You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [Florida Atlantic University]

On: 20 January 2015, At: 18:21


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Applied Animal Welfare


Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20

Time Budget and Activity Patterns of


Oncilla Cats (Leopardus tigrinus) in
Captivity
a a
Letícia de Souza Resende , Glauce Lima e Neto , Patrícia
a b
Gonçalves Duarte Carvalho , Gabriella Landau-Remy , Valdir de
b a a
Almeida Ramos-Júnior , Artur Andriolo & Gelson Genaro
a
Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Animal
Behavior and Biology , Federal University of Juiz de Fora , Brazil
b
Rio de Janeiro Zoological Park , Rio de Janeiro , Brazil
Click for updates Published online: 31 Jan 2014.

To cite this article: Letícia de Souza Resende , Glauce Lima e Neto , Patrícia Gonçalves Duarte
Carvalho , Gabriella Landau-Remy , Valdir de Almeida Ramos-Júnior , Artur Andriolo & Gelson Genaro
(2014) Time Budget and Activity Patterns of Oncilla Cats (Leopardus tigrinus) in Captivity, Journal of
Applied Animal Welfare Science, 17:1, 73-81, DOI: 10.1080/10888705.2014.856253

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2014.856253

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE, 17:73–81, 2014
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1088-8705 print/1532-7604 online
DOI: 10.1080/10888705.2014.856253

Time Budget and Activity Patterns of Oncilla


Cats (Leopardus tigrinus) in Captivity
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

Letícia de Souza Resende,1 Glauce Lima e Neto,1


Patrícia Gonçalves Duarte Carvalho,1 Gabriella Landau-Remy,2
Valdir de Almeida Ramos-Júnior,2 Artur Andriolo,1 and Gelson Genaro1
1
Department of Biological Sciences, Graduate School of Animal Behavior and Biology,
Federal University of Juiz de Fora, Brazil
2
Rio de Janeiro Zoological Park, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Researchers have reported on the diet of Leopardus tigrinus and ecological aspects, but studies
of behavior are scarce. The aims of this study were to describe the time budget and activity
patterns of 10 captive Leopardus tigrinus individuals. The group had an activity budget of 66%
resting, 20.66% moving, 6.08% vigilant, 3.12% feeding, and 4.14% other activities during 720
hr of observations. The activity budgets of the males and females did not differ significantly;
however, males ate more than did females. The nonhuman animals spent more time resting during
the day than during the night. Moving, socializing, maintenance, and vigilance showed statistically
higher mean values at night. Group analysis of the temporal pattern of behavior showed bimodal
peaks. Activity levels were high from 5 a.m. to 6 a.m. and decreased through the day only to peak
again at 7 p.m. Stereotypic pacing peaked at dawn and at dusk. Patterns of vigilance, feeding,
and maintenance were also determined for the group during a 24-hr period. These results may be
useful for the development of management plans and effective conservation strategies for captive
cats.

Keywords: behavior, felidae, little spotted cat, activity budget, zoo

With rapid loss of species worldwide, long-term maintenance of captive populations has become
a common approach to species conservation. However, scientists have recognized that captivity
can drastically alter nonhuman animal behavior, and in particular, concerns have been expressed
about the possible effect of captivity in the loss of behaviors essential for survival in the wild.
The Leopardus tigrinus (Oncilla; Schreber, 1775) is the smallest Brazilian cat. Males are
generally larger than females; body weight varies from 1.75 kg to 3.5 kg and length varies

Correspondence should be sent to Letícia de Souza Resende, Pós-Graduação em Ciências Biológicas Comporta-
mento e Biologia Animal, Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora, Campus Universitário, Martelos, 36036-900 Juiz de
Fora, Minas Gerais, Brazil. Email: leticiasresende@gmail.com

73
74 RESENDE ET AL.

from 40 cm to 50 cm (Oliveira & Cassaro, 1999). Although information from the field and
captivity indicates that the species is nocturnal (Oliveira, 1994), some food items identified in
specimens from Northeastern Brazil indicate a high degree of daytime activity (Olmos, 1993).
There are several reports on the diet of the oncilla as well as its ecological aspects (Bitetti,
Angelo, Blanco, & Paviolo, 2010; Gardner, 1971; Konecny, 1989; Ximenez, 1982); however,
studies of time budget and activity patterns are scarce.
Nevertheless, activity patterns and time budgets are two important aspects of the temporal
behavior of animals. In the only study of activity patterns in this species, Moreira, Brown,
Moraes, Swanson, and Monteiro-Filho (2007) described a bimodal pattern of activity for
oncillas in captivity with peaks occurring at dusk and dawn. During the night, the cats began
pacing at 10 p.m. and ceased around 1 a.m. During the day, the animals spent most of the
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

time resting in trees or in boxes. The animals showed a reduction in active behavior during the
hottest hours of the day (1 p.m.–4 p.m.).
Probably because of the ease of observation, most studies of activity patterns and time
budget were performed in endothermic, diurnal, and terrestrial animals. Studies concerning
neotropical cats are difficult because of the solitary behavior of these animals and their
preference for nocturnal activities, making reports of field observation sparse (Oliveira &
Cassaro, 1999). Thus, studies on captive populations can provide information about the habits
of such species, and this information is essential for the development of management plans and
effective conservation strategies. Modified activity rhythms have been proposed to explain the
high susceptibility of captive animals after their release into the wild. Small cats in captivity
often remain inactive or hidden from public view. Furthermore, when they are active, they
may frequently engage in stereotypical locomotor patterns (Shepherdson, Carlstead, Mellen, &
Seidenslicker, 1993).
Genaro, Moraes, Silva, Adania, and Franci (2007) reported that oncilla reproduction in
captivity is poor because of several factors, but stress is probably the most important. These
problems suggest a limited understanding of the management conditions and housing require-
ments of small cats in captivity (Carlstead, Brown, & Seidensticker, 1993; Mellen, 1991).
Knowledge about how animals divide up their activities throughout the day is relevant for
understanding their lifestyles and for identifying in a general way how animals interact with
their environment (Defler, 1995). The aims of this study were to describe the time budget and
activity patterns of captive Leopardus tigrinus individuals located at the Center for Reproduction
of the RIOZOO Foundation (Rio de Janeiro Zoological Park).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The study was developed in the Reproduction Center for Small Felids in the Rio de Janeiro Zoo–
RIOZOO Foundation, located at Quinta da Boa Vista Park in the São Cristóvão neighborhood.
The center has a total area of 273 m2 and is divided into nine enclosures. Enclosures (15 m2 )
are made of stone masonry walls, with the exception of the front, which consists of wire mesh.
The floors of these enclosures are covered with dust and concrete substrate. Plants and trunks
are spread randomly, and a wooden box measuring 40 cm3 is located in the upper part, which
is used as a shelter. The animals remain in a restricted area and have contact only with their
caretakers and technicians.
TIME AND ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF ONCILLA CATS 75

We studied 10 adult oncillas (Leopardus tigrinus) placed in five different enclosures: two
enclosures with 2 males, two with a male and a female, and one with 2 females. All the animals
were born in free-living environments and had been in captivity for at least 12 years before
the start of the study. During this study, the animals were fed regularly between 1 p.m. and
3 p.m. with commercial cat food, morsels of chicken, and lab rats, who had been kept in the
laboratory animal housing and killed following the appropriate bioethics protocol.
The behavioral parameters were derived from an ethogram developed by Weller and Bennett
(2001). We later modified it to adapt to our own conditions (Table 1). Behavioral data were
collected in January and February 2007 by using two microcameras (Sharp CCD, 420TVL,
0.1 l day/night) installed inside the enclosures. The behavior of each animal was recorded
24 hr per day during 3 consecutive days, totaling 720 hr of observation. Behavioral data were
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

collected from the videotaped records using a scan methodology (Altmann, 1974), with 5-min

TABLE 1
Mean Percentage of Time Behaviors Displayed by Leopardus tigrinus Kept in the Center
for Small Felids–RIOZOO Foundation (Total Observation Time Was 720 min)

Mean Mean
Percentage/ Percentage/
Behavior Definition Day % Night % p

Resting 79.51 ˙ 10.51% 54.33 ˙ 7.44% .002


Lying Subject is resting on haunches, fully extended on
the ground, and may have eyes open or closed
Inside the box Animal inside a wooden box
Sitting Subject in bipedal position, with front legs resting
against vertical surface
Moving 12.59 ˙ 1.47% 28.05 ˙ 1.48% .002
Jumping Subject leaps from one point to another, either
vertically or horizontally
Pacing Repetitive walking without apparent aim
Locomotion Movement toward a specific direction with an aim
Exploring Animal moving attentively, normally sniffing the
ground and the walls
Vigilant Alert, attentive standing or sitting 2.34 ˙ 1.79% 9.67 ˙ 2.73% .002
Feeding 3.42 ˙ 1.47% 2.83 ˙ 1.98% .690
Drinking Animal drinking water
Eating Grinding an object, usually with canines
Hunting All the behavioral acts involved in hunting,
lurking, stalking, and killing the prey
Maintenance 1.75 ˙ 2.34% 2.91 ˙ 2.47% .050
Sharpen claws Front claws are used to scratch on object
Cheek rub Cheek is rubbed against an object or another cat
Urinating Animal urinating; can be related to territory
demarcation
Defecating Animal releasing feces
Scratching body Animal uses the claws of hind legs to rub
Socializing 0.25 ˙ 0.03% 1.67 ˙ 0.56% .050
Playing Usually the individual interacts with the food (live 0.14 ˙ 0.02% 0.55 ˙ 0.61% .080
prey), throwing it into the air and chasing it
76 RESENDE ET AL.

intersample intervals totaling 12 scans per hr for each individual. Four 40-kW red lamps aided
nocturnal image recording.
To describe the time budget and the activity patterns, the behaviors observed were divided
into seven categories: resting, moving, vigilant, feeding, maintenance, socializing, and playing.
All behavioral categories were classified as active, with the exception of resting. The number
of scans of each animal performing behaviors from each category was reported. Means for
each category were calculated for each animal before averaging totals for the group.
Because the behavioral data recorded were not normally distributed, nonparametric statistics
were used for data analyses. A Friedman’s test was used to analyze differences in time spent
in each behavioral category. The Wilcoxon test was used to verify significant differences in
mean percentages of time devoted to behavioral categories during the day (7 a.m.–6:55 p.m.)
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

versus the night hours (7 p.m.–6:55 a.m.). The Mann-Whitney U Test was used to test the
differences between males and females. All tests were performed in BioEstat 2.0 software, and
the significance level adopted was 5% (p < .05). The display of behavioral states, as recorded in
the instantaneous scans, has been expressed as the percentage of time spent in these particular
states of the total time that an individual was observed.

RESULTS

Leopardus tigrinus individuals spent the largest portion of the time (66%) resting. Moving
(20.66%) and vigilance (6.08%) were the second and third most common activities, respectively,
followed by feeding (3.12%). Maintenance, socializing, and playing made up only 4.14% of
their activities. The behavioral categories analyzed yielded values that differed significantly for
duration of the behavior (Friedman, 2 D 123, 27; p < .001; Figure 1).
The animals spent more time resting during the day than during the night (Wilcoxon, Z D
3.05, p D .002). Moving, socializing, maintenance, and vigilance showed statistically higher
mean values at night. There was no significant difference between time period and the mean
percentage of time spent feeding and playing (Table 1).
There also was no significant difference between males and females in time spent in
locomotion (Mann-Whitney, U D 0.68, p D .490) and maintenance (U D 1.21, p D .220),
being social (U D 1.27, p D .200), resting (U D 1.43, p D .150), being vigilant (U D 1.50,
p D .130), and playing (U D 1.85, p D .060). However, the Mann-Whitney Test showed a
significant difference between the mean percentage of time devoted to feeding and the sex of
the individuals. Males spent more time eating than did females (U D 2.34, p D .010).
Group analysis of the temporal pattern of active behavior showed bimodal peaks for this
behavior. Activity levels were high from 5 a.m. to 6 a.m. and gradually decreased through the
day only to peak again at 7 p.m. Stereotypic pacing peaked at dawn (5 a.m.–6 a.m.) and at dusk
(6 p.m.–7 p.m.). Higher proportions of resting behavior were exhibited in the morning at 9 a.m.
Patterns of vigilance, feeding, and maintenance were also determined for the group during a
24-hr period. The animals exhibited high percentages of vigilant behavior at dawn (5 a.m.),
which decreased during the day and increased again at dusk (7 p.m.). High percentages of
vigilant behavior were also reported around 1 a.m. Feeding behavior occurred mainly during
the feeding time period (1 p.m.–3 p.m.) and at dusk (6 p.m.–7 p.m.). Maintenance peaked at
1 a.m. (Figure 2).
TIME AND ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF ONCILLA CATS 77
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

FIGURE 1 Time budget (percentage of time allocated to different activities) of Leopardus tigrinus housed
in the Reproduction Center for Small Felids–RIOZOO Foundation (November 2006 through June 2007). Bars
represent mean ˙ standard error (SE).

DISCUSSION

The finding that Leopardus tigrinus in captivity spent 66% of their time resting is consistent
with Baldwin (1985), who found that captive cats in the National Zoo in the United States
rested 75% of the time. Also, Pitsko (2003) observed that tigers in captivity spent a majority
(76%) of their time resting. In the natural environment, small cats rested 52.5% of the time
(Crawshaw & Quigley, 1989). Ludlow and Sunquist (1987) observed that wild ocelots were
inactive from 41% to 50% of the time during a 24-hr period.
The higher percentage of time in which this behavior was observed in captive cats may be
an indication of the low stimulus diversity in this environment. According to Carlstead (1996),
chronically understimulated animals in captivity depress their needs for stimulation by lowering
their expectations of the level of stimulatory input from their surroundings.
Comparing the total amount of time resting during the day and night hours, Leopardus
tigrinus spent more time resting during the day than during the night. According to Navarro-
Lopez (1985) and Crawshaw (1995), free-living neotropical small cats are active 52% to 92% of
the hours of the night, and foraging behavior encompasses most of the activities in this period.
Our results are consistent with these data obtained for free-living small cats and indicate the
maintenance of nocturnal activity even in captivity.
The second most frequent behavioral category observed was moving. In the natural en-
vironment, small cats travel long distances searching for food, shelter, and potential mates.
Ludlow and Sunquist (1987) recorded the distance traveled by ocelots in the wild, which
averaged around 0.45 km per hr. In captivity, the need that animals have to move may be
related to the high frequency of stereotypic pacing that is sometimes observed. In this study,
the pacing behavior accounted for 64.83% of the behaviors observed in the moving category. To
78 RESENDE ET AL.
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

(a)

(b)

FIGURE 2 Temporal pattern of behaviors observed in individual Leopardus tigrinus housed in the
Reproduction Center for Small Felids–RIOZOO Foundation (November 2006 through June 2007). Activity
and pacing (a), resting and moving (b), and vigilant, feeding, and maintenance (c). (continued )
TIME AND ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF ONCILLA CATS 79
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

(c)

FIGURE 2 (Continued ).

investigate whether variation in the welfare of different species could arise from a differential
impact of captivity on their natural behaviors, Clubb and Mason (2003) calculated the mean
frequency of stereotypic pacing by 35 species of captive carnivores and its relationships with
a number of variables. Clubb and Mason (2003) showed that the degree of natural foraging
and general activity levels did not predict captive stereotypy; in contrast, daily travel distances
were positively correlated with pacing.
Many species pace, which is thought to have a variety of causes. For example, pacing
may arise when animals are consistently unable to meet a need, such as natural feeding
behavior (Carlstead, 1996; Shepherdson et al., 1993). According to Broom and Johnson (1993),
animals who spend long portions of time engaged in stereotyped behaviors show a reduction
in behavioral diversity, disintegration of the natural behavioral repertoire, and an inability to
interact typically with new stimuli. In such cases, the presence of the stereotyped behavior
provides direct evidence of poor welfare and suffering.
The oncillas moved significantly more during the night, which is consistent with other results
obtained for free-living small cats. For example, Konecny (1989) reported an average distance
traveled of 273 m per hr for an individual male Felis wiedii in Belize. According to the author,
this rate ranged from 197 m per hr during daylight hours to 305 m per hr at night.
The animals spent a mean of 3.12% of the time feeding in the current study. Cats typically eat
10 to 20 small meals throughout the day and night (Macdonald, 1983). In a study conducted in
Venezuela, Ludlow and Sunquist (1987) recorded that the ocelot diet consisted of 65% of prey
weighing less than 100 g, which corresponds to 1% of the animal’s body weight. Similarly,
Emmons, Sherman, Bolster, Goldizen, and Terborgh (1989) noted that at least 60% of the
prey of ocelots in Peru weighed less than 300 g. For that reason, repeated cycles of hunting
throughout the day and night are required to provide sufficient food for a typical cat. These
data explain the absence of a significant difference in the percentage of time spent feeding
80 RESENDE ET AL.

during the day and night and indicate the need for these animals to feed on small amounts of
food several times a day.
The activity patterns of Leopardus tigrinus identified here are congruent with those of other
small felids. In his study on captive carnivores, Baldwin (1985) recorded a strong morning peak
and a small late-afternoon peak in the daily activity pattern of felids. Moreira et al. (2007)
reported a bimodal pattern of activity for captive L. tigrinus and L. wiedii, with peaks at dusk
and dawn. Small felids in the wild are crepuscular by nature, being active in the early mornings
and late evenings and resting through the hotter hours of the day (Bailey, 1993). The peaks
in activity and resting behavior observed in the daily activity budget of captive oncillas were
likely the result of these crepuscular tendencies.
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

CONCLUSION

Overall, our results indicate that Leopardus tigrinus in captivity retain similarities to the activity
patterns seen in wild individuals, including nocturnal activity. This information is essential
to reintroduction programs. The success of release programs of captive-bred or translocated
animals depends to a large extent upon their behavioral skills. However, the captive cats were
more sedentary than their wild counterparts and exhibited the stereotypic behavior of pacing.
Management plans for these animals in captivity should include the use of environmental
enrichment techniques, especially during the dusk and dawn, which are the periods that animals
display high rates of pacing behavior. Moreover, the information that these animals feed on
small portions of food throughout the day is essential to establish the diet of these animals in
captivity. More emphasis on behavioral studies of mammals in captivity is urgently needed to
improve the effectiveness of captive conservation efforts.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

We would like to thank the staff and volunteers of the Rio de Janeiro Zoo (Fundação RIOZOO)
for their assistance in helping us with data collection.

FUNDING

Letícia de Souza Resende received a scholarship from CAPES (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoa-


mento de Pessoal de Nível Superior [Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education
Personnel]).

REFERENCES

Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behavior, 49, 227–267.
Bailey, T. N. (1993). The African leopard: A study of the ecology and behavior of a solitary felid. New York, NY:
Columbia University Press.
TIME AND ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF ONCILLA CATS 81

Baldwin, C. F. (1985). Behavior of carnivores in outdoor exhibits at the National Zoological Park (Unpublished
doctoral dissertation). University of George Mason, Braddock, VA.
Bitetti, M. S. D., Angelo, C. D., Blanco, Y. E. D., & Paviolo, A. (2010). Niche partitioning and species coexistence
in a neotropical felid assemblage. Acta Oecologica, 36, 403–412.
Broom, D. M., & Johnson, K. G. (1993). Stress and animal welfare. London, England: Chapman & Hall—Animal
Behavior Series.
Carlstead, K. (1996). Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild mammals. In D. G. Kleiman, M. E. Allen, K. V.
Thompson, & S. Lumpkin (Eds.), Wild mammals in captivity (pp. 317–333). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.
Carlstead, K., Brown, J. L., & Seidensticker J. (1993). Behavioral and adrenocortical responses to environmental
changes in leopard cats (Felis bengalensis). Zoo Biology, 12, 321–331.
Clubb R., & Mason G. (2003). Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores. Nature, 425, 473–474.
Crawshaw, P. G. (1995). Comparative ecology of ocelot (Felis pardalis) and Jaguar (Panthera onca) in a protected
subtropical forest in Brazil and Argentina (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Florida, Gainesville.
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 18:21 20 January 2015

Crawshaw, P. G., & Quigley, H. B. (1989). Notes on ocelot movement and activity in the Pantanal region, Brazil.
Biotropica, 21, 377–379.
Defler, T. R. (1995). The time budget of a group of wild woolly monkeys (Lagothrix lagotricha). International Journal
of Primatology, 16, 107–120.
Emmons, L. H., Sherman, P., Bolster, D., Goldizen, A., & Terborgh, J. (1989). Ocelot behavior in moonlight. In K. H.
Redford & J. F. Eisenberg (Eds.), Advances in neotropical mammalogy (pp. 233–242). Gainesville, FL: Sandhill
Crane.
Gardner, A. L. (1971). Notes on the little spotted cat, Felis tigrina oncilla (Thomas) in Costa Rica. Journal of
Mammalogy, 52, 464–465.
Genaro, G., Moraes, W., Silva, J. C. R., Adania, C. H., & Franci, C. R. (2007). Plasma hormones in neotropical and
domestic cats undergoing routine manipulations. Research in Veterinary Science, 82, 263–270.
Konecny, M. J. (1989). Movement patterns and food habits of four sympatric carnivore species in Belize, Central
America. In K. H. Redford & J. F. Eisenberg (Eds.), Advances in neotropical mammalogy (pp. 243–264). Gainesville,
FL: Sandhill Crane.
Ludlow, M. E., & Sunquist, M. E. (1987). Ecology and behavior of ocelots in Venezuela. National Geographic
Research, 3, 447–461.
Macdonald, D. W. (1983). The ecology of carnivore social behavior. Nature, 301, 379–384.
Mellen, J. (1991). Factors influencing reproductive success in small captive exotic felids. Zoo Biology, 10, 95–110.
Moreira, N., Brown, J. L., Moraes, W., Swanson, W. F., & Monteiro-Filho, E. L. A. (2007). Effect of housing
and environmental enrichment on adrenocortical activity, behavior and reproductive cyclicity in the female tigrina
(Leopardus tigrinus) and margay (Leopardus wiedii). Zoo Biology, 26, 441–460.
Navarro-Lopez, D. (1985). Status and distribution of the ocelot (Felis pardalis) in South Texas (Unpublished master’s
thesis). Texas A&I University, Kingsville.
Oliveira, T. G. (1994). Neotropical cats: Ecology and conservation. São Luís, Brazil: Universidade Federal do
Maranhão.
Oliveira, T. G., & Cassaro, K. (1999). Guia de identificação dos felinos brasileiros [Identification guide of Brazilian
felines]. São Paulo, Brazil: Sociedade de Zoológicos do Brasil.
Olmos, F. (1993). Notes on the food habits of Brazilian Caatinga carnivores. Mammalia, 57, 126–130.
Pitsko, L. E. (2003). Wild tigers in captivity: A study of the effects of the captive environment on tiger behavior
(Unpublished master’s thesis). Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg.
Shepherdson, D. J., Carlstead, K., Mellen, J. D., & Seidenslicker, J. (1993). Influence of food presentation on the
behavior of small cats in confined environments. Zoo Biology, 12, 203–216.
Weller, S. H., & Bennett, C. L. (2001). Twenty-four hour activity budgets and patterns of behavior in captive ocelots
(Leopardus pardalis). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 71, 67–79.
Ximenez, A. (1982). Notas sobre felidos neotropicales, VIII: Observaciones sobre el contenido estomacal y el
comportamiento alimentar de diversas espécies de felinos [Notes on neotropical felids, VIII: Observations on the
stomach contents and feeding behavior of various species of felines]. Revista Nordestina de Biologia, 5, 89–91.

You might also like