You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/256472256

Judging the Behavior of People we Know: Objective Assessment, Confirmation of


Pre-Existing Views, or Both?

Article  in  Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin · December 2013


DOI: 10.1177/0146167213507287

CITATIONS READS

35 329

3 authors:

Daniel Leising Anne-Marie Brigitte Gallrein


Technische Universität Dresden Technische Universität Dresden
81 PUBLICATIONS   1,717 CITATIONS    5 PUBLICATIONS   76 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Michael Dufner
Universität Witten/Herdecke
62 PUBLICATIONS   2,057 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Moderators of the Effects of Narcissism on Social Outcomes View project

Social processes underlying the joint development of personality and social relationships during university studies View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Michael Dufner on 10 May 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin http://psp.sagepub.com/

Judging the Behavior of People We Know: Objective Assessment, Confirmation of Preexisting Views, or
Both?
Daniel Leising, Anne-Marie B. Gallrein and Michael Dufner
Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2014 40: 153 originally published online 9 October 2013
DOI: 10.1177/0146167213507287

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://psp.sagepub.com/content/40/2/153

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

Society for Personality and Social Psychology

Additional services and information for Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://psp.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://psp.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Jan 13, 2014

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Oct 9, 2013

What is This?

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


507287
research-article2013
PSPXXX10.1177/0146167213507287Personality and Social Psychology BulletinLeising et al.

Article

Personality and Social

Judging the Behavior of People We Know:


Psychology Bulletin
2014, Vol. 40(2) 153­–163
© 2013 by the Society for Personality
Objective Assessment, Confirmation of and Social Psychology, Inc
Reprints and permissions:

Preexisting Views, or Both? sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/0146167213507287
pspb.sagepub.com

Daniel Leising1, Anne-Marie B. Gallrein1,


and Michael Dufner2

Abstract
The present study investigates the relative extent to which judgments of people’s behavior are influenced by “truth” (as
measured by averaged observer-judgments) and by systematic bias (i.e., perceivers’ preexisting views of target persons).
Using data from online questionnaires and laboratory sessions (N = 155), we demonstrate that self- and peer-judgments of
people’s actual behavior in specific situations are somewhat accurate but are also affected by what perceivers thought of the
targets before observing their behavior. The latter effect comprises a general evaluative component (generally positive or
negative views of targets) and a content-specific component (views of targets in terms of specific characteristics, for example,
“restrained”). We also found that friends, but not targets themselves, tend to judge targets’ behaviors more positively than
unacquainted observers do. The relevance of these findings for person perception in everyday life and in research contexts
is discussed.

Keywords
behavior, judgment, accuracy, evaluation, acquaintance

Received October 30, 2012; revision accepted September 8, 2013

A real-life example: A board of assessors meets to see a According to West and Kenny’s (2011) Truth and Bias
candidate present his research, and then, based on their Model, the variables affecting all sorts of judgments, includ-
observations, decide whether the candidate should be ing judgments of persons, can be categorized as either “truth”
granted a “certificate of eligibility for tenure” (Habilitation, or “bias” variables. Truth variables represent what is “real”
specific to the academic system in some European coun- or “objectively correct” about a judgment, whereas bias vari-
tries). The meeting is public, thus many people who are not ables represent any systematic influences apart from that.
members of the board—including one of the candidate’s The extent to which a truth or bias variable affects judgments
close friends—are also part of the audience. After the talk, is called the “force” of that variable. In the present study, we
the candidate’s friend approaches some colleagues and investigate the relative extents to which self- and peer-
praises the candidate’s performance: “Now, this is how it’s judgments of people’s behavior in specific situations are
done!” and so on. It is obvious that he actually believes the affected by how the target actually behaved (truth) and by the
candidate performed really well and must have impressed preexisting views that the perceivers had of the targets before
everybody in the room quite a bit. Praising the candidate’s observing their behavior (bias).
performance for other reasons (e.g., to influence the deci-
sion that is about to be made) would make no sense for him
because the people he talks to have nothing to do with the
Truth in Judgments of Behavior
formal evaluation of the candidate. Notably, nobody agrees There are at least two distinct ways in which behavior may
with him. Most people just nod politely to what he says and be described. On the one hand, one may focus on qualities of
remain silent. Obviously, the other people in the room, who
just saw the candidate for the very first time, agree that the 1
Technical University Dresden, Germany
candidate’s performance was actually quite horrible. How 2
Humboldt-University Berlin, Germany
may such discrepant perceptions of the same person’s
Corresponding Author:
behavior be explained? May the personal relationship Daniel Leising, Department of Psychology, Technical University Dresden,
between the candidate and the friend have something to do 01062 Dresden, Germany.
with it? Email: Daniel.Leising@tu-dresden.de

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


154 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(2)

behavior that are measurable with great exactness (e.g., previously neglected question of whether the same is true
whether person A decreases or increases her physical dis- for behavioral judgments that are provided by close
tance from person B). Such qualities of behavior may often acquaintances of the targets.
be assessed without relying much on any subjective human
judgments (e.g., by means of a ruler). The downside of such Preexisting Views as Biases in
assessments is that their meaning is often psychologically
ambiguous, and thus needs to be interpreted a posteriori by
Judgments of Behavior
the researcher. In everyday life, on the other hand, people The systematic biases that we focus on in the present article
tend to describe their own and others’ behaviors by meaning- concern perceivers’ preexisting views of the target persons
laden terms like, for example, “friendly,” “hasty,” “hostile,” whose behavior they judge. In terms of the Truth and Bias
or “relaxed.” That is, they focus on the psychological rele- model, such preexisting views are bias variables that may
vance of people’s behavior rather than on its physical quali- affect behavioral judgments independent of the truth. There
ties. This latter kind of behavioral judgment is also the one are at least two conceptually distinct ways in which such
that we are dealing with in the present article. However, views may influence subsequent judgments of behavior, one
determining whether a perceiver’s judgment of a target’s of them being globally evaluative and the other being con-
behavior by means of a meaning-laden term reflects the tent-specific. In the first case, a perceiver may simply have
“truth” is less straightforward than in the first case. an overall positive (or negative) attitude toward a target, and
It may be argued that the best way to assess whether thus tend to use all sorts of positive (or negative) terms to
some behavior is “actually” friendly, hasty, hostile, or describe that target’s behavior in a given situation, irrespec-
relaxed is the average view that many neutral observers tive of how the target “actually” behaved. In the second case,
have of that behavior (cf. Kenny, 2004). Averaging different a perceiver may have a content-specific view of a target (e.g.,
perceivers’ judgments of the same behavior reduces the as being arrogant, witty, or aloof), and thus tend to judge that
impact of the individual perceivers’ idiosyncratic term uses target’s behavior in a given situation accordingly, irrespec-
and focuses on the common element instead. The existence of tive of how the target “actually” behaved.
the latter constitutes the very reason why person-descriptive Regarding global evaluation, there is a considerable varia-
language may be effectively used at all. Moreover, averag- tion in how positively or negatively people tend to view them-
ing judgments of behavior will yield an impression that is selves and others (cf. Bono & Judge, 2003; Furr & Funder,
more representative of how a behavior will be perceived by 1998; Kim, Schimmack, & Oishi, 2012; Leising, Erbs, &
the average other person. This in turn is important because Fritz, 2010; Nisbett & Wilson, 1977; Saucier, 1994;
how others interpret our behaviors will strongly determine Srivastava, Guglielmo, & Beer, 2010; Wood, Harms, &
the consequences of those behaviors (Leising & Müller- Vazire, 2010), and such different attitudes strongly predict
Plath, 2009), so the “social reality” that only exists in other whether perceivers will use positive or negative terms in
people’s minds at first (e.g., “the remark that Pete just made characterizing targets (Leising et al., 2010; Leising, Ostrovski,
was really offensive”) may eventually have very real effects & Zimmermann, 2013). Sometimes, a perceiver’s character-
on the target person (e.g., Pete may be asked to leave). In ization of a target’s behavior in a given situation may even
accordance with this view, we define accuracy as the agree- depend more on whether the perceiver is fond of the target or
ment between a given perceiver’s judgment of a given not than on how the target “actually” behaved. We are talking
behavior and the average judgment of the same behavior by about partisanship here. Research in social psychology has
several neutral observers. In terms of West and Kenny’s demonstrated that perceptions of behavior may be strongly
(2011) Truth and Bias model, the truth variable in our study affected by a perceiver’s loyalty or allegiance regarding the
is the average behavior judgment by several neutral observ- target person: Studies of presidential debates in the United
ers, and the extent to which this variable predicts judgments States showed that this factor predicted which candidate
of the same behavior by any other perceivers (e.g., targets viewers would see as the winner (cf. Munro et al., 2002). In
or informants, see below) is the “force” of the truth the present study, we expect that a perceiver who already
variable. holds a generalized positive image of a target person will see
Using such a definition, research has shown that indi- the subsequent behaviors of that target person in a more posi-
viduals are in fact able to judge their own behavior within tive light, whereas a perceiver with a more negative attitude
specific situations with some degree of accuracy (e.g., toward a target will do the opposite. We apply this logic to
Leising, 2011; Sadler & Woody, 2003). In these studies, self- and peer-judgments of behavior alike.
participants were asked to judge their own behavior during Apart from global evaluation, a perceiver may think that a
laboratory interactions, and these self-ratings did predict person’s behavior in a given situation was highly (for exam-
judgments of the same interactions by unacquainted observ- ple) sophisticated, friendly, or arrogant because he or she
ers. Therefore, we expect that in the present study, self- tended to think of the target as being sophisticated, friendly,
judgments of behavior will converge at least somewhat or arrogant even before actually observing the behavior in
with judgments by neutral observers. We also address the question. Note that here it is the specific content of the word,

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


Leising et al. 155

not its evaluative connotation, that matters. Let us consider, own behavior (Leising, 2011), or the personality assessment
for example, a perceiver who is supposed to judge a known took place shortly after the participants had judged their
target’s “quickness” at a problem-solving task. Regardless of own behavior (Sadler & Woody, 2003). In the present study,
how positively or negatively the perceiver thinks of the tar- we use much longer intervals between the two kinds of
get in general, if she is convinced that the target is usually assessment in order to rule out this alternative explanation.
“quick” at accomplishing things, she may tend to judge the Finally, our study extends the previous work by testing
quickness of the target’s problem solving in the situation whether the preexisting views affect not only self- but also
accordingly, regardless of how quick the target actually was. peer-judgments of behavior.
There is some evidence supporting the notion that people
tend to characterize the behaviors of targets in line with pre-
existing images, and that to some extent, this happens inde-
Possible Mechanisms
pendent of the targets’ actual behaviors. For example, in the Even though we do not directly address psychological mecha-
study by Sadler and Woody (2003), the participants tended to nisms in the present article, we think it should still be asked
rate their own behavior during an interpersonal interaction in why one may expect effects of perceivers’ preexisting views
accordance with their general self-views, even if their “actual” of targets on those perceivers’ subsequent judgments of the
behavior during the interaction (as consensually judged by same targets’ behaviors. Would it not be more reasonable for
several observers) was controlled for. In other words, the par- a perceiver to form as accurate an impression of the target’s
ticipants overestimated the extent to which their behavior in behavior as possible and not rely on any other source of infor-
the situation concurred with what they generally thought of mation than “what is actually there” (truth)? A large number
themselves. Sadler and Woody (2003) called this phenome- of studies have demonstrated that people tend to interpret new
non a “consistency bias” (cf. Leising, 2011). information in accordance with their preexisting beliefs or
In the present article, we will use the term preexisting expectations (see Nickerson, 1998, for a review). This phe-
views (PEV) to denote the images of the targets that already nomenon has often been attributed to motivational factors
existed in the perceivers’ minds before they engage in the such as a basic need to maintain cognitive congruence
task of judging the targets’ behavior in specific situations. An (Festinger, 1957; Kunda, 1990; Nickerson, 1998). It may be
important limitation of the aforementioned studies is that argued that such congruence is needed because only congru-
they did not distinguish between preexisting views that are ent views permit predictions of future events. With regard to
globally evaluative and preexisting views that are content- behavior perception, this would imply that once a perceiver
specific. Thus, for example, a given perceiver’s judging a has formed a certain view of a target person, he or she may be
target’s personality and behavior as “witty” may have been inclined to actively seek or prioritize novel information that
due to the specific content of the term or simply due to the concurs with this view. In fact, research has shown that once
fact that the term has a positive evaluative connotation. In the individuals have developed some belief about another person
present study, we disentangle these two possibilities from (e.g. “the person is friendly”), they selectively seek behav-
one another by separately investigating the influences of pre- ioral evidence that confirms rather than falsifies this belief
existing views that are globally evaluative (PEV-GE) and (Lord, Lepper, & Preston, 1984; Snyder & Campbell, 1980;
preexisting views that are content-specific (PEV-CS). Snyder & Swann, 1978).
Moreover, we also disentangle two components of behav- Another possible explanation is more informational in
ioral judgments that were not disentangled in previous stud- nature. For a perceiver who has access to several sources of
ies of the “consistency bias”: Preexisting views of targets information about a target (e.g., observations of the target’s
may affect subsequent judgments of those targets’ behaviors behavior in several situations), it may be wise to pool that
because (a) perceivers judge particular targets in certain information in order to form the most accurate impression of
ways and/or because (b) perceivers tend to judge all targets the target possible. The upside of doing so would be that the
in certain ways. In Kenny’s (1994) terminology, the former resulting image of the person would likely become more rep-
would be denoted as “relationship effects,” whereas the latter resentative of how the person is on average, possibly permit-
would be denoted as “perceiver effects.” We investigate how ting more accurate predictions of the target’s behavior in the
strongly both components contribute to the consistency long run. Thus, if the target’s behavior in a new situation
between preexisting views and subsequent judgments of deviates from what the perceiver has previously learned
behavior. about the person, the new information may be partially dis-
Furthermore, in the studies described above the interval counted as an “exception,” and the interpretation of the new
between the global self-assessment of personality and the behavior may be “colored” by the perceiver’s previous expe-
momentary self-assessment of situated behavior was very riences with the target. The downside of doing so, however,
short (less than an hour). It is thus conceivable that the influ- would be that the perceiver’s ability to judge the target’s
ence of the perceivers’ preexisting views was overestimated behavior in just one situation objectively may at least partly
because the participants’ general images of themselves were be compromised. The present study investigates the extent to
made salient shortly before they were asked to judge their which this is the case.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


156 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(2)

Overview of the Present Study Procedure


Given the ubiquity and importance of self- and peer-judg- Recruitment.  We advertised the study on a campus website
ments of behavior in everyday life as well as in psychologi- and in local community centers. The announcement of the
cal research, it seems worthwhile to examine the factors that study contained an e-mail address to which participants were
contribute to the formation of such judgments. In the present asked to write in order to register for the study. When pro-
study, we use a design that allows us to simultaneously spective participants contacted the research team, they
investigate the influence of truth and bias (i.e., perceivers’ received an e-mail containing some broad information
preexisting view of targets) on self- and peer-judgments of regarding the aims and procedures of the study. The e-mail
behavior. In order to maximize external validity (cf. also asked the participants to recruit a second person who
Baumeister, Vohs, & Funder, 2007), we do not use written knew them reasonably well and would also be willing to take
vignettes of people’s behavior (which was the predominant part in the study. When a prospective participant contacted
method in previous studies addressing similar issues) but the research team again and named a second person who had
rather investigate judgments of people’s actual behaviors. agreed to participate, we randomly determined which of the
Specifically, we invite target persons and their peers to the two persons would serve as the target and which would serve
laboratory and then videotape the targets’ behaviors in a vari- as the informant. In the following, we will call these target-
ety of standardized situations. Afterward, the targets and informant pairs dyads.
their peers judge the targets’ behaviors. In order to obtain an
accuracy criterion, the same behaviors are also judged by Personality questionnaires.  Both the target and the informant
unacquainted observers. The general images that the targets of a dyad received an e-mail containing a link to an online
and their peers have of the targets’ personalities are assessed questionnaire. They were asked to log in and describe the
by means of online questionnaires several days prior to the target person’s personality by means of an adjective list (see
laboratory assessments. The design enables us to compare below). We emphasized that the two persons should com-
the relative influence of the truth (= averaged observer-judg- plete the questionnaire independently and that they should
ments), as compared to PEV-GE and PEV-CS, on the partici- not communicate with each other about their ratings. The
pants’ judgments of their own and their peers’ behaviors. online questionnaire also contained a number of questions
regarding the two participants’ personal backgrounds (e.g.,
socioeconomic status) and their relationship with each other
Method (e.g., knowing and liking).
Sample Laboratory session.  A few days later, the target and the infor-
The main sample comprised two groups of participants, the mant were invited to come to the lab together for a series of
target persons (N = 155) and the informants (N = 155), who observational assessments. These assessments all took place
came to the lab together in dyads (see below). Each partici- on the same day and comprised three stages. In the first stage,
pant (i.e., target or informant) received 15 Euros as a reward the target person engaged in four tasks, which were presented
for participating. One hundred and fifty target persons com- in random order. Task 1 consisted of answering a number of
pleted the online questionnaire. Of these, 86 were female, 63 questions pertaining to general knowledge (e.g., “How high
were male, and 1 did not report sex. The mean age of the is Mount Everest?” “How many people live in the Tokyo
targets was 23.2 years (SD = 4.06). Of the informants, 153 area?” “Who wrote the opera ‘Fidelio’?”). Task 2 consisted
completed the online questionnaire. Of these, 93 were of a role-play in which the target person was to call a “neigh-
female, 59 were male, and 1 did not report sex. The mean age bor” (actually played by a confederate) and to demand that
of the informants was 23.8 years (SD = 4.44). The majority the neighbor turns down the volume on her stereo (cf. Borke-
(n = 85) of the informants were friends of the targets’. nau, Mauer, Riemann, Spinath, & Angleitner, 2004). In Task
Another large proportion of the informants were the targets’ 3, the target person was asked to spontaneously tell a brief
romantic partners (n = 53). Other kinds of relationships story incorporating the terms “Corkscrew,” “Holiday,”
between informants and partners (e.g., colleagues, siblings) “Catastrophe,” and “Glove Box.” In Task 4, the target person
were less frequent (n < 10). The informants reported know- was asked to (a) sing a song of his or her own choice, (b) tell
ing the targets quite well (M = 4.17, SD = 0.79, on a scale a joke of his or her own choice, and (c) pantomime the term
ranging from 1 [“not at all”] to 5 [“very well”]) and liking party (which in German does not also mean “political party”
them very much (M = 4.70, SD = 0.51, on a scale ranging but only “festivity”). The four tasks were selected to enable
from 1 [“not at all”] to 5 [“very much”]). The values for the an assessment of diverse qualities of behavior. Completing a
targets’ knowing and liking of the informants were virtually task usually took a target less than 90 s. The targets’ behavior
identical (Knowing: M = 4.21, SD = 0.73; Liking: M = 4.67, while engaging in the tasks was videotaped. While the target
SD = 0.55), which is unsurprising given that the roles of tar- of a given dyad completed the four tasks, the respective
get and informant were assigned at random (see below). informant was seated in another room and asked to wait.

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


Leising et al. 157

In the second stage of the laboratory session, the target and more stable behavioral dispositions (in the online personality
the informant each rated four video-clips, by means of the questionnaire) and their more transient situation-bound behav-
same adjective list that was also used for the personality iors (in the lab). This conforms to the personality states
assessments (see below). These video-ratings took place in approach, which posits that both traits and states may be
separate rooms. One member of the dyad (the target or the assessed by means of the same vocabulary (Fleeson, 2001;
informant) rated the video-clips that had just been produced— Bleidorn, 2009). For example, in the online questionnaire, the
the ones showing the current target engage in the four tasks. targets and their informants rated how “shy” or “egoistic” they
The tasks were presented in the same order in which the target considered the targets to be in general, whereas in the lab ses-
had completed them. Meanwhile, the other member of the sion, they rated how “shy” or “egoistic” they considered the
dyad watched four video-clips showing the target of the pre- targets’ behavior during the tasks to be. However, because fac-
vious dyad (i.e., an unknown person) engage in the same tor structures of measures may differ considerably depending
tasks. Both the target and the informant of the current dyad on whether a measure is used for assessing states versus traits
were asked to watch one of the respective video-clips that had (e.g., Leising & Bleidorn, 2011), we only report analyses at
been assigned to them, then rate the respective target’s behav- the level of individual items in the following. This is also the
ior by means of the adjectives, then watch the next video-clip, level of abstraction at which everyday communication about
and so on. Whether the informant watched the current target individual differences in personality and behavior takes place.
and the current target watched the previous target, or vice The intervals between the completion of the online question-
versa, was also determined at random. naire and the laboratory assessments had an average length of
In the third stage of the laboratory session, the previous M = 4.08 days (SD = 3.24) for the targets and of M = 4.43 days
rating assignments were reversed: If the informant had just (SD = 4.37) for the informants.
rated the current target, and the current target had just rated Each of the 30 adjectives was also rated in terms of how
the previous target, then the informant was now supposed to positive or negative an impression of a target person it evokes
rate the previous target, whereas the current target was sup- (i.e., social desirability). For this, we used the ratings that
posed to rate himself or herself. If the rating assignments in were provided by 24 student raters as part of the study by
the second stage had been the other way round, then the Leising et al. (2010). The scale ranged from 1 (very negative)
assignments in the third stage were reversed accordingly. to 7 (very positive). Interrater reliability for these ratings was
This way, we let both the informants and the targets judge (a) ICC(3, 24) = .98. The desirability ratings were needed to
targets they knew and (b) targets they did not know. compute global evaluation indices by correlating a perceiv-
er’s ratings of a target on the 30 items with the profile of item
External observers’ ratings.  Each of the 620 video-clips (= 155 desirabilities (Edwards, 1953). The resulting correlations
targets × 4 tasks) was also independently rated by at least reflect the extent to which the perceiver describes the target
three observers who were unacquainted with the targets (the in a more positive or negative manner overall.
maximum number of observers per video was six). The over-
all pool of observers comprised 18 persons from which sub-
groups were selected randomly to judge the individual Results
video-clips. The ratings were balanced such that each observer Agreement Between the Observers
would get to see all targets but only see each target in one
randomly chosen situation. Hence, the observer-ratings We used averaged observer-ratings as a measure of the tar-
always reflected judgments by persons who saw the respec- gets’ “true” behaviors during the laboratory tasks. The aver-
tive target in just one situation. The observers did not receive age interrater agreement, ICC(1, 3), between three observers
any training (apart from general technical instructions on how in judging the targets’ behavior by means of a single item
to watch the videos and provide their ratings) in order to pre- was .45 (SD = 0.15) for the story task, .47 (SD = 0.19) for the
serve the representativeness of their intuitive ways of apply- pantomime task, .44 (SD = 0.18) for the role-play task, and
ing the adjectives to the target persons’ behaviors. .49 (SD = 0.11) for the knowledge task. These levels of inter-
rater agreement are comparable to the levels of agreement
that were obtained in previous studies (e.g., Borkenau et al.,
Measure 2004) and can be regarded as quite satisfactory. Note that
The adjective list that we employed for the ratings of the tar- they reflect lower-bound estimates for interrater agreement
gets’ personalities and behavior in the lab was devised by because the performances of most targets in most situations
Borkenau and Ostendorf (1998) and has been used in several were judged by more than three observers.
person perception studies (e.g., Leising et al., 2010; Leising et
al., 2013). It consists of 30 terms that assess the Big Five per-
Correlations Among the Predictors
sonality factors by six items each. Three terms for each factor
have a positive valence and three have a negative valence. We In West and Kenny’s (2011) Truth and Bias Model, the truth
let the participants use the same terms for assessing the targets’ and bias variables may or may not be correlated with one

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


158 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(2)

Table 1.  Prediction of the Targets’ and the Informants’ Behavior Ratings From the Truth and the Two Bias Variables.

Standardized Betas
Without
Criterion Predictor Betas Raw perceiver effects
Behavior rating by target Truth .22 (.16, .28) .14 (.10, .18) .17 (.13, .21)
  PEV-CS bias .18 (.14, .22) .17 (.13, .21) .13 (.10, .15)
  PEV-GE bias .28 (.21, .35) .09 (.06, .11) .03 (.00, .05)
Behavior rating by Truth .31 (.23, .38) .18 (.13, .23) .18 (.15, .22)
informant PEV-CS bias .21 (.17, .25) .19 (.15, .23) .08 (.05, .11)
   PEV-GE bias .22 (.13, .30) .08 (.04, .10) .04 (.02, .06)

Note. Coefficients were first computed for each task and item separately and then averaged (99% bootstrap confidence intervals are in parentheses).
Truth = averaged observer-ratings of the targets’ behavior, PEV-CS = Perceivers’ preexisting views of targets (content-specific) as measured by the
perceivers’ response to the same item in the personality questionnaire. PEV-GE = Perceivers’ preexisting views of targets (global evaluation) as measured
by the correlation between the perceiver’s description of the target in the personality questionnaire and the social desirabilities of the 30 items. PEV-GE
was multiplied with minus 1 when predicting behavior ratings on items with a negative valence (average desirability rating < 4).

another. In our sample, all correlations between the three pre- preexisting views of the targets in terms of particular items
dictors (averaged across items) were statistically significant (PEV-CS), and (c) the same perceivers’ preexisting global
as indicated by 99% bootstrap confidence intervals that did evaluations of the targets (PEV-GE). For these analyses, we
not include zero. The targets’ self-ratings on an individual used the targets’ and the informants’ ratings of persons they
questionnaire item (PEV-CS) predicted the observers’ ratings knew (i.e., the current targets) because without prior acquain-
of the targets’ behavior on the same item (truth) at r = .14 on tance between target and perceiver, behavior judgments
average. The respective correlation for the informants’ ratings could not be affected by the latter two factors. Note that by
of the targets’ personalities was r = .11. Thus, the targets’ and using multiple regressions, we determined the independent
the informants’ item-specific views of the targets’ personali- contributions of each predictor, controlling for the other two
ties had some validity, in that they could predict independent predictors.
ratings of the targets’ behaviors in the lab later on. Table 1 displays the average Betas that emerged for the
Before computing the average correlation of PEV-GE three predictors, along with 99% confidence intervals that
with PEV-CS and with the observer-ratings of the targets’ were determined by bootstrapping with the 30 items as cases.
behavior, PEV-GE had to be inverted (i.e., multiplied with To arrive at these correlations, we first ran multiple regres-
−1) in all cases where it was correlated with an item that had sions for each item and task separately (= 120 regressions),
a negative valence (i.e., an average desirability rating < 4). then averaged the resulting coefficients across tasks for each
Otherwise, the correlations of PEV-GE with positive and item (distinguishing between tasks did not lead to any differ-
negative items might have canceled out when averaging ent conclusions), and then averaged across the 30 items. In
across items. After implementing this rule, the average cor- computing average Betas for PEV-GE, we again reversed the
relation between PEV-GE and PEV-CS was .28 for the tar- direction of the predictor for all items with a negative
gets’ self-ratings and .34 for the informant-ratings, suggesting valence. Otherwise, positive and negative associations might
that targets who received more positive overall personality have canceled out. In fact, when not inverting PEV-GE in
ratings were also judged more positively on individual items. predictions of negative items, the average standardized Betas
The averaged observer-ratings of the targets’ behavior (truth) for the individual items correlated at r(28) = .94 (targets) and
correlated at .06 (on average) with PEV-GE in the targets’ r(28) = .87 (informants) with the items’ social desirability
self-ratings of their personalities and also at .06 with PEV-GE ratings. Thus, the more an item entailed a positive or nega-
in the respective informant-ratings. Thus, targets who had tive evaluation, the better the perceivers’ ratings of the tar-
received more positive overall personality ratings were also gets’ behavior by means of that item could be predicted from
judged in a slightly more positive manner by the average the same perceivers’ preexisting global evaluations of the
observer, on individual items. targets (PEV-GE).
In our discussion of the average contributions of the three
Relative Influence of Truth and Bias on predictors, we will concentrate on the standardized Betas
that are displayed in the second and third data column of
Judgments of the Behavior of Known Targets Table 1. As can be seen from the entries in the second data
Using multiple regressions, we predicted the participants’ column, all three predictors made significant contributions in
(i.e., the targets’ and the informants’) judgments of the tar- predicting the targets’ and the informants’ judgments of the
gets’ behavior in the lab from (a) averaged observer-judg- targets’ behavior: The targets’ actual behavior (truth)—as
ments of the same behavior (truth), (b) the same perceivers’ judged by unacquainted observers—and the perceivers’

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


Leising et al. 159

content-specific preexisting views of the targets (PEV-CS) with a target impairs validity, then accuracy should be lower
both made significant contributions of approximately the for “a” and “b” as compared to “c” and “d.”
same size. In addition, the perceivers’ globally evaluative First, we computed the correlations between each of the
views of the targets (PEV-GE) made another significant, but four perspectives and the averaged observer-ratings sepa-
somewhat smaller, contribution of their own. The relative rately for each task and item. Then we averaged across tasks.
strengths of the three effects were approximately the same Across the 30 items, the mean correlation was r = .18 (99%
for self- and informant-ratings of behavior. bootstrap CI = [.13, .22]) for the current targets’ ratings of
We also controlled for perceiver effects in order to deter- their own behavior (a), r = .22 (CI = [.17, .27]) for the current
mine how much of the influence of the two bias variables informants’ ratings of the current targets’ behavior (b), r =
was due to general perceptual tendencies as opposed to the .26 (CI = [.22, .30]) for the current targets’ ratings of the
perceivers’ views of the particular targets at hand. In the previous targets’ behavior (c), and r = .21 (CI = [.16, .26]) for
present study, a perceiver’s perceiver-effect was obtained by the current informants’ ratings of the previous targets’ behav-
averaging his or her judgments of the behavior of the known ior (d). Confidence intervals revealed that only the greatest
target (i.e., himself or herself or the other person in his or her difference (.18 vs. .26) was significant, suggesting that the
dyad) and the unknown target (i.e., the target of the previous accuracy of the targets’ judgments was greater when they
dyad). Perceiver-effects were calculated separately for each judged someone else’s behavior as compared to when they
of the 30 items and subtracted from the participants’ ratings judged their own behavior.
of the targets’ behavior (i.e., the DV) before repeating the
item-wise multiple regressions described above. The results
are displayed in the third data column of Table 1: Controlling
Global Evaluation Across Different Assessments
for perceiver effects did not change the effect size for the Finally, we compared how positively or negatively partici-
truth variable (i.e., observer-ratings of the targets’ behav- pants’ personalities and behaviors were judged by the dif-
ior). It did, however, significantly reduce the influence of the ferent types of perceivers. In each case, a perceiver’s overall
perceivers’ global evaluations of the targets (PEV-GE) on evaluation of a target was measured in terms of the within-
both self- and informant-ratings of behavior. Thus, a substan- person correlation between the perceiver’s description of
tial proportion of the above-reported effect of this predictor the target across the 30 adjectives and the social desirability
was due to perceiver effects. For self-ratings, the respective ratings for these adjectives. Thus, a positive correlation was
confidence interval almost included zero (the lower confi- indicative of positive global evaluation. Note that global
dence limit was minimally larger than zero, which is obscured evaluation coefficients were computed for the participants’
in the table because of rounding). Finally, controlling for per- judgments of the targets’ personalities by means of the
ceiver effects also lowered the impact of PEV-CS on behav- online questionnaire (= PEV-GE) as well as for their judg-
ior judgments, with the decrease being considerably stronger ments of the targets’ behavior in the lab. Each target’s
for the informants’ (compared to the targets’) judgments of behavior in the lab was rated by one previously acquainted
the targets’ behavior. target (i.e., the target’s self-rating) and one previously
acquainted informant, as well as by one unacquainted target
Loss of Validity Due to Target-Perceiver and one unacquainted informant. In addition, there were
three to six observer-ratings of each target’s behavior. In
Relationship? order to ensure comparability between perspectives, we
Given that the perceiver’s preexisting views of the targets first computed a global evaluation index for each individual
were found to affect their subsequent judgments of the tar- observer and then averaged across observers. For behavior
gets’ behavior, we investigated whether the accuracy of judg- ratings, evaluation indices were first computed separately
ments of known targets was lower than the accuracy of for each task and then averaged across tasks.
judgments of unknown targets. To this aim, we compared the The average global evaluation indices were r = .58 (range
accuracy of four kinds of judgments with each other: (a) The = −.14 to .92) for the targets’ self-descriptions and r = .68
targets’ judgments of their own behavior, (b) the informants’ (range = .05 to .93) for the informants’ descriptions of “their”
judgments of the behavior of “their” targets, (c) the targets’ targets’ personalities in the online questionnaire. The targets’
judgments of the behavior of the previous targets, and (d) the average evaluation was r = .43 (range = −.62 to .88) for their
informants’ judgments of the behavior of the previous tar- own behavior in the lab and r = .45 (range = −.36 to .89) for
gets. Thus, we compared the accuracy of perceivers who other targets’ behavior. The informants’ average evaluation
judged targets they already knew (a, b) with the accuracy of was r = .54 (range = −.48 to .93) for their “own” targets’
perceivers who judged targets they did not know (c, d). In all behavior in the lab and r = .40 (range = −.41 to .88) for other
four cases, accuracy was defined as the agreement between targets’ behavior. Finally, the average evaluation of the tar-
the respective kind of judgment (a, b, c, d) and the average gets’ behavior in the lab by a single neutral observer was r =
observer-judgment of the same behavior. If acquaintance .41 (range = −.31 to .77).

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


160 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(2)

We first compared the evaluation coefficients for the five informant-ratings of a person’s behavior are at least partly
different kinds of judgments of the targets’ behavior in the accurate or valid.
lab with one another, using one-way repeated measures Apart from that, however, the present article also demon-
ANOVA. The comparison was statistically significant, F(4, strates that people’s judgments of the behavior of targets they
147) = 12.274, p < .001. Pairwise comparisons with know are affected by two different kinds of bias: evaluative
Bonferroni corrections for multiple testing (α = .01) sug- and content-specific preexisting views of a person. The for-
gested that the informants’ judgments of their own targets’ mer bias closely resembles the effect of partisanship on judg-
behavior were significantly more positive than all other ments of candidates’ performances in presidential debates
behavior judgments but that none of the other behavior judg- (cf. Munro et al., 2002). However, the findings of the present
ments differed from each other in terms of global evaluation. study pertain more directly to everyday interpersonal rela-
In a second analysis, we compared the evaluation coeffi- tionships: Preexisting positive or negative attitudes do not
cients for the targets’ versus the informants’ judgments only affect people’s judgments of the debate performances of
(Factor 1) and for questionnaire versus behavior ratings presidential candidates they (dis-)like, they also affect peo-
(Factor 2) with one another, using two-way repeated mea- ple’s judgments of their own behavior, and of the behavior of
sures ANOVA. Note that in this analysis, we only used judg- their close acquaintances, across diverse situations.
ments within the same dyads, that is, judgments of known Notably, the strength of the PEV-GE bias is strongly mod-
targets. The analysis yielded significant effects for both fac- erated by the evaluative connotation of the item that is used
tors, target-informant, F(1, 146) = 29.009, p < .001; behav- to describe the behavior. The more evaluative the item is, the
ior-personality, F(1, 146) = 94.784, p < .001; but no larger the (positive or negative) impact of the perceiver’s
significant interaction effect, F(1, 146) = 0.215, p = .643. global evaluation of the target will be (cf. Leising &
Hence, informants viewed targets more positively than tar- Borkenau, 2011). The present study is already the third to
gets viewed themselves, and for both targets and observers, empirically corroborate this interaction between perceivers’
ratings of overall personality were more positive than ratings global evaluations of targets (sometimes assessed as “lik-
of behavior in the lab. ing”) and the evaluativeness of person descriptors (cf.
Leising et al., 2010; Leising et al., 2013). In fact, the correla-
tions that we found between the Betas for PEV-GE and the
Discussion item desirability ratings were so high (r > .86) that it may be
reasonable to interpret the latter as directly reflecting the
Truth and Bias in Judgments of Behavior
extent to which items will respond to a perceiver’s more pos-
The current study aimed to investigate the relative extents to itive or negative attitude toward a target. This renders it
which self- and informant-ratings of targets’ behavior in spe- likely that controlling for item desirability may be an effec-
cific situations reflect “the truth,” as opposed to two different tive way of addressing socially (un-) desirable responding
kinds of bias (i.e., PEV-GE and PEV-CS). Taken together, we (cf. Paulhus, 2002), that is, individual differences between
found that the truth and both bias variables had significant and perceivers in terms of their judging (all or particular) targets
independent effects on self- and informant-ratings of behavior. too positively or too negatively. Future research needs to
To some extent, such ratings reflect (a) the views that external address this highly intriguing possibility.
observers would have of the same behaviors (truth), (b) the The other kind of bias that independently affects behavior
generally positive or negative views that the perceivers had of judgments is content-specific: If a perceiver has come to
the targets before observing their behaviors (PEV-GE), and (c) generally attribute some level of a particular characteristic to
the views that the perceivers had of the targets before observ- a target person, then his or her judgments of that target’s sub-
ing their behaviors in terms of specific judgment dimensions sequent behavior in specific situations will partly reflect this
(e.g., “restrained”; PEV-CS). The “forces” of the truth and general view, even when controlling for the target’s “actual”
PEV-CS variables were about equally strong and stronger than behavior in the situation. This finding clearly stands in line
the force of the PEV-GE variable. with the previous research (cf. Leising, 2011; Sadler &
With regard to truth, we found that self- and informant- Woody, 2003). Going beyond previous findings, however,
ratings of behavior moderately agreed with average judg- the present study demonstrates that this bias effect still holds
ments of the same behavior by three or more observers. This if one controls for general evaluation. It is also the first study
largely confirms the findings of previous studies (Leising, to show that informant-ratings of behavior are just as suscep-
2011; Sadler & Woody, 2003) and implies that momentary tible to this bias as are self-ratings. Moreover, our study dem-
self- and informant-ratings of behavior do in fact concur to onstrates that a substantial proportion of the effect of PEV-CS
some extent with how the same behavior would be inter- is target-specific and cannot merely be explained in terms of
preted by persons who see the target person for the first time. perceivers’ general judgment tendencies (cf. Kenny, 1994).
If one accepts averaged behavior ratings by several neutral Finally, the present study demonstrates that the effect occurs
observers as a valid measure of the behavior’s “true” mean- even if the interval between the assessment of the perceiver’s
ing, then this finding supports the notion that self- and general image of the target and same perceiver’s judgment of

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


Leising et al. 161

the same target’s behavior in a particular situation is several that the informants’ judgments of “their” targets’ behavior in
days long (about 4 days on average). Therefore, it appears the lab were more positive than any other judgments of that
highly unlikely that the effect is merely rooted in the research- behavior. This clearly supports the notion of a “pal-serving
ers’ making the perceivers’ general images of the targets bias” (Leising et al., 2010), that is, partisanship in judgments
salient shortly before the behavioral assessment takes place. of the behavior of others we know and like. It should be
An explanation in terms of motivational or informational noted that this finding concerns a main effect (people in gen-
mechanisms (see above) seems more viable. Future research eral are partisan to their friends), whereas the above-
should compare the validities of such theoretical explana- described effects of PEV-GE concern individual differences
tions with one another. (people who generally see their targets more positively will
describe those targets’ subsequent behaviors more posi-
Loss of Validity Due to Acquaintance Between tively). It should also be noted that we did not find evidence
for a general self-serving bias in judgments of behavior. One
Target and Perceiver? possible explanation for this discrepancy may be that prais-
We also investigated whether judgments of the behavior of ing others is less “taboo” than praising oneself (cf. Gallrein,
known targets are less accurate than judgments of the behav- Carlson, Holstein, & Leising, 2013).
ior of unknown targets. Using averaged observer-ratings as Second, we found that judgments of personality were
an accuracy criterion, we found that the targets were in fact more positive than judgments of behavior in the lab. This
systematically less accurate in judging their own behavior as may be explained by our tasks eliciting behaviors that were
compared to how accurate they were in judging the behavior actually less “positive” than the behaviors that most people
of other people they did not know. However, it may not be tend to show in their everyday lives (the latter constituting
concluded that self-judgments of behavior are generally less the basis for personality judgments). Alternatively, it may be
accurate than are other-ratings because we did not find sig- the case that judgments of behavior from video are more
nificant differences in accuracy between self-ratings and data-driven than broad retrospective judgments of personal-
informant-ratings of behavior. Also, the informants were ity in a questionnaire. Because the latter kind of judgment is
about equally accurate in judging the behavior of known tar- more abstract, it may permit more target-serving, selective
gets and unknown targets. Therefore, the present study does recall of relevant behavioral episodes.
not suggest that prior acquaintance between perceivers and
targets is always detrimental to the perceivers’ accuracy in
Broader Implications and Outlook
judging the targets’ behavior. According to our view, the
finding that the targets were more accurate in judging some- Which implications do the present results have for behav-
one else than in judging themselves may be attributed to the ioral assessment in general? Most importantly, they suggest
operation of self-protective mechanisms in self-judgments that, apart from “truth,” momentary self- and other-judg-
(e.g., Tice, 1991) as opposed to the targets’ heightened atten- ments of behavior reflect a substantial proportion of system-
tion to someone else’s performance in the same situations atic bias variance. If a target’s behavior is judged by someone
that they had just undergone themselves. who is previously acquainted with the target, the resulting
It should be noted, however, that the targets and the infor- judgments will partly reflect the perceiver’s preexisting view
mants in the present study judged the targets’ behavior of the target, both in terms of general evaluation and in terms
directly after seeing them on video. This probably made it of the specific judgment dimension at hand. In other words,
relatively easy for them to be accurate because they received if we know how a perceiver generally views a target, we will
the exact same behavioral information on which the observ- be able to partly predict that perceiver’s judgments of that
ers also based their judgments. The fact that, even under such target’s subsequent behaviors, irrespective of how the target
optimal conditions for accuracy, we still found significant actually behaves. This might explain some of the stunning
effects for our bias variables (see above) renders these effects discrepancies that sometimes exist between different per-
even more impressive. We suspect that, under conditions ceivers’ views on the same targets’ behaviors (e.g., in our
where the interval between the observation and the judgment introductory real-life example).
of behavior is larger, the impact of the perceivers’ preexisting The bias effects that we found may become a problem
views of the targets on their judgments of the targets’ behav- especially when multiple momentary assessments of behav-
ior might become even stronger and possibly come to impair ior are averaged (as is often the case in experience sampling
accuracy. This issue needs to be addressed by future research. studies). For example, if we assume that people’s self-images
are relatively stable over time, they might be expected to
affect all momentary self-assessments of behavior in a simi-
Global Evaluation Across Perspectives lar manner. In contrast, the targets’ actual behaviors in the
We also investigated mean differences in how positively or different situations may contribute less to the average of
negatively the targets’ behaviors and personalities were momentary self-assessments, to the extent that they vary
described across the various assessments. First, we found over time. Taken to the extreme, it would be conceivable that

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


162 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 40(2)

an average of many momentary self-assessments may only more negative self-images (e.g., people with elevated levels
reflect the target’s overall self-images, but none of the tar- of depression). With such a sample, we would expect global
get’s actual behaviors in specific situations anymore, because evaluation to play an even more prominent role in predicting
the latter have averaged out. In such a case, the momentary the targets’ judgments of their own behaviors. When infor-
assessments would become superfluous because a single mant-ratings are used, a more realistic estimate of the global
overall self-assessment would do the job (of assessing what evaluation effect may be obtained by also recruiting infor-
people think of themselves) just as well. As the current study mants who have no particular loyalties toward, or who even
shows, using informant-instead of self-judgments of behav- dislike, their respective target persons. Such recruitment may
ior would not be a solution to this problem because infor- be challenging, but would clearly be worthwhile at the same
mant-judgments are just as prone to being biased by time, because the resulting samples of targets and perceivers
preexisting views as are self-judgments. The question of how would reflect the interpersonal world we inhabit (comprising
much incremental validity (e.g., in predicting important out- friends and foes, allies and competitors, love interests, loved
comes) aggregated momentary self- or informant-judgments ones and “exes”) more accurately.
of behavior actually have beyond the respective perceivers’
general images of the targets remains to be answered by Declaration of Conflicting Interests
future research. The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
The present study also shows that people tend to judge the to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
behavior of their friends, but not their own behavior, in
overly positive ways. It seems appropriate to use the term Funding
overly here because we could show that these judgments
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
were significantly more positive than judgments of the very for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
same behaviors, by means of the very same terms, by any- research was supported by a grant (LE 2151/4-1) to Daniel Leising
body else, including neutral observers. The finding is in line provided by the German Research Foundation (DFG).
with previous research showing that people tend to view
their close acquaintances more positively than they view References
themselves (Barelds & Dijkstra, 2009; Leising et al., 2010;
Barelds, D. P. H., & Dijkstra, P. (2009). Positive illusions about
McNulty, O’Mara, & Karney, 2008; Murray, 1999; Murray,
a partner’s physical attractiveness and relationship quality.
Holmes, & Griffin, 1996; Swami, Stieger, Haubner, Voracek, Personal Relationships, 16, 263-283. doi:10.1111/j.1475-
& Furnham, 2009) but—according to our knowledge—the 6811.2009.01222.x
present study is the first study to actually demonstrate that Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology
informant-ratings of behavior deviate positively from inde- as the science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever
pendent assessments of “the truth.” happened to actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological
This has several important implications for person assess- Science, 2, 396-403. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00051.x
ments in real life: For targets, systematically befriending Bleidorn, W. (2009). Linking personality states, current social roles
people who might be asked to judge their behavior later on and major life goals. European Journal of Personality, 23, 509-
(e.g., as members of the board during job applications or as 530. doi:10.1002/per.731
reviewers for scientific journals) is likely to pay off in terms Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Core self-evaluations: A review
of the trait and its role in job satisfaction and job performance.
of obtaining better evaluations. Institutions (e.g., employers,
European Journal of Personality, 17, 5-18. doi:10.1002/
scientific journals) that are interested in assessing targets’ per.481
performances objectively, however, should take care that no Borkenau, P., Mauer, N., Riemann, R., Spinath, F. M., & Angleitner,
particularly positive or negative personal relationships exist A. (2004). Thin slices of behavior as cues of personality and
between targets and judges because otherwise the resulting intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86,
judgments might reflect the influence of those relationships 599-614. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.4.599
quite strongly (e.g., the average difference between the infor- Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1998). The Big Five as states: How
mants’ global evaluations of the behavior of known vs. useful is the five-factor model to describe intraindividual varia-
unknown targets was d = .51 in the present study). tions over time? Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 202-
An important limitation of our study is that all informants 221. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1997.2206
were relatively fond of the targets. As a consequence, the Edwards, A. L. (1953). The relationship between the judged
desirability of a trait and the probability that the trait will be
influence of PEV-GE was probably underestimated due to
endorsed. Journal of Applied Psychology, 37, 69-106.
variance restriction (Leising et al., 2010; Peabody & Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance (Vol. 2).
Goldberg, 1989). It seems likely that the force of this vari- Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
able would become stronger when including perceivers who Fleeson, W. (2001). Towards a structure- and process-integrated
differ more in how favorable or critical an image they have view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states.
of their respective targets. With regard to self-ratings, this Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011-1027.
may be achieved by including, for example, participants with doi:10.1037//0022-3514.80.6.1011

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014


Leising et al. 163

Furr, R. M., & Funder, D. C. (1998). A multimodal analysis of per- Murray, S. L. (1999). The quest for conviction: Motivated cogni-
sonal negativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, tion in romantic relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 23-
74, 1580-1591. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1580 34. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1001_3
Gallrein, A. M. B., Carlson, E. N., Holstein, M., & Leising, D. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The ben-
(2013). You spy with your little eye—People are “blind” to efits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of
some of the ways in which they are consensually seen by satisfaction in close relationships. Journal of Personality and
others. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 464-471. Social Psychology, 70, 79-98. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.79
doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.04.001 Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenom-
Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations enon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175-
analysis. New York, NY: Guilford. 220. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
Kenny, D. A. (2004). PERSON: A general model of interpersonal Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can
perception. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 265- know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological
280. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_3 Review, 84, 231-259. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231
Kim, H., Schimmack, U., & Oishi, S. (2012). Cultural differ- Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: The evolu-
ences in self- and other-evaluations and well-being: A study tion of a construct. In H. Braun, D. N. Jackson, & D. E. Wiley
of European and Asian Canadians. Journal of Personality and (Eds.), The role of constructs in psychological and educational
Social Psychology, 102, 856-873. doi:10.1037/a0026803 measurement (pp. 67-88). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Peabody, D., & Goldberg, L. R. (1989). Some determinants of fac-
Bulletin, 108, 480-498. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480 tor structures from personality-trait descriptors. Journal of
Leising, D. (2011). The consistency bias in judgments of one’s Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 552-567. doi:10.1037/
own interpersonal behavior. Two possible sources. Journal of 0022-3514.57.3.552
Individual Differences, 32, 137-143. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/ Sadler, P., & Woody, E. (2003). Is who you are who you’re talk-
a000046 ing to? Interpersonal style and complementarily in mixed-sex
Leising, D., & Bleidorn, W. (2011). Which are the basic meaning interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,
dimensions of observable interpersonal behavior? Personality 80-96. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.80
and Individual Differences, 51, 986-990. doi.org/10.1016 Saucier, G. (1994). Separating description and evaluation in the
/j.paid.2011.08.003 structure of personality attributes. Journal of Personality
Leising, D., & Borkenau, P. (2011). Person perception, disposi- and Social Psychology, 66, 141-154. doi:10.1037/0022-
tional inferences and social judgment. In L. M. Horowitz & S. 3514.66.1.141
Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, Snyder, M., & Campbell, B. (1980). Testing hypotheses about
research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (pp. 157- other people: The role of the hypothesis. Personality and
170). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 421-426. doi:10.1177/
Leising, D., Erbs, J., & Fritz, U. (2010). The letter of recommen- 014616728063015
dation effect in informant ratings of personality. Journal of Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in
Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 668-682. doi:10.1037/ social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
a0018771 36, 1202-1212. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.11.1202
Leising, D., & Müller-Plath, G. (2009). Person–situation integra- Srivastava, S., Guglielmo, S., & Beer, J. S. (2010). Perceiving oth-
tion in research on personality problems. Journal of Research ers’ personalities: Examining the dimensionality, assumed sim-
in Personality, 43, 218-227. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.017 ilarity to the self, and stability of perceiver effects. Journal of
Leising, D., Ostrovski, O., & Zimmermann, J. (2013). Are we Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 520-534. doi:10.1037/
talking about the same person here? Inter-rater agreement in a0017057
judgments of personality varies dramatically with how much Swami, V., Stieger, S., Haubner, T., Voracek, M., & Furnham, A.
the perceivers like the targets. Social Psychological and (2009). Evaluating the physical attractiveness of oneself and
Personality Science, 4, 468-474. one’s romantic partner: Individual and relationship correlates
Lord, C. G., Lepper, M. R., & Preston, E. (1984). Considering of the love-is-blind bias. Journal of Individual Differences, 30,
the opposite: A corrective strategy for social judgment. 35-43. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.30.1.35
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1231-1243. Tice, D. M. (1991). Esteem protection or enhancement? Self-
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1231 handicapping motives and attributions differ by trait self-esteem.
McNulty, J. K., O’Mara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 711-725.
Benevolent cognitions as a strategy of relationship mainte- Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.60.5.711
nance: “Don’t sweat the small stuff.” But it is not all small West, T. V., & Kenny, D. A. (2011). The truth and bias model of
stuff. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 631- judgment. Psychological Review, 118, 357-378. doi:10.1037/
646. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.631 a0022936
Munro, G. D., Ditto, P. H., Lockhart, L. K., Fagerlin, A., Gready, Wood, D., Harms, P., & Vazire, S. (2010). Perceiver effects as pro-
M., & Peterson, E. (2002). Biased assimilation of sociopolitical jective tests: What your perceptions of others say about you.
argument: Evaluating the 1996 U.S. Presidential Debate. Basic Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 99, 174-190.
and Applied Psychology, 24, 15-26. doi:10.1037/a0019390

Downloaded from psp.sagepub.com at Universitaetsbibliothek on January 14, 2014

View publication stats

You might also like