You are on page 1of 8

Ontogeny of taste preferences: basic biology and implications for

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
health1–5
Julie A Mennella

ABSTRACT vegetable (5, 6). Thus, it is not surprising that the recently
Health initiatives address childhood obesity in part by encouraging launched Healthy People 2020 lists as one of its goals (Goal
good nutrition early in life. This review highlights the science that NSW-15) an increase in the variety and contribution of vege-
shows that children naturally prefer higher levels of sweet and salty tables to the diets of the population, including its youngest
tastes and reject lower levels of bitter tastes than do adults. Thus, members (4).
their basic biology does not predispose them to favor the recommen- How can we account for patterns of food choice that seem
ded low-sugar, low-sodium, vegetable-rich diets and makes them es- antithetical to health and for the difficulties in changing them?
pecially vulnerable to our current food environment of foods high in Two factors conspire to predispose some children to consume
salt and refined sugars. The good news is that sensory experiences, obesogenic diets: 1) inborn, evolutionarily driven taste prefer-
beginning early in life, can shape preferences. Mothers who consume ences and 2) detrimental consequences of not being exposed to
diets rich in healthy foods can get children off to a good start because flavors of healthful foods early in life (see references 12–14 for
flavors are transmitted from the maternal diet to amniotic fluid and previous reviews). Children have sensory systems that detect
mother’s milk, and breastfed infants are more accepting of these and prefer the once-rare calorie- and mineral-rich foods that
flavors. In contrast, infants fed formula learn to prefer its unique taste sweet or salty (15), while rejecting the potentially toxic
flavor profile and may have more difficulty initially accepting fla- ones that taste bitter (16, 17).
vors not found in formula, such as those of fruit and vegetables. This review highlights research that shows some of the bi-
Regardless of early feeding mode, infants can learn through re- ological drives of food acceptance during infancy and childhood
peated exposure and dietary variety if caregivers focus on the and how early-life experiences interact with inherent plasticity of
child’s willingness to consume a food and not just the facial ex-
the chemical senses to mold preferences for foods. We focus on
pressions made during feeding. In addition, providing complemen-
scientific evidence suggesting that early experience can influence
tary foods low in salt and sugars may help protect the developing
what foods are preferred, as well as the ontogeny of sweet, salty,
child from excess intake later in life. Early-life experiences with
and bitter, the tastes that 1) have been most extensively studied,
healthy tastes and flavors may go a long way toward promoting
2) are directly involved in choices of specific foods of concern
healthy eating, which could have a significant impact in addressing
(sweet and salty foods and bitter-tasting vegetables), and 3)
the many chronic illnesses associated with poor food choice.
Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99(suppl):704S–11S.
exhibit age-related changes in function.

IMPORTANCE OF EARLY-LIFE EXPERIENCES


INTRODUCTION Early-life exposures—both biological and social—explain
Many health initiatives, including “Let’s Move!” launched by trajectories of health in adulthood decades later (18). In partic-
the White House in 2010, are attempting to end the epidemic of ular, excessive intakes of foods high in salt (sodium chloride)
childhood obesity within a generation through new steps to en- 1
courage good nutrition and exercise (1). Recommendations in- From the Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA.
2
Presented at the workshop, “Evaluating the Diet-Related Scientific Lit-
clude that both adults and children limit consumption of salt, fat,
erature for Children from Birth to 24 Months: The B-24 Project,” held in
and simple sugars, which have sensory properties that we find Rockville, MD, February 5–7, 2013.
particularly palatable, and eat more fruit and vegetables, the latter 3
The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not nec-
of which sometimes taste bitter (2–4). essarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National
A major challenge for these health initiatives is that neither Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes
adults nor children are complying with these recommendations of Health.
4
(5, 6). The 2002 and 2008 Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study, Supported by NIH grants R01HD37119 and R01HD072307 from the
designed to update our knowledge on the feeding patterns of Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human De-
velopment and grant R01DC01128 from the National Institute of Deafness
American children, found that 1 in 4 toddlers did not consume
and Other Communication Disorders.
even one vegetable on a given day (5–7) and instead, like older 5
Address correspondence to JA Mennella, Monell Chemical Senses Cen-
children (8–11), were likely consuming French fries, sweet- and ter, 3500 Market Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104. E-mail: mennella@monell.
salty-tasting snacks, and sweet beverages (5, 6). By the second org.
year of life, not 1 of the top 5 vegetables eaten was a dark-green First published online January 22, 2014; doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.067694.

704S Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99(suppl):704S–11S. Printed in USA. Ó 2014 American Society for Nutrition
ONTOGENY OF TASTE PREFERENCES 705S
and refined sugars early in life cause or exacerbate a number of They also inform the gastrointestinal system about the quality and
illnesses and conditions in adulthood (19–22), such as obesity, quantity of the impending rush of nutrients or toxins (46).
metabolic syndrome, and mortality from cardiovascular disease. The tastes of foods that children initially either prefer (eg,
Such long-lasting impacts of early nutrition on health underlie sweet, salty foods) or reject (eg, bitter-tasting foods, including
the life-course approach taken in many medical fields, as well as some vegetables) in part reflect their basic biology. Age-related
the suggestion that both preventive interventions and further changes in taste perception and preference, documented by ex-
scientific inquiry should focus on early life (21, 23, 24). perimental research during the past century, indicate that the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
Early experience has far-reaching effects on behavior. rewarding properties of sweet and salt are more pronounced
Emerging research has shown the existence of postnatal periods during childhood. Many drugs of abuse coopt ancient brain re-
when the developing brain has heightened sensitivity to envi- ward systems that underlie hedonics of salt taste (“our primordial
ronmental influences (25)—epochs of brain plasticity when the narcotic”) (47) and sweet taste (“our oldest reward”) (48).
early environment can shape neural circuits and thus behaviors.
Beginning very early in life, sensory experiences shape and Sweet
modify flavor and food preferences (26–29). The preference for sweet tastes, which is present early in life, is
Although humans generally have inborn, positive responses to remarkably well conserved in primates (49). At birth (and per-
sugar, and perhaps salt, and negative responses to bitter, ge- haps even before), the ability to detect sweet tastes is functioning
netically determined individual differences also exist and interact and interacting with systems that control affect (49–51). Infants
with experience to ensure that children are not genetically re- born preterm and tested between 33 and 40 wk postconception
stricted to a narrow range of foodstuffs (see references 12 and 30 produced stronger and more frequent sucking responses when
for review). Thus, early experiences with nutritious foods and offered a sucrose-sweetened latex nipple compared with an
flavor variety may maximize the likelihood that, as children unsweetened latex nipple (50). Within hours after birth, new-
grow, they will choose a healthier diet because they like the borns differentiate varying degrees of sweetness and consume
tastes and variety of the foods it contains. more of a sugar solution than water (52). When a sweet solution
In support of this hypothesis, longitudinal studies have shown is placed in the oral cavity, infants relax the face and sometimes
that food habits established during infancy track into childhood smile (49, 51).
and adolescence (31–35) for both nutrient-dense and nutrient- Psychophysical studies have shown that the preference for
poor foods (34). Such dietary patterns, which begin to be sweet taste is universal and evident among children around the
identified during childhood (36), are significant determinants of world (53, 54). Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have
the quality of the adult diet (37, 38). The most apparent period shown that preferences for sweets remain heightened throughout
of change in dietary patterns was evident during early childhood childhood (55–58) and early adolescence (53, 55), declining to
(35), with new foods more likely to be accepted when children adult levels during midadolescence (53, 55). In one of the largest
are 2–3 y of age compared with older ages (31). After the age of studies on sweet preference (930 participants), which analyzed
3–4 y, reported dietary patterns/food habits remained quite sta- data for the NIH Toolbox Initiative, children selected a 0.54-mol
ble, further highlighting the importance of getting children on sucrose/L concentration as their most preferred (55), a concen-
the right track from the initial stages of learning to eat (,3 y) tration equivalent to 11 teaspoons (w44 g) of sugar in an 8-
(see also reference 33). ounce glass of water—nearly twice the sugar concentration of
Most studies that tracked dietary patterns/food habits have a typical cola (0.34 mol/L).
been correlational in nature and thus, some have argued, in- Sweet taste in the mouth can also blunt expressions of pain in
conclusive (39). Nevertheless, the evidence suggests positive children (59) and acts as an opioid receptor–dependent pain-
consequences of healthy dietary trajectories from infancy to reducing agent. Its ability to act as an analgesic has been in-
adulthood and how learning to like “healthy” foods is modifiable vestigated extensively in preterm and term infants (see reference
and begins early in life. During childhood, the strongest pre- 60 for review) and to a lesser extent in children (57, 61, 62).
dictors of what foods young children eat are 1) whether they like Sweet taste is effective in reducing spontaneous crying and pain
how the foods taste, 2) how long they were breastfed and in preterm and term infants during an array of painful pro-
whether their mothers ate these foods, and 3) whether they had cedures, such as a heal lance, venipuncture, or intramuscular
been eating these foods from an early age (31, 35, 40–42). An injections (60). Children can keep their hand in a cold-water
important area for future research includes randomized con- bath longer while tasting sweet (57, 61, 62), and the more they
trolled trials on early diet that focus on both caregivers and like sweets, the better its analgesic properties (57).
children (eg, references 43 and 44).
Salt
Children also prefer substantially higher concentrations of salt
BIOLOGY OF THE CHEMICAL SENSES than do adults, and adding salt to foods can drive consumption
Flavor, a powerful determinant of human consummatory be- (56, 63, 64). However, newborns generally do not react strongly
havior throughout the life span, is a product of several sensory to moderate concentrations of salt (51, 63); salty taste detection
systems, most notably those of taste and smell (odors perceived appears to develop later, perhaps at w2–6 mo (63). Compared
retronasally). The anatomically independent flavor senses are with sweet preferences among children (12), salt avidity is more
well developed at birth and continue to change throughout complex and less understood.
childhood and adolescence, serving as gatekeepers throughout Salt preference is affected by prenatal experiences (65–67) and
the life span (45), controlling one of the most important decisions apparently more plastic than is sweet preference. Infants born to
an animal makes: whether to accept or reject a foreign substance. women with moderate to severe morning sickness showed
706S MENNELLA

significantly higher intake of salt solutions at 4 mo of age than did Most foods and beverages manufactured for pediatric pop-
those whose mothers reported having no more than mild morning ulations are very high in both refined sugars or salt or both (80, 81).
sickness (66). Adolescents and young adults preferred higher Research has shown that adding sugar to beverages (56) and to solid
concentrations of salt in a soup base in the laboratory and reported foods such as spaghetti, ricotta, and jicama (82) increases both
significantly higher daily salt use if either they had suffered from liking and acceptance by young children. But 16% of children’s
infantile vomiting or diarrhea themselves or their mothers had total caloric intake now comes from added sugars (83). In addi-
suffered from morning sickness while pregnant with them (65). tion, our food supply now includes several low- or no-calorie

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
sweeteners that provide sweetness with fewer calories (84), yet we
know little of their impact on children’s acceptance, growth, and
Bitter eating patterns (85). We also do not know the long-term conse-
Detection and rejection of bitter are evident shortly after birth quences of children associating sweet taste with foods that are not
(49, 68, 69). Inborn responses have effects on initial acceptance inherently sweet (eg, yogurts, breakfast cereals, sandwich breads).
of foods such as dark-green vegetables. We know less about how Like experiences with sweet, preferences for salt taste are
responses to bitter tastes can be modified during early life (but see shaped by experiences with foods during the first year of life.
below). In animal models, lack of early exposure to bitterness Consumption of moderate and strong salt solutions (86) or salty
affects taste system development. In rodents, early taste depri- cereals (87) by infants ,6 mo of age can modify their dietary
vation remodels the central nervous system (70), whereas ex- patterns once they start eating table foods. In a prospective study
perience with bitterness during early life changes bitter taste in 61 infants, those exposed to starchy table foods (eg, grain
preferences in adulthood (71). products, biscuits), a significant source of sodium in infants’
Much recent research has focused on understanding how diets, were significantly more likely to prefer a salty solution
polymorphisms in the genes that encode taste receptors result in during infancy, and had a greater affinity for salty foods as
differential taste patterns in humans, beginning in childhood (age preschoolers, compared with children fed lower amounts of salt
w3 y onward). Of the 25 bitter taste receptors recently discovered (86). Thus, early experiences modulate hedonic responses to
(see reference 72 for review), TAS2R38 is the most studied. Ge- these basic stimuli.
netic variation in this receptor translates into individual differences Salt is a critical part of the diet and also a key food preservative
in taste sensitivity for phenylthiocarbamide, the related chemical (88); thus, it is not surprising that we have evolved a strong desire to
propylthiouracil, and goitrins found in cruciferous vegetables such consume salt (47). However, it is now widely recognized that the
as broccoli (73). Although some individuals can detect these bit- average American consumes excess sodium chloride. The recent
ter-tasting compounds at low concentrations, others need much Dietary Guidelines for Americans report (89) recommends
higher concentrations to detect them at all, due in part to poly- a maximum sodium intake of 2300 mg/d for healthy adults and
morphisms on the TAS2R38 gene (73). 1500 mg/d for many subpopulations that together make up .50%
Psychophysical studies have shown that propylthiouracil sen- of the total population; sodium intake is well above adequate
sitivity varies with age (58, 74, 75). Children heterozygous for these amounts of 1000 mg/d at 1–3 y and 1200 mg/d at 4–8 y (90).
variants (AVI/PAV) perceived a bitter taste at lower propylthiouracil In adults, the vast majority of salt consumption comes from
concentrations than did heterozygous adults, with intermediate manufactured foods or prepared food at restaurants (91). Thus,
thresholds for heterozygous adolescents. These results imply that consumers have relatively little control over or even knowledge
bitter sensitivity can change over the life span. How these varia- of how much salt they consume. A recent Institute of Medicine
tions contribute to individual differences are important areas for report (92) stressed the importance of understanding salt taste and
future research. salt taste preference in children and how early experiences
modulate these sensory responses. Salt in the food environment
of infants and children—and any changes in lowering the overall
EFFECTS OF EARLY TASTE EXPERIENCES: FIRST amounts—may “have the most profound effects” (92). And the
FOODS, AMNIOTIC FLUID, AND MOTHER’S MILK same may be true for sugar.
Beginning early in life, we learn the rules of cuisine—how,
when, and what to eat. Thus, it is not surprising that preferences
for salt, sweet, and bitter can be modified early through repeated The first foods: amniotic fluid and mother’s milk
exposure to flavors in amniotic fluid, mother’s milk, formula, Relatively little attention has been paid to how humans learn to
and solid foods. These experiences help infants develop pref- like the flavors of foods. During the past 2 decades, we have
erences in later life for foods containing these flavors. systematically investigated the transfer of flavors (dietary vola-
tiles) to amniotic fluid and human breast milk to determine their
effects on behaviors of breastfed infants (see reference 30 for
Evidence of developmental plasticity: sweet and salt review). A wide variety of flavors either ingested (eg, fruit, veg-
Through familiarization, children develop a sense of what etables, spices) or inhaled (eg, tobacco, perfumes) by the mother
should, or should not, taste sweet (56, 76, 77). Children routinely are transmitted to her amniotic fluid and/or milk, significantly
fed sugar water during infancy preferred sweeter liquids than did increasing in intensity in milk within hours after consumption.
children who were rarely exposed (78). Children repeatedly Infants’ experience with these volatiles and tastes modifies their
exposed to a sweetened orange-flavored beverage for 8 con- acceptance in mother’s milk, formula, and solid foods.
secutive days during their daily midmorning snack at school not For weaning, developmental processes must ensure that young
only gave higher preference rankings for the beverage but also mammals learn both what to eat and how to forage (30, 93). In
drank more of it at the end of the exposure period (79). a wide variety of species, they first learn about their mothers’
ONTOGENY OF TASTE PREFERENCES 707S
dietary choices through transmitted flavor cues (see reference 30 effects of early exposure appear to be particularly persistent and
for review), which cause neurologic and physiologic changes lead to heightened preferences for foods that contain volatiles
that influence later behaviors. In a recent study, nursing young also found in ePHF, such as broccoli, several years after the last
mice exposed to retronasally perceived odors (flavors) that ac- formula exposure (101).
tivated specific olfactory receptors on sensory neurons had sig- These findings have important implications for future research.
nificantly larger glomeruli and significant preferences for the Just as breastfed infants each experience a unique flavor palette
flavor compared with unexposed mice (94). Thus, early expe- through their mother’s breast milk, formula-fed infants are ex-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
riences with flavors in milk can result in enhanced detection of posed to different aromas and tastes depending on the type and
these learned odors later in life, which in nature would facilitate brand of formula used. In addition, how and whether the infant
selection of foods with these odors. learns to like the flavor depend on the timing and duration of
There is also evidence, as reviewed above, that dietary learning exposure (see also references 37 and 40). Perhaps most impor-
in humans is more pronounced during early life. In the first tant, unlike breastfed infants, formula-fed infants may be at-
randomized clinical study of development of flavor preferences, tracted to flavors that may not be among the foods the family
we assigned groups of pregnant women to drink carrot juice eats. Foods disliked by mothers typically are not offered to
during the last trimester of pregnancy or during the first 3 mo of children (31); thus, for example, if mothers feed ePHF to their
lactation, or to avoid carrots and carrot juice during both preg- infants but never eat broccoli themselves, the likelihood that
nancy and lactation. When mothers weaned their infants at w6 their infants will express and solidify their preferences for this
mo of age, infants exposed to carrot flavors in either amniotic vegetable is markedly diminished.
fluid or mother’s milk ate more of carrot-flavored cereal and We should emphasize that early milk feedings are typically not
made fewer faces of distaste than did nonexposed infants. either breast milk or formula but rather a combination of the 2,
This continuity in flavor helps the infant transition to solid with most infants feeding formula when they begin to experience
foods—for example, breastfed infants are more accepting of complementary foods (104). Future research should define both
cereal prepared with mother’s milk than cereal prepared with breastfeeding and formula feeding in terms of duration and
water (95). Breastfeeding confers greater acceptance of healthy exclusivity, as well as type and brand of formula, because for-
foods (eg, fruit, vegetables) only if they are part of the mothers’ mulas differ profoundly in composition and flavor profiles and
diet (28, 96). Mothers typically feed children foods that are part have differential effects on flavor preferences (26, 27, 101, 102)
of their own diet and culture, so the breastfeeding infant is and growth trajectories (105) during the first year of life (see
learning about foods his or she will be offered. The sensory reference 13 for previous review).
experiences with food flavors in mother’s milk in children
whose mothers eat a varied diet may explain why children who
were breastfed tend to be less picky (97) and more willing to try EVIDENCE-BASED STRATEGIES ON FRUIT AND
new foods during childhood (41, 42). VEGETABLE ACCEPTANCE DURING INFANCY
Basic research provides insights into the role of repeated
Formula exposure and variety in how infants learn to like fruit and
In striking contrast to the varied and rich sensory experiences vegetables. During the first year of life, infants make the dramatic
of breastfed infants, early flavor experience of formula-fed in- transition from an exclusive liquid diet to a mixed diet consisting
fants is more monotone and lacks the volatiles of the foods of the of breast milk or formula and a variety of complementary foods,
mother’s diet. In addition, there are striking differences in fla- and likely encounter a wide range of flavors and textures.
vors among the different types of formulas [eg, cow milk, soy, Concerns about how to wean children (eg, what types of foods
partial hydrolysate, extensive protein hydrolysate formula (ePHF)] to introduce, in what order) represent one of the primary matters
and brands of formulas, and formula-fed infants learn to prefer the mothers discuss with pediatricians. Because of the lack of re-
flavors of the formula they are fed and foods containing these search, many feeding practices are based on idiosyncratic parental
flavors (13, 26, 29, 98–102). behavior, family traditions, or medical lore. One example of such
For example, the distinctive flavor of ePHF is partly a result of lore relates that infants should not have any previous experience
abundant free amino acids, most notably glutamate (105), a key with fruit before they are introduced to green vegetables because
compound triggering the “umami” or savory taste quality that their inherent sweet preferences will interfere with acceptance of
occurs naturally in many foods, such as broths. ePHF also foods that taste bitter. However, no data support this contention, and
contains aromatics (eg, sulfur volatiles) that are also found in in fact, one study showed no difference in carrot acceptance by
cruciferous vegetables, such as broccoli. Infants fed ePHF ate infants with or without previous exposure to fruit (106).
more of infant cereals that tasted savory, sour, and bitter; ate Regardless of early feeding experiences, infants weaned to
them at a faster rate; and showed fewer facial expressions of complementary foods learn through both repeated exposure (8–
distaste during feeding than did infants fed milk-based formulas 10 exposures) to a particular food and exposure to foods that
(102). Both breastfed and ePHF-fed infants showed more posi- vary in both flavor and texture—this, in turn, promotes will-
tive facial reactions to savory formula, perhaps because breast ingness to eat not only introduced foods but also other, novel
milk (103) and ePHF (105) are both high in glutamate. foods (82, 96, 106–111) (see Table 1). Exposing infants to
Research has also shown that how long infants are fed a type of multiple sensory contrasts (between- and within-meal flavor
formula affects their earliest responses to savory food (27, 102). variety) also provides more opportunities to develop conditioned
In a randomized controlled study, infants fed ePHF for 3 or 8 mo, flavor preferences based on the postingestive reinforcing effects
but not for 1 mo, showed greater acceptance of a savory broth of nutritious foods (107). Feeding a novel food along with a familiar
relative to a plain broth and consumed it at a faster rate (27). The food might be an optimal combination to progressively accustom
708S

TABLE 1
Summary of experimental research findings on effects of exposure to fruit and vegetables during infancy
Type/length of exposure Target food or foods Results Study, year (reference)
1
Repeated exposure
8d Pears Increased acceptance of pears; no effect on acceptance Mennella et al, 2008 (107)
of novel vegetable (green beans)
8d Peaches Increased acceptance of peaches Forestell and Mennella, 2007 (96)
9d Carrots Increased acceptance of carrots Gerrish and Mennella, 2001 (106)
10 d Bananas or peas Increased acceptance of target food; seen as Birch et al, 1998 (108)
early as first day of exposure
8d Green beans Increased acceptance of green beans Sullivan and Birch, 1994 (111); Mennella et al, 2008 (107);
Forestell and Mennella, 2007 (96)
10 d Carrots or artichoke Increased acceptance of target food Caton et al, 2012 (110)2
Between-meal variety3
8d Peaches, prunes, apples Increased acceptance of novel fruit (pears); no effect Mennella et al, 2008 (107)
of acceptance of novel vegetable (green beans)
MENNELLA

9d Peas, potatoes, squash Increased acceptance of novel vegetable (carrots) Gerrish and Mennella, 2001 (106)
and meat (chicken)
8d Squash, spinach, carrots Increased acceptance of carrots and spinach; increased Mennella et al, 2008 (107)
acceptance of novel vegetable (green beans)
10–20 d Puréed, lumpy, and diced compared Increased acceptance of diced apples in those who Lundy et al, 1998 (109)
with puréed and diced apple sauce experienced more complex textures
Between- plus within-meal variety4
8d Squash/peas, carrot/peas, squash/spinach Increased acceptance of carrots and spinach; increased Mennella et al, 2008 (107)
acceptance of novel vegetable (green beans)
1
Infant was fed one target food at the same time of day for a number of days.
2
This study included both infants and toddlers.
3
Infant was fed only one target food each day, and the type of target food was alternated daily.
4
Infant was fed 2 target foods each day, and the pairs of foods varied from day to day.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
ONTOGENY OF TASTE PREFERENCES 709S
infants to novel foods. However, to promote acceptance of bitter- they are pregnant and while they breastfeed. Whereas infants fed
tasting vegetables, infants must be repeatedly exposed to either formula may initially be less accepting of some fruit and veg-
a particular green vegetable or a variety of vegetables, including etables that are not similar to the flavors of their formulas, they,
some bitter ones. too, can learn to like these foods: once weaned, both breastfed and
Although infants’ facial reactivity during feeding is related to formula-fed infants learn to accept foods through repeated expo-
intake, it is governed by different neural substrates; thus, intake sure and dietary variety. However, it is important to emphasize that
can increase sooner than facial expressions change during feeding such foods need to be part of the family’s diet so that once the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
(96)—infants may continue to show facial expressions of distaste preference develops, the infant continues to be exposed to the food
(eg, grimace), even though they are increasing their intake of to maintain the preference, learning to like more complicated fla-
a food with repeated exposure. Therefore, caregivers should vors and textures. Investigating whether there are optimum periods
focus on the infants’ willingness to eat the food and not just the when experience promotes greater liking will go a long way toward
facial expressions made during feeding. Ultimately, the goal is promoting healthy eating and, in turn, addressing the many illnesses
to gradually accustom children to a varied diet that meets related to poor food choices.
nutritional needs for growth and development and provides
I acknowledge the valuable comments from Gary K Beauchamp and the
them with opportunities to learn to like and prefer a variety of editorial assistance of Patricia Watson.
healthy foods. The sole author had responsibility for all parts of the manuscript. The au-
Another important component of foods is their texture. This thor reported no conflicts of interest.
area has received scant scientific attention. Nevertheless, there is
some evidence to suggest that the timing of exposure to varied
textures may also be important. For example, 6- to 12-mo-old
infants who experienced a variety of different-textured apple- REFERENCES
1. White House, Office of the First Lady. The First Lady Michele Obama
sauce (eg, puréed, lumpy, diced) preferred greater texture launches “Let’s Move”: America’s move to raise a healthier generation
complexity when tested later (109). Also, 7-y-old children who of kids. Version current 9 January 2014. Available from: http://www.
were introduced to lumpy solids after 9 mo of age, compared whitehouse.gov/the-press-office/first-lady-michelle-obama-launches-lets-
with between 6 and 9 mo of age, ate less of many food groups, move-americas-move-raise-a-healthier-genera (cited 9 January 2014).
2. Gidding SS, Dennison BA, Birch LL, Daniels SR, Gillman MW,
including all 10 categories of fruit and vegetables, and were Lichtenstein AH, Rattay KT, Steinberger J, Stettler N, Van Horn L.
reported as having significantly more feeding problems (109). Dietary recommendations for children and adolescents: a guide for
Further research is needed to examine, for example, whether practitioners: consensus statement from the American Heart Associ-
there is a sensitive period during which lack of exposure to ation. Circulation 2005;112:2061–75.
3. Lichtenstein AH, Appel LJ, Brands M, Carnethon M, Daniels S,
a variety of textures and flavors hinders acceptance of such Franch HA, Franklin B, Kris-Etherton P, Harris WS, Howard B, et al.
foods in the long term. Likewise, what are the long-term con- Diet and lifestyle recommendations revision 2006: a scientific state-
sequences of mothers who eat a “junk food” diet during preg- ment from the American Heart Association Nutrition Committee.
nancy and lactation. Emerging animal research models have Circulation 2006;114:82–96.
4. US Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2020:
shown that high maternal intakes of sugars and fats during nutrition and weight status objectives. Washington, DC: US De-
pregnancy and lactation result in altered development of the partment of Health and Human Services, 2010. Available from: www.
central reward system in offspring and excessive junk-food in- healthypeople.gov/2020/topicsobjectives2020/objectiveslist.aspx?
take in postnatal life (112, 113). topicid=29 (cited 9 January 2014).
5. Fox MK, Pac S, Devaney B, Jankowski L. Feeding infants and tod-
CONCLUDING REMARKS dlers study: What foods are infants and toddlers eating? J Am Diet
Assoc 2004;104:s22–30.
Many of the chronic illnesses that plague modern society, such 6. Mennella JA, Ziegler P, Briefel R, Novak T. Feeding Infants and
as obesity, diabetes, and hypertension, derive in large part from Toddlers Study: the types of foods fed to Hispanic infants and tod-
dlers. J Am Diet Assoc 2006;106:S96–106.
poor food choices, which relate to food and flavor experiences 7. Siega-Riz AM, Deming DM, Reidy KC, Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel
early in life. Although most people view food choice as a cultural RR. Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: where are we
trait, not directly related to our biology (114), overwhelming now? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:S38–51.
evidence suggests that children’s biology makes them especially 8. Siega-Riz AM, Carson T, Popkin B. Three squares or mostly snacks—
what do teens really eat? A sociodemographic study of meal patterns.
vulnerable to the current food environment of processed foods J Adolesc Health 1998;22:29–36.
high in salt and refined sugars. Thus, the struggle parents have in 9. Nicklas TA, Demory-Luce D, Yang SJ, Baranowski T, Zakeri I,
modifying their children’s diets to reduce added sugars and salt Berenson G. Children’s food consumption patterns have changed over
appears to have a strong biological basis, emphasizing the need two decades (1973-1994): the Bogalusa Heart Study. J Am Diet Assoc
2004;104:1127–40.
for new strategies to approach children’s diets. 10. Mannino ML, Lee Y, Mitchell DC, Smiciklas-Wright H, Birch LL.
Although we cannot easily change children’s innate liking of The quality of girls’ diets declines and tracks across middle child-
sweets and salts and disliking of bitterness—we cannot get them hood. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2004;1:5.
to prefer broccoli to candy—our growing knowledge of how, 11. Schmidt M, Affenito SG, Striegel-Moore R, Khoury PR, Barton B,
Crawford P, Kronsberg S, Schreiber G, Obarzanek E, Daniels S. Fast-
beginning very early in life, sensory experience can shape and food intake and diet quality in black and white girls: the National
modify flavor and food preferences can help us direct children’s Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Growth and Health Study. Arch
preferences more toward healthy flavors and variety and less Pediatr Adolesc Med 2005;159:626–31.
toward excess sweet and salty tastes. Because arrays of dietary 12. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. The role of early life experiences in
flavor perception and delight. In: Dube L, Bechara A, Dagher A,
flavors are transmitted from the maternal diet to both amniotic Drewnowski A, LeBel J, James P, Richard D, Yada RY, eds. Obesity
fluid and mother’s milk, mothers can get their infants off to prevention: the role of society and brain on individual behavior.
a good start by eating a diet rich in healthy foods, both while London, United Kingdom: Elsevier, 2010:203–18.
710S MENNELLA
13. Trabulsi JC, Mennella JA. Diet, sensitive periods in flavour learning, risk of cardiovascular diseases: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young
and growth. Int Rev Psychiatry 2012;24:219–30. Finns Study. Eur J Clin Nutr 2004;58:1038–45.
14. Mennella JA, Ventura AK. Early feeding: setting the stage for healthy 38. Mikkilä V, Rasanen L, Raitakari OT, Marniemi J, Pietinen P, Ronnemaa T,
eating habits. Nestle Nutr Workshop Ser Pediatr Program 2011;68: Viikari J. Major dietary patterns and cardiovascular risk factors from
153–63; discussion 64–8. childhood to adulthood: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study.
15. Beauchamp GK, Mason JR. Comparative hedonics of taste. In: Bolles Br J Nutr 2007;98:218–25.
RC, ed. The hedonics of taste. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Ealbaum 39. Lucas A. Programming by early nutrition: an experimental approach.
Associates, Inc, 1991:159–83. J Nutr 1998;128(suppl):401S–6S.
16. Glendinning JI. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? 40. Resnicow K, Smith M, Baranowski T, Baranowski J, Vaughan R,

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
Physiol Behav 1994;56:1217–27. Davis M. 2-Year tracking of children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
17. Lindemann B. Taste reception. Physiol Rev 1996;76:718–66. J Am Diet Assoc 1998;98:785–9.
18. Glass TA, McAtee MJ. Behavioral science at the crossroads in public 41. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-
health: extending horizons, envisioning the future. Soc Sci Med 2006; related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in
62:1650–71. school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav 2002;34:310–5.
19. Henney JE, Taylor CE, Boon CS. Strategies to reduce sodium intake 42. Cooke LJ, Wardle J, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson
in the United States. Washington, DC: Committee on Strategies to M. Demographic, familial and trait predictors of fruit and vegetable
Reduce Sodium Intake; Institute of Medicine, 2010. consumption by pre-school children. Public Health Nutr 2004;7:
20. Gidding SS, Lichtenstein AH, Faith MS, Karpyn A, Mennella JA, 295–302.
Popkin B, Rowe J, Van Horn L, Whitsel L. Implementing American 43. Lioret S, Campbell KJ, Crawford D, Spence AC, Hesketh K,
Heart Association pediatric and adult nutrition guidelines: a scientific McNaughton SA. A parent focused child obesity prevention in-
statement from the American Heart Association Nutrition Committee tervention improves some mother obesity risk behaviors: the Mel-
of the Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity and Metabolism, bourne inFANT program. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2012;9:100.
Council on Cardiovascular Disease in the Young, Council on Arte- 44. Wolfenden L, Wyse RJ, Britton BI, Campbell KJ, Hodder RK, Stacey
riosclerosis, Thrombosis and Vascular Biology, Council on Cardio- FG, McElduff P, James EL. Interventions for increasing fruit and
vascular Nursing, Council on Epidemiology and Prevention, and vegetable consumption in children aged 5 years and under. Cochrane
Council for High Blood Pressure Research. Circulation 2009;119: Database Syst Rev 2012;11:CD008552.
1161–75. 45. Forestell CA, Mennella JA. The ontogeny of taste perception and
21. Ekelund U, Ong KK, Linne Y, Neovius M, Brage S, Dunger DB, preference throughout childhood. In: Doty RL, ed. Handbook of
Wareham NJ, Rossner S. Association of weight gain in infancy and olfaction and gustation. 3rd ed. Wilmington, DE: John Wiley & Sons,
early childhood with metabolic risk in young adults. J Clin Endocrinol 2013;823–46.
Metab 2007;92:98–103. 46. Sclafani A. Sweet taste signaling in the gut. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
22. Barker DJ. The developmental origins of adult disease. J Am Coll 2007;104:14887–8.
Nutr 2004;23:588S–95S. 47. Liedtke WB, McKinley MJ, Walker LL, Zhang H, Pfenning AR,
23. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA. Developmental and epigenetic pathways Drago J, Hochendoner SJ, Hilton DL, Lawrence AJ, Denton DA.
to obesity: an evolutionary-developmental perspective. Int J Obes Relation of addiction genes to hypothalamic gene changes subserving
(Lond) 2008;32(suppl 7):S62–71. genesis and gratification of a classic instinct, sodium appetite. Proc
24. Lucas A. The developmental origins of adult health and well-being. Natl Acad Sci USA 2011;108:12509–14.
Adv Exp Med Biol 2005;569:13–5. 48. Berridge KC. Pleasures of the brain. Brain Cogn 2003;52:106–28.
25. Roth T, Sweatt JD. Epigenetic mechanisms and enviromental shaping 49. Steiner JE, Glaser D, Hawilo ME, Berridge KC. Comparative ex-
of the brain during sensitive periods of development. J Child Psychol pression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human
Psychiatr 2011;52:398–408. infants and other primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2001;25:53–74.
26. Mennella JA, Griffin CE, Beauchamp GK. Flavor programming 50. Maone TR, Mattes RD, Bernbaum JC, Beauchamp GK. A new
during infancy. Pediatrics 2004;113:840–5. method for delivering a taste without fluids to preterm and term in-
27. Mennella JA, Castor SM. Sensitive period in flavor learning: effects of fants. Dev Psychobiol 1990;23:179–91.
duration of exposure to formula flavors on food likes during infancy. 51. Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to four basic
Clin Nutr 2012;31:1022–5. tastes in newborns. Child Dev 1988;59:1555–68.
28. Mennella JA, Jagnow CP, Beauchamp GK. Prenatal and postnatal 52. Maller O, Turner RE. Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants.
flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics 2001;107:E88. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1973;84:496–501.
29. Mennella JA, Lukasewycz LD, Castor SM, Beauchamp GK. The 53. Desor JA, Beauchamp GK. Longitudinal changes in sweet preferences
timing and duration of a sensitive period in human flavor learning: in humans. Physiol Behav 1987;39:639–41.
a randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;93:1019–24. 54. Mennella JA. The sweet taste of childhood. In: Basbaum AI, Kaneko
30. Mennella JA. The chemical senses and the development of flavor A, Shepherd CM, Westheimer G, eds. The senses: a comprehensive
preferences in humans. In: Hale TW, Hartmann PE, eds. Textbook on reference. Vol 4. Olfaction and taste. San Diego, CA: Academic Press,
human lactation. Amarillo, TX: Hale Publishing, 2007:403–14. 2008:183–8.
31. Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Wendy B, Ziegler PJ. Children’s food 55. Mennella JA, Lukasewycz LD, Griffith JW, Beauchamp GK. Evalu-
preferences: a longitudinal analysis. J Am Diet Assoc 2002;102: ation of the Monell forced-choice, paired-comparison tracking pro-
1638–47. cedure for determining sweet taste preferences across the lifespan.
32. Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study Chem Senses 2011;36:345–55.
of food preferences in childhood. Food Qual Prefer 2004;15:805–18. 56. Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Acceptance of sweet and salty tastes in
33. Singer MR, Moore LL, Garrahie EJ, Ellison RC. The tracking of 2-year-old children. Appetite 1984;5:291–305.
nutrient intake in young children: the Framingham Children’s Study. 57. Pepino MY, Mennella JA. Sucrose-induced analgesia is related to
Am J Public Health 1995;85:1673–7. sweet preferences in children but not adults. Pain 2005;119:210–8.
34. Lioret S, McNaughton SA, Spence AC, Crawford D, Campbell KJ. 58. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Reed DR. Genetic and environmental de-
Tracking of dietary intakes in early childhood: the Melbourne In- terminants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics
FANT Program. Eur J Clin Nutr 2013;67:275–81. 2005;115:e216–22.
35. Northstone K, Emmett PM. Are dietary patterns stable throughout 59. Blass EM, Watt LB. Suckling- and sucrose-induced analgesia in hu-
early and mid-childhood? A birth cohort study. Br J Nutr 2008;100: man newborns. Pain 1999;83:611–23.
1069–76. 60. Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T,
36. Kaikkonen JE, Mikkila V, Magnussen CG, Juonala M, Viikari JS, Beyene J, Ohlsson A. Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in
Raitakari OT. Does childhood nutrition influence adult cardiovascular infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review. Arch
disease risk? Insights from the Young Finns Study. Ann Med 2013;45: Dis Child 2010;95:406–13.
120–8. 61. Miller A, Barr RG, Young SN. The cold pressor test in children:
37. Mikkilä V, Rasanen L, Raitakari OT, Pietinen P, Viikari J. Longitu- methodological aspects and the analgesic effect of intraoral sucrose.
dinal changes in diet from childhood into adulthood with respect to Pain 1994;56:175–83.
ONTOGENY OF TASTE PREFERENCES 711S
62. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Lehmann-Castor SM, Yourshaw LM. Sweet ence Intakes. Dietary Reference Intakes for water, potassium, sodium,
preferences and analgesia during childhood: effects of family history chloride, and sulfate. Washington, DC: The National Academies
of alcoholism and depression. Addiction 2010;105:666–75. Press, 2005. Available from: http://www.nap.edu/openbook.php?
63. Beauchamp GK, Cowart BJ, Moran M. Developmental changes in salt record_id=10925 (cited 9 January 2014).
acceptability in human infants. Dev Psychobiol 1986;19:17–25. 91. Mattes RD, Donnelly D. Relative contributions of dietary sodium
64. Bertino M, Beauchamp GK, Engelman K. Increasing dietary salt al- sources. J Am Coll Nutr 1991;10:383–93.
ters salt taste preference. Physiol Behav 1986;38:203–13. 92. Institute of Medicine; Committee on Strategies to Reduce Sodium
65. Crystal SR, Bernstein IL. Morning sickness: impact on offspring salt Intake; Food and Nutrition Board. In: Henney JE, Taylor CL, Boon
preference. Appetite 1995;25:231–40. CS, eds. Strategies to reduce sodium intake in the United States.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article/99/3/704S/4577479 by Instituto Nacional de Salud Publica (INSP) user on 03 October 2022
66. Crystal SR, Bernstein IL. Infant salt preference and mother’s morning Washington, DC: National Academic Press, 2010.
sickness. Appetite 1998;30:297–307. 93. Provenza FD, Launchbauch KL. Foraging on the edge of chaos:
67. Stein LJ, Cowart BJ, Beauchamp GK. Salty taste acceptance by in- grazing behavior of livestock and wildlife. In: Launchbaugh KL,
fants and young children is related to birth weight: longitudinal Sanders KD, Mosley JC, eds. Grazing behavior of livestock and
analysis of infants within the normal birth weight range. Eur J Clin wildlife. Idaho Forest, Wildlife and Range Experiment Station, Uni-
Nutr 2006;60:272–9. versity of Iowa, Moscow, Idaho, 1999:1–12.
68. Kajiura H, Cowart BJ, Beauchamp GK. Early developmental change 94. Todrank J, Heth G, Restrepo D. Effects of in utero odorant exposure
in bitter taste responses in human infants. Dev Psychobiol 1992;25: on neuroanatomical development of the olfactory bulb and odour
375–86. preferences. Proc Biol Sci 2011;278:1949–55.
69. Desor JA, Maller O, Andrews K. Ingestive responses of human 95. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. Mothers’ milk enhances the accep-
newborns to salty, sour, and bitter stimuli. J Comp Physiol Psychol tance of cereal during weaning. Pediatr Res 1997;41:188–92.
1975;89:966–70. 96. Forestell CA, Mennella JA. Early determinants of fruit and vegetable
70. Mangold JE, Hill DL. Extensive reorganization of primary afferent acceptance. Pediatrics 2007;120:1247–54.
projections into the gustatory brainstem induced by feeding a sodium- 97. Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL. Predictors and consequences of food
restricted diet during development: less is more. J Neurosci 2007;27: neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:
4650–62. 692–8.
71. Harder DB, Maggio JC, Whitney G. Assessing gustatory detection 98. Mennella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK. Vegetable acceptance by
capabilities using preference procedures. Chem Senses 1989;14: infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Dev 2006;82:463–8.
547–64. 99. Liem DG, Mennella JA. Sweet and sour preferences during child-
72. Meyerhof W, Batram C, Kuhn C, Brockhoff A, Chudoba E, Bufe B, hood: role of early experiences. Dev Psychobiol 2002;41:388–95.
Appendino G, Behrens M. The molecular receptive ranges of human 100. Mennella JA. The human infants’ responses to vanilla flavors in hu-
TAS2R bitter taste receptors. Chem Senses 2010;35:157–70. man milk and formula. Infant Behav Dev 1996;19:13–9.
73. Lipchock SV, Mennella JA, Spielman AJ, Reed DR. Human bitter 101. Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. Flavor experiences during formula
perception correlates with bitter receptor mRNA expression in taste feeding are related to preferences during childhood. Early Hum Dev
cells. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:1136–43. 2002;68:71–82.
74. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Duke FF, Reed DR. Age modifies the 102. Mennella JA, Forestell CA, Morgan LK, Beauchamp GK. Early milk
genotype-phenotype relationship for the bitter receptor TAS2R38. feeding influences taste acceptance and liking during infancy. Am J
BMC Genet 2010;11:60. Clin Nutr 2009;90:780S–8S.
75. Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Duke FF, Reed DR. Psychophysical dis- 103. Agostoni C, Carratu B, Boniglia C, Lammardo AM, Riva E, Sanzini
section of genotype effects on human bitter perception. Chem Senses E. Free glutamine and glutamic acid increase in human milk through
2011;36:161–7. a three-month lactation period. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 2000;31:
76. Beauchamp GK, Cowart BJ. Congenital and experiential factors in the 508–12.
development of human flavor preferences. Appetite 1985;6:357–72. 104. Grummer-Strawn LM, Scanlon KS, Fein SB. Infant feeding and
77. Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Dietary experience and sweet taste pref- feeding transitions during the first year of life. Pediatrics 2008;122
erence in human infants. Appetite 1982;3:139–52. (Suppl 2):S36–42.
78. Pepino MY, Mennella JA. Factors contributing to individual differ- 105. Mennella JA, Ventura AK, Beauchamp GK. Differential growth pat-
ences in sucrose preference. Chem Senses 2005;30(suppl 1):i319–20. terns among healthy infants fed protein hydrolysate or cow-milk
79. Liem DG, de Graaf C. Sweet and sour preferences in young children formulas. Pediatrics 2011;127:110–8.
and adults: role of repeated exposure. Physiol Behav 2004;83:421–9. 106. Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA. Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in
80. Taras HL, Gage M. Advertised foods on children’s television. Arch formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:1080–5.
Pediatr Adolesc Med 1995;149:649–52. 107. Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM. Variety is the
81. Coon KA, Tucker KL. Television and children’s consumption pat- spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance
terns: a review of the literature. Minerva Pediatr 2002;54:423–36. during infancy. Physiol Behav 2008;94:29–38.
82. Filer LJ. Studies of taste preference in infancy and childhood. Pedi- 108. Birch LL, Gunder L, Grimm-Thomas K, Laing DG. Infants’ con-
atric Basics 1978;12:5–9. sumption of a new food enhances acceptance of similar foods. Ap-
83. Ervin RB, Kit BK, Carroll MD, Ogden CL. Consumption of added petite 1998;30:283–95.
sugar among U.S. children and adolescents, 2005-2008. NCHS Data 109. Lundy BFT, Carraway K, Hart S, Malphurs J, Rosenstein M, Pelaez-
Brief No. 87, 2012:1–8. Nogueras M, Coletta F, Ott D, Hernandez-Reif M. Food texture
84. Mattes RD, Popkin BM. Nonnutritive sweetener consumption in hu- preferences in infants versus toddlers. Early Child Dev Care 1998;
mans: effects on appetite and food intake and their putative mecha- 146:69–85.
nisms. Am J Clin Nutr 2009;89:1–14. 110. Caton SJ, Ahern SM, Remy E, Nicklaus S, Blundell P, Hetherington
85. Drewnowski A, Mennella JA, Johnson SL, Bellisle F. Sweetness and MM. Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel
food preference. J Nutr 2012;142(suppl):1142S–8S. vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavour-flavour and
86. Stein LJ, Cowart BJ, Beauchamp GK. The development of salty taste flavour-nutrient learning. Br J Nutr 2013;109:2089–97.
acceptance is related to dietary experience in human infants: a pro- 111. Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Infant dietary experience and acceptance of
spective study. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;95:123–9. solid foods. Pediatrics 1994;93:271–7.
87. Harris G, Thomas A, Booth DA. Development of salt taste in infancy. 112. Ong ZY, Muhlhausler BS. Maternal “junk-food” feeding of rat dams
Dev Psychol 1990;26:534–8. alters food choices and development of the mesolimbic reward
88. Beauchamp GK, Engelman K. High salt intake: sensory and behav- pathway in the offspring. FASEB J 2011;25:2167–79.
ioral factors. Hypertension 1991;17:I176–81. 113. Bayol SA, Macharia R, Farrington SJ, Simbi BH, Stickland NC.
89. US Department of Health and Human Services; USDA; US Dietary Evidence that a maternal “junk food” diet during pregnancy and
Guidelines Advisory Committee. Dietary guidelines for Americans, lactation can reduce muscle force in offspring. Eur J Nutr 2009;48:
2010. 7th ed. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office, 2010. 62–5.
90. Panel on Dietary Reference Intakes for Electrolytes and Water, 114. Korthals M. Coevolution of nutrigenomics and society: ethical con-
Standing Committee on the Scientific Evaluation of Dietary Refer- siderations. Am J Clin Nutr 2011;94(suppl):2025S–9S.

You might also like