You are on page 1of 8

Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire shortened form T


(ZKA-PQ/SF)

Anton Alujaa,b, , Ignacio Lucasa,b, Angel Blancha,b, Oscar Garcíab,c, Luis F. Garcíab,d
a
University of Lleida, Spain
b
Institut of Biomedical Research of Lleida, Spain
c
European University of Madrid, Spain
d
Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study developed a shortened version of the Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-PQ/
ZKA-PQ SF) and tests its psychometric properties using different samples. This brief version has only 80 items (four items
Zuckerman's personality model per facet), in contrast with the 200 items (10 items per facet) of the long version. The validation sample was
ZKA-PQ/SF formed of 1416 volunteers (696 males and 720 females) with a mean age of 34.62 (SD: 14.06). The ZKA-PQ/SF
Personality shortened versions
has a robust five-factor structure, highly similar to the long questionnaire. The average factor congruence is quite
high (0.98). The alpha reliability coefficients of four-item facets range between 0.64 and 0.86. Test-retest
coefficients for a three-month period were 0.83, 0.84, 0.80, 0.78, and 0.82 for Neuroticism, Sensation Seeking,
Extraversion, Activity and Aggressiveness, respectively. Controlling for item overlap, correlations between long
and short forms ranged between 0.65 and 0.80. Regarding validity, the ZKA-PQ/SF showed adequate convergent
and divergent validity with regard to the Revised NEO Personality Inventory shortened form (NEO-FFI-R). The
ZKA-PQ/SF has good psychometric properties retaining the original 20 facets of the ZKA-PQ and, is therefore
well suited for research purposes based on Zuckerman's Alternative Five-Factor model in those cases where the
long version is inappropriate.

1. Introduction factor, using a Likert scale for the response format (see description
below). The ZKA-PQ was adapted and validated in several cultures in
Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Joireman, Teta, and Kraft (1993) proposed a different languages (Rossier et al., 2016). The five-factor structure of
new Five-factor personality model for research in human personality. the ZKA-PQ replicates well across cultures, suggesting that it can be
This new model was developed through a process of factor analysis of used in a large diversity of cultures, and that Zuckerman's model is as
previous personality questionnaires assessing temperamental and bio- universal as the Five Factor Model (FFM) or Eysenck's Psychoticism-
logical aspects of personality (Zuckerman, Kuhlman, & Camac, 1988; Extraversion-Neuroticism (PEN) model.
Zuckerman, Kuhlman, Thornquist, & Kiers, 1991). As a result of this
process, Zuckerman et al. (1993) developed the Zuckerman-Kuhlman 1.1. The ZKA-PQ and other personality questionnaires
Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ). This questionnaire consisted of 89
items with a true-false answer format measuring five factors: Neuroti- The relationships between the ZKA-PQ and other factorial person-
cism-Anxiety (N-Anx), Sociability (Sy), Aggression-Hostility (Agg- ality questionnaires have also been researched. García, Escorial, García,
Host), Impulsive Sensation Seeking (ImpSS), and Activity (Act). The Blanch, and Aluja (2012) found that Neuroticism and Extraversion fa-
original questionnaire has shortened forms of 69 and 50 items (Aluja cets and factors from ZKA-PQ and the Revised NEO Personality In-
et al., 2006; Aluja, García, & García, 2003a). For more information on ventory (NEO-PI-R) instruments tapped the same constructs with few
the ZKPQ and shortened forms, see Zuckerman (2002, 2008), Joireman differences. ZKA-Aggressiveness had a negative and strong relationship
and Kuhlman (2004) and a revision of Zuckerman and Aluja (2014). with NEO-Agreeableness. On the other hand (see Aluja, García, &
Later, Aluja, Kuhlman, and Zuckerman (2010) developed a new García, 2003b; García, Aluja, García, & Cuevas, 2005), ZKA-Activity
200-item questionnaire (the Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality was positively related with NEO-Conscientiousness rather than NEO-
Questionnaire; ZKA-PQ) including five factors with four facets for each Extraversion. ZKA-Sensation Seeking presented a more diverse pattern,


Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Lleida, Avda. Estudi General, 4, 25001 Lleida, Catalonia, Spain.
E-mail address: aluja@pip.udl.cat (A. Aluja).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.015
Received 17 March 2018; Received in revised form 7 June 2018; Accepted 10 June 2018
0191-8869/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

though some reported relationships (negative with NEO Con- Martí-Guiu, & Balada, 2014), and the PID5 (Maples et al., 2015).
scientiousness, positive with Extroversion, and positive with Openness However, the development of short forms does have several draw-
to Experience) were in agreement with previous evidence using the old backs (Levy, 1968; Smith, McCarthy, & Anderson, 2000). Among them
ZKPQ and the NEO-PI-R. is the reduction of internal consistency reliability coefficients and the
Additionally, the ZKA-PQ factors presented a clear pattern of re- loss of the content of the construct mainly due to having fewer items
lationships with Temperament and Character traits from Cloninger's than in the longer forms. To avoid these pitfalls, it is necessary that the
TCI-R (García, Aluja, García, Escorial, & Blanch, 2012). ZKA Neuroti- short form items have high correlations with the total of the scale, and
cism was positively related with TCI Harm Avoidance and negatively that item redundant in form and content are excluded. Additionally, the
with Self-Directedness. ZKA Sensation Seeking was strongly related factor structure must be equivalent to the long version, and the corre-
with TCI-R Novelty Seeking, and ZKA Aggression and negatively with lations of the factors of the long and short version must be high after
TC-RI Cooperativeness and Self-Direction. ZKA Extraversion correlated controlling for spurious variance or item overlap (Levy, 1967). An ad-
positively with Reward Dependence and Cooperativeness and nega- ditional desirable condition is that facets should be retained in the short
tively with Harm Avoidance and TCI-R Reward Dependence. Lastly, versions.
ZKA Activity correlated positively with Persistence.
Other studies have explored the relationships of the dimensions and
facets of the ZKA-PQ with the EPQ-RS (Eysenck Personality 1.4. The proposal of a shortened version of the ZKA-PQ
Questionnaire Revised, shortened version), I7 (Impulsiveness
Questionnaire) and the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to The aim of this work was to develop an abbreviated, research-
Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ-20), which is based on Gray's theory. screening short form of the ZKA-PQ. For the development of the shorter
Factor Analyses showed that Extraversion and Neuroticism from the ZKA-PQ version, we have adopted the following criteria: a) a robust
EPQ-RS grouped with Extraversion and Neuroticism factors of ZKA-PQ, orthogonal factor structure equivalent to the long version structure, b)
respectively. SPSRQ Sensitivity to Reward loaded on the ZKA satisfactory internal consistency reliability for dimensions and facets
Aggressiveness and Sensation Seeking factors, and SPSRQ Sensitivity to and good test-retest reliability, c) high correlations between the long
Punishment loaded on the ZKA Neuroticism factor. EPQ-RS and short factors, and d) good convergent and divergent validity with
Psychoticism, I7 Impulsivity and Venturesomeness mainly loaded on the other personality measures. The factorial structure is expected to be
ZKA Sensation Seeking factor (see Aluja et al., 2013). equivalent to the original validation, reaching good congruent coeffi-
cients. Further, we compare the structure of the short and long versions
1.2. The ZKA-PQ and psychopathology through confirmatory factor analysis. We also expect strong correla-
tions of factors from the long and short versions. Although alpha reli-
The ZKA-PQ dimensions accounted for between 35% and 61% of the abilities for four-item scales are expected to be lower than the facets
variance of the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology-Basic with 10 items, we expect alpha reliabilities around 0.60–0.70.
Questionnaire (DAPP-BQ; Livesley & Jackson, 2009), whereas ZKA-PQ Regarding convergent and divergent validity with other personality
facets accounted for between 54% and 63% of the variance. Local re- questionnaire, we expect positive correlations of the ZKA-PQ/SF
gression graphical analysis (LOESS) showed ZKA-PQ Neuroticism and Neuroticism and Extraversion dimensions with the same dimensions of
Aggressiveness were the dimensions most related with Emotional Dys- the NEO-FFI-R. In addition, according to previous results obtained with
regulation. ZKA-PQ Sensation Seeking and Aggressiveness were asso- the old ZKPQ and other personality questionnaires, we expect positive
ciated with Dissocial Behavior. ZKA-PQ Neuroticism and Extraversion correlations of ZKA Activity and NEO Conscientiousness, ZKA Sensation
(negative) were associated with Social Avoidance, and ZKA-PQ Ac- Seeking with NEO Extraversion, and ZKA Aggressiveness with NEO
tivity, and Sensation Seeking (negative) with Compulsiveness (Aluja, Neuroticism. In addition, ZKA Aggressiveness should correlate nega-
Blanch, & Balada, 2013). tively with NEO Agreeableness.
The ZKA-PQ was a good predictor of personality disorders (PDs)
measured through the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory III (MCMI
III) in healthy subjects (Aluja, Blanch, García, García, & Escorial, 2012). 2. Method
ZKA-PQ Neuroticism correlated with most PD scales. ZKA-PQ Aggres-
siveness and Sensation Seeking correlated with the Antisocial PD scale, The work presented here is based on a set of five independent
and ZKA-PQ Extraversion correlated with several scales such as Avoi- samples. All the subjects were recruited with the aid of undergraduate
dant and Histrionic PDs. students, who, as practical work in their degree subject Personality
Psychology, used several questionnaires in the “paper and pencil” form
1.3. The need for a ZKA-PQ shortened form to collect data from volunteers from the general population. These
students received credits for participating in the internship program.
In the development of brief forms of personality questionnaires, it is The participants were neighbors, friends, relatives, etc. The participants
important to demonstrate that, a) there is great interest in the long-form also were selected proportionally with this age range: 18–25, 26–35,
measure, b) the long-form measure is valid for its intended purpose, c) 36–50, and higher than 50 years.
its length prevents its use in important ways, and d) the short form The first sample (n = 5874) was a comprehensive database based on
represents a significant time saving. The development of a brief form of all the studies conducted since 2011 by the first author in the Spanish
ZKA-PQ is necessary for several reasons. On the one hand, it can in- version of the above-mentioned ZKA-PQ. Analyses of this first sample
crease the possibility of being included in epidemiological, long- were used to develop a preliminary version of a short form, which was
itudinal, laboratory and cross-sectional studies on health or aging. Short then further refined in three new calibration samples. These calibration
versions are also useful for on-line research or cross-cultural studies, samples (n's = 463, 437 and 472) were collected for the purposes of
and in genetic or clinical studies. In general, short versions save time excluding items, incorporating new items, and confirming that the
and therefore increase the possibility of including in a single session factor structure was good and the alphas acceptable. These calibration
other measures of interest. For example, short forms have been devel- goals were achieved by the fourth sample. A fifth and final step was to
oped for the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (Patrick, use a new sample (n = 1416) of the ZKA-PQ/SF, developed by the
Curtin, & Tellegen, 2002), the NEO-PI-R (McCrae & Costa, 2004), the fourth calibration sample, to compare with the long form to demon-
SPSRQ (Aluja & Blanch, 2011), the HEXACO (Ashton & Lee, 2009), the strate that the factor structure and reliabilities of the short form were
TCI-R (Farmer & Goldberg, 2008), the DAPP-BQ (Aluja, Blanch, Blanco, adequate with regard to the long form.

175
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

2.1. Development of the ZKA-PQ/SF ranged from 0.85 to 0.92 and from 0.65 to 0.90 for the facets (Aluja
et al., 2010). The psychometric properties of the ZKA-PQ were de-
2.1.1. Sample 1 monstrated in 23 cultures and languages (Rossier et al., 2016) using an
For the development of the current ZKA-PQ shortened version (ZKA- innovative method of translation and back-translation (Blanch & Aluja,
PQ/SF), we first performed a factor analysis of the previous Spanish 2016). The ZKA-PQ has five main factors: Aggressiveness (AG), Activity
ZKA-PQ database of 5874 subjects (2900 men and 2974 women) with (AC), Extraversion (EX), Neuroticism (NE), and Sensation Seeking (SS).
an average age of 38.9 (SD = 16.04). In this initial analysis, we selected Each main factor comprises 40 items for the long form and 16 for the
four items per facet with the best factor loadings, after we computed the short form, divided into 4 facets. AG includes Physical Aggression
alpha reliability coefficients for every dimension and the correlations of (AG1), Verbal Aggression (AG2), Anger (AG3), and Hostility (AG4). AC
each item to total facet. is divided into Work Compulsion (AC1), General Activity (AC2), Rest-
In this first sample, the AG4 facet had a high secondary loading in lessness (AC3), and Work Energy (AC4). EX is divided into Positive
the Neuroticism factor. AC3 obtained a low loading on its factor and Emotions (EX1), Social Warmth (EX2), Exhibitionism (EX3), and
high loading on the SS factor. SS2 obtained high secondary loadings in Sociability (EX4). NE is divided into Anxiety (NE1), Depression (NE2),
EX1. In addition, the facet SS4 had a low loading on its factor. AC3 and Dependency (NE3), and Low self-esteem (NE4). Finally, SS is divided
SS4 had an internal consistency lower than 0.50. New items were into Thrill and Adventure Seeking (SS1), Experience Seeking (SS2),
written for AG4, AC3, SS2 and SS4 and tested in the next samples. In Disinhibition (SS3), and Boredom Susceptibility/Impulsivity (SS4). The
each sample, the factor structure was analyzed in order to verify that response format is a 4-point Likert-type scale ranging from strongly
the facets had appropriate loads in their respective factors and the disagree (1) to strongly agree (4).
alpha internal consistency was satisfactory. This objective was accom-
plished by the fourth sample.
2.3.2. The revised NEO personality inventory shortened form (NEO-FFI-R)
2.1.2. Samples 2, 3 and 4 The NEO-FFI-R is a revision of the NEO-FFI shortened version of the
For sample 2, n = 463 participants (229 males and 234 females; NEO-PI-R (McCrae & Costa, 2004), and measures five personality di-
mean of age of 35.33 and SD: 14.23); for sample 3, n = 437; (205 males mensions: Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness to Experience
and 232 females; mean of age of 32.3 and SD: 12.263); for sample 4, (O), Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C). The answer format
n = 472 (225 males and 247 females; mean of age of 37.60 and SD: is a 5-point Likert-type scale (0–4), ranging from “Strongly disagree” (0)
10.58). to “Strongly agree” (4). Internal consistency reliability coefficients of the
In samples 2 and 3, the SS4 continued to have a low loading in its NEO-FFI-R scales ranged from 0.75 to 0.82. In a Spanish validation
factor and also a low alpha. AG4 reduced its secondary factorial loading sample, the alpha reliability ranged from 0.71 to 0.82 (Aluja, Garcıa,
in the Neuroticism factor, albeit insufficiently. New items were written Rossier, & Garcıa, 2005).
for AG4 and SS4 and tested in sample 3. In this sample, the factorial
loading improved for AG4, but did not improve for SS4, and new items
were incorporated to be analyzed in sample 4. The structure and alphas 2.4. Data analysis
of the facets were considered satisfactory for all the facets in sample 4.
The questionnaire was then administered to sample 5, confirming the Means, Standard Deviations, Kurtosis, Skewness and Cronbach
good structure (factor loadings > 0.50) of the facets and obtaining an alpha reliabilities of scales were computed. Cohen's d was computed to
acceptable reliability (between 0.63 and 0.90). The ZKA-PQ/SF final analyze gender differences. Factor analysis was conducted using the
version has eight new or rewritten items (see Appendix). The rest of the same method as the ZKA-PQ original instrument: Principal Axis and
items come from the long version of the ZKA-PQ. Varimax rotation (Aluja et al., 2010). We used the O'Connor (2000)
programs for SPSS for determining the number of components using
2.2. Sample 5: evaluation of the ZKA-PQ/SF: comparison with the ZKA-PQ parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. After Procrustes rotation, factor
long form, and correlations with the NEO-FFI-R congruency coefficients were obtained. Correlations between factors
and dimensions were performed with Pearson product-moment corre-
This sample consisted of 1416 volunteers (696 males and 720 fe- lations statistic. Cohen (1988) suggested Small, Medium and Large ef-
males) with a mean age of 34.62 (SD = 14.06). The mean age for males fect sizes of product-moment correlations for coefficients equal or
was 35.07 (SD = 15.90) and for females 34.18 (SD = 16.17). The age larger than 0.10, 0.30 and 0.50, respectively. A correction of short
difference for males and females did not differ (t-test: 1.04; p < 0.30; versus long test forms was estimated controlling for spurious inflation
d: 0.06). The age range was between 18 and 90 years. Percentages of (item overlap) due to shared error variance using Levy's formula1 (Levy,
participants for age ranges were as follows: 18–25 (39.6%), 26–35 1967). Differences between alpha reliabilities of long and short ZKA
(24.2%), and 36–50 (16.2%) and higher than 50 years old (20%). The versions were analyzed by the Feldt formula for independent samples
data were collected between 2014 and 2016. All participants completed (Feldt, 1980) using ALPHATST software (Lautenschlager, 1989). Test-
the following questionnaires: (1) the ZKA-PQ/SF, and (2) the NEO-FFI- retest reliabilities were determined using a second application of the
R. Details for each scale are provided below. In addition to the data ZKA-PQ/SF in a three-month interval. We compared the goodness-of-fit
provided by sample 5 for the ZKA-PQ/SF, we also used previous data on indexes obtained in an original ZKA-PQ sample (Aluja et al., 2010) with
the ZKA-PQ to compare the factor structure and facets of these two the ZKA-PQ/SF of the present study. Convergent and divergent validity
scales. of the ZKA-PQ/SF was analyzed by correlating dimensions and facets of
ZKA-PQ/SF with the NEO-FFI-R. SPSS (2011), AMOS (Arbuckle, 2010),
2.3. Instruments in this study ALPHATST (Lautenschlager, 1989), and Shortform 1.11 packages were
used for statistical analyses.
2.3.1. The Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-
PQ) and the ZKA-PQ/SF
The ZKA-PQ (Aluja et al., 2010) and the ZKA-PQ/SF have the same
five-factor structure and the same four facets for each factor. Thus, the
following description of the ZKA-PQ factors and facets applies just as
well to those of the ZKA-PQ/SF. A Spanish validation sample of the 1
Available in Paul Barrett Homepage (free statistical software: http://www.pbarrett.
ZKA-PQ found that the alpha consistency reliability of the five factors net/Shortform/sform.html#:1. Last access: March 2018).

176
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

Table 1
Descriptive, sex differences, alpha internal consistency reliability and correlation with age (r).
Males Females All Alpha Feldt χ2 p< r age

M SD M SD K S d α 1
α 2

Age 35.07 15.90 34.18 16.18 0.10 0.94 0.06 –


AG 34.99 8.91 34.52 8.96 −0.02 0.45 0.06 0.90 0.90 0.000 0.304 −0.22
AC 42.14 7.40 42.28 6.90 −0.05 0.00 −0.02 0.82 0.87 2.210 0.133 −0.03
EX 49.24 7.35 49.56 7.65 −0.03 −0.45 −0.04 0.86 0.88 0.493 0.487 −0.14
NE 32.49 8.59 35.95 8.67 −0.32 0.25 −0.40 0.86 0.92 6.474 0.010 −0.08
SS 39.78 8.76 36.64 9.16 −0.44 0.09 0.35 0.88 0.85 1.041 0.308 −0.41
AG1 7.43 3.24 6.22 2.79 0.34 1.05 0.40 0.86 0.90 2.365 0.120 −0.15
AG2 10.75 2.77 10.75 2.99 −0.57 −0.17 0 0.76 0.75 0.349 0.659 −0.27
AG3 8.65 2.81 9.16 2.91 −0.50 0.29 −0.18 0.78 0.81 0.450 0.509 −0.08
AG4 8.17 2.36 8.38 2.63 −0.04 0.44 −0.08 0.66 0.68 0.077 0.688 −0.20
AC1 7.98 2.91 7.94 2.70 −0.39 0.44 −0.01 0.74 0.73 0.029 0.649 −0.08
AC2 11.37 2.71 11.52 2.59 −0.34 −0.24 −0.06 0.79 0.76 0.373 0.547 −0.06
AC3 9.84 2.52 10.04 2.43 −0.36 0.14 −0.08 0.63 0.68 0.441 0.513 −0.06
AC4 12.95 2.40 12.78 2.32 0.49 −0.74 0.07 0.79 0.83 0.934 0.775 0.15
EX1 13.32 2.12 13.32 2.14 0.91 −0.85 0 0.76 0.73 0.290 0.591 −0.08
EX2 11.57 2.97 12.46 2.96 −0.65 −0.43 −0.30 0.81 0.81 0.000 0.304 0.04
EX3 11.91 2.60 11.27 2.86 −0.27 −0.49 0.23 0.84 0.76 3.418 0.060 0.24
EX4 12.44 2.35 12.51 2.30 0.06 −0.55 −0.03 0.64 0.75 0.277 0.091 −0.13
NE1 9.74 2.54 9.29 2.56 −0.29 0.21 0.18 0.70 0.72 0.099 0.687 −0.10
NE2 8.65 2.65 9.08 2.57 −0.40 0.35 −0.16 0.74 0.73 0.029 0.649 −0.02
NE3 7.99 2.65 9.06 2.79 −0.35 0.19 −0.39 0.69 0.70 0.022 0.630 −0.03
NE4 8.40 2.52 9.40 2.67 −0.21 0.56 −0.39 0.83 0.81 0.259 0.609 −0.11
SS1 7.45 2.69 8.40 2.85 −0.89 0.36 −0.34 0.78 0.73 0.872 0.351 −0.28
SS2 9.74 3.32 7.38 3.12 −0.55 −0.34 0.73 0.73 0.70 0.235 0.624 −0.33
SS3 11.04 2.97 10.81 3.01 −0.59 0.00 0.08 0.77 0.72 0.805 0.371 −0.45
SS4 10.09 2.95 9.78 2.84 −0.30 0.12 0.11 0.66 0.65 0.017 0.612 −0.29

Note: M: Mean; SD: Standard deviation: K: Kurtosis; S: Skewness; d: Cohen d. α1: Alpha reliability of the ZKA-PQ/SF in the current sample. Α2: Alpha reliability of ZKA-
PQ in the original Spanish sample (Aluja et al., 2010). R: Correlation with the age. AG: Aggressiveness; AC: Activity; EX: Extraversion; NE: Neuroticism; SS:
Impulsive-Sensation Seeking; AG1: Physical Aggression; AG2: Verbal Aggression; AG3: Anger; AG4: Hostility; AC1: Work Compulsion); AC2: General Activity; AC3:
Restlessness; and AC4: Work Energy; EX1: Positive Emotions; EX2: Social Warmth; EX3: Exhibitionism; EX4: Sociability; NE1: Anxiety; NE2: Depression; NE3:
Dependency; NE4: Low Self-Esteem; SS1: Thrill and Adventure Seeking); SS2: Experience Seeking; SS3: Disinhibition; SS4: Boredom Susceptibility/Impulsivity).

Table 2
Factor structure (principal axis) of the ZKA-PQ original Spanish (Aluja et al., 2010) vs the current ZKA-PQ/SF, Varimax Procrustes rotation and congruency coef-
ficients (C·C.).
ZKA-PQ (Sample; n = 1042) ZKA-PQ/SF (n = 1416) Procrustes rotation C.C.

I II III IV V I II III IV V I II III IV V

AG1 0.60 −0.06 −0.20 −0.01 0.27 0.62 0.00 −0.08 −0.07 0.24 0.61 0.01 −0.09 −0.09 0.26 0.98
AG2 0.74 0.01 0.17 0.00 0.23 0.69 −0.08 0.10 0.09 0.19 0.69 −0.07 0.08 0.07 0.21 0.98
AG3 0.87 0.11 −0.09 0.23 0.11 0.82 0.02 −0.06 0.29 0.02 0.82 0.03 −0.09 0.27 0.05 0.99
AG4 0.68 −0.04 −0.21 0.35 0.11 0.63 0.02 −0.14 0.28 0.06 0.63 0.03 −0.16 0.26 0.08 0.99
AC1 0.01 0.60 0.00 0.02 −0.04 −0.06 0.52 −0.09 0.04 0.07 −0.07 0.52 −0.08 0.04 0.08 0.96
AC2 −0.06 0.69 0.05 0.00 0.19 0.00 0.70 0.17 −0.01 0.13 −0.01 0.69 0.19 −0.01 0.14 0.98
AC3 0.24 0.52 0.11 0.09 0.23 0.29 0.52 0.11 0.13 0.21 0.28 0.52 0.11 0.12 0.23 1
AC4 −0.09 0.62 0.27 −0.18 −0.27 −0.10 0.52 0.19 −0.21 −0.15 −0.10 0.52 0.21 −0.20 −0.15 0.99
EX1 −0.14 0.20 0.69 −0.36 0.04 −0.13 0.28 0.57 −0.33 0.12 −0.13 0.26 0.59 −0.32 0.11 0.99
EX2 −0.11 0.09 0.75 −0.18 −0.05 −0.13 −0.04 0.56 −0.11 −0.07 −0.11 −0.05 0.56 −0.10 −0.08 0.97
EX3 0.11 0.09 0.50 −0.18 0.34 0.18 0.08 0.49 −0.09 0.37 0.18 0.07 0.49 −0.09 0.37 0.98
EX4 −0.10 0.03 0.78 −0.14 0.24 −0.01 0.15 0.75 −0.14 0.23 0.00 0.13 0.76 −0.13 0.23 0.98
NE1 0.35 0.15 −0.20 0.62 0.14 0.36 0.24 −0.13 0.61 0.14 0.36 0.25 −0.14 0.60 0.16 0.99
NE2 0.23 0.00 −0.22 0.75 −0.05 0.13 −0.04 −0.24 0.76 0.00 0.14 −0.03 −0.26 0.75 0.02 0.99
NE3 0.01 0.00 −0.05 0.71 −0.15 0.11 0.02 0.02 0.72 −0.04 0.13 0.02 0.00 0.72 −0.03 0.97
NE4 0.02 −0.08 −0.33 0.80 0.03 0.05 −0.13 −0.26 0.75 −0.01 0.06 −0.12 −0.38 0.74 0.00 1
SS1 0.06 0.12 0.04 −0.16 0.72 0.12 0.10 −0.07 −0.17 0.65 0.10 0.09 −0.06 −0.18 0.65 0.99
SS2 0.14 0.02 0.21 −0.02 0.74 0.01 0.07 0.11 0.05 0.72 −0.01 0.06 0.12 0.04 0.72 0.97
SS3 0.19 0.04 0.17 0.03 0.76 0.21 0.08 0.24 0.06 0.73 0.20 0.07 0.24 0.05 0.74 0.99
SS4 0.24 −0.02 −0.07 0.07 0.62 0.18 0.01 0.05 0.10 0.67 0.16 0.00 0.05 0.09 0.68 0.98
C.C 0.98 0.97 0.97 0.99 0.99 0.98
SSL 2.10 1.36 1.44 3.99 4.79 4.24 3.79 1.88 1.52 1.39
% 10.48 6.81 7.21 19.96 23.94 21.26 18.95 9.42 7.63 6.93

Note: AG1: Physical Aggression; AG2: Verbal Aggression; AG3: Anger; AG4: Hostility; AC1: Work Compulsion); AC2: General Activity; AC3: Restlessness; and AC4:
Work Energy; EX1: Positive Emotions; EX2: Social Warmth; EX3: Exhibitionism; EX4: Sociability; NE1: Anxiety; NE2: Depression; NE3: Dependency; NE4: Low Self-
Esteem; SS1: Thrill and Adventure Seeking); SS2: Experience Seeking; SS3: Disinhibition; SS4: Boredom Susceptibility/Impulsivity). SSL: sums of squared loadings.

177
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

3. Results Table 4
Correlation between the ZKA-PQ and the ZKA-PQ/SF dimension and correla-
3.1. Descriptive, sex differences and alpha reliability tions controlling the item overlap (ZKA-PQ*).
ZKA-PQ ZKA-PQ*
Table 1 shows descriptive statistics, alphas, gender differences
(Cohen, 1988) and correlations between ZKA-PQ/SF and age. Kurtosis AG AC EX NE SS AG AC EX NE SS
and skewness values ( ± 1) indicate that frequencies were normally
ZKA-PQ/
distributed, whereas gender differences were equivalent to those re- SF AG 0.71 0.65
ported by Aluja et al. (2010). Men scored significantly higher on SS, and AC 0.90 0.85
women in NE dimensions with a medium effect size. We found, as ex- EX 0.88 0.83
NE 0.86 0.82
pected, a strong negative correlation between SS and age. Alpha coef-
SS 0.88 0.82
ficients were satisfactory and, most important, similar to those reported
by Aluja et al. (2010), although some facet alphas in the short version Note: AG: Aggressiveness; AC: Activity; EX: Extraversion; NE: Neuroticism; SS:
were moderately lower than the long version. Sensation Seeking.
The average of twenty alpha facets was 0.75 in the present study,
similar to that obtained by the long form in the original one (Aluja three months. The correlations were 0.83 for NE, 0.84 for SS, 0.80 for
et al., 2010). No statistically significant differences were found in the EX, 0.78 for AC and 0.82 for AG.
alpha internal consistency of the long and short version of the ZKA, We computed test-retest correlations over a two-month period be-
except for NE (Feldt χ2: 6474; p < 0.01). tween the long and the short ZKA-PQ forms using another independent
sample of 86 ungraduated students (41 males and 45 females; mean of
3.2. Comparison of ZKA-PA and ZKA-PQ/SF structure age of 21.56 and SD: 1.89). The correlations ranged between 0.86 and
0.90. However, given that the short form includes most of the items
We used the same original factor extraction method adopted by from the long form, we used public Paul Barret software (Shortform) for
Aluja et al. (2010) in the original ZKA-PQ study: Principal Axis with calculating the correction published by Levy (1967), which adjusts a
Varimax rotation. In this study, we retained five factors using several short vs long form (Part-Whole) correlation for the correlated error
methods: eigenvalue ≥1 (Kaiser, 1960), Scree test (Cattell, 1966), variance associated with the short form items originally embedded in
Horn's parallel analysis (Horn, 1965) and Velicer's minimum average the long form test. Adjusted correlations were higher than 0.80 for NE,
partial test (Velicer, 1976). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sam- EX, SS, AC and 0.65 for AG (Table 4).
pling Adequacy was 0.83. Bartlett's Test of Sphericity was 10,734.9
(190 degrees of freedom; p < 0.001). The five extracted factors ex- 3.5. Confirmatory factor analysis: ZKA-PQ versus ZKA-PQ/SF
plained 64.16% of the variance (6.93% to 21.26%). We compared the
factor structure of the ZKA-PQ obtained in the original study (Aluja For confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), we used a Maximum
et al., 2010) with the current shortened ZKA-PQ/SF. After a Varimax Likelihood method on the variance-covariance matrices of the 20 ZKA-
Procrustes rotation between both factor structures, we obtained a ro- PQ facets. Several criteria of model fit were used: The standardized root
bust factorial congruence between factors (columns) and facets (rows). mean square residual (SRMS) (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1989); the Tucker-
Facets ranged between 0.96 and 1, and factors between 0.97 and 0.99) Lewis index (TLI) (Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Tucker & Lewis, 1973); The
(Table 2). Expected Cross- Validation Index (ECVI)(Browne & Cudeck, 1993); the
comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, 1990); and the root mean square
3.3. Correlations between factors (saturations) and dimensions (scores) of error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger, 1990).
the ZKA-PQ/SF For comparing the ZKA-PQ and ZKA-PQ/SF, we used the same
procedure described in Aluja et al. (2010). The following models were
The AG dimension correlated 0.34 and 0.30 with NE and SS re- computed for the short version: Simple Structure: Each facet was only
spectively. AC correlated with EX (0.24) positively, and EX correlated linked to its own single latent factor. The remaining loadings were fixed
negatively with NE (−0.24) and positively with SS (0.25). Correlations to zero. Salient Loadings: All secondary loadings greater than ± 0.40
between factors (factor loadings/saturations) and dimensions (scores were included in this model. Modest Loadings: All secondary loadings
adding facets values) were higher than 0.93 (Table 3). greater than ± 0.20 were included in this model, and Correlated error
terms: In each model, the five pairs of facets with the largest mod-
3.4. Test-retest reliability ification indices (MI's) were included. The parameters were freely es-
timated in all CFA analyses. Additionally, in order to identify the
Test-retest reliability of the ZKA-PQ/SF factors was conducted in an models, the variances of the latent exogenous variables (the factors)
independent sample of 240 undergraduate students (113 males and 127 and the regression coefficients of the error terms over the observed
females; mean of age of 22.31 and SD: 2.18). The temporal period be- endogenous variables (the facets) were fixed to 1.
tween the first and second administration of the ZKA-PQ/SF was of In all four models for both ZKA versions, the goodness of fit indexes

Table 3
Correlation between the ZKA-PQ/SF dimensions (scores), dimensions and factors (factor loadings).
ZKA-PQ/SF dimensions ZKA-PQ factors (saturations)

AG AC EX NE SS AG AC EX NE SS

Dimensions AG 0.03 −0.07 0.34 0.30 0.96


AC 0.24 0.00 0.16 0.98
EX −0.24 0.25 0.93
NE 0.04 0.95
SS 0.94

Note: AG: Aggressiveness; AC: Activity; EX: Extraversion; NE: Neuroticism; SS: Sensation Seeking.

178
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

Table 5
Goodness of fit indices for ZKA-PQ and ZKA-PQ/SF models.
Oblique five-factor models χ2* d.f. ECVI CFI TLI SRMR RMSEA (90% CI)

ZKA-PQ (n = 1042)(Aluja et al., 2010)


Simple structure 2506.50 160 2.50 0.76 0.72 0.11 0.12 (0.11–0.12)
Salient loadings (> 0.40) 2506.50 160 2.50 0.76 0.72 0.11 0.12 (0.11–0.12)
Modest loadings (> 0.20) 1265.18 142 1.35 0.89 0.85 0.06 0.09 (0.08–0.09)
Correlated error termsa 1000.47 137 1.06 0.91 0.88 0.05 0.08 (0.07–0.08)
AG1-SS1; EX4-SS4; EX1-SS4; NE3-SS1; AC1-AC4

ZKA-PQ/SF (n = 1416)(Current study)


Simple structure 2407.94 160 1.77 0.75 0.79 0.10 0.10 (0.09–0.10)
Salient loadings (> 0.40) 2407.94 160 1.77 0.75 0.79 0.10 0.10 (0.09–0.10)
Modest loadings (> 0.20) 1301.03 144 1.01 0.89 0.86 0.07 0.07 (0.07–0.08)
Correlated error terms AG1-SS1;SS1-NE1; AC4-SS1; AC1-AC4; AC1-AC4 1078.32 139 0.85 0.91 0.88 0.06 0.07 (0.06–0.07)

Note:*The associated p values were always lower than 0.001; S-1: d.f. Degree of freedom; ECVI: Expected Cross-Validation Index. TLI: Tucker-Lewis Index; CFI:
Comparative Fit Index; SRMR: Standardized root mean Square residual; RMSEA: Root mean square error of approximation and its 90% confidence interval.
a
The right number of degrees of freedom and the concrete pairs of facets correlated in this model appear in this table.

were unsatisfactory (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). When salient loadings Table 7
and correlated error terms were included, the goodness-of-fit-indices Correlations between ZKA-PQ/SF and NEO-FFI-R.
improved and the results were very similar for the ZKA-PQ and the O C E A N
ZKA-PQ/SF (Table 5).
Further, a new series of analyses were performed, in which the fa- AG1 −0.06 −0.18 0.08 −0.42 0.12
AG2 0.05 −0.26 0.07 −0.33 0.26
cets of each factor were treated as observed variables and the factor as a
AG3 −0.02 −0.23 −0.10 −0.33 0.53
latent variable. When the five domains were assessed separately for AG4 0.00 −0.27 −0.12 −0.50 0.53
each of the five dimensions, the goodness of fit indices for both ZKA AC1 0.12 0.17 0.13 −0.01 −0.06
versions were satisfactory, excluding the AC domain, in which the TLI AC2 0.03 0.34 0.37 0.02 −0.02
was low (< 0.90) (Table 6). In regard to the results for the Activity AC3 0.04 0.06 0.36 −0.18 0.16
AC4 −0.07 0.66 0.20 0.15 −0.32
trait, the bad fit reported for the long form was replicated (Aluja et al.,
EX1 0.14 0.36 0.52 0.14 −0.40
2010). The reason for the bad fit in the long form was the high corre- EX2 0.14 0.20 0.42 0.34 −0.22
lation between error terms of AC1 (Work Compulsion) and AC4 (Work EX3 0.26 −0.06 0.48 −0.14 −0.05
Energy). When this correlated error was allowed to be freely estimated EX4 0.17 0.14 0.69 0.17 −0.22
in the ZKA-PQ/SF model, the fit was also adequate (χ2 = 22.13; d.f.: 1; NE1 0.08 −0.22 −0.10 −0.13 0.61
NE2 0.14 −0.31 −0.29 −0.10 0.73
TLI: 0.91; CFI: 0.98). NE3 0.04 −0.13 −0.15 0.03 0.57
NE4 0.04 −0.36 −0.34 0.08 0.63
SS1 0.22 −0.11 0.35 −0.24 −0.05
3.6. Convergent and divergent validity
SS2 0.48 −0.21 0.38 −0.15 0.06
SS3 0.40 −0.22 0.49 −0.23 0.10
To assess the convergent and divergent validity of the ZKA-PQ/SF, a SS4 0.40 −0.29 0.28 −0.26 0.13
correlation matrix between the facets and dimensions of the ZKA-PQ/SF AG −0.01 −0.29 −0.02 −0.49 0.44
and the dimensions of the NEO-FFI-R was made (Table 7). Convergent AC 0.05 0.44 0.38 −0.01 −0.08
EX 0.24 0.21 0.71 0.18 −0.29
and divergent validity was demonstrated by strong positive and nega- NE 0.09 −0.32 −0.28 −0.03 0.79
tive correlations of the ZKA-PQ/SF. For the NEO, we found correlations SS 0.46 −0.25 0.46 −0.27 0.07
between ZKA-AG and NEO-N, ZKA-AC and NEO-C, ZKA-EX and NEO-E,
ZKA-NE and NEO-N, and ZKA-SS and both NEO-O and NEO-E (con- Note: N. Neuroticism; E. Extraversion; O. Openness; A. Agreeableness; C.
vergent). ZKA-AG negatively correlated to NEO-A and NEO-C, ZKA-SS Conscientiousness.
has a negative correlation with NEO-A and NEO-C, ZKA-EX correlates Correlations were significant to p < 0.01 starting r: 0.16.
Correlations were significant to p < 0.001 starting r: 0.19.
negatively with NEO-N, and ZKA-NE with NEO-C and NEO-E (di-
vergent). It should be noted that large effect sizes (r ≥ 0.50) were

Table 6
Goodness of fit indices for the factor structure for each of the five ZKA-PQ and ZKA-PQ/SF domains. Each domain consists of four facets (one-factor model).
χ2* d.f. ECVI TLI CFI SRMR RMSEA (90% CI)

ZKA-PQ AG 6.79 2 0.02 0.99 1 0.01 0.05 (0.01–0.09)


(Aluja et al., 2010) AC 166 2 0.05 0.39 0.80 0.09 0.28 (0.24–0.32)
EX 32.25 2 0.05 0.94 0.98 0.03 0.12 (0.08–0.15)
NE 39.24 2 0.05 0.93 0.98 0.03 0.13 (0.10–0.17)
SS 38.49 2 0.05 0.93 0.98 0.03 0.13 (0.09–0.17)
ZKA-PQ/SF AG 33.16 2 0.04 0.95 0.98 0.03 0.10 (0.08–0.14)
(Current study) AC 103.15 2 0.08 0.65 0.88 0.07 0.18 (0.16–0.22)
EX 14.73 2 0.02 0.97 0.99 0.02 0.07 (0.04–0.10)
NE 24.97 2 0.04 0.95 0.98 0.03 0.11 (0.08–0.14)
SS 2.74 2 0.01 0.99 1 0.00 0.02 (0.00–0.06)

Note:*The associated p values were always lower than 0.001 except for SS [S-1 (ns, S-2 (0.05)], AG [S-1 and S-3 (0.05)]. S-1: Spanish calibration sample; S-2: Spanish
validation sample; S-3: English sample; AG: Aggressiveness; AC: Activity; EX: Extraversion; SS: NE: Neuroticism; Sensation Seeking; d.f. Degree of freedom; ECVI:
Expected Cross-Validation Index. TLI: Tucker-Lewis index; CFI: Comparative Fit Index; SRMR: Standardized root mean square residual; RMSEA: root mean square
error of approximation and its 90% confidence interval.

179
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

observed among theoretically analogous scales. measures are noteworthy. The correlations between the ZKA-PQ/SF and
the NEO-FFI-R were similar to the ZKA-PQ and the NEO-PI-R (García,
4. Discussion Aluja, et al., 2012). Neuroticism and Extraversion obtained strong
correlations with similar scales in the NEO-PI-R. Activity correlated
The main objective of this study was to develop a short version of with Conscientiousness, and Sensation Seeking with Extraversion and
the ZKA-PQ with acceptable reliability for facets, and retaining the Openness.
same theoretical structure as the original long form and the 20 facets Summing up, the ZKA-PQ/SF is a short version of the ZKA-PQ, si-
(Aluja et al., 2010). The current trend in personality research is towards milar in factor structure (20 facets), reliability and convergent and di-
reduced questionnaires in order to assess personality in a faster, but also vergent validity. In the introduction, we mentioned several reasons for
accurate, way. As with other personality models mentioned in the in- developing brief forms of personality questionnaires. In our opinion,
troduction section, Zuckerman's personality model now has a short this study meets these considerations. The ZKA-PQ/SF (available in the
version of the ZKA-PQ. The process of selection and adjustment of items appendix for researchers for non-commercial use) is highly re-
was long and laborious, and required an initial large database grouped commended for research within the Zuckerman personality model fra-
from past studies and three calibration studies prior to the current mework in any context which calls for brief measures. A strong point of
study. Finally, eight items were replaced or rewritten, but the rest of the this study is the large sample, which is quite representative of the
items were included in the long form. general population, has a similar number of men and women, and is
The results of the present study support the use of the ZKA-PQ/SF without age differences between sexes. Further, the psychometric
given its good psychometric properties and strong equivalence to the characteristics of the ZKA-PQ/SF should be studied in different cultures
long version. It should be noted that one advantage of the ZKA-PQ/SF and countries, as has been done with the ZKA-PQ, in order to demon-
over other short personality questionnaires (Aluja et al., 2014; Farmer strate the short form's usefulness and Cross-cultural Generalizability in
& Goldberg, 2008; Maples et al., 2015; McCrae & Costa, 2004; Patrick the study of Zuckerman's Alternative Five-factor Model (Rossier et al.,
et al., 2002) is that ZKA-PQ/SF retains all 20 facets of the original long 2016).
questionnaire. Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
The alpha reliabilities of the factors were higher than 0.86, and the doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.015.
average of alpha of facets was 0.75 (range between 0.64 and 0.86),
similar to original ZKA-PQ validation results (Aluja et al., 2010). Only Acknowledgments
four facets obtained alpha values slightly lower than 0.70 (AG4: 0.66,
EX4: 64, NE3: 0.69 and SS4: 0.66). Note that somewhat smaller alphas We want to thank Dr. Michel Kuhlman (University of Delaware) for
were expected, since facets have only four items. Only the Neuroticism reviewing the manuscript and his advice for improving the quality of
factor obtained a significantly higher alpha reliability in the long ver- the study. We would also like to thank Dr. Marvin Zuckerman of the
sion compared to the short version. There was no significant difference University of Delaware for the opportunity to collaborate with him on
in the facets. many articles and publications over the past 15 years. This article is in
Test-retest reliability indices and the adjusted correlations between honor to Prof. Zuckerman in recognition of his important contribution
the short and long versions were also quite satisfactory. Therefore, this to the psychology of personality.
short version has similar reliabilities to other short forms such as the
HEXACO-60 (range from 0.77 to 0.80 [average of 0.78] in the college References
sample, and from 0.73 to 0.80 [average of 0.75] in the community
sample for 10-item factors) (Ashton & Lee, 2009). Also, the NEO-FFI-R, Aluja, A., & Blanch, A. (2011). Neuropsychological behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and
alpha reliabilities of 12-item factors ranged from 0.73 to 0.84 (McCrae behavioral approach system (BAS) assessment: A shortened sensitivity to punishment
and sensitivity to reward questionnaire version (SPSRQ–20). Journal of Personality
& Costa, 2004; also similar to the original NEO-FFI). Assessment, 93(6), 628–636. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.608760.
The factor structure of the ZKA-PQ/SF is very similar to the long Aluja, A., Blanch, A., & Balada, F. (2013). Normal personality versus pathological per-
version, but the secondary factor loadings in other factors of the facets sonality: Dimensional and predictive study. Personality and Mental Health, 7(4),
288–297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1240.
of the AG4, EX1 and NE4 are somewhat smaller. Different rules for the Aluja, A., Blanch, À., Blanco, E., Martí-Guiu, M., & Balada, F. (2014). The dimensional
number of factors to retain concur on extracting five factors (Cattell, assessment of personality psychopathology basic questionnaire: Shortened versions
1966; Kaiser, 1960; O'Connor, 2000). As Zuckerman's theory suggests, item analysis. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17(102), 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1017/sjp.2014.109.
factor inter-correlations indicate a relative orthogonality among the Aluja, A., Blanch, A., García, L. F., García, O., & Escorial, S. (2012). Zuckerman-Kuhlman-
five factors. Aggressiveness obtained moderate correlations with Neu- Aluja Personality Questionnaire as a predictor of MCMI-III personality disorder
roticism (0.34) and Sensation Seeking (0.30), while the rest of the ZKA- scales: The role of facets. Personality and Mental Health, 6(3), 217–227. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1002/pmh.1185.
PQ/SF inter-correlations ranged between 0.00 and 0.25. Nevertheless,
Aluja, A., Escorial, S., García, L. F., García, Ó., Blanch, A., & Zuckerman, M. (2013).
these coefficients make sense from a theoretical point of view Reanalysis of Eysenck's, Gray's, and Zuckerman's structural trait models based on a
(Zuckerman, 2008; Zuckerman et al., 1993). new measure: The Zuckerman–Kuhlman–Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-PQ).
As with the long version of the ZKA-PQ (or other personality Personality and Individual Differences, 54(2), 192–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2012.08.030.
questionnaires such as the NEO-PI-R; see McCrae, Zonderman, Costa, Aluja, A., García, O., & García, L. F. (2003a). Psychometric properties of the Zuckerman-
Bond, & Paunonen, 1996), the simple structure of ZKA-PQ/SF obtained Kuhlman personality questionnaire (ZKPQ-III-R): A study of a shortened form.
unsatisfactory goodness-of-fit indices in confirmatory factor analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(7), 1083–1097. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0191-8869 (02)00097-1.
One of the reasons for poor fit is the non-perfect orthogonality between Aluja, A., García, O., & García, L. F. (2003b). Relationships among extraversion, openness
factors and facets. When modest loadings > 0.20 and the correlated to experience, and sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(3),
error terms are incorporated in the analyses, the adjustments improve 671–680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869 (02)00244-1.
Aluja, A., García, Ó., García, L. F., & Seisdedos, N. (2005). Invariance of the “NEO-PI-R”
significantly (Aluja, García, García, & Seisdedos, 2005; McCrae et al., factor structure across exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses. Personality and
1996). The present results reproduce this pattern since simple structure Individual Differences, 38(8), 1879–1889. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.11.
does not fit the data well, but as with ZKA-PQ and NEO-PI-R, the in- 014.
Aluja, A., Garcıa, O., Rossier, J., & Garcıa, L. F. (2005). Comparison of the NEO-FFI, the
clusion of modest loadings and correlated error terms produces a sa- NEO-FFI-R and an alternative short version of the NEO-PI-R (NEO-60) in Swiss and
tisfactory fit. Spanish samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(3), 591–604. http://dx.doi.
The convergent and divergent validity were the last criteria adopted org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.05.014.
Aluja, A., Kuhlman, M., & Zuckerman, M. (2010). Development of the
in the current study. Some substantive aspects of the observed re-
Zuckerman–Kuhlman–Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA–PQ): A factor/facet
lationships between the ZKA-PQ/SF factors and other personality

180
A. Aluja et al. Personality and Individual Differences 134 (2018) 174–181

version of the Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ). Journal of Livesley, W. J., & Jackson, D. N. (2009). Manual for the dimensional assessment of per-
Personality Assessment, 92(5), 416–431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2010. sonality pathology — Basic questionnaire. Port Huron, MI: Sigma Press.
497406. Maples, J. L., Carter, N. T., Few, L. R., Crego, C., Gore, W. L., Samuel, D. B., ... D, J.
Aluja, A., Rossier, J., García, L. F., Angleitner, A., Kuhlman, D. M., & Zuckerman, M. (2015). Testing whether the DSM-5 personality disorders trait model can be mea-
(2006). A cross-cultural form of the ZKPQ (ZKPQ-50-CC) adapted to English, French, sured with a reduced set of items: An item response theory investigation of the
German, and Spanish languages. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(4), personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychological Assessment, 27(4), 1195–1210. http://
619–628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.001. dx.doi.org/10.1037/pas0000120.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2010). IBM SPSS Amos 19 user's guide. Crawfordville, FL: Amos McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2004). A contemplated revision of the NEO five-factor
Development Corporation635. inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(3), 587–596. http://dx.doi.org/
Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions 10.1016/S0191-8869 (03)00118-1.
of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340–345. http://dx.doi.org/ McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., Bond, M. H., & Paunonen, S. V. (1996).
10.1080/00223890902935878. Evaluating replicability of factors in the revised NEO personality inventory:
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, Confirmatory factor analysis versus Procrustes rotation. of Personality and Social
107(2), 238–246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. Psychology, 70(3), 552–566. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.552.
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis O'Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of compo-
of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 588–606. http://dx.doi.org/10. nents using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. Behavior Research Methods,
1037/0033-2909.88.3.588. Instruments, & Computers. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200807 (UNITED
Blanch, A., & Aluja, A. (2016). Text mining a self-report back-translation. Psychological STATES).
Assessment, 28(6), 750–764. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000213. Patrick, C. J., Curtin, J. J., & Tellegen, A. (2002). Development and validation of a brief
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. form of the multidimensional personality questionnaire. Psychological Assessment,
Bollen, & J. S. Long (Vol. Eds.), Testing structural equation models. Vol. 154. Testing 14(2), 150–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.14.2.150.
structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park: Sage Publications. Rossier, J., Aluja, A., Blanch, A., Barry, O., Hansenne, M., Carvalho, A. F., ... Karagonlar,
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The screen test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral G. (2016). Cross-cultural generalizability of the alternative five-factor model using
Research, 1(2), 245–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr0102_10. the Zuckerman-Kuhlman-Aluja Personality Questionnaire. European Journal of
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral (2nd Ed.). New Jersey: Personality, 30(2), 139–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.2045.
Lawrence Earlbam Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Anderson, K. G. (2000). On the sins of short-form de-
Farmer, R. F., & Goldberg, L. R. (2008). A psychometric evaluation of the revised tem- velopment. Psychological Assessment, 12, 102–111.
perament and character inventory (TCI-R) and the TCI-140. Psychological Assessment, SPSS, I. (2011). IBM SPSS statistics for Windows, version 20.0. New York: IBM Corp.
20(3), 281–291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012934. Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estima-
Feldt, L. S. (1980). A test of the hypothesis that Cronbach's alpha coefficient is the same tion approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173–180. http://dx.doi.org/
for two tests administered to the same sample. Psychometrika, 49(1), 99–105. 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4.
García, L. F., Aluja, A., García, Ó., & Cuevas, L. (2005). Is openness to experience an Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor
independent personality dimension? Journal of Individual Differences, 26(3), 132–138. analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02291170.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.26.3.132. Velicer, W. F. (1976). Determining the number of components from the matrix of partial
García, L. F., Escorial, S., García, Ó., Blanch, A., & Aluja, A. (2012). Structural analysis of correlations. Psychometrika, 41(3), 321–327. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
the facets and domains of the Zuckerman–Kuhlman–Aluja Personality Questionnaire BF02293557.
(ZKA–PQ) and the NEO PI–R. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94(2), 156–163. Zuckerman, M. (2002). Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ): An al-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.645935. ternative five-factorial model. In B. de Raad, & M. Perugini (Eds.). Big five assessment
García, Ó., Aluja, A., García, L. F., Escorial, S., & Blanch, A. (2012). Zuckerman-Kuhlman- (pp. 377–396). Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
Aluja Personality Questionnaire (ZKA-PQ) and Cloninger's temperament and char- Zuckerman, M. (2008). Zuckerman-Kuhlman personality questionnaire (ZKPQ): An op-
acter inventory revised (TCI-R): A comparative study. Scandinavian Journal of erational definition of the alternative five factorial model of personality. The SAGE
Psychology, 53(3), 247–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00943.x. handbook of personality theory and assessment: Volume 2 — Personality measurement and
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. testing (pp. 219–238). Thousand Oaks, CA, US: SAGE Publications Ltd. http://dx.doi.
Psychometrika, 30(2), 179–185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447. org/10.4135/9781849200479.n11.
Joireman, J., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2004). The Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Zuckerman, M., & Aluja, A. (2014). Measures of sensation seeking. In G. J. Boyle, D. H.
Questionnaire: Origin, development, and validity of a measure to assess an alter- Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.). Measures of personality and social psychological con-
native five-factor model of personality. On the psychobiology of personality (pp. 49– structs (pp. 352–380). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
64). Elsevier. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-008044209-9/50005-1. B978-0-12-386915-9.00013-9.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A guide to the program and applications. Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., & Camac, C. (1988). What lies beyond E and N? Factor
Chicago: SPSS publications. analyses of scales believed to measure basic dimensions of personality. Journal of
Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Personality and Social Psychology, 54(1), 96–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20(1), 141–151. http://dx.doi.org/10. 3514.54.1.96.
1177/001316446002000116. Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P., & Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison
Lautenschlager, G. J. (1989). ALPHATST: Testing for differences in values of coefficient of three structural models for personality: The big three, the big five, and the alter-
alpha. Applied Psychological Measurement, 13, 284. native five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 757. http://dx.doi.org/
Levy, P. (1967). The correction for spurious correlation in the evaluation of short-form 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.757.
tests. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(1), 84–86 (https://doi.org/10.1002/1097- Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Thornquist, M., & Kiers, H. (1991). Five (or three) robust
4679 (196701)23:1 < 84: AID-JCLP2270230123 > 3.0.CO; 2-2). questionnaire scale factors of personality without culture. Personality and Individual
Levy, P. (1968). Short-form tests: A methodological review. Psychological Bulletin, 69(6), Differences, 12(9), 929–941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869 (91)90182-B.
410–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025736.

181

You might also like