You are on page 1of 19

Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research review

A systematic review of the psychosocial correlates of intuitive eating


among adult women
Lauren J. Bruce, Lina A. Ricciardelli*
School of Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Intuitive eating has been proposed as an eating style that fosters a positive attitude towards food, the
Received 6 May 2015 body, and physical activity. A systematic review of the literature was undertaken to examine intuitive
Received in revised form eating in relation to disordered eating, body image, emotional functioning, and other psychosocial cor-
8 October 2015
relates in adult women. Articles were identified through Academic Search Complete, Cumulative Index to
Accepted 9 October 2015
Available online 22 October 2015
Nursing and Allied Health Literature (CINAHL), Health Source (Nursing and Academic Edition), Medline
Complete, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, PubMed and Scopus.
Eligible studies were those that examined women aged 18 years and older, measured intuitive eating,
Keywords:
Intuitive eating
and assessed a psychosocial correlate of intuitive eating. Twenty-four cross-sectional studies, published
Disordered eating between 2006 and September 2015, met eligibility criteria. Intuitive eating was associated with less
Body image disordered eating, a more positive body image, greater emotional functioning, and a number of other
Emotions psychosocial correlates that have been examined less extensively. However, given that all studies used
Adult women cross-sectional designs, no conclusions regarding the direction of the relationship between intuitive
Systematic review eating and psychosocial correlates can be drawn. Participants in the majority of studies were university
students in the United States so findings cannot be generalised to the wider population of female adults.
Prospective studies are now needed to verify these cross-sectional findings, and show if intuitive eating
may reduce disordered eating and body image concerns, and promote women's psychological health and
well-being.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
2.1. Search strategy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
2.2. Eligibility criteria . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
2.3. Selection process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 455
2.4. Data extraction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 456
2.5. Assessment of study quality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 456
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 456
3.1. Methodological quality of studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 456
3.2. Summary of included studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 466
3.3. Eating attitudes and behaviours . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 466
3.4. Body image . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467
3.5. Emotional functioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467
3.6. Other psychosocial correlates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 467

* Corresponding author. School of Psychology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood


Highway, Burwood VIC 3125, Australia.
E-mail address: lina.ricciardelli@deakin.edu.au (L.A. Ricciardelli).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.012
0195-6663/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472 455

4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 468
4.1. Limitations and recommendations for future research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 469
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 470

1. Introduction practices.
The aim of this study was to conduct a systematic review of the
Extensive research shows that weight loss strategies that pro- literature with a focus on intuitive eating in adult women, in
mote the restriction of food intake are largely ineffective for long relation to other eating attitudes and behaviours, body image, and
term weight loss and weight maintenance (Field et al., 2003; Mann emotional functioning. These are three of the main domains that
et al., 2007; Neumark-Sztainer, Wall, Story, & Standish, 2012). address the principles underlying the intuitive eating approach
Moreover, restrictive eating practices have been associated with a (Tribole & Resch, 2012). In order to make a significant contribution
higher body mass index (BMI) (Enriquez, Duncan, & Schur, 2013; to the field, the review also systematically examined all other
Quick & Byrd-Bredbenner, 2012; Rideout & Barr, 2009; van Strien, psychosocial correlates that have been studied in relation to intu-
Herman, & Verheijden, 2014); weight gain (Field et al., 2003; Mann itive eating. This includes factors such as self-esteem, messages
€inen, Saarni, Kaprio, & Rissanen, 2012; Ulen,
et al., 2007; Pietila received by women relating to eating and body size and shape, and
Huizinga, Beech, & Elasy, 2008); an increased risk of disordered motivation for engaging in physical activity. Women were the focus
eating (Appleton & McGowan, 2006; Goldschmidt, Wall, Loth, Le of this review as a majority of intuitive eating studies have been
Grange, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2012; Holmes, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, conducted with women. Women are also more likely than men to
Skouteris, & Broadbent, 2014; Kenardy, Brown, & Vogt, 2001; engage in restrictive and disordered eating practices for the pur-
Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2006); and psychological problems such as pose of managing their weight (Enriquez et al., 2013; Forrester-
emotional difficulties, body image concerns and reduced cognitive Knauss & Zemp Stutz, 2012; French, Jeffery, & Wing, 1994) and
functioning (Appleton & McGowan, 2006; Hawks, Madanat, & are at greater risk of developing an eating disorder (e.g., Elgin &
Christley, 2008; Kenardy et al., 2001). Pritchard, 2006).
Intuitive eating has been proposed as an eating style that en-
courages a positive relationship with food, the body, and physical 2. Methods
activity (Tribole & Resch, 2012). The principles of intuitive eating
include: focussing on physical cues for hunger and satiety; The review of quantitative studies was conducted according to
permitting an unconditional permission to eat; making food the guidelines specified by the Preferred Reporting Items for Sys-
choices for both health and eating satisfaction; not using food to tematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher,
cope with emotions; respecting the body regardless of weight and Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman, & PRISMA Group, 2010).
shape; and being physically active for the enjoyment and health
rather than calorie-burning for weight loss (Tribole & Resch, 2012). 2.1. Search strategy
This approach discourages a focus on weight control, though the
emergence and maintenance of an individual's natural weight and A search was conducted between September, 2014 and
shape is acknowledged as a potential outcome (Tribole & Resch, September, 2015 of the following bibliographic databases: Aca-
2012). The Intuitive Eating Scale (IES-T) is one scale, developed by demic Search Complete, Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied
Tylka (2006), to assess the principles proposed by Tribole and Resch Health Literature Complete (CINAHL Complete), Health Source
(2012). The scale assesses three key features: (1) relying on internal (Nursing and Academic Edition), Medline Complete, PsycINFO,
hunger and satiety cues to guide food intake; (2) permitting oneself PsycARTICLES, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection,
to eat unconditionally; and (3) eating for physical rather than PubMed and Scopus. All databases were accessed using EbscoHost
emotional reasons. Tylka and Kroon Van Diest (2013) developed a with the exception of the PubMed and Scopus databases. Papers
revised version of the scale and identified a fourth feature: making were limited to quantitative studies published in peer-reviewed
food choices to enhance body functioning. An alternative measure journals in the English language up until September 2015. An
is the Intuitive Eating Scale (IES-H; Hawks, Merrill, & Madanat, example of the search strategy and search terms used is provided in
2004) which also assesses four features: intrinsic eating (i.e., Box 1.
motivation to eat based on internal cues of hunger); extrinsic eating
(i.e., lack of eating based on external cues); antidieting (i.e., 2.2. Eligibility criteria
disagreement with dieting behaviours); and self-care (i.e., focus on
health/fitness rather than appearance). Eligible studies were those that included: (1) females aged 18
A review of the literature conducted by Van Dyke and years and older; (2) a measure of intuitive eating; and (3) assessed a
Drinkwater (2014) has shown that intuitive eating (defined as psychosocial correlate of intuitive eating. Studies were excluded for
any eating approach based on hunger and satiety that does not the following reasons: (1) sampled from females under the age of
restrict food type, unless for medical reasons) is associated with a 18 years; (2) sampled only from males; (3) sampled from both
lower BMI; weight maintenance but not weight loss; and factors males and females but findings were not analysed by gender; (4)
such as body image, self-esteem, affect, optimism, and life satis- examined the effect of an intuitive eating intervention but did not
faction. The relationship between intuitive eating and health be- include a measure of intuitive eating; or (5) did not assess a psy-
haviours such as physical activity and dietary intake was less clear chosocial correlate of intuitive eating.
(Van Dyke & Drinkwater, 2014). A major limitation of this previous
review is that it was not a systematic review and it did not examine 2.3. Selection process
intuitive eating in relation to other eating attitudes and behav-
iours, such as disordered eating, dieting, and other weight loss The first author independently screened the titles and abstracts
456 L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

Box 1
Search terms and strategy Records identified through
database searching
(n = 349)
“Intuitive Eating” OR “intuitive eater” OR “eating intuitively”
OR “Intuitive Eating Scale”.
Combined with each of the following:
Records after
psych* OR soci* OR emotions OR affect OR depression OR duplicates removed
anxiety OR stress OR body image OR body acceptance OR (n = 237)
body satisfaction OR eating disorders OR disordered eating
OR eating behaviour
correlat* OR predict* OR factor OR associat* OR determin*
Records screened Records excluded
Limiters: peer reviewed, English language, adult 18 years (n = 237) (n = 200)
and older, female
349 articles found

Full-text articles assessed Full-text articles excluded,


for eligibility with reasons
of identified citations for potential eligibility. If eligibility criteria (n = 16)
(n = 37)
could not be determined then papers were read in their entirety. • Did not assess intuitive
Both authors then examined the full texts of potential papers for eating (n=2)
eligibility criteria. Fig. 1 shows a flow diagram of the processing of • Did not examine
the initial literature review. psychosocial correlates
Papers included in the of intuitive eating (n=4)
review
• Included males in sample
2.4. Data extraction (n = 21)
and did not analyse
findings by gender (n= 9)
Data extracted from each study included: authors, date of • Research protocol (n=1)
publication, and country; study design, sample size and participant
characteristics; measure used to assess intuitive eating; psycho-
Fig. 1. Flow diagram of included studies.
social correlates and measure used; and study findings. These data
were extracted to allow for comparison across studies. Where
studies reported multiple summary statistics, data extracted were Diest & Tylka, 2010; Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013;
limited to bivariate analyses that were evaluated against Cohen's Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). All studies reported in each of the six pa-
criteria for strength of association (Rosenthal, 1996). pers met eligibility criteria except two studies in Tylka (2006), two
studies in Tylka and Kroon Van Diest (2013), and one study each in
2.5. Assessment of study quality Kroon Van Diest and Tylka (2010) and Dittmann et al. (2009). Each
study in the six papers mentioned previously was assessed sepa-
The Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary rately, resulting in a total of 24 studies that met eligibility criteria.1
Research Papers from a Variety of Fields (Kmet, Lee, & Cook, 2004)
was used to assess the quality of eligible studies. It is a widely used 3.1. Methodological quality of studies
tool for assessing the quality of study designs (e.g., Bukowska-
Durawa & Luszczynska, 2014; Fiszer, Dolbeault, Sultan, & Bre dart, Quality assessment criteria were applied to each of the 24
2014; Satherley, Howard, & Higgs, 2015). Each study was scored studies. Authors used the cut-off recommended by Kmet et al.
according to a set of criteria using 14 items. Items assessed criteria (2004) for article inclusion. Studies meeting at least 75 percent of
such as the inclusion of study objectives, study design and method criteria were included for review. All 24 studies met the recom-
of subject selection, description of analyses, and adequate reporting mended cut-off. The summary score for each study according to
of results. Scores for each item were indicative of the degree to relevant criteria is presented in Table 1. While the summary score
which each paper met specific criteria (‘yes’ ¼ 2, ‘partial’ ¼ 1, was useful in assessing the eligibility of studies based on method-
‘no’ ¼ 0, or ‘N/A’ ¼ not applicable). Three criteria were not appli- ological quality, authors also reported the strengths and weak-
cable for all 24 studies as they related to randomised designs. A nesses according to the relevant criteria that were common across
summary score was calculated for each study by dividing the sum studies. Methodological strengths of studies included: describing
of each relevant item score by the total possible score. study objectives and participant characteristics; describing ana-
lyses and results in sufficient detail; and appropriate conclusions
3. Results

1
The initial search produced 349 records. After the removal of Eleven eligible studies published between 2009 and 2015 were not examined in
the review by Van Dyke and Drinkwater (2014) (Brown et al., 2012; Carbonneau
duplicates, 237 potential papers were identified, and of these, 200
et al., 2015; Dittmann et al., 2009; Galloway et al., 2010; Gast et al., 2015; Herbert
abstracts did not meet eligibility criteria. Authors then examined et al., 2013; Homan & Cavanaugh, 2013; Schoenefeld & Webb, 2013; Shouse &
the full texts of 37 potential papers to determine eligibility for in- Nilsson, 2011; Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Homan, 2015). Five cross-sectional
clusion in the review. A further 16 papers did not meet eligibility studies reviewed by Van Dyke and Drinkwater (2014) were not eligible for inclu-
criteria. Further information on reasons for exclusion is provided in sion in this review due to the following reasons: (1) two studies included males in
their sample but findings were not analysed by gender (Hawks, Merrill, & Madanat,
Fig. 1. This resulted in 21 eligible papers published between 2006 2004; Hawks, Merrill, Madanat, & Miyagawa et al., 2004); (2) two studies were not
and 2015, six of which reported multiple studies (Avalos & Tylka, published in a peer-reviewed publication (Banks, 2008; Nielson, 2009); and (3) one
2006; Dittmann, Freedman, Beddoe, & Waldrop, 2009; Kroon Van study assessed mindful eating, not intuitive eating (Framson et al., 2009).
Table 1
Summary of cross-sectional studies examining psychosocial correlates of intuitive eating.

Reference Country Participant characteristics Measure of Psychosocial correlate(s) Measure(s) of psychosocial Main findings Quality
Study design intuitive eating correlate(s) assessment
Sore (%)

Augustus-Horvath Participants: emerging, early and IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Body acceptance by others Body Acceptance by Others Scale IE positively associated with body 90.91
& Tylka (2011) middle adult females Body appreciation (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) acceptance by others in emerging
United States Mean age: emerging: 19.47 years Resistance in adopting the observer Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos (r ¼ .43, p < .001), early (r ¼ .50,
(SD 1.90); early: 32.63 years (SD perspective of the body et al., 2005) p < .001), and middle adulthood
4.06); middle: 51.38 years (SD 7.07) Perceived social support Body Surveillance subscale, (r ¼ .45, p < .05).
Age range: 18e65 years Objectified Body Consciousness IE positively related to body
Ethnicity: Caucasian 82.4%, African Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) appreciation in emerging (r ¼ .60,
American 6.2%, Other 11.4% Social Provisions Scale (Cutrona & p < .001), early (r ¼ .56, p < .001)
Mean BMI: emerging, 23.95 kg/m2; Russell, 1987) and middle adulthood (r ¼ .60,
early, 25.55 kg/m2; middle, p < .05).
28.86 kg/m2 IE positively associated with
Sample size: 801 (emerging, 318; resisting an observer perspective of
early, 238; middle, 245) the body (r ¼ .52, p < .001), early
Study design: cross-sectional (r ¼ .53, p < .001, and middle
adulthood (r ¼ .52, p < .05).

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


IE positively related to perceived
social support for emerging (r ¼ .24,
p < .001), early (r ¼ .35, p < .001),
and middle (r ¼ .23, p < .05)
adulthood.
Avalos & Tylka (2006) Study One: IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Body acceptance by others Body Acceptance by Others Scale Study One: Study One 90.00
United States Participants: female university Body function (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) IE positively associated with body Study Two 90.00
students Body appreciation Body Surveillance Subscale, acceptance by others (r ¼ .45,
Mean age: 20.24 years (SD 5.17) Perceived unconditional acceptance Objectified Body Consciousness p < .001), body function, (r ¼ .43,
Age range: 17e55 years from others Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) p < .001) and body appreciation
Ethnicity: Caucasian 82.2%, African Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos (r ¼ .58, p < .001), but not perceived
American 5%, Other 12.8% et al., 2005) general unconditional acceptance
Sample size: 181 Barrett-Lennard Relationship from others (r ¼ .07, ns).
Study design: cross-sectional Inventory (Claiborn, Crawford, & Study Two:
Study Two: Hackman, 1983) IE positively related to perceived
Participants: female university unconditional acceptance from
students others (r ¼ .19, p < .01), body
Mean age: 19.92 years (SD 4.60) acceptance by others (r ¼ .38,
Age range: 17e50 years p < .001), body function (r ¼ .50,
Ethnicity: Caucasian 77.6%, African <.001), and body appreciation
American 9.1%, Other 13.3% (r ¼ .64, p < .001).
Sample size: 416
Study design: cross-sectional
Brown et al. (2012) Participants: female university IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Self-oriented perfectionism Multidimensional Perfectionism IE inversely associated with self- 85.00
United States students Scale (Hewitt & Flett, 1991) oriented perfectionism (b ¼ .40,
Mean age: 19.2 years (SD 2.5) p < .01)
Age range: 18e35 years
Ethnicity: Caucasian 66.7%, African
American 4.2%, Asian 18.8%, Other
10.3%
Sample size: 48
Study design: cross-sectional
Carbonneau et al. (2015) Participants: French-Canadian IES-2 (Tylka & Kroon Mother's interpersonal Perceived Parental Autonomy IE positively correlated with the 90.00
women in a heterosexual Van Diest, 2013) style (autonomy- Support Scale (Mageau et al., 2015) autonomy-supportive
relationship supported, controlling) The 6-item autonomy interpersonal style of the mother
Mean age: 29.86 years (SD 6.57) Partner's interpersonal support subscale (Koestner, Powers, (r ¼ .23, p < .001) but not the
Age range: 20e45 years style (autonomy- Carbonneau, Milyavskaya, & Chua, partner (r ¼ .09, ns), and negatively
Ethnicity: Caucasian 94.9%, Other supported, controlling) 2012) correlated with the controlling
(continued on next page)

457
Table 1 (continued )

458
Reference Country Participant characteristics Measure of Psychosocial correlate(s) Measure(s) of psychosocial Main findings Quality
Study design intuitive eating correlate(s) assessment
Sore (%)

5.2% Eating regulation (autonomous, Adapted version of two scales to interpersonal style of the mother
Sample size: 272 controlled) assess partner's controlling (r ¼ .27, p < .001) but not the
Study design: cross-sectional interpersonal style (Bartholomew, partner (r ¼ .09, ns).
Ntoumanis, & Thogersen- UPE subscale positively correlated
Ntoumani, 2010; Moreau & with the autonomy-supportive
Mageau, 2012) interpersonal style of the mother
Regulation of Eating Behaviors (r ¼ .17, p < .01) but not the partner
Scale (Pelletier et al., 2004). (r ¼ .12, ns), and negatively
correlated with the controlling
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .20, p < .01) but not the
partner (r ¼ .07, ns).
EPR Subscale positively correlated
with the autonomy-supportive
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .16, p < .01) but not the partner
(r ¼ .05, ns), and negatively

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


correlated with the controlling
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .23, p < .01) but not the
partner (r ¼ .06, ns).
RHSC subscale positively correlated
with the autonomy-supportive
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .19, p < .01) but not the partner
(r ¼ .00, ns), and negatively
correlated with the controlling
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .20, p < .01) but not the
partner (r ¼ .09, ns).
B-FCC subscale positively correlated
with the autonomy-supportive
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .17, p < .01) and the partner
(r ¼ .19, p < .01), but did not
correlate with the controlling
interpersonal style of the mother
(r ¼ .07, ns) or the partner
(r ¼ .03, ns).
IE positively correlated with
autonomous eating regulation
(r ¼ .29, p < .001) and negatively
associated with controlled eating
regulation (r ¼ .47, p < .001).
UPE subscale negatively correlated
with both autonomous eating
regulation (r ¼ .15, p < .05) and
controlled eating regulation
(r ¼ .29, p < .001).
EPR subscale positively correlated
with autonomous eating regulation
(r ¼ .20, p < .01) and negatively
associated with controlled eating
regulation (r ¼ .40, p < .001).
RHSC subscale positively correlated
with autonomous eating regulation
(r ¼ .33, p < .001) and negatively
associated with controlled eating
regulation (r ¼ .36, p < .001).
B-FCC subscale positively correlated
with autonomous eating regulation
(r ¼ .58, p < .001) and negatively
associated with controlled eating
regulation (r ¼ .24, p < .001).
Denny et al. (2013) Participants: young adult males and Two items from Chronic dieting One item each assessing chronic Women who reported trusting their 86.36
United States females IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Unhealthy weight control practices dieting, unhealthy weight control, body to tell them how much to eat
. Mean age (males and females): 25.3 (fasting, starvation, food and extreme weight control reported lower odds of chronic
years (SD 1.7) supplement, skipping meals, practices dieting (OR ¼ .58, 95% CI ¼ [.38, .87],
Age range: not reported smoking) Two items assessing binge eating p < .05), unhealthy (OR ¼ .55,
Ethnicity (females): Caucasian 45.6%, Extreme weight control practices CI ¼ [.41, .72], p < .05) and extreme
African American 21.2%, Asian (diet pills, laxatives, diuretics) (OR ¼ .62, CI ¼ [.45, .85], p < .05)
18.8%, Other 14.4% Binge eating weight control practices, and binge
Mean BMI: not reported. Females eating (OR ¼ .45, [.31, .65], p < .05)
classified as normal weight than women who did not trust their
(n ¼ 630), overweight body to tell them how much to eat
(n ¼ 297), obese (n ¼ 305) (controlled for ethnicity, socio-

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


Sample size: 2287 (females, economic status and BMI).
n ¼ 1257) Women who reported that they
Study design: stopped eating when full reported
cross-sectional lower odds of chronic dieting
(OR ¼ .62, CI ¼ [.41, .95], p < .05)
and binge eating (OR ¼ .34,
CI ¼ [.23, .48], p < .05), but not
unhealthy (OR ¼ .75, CI ¼ [.55,
1.02], ns) or extreme weight control
practices (OR ¼ .81, CI ¼ [.58, 1.14],
ns) (controlled for ethnicity, socio-
economic status and BMI).
Dittmann et al. (2009) Participants: females who regularly IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Body satisfaction Single item assessing body IE positively associated with body 86.36
United States practice yoga Body awareness satisfaction satisfaction (r ¼ .47, p < .01), body
Mean age: 47.4 years (SD 11.19) Body responsiveness Observe subscale, Five Facet awareness (r ¼ .22, p < .01) and
Age range: 22e72 years Spiritual readiness Mindfulness Questionnaire (Baer, body responsiveness (r ¼ .415,
Ethnicity: majority Caucasian Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & p < .01), but not spiritual readiness
(proportion not reported) Toney, 2006) (r ¼ .036, ns).
Mean BMI: 22.2 kg/m2 7-item scale assessing body
Sample size: 157 responsiveness (Daubenmier, 2005)
Study design: 17-item scale assessing spiritual
cross-sectional readiness
Galloway et al. (2010) Participants: male and female IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Caregiver eating messages (parent- Child Feeding Questionnaire for UPE subscale not associated with 90.91
United States university students reported) Children (Carper, Orlet Fisher, & pressure to eat (r ¼ .08, ns),
Mean age (females): 18.5 years (SD Birch, 2000), adapted version restriction (r ¼ .05, ns), or
.95) completed by parents. monitoring (r ¼ .19, ns) caregiver
Age range (males and females): 17 eating messages.
e23 years RHSC subscale were not associated
Ethnicity (males and females): with pressure to eat (r ¼ .07, ns),
Caucasian 96%, African American restriction (r ¼ .03, ns), or
3%, Asian American 1% monitoring (r ¼ .17, ns) caregiver
Sample size: 93 (females, n ¼ 71) eating messages.
Study design: cross-sectional EPR subscale inversely associated
with caregiver eating messages of
restriction (r ¼ .33, p < .01) and
monitoring (r ¼ .40, p < .01) but
not pressure to eat caregiver eating
messages (r ¼ .13, ns).

459
(continued on next page)
Table 1 (continued )

460
Reference Country Participant characteristics Measure of Psychosocial correlate(s) Measure(s) of psychosocial Main findings Quality
Study design intuitive eating correlate(s) assessment
Sore (%)

Gast et al. (2015) Participants: female university IES-H (Hawks, Merrill, Motivation for physical activity Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Total IES-H scores inversely 90.91
United States students & Madanat, 2004) (external pressure, guilt or shame, Questionnaire (Mullan, Markland, & associated with external regulation
Mean age: 19.58 (SD 2.42) values physical activity, feels Ingledew, 1997) (b ¼ .008, p < .05) and introjected
Age range: not reported pleasure in physical activity) regulation (b ¼ .04, p < .05), and
Ethnicity: Caucasian 90%, Hispanic positively associated with intrinsic
4%, Asian 3%, Other 4% regulation (b ¼ .005, p < .05), but
Mean BMI: 23.23 kg/m2 not identified regulation (b ¼ .0001,
Underweight 6.5%, Normal weight ns).
69%, Overweight 17.5%, Obese 7% Extrinsic subscale negatively
Sample size: 200 correlated with introjected
Study design: cross-sectional regulation (r ¼ .482, p < .0001) but
not external (r ¼ .141, ns),
identified (r ¼ .103, ns) or intrinsic
(r ¼ .088, ns) regulation.
Antidieting subscale inversely
associated with external (r ¼ .348,
p < .0001) and introjected

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


(r ¼ .563, p < .0001) regulation,
and positively associated with
intrinsic regulation (r ¼ .206,
p < .05) but did not correlate with
identified regulation (r ¼ .07, ns).
Intrinsic subscale did not correlate
with any of the four regulation
types (p > .0003).
Self-care subscale correlated
negatively with introjected
regulation (r ¼ .227, p < .001) and
positively with identified
regulation (r ¼ .291, p < .0001) and
intrinsic regulation (r ¼ .433,
p < .0001), but not external
regulation (r ¼ .151, ns).
*Bonferroni adjustments applied to
correlations with IES-H subscales
(alpha level of .0003).
Herbert et al. (2013) Participants: female university IES-T (Tylka, 2006) State anxiety StateeTrait-Anxiety Inventory IE total scores were not associated 90.00
Germany students Trait anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, with state anxiety (r ¼ .02, ns) or
Mean age: 25.4 years (SD 4.8) Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) trait anxiety (r ¼ .05, ns). IES-T
Age range: not reported subscale scores were not associated
Ethnicity: not reported with state anxiety or trait anxiety
Mean BMI: 22.21 kg/m2 (p > .05).
Sample size:111
Study design: cross-sectional
Homan & Cavanaugh Participants: female undergraduate IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Parent attachment Anxiety and Avoidance subscales, Intuitive eating negatively 90.00
(2013) students from a Christian liberal Anxious and avoidant attachment Attachment to God Inventory (Beck associated with an anxious
United States arts college. to God & McDonald, 2004). attachment to God (r ¼ .30,
Mean age: 20 (SD .88) Parent Trust subscale, Inventory of p < .01). After controlling for
Age range: 18e22 years Parent and Peer Attachment religious service attendance, the
Ethnicity: Caucasian 95.2%; Other (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). relationship remained significant
4.8% (b ¼ .29, p ¼ .006).
Sample size: 104 Intuitive eating did not correlate
Study design: cross-sectional with an avoidant attachment to God
(r ¼ .07, ns) and was not
significantly associated with parent
attachment (r ¼ .18, ns).
Iannantuono & Tylka Participants: female university IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Body appreciation Body Appreciation IE positively related to body 90.00
(2012) students Caregiver eating messages Scale (Avalos et al., 2005) appreciation (r ¼ .58, p < .001) and
United States Mean age: 19.1 years (SD 1.7) Adult attachment Caregiver Eating Messages Scale negatively associated with
Age range: 18e28 years Perfectionism (Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010) depressive symptomatology
Ethnicity: Caucasian 86.3%, African Depressive symptomatology Experiences in Close Relationships (r ¼ .35, p < .001), discrepancy
American 4.8%, Other 8.9% Scale (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, perfectionism (r ¼ .45, p < .001),
Mean BMI: 23.1 kg/m2 1998) avoidant (r ¼ .22, p < .001) an
Sample size: 249 Almost Perfect Scale-Revised anxious attachment (r ¼ .43,
Study design: (Slaney, Rice, Mobley, Trippi, & p < .001), and restrictive/critical
cross-sectional Ashby, 2001) caregiver eating messages
Beck Depression Inventory-II (Beck, (r ¼ .40, p < .001).
Steer, & Brown, 1996) IE not associated with pressure to
eat caregiver eating messages
(r ¼ .11, ns), high standards
perfectionism (r ¼ .03, ns) or order
perfectionism (r ¼ .07, ns).
Kroon Van Diest Participants: male and female IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Caregiver eating messages Caregiver Eating Messages Scale IE had a positive relationship with 90.91
& Tylka (2010) university students Perceived body acceptance by (Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010) perceived body acceptance by
United States Mean age (males and females): 20.87 family Body Acceptance by Others Scale family (r ¼ .49, p < .05) and body

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


years (SD 3.92) Body appreciation (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) appreciation (r ¼ .53, p < .05), and a
Age range: 18e35 years Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos negative relationship with
Ethnicity (males and females): et al., 2005) restrictive/critical caregiver eating
Caucasian 91.6%, African American messages (r ¼ .42, p < .05) in
2.5%, Asian 2.5%, Other 3.4% females. IE did not correlate with
Mean BMI (females) 24.80 kg/m2 pressure to eat caregiver eating
Sample size: 238 messages (r ¼ .01, ns).
(females, n ¼ 160)
Study design: cross-sectional
Madden et al. (2012) Participants: mid-age females IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Binge eating One item assessing frequency of A one unit increase in IE was 95.00
New Zealand Mean age: 45.5 years (SD 3.2) binge eating in the past year associated with a 1.62% (CI 1.79,
Age range: 40e50 years 1.46; p < .001) decrease in binge
Ethnicity: Caucasian 80.3%, Maori eating frequency.
11.4%, Asian 5.3%, Pacific 3.0%
Mean BMI: 25.8 kg/m2
Sample size: 1441
Study design: cross-sectional
Oh et al. (2012) Participants: IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Perceived unconditional acceptance Barrett-Lennard Relationship After controlling for BMI, IE 90.00
United States female college athletes from significant others Inventory (BarretteLennard, 1962) positively associated with body
Mean age: 19.88 years (SD 1.91) Body acceptance by others Body Acceptance by Others Scale acceptance by others (r ¼ .40,
Age range: 18e23 years Body function (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) p < .001), body function (r ¼ .57,
Ethnicity: Caucasian 88.8%, African Body appreciation Body Surveillance subscale, p < .001) and body appreciation
American 4.4%, Other 6.8% Objectified Body Consciousness (r ¼ .48, p < .001), but not perceived
Sample size: 160 Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) unconditional acceptance by others
Study design: Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos (r ¼ .12, ns).
cross-sectional et al., 2005)
Schoenefeld Participants: female university IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Self-compassion Self-compassion Scale (Neff, 2003) IE correlated positively with self- 90.00
& Webb (2013) students Distress tolerance Distress Tolerance Scale (Simons & compassion (r ¼ .39, p < .01),
United States Mean age: 19.48 years (SD 1.46) Body image flexibility Gaher, 2005) distress tolerance (r ¼ .21, p < .01),
Age range: 18e24 years Self-esteem Body Image-Acceptance and Action body image flexibility (r ¼ .69,
Ethnicity: Caucasian 67.4%, African Questionnaire (Hayes, Strosahl, & p < .01), and self-esteem (r ¼ .40,
American 21.1%, Other 11.5% Wilson, 1999) p < 01).
Mean BMI : 23.55 kg/m2 Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale
Sample size: 322 (Rosenberg, 1965)
Study design: cross-sectional
Shouse & Nilsson Participants: female university IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Self-silencing Silencing the Self Scale (Jack & Dill, IE correlated negatively with self- 90.91
(2011) students Disordered eating 1992) silencing (r ¼ .28, p < .01) and
United States Mean age: 20.8 years (SD 1.9) Emotional awareness Eating Attitudes Test-26 (Garner disordered eating (r ¼ .58,

461
(continued on next page)
Table 1 (continued )

462
Reference Country Participant characteristics Measure of Psychosocial correlate(s) Measure(s) of psychosocial Main findings Quality
Study design intuitive eating correlate(s) assessment
Sore (%)

Age range: 18e24 years et al., 1982) p < .01), and correlated positively
Ethnicity: Caucasian 52%, African Clarity of Feeling subscale, Trait with emotional awareness (r ¼ .19,
American 36%, Asian 4%, Other 8% Meta-Mood Scale (Salovey, Mayer, p < .05).
Sample size: 140 Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995)
Study design:
cross-sectional
Tylka (2006) Study One: IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Study One: Eating Attitudes Test-26 (Garner Study One: Study One: 90.00
United States Participants: female university Disordered eating et al., 1982) IE was negatively related to Study Two: 90.00
students Body dissatisfaction Body Dissatisfaction and disordered eating (r ¼ .66,
Mean age: 20.85 years (SD 6.21) Interoceptive awareness Interoceptive Awareness subscales, p < .001), body dissatisfaction
Age range: 17e61 years Pressure for thinness from Eating Disorder Iventory-2 (Garner, (r ¼ .53, p < .001), poor
Ethnicity: Caucasian 87.7%, Asian significant others 1991) interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .46,
3.8%, African American 3.1%, Other Internalisation of the thin-ideal Perceived Sociocultural Pressures p < .001), pressure for thinness
5.4% Study Two: Scale (Stice, 1996) (r ¼ .52, p < .001) and
Sample size: 391 Self-esteem Internalization subscale, internalisation of the thin-ideal
Study design: cross-sectional Optimism Sociocultural Attitudes Toward (r ¼ .47, p < .001).

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


Study Two: Proactive coping Appearance Questionnaire UPE subscale negatively associated
Participants: female university Global life satisfaction (Heinberg, Thompson, & Stormer, with disordered eating (r ¼ .72,
students 1995) p < .001), body dissatisfaction
Mean age: 19.70 years (SD 4.50) Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (r ¼ .44, p < .001), poor
Age range: 17e50 years (Rosenberg, 1965) interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .31,
Ethnicity: Caucasian 86.2%, Asian Life Orientation TestdRevised p < .001), pressure for thinness
5.3%, African American 3.9%, Other (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) (r ¼ .41, p < .001), and
4.5% Proactive Coping subscale, internalisation of the thin-ideal
Sample size: 476 Proactive Coping Inventory (r ¼ .41, p < .001).
Study design: cross-sectional (Greenglass, Schwarzer, & Taubert, EPR subscale negatively associated
1999) with disordered eating (r ¼ .23,
Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener, p < .001), body dissatisfaction
Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985) (r ¼ .31, p < .001), poor
interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .41,
p < .001), pressure for thinness
(r ¼ .37, p < .001), and
internalisation of the thin-ideal
(r ¼ .29, p < .001).
RHSC subscale negatively
associated with disordered eating
(r ¼ .27, p < .001), body
dissatisfaction (r ¼ .35, p < .001),
poor interoceptive awareness
(r ¼ .28, p < .001), pressure for
thinness (r ¼ .28, p < .001), and
internalisation of the thin-ideal
(r ¼ .23, p < .001).
Study Two:
IE positively correlated with self-
esteem (r ¼ .44, p < .001), optimism
(r ¼ .29, p < .001), proactive coping
(r ¼ .29, p < .001), and life
satisfaction (r ¼ .41, p < .001).
EPR subscale positively related to
self-esteem (r ¼ .28, p < .001),
optimism (r ¼ .14, p < .01), and life
satisfaction (r ¼ .26, p < .001) but
not proactive coping (r ¼ .10, ns)
UPE positively related to self-
esteem (r ¼ .36, p < .001), optimism
(r ¼ .24, p < .001), proactive coping
(r ¼ .27, p < .001) and life
satisfaction (r ¼ .26, p < .001).
RHSC subscale positively related to
self-esteem (r ¼ .35, p < .001),
optimism (r ¼ .31, p < .001),
proactive coping (r ¼ .34, p < .001)
and life satisfaction (r ¼ .38,
p < .001).
Tylka et al. (2015) Participants: male and female IES-2 (Tylka & Kroon Flexible control of eating Flexible Control and Rigid Control IE was negatively associated with 90.00
online community participants Van Diest, 2013) Rigid control of eating subscales, Cognitive rigid control (r ¼ .51, p < .001) and
Mean age (females): 35.01 years (SD Life satisfaction Restraint Scale (Westenhoefer, flexible control (r ¼ .27, p < .001)
12.45) Positive affect Stunkard, & Pudel, 1999) of eating, negative affect (r ¼ .30,
Age range: 18e63 years Negative affect Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener p < .001), poor interoceptive
Ethnicity (males and females): Body appreciation et al., 1985) awareness (r ¼ .60, p < .001),
Caucasian 71.9%, African American Interoceptive awareness Positive and Negative Affect binge eating (r ¼ .68, p < .001),
8.4%, Asian 9.2%, Other 10.4% Binge eating Schedule-Expanded (Watson, Clark, and food preoccupation (r ¼ .65,
Mean BMI (females):26.82 kg/m2 Food preoccupation & Tellegen, 1988) p < .001).
Sample size: 382 Body Appreciation Scale-2 (Tylka & IE was positively associated with
(females, n ¼ 192) Wood-Barcalow, 2015) life satisfaction (r ¼ .35, p < .001),

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


Study design: Interoceptive Awareness subscale, positive affect (r ¼ .29, p < .001),
cross-sectional Eating and body appreciation (r ¼ .64,
Disorder Inventory-2 (Garner, p < .001).
1991)
Binge Eating Scale (Gormally et al.,
1982)
Frequency subscale, Food
Preoccupation
Questionnaire (Tapper & Pothos,
2010)
Tylka & Homan Participants: male and female IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Body acceptance by others Body Acceptance by Others Scale IE correlated positively with body 90.00
(2015) undergraduate university students Internal body orientation (Avalos & Tylka, 2006) acceptance by others (r ¼ .28,
Mean age (females): 19.62 years (SD Exercise motives (functional, Body Surveillance subscale, p < .001), internal body orientation
2.87) appearance) Objectified Body Consciousness (r ¼ .46, p < .001) and body
Age range: 18e47years Body appreciation Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) appreciation (r ¼ .48, p < .001).
Ethnicity (males and females): The Function of Exercise Scale IE correlated negatively with
Caucasian 88.5%, African American (DiBartolo, Lin, Montoya, Neal, & appearance exercise motives
5.2%, Asian 2.0%, Other 4.2% Shaffer, 2007) (r ¼ .61, p < .001) but did not
Mean BMI (females) 22.59 kg/m2 Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos correlated with functional exercise
Sample size: 406 et al., 2005) motives (r ¼ .10, ns).
(females, n ¼ 258)
Study design: cross-sectional
Tylka & Kroon Van Participants: male and female IES-2 (Tylka & Kroon Disordered eating Eating Attitudes IE negatively associated with 90.91
Diest (2013) university students Van Diest, 2013) Interoceptive awareness Teste26 (Garner et al., 1982) disordered eating (r ¼ .50,
United States Mean age (males and females): 20.45 Body appreciation Interoceptive Awareness subscale, p < .001), body surveillance
years (SD 5.06) Body surveillance Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (r ¼ .36, p < .001), body shame
Age range (males and females): 18 Body shame (Garner, 1991) (r ¼ .56, p < .001), internalisation
e53 years Internalisation of thin media ideal Body Appreciation Scale (Avalos (r ¼ .37, p < .001), poor
Ethnicity: Caucasian 81.7%, African Self-esteem et al., 2005) interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .35,
American 5.5%, Asian 3.5%, Other Positive and negative affect Body Surveillance and Body Shame p < .001), and negative affect
9.3% Global life satisfaction subscales, Objectified Body (r ¼ .36, p < .001).
Mean BMI (females): 24.02 kg/m2 Consciousness Scale (McKinley & EPR subscale negatively related to
Sample size: 1200 (females, n ¼ 680) Hyde, 1996) disordered eating (r ¼ .20,
Study design: Internalization subscale, p < .001), body surveillance
cross-sectional Sociocultural Attitudes Toward (r ¼ .25, p < .001), body shame
Appearance Questionnaire (r ¼ .35, p < .001), internalisation
eRevised (Heinberg et al., 1995) (r ¼ .30, p < .001), poor
Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .28,

463
(continued on next page)
Table 1 (continued )

464
Reference Country Participant characteristics Measure of Psychosocial correlate(s) Measure(s) of psychosocial Main findings Quality
Study design intuitive eating correlate(s) assessment
Sore (%)

(Rosenberg, 1965) p < .001), and negative affect


Positive and Negative Affect (r ¼ .31, p < .001).
ScheduleeExpanded (Watson et al., UPE subscale negatively related to
1988) disordered eating (r ¼ .68,
Satisfaction With Life Scale (Diener p < .001), body surveillance
et al., 1985) (r ¼ .26, p < .001), body shame
(r ¼ .52, p < .001), internalisation
(r ¼ .30, p < .001), poor
interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .17,
p < .001), and negative affect
(r ¼ .15, p < .001).
RHSC subscale negatively related to
disordered eating (r ¼ .37,
p < .001), body surveillance
(r ¼ .24, p < .001), body shame
(r ¼ .39, p < .001), internalisation
(r ¼ .22, p < .001), poor

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


interoceptive awareness (r ¼ .24,
p < .001), and negative affect
(r ¼ .21, p < .001).
B-FCC subscale negatively related to
body surveillance (r ¼ .19,
p < .001), internalisation (r ¼ .13,
p < .001), poor interoceptive
awareness (r ¼ .13, p < .001), and
negative affect (r ¼ .19, p < .001)
but not disordered eating (r ¼ .05,
ns) or body shame (r ¼ .08, ns).
IE positively associated with body
appreciation (r ¼ .52, p < .001), self-
esteem (r ¼ .41, p < .001), positive
affect (r ¼ .26, p < .001), and life
satisfaction (r ¼ .33, p < .001).
EPR subscale positively associated
with body appreciation (r ¼ .32,
p < .001), self-esteem (r ¼ .26,
p < .001), positive affect (r ¼ .18,
p < .001), and life satisfaction
(r ¼ .28, p < .001).
UPE subscale positively associated
with body appreciation (r ¼ .33,
p < .001), self-esteem (r ¼ .27,
p < .001), but not positive affect
(r ¼ .00, ns), or life satisfaction
(r ¼ .12, ns).
RHSC subscale positively associated
with body appreciation (r ¼ .40,
p < .001), self-esteem (r ¼ .30,
p < .001), positive affect (r ¼ .22,
p < .001), and life satisfaction
(r ¼ .21, p < .001).
B-FCC subscale positively
associated with body appreciation
(r ¼ .29, p < .001), self-esteem
(r ¼ .22, p < .001), positive affect
(r ¼ .37, p < .001), and life
satisfaction (r ¼ .18, p < .001)
Tylka & Wilcox (2006) Study One: IES-T (Tylka, 2006) Study One: Study One: Study One: Study One:
United States Participants: Disordered eating (dieting, bulimia/ Dieting and Bulimia/food UPE negatively associated with 90.00
female university students food preoccupation) preoccupation subscales, Eating dieting (r ¼ .67, p < .001) and Study Two:
Mean age: 18.44 years (SD 1.02) Positive affect Attitudes Teste26 (Garner et al., bulimia/food preoccupation 90.00
Age range: 17e30 years Self-esteem 1982) (r ¼ 40, p < .001).
Ethnicity: Caucasian 85.9%, Asian Proactive coping Positive Affect subscale of Positive EPR negatively associated with
5.0%, African American 5.3%, Other Study Two: and Negative Affect Schedule- dieting (r ¼ .13, p < .05) and
3.8% Unconditional self-regard Expanded (Watson et al., 1988) bulimia/food preoccupation
Sample size: 388 Psychological hardiness Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (r ¼ .26, p < .001).
Study design: cross-sectional Social problem-solving (Rosenberg, 1965) RHSC negatively associated with
Study Two: Proactive Coping subscale of the dieting (r ¼ .35, p < .001) and
Participants: Proactive Coping Inventory bulimia/food preoccupation
female university students (Greenglass et al., 1999) (r ¼ .36, p < .001).
Mean age: 18.72 years (SD 2.44) Study Two: UPE positively associated with self-
Age range: 17e55 years Life Orientation Test-Revised esteem (r ¼ .23, p < .001) and
Ethnicity: Caucasian 81.6%, African (Scheier et al., 1994) proactive coping (r ¼ .11, p < .05)
American 8.3%, Asian 4.3%, Other Unconditional Self-Regard Scale but not positive affect (r ¼ .03, ns).
5.8% (Betz, Wohlgemuth, Serling, EPR positively related to positive
Sample size: 396 Harshbarger, & Klein, 1995) affect (r ¼ .25, p < .001), self-esteem

L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472


Study design: cross-sectional Psychological Hardiness Scale- (r ¼ .30, p < .001) and proactive
Short Form (Betz & Campbell, 2003) coping (r ¼ .26, p < .001).
Social Problem Solving Inventory- RHSC positively related to positive
Revised (D'Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu- affect (r ¼ .33, p < .001), self-esteem
Olivares, 1997) (r ¼ .34, p < .001) and proactive
coping (r ¼ .33, p < .001).
Study Two:
UPE negatively associated with
dieting (r ¼ .73, p < .001) and
bulimia/food preoccupation
(r ¼ .36, p < .001).
EPR negatively associated with
dieting (r ¼ .21, p < .001) and
bulimia/food preoccupation
(r ¼ .37, p < .001).
RHSC negatively associated with
dieting (r ¼ .26, p < .001) and
bulimia/food preoccupation
(r ¼ .30, p < .001).
UPE positively related to optimism
(r ¼ .10, p < .05), unconditional self-
regard (r ¼ .24, p < .001), and
psychological hardiness (r ¼ .11,
p < .05), but not social problem
solving (r ¼ .01, ns).
EPR positively associated with
optimism (r ¼ .25, p < .001),
unconditional self-regard (r ¼ .28,
p < .001), psychological hardiness
(r ¼ .34, p < .001), and social
problem solving (r ¼ .30, p < .001).
RHSC subscale had a positive
relationship with optimism (r ¼ .24,
p < .001), unconditional self-regard
(r ¼ .32, p < .001), psychological
hardiness (r ¼ .25, p < .001) and
social problem solving (r ¼ .23,
p < .001).

465
IE ¼ intuitive eating; IES-T ¼ Intuitive Eating Scale (Tylka, 2006); IES-H ¼ Intuitive Eating Scale (Hawks, Merrill, & Madanat, 2004); IES-2 ¼ Intuitive Eating Scale-2 (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013); EPR ¼ Eating for Physical
Reasons; UPE ¼ Unconditional Permission to Eat; RHSC ¼ Reliance on Hunger/Satiety Cues; B-FCC ¼ BodyeFood Choice Congruence; BMI ¼ Body Mass Index; ns ¼ non-significant correlation.
466 L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

that are drawn from the results. All 24 studies gained full scores for other eating attitudes and behaviours (Denny et al., 2013; Madden
these criteria. Methodological weaknesses that were common et al., 2012; Shouse & Nilsson, 2011; Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van
across studies included: method of subject selection and possible Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). This included disordered eating
measurement bias in the use of self-report measures. A majority of symptomatology, restrained eating and dieting, eating regulation,
studies used convenience sampling methods, such as recruiting and other weight loss practices. Three studies (Shouse & Nilsson,
university students who were part of an existing research pool (e.g., 2011; Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013) used all three
Brown, Parman, Rudat, & Craighead, 2012), or undergraduate psy- subscales of the Eating Attitudes Test-26 (EAT-26; Garner, Olmsted,
chology students who received class credit for their participation in Bohr, & Garfinkel, 1982); two studies used two subscales of the EAT-
the study (e.g., Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 26 (Tylka & Wilcox, 2006); one study (Tylka et al., 2015) assessed
2010; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). One exception was Madden, Leong, binge eating and food preoccupation using The Binge Eating Scale
Gray, and Horwath (2012), who randomly selected the sample (Gormally, Black, Daston, & Rardin, 1982) and a subscale from the
from electoral rolls in New Zealand. All studies used self-report Food Preoccupation Questionnaire (Tapper & Pothos, 2010); and
measures to assess intuitive eating and psychosocial correlates, one study (Carbonneau et al., 2015) used the Regulation of Eating
including the use of individual items (e.g., Denny, Neumark- Behaviors Scale (Pelletier, Dion, Slovinec-D’Angelo, & Reid, 2004) to
Sztainer, Loth, & Eisenberg, 2013; Dittmann et al., 2009; Madden assess regulation of eating. The remaining two studies used one or
et al., 2012). Additionally, a proportion of studies did not consider two individual items to assess binge eating (Denny et al., 2013;
confounding factors in their study, such as participant character- Madden et al., 2012), dieting, and unhealthy or extreme weight
istics. Seventeen of 24 studies considered at least one participant loss practices (Denny et al., 2013).
characteristic in their analyses, including: age, ethnicity, gender, Studies demonstrated that intuitive eating was inversely asso-
body mass index, socioeconomic status, relationship status and ciated with disordered eating symptomatology, according to total
level of education. Overall, studies had more methodological scores on the EAT-26 (Shouse & Nilsson, 2011; Tylka, 2006; Tylka &
strengths than weaknesses. Kroon Van Diest, 2013), and correlated negatively with bulimia and
food preoccupation (Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), binge
3.2. Summary of included studies eating behaviours (Denny et al., 2013; Madden et al., 2012; Tylka
et al., 2015), and dieting (Denny et al., 2013; Tylka & Wilcox,
Table 1 provides a summary of each of the 24 cross-sectional 2006). Additionally, intuitive eating was negatively associated
studies that assessed at least one psychosocial correlate of intui- with two forms of restrained eating: rigid control (an “all-or-
tive eating. The most frequently used measure of intuitive eating nothing” approach to eating) and flexible control (a more balanced
was the 21-item IES-T (Tylka, 2006): nineteen of the 24 studies approach to managing food intake) (Tylka et al., 2015). Correlations
assessed intuitive eating using this measure (Augustus-Horvath & between intuitive eating and disordered eating, including
Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Brown et al., 2012; Dittmann restrained eating and dieting, ranged from .50 to .68 in size.
et al., 2009; Galloway, Farrow, & Martz, 2010; Herbert, Blechert, Studies examining individual subscales of the IES-T (Tylka,
Hautzinger, Matthias, & Herbert, 2013; Homan & Cavanaugh, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006)
2013; Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010; demonstrated relationships consistent with the overall scale for
Madden et al., 2012; Oh, Wiseman, Hendrickson, Phillips, & disordered eating and dieting (Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006).
Hayden, 2012; Schoenefeld & Webb, 2013; Shouse & Nilsson, An exception was Tylka and Kroon Van Diest (2013), who found no
2011; Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Homan, 2015; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). association between the Body-Food Choice Congruence subscale of
The remaining five studies used either the revised 23-item IES-2 the revised IES-T (assesses the extent to which individuals match
(Carbonneau, Carbonneau, Cantin, & Gagnon-Girouard, 2015; food choice with the body's needs) and disordered eating among
Tylka, Calogero, & Danielsdottir, 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, university students. In addition, Denny et al. (2013) showed that
2013); the 27-item IES-H developed by Hawks, Merrill, and women who trusted their body to tell them how much to eat re-
Madanat (2004) (Gast, Campbell Nielson, Hunt, & Leiker, 2015); ported that they were less likely to engage in unhealthy weight loss
or two individual items from the Tylka's (2006) IES-T (Denny et al., practices (i.e., fasting, starvation, food supplementation, skipping
2013). meals, smoking) and extreme weight loss practices (i.e., diet pills,
The majority of studies included predominantly university laxatives and diuretics). However, there was no difference in the
students (n ¼ 17). Other studies included: university athletes (Oh likelihood of engaging in these behaviours between women who
et al., 2012); members of an online community (Tylka et al., did or did not stop eating when experiencing fullness. Correlations
2015); women involved in a heterosexual relationship between intuitive eating subscales and disordered eating, including
(Carbonneau et al., 2015); women aged 40e50 years (Madden dieting, ranged from .13 to .73. Correlations between the Un-
et al., 2012); women in their mid-twenties (Denny et al., 2013); conditional Permission to Eat subscale and disordered eating
three groups of women aged between 18 and 25, 26 to 39, and ranged from .36 to .73. Lower correlations were found between
40e65 years (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011); and women who the Eating for Physical Reasons subscale and disordered eating
regularly practice yoga (Dittmann et al., 2009). All studies except (ranged from .13 to .37), and between the Reliance on Hunger/
for three were conducted in the United States. One of the ex- Satiety Cues subscale and disordered eating (ranged from .23
ceptions was conducted in New Zealand (Madden et al., 2012), to .37).
one in Canada (Carbonneau et al., 2015), and the other in Ger- Carbonneau et al. (2015) investigated intuitive eating and its
many (Herbert et al., 2013). Nine of 21 papers (12 of 24 studies) association with two forms of regulated eating: autonomous (self-
included Tylka as an author. determined or feeling empowered towards eating choices) and
The correlates of intuitive eating in each of the 24 studies were controlled (feeling pressured or controlled in eating choices). Ex-
categorised as follows: eating attitudes and behaviours; body im- amples of items were “… because I take pleasure in fixing healthy
age; emotional functioning; and other psychosocial correlates. meals” (autonomous regulation) and “… because I would feel
ashamed of myself if I was not eating healthy” (controlled regula-
3.3. Eating attitudes and behaviours tion) (Carbonneau et al., 2015). Intuitive eating was positively
associated with autonomous regulation and negatively associated
Eight of the 24 studies examined intuitive eating in relation to with controlled regulation. Further examination of the subscales of
L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472 467

the revised IES-T showed that three of the four subscales reported (Herbert et al., 2013; Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Schoenefeld &
correlations consistent with the overall scale. The exception was Webb, 2013; Shouse & Nilsson, 2011; Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka &
Unconditional Permission to Eat subscale, which was inversely Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), including affect,
associated with both forms of eating regulation. mood and the management of emotions. Intuitive eating was
inversely associated with negative affect, for both total and subscale
3.4. Body image scores of the revised IES-T (Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van
Diest, 2013), and depressive symptomatology (Iannantuono &
Eleven studies examined the relationship between intuitive Tylka, 2012). However, intuitive eating did not correlate with
eating and body image. Body appreciation, the extent to which one levels of state and trait anxiety, according to both total and subscale
has a favourable attitude and treatment of one's body, regardless of scores of the IES-T, among German university students (Herbert
perceived appearance-related flaws, was examined in eight studies et al., 2013). Positive affect was examined in three studies (Tylka
(Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; et al., 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox,
Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010; Oh 2006), each of which demonstrated a positive association be-
et al., 2012; Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Homan, 2015; Tylka & tween intuitive eating and positive affect. Subscale scores for
Kroon Van Diest, 2013). All studies used the 13-item Body Appre- intuitive eating were also assessed in two of three studies. Positive
ciation Scale (Avalos, Tylka, & Wood-Barcalow, 2005) or the revised affect correlated with higher scores on all subscales, except the
10-item scale (Tylka & Wood-Barcalow, 2015). Intuitive eating Unconditional Permission to Eat subscale (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest,
correlated with body appreciation in a positive direction in all eight 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006).
studies, and was consistent for total scores and subscale scores of In addition to aspects of affect and mood, two studies examined
intuitive eating (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013), across three the management of emotions in university students (Schoenefeld &
different age groups (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011), and after Webb, 2013; Shouse & Nilsson, 2011). Shouse and Nilsson (2011)
controlling for BMI (Oh et al., 2012). demonstrated that higher levels of intuitive eating were associ-
Body satisfaction or dissatisfaction (the overall evaluation of ated with greater emotional awareness and less self-silencing of
one's body size, shape or appearance) was examined in two studies emotions. Schoenefeld and Webb (2013) found a positive associa-
(Dittmann et al., 2009; Tylka, 2006). Dittmann et al. (2009) used a tion between intuitive eating and distress tolerance. Correlations
single item to assess body satisfaction, while Tylka (2006) assessed between total intuitive eating scores and positive emotional func-
body dissatisfaction using a subscale of the Eating Disorder In- tioning ranged from .21 to .29 in size. Correlations between total
ventory (EDI-2; Garner, 1991). Both studies demonstrated that intuitive eating scores and negative emotional functioning ranged
eating more intuitively was associated with greater body satisfac- from .28 to .36 in size. Correlations between each subscale of the
tion (Dittmann et al., 2009; Tylka, 2006). Tylka (2006) also IES-T and emotional functioning were similar, except for the Un-
demonstrated that this relationship was consistent when exam- conditional Permission to Eat subscale, which did not correlate
ining subscales of the IES-T. with positive affect. Correlations for the two remaining subscales of
Body surveillance (adopting an observer perspective of the the IES-T ranged from .15 to .31 for negative emotional func-
body, or habitually monitoring appearance), body function or an tioning and .18 to .33 for positive emotional functioning.
internal body orientation (a focus on how the body functions and
feels rather than appearance) were examined in five studies 3.6. Other psychosocial correlates
(Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Oh et al.,
2012; Tylka & Homan, 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013) using Twenty studies examined the association between intuitive
the Body Surveillance subscale of the Objectified Body Con- eating and one or more psychosocial correlates that could not be
sciousness Scale (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). Higher levels of intui- categorised into eating attitudes and behaviours, body image or
tive eating correlated with greater resistance in adopting an emotional functioning. Five studies (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka,
observer's perspective of the body, according to total (Augustus- 2011; Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010; Oh
Horvath & Tylka, 2011; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013) and sub- et al., 2012; Tylka & Homan, 2015) assessed the perceived accep-
scale intuitive eating scores (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013). In tance of weight and shape by others using the 10-item Body
addition, intuitive eating correlated with a greater focus on body Acceptance by Others Scale (Avalos & Tylka, 2006). Intuitive eating
function and feeling rather than appearance (Avalos & Tylka, was associated with greater body acceptance by others among
2006; Oh et al., 2012; Tylka & Homan, 2015). Body image flexi- women aged 18e65 years (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011), fe-
bility, described as the degree to which one accepts thoughts and male university athletes (Oh et al., 2012) and physically active
feelings about the body, was examined by Schoenefeld and Webb university students (Tylka & Homan, 2015), and undergraduate
(2013). Intuitive eating correlated with body image flexibility in a psychology students (Avalos & Tylka, 2006; Kroon Van Diest &
positive direction. Overall, correlations between total scores of Tylka, 2010).
intuitive eating and positive aspects of body image ranged from Three studies examined women's (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012;
.43 to .69. Correlations for negative aspects of body image ranged Kroon Van Diest & Tylka, 2010) or their parent's (Galloway et al.,
between .36 and .56. Correlations between subscales of the 2010) retrospective reports of eating messages delivered by care-
IES-T and negative aspects of body image ranged from .26 to .52 givers. Findings were dependent on the type of eating message.
for the Unconditional Permission to Eat subscale, .25 to .35 for Two studies showed intuitive eating was negatively associated with
the Eating for Physical Reasons subscale, and .24 to .39 for the restrictive or critical messages (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012; Kroon
Reliance on Hunger/Satiety Cues subscale. Correlations between Van Diest & Tylka, 2010). Galloway et al. (2010) reported an in-
IES-T subscales and positive aspects of body image ranged from .29 verse correlation between the Eating for Physical Reasons subscale
to .40, with the Reliance on Hunger/Satiety Cues reporting the of Tylka's IES-T (2006) and messages relating to the restriction and
largest correlation. monitoring of food intake, however, no association was found for
remaining IES-T subscales with caregiver messages of restriction or
3.5. Emotional functioning monitoring of food intake. No association was found for intuitive
eating and pressure to eat messages from caregivers in both studies
Seven studies examined aspects of emotional functioning which examined this type of message (Galloway et al., 2010;
468 L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012). for functional reasons (Tylka & Homan, 2015), and did not correlate
Other factors shown to correlate with higher levels of intuitive with unconditional acceptance by others in university athletes (Oh
eating included self-esteem (Schoenefeld & Webb, 2013; Tylka, et al., 2012) or in one of the two student samples examined by
2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), Avalos and Tylka (2006).
self-compassion (Schoenefeld & Webb, 2013), unconditional self-
regard (Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), responsiveness to internal bodily 4. Discussion
sensations (Dittmann et al., 2009), greater life satisfaction (Tylka,
2006; Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013), opti- The aim of this paper was to systematically review the literature
mism (Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), an autonomy- on intuitive eating and its relationship with other eating attitudes
supportive interpersonal style of a woman's mother with regards and behaviours, body image and emotional functioning, in adult
to eating behaviours (Carbonneau et al., 2015), and perceived social women. In addition, this review systematically examined other
support (Augustus-Horvath & Tylka, 2011). Studies have also shown psychosocial correlates that have been examined in relation to
that intuitive eating correlates positively with proactive coping intuitive eating. This review has furthered our understanding of
(Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006), psychological hardiness, and intuitive eating and highlighted areas that require additional
social problem solving (Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). The three studies research.
that assessed individual subscales of the original and revised IES-T Intuitive eating is viewed as promoting a healthy relationship
demonstrated relationships consistent with the total scale, except with food by discouraging restrictive eating, emotional eating, and
the Unconditional Permission to Eat subscale did not correlate with eating in response to external cues (Tribole & Resch, 2012). More
the life satisfaction (Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013), proactive specifically, intuitive eating is viewed as promoting eating in
coping (Tylka, 2006), or social problem solving (Tylka & Wilcox, response to bodily cues for hunger and satiety and permits women
2006). to eat unconditionally, thus removing rules for what, when and
In addition, studies demonstrated that total and subscale scores how much to eat. On the whole, the findings of this review showed
of intuitive eating correlated negatively with internalisation of the that intuitive eating, when assessed with total scores, was inversely
thin ideal (Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013), pressure for related to disordered eating and dieting, with moderate to high
thinness from others (Tylka, 2006), and poor interoceptive aware- correlations. However, it is also important to consider the intuitive
ness (Tylka, 2006; Tylka et al., 2015; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013). eating subscales. This review showed that in all three studies that
Eating intuitively also correlated negatively with a controlling examined the IES-T subscales, the Unconditional Permission to Eat
interpersonal style of a woman's mother, in terms of eating subscale demonstrated the highest correlation with disordered
behaviour (Carbonneau et al., 2015), and attachment style and eating (Tylka, 2006; Tylka & Kroon Van Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox,
perfectionistic traits, including: an anxious or avoidant attachment 2006), thus suggesting that this feature of intuitive eating is less
style (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012), an anxious or insecure attach- conceptually distinct from disordered eating (Tylka & Kroon Van
ment to God (Homan & Cavanaugh, 2013), self-oriented perfec- Diest, 2013; Tylka & Wilcox, 2006). The Eating for Physical Rea-
tionism (applying unrealistic standards and scrutiny to one's self) sons subscale and the Reliance on Hunger/Satiety Cues subscale
(Brown et al., 2012) and discrepancy perfectionism (perceiving a demonstrated small to medium correlations with disordered
discrepancy between self-imposed standards and performance) eating, while the Body-Food Choice Congruence subscale was un-
(Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012). related with disordered eating, thus suggesting that these three
Two studies examined intuitive eating in relation to motivation aspects of intuitive eating are more conceptually distinct from
for physical activity. Gast et al. (2015) examined four types of disordered eating. Studies which examine the factor structure of
motivation using the IES-H (Hawks, Merrill, & Madanat, 2004). the IES-T in relation to measures of disordered eating are now
Total intuitive eating scores correlated negatively with motivation needed to more fully address this question.
based on external pressures and feelings of guilt or shame, and Research is also emerging that supports intuitive eating as an
correlated in a positive direction with motivation based on feelings effective intervention for reducing dieting attitudes and behav-
of pleasure. Findings varied when subscales of the IES-H were iours. For example, a 15-week classroom-based program that
examined. Only one of four IES-H subscales correlated negatively encouraged hunger-based eating led to a decrease in dieting in
with motivation for physical activity based on external pressures college females (Hawks, Madanat, Smith & De La Cruz, 2008); and a
(Antidieting subscale); three of four subscales correlated negatively 10 week group-based program that promoted the ten principles of
with motivation based on feelings of guilt or shame (Extrinsic intuitive eating (as proposed by Tribole & Resch, 2012) reduced
eating, Antidieting and Self-care subscales); and only two of four dieting attitudes in female military spouses (Cole & Horacek, 2010).
subscales (Antidieting, Self-care subscales) positively correlated A review by Schaefer and Magnuson (2014) has shown in-
with motivation based on feelings of pleasure. Tylka and Homan terventions that have implemented intuitive eating principles
(2015) used the IES-T to examine intuitive eating in relation to reduced disordered eating practices such as dieting, disinhibited
exercise motivation in physically active university students. Total eating and binge eating.
intuitive eating scores were inversely associated with motivation to There is limited research on intuitive eating and its association
exercise based on appearance-related reasons. with other eating patterns in adult women, such as emotional
Factors that did not correlate with intuitive eating included: eating and external eating. Emotional eating describes eating in
high standards perfectionism (setting goals that are realistic, response to negative emotions and external eating describes eating
motivating and encouraging), order perfectionism (a focus on in response to food-related cues (van Strien, Frijters, Bergers, &
organisation and order) (Iannantuono & Tylka, 2012), spiritual Defares, 1986). None of the 24 studies examined in this system-
readiness (Dittmann et al., 2009), an avoidant attachment to God atic review investigated these eating patterns. One study that was
and trust in parental relationships (Homan & Cavanaugh, 2013), ineligible for inclusion in this review, due to the inclusion of males
and the interpersonal style of a woman's partner with regards to in the sample and not analysing findings by gender, examined
eating behaviour (autonomous-supportive or a controlling inter- intuitive eating in relation to emotional eating and uncontrolled
personal style) (Carbonneau et al., 2015). In addition, total scores of eating (i.e., loss of control over food intake that results in over-
intuitive eating did not correlate with physical activity motivation eating) in a sample of French adults (Camilleri et al., 2015). Total
based on the value placed on physical activity (Gast et al., 2015) or intuitive eating scores were moderately to strongly and inversely
L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472 469

correlated with both the emotional eating and uncontrolled eating cues for hunger/satiety (Tribole & Resch, 2012). In addition, intui-
subscales of the revised Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (Tholin, tive eating encourages an attitude towards physical activity that is
Rasmussen, Tynelius, & Karlsson, 2005). However, findings varied not focused on calorie-burning for weight control. Instead, the
when examining the intuitive eating subscales, thus again high- approach is focused on the body's response to exercise and the
lighting the importance of using the subscales and not relying enjoyment of being physically active (Tribole & Resch, 2012). In line
exclusively on the total scores. The Unconditional Permission to Eat with this principle, studies showed that intuitive eating correlated
subscale did not correlate with uncontrolled eating and showed a with greater motivation to engage in physical activity when focused
weak correlation with emotional eating; the Reliance on Hunger/ on feelings of pleasure (Gast et al., 2015), and less motivation when
Satiety Cues subscale was weakly correlated with both emotional focused on feelings of pressure or guilt (Gast et al., 2015) or
and uncontrolled eating; but the Eating for Physical Reasons sub- appearance (Tylka & Homan, 2015).
scale correlated strongly with both emotional and uncontrolled
eating (Camilleri et al., 2015). Further studies, using the subscales of 4.1. Limitations and recommendations for future research
the IES-T, are needed to investigate these relationships, given
intuitive eating is hypothesised to discourage eating based on non- A limitation of the reviewed studies is that all studies used a
physiological cues and eating to regulate emotions. cross-sectional design. Thus, no conclusion about the causeeeffect
This review further showed that intuitive eating correlated with relationship between intuitive eating and psychosocial correlates is
aspects of positive body image such as body appreciation and body possible. Prospective studies with a range of follow up intervals are
satisfaction; and other psychosocial factors associated with body needed to verify these cross-sectional findings. This would allow an
image in women, such as self-esteem, perceived acceptance of examination of intuitive eating and these correlates over time to
weight and shape by others, pressure for thinness, and internal- assess if intuitive eating is an approach that may improve women's
isation of the thin-ideal (e.g., Green & Pritchard, 2003; Lewis & psychological health and well-being. Studies that have examined
Cachelin, 2001; Lowery et al., 2005; Tylka & Hill, 2004; Webster other eating behaviours, such as disordered eating, have included
& Tiggemann, 2003). This contrasts with research that shows that follow up intervals as short as five and a half months (Lowe, Doshi,
disordered eating practices, which are often employed for the Katterman, & Feig, 2013) to nine months (Stice, 2002), and longer
purpose of modifying weight and shape to improve appearance term intervals from two to 10 years (e.g., Goldschmidt et al., 2012;
(O'Brien et al., 2007; Putterman & Linden, 2004), are associated Skinner, Haines, Austin, & Field, 2012; Sonneville et al., 2013).
with greater weight and shape concerns in women (Quick & Byrd- Studies were also limited with regards to sample characteristics.
Bredbenner, 2012) and greater body dissatisfaction (Putterman & This review showed that intuitive eating has been examined pri-
Linden, 2004). Intuitive eating is viewed as promoting respect marily among female university students, who reported their
and care of the body, realistic expectations of body size, and dis- ethnicity as Caucasian and lived in the United States. The sample
courages women from making body comparisons and engaging in size of some studies was also small. For example, Brown et al.'s
negative self-talk about body size and shape (Tribole & Resch, (2012) study included 48 female university students and
2012). Galloway et al.’s (2010) study included 71 female university stu-
Non-dieting approaches (Clifford et al., 2015) and interventions dents. It is recommended that future studies examine more diverse
that promote principles of intuitive eating (i.e., eating based on samples of adult women with a focus on different age and ethnic
internal cues of hunger and satiety) (Schaefer & Magnuson, 2014) groups, level of education, and socioeconomic status. It also needs
have demonstrated improvements in body image, including: to be noted that 12 of 24 studies of studies included Tylka as an
reduced weight concerns and increased perceived sexual attrac- author. However, a comparison of findings between Tylka's studies
tiveness (Hawks, Madanat, Smith & De La Cruz, 2008), and im- and the remaining 12 reviewed studies showed similar findings.
provements in body image avoidance and body dissatisfaction Only one of 24 studies (Gast et al., 2015) used the intuitive eating
(Bacon et al., 2002, Bacon, Stern, Van Loan, & Keim, 2005; Jackson, measure developed by Hawks, Merrill, and Madanat (2004). There
2008). In addition, these interventions have also reduced the drive have been no studies directly comparing the two different mea-
for thinness and internalisation of the thin-ideal (Schaefer & sures of intuitive eating (the IES-T and IES-H). However, the IES-H
Magnuson, 2014). has demonstrated lower internal consistency than Tylka's IES-T
This review also showed that intuitive eating correlated with (2006). Also, some subscales of the IES-H (Hawks, Merrill, &
positive emotional functioning in women. This is consistent with Madanat, 2004) have been found to demonstrate inadequate or
not using food to cope with emotions, another principle of intuitive low Cronbach's coefficient alphas (e.g., .22 to .43 for Intrinsic Eating
eating (Tribole & Resch, 2012). This contrasts with research and .49 to .68 for Self-care) (e.g., Gast et al., 2015; Hawks, Merrill, &
demonstrating that restrictive and disordered eating practices are Madanat, 2004; Hawks, Merrill, Madanat, & Miyagawa et al., 2004),
associated with depressive symptomatology and poor emotional and poorer test-retest reliability in comparison to subscales of
regulation (Ackard, Croll, & Kearney-Cooke, 2002; Gillen, Markey, & Tylka's IES-T (e.g., Hawks, Merrill, & Madanat, 2004). Several of the
Markey, 2012). In intuitive eating, emphasis is placed on being able psychosocial correlates have also not been examined as extensively
to distinguish between biological and emotional hunger and as disordered eating, body image and emotional functioning.
regulate emotions with alternative strategies. In our view, it is also Moreover, the assessment of intuitive eating and psychosocial
possible that the relationship between intuitive eating and correlates across all reviewed studies was conducted using self-
emotional functioning may be bidirectional, that is, women may eat reports. Experimental and prospective cohort studies are now
more intuitively in the absence of negative emotions but they may needed to verify findings.
also experience more positive emotions as a consequence of intu- Traditional methods of weight control encourage restrictive
itive eating. Studies are now needed to test this hypothesis. eating practices. However, research suggests these methods are
Other psychosocial correlates of intuitive eating that were found largely ineffective for weight loss and maintenance in the longer
in this review included the awareness and responsiveness of term, they increase the risk of disordered eating, and have been
women towards bodily states (e.g., sensations, emotions, hunger associated with poor psychological health and well-being
and satiety) and motivation to engage in physical activity. Greater (Appleton & McGowan, 2006; Goldschmidt et al., 2012; Hawks,
awareness and responsiveness to bodily states is related to the Madanat, & Christley, 2008; Mann et al., 2007). Intuitive eating
feature of intuitive eating that focuses on eating based on internal has been proposed as a non-dieting approach that aims to address
470 L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

unhealthy attitudes towards food, the body and physical activity. interpersonal styles for a better understanding of their eating regulation and
intuitive eating. Appetite, 92, 156e166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
This systematic review showed that intuitive eating is associated
j.appet.2015.05.020.
with lower levels of disordered eating, a more positive body image, Carper, J. L., Orlet Fisher, J., & Birch, L. L. (2000). Young girls' emerging dietary re-
and positive emotional functioning. However, further studies are straint and disinhibition are related to parental control in child feeding.
needed to build on the cross-sectional findings of current research. Appetite, 35, 121e129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.2000.0343.
Claiborn, C. D., Crawford, J. B., & Hackman, H. W. (1983). Effects of intervention
Future research in this area will help establish if intuitive eating is discrepancy in counseling for negative emotions. Journal of Counseling Psy-
an approach that could prevent and/or reduce disordered eating chology, 30, 164e171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.30.2.164.
practices, body image concerns, and negative emotional Clifford, D., Ozier, A., Bundros, J., Moore, J., Kreiser, A., & Morris, M. N. (2015). Impact
of non-diet approaches on attitudes, behaviors, and health outcomes: a sys-
functioning. tematic review. Journal of Nutrition Education & Behavior, 47, 143e155.e1. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2014.12.002.
Cole, R. E., & Horacek, T. (2010). Effectiveness of the “My Body Knows When”
References intuitive-eating pilot program. American Journal of Health Behavior, 34,
286e297. http://dx.doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.34.3.4.
Ackard, D. M., Croll, J. K., & Kearney-Cooke, A. (2002). Dieting frequency among Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and
college females: association with disordered eating, body image, and related adaptation to stress. In W. H. Jones, & D. Periman (Eds.), Advances in personal
psychological problems. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 129e136. http:// relationships (Vol. 1, pp. 37e67). Greenwich CT: JAI Press.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00269-0. Daubenmier, J. J. (2005). The relationship of yoga, body awareness, and body
Appleton, K. M., & McGowan, L. (2006). The relationship between restrained eating responsiveness to self-objectification and disordered eating. Psychology of
and poor psychological health is moderated by pleasure normally associated Women Quarterly, 29, 207e219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-
with eating. Eating Behaviour, 7, 342e347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 6402.2005.00183.x.
j.eatbeh.2005.11.008. Denny, K. N., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Loth, K. A., & Eisenberg, M. E. (2013). Intuitive
Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer eating in young adulthood: who is doing it and how is it associated with
attachment: individual differences and their relationship to psychological well- disordered eating behaviors? Appetite, 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
being in adolescence. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 16, 427e454. http:// j.appet.2012.09.029, 31e19.
dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02202939. DiBartolo, P. M., Lin, L., Montoya, S., Neal, H., & Shaffer, C. (2007). Are there “healthy”
Augustus-Horvath, C. L., & Tylka, T. L. (2011). The acceptance model of intuitive and “unhealthy” reasons for exercise? Examining individual differences in ex-
eating: a comparison of women in emerging adulthood, early adulthood, and ercise motivations using the Function of Exercise Scale. Journal of Clinical Sport
middle adulthood. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 110e125. http:// Psychology, 1(2), 93e120.
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022129. Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life
Avalos, L. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). Exploring a model of intuitive eating with college Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71e75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/
women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 486e497. http://dx.doi.org/ s15327752jpa4901_13.
10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.486. Dittmann, K., Freedman, M., Beddoe, A., & Waldrop, J. (2009). Body awareness,
Avalos, L., Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. (2005). The Body Appreciation Scale: eating attitudes, and spiritual beliefs of women practicing yoga. Eating Disor-
development and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 2, 285e297. http:// ders: The Journal of Treatment & Prevention, 17, 273e292. http://dx.doi.org/
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.06.002. 10.1080/10640260902991111.
Bacon, L., Keim, N. L., Van Loan, M. D., Derricote, M., Gale, B., Kazaks, A., et al. (2002). D'Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (1997). Manual for the Social
Evaluating a 'non-diet' wellness intervention for improvement of metabolic Problem-Solving Inventory-Revised. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health
fitness, psychological well-being and eating and activity behaviors. Interna- Systems.
tional Journal of Obesity Related Metabolic Disorders, 26, 854e865. http:// Elgin, J., & Pritchard, M. (2006). Gender differences in disordered eating and its
dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802012. correlates. Eating & Weight Disorders, 11, e96e101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
Bacon, L., Stern, J. S., Van Loan, M. D., & Keim, N. L. (2005). Size acceptance and BF03327565.
intuitive eating improve health for obese, female chronic dieters. Journal of the Enriquez, E., Duncan, G. E., & Schur, E. A. (2013). Age at dieting onset, body mass
American Dietetic Association, 105, 929e936. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ index, and dieting practices. A twin study. Appetite, 71, 301e306. http://
j.jada.2005.03.011. dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.09.001.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self Field, A. E., Austin, S. B., Taylor, C. B., Malspeis, S., Rosner, B., Rockett, H. R., et al.
report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, (2003). Relation between dieting and weight change among preadolescents and
27e45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504. adolescents. Pediatrics, 112, 900e906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.112.4.900.
Banks, A. W. (2008). Nutritional analyses of intuitive eaters as compared to dieters. All Fiszer, C., Dolbeault, S., Sultan, S., & Bre dart, A. (2014). Prevalence, intensity, and
Graduate Theses and Dissertations. Paper 100 http://digitalcommons.usu.edu/ predictors of the supportive care needs of women diagnosed with breast can-
etd/100. cer: a systematic review. Psycho-Oncology, 23, 361e374. http://dx.doi.org/
Barrett-Lennard, G. T. (1962). Dimensions of therapists' response as causal factors in 10.1002/pon.3432.
therapeutic change. Psychological Monographs, 76, 1e36. http://dx.doi.org/ Forrester-Knauss, C., & Zemp Stutz, E. (2012). Gender differences in disordered
10.1037/h0093918. eating and weight dissatisfaction in Swiss adults: which factors matter? BMC
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Thogersen-Ntoumani, C. (2010). The control- Public Health, 12, 809. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-809.
ling interpersonal style in coaching context. Development and initial validation Framson, C., Kristal, A. R., Schenk, J. M., Littman, A. J., Zeliadt, S., & Benitez, D. (2009).
of a psychometric scale. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32(2), 193e216. Development and validation of the mindful eating questionnaire. Journal of the
Beck, R., & McDonald, A. (2004). Attachment to God: the attachment to God in- American Dietetic Association, 109, 1439e1444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
ventory, tests of working model correspondence, and an exploration of faith j.jada.2009.05.006.
group differences. Journal of Psychology & Theology, 32(2), 92e103. French, S., Jeffery, R., & Wing, R. (1994). Sex differences among participants in a
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression in- weight-control program. Addictive Behaviors, 19, 147e158. http://dx.doi.org/
ventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90039-6.
Betz, N. E., & Campbell, C. (2003). Self-efficacy and personality correlates of instru- Galloway, A. T., Farrow, C. V., & Martz, D. M. (2010). Retrospective reports of child
mentality (Unpublished manuscript). feeding practices, current eating behaviors, and BMI in college students. Obesity,
Betz, N. E., Wohlgemuth, E., Serling, D., Harshbarger, J., & Klein, K. (1995). Evaluation 18, 1330e1335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.393.
of a measure of self-esteem based on the concept of unconditional self-regard. Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating disorder inventorye2 manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Journal of Counseling Development, 74, 76e83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1556- Assessment Resources.
6676.1995.tb01826.x. Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The eating attitudes
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult test: psychometric features and clinical correlates. Psychological Medicine, 12,
attachment: an integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson, & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), 871e878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700049163.
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46e76). New York: Guilford Press. Gast, J., Campbell Nielson, A., Hunt, A., & Leiker, J. J. (2015). Intuitive eating: asso-
Brown, A. J., Parman, K. M., Rudat, D. A., & Craighead, L. W. (2012). Disordered ciations with physical activity motivation and BMI. American Journal of Health
eating, perfectionism, and food rules. Eating Behaviors, 13, 347e353. http:// Promotion, 29, e91ee99. http://dx.doi.org/10.4278/ajhp.130305-QUAN-97.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.05.011. Gillen, M. M., Markey, C. N., & Markey, P. M. (2012). An examination of dieting
Bukowska-Durawa, A., & Luszczynska, A. (2014). Cervical cancer screening and behaviors among adults: links with depression. Eating Behaviors, 13, 88e93.
psychosocial barriers perceived by patients. A systematic review. Contemp Oncol http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.11.014.
(Pozn), 18, 153e159. http://dx.doi.org/10.5114/wo.2014.43158. Goldschmidt, A. B., Wall, M., Loth, K. A., Le Grange, D., & Neumark-Sztainer, D.
Camilleri, G. M., Me jean, C., Bellisle, F., Andreeva, V. A., Sautron, V., Hercberg, S., (2012). Which dieters are at risk for the onset of binge eating? A prospective
et al. (2015). Cross-cultural validity of the Intuitive Eating Scale-2. Psychometric study of adolescents and young adults. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, 86e92.
evaluation in a sample of the general French population. Appetite, 84, 6e14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.11.001.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.009. Gormally, J., Black, S., Daston, S., & Rardin, D. (1982). The assessment of binge eating
Carbonneau, N., Carbonneau, E., Cantin, M., & Gagnon-Girouard, M. P. (2015). severity among obese persons. Addictive Behaviors, 7, 47e55. http://dx.doi.org/
Examining women's perceptions of their mother's and romantic partner's 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90024-7.
L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472 471

Greenglass, E. R., Schwarzer, R., & Taubert, S. (1999). The Proactive Coping Inventory development and validation. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 181e215.
(PCI): A multidimensional research instrument. Retrieved from http://www. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00467.x.
psych.yorku.ca/greenglass/pcinven.php. Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., &, PRISMA Group. (2010). Preferred
Green, S. P., & Pritchard, M. E. (2003). Predictors of body image dissatisfaction in reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA state-
adult men and women. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, ment. International Journal of Surgery, 8, 336e341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
31, 215e222. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2003.31.3.215. j.ijsu.2010.02.007.
Hawks, S. R., Madanat, H. N., & Christley, H. S. (2008). Psychosocial associations of Moreau, E., & Mageau, G. A. (2012). The importance of perceived autonomy support
dietary restraint: implications for healthy weight promotion. Ecology of Food & for the psychological health and work satisfaction of health professionals. Not
Nutrition, 47, 450e483. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03670240701821527. only supervisors count, colleagues too! Motivation and Emotion, 36, 268e286.
Hawks, S. R., Madanat, H., Smith, T., & De La Cruz, N. (2008). Classroom approach for http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9250-9.
managing dietary restraint, negative eating styles, and body image concerns Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D. K. (1997). A graded conceptualization of
among college women. Journal of American College Health, 56, 359e366. http:// self-determination in the regulation of exercise behavior: development of a
dx.doi.org/10.3200/JACH.56.44.359-368. measure using confirmatory factor analytic procedures. Personality & Individual
Hawks, S. R., Merrill, R. M., & Madanat, H. N. (2004). The Intuitive Eating Scale (IES): Differences, 23, 745e752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00107-4.
development and preliminary validation. American Journal of Health Education, Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-
35, 90e99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19325037.2004.10603615. compassion. Self & Identity, 2, 223e250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Hawks, S. R., Merrill, R. M., Madanat, H. N., Miyagawa, T., Suwanteerangkul, J., 15298860309027.
Guarin, C. M., et al. (2004). Intuitive eating and the nutrition transition in Asia. Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Guo, J., Story, M., Haines, J., & Eisenberg, M. (2006).
Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 13(2), 194e203. Obesity, disordered eating, and eating disorders in a longitudinal study of ad-
Hayes, S., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. (1999). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An olescents: how do dieters fare 5 years later? Journal of American Dietetic Asso-
experiential approach to behavior change. New York, NY: Guilford Press. ciation, 106, 559e568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2006.01.003.
Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of Neumark-Sztainer, D., Wall, M., Story, M., & Standish, A. R. (2012). Dieting and
the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire. International unhealthy weight control behaviors during adolescence: associations with 10-
Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81e89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098- year changes in body mass index. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 80e86.
108X(199501)17:1<81::AID-EAT2260170111>3.0.CO;2-Y. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.05.010.
Herbert, B. M., Blechert, J., Hautzinger, M., Matthias, E., & Herbert, C. (2013). Intu- Nielson, A. C. (2009). Intuitive eating and its relationship with physical activity
itive eating is associated with interoceptive sensitivity. Effects on body mass motivation. All Graduate Theses and Dissertations. Paper 409 http://
index. Appetite, 70, 22e30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.082. digitalcommons.usu.edu/etd/409/.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts: Oh, K. H., Wiseman, M. C., Hendrickson, J., Phillips, J. C., & Hayden, E. W. (2012).
conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal Testing the acceptance model of intuitive eating with college women athletes.
of Personality & Social Psychology, 60, 456e470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- Psychology of Women Quarterly, 36, 88e98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
3514.60.3.456. 0361684311433282.
Holmes, M., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Skouteris, H., & Broadbent, J. (2014). Improving O'Brien, K., Venn, B. J., Perry, T., Green, T. J., Aitken, W., Bradshaw, A., et al. (2007).
prediction of binge episodes by modelling chronicity of dietary restriction. Reasons for wanting to lose weight: different strokes for different folks. Eating
European Eating Disorders Review, 22, 405e411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ Behaviors, 8, 132e135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.01.004.
erv.2315. Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-D’Angelo, M., & Reid, R. (2004). Why do you
Homan, K. J., & Cavanaugh, B. N. (2013). Perceived relationship with God fosters regulate what you eat? Relationships between forms of regulation, eating be-
positive body image in college women. Journal of Health Psychology, 18, haviors, sustained dietary behavior change, and psychological adjustment.
1529e1539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105312465911. Motivation & Emotion, 28, 245e277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B:
Iannantuono, A. C., & Tylka, T. L. (2012). Interpersonal and intrapersonal links to MOEM.0000040154.40922.14.
body appreciation in college women: an exploratory model. Body Image, 9, €inen, K. H., Saarni, S. E., Kaprio, J., & Rissanen, A. (2012). Does dieting make
Pietila
227e235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012.01.004. you fat? A twin study. International Journal of Obesity (Lond), 36, 56e464. http://
Jack, D. C., & Dill, D. (1992). The Silencing the Self Scale: Schemas of intimacy dx.doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2011.160.
associated with depression in women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 16, Putterman, E., & Linden, W. (2004). Appearance versus health: does the reason for
97e106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1992.tb00242.x. dieting affect dieting behavior? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 27, 185e204.
Jackson, E. G. (2008). Eating order: a 13-week trust model class for dieting casu- http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/B: JOBM.0000019851.37389.a7.
alties. Journal of Nutrition Education & Behavior, 40, 43e48. http://dx.doi.org/ Quick, V. M., & Byrd-Bredbenner, C. (2012). Weight regulation practices of young
10.1016/j.jneb.2007.01.007. adults. Predictors of restrictive eating. Appetite, 59, 425e430. http://dx.doi.org/
Kenardy, J., Brown, W. J., & Vogt, E. (2001). Dieting and health in young Australian 10.1016/j.appet.2012.06.004.
women. European Eating Disorders Review, 9, 242e254. http://dx.doi.org/ Rideout, C. A., & Barr, S. I. (2009). “Restrained eating” vs “trying to lose weight”:
10.1002/erv.388. how are they associated with body weight and tendency to overeat among
Kmet, L. M., Lee, R. C., & Cook, L. S. (2004). Standard quality assessment criteria for postmenopausal women? Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109,
evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. Alberta, Canada: 890e893. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2009.02.009.
Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Koestner, R., Powers, T. A., Carbonneau, N., Milyavskaya, M., & Chua, S. N. (2012). University Press.
Distinguishing autonomous and directive forms of goal support. Their effects on Rosenthal, J. A. (1996). Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect
goal progress, relationship quality, and subjective well-being. Personality & size. Journal of Social Service Research, 21, 37e59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/
Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1609e1620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ J079v21n04_02.
0146167212457075. Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional
Kroon Van Diest, A. M., & Tylka, T. L. (2010). The Caregiver Eating Messages Scale: attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait
development and psychometric investigation. Body Image, 7, 317e326. http:// Meta-Mood Scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health (pp.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.06.002. 125e154). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lewis, D. M., & Cachelin, F. M. (2001). Body image, body dissatisfaction, and eating Satherley, R., Howard, R., & Higgs, S. (2015). Disordered eating practices in gastro-
attitudes in midlife and elderly women. Eating disorders, 9, 29e39. http:// intestinal disorders. Appetite, 84, 240e250. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
dx.doi.org/10.1080/106402601300187713. j.appet.2014.10.006.
Lowe, M. R., Doshi, S. D., Katterman, S. N., & Feig, E. H. (2013). Dieting and restrained Schaefer, J. T., & Magnuson, A. B. (2014). A review of interventions that promote
eating as prospective predictors of weight gain. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 577. eating by internal cues. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition & Dietetics, 114,
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00577. 734e760. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.12.024.
Lowery, S. E., Kurpius, S. E. R., Befort, C., Blanks, E. H., Sollenberger, S., Nicpon, M. F., Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from
et al. (2005). Body image, self-esteem, and health-related behaviors among neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a re-evaluation
male and female first year college students. Journal of College Student Devel- of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67,
opment, 46, 612e623. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/csd.2005.0062. 1063e1078. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063.
Madden, C. E. L., Leong, S. L., Gray, A., & Horwath, C. C. (2012). Eating in response to Schoenefeld, S. J., & Webb, J. B. (2013). Self-compassion and intuitive eating in
hunger and satiety signals is related to BMI in a nationwide sample of 1601 college women: examining the contributions of distress tolerance and body
mid-age New Zealand women. Public Health Nutrition, 15, 2272e2279. http:// image acceptance and action. Eating Behaviors, 14, 493e496. http://dx.doi.org/
dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980012000882. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.09.001.
Mageau, G. A., Ranger, F., Joussemet, M., Koestner, R., Moreau, E., & Forest, J. (2015). Shouse, S. H., & Nilsson, J. (2011). Self-silencing, emotional awareness, and eating
Validation of the Perceived Parental Autonomy Support Scale (P-PASS). Cana- behaviors in college women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35, 451e457.
dian Journal of Behavioural Science, 47, 251e262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684310388785.
a0039325. Simons, J., & Gaher, R. (2005). The Distress Tolerance Scale: development and
Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, A. M., Samuels, B., & Chatman, J. (2007). validation of a self-report measure. Motivation & Emotion, 29, 83e102. http://
Medicare's search for effective obesity treatments: diets are not the answer. dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-005-7955-3.
American Psychologist, 62, 220e233. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003- Skinner, H. H., Haines, J., Austin, S. B., & Field, A. E. (2012). A prospective study of
066X.62.3.220. overeating, binge eating, and depressive symptoms among adolescent and
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The Objectified Body Consciousness Scale: young adult women. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 478e483. http://
472 L.J. Bruce, L.A. Ricciardelli / Appetite 96 (2016) 454e472

dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.10.002. Tylka, T. L., Calogero, R. M., & Danielsdottir, S. (2015). Is intuitive eating the same as
Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised flexible dietary control? Their links to each other and well-being could provide
Almost Perfect Scale. Measurement & Evaluation in Counseling and Development, an answer. Appetite, 95, 166e175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.004.
34(3), 130e145. Tylka, T. L., & Hill, M. S. (2004). Objectification theory as it relates to disordered
Sonneville, K. R., Horton, N. J., Micali, N., Crosby, R. D., Swanson, S. A., Solmi, F., et al. eating among college women. Sex Roles, 51, 719e730. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
(2013). Longitudinal associations between binge eating and overeating and s11199-004-0721-2.
adverse outcomes among adolescents and young adults: does loss of control Tylka, T. L., & Homan, K. J. (2015). Exercise motives and positive body image in
matter? JAMA Pediatrics, 167, 149e155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/ physically active college women and men: exploring an expanded acceptance
2013.jamapediatrics.12. model of intuitive eating. Body Image, 15, 90e97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. C., Lushene, R. E., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). j.bodyim.2015.07.003.
Manual for the StateeTrait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psy- Tylka, T. L., & Kroon Van Diest, A. M. (2013). The Intuitive Eating Scale-2: item
chologist Press. refinement and psychometric evaluation with college women and men. Journal
Stice, E. (1996). The dual pathway model differentiates bulimics, subclinical bu- of Counseling Psychology, 60, 137e153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030893.
limics, and controls: testing the continuity hypothesis. Behavior Therapy, 27, Tylka, T. L., & Wilcox, J. A. (2006). Are intuitive eating and eating disorder symp-
531e549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(96)80042-6. tomatology opposite poles of the same construct? Journal of Counseling Psy-
Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: a meta-analytic chology, 53, 474e485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.4.474.
review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033- Tylka, T. L., & Wood-Barcalow, N. L. (2015). The body appreciation scale-2: item
2909.128.5.825. refinement and psychometric evaluation. Body Image, 12, 53e67. http://
van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E., Bergers, G., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch Eating dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.006.
Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and Ulen, C. G., Huizinga, M. M., Beech, B., & Elasy, T. A. (2008). Weight regain pre-
external eating behavior. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5, 295e315. vention. Clinical Diabetes, 26, 100e113. http://dx.doi.org/10.2337/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1098-108X(198602)5:2<295::AID- diaclin.26.3.100.
EAT2260050209>3.0.CO;2-T. Van Dyke, N., & Drinkwater, E. J. (2014). Relationships between intuitive eating and
van Strien, T., Herman, C. P., & Verheijden, M. W. (2014). Dietary restraint and body health indicators: literature review. Public Health Nutrition, 17, 1757e1766.
mass change. A 3-year follow up study in a representative Dutch sample. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980013002139.
Appetite, 76, 44e49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.01.015. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
Tapper, K., & Pothos, E. M. (2010). Development and validation of a food preoccu- measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality
pation questionnaire. Eating Behaviors, 11, 45e53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ & Social Psychology, 54, 1063e1070. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
j.eatbeh.2009.09.003. 3514.54.6.1063.
Tholin, S., Rasmussen, F., Tynelius, P., & Karlsson, J. (2005). Genetic and environ- Webster, J., & Tiggemann, M. (2003). The relationship between women's body
mental influences on eating behavior. The Swedish young male twins study. The satisfaction and self-image across the life span: the role of cognitive control. The
American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 81(3), 564e569. Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research & Theory on Human Development, 164,
Tribole, E., & Resch, E. (2012). Intuitive eating: A recovery book for the chronic dieter: 241e252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221320309597980.
Rediscover the pleasures of eating and rebuild your body image (3rd ed.). New Westenhoefer, J., Stunkard, A. J., & Pudel, V. (1999). Validation of the flexible and
York: St. Martin’s Press. rigid control dimensions of dietary restraint. International Journal of Eating
Tylka, T. L. (2006). Development and psychometric evaluation of a measure of Disorders, 26, 53e64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199907)26:
intuitive eating. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 226e240. http:// 1<53::AID-EAT7>3.0.CO;2-N.
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.226.

You might also like