional self-disclosure has stronger ef- Eggins, S., & Slade, D. (1997). Analyzing casual conversation. Washington, DC: Cassell.
Evans, C. (2014). Twitter for teaching: can social media be used to enhance the
fects on intimacy development (e.g., Greenberg & Safran, 1987;
process of learning? British Journal of Educational Technology, 45(5), 902e915.
Laurenceau, Barrett, & Pietromonaco, 1998; Lin, 1992; Reis & http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12099.
Patrick, 1996). Future research on emotional vs. factual self- Fox, J., Osborn, J. L., & Warber, K. M. (2014). Relational dialectics and social
disclosure would help better understand how celebrities' networking sites: the role of Facebook in romantic relationship escalation,
maintenance, conflict, and dissolution. Computers in Human Behavior, 35,
emotional or factual self-disclosure styles would affect fans’ atti- 527e534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.031.
tudes and perceptions. Frederick, E. L., Lim, C. H., Clavio, G., & Walsh, P. (2012). Why we follow: an ex-
Lastly, future researchers should also address effects of fans' amination of parasocial interaction and fan motivations for following athlete
archetypes on Twitter. International Journal of Sport Communication, 5(4),
emotional responses to celebrities' self-disclosure. Previous 481e502.
research suggests that the way receivers respond to the other Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford
person's self-disclosure plays an important role of fostering in- Press.
Hambrick, M. E., Simmons, J. M., Greenhalgh, G. P., & Greenwell, T. C. (2010). Un-
timacy (Reis, 2007). Taking this approach to the celebrity research, derstanding professional athletes' use of Twitter: a content analysis of athlete
future research should address how fans' liking or disliking a tweets. International Journal of Sport Communication, 3(4), 454e471.
celebrity's self-disclosed message influences fans' overall attitudes. Hargittai, E., & Litt, E. (2011). The tweet smell of celebrity success: explaining
variation in Twitter adoption among a diverse group of young adults. New
Media and Society, 13, 824e842. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1461444811405805.
5.4. Conclusion Horton, D., & Wohl, R. R. (1956). Mass communication and para-social interaction:
observations on intimacy at a distance. Psychiatry, 19, 185e206.
IJsselsteijn, W. A., Ridder, H., Freeman, J., & Avons, S. E. (2000). Presence: concept,
This study investigated how celebrities' self-disclosure on determinants and measurement. In , 3959. Proceedings of the SPIE, human vision
Twitter affects fans' perceptions. Overall, the investigation reveals and electronic imaging V (pp. 520e529).
that celebrities' professional and personal self-disclosure and fans' Jackson, N., & Lilleker, D. (2011). Microblogging, constituency service and impres-
sion management: UK MPs and the use of Twitter. The Journal of Legislative
act of retweeting celebrities' tweets enhance feelings of social Studies, 17, 86e105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13572334.2011.545181.
presence. Further, social presence is found to facilitate favorable PSI Jin, S. (2009). Modality effects in second life: the mediating role of social presence
experiences with celebrities. Social presence plays a role as a and the moderating role of product involvement. Cyber Psychology & Behavior,
12, 717e721. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0273.
mediator that links celebrities' self-disclosure and fans' retweeting
Jin, S. (2011). I feel present. therefore I experience flow: a structural equation
behavior with PSI experiences. Given that little is known in the modeling approach to flow and presence in video games. Journal of Broadcasting
context of celebrities’ social media use, findings from the current & Electronic Media, 55, 114e136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
research are particularly meaningful. This exploratory investigation 08838151.2011.546248.
Jin, S., & Park, N. (2009). Parasocial interaction with my avatar: effects of interde-
calls for more research in this area. pendent self-construal and the mediating role of self-presence in an avatar-
based console game, Wii. Cyber Psychology & Behavior, 12, 723e727. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0289.
Disclosure statement
Jourard, S. M. (1971). Self-disclosure: An experimental analysis of the transparent self.
New York: Wiley.
No competing financial interests exist. Kassing, J. W., & Sanderson, J. (2010). Faneathlete interaction and Twitter tweeting
through the Giro: a case study. International Journal of Sport Communication,
3(1), 113e128.
References Lachlan, K. A., Spence, P. R., Lin, X., Najarian, K., & Del Greco, M. (2016). Social media
and crisis management: CERC, search strategies, and Twitter content. Computers
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal in Human Behavior, 54, 647e652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.05.027.
relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998). Intimacy as an inter-
Antheunis, M. L., Tates, K., & Nieboer, T. E. (2013). Patients' and health professionals' personal process: the importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and
use of social media in health care: motives, barriers and expectations. Patient perceived partner responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Per-
Education and Counseling, 92(3), 426e431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ sonality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1238e1251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
j.pec.2013.06.020. 0022-3514.74.5.1238.
J. Kim, H. Song / Computers in Human Behavior 62 (2016) 570e577 577

Lee, K. (2004). Presence, explicated. Communication Theory, 14, 27e50. http:// Rubin, A. M., & Perse, E. M. (1987). Audience activity and soap opera involvement a
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00302.x. uses and effects investigation. Human Communication Research, 14(2), 246e268.
Lee, E. J. (2013). Effectiveness of politicians' soft campaign on Twitter versus TV: Rubin, A. M., Perse, E. M., & Powell, R. A. (1985). Loneliness, parasocial interaction,
cognitive and experiential routes. Journal of Communication, 63(5), 953e974. and local television viewing. Human Communication Research, 12, 155e180.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12049. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1985.tb00071.x.
Lee, K.-M., & Nass, C. (2005). Social-psychological origins of feelings of presence: Sanderson, J. (2008). You are the type of person that children should look up to as a
creating social presence with machine-generated voices. Media Psychology, 7(1), hero: parasocial interaction on 38pitches.com. International Journal of Sport
31e45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0701_2. Communication, 1(3), 337e360.
Lee, K. M., Park, N., & Song, H. (2005). Can a robot be perceived as a developing Sanderson, J. (2009). Thanks for fighting the good fight: cultivating dissent on
creature?: effects of a robot's long-term cognitive developments on its social blogmaverick.com. The Southern Communication Journal, 74, 390e405. http://
presence and people's social responses toward it. Human Communication dx.doi.org/10.1080/10417940802650153.
Research, 31, 538e563. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2005.tb00882.x. Sanderson, J. (2011). To tweet or not to tweet: exploring division I athletic de-
Lee, K. M., Peng, W., Yan, C., & Jin, S. (2006). Can robots manifest personality?: an partments' social-media policies. International Journal of Sport Communication,
empirical test of personality recognition, social responses, and social presence 4, 492e513.
in human-robot interaction. Journal of Communication, 56, 754e772. http:// Savage, M. E., & Spence, P. R. (2014). Will you listen? an examination of parasocial
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00318.x. interaction and credibility in radio. Journal of Radio & Audio Media, 21(1), 3e19.
Lin, Y. C. (1992). The construction of the sense of intimacy from every- day social in- http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19376529.2014.891214.
teractions. Rochester, New York: University of Rochester. Unpublished doctoral Schiappa, E., Allen, M., & Gregg, P. B. (2007). Parasocial relationships and television:
dissertation. a meta-analysis of the effects. Mass Media Effects Research: Advances Through
Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. B. (1997). At the heart of it all: the concept of presence. Meta-Analysis, 301e314.
Journal of Computer Mediated Communication, 3(2). http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Schroeder, R. (2006). Being there together and the future of connected presence.
j.1083-6101.1997.tb00072.x. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 15(4), 438e454. http://
Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. B. (2000). Measuring presence: a literature-based dx.doi.org/10.1162/pres.15.4.438.
approach to the development of a standardized paper-and-pencil instrument. Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976). The social psychology of telecommuni-
In A Proceeding at Presence: The third international workshop on presence. cation. London: Wiley.
Marshall, P. D. (2010). The promotion and presentation of the self: celebrity as Small, T. (2010). Canadian politics in 140 characters: Party politics in the Twitter-
marker of presentational media. Celebrity Studies, 1(1), 35e48. http://dx.doi.org/ verse. Canadian Parliamentary Review, 33, 39e45. Retrieved from http://www.
10.1080/19392390903519057. revparl.ca/english/issue.asp?param¼199&art¼1397.
McGreevy, M. (1993). Virtual reality and planetary exploration. In A. Wexelblat Song, H., Kim, J., & Lee, K. M. (2014). Virtual body vs. real body in exergames:
(Ed.), Virtual reality: Applications and explorations (pp. 163e197). Academic Press reducing social physique anxiety in exercise experiences. Computers in Human
Inc. Behavior, 36, 282e285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.059.
Moore, M. G., & Kearsley, G. (2005). Distance education: A systems view (2nd ed.). Spence, P. R., Lachlan, K. A., Edwards, A., & Edwards, C. (2016). Tweeting fast mat-
New York: Wadsworth. ters, but only if I think about it: information updates on social media.
Morton, T. L. (1978). Intimacy and reciprocity of exchange: a comparison of spouses Communication Quarterly, 1e17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
and strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 72e81. http:// 01463373.2015.1100644.
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.36.1.72. Spence, P. R., Lachlan, K. A., Lin, X., & del Greco, M. (2015). Variability in Twitter
Park, S.-H. (2010). Twitter as personal political communities and networked public content across the stages of a natural disaster: implications for crisis commu-
sphere. Korean Political Communication Research, 18, 81e115. Retrieved from nication. Communication Quarterly, 63(2), 171e186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
http://www.riss.kr/link?id¼A82403373. 01463373.2015.1012219.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect Spence, P. R., Westerman, D., Edwards, C., & Edwards, A. (2014). Welcoming our
effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & robot overlords: initial expectations about interaction with a robot. Communi-
Computers, 36, 717e731. cation Research Reports, 31(3), 272e280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for 08824096.2014.924337.
assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Stever, G. (2009). Parasocial and social interaction with celebrities: classification of
Research Methods, 40, 879e891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. media fans. Journal of Media Psychology, 14(3), 1e39.
Reeves, B., & Nass, C. (1996). The media equation: How people treat computers, Stever, G. S., & Lawson, K. (2013). Twitter as a way for celebrities to communicate
television, and new media like real people and places. New York: Cambridge with fans: implications for the study of parasocial interaction. North American
University Press. Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 339e354.
Reis, H. T. (2007). Steps toward the ripening of relationship science. Personal Re- Swan, K., & Shih, L. F. (2005). On the nature and development of social presence in
lationships, 14(1), 1e23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00139.x. online course discussions. Journal of Asynchronous Learning Networks, 9(3),
Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: component processes. 115e136.
In E. T. Higgins, & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston:
principles (pp. 523e563). New York: Guilford Press. Allyn & Bacon.
Richardson, J. C., & Swan, K. (2003). Examining social presence in online courses in Tu, C. (2000). On-line learning migration: from social learning theory to social
relation to students' perceived learning and satisfaction. Journal of Asynchro- presence theory in a CMC environment. Journal of Network and Computer Ap-
nous Learning Networks, 7, 68e88. plications, 23, 27e37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jnca.1999.0099.
Rourke, L., Anderson, T., Garrison, D. R., & Archer, W. (2001). Assessing social Wheeless, L. R. (1978). A follow-up study of the relationships among trust, disclo-
presence in asynchronous text-based computer conferencing. Journal of Dis- sure, and interpersonal solidarity. Human Communication Research, 4(2),
tance Education, 14, 1e18. 143e157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1978.tb00604.x.
Rubin, R. B., & McHugh, M. P. (1987). Development of parasocial interaction re- Witmer, B. G., & Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: a
lationships. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 31, 279e292. http:// presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(3),
dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838158709386664. 225e240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/105474698565686.

You might also like