You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Youth and Adolescence

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01285-z

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Classroom Status Hierarchy Moderates the Association between


Social Dominance Goals and Bullying Behavior in Middle Childhood
and Early Adolescence
Bin Pan1 Liang Zhang

1 ●
Linqin Ji1 Claire F. Garandeau2 Christina Salmivalli1,2 Wenxin Zhang1
● ● ●

Received: 10 April 2020 / Accepted: 1 July 2020


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
Social dominance goals represent desires to be powerful and prominent among peers. Previous studies have documented that
endorsing social dominance goals is positively associated with bullying behavior. However, little is known about how
classroom context moderates the social dominance goals–bullying association. The present study examined the role of
classroom status hierarchy in the longitudinal association between social dominance goals and bullying in a sample of 1,603
1234567890();,:

children attending 17 grade 3 classrooms (n = 558, 46.2% girls, Mage = 9.33 years, SD = 0.44), 15 grade 4 classrooms (n =
1234567890();,:

491, 45.0% girls, Mage = 10.31 years, SD = 0.38) and 16 grade 7 classrooms (n = 554, 49.3% girls, Mage = 13.2 years, SD =
0.46) in China, followed for 1 year. Classroom peer status hierarchy was assessed by the within-classroom standard
deviation in perceived popularity. Social dominance goals were obtained through self-reports. Bullying was measured via
peer nomination. The multilevel models revealed that social dominance goals at Wave 1 predicted increases in bullying at
Wave 2 only in classrooms with higher status hierarchies, after controlling for gender, grade, classroom size, and classroom
gender distribution. These findings indicate that children who strive for social dominance goals are more likely to bully
others when power is less equally distributed in the classroom.
Keywords Classroom status hierarchy Social dominance goals Bullying Multilevel analysis
● ● ●

Introduction dominance goals and bullying, defined as repeated aggres-


sion against a more vulnerable peer (Olweus 2013).
From an evolutionary perspective (Pellegrini 2002; Salmi- Classroom status hierarchy is a possible contextual factor
valli and Peets 2008), most bullying behaviors are goal- that moderates this association (Garandeau et al. 2011). A
oriented and motivated by a desire for social dominance, clear status hierarchy, which reflects strong classroom
that is a desire for high status, influence, and visibility inequalities in social status, might promote bullying beha-
among peers. There is indeed evidence that those who vior among social dominance-oriented children by making
endorse social dominance goals are more likely to engage in bullying more socially rewarding and mitigating risks of
bullying behavior (Caravita and Cillessen 2012; Olthof physical hurt and social costs (Reijntjes et al. 2013). Using a
et al. 2011; Sijtsema et al. 2009). As goals and goal- 1-year longitudinal design, the present study examines the
concordant behaviors could be activated and aroused by moderating role of classroom status hierarchy in the pro-
social contexts (Lindenberg 2013), it is critical to under- spective association between social dominance goals and
stand the role of context in the association between social bullying behavior with a large Chinese sample.

Social Dominance Goals and Bullying


* Liang Zhang
zhangliang1@sdnu.edu.cn
Social dominance goals refer to desires to be powerful and
prominent among peers (Jarvinen and Nicholls 1996; Kiefer
1
Department of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, and Ryan 2008). An evolutionary approach has identified
Jinan, China bullying behavior as a strategy to establish and maintain social
2
Department of Psychology, University of Turku, Turku, Finland dominance within the group (Hawley 1999; Volk et al. 2012)
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

and empirical evidence shows that this strategy can be effec- A longitudinal study found that a high level of status hier-
tive (e.g., Reijntjes et al. 2013). By picking on more vulnerable archy predicted increases in bullying 6 months later in a
peers, bullies can induce obedience and feelings of fear in their sample of 11,296 adolescents from 583 classes in Finland
peers (Volk et al. 2012), and further consolidate their dominant (Garandeau et al. 2014).
position in the peer group (Pellegrini and Long 2002). Highly hierarchical contexts may encourage children
Therefore, children who are oriented to social dominance are who strive for social dominance to engage in bullying,
inclined to display bullying. because such contexts might make it more likely that bul-
The positive association between dominance-like goals and lying behaviors are rewarded with social benefits in the
bullying among children and adolescents has been reported by form of high popularity. Consistent with this proposition,
a substantial body of studies. For example, a cross-sectional several cross-sectional studies have found that in middle
study reported that the endorsement of agentic goals (i.e., aims childhood the association between aggression and popu-
toward power, mastery, and status) was positively associated larity was stronger in hierarchical classrooms where a small
with bullying in middle childhood and early adolescence, number of students play a prominent role in interpersonal
especially for children who were perceived as popular in the connections (Ahn et al. 2010), and where levels of per-
classroom (Caravita and Cillessen 2012). Moreover, using a ceived popularity vary considerably across students (Gar-
dyadic approach, a study found that adolescents’ probability andeau et al. 2011, but see Zwaan et al. 2013). Similarly, a
of being a bully in bully-victim dyads was related to a high recent longitudinal study revealed that higher levels of
level of agentic goals (Sijtsema et al. 2009). Furthermore, classroom status hierarchy led to higher aggression–popu-
another study found that bullies reported more desire to gain larity norms in the classroom (i.e., stronger positive within-
power, dominance, and prestige than their peers in early classroom association between bullying and popularity)
adolescence (Olthof et al. 2011). during adolescence (Laninga-Wijnen et al. 2019). Further-
more, dominance positions are scarce and valuable in highly
Classroom Status Hierarchy and the Social hierarchical classrooms (Garandeau et al. 2014). The pop-
Dominance Goals–Bullying Association ular children in such classrooms possess more power and
visibility than in the classrooms where social status is more
Social dominance goals alone are likely insufficient to equally distributed. The salient reputational rewards
explain bullying behavior. According to the goal-framing attached to bullying in such social environments should
theory (Lindenberg 2013), individuals become more sensi- increase the accessibility of social dominance goals, thereby
tive to the information about opportunities to realize their facilitating and reinforcing the bullying behavior of children
goals when their specific goals are activated. Children who who desire dominance (Custers and Aarts 2010).
view social dominance as their major goal will pay special In addition to bringing social benefits to bullying chil-
attention to the potential benefits and costs of bullying dren, hierarchical classrooms might also reduce the costs of
behavior in the social contexts in which they find themselves bullying. The potential costs of bullying, such as physical
(Pouwels et al. 2019), as such behavior is their customary harm and loss of social approval, might often inhibit the
means of attaining dominance. If they detect the cues that motivation to bully others (Veenstra et al. 2007). However,
bullying may facilitate the realization of their goals, at victimized children are more likely to be unpopular and
minimal costs for themselves, they are more likely to engage rejected in highly hierarchical classrooms (Ahn et al. 2010).
in bullying (Veenstra et al. 2007). Thus, it is important to Due to their low status, victims are less likely to resist or
investigate how contextual factors influence the association receive protection from other peers when being bullied
between social dominance goals and bullying behavior. (Huitsing et al. 2014). Therefore, children who endorse
Social status hierarchy is a pervasive and fundamental social dominance goals may pick on these easy targets to
feature of social organization in human groups (Halevy gain dominance at a low cost (Sijtsema et al. 2009).
et al. 2011). In classroom settings, status hierarchy has been Taken together, the above findings suggest that class-
generally operationalized as the standard deviation in per- room status hierarchy may relate to higher benefits and
ceived popularity among the students in a classroom (Gar- lower costs for bullying behaviors. In classrooms of higher
andeau et al. 2011; Zwaan et al. 2013). As perceived status hierarchy, dominance-hungry children may be more
popularity reflects power, dominance, and visibility among motivated to engage in bullying behavior due to the clear
peers (Cillessen and Marks 2011), classroom status hier- social rewards of bullying and the victims’ higher vulner-
archy represents the distribution of power and dominance in ability. However, to date, no study has investigated the role
the classroom. In classrooms with high levels of status of classroom status hierarchy in the association between
hierarchy, only few students are perceived as “popular” and social dominance goals and bullying. Therefore, the present
hold the power in the classroom, while in low-hierarchy study tests whether it has a moderating effect on social
classrooms, children’s social status is relatively egalitarian. dominance goals–bullying associations.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Other Possible Moderators of Social Dominance across age groups were not significant, according to
Goals–Bullying Associations Fisher’s r-to-Z comparison (Caravita and Cillessen 2012).
To compare the social dominance goals–bullying asso-
Individual- and classroom-level demographic factors are ciation in middle childhood and in early adolescence,
suspected to be predictive of the association between social potential grade differences were examined using third-,
dominance goals and bullying behavior (e.g., Caravita and fourth- and seventh-graders.
Cillessen 2012). Therefore, the current study examined the
moderating effects of gender, grade, classroom size, and Classroom size
classroom gender distribution.
Classroom size, referring to the number of students in a
Gender classroom, is related to classroom bullying rates. There is
often a higher prevalence of bullying in smaller classrooms
Gender is an important factor to consider when examining (e.g., Garandeau et al. 2014, 2019). Besides, the link
the association between social dominance goals and bully- between bullying and popularity may be stronger in smaller
ing behavior (Rose and Rudolph 2006). Bullying, especially classrooms than in larger classrooms (Garandeau et al.
physical bullying, was more common in boys (Smith et al. 2019, the Austrian sample; but see Garandeau et al. 2019,
2019). In China, both physical and relational bullying were the Dutch sample). As bullying may bring more social
more prevalent among boys (Zhang et al. 2016), as col- rewards in smaller classrooms, children who strive for
lectivistic cultures may inhibit the use of physical aggres- social dominance might be more likely to bully others in
sion but facilitate relational aggression for both genders such contexts. Therefore, this study explored whether
(Chen et al. 2019). Moreover, bullying is more likely to be classroom size moderated social dominance goals–bullying
rewarded with status among boys than among girls (de association in the present study.
Bruyn et al. 2010; Caravita and Cillessen 2012). Therefore,
boys are more likely to regard bullying as an effective Classroom gender distribution
approach to obtain social dominance (Rose and Rudolph
2006). Compared to girls, boys who endorse social dom- Classroom gender distribution might be associated with class
inance goals are more likely to engage in physical aggres- norms related to bullying. Due to its higher normativeness
sion (Kiefer and Wang 2016; Ojanen et al. 2012) and less among boys (Caravita and Cillessen 2012; Smith et al. 2019),
likely to follow school rules (Kiefer and Ryan 2008). bullying is more likely to be reinforced, and less likely to be
Therefore, boys who endorse social dominance goals are punished by peers in classrooms with a higher proportion of
more likely than girls to engage in bullying. boys (Laninga-Wijnen et al. 2020). Partly supporting this
notion, a study found that a high ratio of boys in the class-
Grade room strengthened the popularity–aggression association
among boys (Zwaan et al. 2013). Given that the unbalanced
The association of social dominance goals with bullying classroom gender distribution is assumed to promote gender-
behavior may be different in middle childhood and in specific behavior (Kuppens et al. 2008), there was a reason to
early adolescence. Along with the developmental transi- expect that social dominance goals predict increases in bul-
tion from childhood to adolescence, children transit from lying especially in classrooms with a higher proportion
elementary schools to secondary schools. During the re- of boys.
establishment of dominance in a new, larger social system
(Pellegrini and Long 2002), both endorsement of social Cultural consideration
dominance goals and bullying behavior tend to increase
(LaFontana and Cillessen 2010). Furthermore, bullying is Chinese culture is characterized as vertical-collectivistic
increasingly reinforced by social rewards in peer inter- with emphasis on both interdependence and hierarchy
actions from middle childhood to early adolescence among peers (Schwartz et al. 2010; Triandis 1995). One the
(Caravita and Cillessen 2012; Cillessen and Mayeux one hand, due to the significance of interdependence, Chi-
2004; Garandeau et al. 2011). These findings led us to nese children can be more concerned with establishing
expect that children who strive more for social dominance harmonious peer relationships than acquiring social dom-
might be more likely to bully others in early adolescence inance in the classroom (Wright et al. 2014). Moreover,
than in middle childhood. However, a study found that the bullying behavior is highly discouraged in Chinese culture,
correlation coefficients between agentic goals and bully- because it may threaten group cohesion (Chen et al. 2019).
ing in middle childhood and early adolescence were 0.06 Accordingly, bullies are more likely to be unpopular and be
and 0.09, respectively, which indicated that differences rejected by peers (Ji et al. 2016). One the other hand,
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Chinese classrooms are hierarchical in terms of status, as secondary school grades) in Jinan and Tai’an, PR China.
teachers formally appoint specific children to leadership Nearly all of the participants were Han Chinese ethnicity
positions (Schwartz et al. 2010). Reflecting the vertical (the vast majority ethnic group in China, at 92% of total
feature in collectivism, each group member has a clear population) and native Mandarin speakers (the majority
position in the social hierarchy and is willing to sacrifice language in China, at 70% of total population). In the ori-
their own interests for the collective well-being (Chen et al. ginal sample, 89.9% of mothers and 91.3% of fathers had an
2018; Schwartz et al. 2010). As such, victimizing low-status educational attainment of senior high school degree or
students could be recognized as a legitimate means to higher. In China, students take almost all their lessons with
maintain social order and reinforce their roles in the group same classmates during an academic year, in both elemen-
(Schwartz et al. 2010), especially in the classroom with tary and secondary schools. The schedule of courses and
clear hierarchy. Therefore, the Chinese cultural context other activities is typically identical for all students in the
provides an ideal setting for testing the function of class- same class.
room status hierarchy. Data were collected in the 3rd month of the spring
semester (i.e., May). Prior to data collection, researchers
sent consent letters to students, parents, teachers and school
Current Study principals, in which the research aims and procedures were
described briefly. The children involved were offered a gift
Although the association between social dominance goals (about $1). Through these procedures, 94.5% of the chil-
and bullying is well established in the extant literature, dren contacted for the study both received parental per-
much remains unknown about how the classroom context mission to participate and gave their assent. The participants
affects the extent to which the pursuit of social dominance completed a battery of self-report measures regarding their
translates into bullying behavior (Kiefer and Ryan 2008). In social goals and peer nominations regarding bullying and
the current 1-year longitudinal study, a large sample of social status during a single class period in schools. There
more than 1600 Chinese students in middle childhood were also other measures not utilized in the present study.
(grade 3 and grade 4) and early adolescence (grade 7) from To ensure confidentiality and anonymity, everyone also
41 classrooms was recruited to examine how classroom received a list of their classmates with their corresponding
hierarchy moderated the associations between social dom- three-digit numbers. These numbers were used to respond to
inance goals and bullying behavior. The central hypothesis the peer nomination questions, so no names were presented
of this study was that the association between social dom- in the questionnaires. Answering the questionnaires took
inance goals and bullying was stronger in classrooms with ~40 min. During the surveys, school teachers were not
higher levels of status hierarchy. In addition, gender, grade, present. The research design and procedure were reviewed
classroom size and classroom gender distribution are and approved by the Human Subjects Research Ethics
included as possible moderators of social dominance Committee in the department of psychology in the first
goals–bullying association. The hypotheses were as fol- author’s university.
lows: (1) boys and older children who desire social dom-
inance will be more likely to engage in bullying; (2) the Measures
association between social dominance goals and bullying
will be stronger in the classrooms with fewer students and Given that all the measures were originally developed in
higher proportion of boys. English, a standard translation and back-translation proce-
dure was used to ensure equivalence of all the measures
between the original English version and the Chinese
Method translation. This consisted of first translating the scale from
English into Chinese and then from Chinese back into
Participants and Procedures English and finally evaluating the level of agreement
between the original and back-translated English versions.
At Wave 1, participants were 1,603 children from in grade 3 Two bilingual (English and Chinese) professors in devel-
(n = 558, 46.2% girls, Mage = 9.33 years, SD = 0.44), grade opmental psychology and a professional translator carried
4 (n = 491, 45.0% girls, Mage = 10.31 years, SD = 0.38) out the process.
and grade 7 (n = 554, 49.3% girls, Mage = 13.2 years, SD =
0.46), recruited from 41 classrooms (including 17 grade 3 Demographic variables
classrooms, 15 grade 4 classrooms and 16 grade 7 class-
rooms) in 1 elementary school, 1 secondary school and Demographic characteristics, including gender, grade and
3 combined schools (comprising both elementary and class, were reported by the children themselves. Gender was
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

dummy-coded as 0 = girl and 1 = boy, and grade was Classroom status hierarchy
dummy-coded as 0 = grade 3 and grade 4, and 1 = grade 7.
The level of classroom status hierarchy was measured by
Bullying the standard deviation of individual perceived popularity
scores within a classroom (Garandeau et al. 2011; Zwaan
Bullying was assessed with the bullying subscale from the et al. 2013). For perceived popularity, participants received
Participant Role Questionnaire (Salmivalli and Voeten a roster of all consenting students in their classroom and
2004). To begin with, the research assistants gave the were asked to indicate “Who is the most popular one in
participants a definition of bullying, which highlighted its your classroom”. Following previous studies (Li et al. 2012;
intentionality, repetition and the power imbalance. Then, Tseng et al. 2013), the Chinese term “shou huan ying”
children were asked to nominate up to three classmates (受欢迎) was used to represent popularity in this study.
who fit the description in three items about bullying: “Starts Because this Chinese term may not have the exact same
bullying”, “Makes the others join in the bullying”, and meaning as the English term (Niu et al. 2016), the research
“Always finds new ways of harassing the victim” (Salmi- assistants further explained to participants that the popular
valli and Voeten 2004). Given that Chinese students take students refer to the “super stars” in their classrooms.
almost all their lessons only with the students from their Through this approach, it is expected that participants
own classroom, nominations were limited to classmates nominated the classmates with the highest visibility among
only. For each item, a proportion score was calculated for peers. Perceived popularity was indicated by a proportion
each child by dividing the number of raw nominations by score, which was created by dividing the total number of
the number of nominators within each classroom nominations a student received by the number of nomina-
(Garandeau et al. 2014). Scores for each item ranged from tors within each classroom. The standard deviation of the
0 (no nominations) to 1 (nominated by all classmates). The proportion score of popularity was computed within each
three items’ scores were averaged to calculate bullying classroom to reflect classroom status hierarchy. A large
scores, with a higher score indicating more bullying standard deviation of perceived popularity within the
behavior. The Cronbach’s α for peer-reported bullying was classroom indicates a higher degree of classroom status
0.97 at both waves. hierarchy (Garandeau et al. 2011).

Social dominance goals Data Analyses

Social dominance goals were assessed via three items from To test the hypotheses, several multilevel models were
the Social Goals Questionnaire (Jarvinen and Nicholls employed. Bullying at Wave 2 served as the outcome
1996; Kiefer and Ryan 2008). One item from the original variable in the different models. Individual-level predictors
scales was excluded as it lowered the reliability of the scale included Wave 1 bullying, gender and social dominance
in this study (i.e., “When I am with people my own age, I goals. Individual-level continuous variables were centered
like it when I make them do what I want”). For each item, at the classroom mean. Gender was weighted-effects-coded
participants were asked to respond to items on a five-point as boy = 0.48 and girl = −0.52, to ensure that the values for
scale (e.g., “When I’m with people my own age I like it gender added up to 0 (te Grotenhuis et al. 2017) for all
when they worry that I’ll hurt them”; 1 = not at all true of participants. To explore gender differences in the social
me, 5 = really true of me). Scores for subscale items were dominance goals–bullying association, the gender × social
averaged to create the social dominance goals measure, with dominance goals interaction was included as an individual-
a higher score indicating higher endorsement of social level factor. Classroom-level predictors, including grade,
dominance goals. The Cronbach’s α for social dominance size, proportion of boys and status hierarchy, were centered
goals at Wave 1 was 0.70. at the grand mean.
Data analyses included three stages. First, an uncondi-
Classroom size tional model was conducted without any individual- or
classroom-level predictors to obtain intraclass correlation
Classroom size was the number of students in a classroom. (ICC, the proportion of total variance that is between
classrooms) of bullying behavior. Second, individual-level
Classroom gender distribution predictors were added into the unconditional model to test
the prospective association between social dominance goals
Classroom gender distribution was indicated by class pro- and bullying and its gender differences. Third, to test
portion of boys, i.e., dividing the number of boys by the whether the association between social dominance goals
number of students in a classroom. and bullying varied between classrooms as function of
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Table 1 Descriptive statistics


M SD Min. Max. 1 2 3
and correlations of individual
and classroom-level variables Individual-level variables
1 Social dominance goals 1.23 0.55 1.00 5.00 –
2 Wave1 bullying 0.04 0.08 0.00 0.79 0.14*** –
3 Wave2 bullying 0.04 0.08 0.00 0.76 0.15*** 0.79*** –
Classroom-level variables
1 Grade – – 0.00 1.00 –
2 Class size 38.17 10.16 18.00 54.00 0.26 –
3 Classroom proportion of boys 0.53 0.05 0.41 0.64 −0.21 −0.07 –
4 Classroom status hierarchy 0.13 0.05 0.04 0.23 −0.29 −0.46** −0.36*
Grade 3, Grade 4 = 0, Grade 7 = 1
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

grade, class size, classroom proportion of boys and class- goals (t[1573] = 0.99, p = 0.322, d = 0.05). Bullying was
room status hierarchy, classroom-level predictors and cross- highly stable between the two waves (r = 0.79, p < 0.001),
level interactions were added to the models. All Models and social dominance goals was positively correlated
were estimated using Mplus 7.0 (Muthén and Muthén with bullying at Wave 1 (r = 0.28, p < 0.001) and Wave 2
2012). (r = 0.27, p < 0.001). Among classroom-level variables,
classroom status hierarchy was negatively associated with
classroom size (r = −0.46, p = 0.002) and classroom pro-
Results portion of boys (r = −0.36, p = 0.020).

Missing Data Multilevel Models

At the second wave, a total of 125 children (7.8%) dropped The unconditional model
out because of hectic schedules and transferring to a new
school. The percentages of missing data for social dominance The ICC of Wave 2 bullying was 0.008, indicating that only
goals, bullying at Wave 1 and bullying at Wave 2 were 0.0%, 0.8% of the variance in Wave 2 bullying was due to dif-
1.7% and 7.8%, respectively. Little’s Missing Completely at ferences between classrooms. Although the between-
Random test (MCAR; Little 2013) revealed that missing data classroom variability was small, multi level models were
were missing completely at random, χ2(5) = 2.17, p = 0.825. used due to the hierarchical nature of the data (Julian 2001).
For children with and without missing data, there were no Besides, the aim of the present study was not to explain the
significant differences in terms of gender (χ2[1] = 2.60, class-level variance of bullying, but the class-level variance
p = 0.107), age (t[139.108] = 1.835, p = 0.068, d = 0.19), of the social dominance goal–bullying association.
social dominance goals (t[134.01] = 1.31, p = 0.192, d =
0.16), bullying at Wave1 (t[1601] = 0.68, p = 0.496, d = The individual-level model
0.06). Missing data were handled using the full information
maximum likelihood (FIML) method with the MLR estima- First, a random intercept model was tested. In this model, the
tion in Mplus. In order to promote the precision of FIML, effects of individual-level predictors were fixed across
parental education levels were included in the analyses as classrooms. As shown in Table 2, after controlling for prior
auxiliary variables. The FIML method can yield unbiased bullying behavior, being a boy (b = 0.027, SE = 0.041,
estimations of coefficients with auxiliary variables at present p = 0.003) and having social dominance goals (b = 0.014,
(Little 2013). SE = 0.006, p = 0.013) were positively associated with
increases in bullying. Furthermore, the results showed that
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations gender moderated the longitudinal association between
social dominance goals and bullying (b = 0.016, SE = 0.006,
The descriptive statistics and correlations are presented in p = 0.005). As shown in Fig. 1, the association between
Table 1. At the individual level, boys scored higher than girls social dominance goals and bullying behavior was stronger
in bullying at Wave 1 (t[1601] = 11.46, p < 0.001, d = 0.57) for boys than for girls (for boys: b = 0.021, SE = 0.008,
and Wave 2 (t[1476] = 11.34, p < 0.001, d = 0.58). How- p = 0.009; for girls: b = 0.005, SE = 0.003, p = 0.068).
ever, there was no gender difference in social dominance Turning to random effects model, the paths from social
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Table 2 Effects of individual- and classroom-level predictors at Wave


1 on bullying behavior at Wave 2
Individual- Classroom-
level model level model
b SE b SE

Individual-level predictors
Wave1 bullying 0.759*** 0.041 0.757*** 0.041
Gender 0.027** 0.009 0.025** 0.009
Social dominance goals 0.014* 0.006 0.014* 0.006
Social dominance 0.016** 0.006 0.015* 0.006
goals × gender
Classroom-level predictors
Grade −0.008† 0.004
Class size −0.001*** 0.000
Classroom proportion 0.022 0.038 Fig. 1 The moderating role of gender in the association between social
of boys dominance goals and bullying
Classroom status 0.093† 0.054
hierarchy
Cross-level interactions
SE = 0.0002, p < 0.001). Grade (b = −0.008, SE = 0.004,
p = 0.076) and classroom status hierarchy (b = 0.093, SE =
Social dominance 0.008 0.006
goals × grade 0.054, p = 0.083) were marginally significant predictors of
Social dominance 0.000 0.000 bullying at Wave 2, suggesting that bullying may be more
goals × class size prevalent in middle childhood (i.e., lower grades) and in
Social dominance 0.210** 0.081 classrooms with a stronger status hierarchy. Moreover,
goals × classroom consistent with our hypotheses, significant cross-level
proportion of boys interactions indicated that the longitudinal association
Social dominance 0.275*** 0.069 between social dominance goals and bullying was moder-
goals × classroom status
hierarchy ated by classroom proportion of boys (b = 0.210, SE =
Random effect
0.081, p = 0.010) and classroom status hierarchy (b = 0.275,
SE = 0.069, p < 0.001). To clarify the nature of the sig-
σ2 (residual) 0.002*** 0.000 0.002*** 0.000a
nificant interactions of interest, post-hoc probing was con-
τ00 (intercept) a,
0.000 *** 0.000 a
0.000a,† 0.000a
ducted as recommended by Aiken and West (1991). Simple
τ01 (social dominance 0.000a
0.000a
goal slope) slope tests were employed to analyze the effect of social
dominance goals on self-reported bullying by comparing
Weighted-effects-codes of gender: Boys = 0.48 and Girls = −0.52.
simple slopes for low (−1 SD) and high (+1 SD) levels of
Grade 3, Grade 4 = 0, and Grade 7 = 1. Unstandardized coefficients
are presented. All individual-level continuous predictors were centered proportion of boys and classroom status hierarchy. As
at group means, while all classroom-level predictors were centered at shown in Fig. 2, social dominance goals had positive asso-
grand mean ciation with bullying in the classrooms with a high propor-
tion of boys (b = 0.025, SE = 0.006, p < 0.001), rather than
a
Mplus rounds numbers to three decimal places

p < 0.10; *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001 in those with a low proportion of boys (b = 0.003, SE =
0.008, p = 0.704). As shown in Fig. 3, children who
dominance goals to bullying were allowed to vary randomly endorsed social dominance goals were more likely to display
across classrooms. The results revealed that the effect of increased levels of bullying in high-hierarchy classrooms
social dominance goals on bullying varied significantly (b = 0.026, SE = 0.008, p < 0.001) but not in low-hierarchy
across classrooms (Var = 1.67 × 10−04, p = 0.033). Alto- classrooms (b = 0.001, SE = 0.005, p = 0.790). Altogether,
gether the individual-level predictors explained 65.5% of the classroom-level predictors explained 72.0% of the variance
variability in bullying behavior at Wave 2. in bullying between classrooms, and 64.3% of the variance
in the social dominance goals–bullying slope.
The classroom-level model
Supplementary Analyses
As shown in Table 2, classroom size was negatively asso-
ciated with bullying, indicating that bullying was more As a supplementary analysis, whether the moderating effect
prevalent in classrooms with fewer students (b = −0.001, of classroom status hierarchy varied across genders or
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

and gender was replicated in eight out of ten iterations; the


interaction between social dominance goals and classroom
gender distribution was replicated in seven out of ten
iterations; and the interaction between social dominance
goals and classroom status hierarchy was replicated in ten
out of ten iterations.

Discussion

Bullying behavior is widely considered to be driven by the


quest for high status and social dominance in the peer group
(Pellegrini 2002; Salmivalli and Peets 2008). However, it
remains unclear how contextual factors have an impact on
the link between children and adolescents’ endorsement of
social dominance goals and bullying behavior. In particular,
Fig. 2 The moderating role of classroom proportion of boys in the classroom status hierarchy, which reflects the social status
association between social dominance goals and bullying inequality in a classroom, may further motivate bullying
behaviors among those with strong social dominance goals
(Garandeau et al. 2011, 2014).To fill this gap, the present 1-
year longitudinal multilevel study examined whether
classroom status hierarchy moderates the prospective asso-
ciation between social dominance goals and bullying.
Moreover, the moderating effects of other individual and
classroom characteristics (e.g., gender, grade, classroom
size, gender distribution) were also tested.
Previous cross-sectional studies reported a positive con-
current association between social dominance goals and
bullying (Caravita and Cillessen 2012; Olthof et al. 2011;
Sijtsema et al. 2009). In line with previous studies, the
present study revealed that social dominance goals pre-
dicted increases in bullying over time after controlling the
stability of bullying. The current finding indicated that,
similar to their Western counterparts, Chinese children who
were oriented toward social dominance engaged in bullying,
although bullying is highly discouraged in Chinese culture.
Thus, this finding echoed the evolutionary perspective of
Fig. 3 The moderating role of classroom status hierarchy in the
association between social dominance goals and bullying
bullying, according to which, bullying can be viewed as a
functional behavior that serves to achieve a dominant status
in the group (Hawley 1999; Volk et al. 2012). Notably, the
grades was explored. Four interaction terms were added to present study found high stability of bullying over 1 year
the classroom-level model, including gender × classroom (r = 0.79), suggesting that the role of bully is highly stable
status hierarchy, grade × classroom status hierarchy, gen- across time. This stability coefficient is in accordance with
der × social dominance goals × classroom status hierarchy, previous findings (Garandeau et al. 2014; Reijntjes et al.
and grade × social dominance goals × classroom status 2013). For example, Reijntjes et al. (2013) found that the
hierarchy. The results revealed that the moderating role of stability coefficient of peer-reported bullying ranges
classroom status hierarchy was consistent across grades 0.74–0.81 across 1 year. Our analyses revealed significant
(b = 0.070, SE = 0.190, p = 0.715) and genders (b = 0.137, longitudinal effects of social dominance goals on bullying
SE = 0.235, p = 0.561). despite the high stability of bullying, which underscores the
In addition, to confirm the robustness of the present determining role of endorsing social dominance goals in the
findings, sensitivity analyses were employed with ten ran- development of bullying behavior.
domly selected pooled samples, each of which contained 30 Next, this study tested the moderating role of classroom
classes. The interaction between social dominance goals status hierarchy in the social dominance goals–bullying
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

association. Given the emphasis on status hierarchy in The moderating effect of gender and gender distribution
Chinese classrooms (Schwartz et al. 2010), it is important to may reflect that, although the Participant Role Ques-
evaluate the function of classroom status hierarchy under tionnaire does not distinguish between forms of bullying,
Chinese culture. As expected, these results showed that most participants might recognize bullying as an overt
children who endorsed social dominance goals were more behavior. As studies have shown higher popularity
likely to engage in bullying a year later in classrooms where rewards for relationally aggressive girls than relationally
power was less equally distributed. In contrast, there was no aggressive boys (Cillessen and Mayeux 2004), social
prospective association between social dominance goals and dominance goals may be more strongly related to rela-
bullying in more egalitarian classrooms. A possible expla- tional bullying among girls. However, studies have failed
nation could be generated from the goal-framing approach to find gender differences in the association between social
(Lindenberg 2013). Specifically, dominance-seeking chil- dominance goals and relational aggression (Kiefer and
dren are sensitive to, and their goal is aroused by clear Wang 2016; Ojanen et al. 2012).
social benefits and low costs related to bullying (Veenstra The results also revealed that the association between
et al. 2007). In classrooms with high social status hierarchy social dominance goals and bullying did not vary across
(i.e., hierarchical contexts), aggressive behavior is asso- grade level. This finding is consistent with previous stu-
ciated with higher popularity (Garandeau et al. 2011; dies (Caravita and Cillessen 2012), indicating that the
Laninga-Wijnen et al. 2019) and victims are more easily quest of social dominance equally predicts bullying in
dominated (Ahn et al. 2010). When spotting these social middle childhood and early adolescence. With regard to
cues in classrooms with a clear status hierarchy, children are classroom size, in line with previous studies (Garandeau
more likely to bully the weak to gain social dominance. In et al. 2014, 2019), the results showed that classroom size
summary, the current findings extend the literature on how was negatively related with bullying. However, classroom
individual characteristics interact with contextual char- size did not moderate the social dominance goals–bullying
acteristics in predicting bullying by providing evidence that association. The results might be related to the relatively
classroom status hierarchy influences the behavioral strate- large classroom size in the sample. Larger classrooms are
gies of dominance-aspiring children. typically made up of multiple peer groups, which could
These findings revealed how social status inequality in a allow victims to escape the control of ringleader bullies
classroom can harm students’ psychosocial adjustment, and their followers, thereby lowering the status-rewards
which echoes findings from research on the causal relations attached to bullying (Garandeau et al. 2019).
between economic inequality and population health (Pickett The present study has several strengths. First, whereas
and Wilkinson 2015). National income inequality intensifies previous studies have examined the concurrent association
competition and feelings of insecurity and weakens inter- between social dominance goals and bullying behavior,
personal relationships within societies by decreasing trust this study, guided by the goal-framing theory (Lindenberg
among people (Pickett and Wilkinson 2015), thereby 2013), focused on how classroom status hierarchy as well
increasing the prevalence of social-emotional problems, as other classroom context features moderate this asso-
including bullying behavior (Elgar et al. 2013; Melgar and ciation with a longitudinal design. Second, using a 1-year
Rossi 2012). The present findings corroborate those from longitudinal design, the present study controlled for prior
this branch of literature by highlighting the psychological bullying and provides a rigorous test of the effects of
cost of inequality in human groups, regardless of the type individual-level and classroom-level factors on subsequent
(e.g., economic or social status) or the group size (e.g., a bullying. Finally, this study investigated an understudied,
classroom or a nation). but very large, non-western culture and population, pro-
Finally, this study tested the role of demographic vari- viding a new vision of group dynamics in vertical-
ables in the association between social dominance goals collectivistic cultural contexts.
and bullying. Consistent with previous research (Kiefer The findings of this study inform anti-bullying pro-
and Ryan 2008; Kiefer and Wang 2016; Ojanen et al. grams in two ways. To begin with, children who desire
2012), the results revealed that boys with higher social social dominance are more likely to engage in bullying.
dominance goals tended to bully others more than girls For these children, it may be useful to teach them alter-
with similar goals. This gender difference might be due to native, prosocial ways to obtain status, power, and dom-
bullying being more normative (Smith et al. 2019) and inance. According to the meaningful roles approach (Ellis
more rewarded with social status among boys (Caravita et al. 2016), teachers could assign these children to high-
and Cillessen 2012). As expected, such gender-specific status jobs that require responsibility and altruism (e.g.,
norms could also be observed at the classroom level: the technology assistant) and reinforce their behaviors through
social dominance goals–bullying association was only peer-to-peer praise notes. However, more evidence of the
evident in classrooms with a higher proportion of boys. effectiveness of this approach is needed before its use can
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

be recommended, as there is a risk that dominance- cognitive mechanism, children who are oriented to social
oriented children may use their newly gained high status dominance may perceive bullying instead of other positive
(from the meaningful roles tasks) to engage in bullying. behaviors as an instrument to gain dominance in hierarchical
Moreover, randomized control trials are needed to evaluate classroom contexts (Li and Hu 2018).
this approach as a promising method to reduce children’s
bullying behavior. Second, the results suggest that mini-
mizing status discrepancies among children could diminish Conclusion
the likelihood that dominance-oriented children will resort
to bullying others. That is, in order to build a safer Bullying behavior is motivated by the pursuit of social
classroom environment, teachers should intervene in dominance in the peer group (Pellegrini 2002; Salmivalli
existing hierarchical relationships between children and and Peets 2008). Research has shown that social dominance
create more egalitarian classroom structures by mitigating goals are positively associated with bullying in middle
status extremes and supporting isolated students (Gar- childhood and early adolescence (Caravita and Cillessen
andeau et al. 2011, 2014; Gest and Rodkin 2011). More 2012; Olthof et al. 2011; Sijtsema et al. 2009). Yet, it is
importantly, considering the characteristics of Chinese important to note that this association may be influenced by
classrooms, teachers should not grant excessive power to the classroom context, as bullying may be related to dif-
specific students. ferent costs and benefits across classrooms. The current
Despite these strengths, it is important to consider the study examined the role classroom status hierarchy in the
methodological limitations of this study. First, although longitudinal association between social dominance goals
there was significant classroom-level variation in the and bullying in middle childhood and early adolescence.
association between social dominance goal and bullying, The findings indicated that youth striving for social dom-
the classroom-level variation of bullying was relatively inance goals were more likely to engage in bullying beha-
small, which limited the detection of classroom-level vior in more hierarchical classrooms. The current findings
predictors’ main effects on bullying. Second, a maximum have implications for future antibullying practices. Speci-
of three bullies were allowed to be nominated, which fically, to prevent dominance-oriented children from bul-
might underestimate students’ bulling behavior. However, lying others, teachers should make efforts to minimize
there is evidence that utilizing limited and unlimited status discrepancies and build egalitarian relationships
nominations of bullying lead to comparable results among classmates.
(Gommans and Cillessen 2015). Further research that
replicate our findings using unlimited nominations is Acknowledgements The authors would like to sincerely thank all
warranted. Finally, because bullying was operationalized participating students for their contributions, as well as research
as a unified construct, potential differences between overt assistants who have helped in designing the study and collecting
the data.
and relational forms of bullying could not be explicitly
tested. As relational bullying may be a more effective Authors’ Contributions B.P. conceived of the study and participated in
strategy in acquiring and maintaining social dominance in the interpretation of the data, performed the statistical analysis and
the peer group (Cillessen and Mayeux 2004; Ojanen and drafted the manuscript; L.Z. helped in performing the statistical ana-
Findley-Van Nostrand 2014), considering the distinctions lysis and the interpretation of the data, and drafted the manuscript; L.J.
participated in the research design; C.F.G. and C.S. helped in drafting
between relational and overt bullying would be an the manuscript and participated in the interpretation of the data; W.Z.
important direction for future research. conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, and
Future research may further expand on the current find- drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final
ings in two ways. First, it may focus on social goals other manuscript.
than social dominance goals and their association with bul-
Funding This study was supported by the Major Projects of Philo-
lying. For example, a lack of communal goals (i.e., striving sophy and Social Sciences Research, Ministry of Education
for positive relationships with others, Caravita and Cillessen (17JZD058), Science and Technology Plan for Young Scholars sup-
2012) might be considered as one of the reasons for bullying ported by Education Department of Shandong (2019RWF012), and the
(Caravita and Cillessen 2012). A recent study also revealed Academy of Finland Flagship Programme (320162).
that communal goals mitigated the positive association
Data Sharing and Declaration This manuscript’s data will not be
between agentic goals and direct aggression (Sijtsema et al. deposited.
2020). Future studies would benefit from considering mul-
tiple goals simultaneously. Furthermore, research could take Compliance with Ethical Standards
further steps in examining the process through which class-
room status hierarchy moderates the link between social Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
dominance goals and bullying. For example, as a possible interest.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving hierarchy, academic achievement, and grade. Developmental Psy-
human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of chology, 47(6), 1699–1710. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025271.
the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Garandeau, C. F., Lee, I. A., & Salmivalli, C. (2014). Inequality
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical matters: classroom status hierarchy and adolescents’ bullying.
standards. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(7), 1123–1133. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10964-013-0040-4.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual Garandeau, C. F., Yanagida, T., Vermande, M. M., Strohmeier, D., &
participants included in the study. Salmivalli, C. (2019). Classroom size and the prevalence of
bullying and victimization: testing three explanations for the
negative association. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2125 https://
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02125.
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Gest, S. D., & Rodkin, P. C. (2011). Teaching practices and ele-
mentary classroom peer ecologies. Journal of Applied Develop-
mental Psychology, 32(5), 288–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
appdev.2011.02.004.
References Gommans, R., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2015). Nominating under con-
straints: a systematic comparison of unlimited and limited peer
Ahn, H. J., Garandeau, C. F., & Rodkin, P. C. (2010). Effects of class- nomination methodologies in elementary school. International
room embeddedness and density on the social status of aggressive Journal of Behavioral Development, 39, 77–86. https://doi.org/
and victimized children. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 30(1), 10.1177/0165025414551761.
76–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431609350922. Halevy, N., Chou, E. Y., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). A functional
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: testing and model of hierarchy. Organizational Psychology Review, 1(1),
interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 32–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386610380991.
Caravita, S. C., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2012). Agentic or communal? Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: a
Associations between interpersonal goals, popularity, and bully- strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review,
ing in middle childhood and early adolescence. Social Develop- 19(1), 97–132. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1998.0470.
ment, 21(2), 376–395. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011. Huitsing, G., Snijders, T. A., Van Duijn, M. A., & Veenstra, R.
00632.x. (2014). Victims, bullies, and their defenders: a longitudinal
Chen, G., Kong, Y., Deater-Deckard, K., & Zhang, W. (2018). study of the coevolution of positive and negative networks.
Bullying victimization heightens cortisol response to psycho- Development and Psychopathology, 26(3), 645–659. https://doi.
social stress in Chinese children. Journal of Abnormal Child org/10.1017/S0954579414000297.
Psychology, 46(5), 1051–1059. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802- Jarvinen, D. W., & Nicholls, J. G. (1996). Adolescents’ social goals,
017-0366-6. beliefs about the causes of social success, and satisfaction in peer
Chen, L., Zhang, W., Ji, L., & Deater‐Deckard, K. (2019). Develop- relations. Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 435–441. https://doi.
mental trajectories of Chinese adolescents’ relational aggression: org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.3.435.
associations with changes in social‐psychological adjustment. Ji, L., Zhang, W., & Jones, K. (2016). Chinese children’s experience
Child Development, 90(6), 2153–2170. https://doi.org/10.1111/ of and attitudes towards bullying and victimization: A cross-
cdev.13090. cultural comparison between China and England. In P. Smith, K.
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Marks, P. (2011). Conceptualizing and mea- Kwak & Y. Toda. (Eds), School bullying in different cultures:
suring popularity. In A. Cillessen, D. Schwartz & L. Mayeux Eastern and western perspectives (pp. 170–188). Cambridge:
(Eds), Popularity in the peer system (pp. 25–56). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Guilford. Julian, M. W. (2001). The consequences of ignoring multilevel data
Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinfor- structures in nonhierarchical covariance modeling. Structural
cement: developmental changes in the association between Equation Modeling, 8(3), 325–352. https://doi.org/10.1207/
aggression and social status. Child Development, 75(1), 147–163. S15328007SEM0803_1.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00660.x. Kiefer, S. M., & Wang, J. H. (2016). Associations of coolness and
Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: how the pursuit social goals with aggression and engagement during adolescence.
of goals operates outside of conscious awareness. Science, 329 Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 44, 52–62.
(5987), 47–50. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1188595. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.007.
de Bruyn, E. H., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Wissink, I. B. (2010). Kiefer, S., & Ryan, A. (2008). Striving for social dominance over
Associations of peer acceptance and perceived popularity with peers: the implications for academic adjustment during early
bullying and victimization in early adolescence. The Journal of adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2),
Early Adolescence, 30(4), 543–566. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 417–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.417.
0272431609340517. Kuppens, S., Grietens, H., Onghena, P., Michiels, D., & Subramanian,
Elgar, F. J., Pickett, K. E., Pickett, W., Craig, W., Molcho, M., Hur- S. V. (2008). Individual and classroom variables associated with
relmann, K., & Lenzi, M. (2013). School bullying, homicide and relational aggression in elementary-school aged children: a mul-
income inequality: a cross-national pooled time series analysis. tilevel analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 46(6), 639–660.
International Journal of Public Health, 58(2), 237–245. https:// https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2008.06.005.
doi.org/10.1007/s00038-012-0380-y. LaFontana, K. M., & Cillessen, A. H. (2010). Developmental changes
Ellis, B. J., Volk, A. A., Gonzalez, J. M., & Embry, D. D. (2016). The in the priority of perceived status in childhood and adolescence.
meaningful roles intervention: an evolutionary approach to Social Development, 19(1), 130–147. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
reducing bullying and increasing prosocial behavior. Journal of 1467-9507.2008.00522.x.
Research on Adolescence, 26(4), 622–637. https://doi.org/10. Laninga-Wijnen, L., Harakeh, Z., Dijkstra, J. K., Veenstra, R., &
1111/jora.12243. Vollebergh, W. (2020). Who sets the aggressive popularity norm
Garandeau, C. F., Ahn, H. J., & Rodkin, P. C. (2011). The social status in classrooms? It’s the number and strength of aggressive, pro-
of aggressive students across contexts: the role of classroom status social, and bi-Strategic adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

Psychology, 48(1), 13–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019- Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M., Olthof, T., Goossens, F. A., Van De
00571-0. Schoot, R., Aleva, L., & Van Der Meulen, M. (2013). Costs and
Laninga-Wijnen, L., Harakeh, Z., Garandeau, C. F., Dijkstra, J. K., benefits of bullying in the context of the peer group: a three
Veenstra, R., & Vollebergh, W. A. (2019). Classroom popularity wave longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
hierarchy predicts prosocial and aggressive popularity norms chology, 41(8), 1217–1229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-
across the school year. Child Development, 90(5), 637–653. 013-9759-3.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13228. Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in
Li, Y., & Hu, Y. (2018). How to attain a popularity goal? Examining peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emo-
the mediation effects of popularity determinants and behaviors. tional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psycho-
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(9), 1842–1852. https://doi. logical Bulletin, 132(1), 98–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
org/10.1007/s10964-018-0882-x. 2909.132.1.98.
Li, Y., Xie, H., & Shi, J. (2012). Chinese and American children’s Salmivalli, C., & Peets, K. (2008). Bullies, victims, and bully–victim
perceptions of popularity determinants: cultural differences relationships. In K. Rubin, W. Bukowski & B. Laursen (Eds),
and behavioral correlates. International Journal of Behavioral Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp.
Development, 36(6), 420–429. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 322–340). New York: Guilford Press.
0165025412446393. Salmivalli, C., & Voeten, M. (2004). Connections between attitudes,
Lindenberg, S. (2013). Social rationality, self-regulation, and well- group norms, and behaviour in bullying situations. International
being: The regulatory significance of needs, goals, and the self. In Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(3), 246–258. https://doi.
R. Wittek, T. A. B. Snijders & V. Nee (Eds), Handbook of org/10.1080/01650250344000488.
rational choice social research (pp. 72–112). Stanford, CA: Schwartz, D., Tom, S. R., Chang, L., Xu, Y., Duong, M. T., & Kelly,
Stanford University Press. B. M. (2010). Popularity and acceptance as distinct dimensions of
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. social standing for Chinese children in Hong Kong. Social
New York: Guilford Press. Development, 19(4), 681–697. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
Melgar, N., & Rossi, M. (2012). A cross‐country analysis of the risk 9507.2009.00558.x.
factors for depression at the micro and macro levels. American Sijtsema, J. J., Lindenberg, S. M., Ojanen, T. J., & Salmivalli, C.
Journal of Economics and Sociology, 71(2), 354–376. https://doi. (2020). Direct aggression and the balance between status and
org/10.1111/j.1536-7150.2012.00831.x. affection goals in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence.
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus statistical modeling https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01166-0.
software. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén. Sijtsema, J. J., Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., & Salmivalli, C. (2009).
Niu, L., Jin, S., Li, L., & French, D. C. (2016). Popularity and social Empirical test of bullies’ status goals: assessing direct goals,
preference in Chinese adolescents: associations with social and aggression, and prestige. Aggressive Behavior, 35(1), 57–67.
behavioral adjustment. Social Development, 25(4), 828–845. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20282.
https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12172. Smith, P. K., López-Castro, L., Robinson, S., & Görzig, A. (2019).
Ojanen, T., Findley, D., & Fuller, S. (2012). Physical and relational Consistency of gender differences in bullying in cross-cultural
aggression in early adolescence: associations with narcissism, surveys. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45, 33–40. https://doi.
temperament, and social goals. Aggressive Behavior, 38(2), org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.04.006.
99–107. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21413. te Grotenhuis, M., Pelzer, B., Eisinga, R., Nieuwenhuis, R., Schmidt-
Ojanen, T., & Findley-Van Nostrand, D. (2014). Social goals, Catran, A., & Konig, R. (2017). When size matters: advantages of
aggression, peer preference, and popularity: longitudinal links weighted effect coding in observational studies. International
during middle school. Developmental Psychology, 50, Journal of Public Health, 62, 163–167. https://doi.org/10.1007/
2134–2143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037137. s00038-016-0901-1.
Olthof, T., Goossens, F. A., Vermande, M. M., Aleva, E. A., & van der Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & collectivism. Boulder, CO:
Meulen, M. (2011). Bullying as strategic behavior: relations with Westview Press.
desired and acquired dominance in the peer group. Journal of Tseng, W. L., Banny, A. M., Kawabata, Y., Crick, N. R., & Gau, S. S.
School Psychology, 49(3), 339–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp. F. (2013). A cross‐lagged structural equation model of relational
2011.03.003. aggression, physical aggression, and peer status in a Chinese
Olweus, D. (2013). School bullying: development and some important culture. Aggressive Behavior, 39(4), 301–315. https://doi.org/10.
challenges. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 751–780. 1002/ab.21480.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185516. Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Zijlstra, B. J., De Winter, A. F., Ver-
Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). Bullying, victimization, and sexual harassment hulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). The dyadic nature of bullying
during the transition to middle school. Educational Psychologist, and victimization: testing a dual‐perspective theory. Child
37(3), 151–163. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3703_2. Development, 78(6), 1843–1854. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
Pellegrini, A. D., & Long, J. D. (2002). A longitudinal study of bul- 8624.2007.01102.x.
lying, dominance, and victimization during the transition from Volk, A. A., Camilleri, J. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2012). Is
primary school through secondary school. British Journal of adolescent bullying an evolutionary adaptation? Aggressive
Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 259–280. https://doi.org/10. Behavior, 38(3), 222–238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s42380-018-
1348/026151002166442. 0005-y.
Pickett, K. E., & Wilkinson, R. G. (2015). Income inequality and Wright, M. F., Li, Y., & Shi, J. (2014). Chinese adolescents’ social
health: a causal review. Social Science & Medicine, 128, status goals: associations with behaviors and attributions for
316–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.12.031. relational aggression. Youth & Society, 46(4), 566–588. https://
Pouwels, J. L., van Noorden, T. H., & Caravita, S. C. (2019). Defending doi.org/10.1177/0044118X12448800.
victims of bullying in the classroom: the role of moral responsi- Zhang, W., Chen, L., & Chen, G. (2016). Research on bullying in
bility and social costs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, mainland China. In P. Smith, K. Kwak & Y. Toda. (Eds),
84, 103831. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103831. School bullying in different cultures: Eastern and western
Journal of Youth and Adolescence

perspectives (pp. 113–132). Cambridge: Cambridge University developmental psychopathology, and adolescent social-emotional
Press. development.
Zwaan, M., Dijkstra, J. K., & Veenstra, R. (2013). Status hierarchy,
attractiveness hierarchy and sex ratio: three contextual factors
explaining the status–aggression link among adolescents. Inter-
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(3), 211–221.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025412471018. Claire F. Garandeau is an Assistant Professor at the University of
Turku. Her major research interests include the causes and
consequences of school bullying, and the development of anti-
bullying interventions.
Bin Pan is a Ph.D. candidate at Shandong Normal University. His
major research interests include school bullying and its intervention.

Christina Salmivalli is a Professor at the University of Turku and


Shandong Normal University. Her major research interests include
Liang Zhang is a Lecturer at Shandong Normal University. His major bullying and victimization and their evidence-based prevention.
research interests include developmental psychopathology, peer
relationships, and longitudinal data analysis.

Wenxin Zhang is a Professor at Shandong Normal University. His


major research interests include school bullying, development of child/
Linqin Ji is a Professor at Shandong Normal University. Her major adolescent problem behaviors, parent-adolescent relationships, peer
research interests include aggression and problem behaviors, relationships, and developmental behavior genetics.

You might also like