You are on page 1of 316

8.

3 Movement Disorders 263

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Klawans, H. L. (1996). Why Michael couldn’t hit. New York: Freeman. A collection of fascinating
sports examples related to the brain and its disorders.
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral
mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley. This classic article in psychology is a
thought-provoking appraisal of what a theory of movement should explain.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, such as sites for Parkinson’s disease and Huntington’s disease.
© Norbert Wu, www.norbertwu.com
Wakefulness and Sleep 9
Chapter Outline Main Ideas
Module 9.1 ​Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 1. The brain generates a wake–sleep cycle of approximately
Endogenous Cycles 24 hours even in an unchanging environment.
Setting and Resetting the Biological Clock
2. Sleep progresses through various stages that differ in
Mechanisms of the Biological Clock
brain activity, heart rate, and other aspects. A stage
In Closing: Sleep–Wake Cycles
known as paradoxical or REM sleep is light in some ways
Module 9.2 ​Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms and deep in others.
Sleep and Other Interruptions of Consciousness 3. Areas in the brainstem and forebrain control arousal and
The Stages of Sleep sleep. Localized brain damage can produce prolonged
Paradoxical or REM Sleep sleep or wakefulness.
Brain Mechanisms of Wakefulness and Arousal 4. Because sleep depends on inhibition of brain activity,
Brain Function in REM Sleep sometimes one brain area is awake while another is
Sleep Disorders asleep, as in the case of sleepwalking.
In Closing: Stages of Sleep
5. Sleep can be impaired in many ways.
Module 9.3  ​Why Sleep? Why REM? Why Dreams? 6. We need sleep and REM sleep, although much about
Functions of Sleep their functions remains uncertain.
Functions of REM Sleep
Biological Perspectives on Dreaming
In Closing: Our Limited Self-Understanding
Interactive Exploration and Study

A
nyone deprived of sleep suffers. But if life evolved
on another planet with different conditions,
could animals evolve life without a need for sleep?
Imagine a planet that doesn’t rotate on its axis. Some
animals evolve adaptations to live in the light area, others
in the dark area, and still others in the twilight zone sep-
arating light from dark. There would be no need for any
animal to alternate active periods with inactive periods
on any fixed schedule and perhaps no need for prolonged
inactive periods. If you were the astronaut who discov-
ered these sleepless animals, you might be surprised.
Now imagine that astronauts from that planet set
out on their first voyage to Earth. Imagine their surprise
to discover animals like us with long inactive periods re-
sembling death. To someone who hadn’t seen sleep be-
fore, it would seem mysterious indeed. For the purposes
of this chapter, let’s adopt their perspective and ask why
OPPOSITE: Rock hyraxes at a national park in Kenya. animals as active as we are spend one third of our lives
doing so little.
265
Module 9.1

Rhythms of Waking
and Sleeping

Y
ou are probably not amazed to learn that your body Animals also produce endogenous circadian rhythms
spontaneously generates its own rhythm of wakefulness that last about a day. (Circadian comes from circum, for
and sleep. Psychologists of an earlier era strongly “about,” and dies, for “day.”) If you go without sleep all night—
resisted that idea. When behaviorism dominated experimental as most college students do, sooner or later—you feel sleepier
psychology during the mid-1900s, many psychologists and sleepier as the night goes on, but as morning arrives, you
believed that every behavior could be traced to external feel more alert, not less. The light from the sun helps you feel
stimuli. For example, alternation between wakefulness and less sleepy, but also your urge to sleep depends partly on the
sleep must depend on something in the outside world, such as time of day, not just how long you have been awake (Babkoff,
changes in light or temperature. Research as early as that of Caspy, Mikulincer, & Sing, 1991).
Curt Richter (1922) implied that the body generates its own Figure 9.1 represents the activity of a flying squirrel kept
cycles of activity and inactivity, but it took a huge amount of in total darkness for 25 days. Each horizontal line represents
research to convince the skeptics. The idea of self-generated one 24-hour day. A thickening in the line represents a period
rhythms was a major step toward viewing animals as active of activity. Even in this unchanging environment, the animal
producers of behaviors. generates a consistent rhythm of activity and sleep. Depend-
ing on the individual and the details of the procedure, the self-
generated cycle may be slightly shorter than 24 hours, as in
Endogenous Cycles Figure 9.1, or slightly longer (Carpenter & Grossberg, 1984).
An animal that produced its behavior entirely in response Humans also generate wake–sleep rhythms, and we find
to current stimuli would be at a serious disadvantage. Ani- it difficult to sleep on anything far from a 24-hour schedule.
mals often need to anticipate changes in the environment. We can modify it a little. If we ever send astronauts to Mars,
For example, migratory birds start flying toward their win- they will have to adjust to the Martian day, which lasts about
ter homes before their summer territory becomes too cold. 24 hours and 39 minutes of Earth time. Researchers have
A bird that waited for the first frost would be in trouble. found that people can adjust to that schedule without much
Similarly, squirrels begin storing nuts and putting on extra difficulty (Scheer, Wright, Kronauer, & Czeisler, 2007). Cir-
layers of fat in preparation for winter long before food be- cadian rhythms may be the least of our problems if we travel
comes scarce. to Mars. However, more severe departures from a 24-hour
Animals’ readiness for a change in seasons comes partly schedule pose difficulties. Naval personnel on U.S. nuclear-
from internal mechanisms. Changes in the light–dark pat- powered submarines are cut off from sunlight for months
tern of the day tell a migratory bird when to fly south for the at a time, living under faint artificial light. In many cases,
winter, but what tells it when to fly back north? In the trop- they live on a schedule of 6 hours of work alternating with
ics, the temperature and amount of daylight are nearly the 12 hours of rest. Even though they sleep (or try to sleep) on this
same throughout the year. Nevertheless, a migratory bird 18-hour schedule, their bodies generate rhythms of alertness
flies north at the right time. Even if it is kept in a cage with and body chemistry that average about 24.3 to 24.4 hours
no clues to the season, it becomes restless in the spring, and (Kelly et al., 1999).
if it is released, it flies north (Gwinner, 1986). Evidently, Circadian rhythms affect much more than just waking
the bird generates a rhythm that prepares it for seasonal and sleeping. We have circadian rhythms in our eating and
changes. We refer to that rhythm as an endogenous circan- drinking, urination, secretion of hormones, sensitivity to
nual rhythm. (Endogenous means “generated from within.” drugs, and other variables. For example, although we or-
Circannual comes from the Latin words circum, for “about,” dinarily think of human body temperature as 37° C, nor-
and annum, for “year.”) mal temperature fluctuates over the course of a day from

266
9.1 Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 267

Waking period 37.2


starts earlier each 98.9
day than the last. 37.1 98.8

Rectal temperature (°C)

Rectal temperature (°F)


Waking period
98.7
37.0 98.6
1
98.5
36.9 98.4
5 Sleep period starts 98.3
earlier each day 36.8
98.2
Days of experiment

than the last.


10 98.1
36.7
98.0
36.6 97.9
15 –12 –10 –8 –6 –4 –2 0 2 4 6 8
Hours from sleep onset

20 Figure 9.2  ​Mean rectal temperatures for nine adults


Body temperature reaches its low for the day about 2 hours after
sleep onset; it reaches its peak about 6 hours before sleep
25 onset. (From “Sleep-Onset Insomniacs Have Delayed Temperature
Rhythms,” by M. Morris, L. Lack, and D. Dawson, Sleep, 1990, 13,
noon 6 pm midnight 6 am noon 1–14. Reprinted by permission.)
Time of day in hours

Figure 9.1  ​Activity record of a flying squirrel kept in


constant darkness Not everyone falls neatly into one extreme or the other,
The thickened segments indicate periods of activity as measured
of course. A convenient way to compare people is to ask,
by a running wheel. Note that this free-running activity cycle lasts
slightly less than 24 hours. (From “Phase Control of Activity in a
“On holidays and vacations when you have no obligations,
Rodent,” by P. J. DeCoursey, Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quan- what time is the middle of your sleep?” For example, if you
titative Biology, 1960, 25, 49–55. Reprinted by permission of Cold sleep from 1 a.m. until 9 a.m. on those days, your middle is
Spring Harbor and P. J. DeCoursey.) 5 a.m. As Figure 9.4 shows, people differ by age. As a child,
you almost certainly went to bed early and woke up early.
As you entered adolescence, you started staying up later and
waking up later, when you had the opportunity. The mean
a low near 36.7° C during the night to almost 37.2° C in preferred time of going to sleep gets later and later until
late afternoon (Figure 9.2). We also have circadian rhythms about age 20 and then starts a gradual reversal (Roenneberg
in mood. In one study, young adults recorded their mood et al., 2004).
every two hours throughout the day. Although the results Do people older than 20 learn to go to bed earlier because
varied among individuals, most showed increases in positive they have jobs that require them to get up early? Maybe, but
mood (happiness) from waking until late afternoon, and two facts point instead to a biological explanation. First, in
then a slight decline from then to bedtime. In a follow-up Figure 9.4, note how the shift continues gradually over de-
study, the same investigators kept young adults awake for cades. If people were simply adjusting to their jobs, we might
30 consecutive hours, starting at either 10 a.m. or 5 p.m., in a expect a sudden shift in the early 20s and then steadiness until
laboratory setting with constant levels of light and tempera- retirement. Second, a similar trend occurs in rats: Older rats
ture. Regardless of whether people started this procedure at reach their best performance shortly after awakening, whereas
10 a.m. or 5 p.m., most reported their most pleasant mood younger rats tend to improve performance as the day pro-
around 5 p.m. and their least pleasant mood at around gresses (Winocur & Hasher, 1999, 2004).
5 a.m. (Murray et al., 2009). These results suggest a biologi-
cally driven circadian rhythm in our emotional well-being
Stop & Check
(Figure 9.3).
Circadian rhythms differ among individuals. Some people 1. What evidence indicates that humans have an internal
(“morning people,” or “larks”) awaken early, quickly become biological clock?
productive, and become less alert as the day progresses. Oth-
ers (“evening people,” or “owls”) warm up more slowly, both
and sleepy on about a 24-hour basis.
ANSWER
literally and figuratively, reaching their peak in the late after-
to follow that schedule and instead become wakeful
light–dark schedule much different from 24 hours fail
noon or evening. They tolerate staying up all night better than 1. People who have lived in an environment with a
morning people do (Taillard, Philip, Coste, Sagaspe, & Bio-
ulac, 2003).
268 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

2.5

Reports of positive mood, relative


2
1.5

to an average of zero
1
0.5
0
–0.5
–1
–1.5
–2
–2.5

.
P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M
A.

A.

A.

A.

A.

A.
1

9
11

1
1

9
11
Time (hr)

Reports of positive mood, relative


1.5

to an average of zero 1
0.5
Figure 9.3  ​Reported positive mood over time 0
During 30 hours in an unchanging laboratory environment,
–0.5
the average young adult reported the most pleasant mood
in the late afternoon or early evening, and the least pleasant –1
mood around 5 to 7 a.m. The pattern was similar for those –1.5
who started the procedure in the morning (above) or in the
evening (below). (From Murray, G., Nicholas, C. L., Kleiman, J., –2
Dwyer, R., Carrington, M. J., Allen, N. B., et al. (2009). Nature’s –2.5
clocks and human mood: The circadian system modulates
.

.
P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M

P.M
A.

A.

A.

A.

A.
reward motivation. Emotion, 9, 705–716.)
7

9
11

9
1

9
11
Time (hr)

Males

5
Time of the middle of sleep on

Females
days without obligations

Figure 9.4  ​Age differences in


circadian rhythms
People reported the time of the middle
of their sleep, such as 3 a.m. or
5 a.m., on days when they had no obli-
gations. (Reprinted from T. Roenneberg 3
et al., “A Marker for the End of Adoles-
cence,” Current Biology, 14, R1038–
R1039.) Figure 1, copyright 2004, with
permission from Elsevier.) 10 20 30 40 50 60
Age (years)
9.1 Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 269

Setting and Resetting Particularly impressive evidence for the importance of


sunlight comes from a study in Germany. The sun time at
the Biological Clock the eastern end of Germany differs by about half an hour
Our circadian rhythms generate a period close to 24 hours, from that at the western edge, even though everyone is on
but they are not perfect. We readjust our internal workings the same clock time. Researchers asked adults for their
daily to stay in phase with the world. Sometimes, we mis- preferred times of awakening and going to sleep and deter-
adjust them. On weekends, when most of us are freer to set mined for each person the midpoint of those values. (For
our own schedules, we expose ourselves to lights, noises, and example, if on weekends and holidays you prefer to go to
activity at night and then awaken late the next morning. By bed at 12:30 a.m. and awaken at 8:30 a.m., your sleep mid-
Monday morning, when the clock indicates 7 a.m., the bio- point is 4:30 a.m.) Figure 9.5 shows the results. People at
logical clock within us may say 5 a.m., and we stagger off to the eastern edge have a sleep midpoint about 30 minutes
work or school without much pep (Moore-Ede, Czeisler, & earlier than those at the west, corresponding to the fact
Richardson, 1983). that the sun rises earlier at the eastern edge (Roenneberg,
Although circadian rhythms persist without light, light is Kumar, & Merrow, 2007). The data shown here apply to
critical for resetting them. Without something to reset your people in towns and cities with populations under 300,000.
circadian rhythm, it would gradually drift away from the People in larger cities show a less consistent trend, presum-
correct time. The stimulus that resets the circadian rhythm ably because they spend more time indoors with less expo-
is referred to by the German term zeitgeber (TSITE-gay- sure to the sun.
ber), meaning “time-giver.” Light is the dominant zeitgeber What about blind people, who need to set their circadian
for land animals (Rusak & Zucker, 1979). (The tides are rhythms by zeitgebers other than light? The results vary. Some
important for many marine animals.) In addition to light, do set their circadian rhythms by noise, temperature, meals,
other zeitgebers include exercise (Eastman, Hoese, Young- and activity. However, others who are not sufficiently sensi-
stedt, & Liu, 1995), arousal of any kind (Gritton, Sutton, tive to these secondary zeitgebers produce circadian rhythms
Martinez, Sarter, & Lee, 2009), meals, and the temperature that are a little longer than 24 hours. When their cycles are
of the environment (Refinetti, 2000). Social stimuli—that in phase with the clock, all is well, but when they drift out of
is, the effects of other people—are weak zeitgebers, unless phase, they experience insomnia at night and sleepiness dur-
they induce exercise or other vigorous activity (Mistlberger ing the day (Sack & Lewy, 2001).
& Skene, 2004). These additional zeitgebers merely supple-
ment or alter the effects of light. On their own, their effects
are weak. For example, people who are working in Antarctica
during the constant darkness of an Antarctic winter try to
maintain a 24-hour rhythm, but they drift away from it. Dif- 4.8
ferent people generate different rhythms, until they find it
more and more difficult to work together (Kennaway & Van 4.6
Dorp, 1991). Astronauts in Earth orbit face a special prob-
Midpoint of the

lem: As they orbit the Earth, a 45-minute period of daylight


night’s sleep

4.4
alternates with 45 minutes of darkness. If they retreat from
the flight deck to elsewhere in the spacecraft, they have con-
stant dim light. As a result, they are never fully alert during 4.2
their wakeful periods and they sleep poorly during their rest
periods (Dijk et al., 2001). On long trips, many of them ex- 4.0
perience depression, irritability, and impaired performance
(Mallis & DeRoshia, 2005). 3.8
Even when we try to set our wake–sleep cycles by the West East
clock, sunlight has its influence. Consider what happens
when we shift to daylight savings time in spring. You set Figure 9.5  ​Sun time competes with social time
your clock to an hour later, and when it shows your usual On days when people have no obligation to awaken at a particular
bedtime, you dutifully go to bed, even though it seems an time, they awaken about half an hour earlier at the eastern edge
hour too early. The next morning, when the clock says it is of Germany than at the western edge. Points along the y axis
7 a.m. and time to get ready for work, your brain registers represent the midpoint between the preferred bedtime and the
preferred waking time. Data are for people living in towns and
6 a.m. Most people remain inefficient and ill-rested for
cities with populations less than 300,000. (From Roenneberg,
days after the shift to daylight savings time. The adjustment T., et al. (2007). “The human circadian clock entrains to sun time.”
is especially difficult for people who were already sleep- Current Biology, 17, R44–R45. Reprinted by permission of the
deprived, including most college students (Lahti et al., 2006; Copyright Clearance Center.)
Monk & Aplin, 1980).
270 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Stop & Check repeated adjustments of the circadian rhythm, although the
problem here could be air travel itself. (A good control group
2. Why do people at the eastern edge of Germany awaken would have been flight attendants who flew long north–south
earlier than those at the western edge on their weekends routes.)
and holidays?

ANSWER
follow the same clock time for their work schedule. Shift Work
controls waking–sleeping schedules even when people
ern edge than at the western edge. Evidently, the sun People who sleep irregularly—such as pilots, medical interns,
2. The sun rises about half an hour earlier at the east- and shift workers in factories—find that their duration of
sleep depends on when they go to sleep. When they have to
sleep in the morning or early afternoon, they sleep only briefly,
even if they have been awake for many hours (Frese & Har-
wich, 1984; Winfree, 1983).
Jet Lag People who work on a night shift, such as midnight to
A disruption of circadian rhythms due to crossing time zones 8 a.m., sleep during the day. At least they try to. Even after
is known as jet lag. Travelers complain of sleepiness during months or years on such a schedule, many workers adjust in-
the day, sleeplessness at night, depression, and impaired con- completely. They continue to feel groggy on the job, they sleep
centration. All these problems stem from the mismatch be- poorly during the day, and their body temperature continues
tween internal circadian clock and external time (Haimov & to peak when they are sleeping in the day instead of while
Arendt, 1999). Most people find it easier to adjust to crossing they are working at night. In general, night–shift workers have
time zones going west than east. Going west, we stay awake more accidents than day–shift workers.
later at night and then awaken late the next morning, already Working at night does not reliably change the circadian
partly adjusted to the new schedule. We phase-delay our cir- rhythm because most buildings use artificial lighting in the
cadian rhythms. Going east, we phase-advance to sleep earlier range of 150–180 lux, which is only moderately effective in
and awaken earlier (Figure 9.6). Most people find it difficult resetting the rhythm (Boivin, Duffy, Kronauer, & Czeisler,
to go to sleep before their body’s usual time and difficult to 1996). People adjust best to night work if they sleep in a
wake up early the next day. very dark room during the day and work under very bright
Adjusting to jet lag is often stressful. Stress elevates blood lights at night, comparable to the noonday sun (Czeisler et
levels of the adrenal hormone cortisol, and many studies have al., 1990).
shown that prolonged elevations of cortisol damage neurons
in the hippocampus, a brain area important for memory. One
study examined flight attendants who had spent the previ- Mechanisms of the
ous 5 years making flights across 7 or more time zones—
such as Chicago to Italy—with mostly short breaks (fewer
Biological Clock
than 6 days) between trips. Most of these flight attendants How does the body generate a circadian rhythm? Curt Rich-
had smaller than average volumes of the hippocampus and ter (1967) introduced the concept that the brain generates its
surrounding structures, and they showed some memory im- own rhythms—a biological clock—and he reported that the
pairments (Cho, 2001). These results suggest a danger from biological clock is insensitive to most forms of interference.

(a) Leave New York at 7 P.M. (b) Arrive in London at 7 A.M., which is 2 A.M. in New York

Figure 9.6  ​Jet lag


Eastern time is later than western time. People who travel six time zones east fall asleep on the plane and then must awaken when it is
morning at their destination but night back home. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
9.1 Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 271

Blind or deaf animals generate circadian rhythms, although Stop & Check
they slowly drift out of phase with the external world. The
circadian rhythm remains surprisingly steady despite food or 3. What evidence strongly indicates that the SCN produces
water deprivation, X-rays, tranquilizers, alcohol, anesthesia, the circadian rhythm itself?
lack of oxygen, most kinds of brain damage, or the removal the pattern of the donor animals.
of endocrine organs. Even an hour or more of induced hiber- ANSWER
planted SCN neurons, their circadian rhythm followed
nation often fails to reset the biological clock (Gibbs, 1983; rest of the body. Also, when hamsters received trans-
Richter, 1975). Evidently, the biological clock is a hardy, ro- even if they are kept in cell culture isolated from the
bust mechanism. 3. SCN cells produce a circadian rhythm of activity

The Suprachiasmatic Nucleus (SCN)


The biological clock depends on part of the hypothalamus,
called the suprachiasmatic (soo-pruh-kie-as-MAT-ik) nu- How Light Resets the SCN
cleus, or SCN. It gets its name from its location just above The SCN is located just above the optic chiasm. Figure 9.7
(“supra”) the optic chiasm (Figure 9.7). The SCN provides the shows the position in the human brain. The relationship is
main control of the circadian rhythms for sleep and body tem- similar in other mammals. A small branch of the optic nerve,
perature (Refinetti & Menaker, 1992), although several other known as the retinohypothalamic path, extends directly from the
brain areas generate local rhythms (Granados-Fuentes, Tseng, retina to the SCN. Axons of that path alter the SCN’s settings.
& Herzog, 2006). After damage to the SCN, the body’s Most of the input to that path, however, does not come
rhythms become erratic. from normal retinal receptors. Mice with genetic defects that
The SCN generates circadian rhythms itself in a geneti- destroy nearly all their rods and cones nevertheless reset their
cally controlled, unlearned manner. If SCN neurons are dis- biological clocks in synchrony with the light (Freedman et al.,
connected from the rest of the brain or removed from the 1999; Lucas, Freedman, Muñoz, Garcia-Fernández, & Foster,
body and maintained in tissue culture, they continue to pro- 1999). Also, consider blind mole rats (Figure 9.8). Their eyes
duce a circadian rhythm of action potentials (Earnest, Liang, are covered with folds of skin and fur. They have neither eye
Ratcliff, & Cassone, 1999; Inouye & Kawamura, 1979). Even muscles nor a lens with which to focus an image. They have
a single isolated SCN cell can maintain a circadian rhythm, fewer than 900 optic nerve axons compared with 100,000
although interactions among cells sharpen the accuracy of the in hamsters. Even a bright flash of light evokes no startle re-
rhythm (Long, Jutras, Connors, & Burwell, 2005; Yamaguchi sponse and no measurable change in brain activity. Neverthe-
et al., 2003). less, light resets their circadian rhythms (de Jong, Hendriks,
A mutation in one gene causes hamsters’ SCN to produce Sanyal, & Nevo, 1990).
a 20-hour instead of 24-hour rhythm (Ralph & Menaker, The surprising explanation is that the retinohypotha-
1988). Researchers surgically removed the SCN from adult lamic path to the SCN comes from a special population
hamsters and transplanted SCN tissue from hamster fetuses of retinal ganglion cells that have their own photopigment,
into the adults. When they transplanted SCN tissue from called melanopsin, unlike the ones found in rods and cones
fetuses with a 20-hour rhythm, the recipients produced a (Hannibal, Hindersson, Knudsen, Georg, & Fahrenkrug,
20-hour rhythm. When they transplanted tissue from fetuses 2001; Lucas, Douglas, & Foster, 2001). These special gan-
with a 24-hour rhythm, the recipients produced a 24-hour glion cells receive some input from rods and cones (Gooley
rhythm (Ralph, Foster, Davis, & Menaker, 1990). That is, the et al., 2010; Güler et al., 2008), but even if they do not re-
rhythm followed the pace of the donors, not the recipients. ceive that input, they respond directly to light (Berson,
Again, the results show that the rhythms come from the SCN Dunn, & Takao, 2002). These special ganglion cells are lo-
itself. cated mainly near the nose, not evenly throughout the retina
(Visser, Beersma, & Daan, 1999). (That is, they see toward
the periphery.) They respond to light slowly and turn off
slowly when the light ceases (Berson et al., 2002). Therefore,
they respond to the overall average amount of light, not to
instantaneous changes in light. The average intensity over a
period of minutes or hours is, of course, exactly the informa-
tion the SCN needs to gauge the time of day.
Note a couple of consequences: First, many people who
are blind because of damage to the rods and cones neverthe-
Curt P. Richter less have enough input to the melanopsin-containing ganglion
Elliott Blass

(1894–1988) cells to entrain their waking and sleeping cycle to the local pat-
I enjoy research more than eating. tern of sunlight. Second, it was formerly puzzling that bright
272 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Corpus callosum
Thalamus Cerebral cortex

Figure 9.7  ​The


suprachiasmatic nucleus
(SCN) of rats and humans
The SCN is located at the base
of the brain, as seen in these
coronal sections through the Basal
plane of the anterior hypothala- ganglia
mus. Each rat was injected with SCN
(a) (b)
radioactive 2-deoxyglucose,
which is absorbed by the most
active neurons. A high level
of absorption of this chemical Cerebral cortex
produces a dark appearance
on the slide. Note the greater
activity in SCN neurons of a
rat injected during the day (a)
than in one injected at night
(b). (From “Suprachiasmatic
nucleus: Use of 14C-labeled
deoxyglucose uptake as a
functional marker,” by Pineal gland
W. J. Schwartz & H. Gainer, Suprachiasmatic
Science, 1977, 197: 1089– nucleus
1091. Reprinted by permission
from AAAS/American Associa-
tion for the Advancement of Optic chiasm
Science.) (c) A sagittal section
through a human brain showing
the location of the SCN and the
Hypothalamus
pineal gland. (© Cengage Learn-
ing 2013)
(c)

light aggravates migraine headaches even for many blind peo-


ple. The explanation is that the melanopsin-containing gan-
glion cells send input to the posterior thalamus, which is part
of the pathway producing pain in migraines (Noseda, et al.,
2010). Someone with no input to the visual cortex, and there-
fore no conscious vision, can nevertheless have light-sensitive
excitation in the thalamus.
Stop & Check
Courtesy of Eviatar Nevo

4. How does light reset the biological clock?

they do not receive input from rods or cones.


ANSWER
glion cells that respond to light by themselves, even if
axons comprising that path originate from special gan-
path, conveys information about light to the SCN. The Figure 9.8  ​A blind mole rat
4. A branch of the optic nerve, the retinohypothalamic Although blind mole rats are blind in other regards, they reset
their circadian rhythms in response to light.
9.1 Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 273

The Biochemistry shorter than 24 hours, as if they were moving about a time zone
west every day (C. R. Jones et al., 1999). They consistently get
of the Circadian Rhythm sleepy early in the evening and awaken early in the morning
The suprachiasmatic nucleus produces the circadian rhythm, (Toh et al., 2001; Xu et al., 2005). Most people look forward
but how? Research on production of the circadian rhythm be- to days when they can stay up late. People with the altered gene
gan with insects. Studies on the fruit fly Drosophila found several look forward to times when they can go to bed early. Most peo-
genes responsible for a circadian rhythm (X. Liu et al., 1992; ple with this sleep abnormality suffer from depression (Xu et al.,
Sehgal, Ousley, Yang, Chen, & Schotland, 1999). Two of these 2005). As we see again in Chapter 15, sleep impairments and
genes, known as period (abbreviated per) and timeless (tim), pro- depression are closely linked. Another PER mutation has been
duce the proteins PER and TIM. The concentration of these identified that is more common but less intrusive. People with
two proteins, which promote sleep and inactivity, oscillates over this gene are normal in most regards except that their alertness
a day, based on feedback interactions among several sets of neu- deteriorates substantially if they are deprived of a good night’s
rons. Early in the morning, the messenger RNA levels respon- sleep (Dijk & Archer, 2010).
sible for producing PER and TIM start at low concentrations.
As they increase during the day, they increase synthesis of the
Stop & Check
proteins, but the process takes time, and so the protein concen-
trations lag hours behind, as shown in Figure 9.9. As the PER 5. How do the proteins TIM and PER relate to sleepiness in
and TIM protein concentrations increase, they feed back to Drosophila?
inhibit the genes that produce the messenger RNA molecules.
Thus, during the night, the PER and TIM concentrations are ANSWER
that their level declines toward morning.
high, but the messenger RNA concentrations are declining (Ni-
feed back to inhibit the genes that produce them, so

tabach & Taghert, 2008). By the next morning, PER and TIM
reach high levels at night, promoting sleep. They also
of the day and begin to increase toward evening. They
protein levels are low, the flies awaken, and the cycle is ready to 5. The proteins TIM and PER remain low during most
start again. Because the feedback cycle takes about 24 hours, the
flies generate a circadian rhythm even in an unchanging environ-
ment. However, in addition to the automatic feedback, light ac-
tivates a chemical that breaks down the TIM protein, thereby
increasing wakefulness and synchronizing the internal clock to Melatonin
the external world (Ashmore & Sehgal, 2003). The SCN regulates waking and sleeping by controlling ac-
Why do we care about flies? The reason is that analyzing the tivity levels in other brain areas, including the pineal gland
mechanism in flies told researchers what to look for in humans (PIN-ee-al; Figure 9.7), an endocrine gland located just pos-
and other mammals. Mammals have three versions of the PER terior to the thalamus (Aston-Jones, Chen, Zhu, & Oshinsky,
protein and several proteins closely related to TIM and the oth- 2001; von Gall et al., 2002). The pineal gland releases the hor-
ers found in flies (Reick, Garcia, Dudley, & McKnight, 2001; mone melatonin, which influences both circadian and circan-
Zheng et al., 1999). Mutations in the genes producing PER pro- nual rhythms (Butler et al., 2010). The pineal gland secretes
teins lead to alterations of sleep schedules. People with a particu- melatonin mostly at night, making us sleepy at that time.
lar PER mutation have been found to have a circadian rhythm When people shift to a new time zone and start following a

Sunrise Sunset Sunrise

Day Night

Per and tim


mRNA concentrations
PER and TIM
protein concentrations

Wakefulness Sleep Wake

Figure 9.9  ​Feedback between proteins and genes to control sleepiness


In fruit flies (Drosophila), the concentrations of the mRNA levels for PER and TIM oscillate over a day, and so do the proteins that they
produce. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
274 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

new schedule, they continue to feel sleepy at their old times Melatonin also feeds back to reset the biological clock
until the melatonin rhythm shifts (Dijk & Cajochen, 1997). through its effects on receptors in the SCN (Gillette &
People who have pineal gland tumors sometimes stay awake McArthur, 1996). A moderate dose of melatonin (0.5 mg) in
for days at a time (Haimov & Lavie, 1996). the afternoon phase-advances the clock. That is, it makes the
Melatonin secretion starts to increase about 2 or 3 hours person get sleepy earlier in the evening and wake up earlier the
before bedtime. Taking a melatonin pill in the evening has lit- next morning. A single dose of melatonin in the morning has
tle effect on sleepiness because the pineal gland produces mel- little effect (Wirz-Justice, Werth, Renz, Müller, & Kräuchi,
atonin at that time anyway. However, people who take mela- 2002), although repeated morning doses can phase-delay the
tonin at other times become sleepy within 2 hours (Haimov clock, causing the person to get sleepy later than usual at night
& Lavie, 1996). Melatonin pills are sometimes helpful when and awaken later the next morning.
people travel across time zones or for other reasons need to
sleep at an unaccustomed time.

Module 9.1  ■  In Closing

Sleep–Wake Cycles

Unlike an electric appliance that stays on until someone turns it that prepare us to wake at certain times and sleep at other times,
off, the brain periodically turns itself on and off. Sleepiness is even if we would prefer different schedules.
not a voluntary or optional act. We have biological mechanisms

Summary
1. Animals, including humans, have circadian rhythms—in- 6. The suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), a part of the
ternally generated rhythms of activity and sleep lasting hypothalamus, generates the body’s circadian rhythms for
about 24 hours, even in an unchanging environment. It is sleep and temperature.  271
difficult to adjust to a sleep schedule much different from 7. Light resets the biological clock by a branch of the optic
24 hours.  266 nerve that extends to the SCN. Those axons originate
2. Some people are most alert early in the morning, and from a special population of ganglion cells that respond
others become more alert later in the day. On average, directly to light in addition to receiving some input from
people show their greatest preference for staying awake rods and cones.  272
late and sleeping late the next morning when they are 8. The genes controlling the circadian rhythm are almost
about 20 years old.  267 the same in mammals as in insects. Circadian rhythms
3. Although the biological clock continues to operate in result from a feedback cycle based on genes that
constant light or constant darkness, the onset of light produce the proteins PER and TIM, and the ability of
resets the clock. Even when people set their waking and those proteins to inhibit the genes that produce
sleeping times by the clock, the timing of sunrise strongly them.  273
influences their circadian rhythm.  268 9. The SCN controls the body’s rhythm partly by directing
4. It is easier for most people to follow a cycle longer than the release of melatonin by the pineal gland. The
24 hours (as when traveling west) than to follow a cycle hormone melatonin increases sleepiness; if given at
shorter than 24 hours (as when traveling east).  270 certain times of the day, it can also reset the circadian
5. If people wish to work at night and sleep during the day, rhythm.  274
the best way to shift the circadian rhythm is to have
bright lights at night and darkness during the day.  270

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
endogenous circadian jet lag  270 suprachiasmatic nucleus
rhythms  266 melatonin  273 (SCN)  271
endogenous circannual pineal gland  273 zeitgeber  269
rhythm  266
9.1 Rhythms of Waking and Sleeping 275

Thought Questions
1. Why would evolution have enabled blind mole rats to 2. If you travel across several time zones to the east and
synchronize their SCN activity to light, even though want to use melatonin to help reset your circadian
they cannot see well enough to make any use of the rhythm, at what time of day should you take it? What
light? if you travel west?
Module 9.2

Stages of Sleep
and Brain Mechanisms

S
uppose you buy a new radio. After you play it for stage higher, with occasional, brief periods of purposeful
4 hours, it suddenly stops. You wonder whether the actions and a limited amount of speech comprehension. A
batteries are dead or whether the radio needs repair. vegetative or minimally conscious state can last for months
Later, you discover that this radio always stops after playing or years.
for 4 hours but operates again a few hours later even without Brain death is a condition with no sign of brain activity
repairs or a battery change. You begin to suspect that the and no response to any stimulus. Physicians usually wait un-
manufacturer designed it this way, perhaps to prevent you til someone has shown no sign of brain activity for 24 hours
from listening to the radio all day. Now you want to find the before pronouncing brain death, at which point most people
device that turns it off whenever you play it for 4 hours. You believe it is ethical to remove life support.
are asking a new question. When you thought that the radio
stopped because it needed repairs or new batteries, you did
not ask which device turned it off.
Stages of Sleep
Similarly, if we think of sleep as something like wearing Nearly every scientific advance comes from new or improved
out a machine, we do not ask which part of the brain produces measurements. Researchers did not even suspect that sleep
it. But if we think of sleep as a specialized state evolved to has different stages until they accidentally measured them.
serve particular functions, we look for the mechanisms that The electroencephalograph (EEG), as described in Chapter 4,
regulate it. records an average of the electrical potentials of the cells and
fibers in the brain areas nearest each electrode on the scalp
(Figure 9.10). If half the cells in some area increase their elec-
Sleep and Other Interruptions trical potentials while the other half decrease, they cancel out.
The EEG record rises or falls when most cells do the same
of Consciousness thing at the same time. You might compare it to a record of
Let’s start with some distinctions. Sleep is a state that the brain the noise in a crowded sports stadium: It shows only slight
actively produces, characterized by decreased response to fluctuations until some event gets everyone yelling at once.
stimuli. In contrast, coma (KOH-muh) is an extended period The EEG enables brain researchers to compare brain activity
of unconsciousness caused by head trauma, stroke, or disease. at different times during sleep.
It is possible to awaken a sleeping person but not someone Figure 9.11 shows data from a polysomnograph, a com-
in a coma. A person in a coma has a low level of brain activ- bination of EEG and eye-movement records, for a college stu-
ity throughout the day, and little or no response to stimuli, dent during various stages of sleep. Figure 9.11a presents a pe-
including those that are ordinarily painful. Any movements riod of relaxed wakefulness for comparison. Note the steady
that occur are purposeless and not directed toward anything. series of alpha waves at a frequency of 8 to 12 per second.
Typically, someone in a coma either dies or begins to recover Alpha waves are characteristic of relaxation, not of all wake-
within a few weeks. fulness.
Someone in a vegetative state alternates between peri- In Figure 9.11b, sleep has just begun. During this period,
ods of sleep and moderate arousal, although even during the called stage 1 sleep, the EEG is dominated by irregular, jag-
more aroused state, the person shows no awareness of sur- ged, low-voltage waves. Overall brain activity is less than in re-
roundings. Breathing is more regular, and a painful stimu- laxed wakefulness but higher than other sleep stages. As Fig-
lus produces at least the autonomic responses of increased ure 9.11c shows, the most prominent characteristics of stage 2
heart rate, breathing, and sweating. The person does not are sleep spindles and K-complexes. A sleep spindle consists
speak, respond to speech, or show any purposeful activity. of 12- to 14-Hz waves during a burst that lasts at least half a
However, people in this state probably have some cognitive second. Sleep spindles result from oscillating interactions be-
activity (Guérit, 2005). A minimally conscious state is one tween cells in the thalamus and the cortex. A K-complex is a
276
9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 277

stage 4, more than half the record includes large waves of at


least a half-second duration. Stages 3 and 4 together constitute
slow-wave sleep (SWS).
Slow waves indicate that neuronal activity is highly syn-
chronized. In stage 1 and in wakefulness, the cortex receives a
great deal of input, much of it at high frequencies. Nearly all
neurons are active, but different populations of neurons are
active at different times. Thus, the EEG is full of short, rapid,
choppy waves. By stage 4, however, sensory input to the cere-
bral cortex is greatly reduced, and the few remaining sources
of input can synchronize many cells.

Stop & Check


6. What do long, slow waves on an EEG indicate?
©Richard Nowitz Photography

with much synchrony of response among neurons.


ANSWER
6. Long, slow waves indicate a low level of activity,

Figure 9.10  ​Sleeping person with electrodes in place on


Paradoxical or REM Sleep
the scalp for recording brain activity Many discoveries occur when researchers stumble upon some-
The printout above his head shows the readings from each thing by accident and then notice that it might be important.
electrode.
In the 1950s, French scientist Michel Jouvet was trying to
test the learning abilities of cats after removal of the cerebral
cortex. Because decorticate mammals don’t do much, Jouvet
sharp wave associated with temporary inhibition of neuronal recorded slight movements of the muscles and EEGs from the
firing (Cash et al., 2009). hindbrain. During certain periods of apparent sleep, the cats’
In the succeeding stages of sleep, heart rate, breathing brain activity was relatively high, but their neck muscles were
rate, and brain activity decrease, while slow, large-amplitude completely relaxed. Jouvet (1960) then recorded the same
waves become more common (Figures 9.11d and e). By phenomenon in normal, intact cats and named it paradoxical

(a) Relaxed, awake (d) Stage 3 sleep

Figure 9.11  ​Polysomnograph records


(b) Stage 1 sleep (e) Stage 4 sleep from a college student
For each of these records, the top line
Sleep spindle K-complex is the EEG from one electrode on the
scalp. The middle line is a record of eye
movements. The bottom line is a time
marker, indicating 1-second units. Note
the abundance of slow waves in stages
3 and 4. (Records provided by T. E. LeVere)
(c) Stage 2 sleep (f) REM, or “paradoxical” sleep
278 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

sleep because it is deep sleep in some ways and light in others. bines deep sleep, light sleep, and features that are difficult to
(The term paradoxical means “apparently self-contradictory.”) classify as deep or light. Consequently, it is best to avoid the
Meanwhile, in the United States, Nathaniel Kleitman and terms deep and light sleep.
Eugene Aserinsky were observing eye movements of sleeping In addition to its steady characteristics, REM sleep has
people as a means of measuring depth of sleep, assuming that intermittent characteristics such as facial twitches and eye
eye movements would stop during sleep. At first, they recorded movements, as shown in Figure 9.11f. The EEG record is
only a few minutes of eye movements per hour because the similar to that for stage 1 sleep, but notice the difference in eye
recording paper was expensive and they did not expect to see movements. The stages other than REM are known as non-
anything interesting in the middle of the night anyway. When REM (NREM) sleep.
they occasionally found periods of eye movements in people When you fall asleep, you start in stage 1 and slowly progress
who had been asleep for hours, the investigators assumed that through stages 2, 3, and 4 in order, although loud noises or other
something was wrong with their machines. Only after re- intrusions can interrupt the sequence. After about an hour of
peated careful measurements did they conclude that periods sleep, you begin to cycle back from stage 4 through stages 3, 2,
of rapid eye movements occur during sleep (Dement, 1990). and then REM. The sequence repeats, with each cycle lasting
They called these periods rapid eye movement (REM) sleep about 90 minutes. (Some people have inferred that because a
(Aserinsky & Kleitman, 1955; Dement & Kleitman, 1957a) cycle lasts 90 minutes, you need to sleep at least 90 minutes to
and soon realized that REM sleep was synonymous with what get any benefit. No evidence supports that claim.)
Jouvet called paradoxical sleep. Researchers use the term REM Early in the night, stages 3 and 4 predominate. Toward
sleep when referring to humans but often prefer the term par- morning, REM occupies an increasing percentage of the time.
adoxical sleep for nonhumans because many species lack eye Figure 9.12 shows typical sequences. The amount of REM
movements. depends on time of day more than how long you have been
During paradoxical or REM sleep, the EEG shows ir- asleep. That is, if you go to sleep later than usual, you still in-
regular, low-voltage fast waves that indicate increased neuro- crease your REM at about the same time that you would have
nal activity. In this regard, REM sleep is light. However, the ordinarily (Czeisler, Weitzman, Moore-Ede, Zimmerman, &
postural muscles of the body, including those that support the Knauer, 1980).
head, are more relaxed during REM than in other stages. In Shortly after the discovery of REM, researchers believed
this regard, REM is deep sleep. REM is also associated with it was almost synonymous with dreaming. William Dement
erections in males and vaginal moistening in females. Heart and Nathaniel Kleitman (1957b) found that people who were
rate, blood pressure, and breathing rate are more variable in awakened during REM reported dreams 80% to 90% of the
REM than in stages 2 through 4. In short, REM sleep com- time. Later research, however, found that people also some-

11 P.M. 12 P.M. 1 A.M. 2 A.M. 3 A.M. 4 A.M. 5 A.M. 6 A.M.


REM

REM

REM

REM

A23 4 32 2 3 432 3 4 2 2 3 2 2 2 A

11 P.M. 12 P.M. 1 A.M. 2 A.M. 3 A.M. 4 A.M. 5 A.M.


REM

REM

REM

A2 3 4 2 3 4 32 3 2 2 3 2 A

11 P.M. 12 P.M. 1 A.M. 2 A.M. 3 A.M. 4 A.M. 5 A.M. 6 A.M.


REM

REM

REM

REM

A 2 34 2 4 3 4 2 2 3 4 2 2 3 4 2 2 3 2A 2 2 A

Figure 9.12  ​Sleep stages on three nights


Columns indicate awake (A) and sleep stages 2, 3, 4, and REM. Deflections in the line at the bottom of each chart indicate shifts in body
position. Note that stage 4 sleep occurs mostly in the early part of the night’s sleep, whereas REM sleep becomes more prevalent toward
the end. (Based on Dement & Kleitman, 1957a)
9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 279

A cut through the midbrain decreases arousal by damag-


William C. Dement ing the reticular formation, a structure that extends from the
The average person would not, at first medulla into the forebrain. Some neurons of the reticular for-
blush, pick watching people sleep as mation have axons ascending into the brain, and some have
the most apparent theme for a spine- axons descending into the spinal cord. Those with axons de-
tingling scientific adventure thriller. scending into the spinal cord form part of the medial tract of
motor control, as discussed in Chapter 8. In 1949, Giuseppe
William Dement

However, there is a subtle sense of


awe and mystery surrounding the Moruzzi and H. W. Magoun proposed that those with as-
“short death” we call sleep. cending axons are well suited to regulate arousal. The term re-
ticular (based on the Latin word rete, meaning “net”) describes
the widespread connections among neurons in this system.
One part of the reticular formation that contributes to cor-
times report dreams when awakened from NREM sleep. tical arousal is known as the pontomesencephalon (Woolf,
REM dreams are more likely than NREM dreams to include 1996). (The term derives from pons and mesencephalon, or
striking visual imagery and complicated plots, but not always. “midbrain.”) These neurons receive input from many sensory
Some people continue to report dreams despite an apparent systems and generate spontaneous activity of their own. Their
lack of REM (Solms, 1997). In short, REM and dreams usu- axons extend into the forebrain, as shown in Figure 9.13, re-
ally overlap, but they are not the same thing. leasing acetylcholine and glutamate, which excite cells in the
hypothalamus, thalamus, and basal forebrain. Consequently,
the pontomesencephalon maintains arousal during wakeful-
Stop & Check ness and increases it in response to new or challenging tasks
(Kinomura, Larsson, Gulyás, & Roland, 1996). Stimulation
7. How can an investigator determine whether a sleeper is
of the pontomesencephalon awakens a sleeping individual or
in REM sleep?
increases alertness in one already awake, shifting the EEG
8. During which part of a night’s sleep is REM most from long, slow waves to short, high-frequency waves (Munk,
common? Roelfsema, König, Engel, & Singer, 1996). However, subsys-
tems within the pontomesencephalon control different sen-
ANSWERS
sleep.
sory modalities, so a stimulus sometimes arouses one part of
the brain more than others (Guillery, Feig, & Lozsádi, 1998).
becomes more common toward the end of the night’s

The locus coeruleus (LOW-kus ser-ROO-lee-us; liter-


7. Examine EEG pattern and eye movements. 8. REM

ally, “dark blue place”), a small structure in the pons, is usu-


ally inactive, especially during sleep, but it emits bursts of
impulses in response to meaningful events, especially those
Brain Mechanisms of that produce emotional arousal (Sterpenich et al., 2006).
Axons from the locus coeruleus release norepinephrine
Wakefulness and Arousal widely throughout the cortex, so this tiny area has a huge
Recall from Chapter 1 the distinction between the “easy” and influence. Anything that stimulates the locus coeruleus
“hard” problems of consciousness. The easy problems include strengthens the storage of recent memories (Clayton & Wil-
such matters as, “Which brain areas increase overall alertness, liams, 2000) and increases wakefulness (Berridge, Stellick,
and by what kinds of transmitters do they do so?” As you are & Schmeichel, 2005).
about to see, that question may be philosophically easy, but it The hypothalamus has several axon pathways that influ-
is scientifically complex. ence arousal. One pathway releases the neurotransmitter his-
tamine ( J.-S. Lin, Hou, Sakai, & Jouvet, 1996), which pro-
duces excitatory effects throughout the brain (Haas & Panula,
Brain Structures of Arousal and Attention 2003). Cells releasing histamine are active during arousal and
After a cut through the midbrain separates the forebrain and alertness. As you might guess, they are less active when you
part of the midbrain from all the lower structures, an animal en- are getting ready for sleep and when you have just awakened
ters a prolonged state of sleep for the next few days. Even after in the morning (K. Takahashi, Lin, & Sakai, 2006). Antihista-
weeks of recovery, the wakeful periods are brief. We might sup- mine drugs, often used for allergies, counteract this transmit-
pose a simple explanation: The cut isolated the brain from the ter and produce drowsiness. Antihistamines that do not cross
sensory stimuli that come up from the medulla and spinal cord. the blood–brain barrier avoid that side effect.
However, if a researcher cuts each individual tract that enters the Another pathway from the hypothalamus, mainly from
medulla and spinal cord, thus depriving the brain of the sensory the lateral and posterior nuclei of the hypothalamus, releases a
input, the animal still has normal periods of wakefulness and peptide neurotransmitter called either orexin or hypocretin.
sleep. Evidently, the midbrain does more than just relay sensory For simplicity, this text will stick to the term orexin, but if you
information; it has its own mechanisms to promote wakefulness. find the term hypocretin in other reading, it means the same
280 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

ho line
cetylc
A

GABA
Ac
et
Histamine

yl
ch
A

ol
AB

in
G

e
Basal forebrain

Hista Hypothalamus Norep


ine
mi

GABA phrin
e
ne

Ac
ety
lc ho Se Locus coeruleus
li ne r
holine
ot

Acetylc
on

Histam
in
ine

His
tam
in e
Dorsal raphe
Pontomesencephalon

Figure 9.13  ​Brain mechanisms of sleeping and


waking
Green arrows indicate excitatory connections. Red
arrows indicate inhibitory connections. Neurotransmit-
ters are indicated where they are known. (Based on
J.-S. Lin, Hou, Sakai, & Jouvet, 1996; Robbins & Everitt,
1995; Szymusiak, 1995)

thing. The axons releasing orexin extend to the basal fore- (Zeitzer et al., 2003). Mice lacking orexin fail to sustain ac-
brain and other areas, where they stimulate neurons respon- tivities, such as running in a running wheel, and therefore fall
sible for wakefulness (Sakurai, 2007). Orexin is not necessary asleep at times when normal mice would remain alert (Anaclet
for waking up, but it is for staying awake. That is, most adult et al., 2009). Drugs that block orexin receptors increase sleep
humans stay awake for roughly 16–17 hours at a time, even (Brisbare-Roch et al., 2007), and procedures that increase
when nothing much is happening. Staying awake depends on orexin (e.g., a nasal spray of orexin) lead to increased wakeful-
orexin, especially toward the end of the day (Lee, Hassani, & ness and alertness (Deadwyler, Porrino, Siegel, & Hampson,
Jones, 2005). A study of squirrel monkeys found that orexin 2007; Prober, Rihel, Ohah, Sung, & Schier, 2006).
levels rose throughout the day and remained high when the Other pathways from the lateral hypothalamus regulate
monkeys were kept awake beyond their usual sleep time. As cells in the basal forebrain (an area just anterior and dorsal
soon as the monkeys went to sleep, the orexin levels dropped to the hypothalamus). Basal forebrain cells provide axons that
9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 281

extend throughout the thalamus and cerebral cortex (Figure a neuron is active, the increased GABA levels cut the activ-
9.13). Some of these axons release acetylcholine, which is ex- ity short and prevent axons from spreading stimulation to
citatory and tends to increase arousal (Mesulam, 1995; Szy- other areas (Massimini et al., 2005). Connections from one
musiak, 1995). Acetylcholine is released during wakefulness brain area to another become weaker (Esser, Hill, & Tononi,
and REM sleep, but not during slow-wave sleep (Hassani, 2009). When stimulation doesn’t spread, you don’t become
Lee, Henny, & Jones, 2009). During wakefulness, its release conscious of it. (Chapter 14 elaborates on that point.)
sharpens attention—that is, it increases the accurate, reliable Because sleep depends on GABA-mediated inhibition, it
detection of sensory stimuli (Goard & Dan, 2009). can be local within the brain (Krueger et al., 2008). That is,
Table 9.1 summarizes the effects of some key brain areas you might have substantial inhibition in one brain area and
on arousal and sleep. not so much in another. Ordinarily, different brain areas wake
up or go to sleep at almost the same time, but not necessarily.
The most extreme case of this principle occurs in dolphins
Stop & Check and other aquatic mammals. At night, they need to be alert
enough to surface for a breath of air. They have evolved the
9. Why do most antihistamines make people drowsy?
ability to sleep on one side of the brain at a time. That is,
10. What would happen to the sleep–wake schedule of some- the two hemispheres take turns sleeping, always leaving one
one who lacked orexin? awake enough to control swimming and breathing (Ratten-
borg, Amlaner, & Lima, 2000).
ANSWERS
and sleeping.
Thinking of sleep as a local phenomenon helps make
sense of some otherwise puzzling phenomena. Take, for
orexin would alternate between brief periods of waking

instance, sleepwalking. Almost by definition, a sleepwalker


barrier block those synapses. 10. Someone without
arousal. Antihistamines that cross the blood–brain
use histamine as their neurotransmitter to increase is awake in one part of the brain and asleep in another. An-
basal forebrain and one to the pontomesencephalon— other example is lucid dreaming. During lucid dreaming,
9. Two paths from the hypothalamus—one to the someone is dreaming but aware of being asleep and dream-
ing. Evidently some brain area is more awake than usual
during dreaming. Another example: Have you ever had the
experience of waking up but finding that you can’t move
your arms or legs? During REM sleep, cells in the pons
Sleep and the Inhibition of Brain Activity send messages that inhibit the motor neurons that control
Figure 9.13 shows axons (in red) that lead to the release of the body’s large muscles. A cat with damage to those cells
GABA, the brain’s main inhibitory transmitter. GABA is moves around awkwardly during REM sleep, as if it were
responsible for sleep. During sleep, body temperature and acting out its dreams (Morrison, Sanford, Ball, Mann, &
metabolic rate decrease slightly, as does the activity of neu- Ross, 1995) (Figure 9.14). Ordinarily, when you awaken
rons, but by less than we might expect. Spontaneously ac- from a REM period, those cells in the pons shut off and
tive neurons continue to fire at close to their usual rate, and you regain muscle control. But occasionally most of the
neurons in the brain’s sensory areas continue to respond to brain wakes up while the pons remains in REM. The result
sounds and other stimuli. Nevertheless, we are unconscious. is your experience of being temporarily unable to move—a
The reason is that GABA inhibits synaptic activity. When very unsettling experience, if you don’t understand it.

Table 9.1  Brain Structures for Arousal and Sleep

Structure Neurotransmitter(s) It Releases Effects on Behavior


Pontomesencephalon Acetylcholine, glutamate Increases cortical arousal
Locus coeruleus Norepinephrine Increases information storage during wakefulness, suppresses REM
sleep
Basal forebrain
  Excitatory cells Acetylcholine Excites thalamus and cortex; increases learning, attention; shifts
sleep from NREM to REM
  Inhibitory cells GABA Inhibits thalamus and cortex
Hypothalamus (parts) Histamine Increases arousal
(parts) Orexin Maintains wakefulness
Dorsal raphe and pons Serotonin Interrupts REM sleep

© Cengage Learning 2013


282 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Brain Function in REM Sleep


Researchers interested in the brain mechanisms of REM de-
cided to use a PET scan to determine which areas increased
or decreased in activity during REM. Although that research
might sound simple, a PET scan requires injecting a radio-
active chemical. Imagine trying to give sleepers an injection
without awakening them. Further, a PET scan yields a clear
image only if the head remains motionless during data col-
lection. If the person tosses or turns even slightly, the image
is worthless.
To overcome these difficulties, researchers in two studies
persuaded young people to sleep with their heads firmly at-
tached to masks that did not permit any movement. They also
inserted a cannula (plastic tube) into each person’s arm so that
they could inject radioactive chemicals at various times during
the night. So imagine yourself in that setup. You have a can-
nula in your arm and your head is locked into position. Now
try to sleep.
Because the researchers foresaw the difficulty of sleeping
under these conditions (!), they had their participants stay
awake the entire previous night. Someone who is tired enough
can sleep even under trying circumstances. (Maybe.)
Now that you appreciate the heroic nature of the proce-
dures, here are the results. During REM sleep, activity in-
creased in the pons (which triggers the onset of REM sleep)
and the limbic system (which is important for emotional re-
sponses). Activity decreased in the primary visual cortex, the
Figure 9.14  ​A cat with a lesion in the pons, wobbling about motor cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex but in-
during REM sleep creased in parts of the parietal and temporal cortex (Braun et
Cells of an intact pons send inhibitory messages to the spinal al., 1998; Maquet et al., 1996).
cord neurons that control the large muscles. (From Morrison, A. R., REM sleep is associated with a distinctive pattern of
Sanford, L. D., Ball, W. A., Mann, G. L., & Ross, R. J., “Stimulus-elicited high-amplitude electrical potentials known as PGO waves,
behavior in rapid eye movement sleep without atonia,” Behav- for pons-geniculate-occipital (Figure 9.15). Waves of neu-
ioral Neuroscience, 109, 972–979, 1995. Published by APA and ral activity are detected first in the pons, shortly afterward
reprinted by permission.) in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, and then
in the occipital cortex (D. C. Brooks & Bizzi, 1963; Lau-
rent, Cespuglio, & Jouvet, 1974). Each animal maintains a
nearly constant amount of PGO waves per day. During a
Stop & Check
prolonged period of REM deprivation in laboratory animals,
11. What would happen to the sleep–wake schedule of some- PGO waves begin to emerge during sleep stages 2 to 4 and
one who took a drug that blocked GABA? even during wakefulness, often in association with strange
12. Someone who has just awakened sometimes speaks in a
behaviors, as if the animal were hallucinating. At the end of
loose, unconnected, illogical way. How could you explain
the deprivation period, when an animal is permitted to sleep
this finding?
without interruption, the REM periods have an unusually
high density of PGO waves.
thinking may have an illogical, dream-like quality. REM sleep apparently depends on a relationship be-
ANSWERS
parts of the brain may still be in a REM-like state, and tween the neurotransmitters serotonin and acetylcholine.
don’t wake up all at once. Shortly after awakening, Injections of the drug carbachol, which stimulates acetyl-
ing when people usually awaken. Different brain areas choline synapses, quickly move a sleeper into REM sleep
REM period, because REM is abundant toward morn- (Baghdoyan, Spotts, & Snyder, 1993). Note that acetyl-
facilitating GABA.) 12. People often awaken from a
choline is important for both wakefulness and REM sleep,
states of brain arousal. Serotonin and norepinephrine inter-
remain awake. (Tranquilizers put people to sleep by

rupt REM sleep (Boutrel, Franc, Hen, Hamon, & Adrien,


11. Someone who took a drug that blocks GABA would

1999; Singh & Mallick, 1996).


9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 283

O
Occipital cortex

Geniculate
P

Pons

Figure 9.15  ​PGO waves


PGO waves start in the pons (P) and then show up in the lateral geniculate (G) and the occipital cortex (O). Each PGO wave is synchro-
nized with an eye movement in REM sleep. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Sleep Disorders Some cases of insomnia relate to shifts in circadian


How much sleep is enough? Different people need different rhythms (MacFarlane, Cleghorn, & Brown, 1985a, 1985b).
amounts. Most adults need about 7 1/2 to 8 hours of sleep per Ordinarily, people fall asleep while their temperature is declin-
night, but some have been known to get by with less than ing and awaken while it is rising, as in Figure 9.16a. Someone
3 hours per night, without unpleasant consequences (H. S. whose rhythm is phase-delayed, as in Figure 9.16b, has trouble
Jones & Oswald, 1968; Meddis, Pearson, & Langford, 1973). falling asleep at the usual time, as if the hypothalamus thinks
Among people who ordinarily get the more typical 7 1/2 to 8 it isn’t late enough (Morris et al., 1990). Someone whose
hours of sleep, some are better than others at withstanding a rhythm is phase-advanced, as in Figure 9.16c, falls asleep easily
temporary lack of sleep. People who tolerate sleep deprivation but awakens early.
relatively well are usually “evening people,” who like to waken Another cause of insomnia is, paradoxically, the use of
late and stay up late. They tend to show greater than average tranquilizers as sleeping pills. Although tranquilizers help
levels of brain arousal, as indicated by fMRI (Caldwell et al., people fall asleep, repeated use causes dependence and an
2005). inability to sleep without the pills (Kales, Scharf, & Kales,
The best gauge of insomnia—inadequate sleep—is how 1978). Similar problems arise when people use alcohol to get
someone feels the following day. If you feel tired during the to sleep.
day, you are not sleeping enough at night. Causes of insomnia
include noise, uncomfortable temperatures, stress, pain, diet,
and medications. Insomnia can also be the result of epilepsy, Sleep Apnea
Parkinson’s disease, brain tumors, depression, anxiety, or other One type of insomnia is sleep apnea, impaired ability to
neurological or psychiatric conditions. Some children suffer breathe while sleeping. People with sleep apnea have breath-
insomnia because they are milk-intolerant, and their parents, less periods of a minute or so from which they awaken gasp-
not realizing the intolerance, give them milk to drink right ing for breath. They may not remember all their awakenings,
before bedtime (Horne, 1992). One man suffered insomnia although they certainly notice the consequences—sleepiness
until he realized that he dreaded going to sleep because he during the day, impaired attention, depression, and some-
hated waking up to go jogging. After he switched his jogging times heart problems. People with sleep apnea have multiple
time to late afternoon, he slept without difficulty. In short, try brain areas that appear to have lost neurons, and conse-
to identify the reasons for your sleep problems before you try quently, they show deficiencies of learning, reasoning, atten-
to solve them. tion, and impulse control (Beebe & Gozal, 2002; Macey et
284 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Sleep period

(a) Normal circadian rhythm of body temperature

© Russell D. Curtis/Photo Researchers


3-hr
Difficulty
phase delay
getting to sleep

Sleep period

(b) Phase delay


Figure 9.17  ​A Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP)
mask
3-hr The mask fits snugly over the nose and delivers air at a fixed pres-
Difficulty
phase advance sure, strong enough to keep the breathing passages open.
staying asleep

Sleep period Stop & Check


(c) Phase advance
13. What kinds of people are most likely to develop sleep
apnea?

Figure 9.16  ​Insomnia and circadian rhythms ANSWER


middle-aged men.
A delay in the circadian rhythm of body temperature is associated a genetic predisposition, old people, and overweight
with onset insomnia. An advance is associated with termination 13. Sleep apnea is most common among people with
insomnia. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

al., 2002). These correlational data do not tell us whether the Narcolepsy
brain abnormalities led to sleep apnea or sleep apnea led to Narcolepsy, a condition characterized by frequent periods
the brain abnormalities. However, research with rodents sug- of sleepiness during the day (Aldrich, 1998), strikes about
gests the latter: Mice that are subjected to frequent periods 1 person in 1,000. It sometimes runs in families, but many
of low oxygen (as if they hadn’t been breathing) lose some cases emerge in people with no affected relatives. No gene for
neurons and impair others, especially in areas responsible narcolepsy has been identified, and many people with narco-
for alertness (Zhu et al., 2007). Sleep impairments may be lepsy have no close relatives with the disease. Narcolepsy has
responsible for cognitive loss not only in people with sleep four main symptoms, although not every patient has all four.
apnea but also in some with Alzheimer’s disease. Each of these symptoms can be interpreted as an intrusion of
Sleep apnea results from several causes, including genetics, a REM-like state into wakefulness:
hormones, and old-age deterioration of the brain mechanisms 1. Gradual or sudden attacks of sleepiness during the day.
that regulate breathing. Another cause is obesity, especially in
2. Occasional cataplexy—an attack of muscle weakness
middle-aged men. Many obese men have narrower than nor-
while the person remains awake. Cataplexy is often
mal airways and have to compensate by breathing frequently
triggered by strong emotions, such as anger or great
or vigorously. During sleep, they cannot keep up that rate of
excitement. (One man suddenly collapsed during his own
breathing. Furthermore, their airways become even narrower
wedding ceremony.)
than usual when they adopt a sleeping posture (Mezzanotte,
Tangel, & White, 1992). 3. Sleep paralysis—an inability to move while falling asleep
People with sleep apnea are advised to lose weight and or waking up. Other people may experience sleep
avoid alcohol and tranquilizers (which impair the breathing paralysis occasionally, but people with narcolepsy
muscles). Medical options include surgery to remove tissue experience it more frequently.
that obstructs the trachea (the breathing passage) or a mask 4. Hypnagogic hallucinations—dreamlike experiences that
that covers the nose and delivers air under enough pressure to the person has trouble distinguishing from reality, often
keep the breathing passages open (Figure 9.17). occurring at the onset of sleep.
9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 285

The cause relates to the neurotransmitter orexin. Peo- havior disorder move around vigorously during their REM
ple with narcolepsy lack the hypothalamic cells that pro- periods, apparently acting out their dreams. They frequently
duce and release orexin (Thanickal et al., 2000). Why they dream about defending themselves against attack, and they
lack them is unknown, but one possibility is an autoim- may punch, kick, and leap about. Most of them injure them-
mune reaction, in which the immune system attacks part selves or other people and damage property (Olson, Boeve, &
of the body—in this case, cells with orexin (Hallmayer Silber, 2000).
et al., 2009). Recall that orexin is important for maintaining REM behavior disorder occurs mostly in older people,
wakefulness. Consequently, people lacking orexin alternate especially older men with brain diseases such as Parkinson’s
between short waking periods and short sleepy periods, in- disease (Olson et al., 2000). Presumably, the damage includes
stead of staying awake throughout the day. Dogs that lack the cells in the pons that send messages to inhibit the spinal
the gene for orexin receptors have symptoms much like neurons that control large muscle movements.
human narcolepsy, with frequent alternations between
wakefulness and sleep (L. Lin et al., 1999). The same is
true for mice that lack orexin (Hara, 2001; Mochizuki Night Terrors and Sleepwalking
et al., 2004). Night terrors are experiences of intense anxiety from which
As discussed in Chapter 8, people with Huntington’s dis- a person awakens screaming in terror. A night terror is more
ease have widespread damage in the basal ganglia. In addition, severe than a nightmare, which is simply an unpleasant dream.
most lose neurons in the hypothalamus, including the neu- Night terrors occur during NREM sleep and are more com-
rons that make orexin. As a result, they have problems stay- mon in children than adults. Dream content, if any, is usually
ing awake during the day and difficulty staying asleep at night simple, such as a single image.
(Morton et al., 2005). Sleepwalking runs in families and occurs mostly in chil-
Theoretically, we might imagine combating narcolepsy dren. Most people who sleepwalk, and many of their rela-
with drugs that restore orexin. Perhaps eventually, such drugs tives, have one or more additional sleep difficulties, such as
will become available. Currently, the most common treatment chronic snoring, disordered sleep breathing, bedwetting,
is stimulant drugs, such as methylphenidate (Ritalin), which and night terrors (Cao & Guilleminault, 2010). The causes
enhance dopamine and norepinephrine activity. of sleepwalking are not well understood, but it is more
common when people are sleep deprived or under unusual
stress (Zadra & Pilon, 2008). It is most common during
Stop & Check stage 3 or 4 sleep early in the night and is usually not accom-
14. What is the relationship between orexin and narcolepsy? panied by dreaming. (It does not occur during REM sleep,
when the large muscles are completely relaxed.) Sleepwalk-
ANSWER
bouts of sleepiness during the day. ing is usually harmless but not always. One teenage girl
tors for orexin develop narcolepsy, characterized by walked out of her house, climbed a crane, and went back
people or animals lacking either orexin or the recep- to sleep on a support beam. Fortunately, a pedestrian saw
14. Orexin is important for staying awake. Therefore, her and called the police. Sleepwalkers have been known to
eat, rearrange furniture, fall off balconies, and drive cars—
while disregarding lanes and traffic lights. Unlike wakeful
actions, the deeds of sleepwalkers are poorly planned and
Periodic Limb Movement Disorder not remembered. Evidently, parts of the brain are awake and
Another sleep disorder is periodic limb movement disor- other parts are asleep (Gunn & Gunn, 2007). Incidentally,
der, characterized by repeated involuntary movement of the contrary to common sayings, it is not dangerous to awaken
legs and sometimes the arms (Edinger et al., 1992). Many a sleepwalker. It is not particularly helpful either, but it is
people, perhaps most, experience an occasional involuntary not dangerous.
kick, especially when starting to fall asleep. Leg movements An analogous condition is sleep sex or “sexsomnia,” in
are not a problem unless they become persistent. In some which sleeping people engage in sexual behavior, either with
people, mostly middle-aged and older, the legs kick once a partner or by masturbation, and do not remember it after-
every 20 to 30 seconds for minutes or hours, mostly dur- ward. Sexsomnia poses a threat to romances and marriages.
ing NREM sleep. Frequent or especially vigorous leg move- As one woman said, “After getting married a few years ago,
ments may awaken the person or his or her partner. In some my husband told me I was masturbating in my sleep. I was
cases, tranquilizers help suppress the movements (Schenck mortified, thinking back to all the slumber parties as a girl,
& Mahowald, 1996). and then when I was older and my little sister stayed the
night at my house! How many others might have witnessed
and not said anything? My new marriage is on the rocks,
REM Behavior Disorder since I’m having such good sex in my sleep, I have NO de-
For most people, the major postural muscles are relaxed and sire while I’m awake. This is killing my relationship with my
inactive during REM sleep. However, people with REM be- husband.” (Mangan, 2004, p. 290)
286 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Module 9.2  ■  In Closing

Stages of Sleep

Chemists divide the world into different elements, biologists biologically and psychologically important. It also demon-
divide life into different species, and physicians distinguish strated that external measurements—in this case, EEG
one disease from another. Similarly, psychologists try to rec- recordings—can be used to identify internal experiences. We
ognize the most natural or useful distinctions among types of now take it largely for granted that an electrical or magnetic
behavior or experience. The discovery of different stages of recording from the brain can tell us something about a per-
sleep was a major landmark in psychology because research- son’s experience, but it is worth pausing to note what a sur-
ers found a previously unrecognized distinction that is both prising discovery that was in its time.

Summary
1. During sleep, brain activity decreases, but a stimulus can 6. Orexin is a peptide that maintains wakefulness. Cells in
awaken the person. Someone in a coma cannot be the lateral and posterior nuclei of the hypothalamus
awakened. A vegetative state or minimally conscious state release this peptide.  279
can last months or years, during which the person shows 7. During sleep, enhanced release of GABA limits
only limited responses. Brain death is a condition without neuronal activity and blocks the spread of activation.
brain activity or responsiveness of any kind.  276 Sometimes this suppression is stronger in one brain
2. Over the course of about 90 minutes, a sleeper goes area than another. That is, sleep can occur in one brain
through stages 1, 2, 3, and 4 and then returns through area and not another at a given time.  281
stages 3 and 2 to a stage called REM. REM is character- 8. REM sleep is associated with increased activity in a
ized by rapid eye movements, more brain activity than number of brain areas, including the pons and limbic
other sleep stages, complete relaxation of the trunk system. Activity decreases in the prefrontal cortex, the
muscles, irregular breathing and heart rate, penile motor cortex, and the primary visual cortex.  282
erection or vaginal lubrication, and an increased
9. REM sleep begins with PGO waves, which are waves of
probability of vivid dreams.  276
brain activity transmitted from the pons to the lateral
3. REM sleep or paradoxical sleep is a condition marked geniculate to the occipital lobe.  282
by more cortical activity than other sleep, complete
10. People with sleep apnea have long periods without
relaxation of the body’s postural muscles, and an
breathing while they sleep. Many have indications of
increased probability of dreaming.  277
neuronal loss, probably as a result of decreased oxygen
4. The brain has multiple systems for arousal. The pontomes- while they sleep.  283
encephalon and parts of the hypothalamus control various
11. People with narcolepsy have attacks of sleepiness during
cell clusters in the basal forebrain that send axons releasing
the day. Narcolepsy is associated with a deficiency of the
acetylcholine throughout much of the forebrain.  279
peptide neurotransmitter orexin.  284
5. The locus coeruleus is active in response to meaningful
events. It facilitates attention and new learning; it also
blocks the onset of REM sleep.  279

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
alpha waves  276 night terrors  285 REM behavior disorder  285
basal forebrain  280 non-REM (NREM) sleep  278 reticular formation  279
brain death  276 orexin (or hypocretin)  279 sleep apnea  283
coma  276 paradoxical sleep  277 sleep spindle  276
insomnia  283 periodic limb movement disorder  285 slow-wave sleep (SWS)  277
K-complex  276 PGO waves  282 vegetative state  276
locus coeruleus  279 polysomnograph  276
minimally conscious state  276 pontomesencephalon  279
narcolepsy  284 rapid eye movement (REM) sleep  278
9.2 Stages of Sleep and Brain Mechanisms 287

Thought Question

When cats are deprived of REM sleep, longer periods of no additional rebound. Speculate on a possible explanation.
deprivation—up to about 25 days—are associated with (Hint: Consider what happens to PGO waves during REM
greater rebound of REM when they can sleep uninterrupted. deprivation.)
However, REM deprivation for more than 25 days produces
Module 9.3

Why Sleep? Why REM?


Why Dreams?

W
hy do you sleep? “That’s easy,” you reply. “I sleep Sleep and Energy Conservation
because I get tired.” Well, yes, but you are not Even if we identified what seems to be the most important
tired in the sense of muscle fatigue. You need function of sleep for humans today, it might not be the func-
almost as much sleep after a day of sitting around the house tion for which sleep originally evolved. By analogy, consider
as after a day of intense physical or mental activity (Horne computers: People use computers today to write papers, send
& Minard, 1985; Shapiro, Bortz, Mitchell, Bartel, & Jooste, e-mail, search the Internet, play video games, store and dis-
1981). Furthermore, you could rest your muscles just as play photographs, play music, and find a date. Someone who
well while awake as while asleep. (In fact, if your muscles didn’t know the history might not guess that computers were
ache after strenuous exercise, you probably find it difficult built originally for mathematical calculations.
to sleep.) Similarly, sleep probably started with a simple function to
You feel tired at the end of the day because inhibitory pro- which evolution added others later. Even bacteria have circa-
cesses in your brain force you to become less aroused and less dian rhythms of activity and inactivity (Mihalcescu, Hsing, &
alert. That is, we evolved mechanisms to force us to sleep. Why? Leibler, 2004). What benefit of sleep applies to species with
little or no nervous system?
A likely hypothesis is that sleep’s original function—
Functions of Sleep and still an important one—is to save energy (Kleitman,
Sleep serves many functions. During sleep, we rest our mus- 1963; Siegel, 2009; Webb, 1974). Nearly every species is
cles, decrease metabolism, rebuild proteins in the brain (Kong more efficient at some times of day than at others. Those
et al., 2002), reorganize synapses, and strengthen memories with good vision are more efficient in the day. Those that
(Sejnowski & Destexhe, 2000). People who don’t get enough rely on olfaction instead of vision are more efficient at night,
sleep have trouble concentrating and become more vulnerable when their predators cannot see them. Sleep conserves en-
to illness, especially mental illness (Wulff, Gatti, Wettstein, & ergy during the inefficient times, when activity would be
Foster, 2010). We all have moments when our attention lapses wasteful and possibly dangerous. NASA’s Rover spacecraft,
and we fail to notice important stimuli. Those periods are lon- built to explore Mars, had a mechanism to make it “sleep”
ger and more frequent after a sleepless night. Furthermore, at night to conserve its batteries. During sleep, a mammal’s
people who have had enough sleep notice their lapses and body temperature decreases by 1º or 2º C, enough to save a
jar themselves into increased arousal. People who are sleep- significant amount of energy. Muscle activity decreases, sav-
deprived fail to do so (Chee et al., 2008). Inadequate sleep is a ing more energy. Animals increase their sleep duration dur-
major cause of accidents by workers and poor performance by ing food shortages, when energy conservation is especially
college students. Driving while sleep deprived is comparable important (Berger & Phillips, 1995).
to driving under the influence of alcohol (Falleti, Maruff, Col- Sleep is therefore in some ways analogous to hiberna-
lie, Darby, & McStephen, 2003). tion. Hibernation is a true need. A ground squirrel that is
People working in Antarctica during the winter sleep prevented from hibernating can become as disturbed as a
poorly and feel depressed (Palinkas, 2003). Even one night of person who is prevented from sleeping. However, the func-
sleeplessness activates the immune system (Matsumoto et al., tion of hibernation is simply to conserve energy while food
2001). That is, you react to sleep deprivation as if you were ill. is scarce.
With more prolonged sleep deprivation, people report dizzi- If one of the main functions of sleep is to shut down activ-
ness, tremors, and hallucinations (Dement, 1972; L. C. John- ity at times of relative inefficiency, we might expect to find lit-
son, 1969). tle or no sleep in species that are equally effective at all times of
Clearly, we need to sleep. Is there, however, one primary day. Indeed, that expectation appears to be confirmed. Certain
or original reason? fish have evolved for life in a cave where “day” and “night” have
288
9.3 Why Sleep? Why REM? Why Dreams? 289

no meaning, because light is always absent and temperature is Stop & Check
virtually constant. Observers report that these fish apparently
15. What kind of animal tends to get more than the average
never sleep (Kavanau, 1998).
amount of sleep?
Several other species turn off their need for sleep un-
der certain circumstances (Siegel, 2009). After a dolphin 16. What might one predict about the sleep of fish that live
or whale gives birth, both mother and baby stay awake deep in the ocean?
24 hours a day for the first couple of weeks while the baby
is especially vulnerable. Neither shows any sign of harm ANSWERS
conserve energy at one time more than another.
from sleep deprivation. Migratory birds face a different
efficient at all times of day and have no reason to

kind of problem. During a week or two in fall and spring,


fish might not need to sleep because they are equally
light and no difference between day and night. These
they forage for food during the day and do their migra- armadillos). 16. The deep ocean, like a cave, has no
tory flying at night. (Flying at night makes sense, because are unlikely to be attacked during their sleep (such as
it is cooler then.) That schedule leaves little time for sleep. 15. Predators get much sleep, and so do species that
They apparently decrease their need for sleep at this time.
If a bird is kept in a cage during the migration season, it
flutters around restlessly at night, sleeping only one third
its usual amount. It compensates to some extent with many
brief periods of drowsiness (less than 30 seconds each)
during the day (Fuchs, Haney, Jechura, Moore, & Bing-
man, 2006). Still, it is getting very little sleep, while re- Applications and Extensions
maining alert and performing normally on learning tasks.
If the same bird is deprived of sleep during other seasons of Hibernation
the year, its performance suffers (Rattenborg et al., 2004). Hibernating animals decrease their body temperature to
Exactly how a bird or a mother dolphin decreases its sleep only slightly above that of the environment (except that
need is unknown, but the fact that it is possible fits with they don’t let it go low enough for their blood to freeze).
the idea that sleep is primarily a way to conserve energy, Brain activity declines to almost nothing, neuron cell
rather than a way to fulfill a function that one could not bodies shrink, and dendrites lose almost one fourth of
fulfill in other ways. their branches, replacing them when body temperature
Animal species vary in their sleep habits in ways that increases (von der Ohe, Darian-Smith, Garner, & Heller,
make sense if we ask how many hours the animal needs to 2006). A few curious facts about hibernation: 
be awake, and therefore how long it can afford to spend con-
1. Hibernation occurs in certain small mammals, such
serving energy (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Campbell & To-
as ground squirrels and bats. Bears’ hibernation dif-
bler, 1984). Grazing animals that need to eat for many hours
fers from that of small animals. Bears lower their
per day get less sleep than carnivores (meat eaters) that can
body temperature from 37° to 33° C and maintain a
satisfy their nutritional needs with a single meal. Animals
lowered but steady metabolic rate throughout hiber-
that need to be on the alert for predators get little sleep,
nation. Even after awakening from hibernation, their
whereas the predators themselves sleep easily. Insect-eating
metabolic rate recovers slowly (Toien et al., 2011).
bats are active in the early evening, when moths and similar
insects are most abundant, and then they sleep the rest of 2. Hamsters also hibernate. If you keep your pet ham-
the day (Figure 9.18). ster in a cool, dimly lit place during the winter, and it
Here’s another bit of miscellaneous trivia about animal appears to have died, make sure that it is not just
sleep: Swifts are small, dark birds that chase insects. They get hibernating before you bury it!
all the nutrition and water they need from the insects. When 3. Hibernating animals other than bears come out of
a baby European swift first takes off from its nest, how long hibernation for a few hours every few days, raising
would you guess its first flight lasts, until it comes to land their body temperature to about normal. However,
again? they spend most of this nonhibernating time asleep
The answer: up to 2 years. Except during treacherous (B. M. Barnes, 1996).
storms, it doesn’t come down until it is old enough to mate 4. Hibernation retards the aging process. Hamsters
and build a nest. In the meantime, it spends both days and that spend longer times hibernating have proportion-
nights in the air. At night it heads into the wind, sticks out ately longer life expectancies than other hamsters
its wings, glides, and presumably sleeps—although confirm- do (Lyman, O’Brien, Greene, & Papafrangos, 1981).
ing sleep would require measuring the EEG of a small bird in Hibernation is also a period of relative invulnerability
flight. It picks an altitude where the air is not too cold, accepts to infection and trauma. Procedures that would ordi-
the risk of being blown a great distance, and awakens the next narily damage the brain, such as inserting a needle
morning to resume its chase of flying insects (Bäckman & Al- into it, produce little if any harm during hibernation
erstam, 2001). (F. Zhou et al., 2001). ■
290 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

Much sleep per day Sleep and Memory


Another apparent function of sleep is improved
19.9 hr Bat
memory. Young adults deprived of a night’s sleep
show deficits on memory tasks (Yoo, Hu, Gujar,
18.5 hr Armadillo Jolesz, & Walker, 2007). In contrast, if people learn
something and then go to sleep, or even take a nap,
their memory often improves beyond what it was
14.5 hr Cat
before the sleep (Hu, Stylos-Allan, & Walker, 2006;
Korman et al., 2007). That is, we see not just an ab-
Moderate amount of sleep per day sence of forgetting but also a gain of memory. The
amount of improvement varies from one study to
9.8 hr Fox another and from one type of learning task to an-
other (Cai & Rickard; Cai, Shuman, Gorman, Sage,
Rhesus monkey & Anagnostaras, 2009; Doyon et al., 2009). Still, it
9.6 hr
appears that reviewing something right before you
go to sleep is an excellent idea.
8.4 hr Rabbit Sleep enhances memory of some events more
than others. In one study, people viewed 50 ob-
Human jects making a sound (such as a cat meowing) in
8.0 hr
various locations on a computer screen. Then they
took a nap, during which they heard some of those
Little sleep, easily aroused sounds again. After the nap, they were tested on
their memory for the location of each object on the
3.9 hr Cow
screen. They showed enhanced memory for the ob-
jects whose sounds they heard during the nap (Ru-
3.8 hr Sheep doy, Voss, Westerberg, & Paller, 2009). Evidently the
sounds reminded them of the information, and the
brain then processed that information again.
3.8 hr Goat
Sleep also helps people reanalyze their memo-
ries: In one study, people who had just practiced a
2.9 hr Horse complex task were more likely to perceive a hidden
rule (an “aha” experience) after a period of sleep
than after a similar period of wakefulness (Wag-
Figure 9.18  ​Hours of sleep per day for various species ner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004). An-
Generally, predators and others that are safe when they sleep tend to sleep other study found that a nap that included REM
a great deal. Animals in danger of being attacked while they sleep spend less
sleep enhanced performance on certain kinds of
time asleep. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
creative problem solving (Cai, Mednick, Harrison,
Kanady, & Mednick, 2009).
How does sleep enhance memory? Research-
ers recorded activity in the hippocampus during learning,
and then recorded from the same locations during sleep, us-
ing microelectrodes within cells for laboratory animals and
electrodes on the scalp for humans. The results: Patterns that
occurred during sleep resembled those that occurred dur-
ing learning, except that they were more rapid during sleep.
Furthermore, the amount of hippocampal activity during
sleep correlated highly with the subsequent improvement in
performance (Derégnaucourt, Mitra, Fehér, Pytte, & Tch-
ernichovski, 2005; Euston, Tatsuno, & McNaughton, 2007;
Alan Williams/Alamy

Huber, Ghilardi, Massimini, & Tononi, 2004; Ji & Wilson,


2007; Maquet et al., 2000; Peigneux et al., 2004). These re-
sults suggest that the brain replays its daily experiences during
sleep. However, further research found that the sleeping brain
A European swift replays its experience backward as often as forward, and that it
sometimes replays less common experiences more often than
9.3 Why Sleep? Why REM? Why Dreams? 291

more common ones (Gupta, van der Meer, Touretzky, & Re-
Stop & Check
dish, 2010). Also, the hippocampus replays recently learned
patterns during quiet waking periods, not just during sleep 17. Does sleep improve memory by strengthening or weaken-
(Karlsson & Frank, 2009). So the role of hippocampal replay ing synapses?
during sleep is less clear than it once appeared to be.
One way for sleep to strengthen memory is by weeding out
strengthened ones to stand out by contrast.
ANSWER
the less successful connections. In Chapter 13, we shall exam-
Weakening these less relevant synapses enables the
synapses that were not strengthened during the day.
ine the phenomenon of long-term potentiation, the ability of 17. The evidence so far points to weakening the
new experiences to strengthen synaptic connections. Suppose
that every time you learn something, your brain strengthened
certain synapses without making adjustments elsewhere. As
you learned more and more, you would have more and more
brain activity. By middle age, your brain might be burning with Functions of REM Sleep
constant activity. To prevent runaway overactivity, your brain An average person spends about one third of his or her life
compensates for strengthening some synapses by weakening asleep and about one fifth of sleep in REM, totaling about
others, mostly during sleep (Liu, Faraguna, Cirelli, Tononi, & 600 hours of REM per year. Presumably, REM serves a bio-
Gao, 2010; Vyazovskiy, Cirelli, Pfister-Genskow, Faraguna, & logical function. But what is it?
Tononi, 2008). Weakening synapses during sleep emphasizes One way to approach this question is to compare the peo-
the ones that were strengthened during wakefulness. ple or animals with more REM to those with less. REM sleep
Another aspect of sleep’s contribution to memory relates is widespread in mammals and birds, indicating that it is part
to sleep spindles. Recall that sleep spindles are waves of activity, of our ancient evolutionary heritage. Some species, however,
about 12–14 Hz, that occur mostly during stage 2 sleep. They have far more than others. As a rule, the species with the most
indicate an exchange of information between the thalamus and total sleep hours also have the highest percentage of REM
cerebral cortex. In both rats and humans, sleep spindles increase sleep ( J. M. Siegel, 1995). Cats spend up to 16 hours a day
in number after new learning (Eschenko, Mölle, Born, & Sara, sleeping, much or most of it in REM sleep. Rabbits, guinea
2006). Most people are fairly consistent in their amount of spin- pigs, and sheep sleep less and spend little time in REM.
dle activity from one night to another, and the amount of spindle Figure 9.19 illustrates the relationship between age and
activity correlates more than .7 with nonverbal tests of IQ (Fo- REM sleep for humans. The trend is the same for other mam-
gel, Nader, Cote, & Smith, 2007). Who would have guessed that malian species. Infants get more REM and more total sleep
brain waves during sleep could predict IQ scores? than adults do, confirming the pattern that more total sleep
predicts a higher percentage of
24 REM sleep. Among adult humans,
those who sleep 9 or more hours
16 per night have the highest percent-
14 age of REM sleep, and those who
sleep 5 or fewer hours have the low-
12 est percentage. This pattern implies
that although REM is no doubt
10 Waking
Hours

important, NREM is more tightly


8 regulated. That is, the amount of
REM sleep NREM varies less among individ-
6 uals and among species.
4 One hypothesis is that REM is
NREM sleep Total daily
important for memory storage, es-
2 sleep pecially for weakening the inappro-
priate connections (Crick & Mitchi-
0
1-15 3-5 6-23 2-3 3-5 5-9 10-13 14-1819-30 33-45 50-70 70-85 son, 1983). REM and non-REM
Day Mo Mo Yr Yr Yr Yr Yr Yr Yr Yr Yr sleep may be important for consoli-
dating different types of memories.
Figure 9.19  ​Sleep patterns for people of various ages Depriving people of sleep early in
REM sleep occupies about 8 hours a day in newborns but less than 2 hours in most adults.
the night (mostly non-REM sleep)
The sleep of infants is not quite like that of adults, however, and the criteria for identifying REM
impairs verbal learning, such as
sleep are not the same. (From “Ontogenetic Development of Human Sleep-Dream Cycle,” by H.
P. Roffwarg, J. N. Muzio, and W. C. Dement, Science, 152, 1966, 604–609. Copyright 1966 AAAS.
memorizing a list of words, whereas
Reprinted by permission.) depriving people of sleep during
the second half of the night (more
292 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

REM) impairs consolidation of learned motor skills (Gais, Pli- spontaneous activity in the pons—the PGO waves previously
hal, Wagner, & Born, 2000; Plihal & Born, 1997). described—that activate some parts of the cortex but not
However, many people take antidepressant drugs that se- others. The cortex combines this haphazard input with what-
verely decrease REM sleep, without incurring memory prob- ever other activity was already occurring and does its best to
lems (Rasch, Pommer, Diekelmann, & Born, 2009). Research synthesize a story that makes sense of the information (Hob-
on laboratory animals indicates that these drugs sometimes son & McCarley, 1977; Hobson, Pace-Schott, & Stickgold,
even enhance memory (Parent, Habib, & Baker, 1999). 2000; McCarley & Hoffman, 1981). Sensory stimuli, such
Another hypothesis sounds odd because we tend to imag- as sounds in the room, occasionally get incorporated into a
ine a glamorous role for REM sleep: David Maurice (1998) dream, although usually they do not (Nir & Tononi, 2010).
proposed that REM just shakes the eyeballs back and forth Consider how this theory handles a couple of common
enough to get sufficient oxygen to the corneas of the eyes. The dreams. Most people have had occasional dreams of falling
corneas, unlike the rest of the body, get oxygen directly from the or flying. Well, while you are asleep, you lie flat, unlike your
surrounding air. During sleep, because they are shielded from posture for the rest of the day. Your brain in its partly aroused
the air, they deteriorate slightly (Hoffmann & Curio, 2003). condition feels the vestibular sensation of your position and
They do get some oxygen from the fluid behind them (see Fig- interprets it as flying or falling. Have you ever dreamed that
ure 6.2), but when the eyes are motionless, that fluid becomes you were trying to move but couldn’t? Most people have. An
stagnant. Moving the eyes increases the oxygen supply to the interpretation based on the activation-synthesis theory is that
corneas. According to this view, REM is a way of arousing a during REM sleep (which accompanies most dreams), your
sleeper just enough to shake the eyes back and forth, and the motor cortex is inactive and your major postural muscles are
other manifestations of REM are just byproducts. This idea virtually paralyzed. That is, when you are dreaming, you re-
makes sense of the fact that REM occurs mostly toward the ally can’t move, you feel your lack of movement, and thus, you
end of the night’s sleep, when the fluid behind the eyes would be dream of failing to move.
the most stagnant. It also makes sense of the fact that individu- One criticism is that the theory’s predictions are vague. If
als who spend more hours asleep devote a greater percentage we dream about falling because of the vestibular sensations
of sleep to REM. (If you don’t sleep long, you have less need from lying down, why don’t we always dream of falling? If we
to shake up the stagnant fluid.) However, as mentioned, many dream we can’t move because our muscles are paralyzed during
people take antidepressants that restrict REM sleep. They are REM sleep, why don’t we always dream of being paralyzed?
not known to suffer damage to the cornea.
The Clinico-Anatomical Hypothesis
Stop & Check
An alternative view of dreams has been labeled the clinico-
18. What kinds of individuals get more REM sleep than oth- anatomical hypothesis because it was derived from clini-
ers? (Think in terms of age, species, and long versus cal studies of patients with various kinds of brain damage
short sleepers.) (Solms, 1997, 2000). Like the activation-synthesis theory,
this theory emphasizes that dreams begin with arousing
stimuli that are generated within the brain combined with
day.
ANSWER
who get little, and of species that sleep much of the
the old, of individuals who get much sleep than those recent memories and any information the brain is receiving
18. Much REM sleep is more typical of the young than from the senses. However, the clinico-anatomical hypoth-
esis puts less emphasis on the pons, PGO waves, or REM
sleep. It regards dreams as thinking that takes place under
unusual conditions, similar to mind-wandering during ev-
eryday life (Domhoff, 2011).
Biological Perspectives on One of those conditions is that the brain is getting little
information from the sense organs, and the primary visual
Dreaming and auditory areas of the cortex have lower than usual activ-
Dream research faces a special problem: All we know about ity, so other brain areas are free to generate images without
dreams comes from people’s self-reports, and researchers have constraints or interference. Also, the primary motor cortex is
no way to check the accuracy of those reports. In fact, we for- suppressed, as are the motor neurons of the spinal cord, so
get most dreams, and even when we do remember them, the arousal cannot lead to action. Activity is suppressed in the
details fade quickly. prefrontal cortex, which is important for working memory
(memory of very recent events). Consequently, we not only
forget most dreams after we awaken, but we also lose track of
The Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis what has been happening within a dream, and sudden scene
According to the activation-synthesis hypothesis, a dream changes are common. We also lose a sense of volition—that
represents the brain’s effort to make sense of sparse and dis- is, planning (Hobson, 2009). It seems that events just happen,
torted information. Dreams begin with periodic bursts of without any intention on our part.
9.3 Why Sleep? Why REM? Why Dreams? 293

Meanwhile, activity is relatively high in the inferior (lower) activation-synthesis hypothesis, is hard to test because it does
part of the parietal cortex, an area important for visuospatial not make specific predictions about who will have what dream
perception. Patients with damage here have problems bind- and when.
ing body sensations with vision. They also report no dreams.
Fairly high activity is also found in the areas of visual cortex
outside V1. Those areas are presumably important for the vi- Stop & Check
sual imagery that accompanies most dreams. Finally, activity is
high in the hypothalamus, amygdala, and other areas impor- 19. What is a key point of disagreement between the
tant for emotions and motivations (Gvilia, Turner, McGinty, activation-synthesis hypothesis and the clinico-anatomical
& Szymusiak, 2006). hypothesis?
So the idea is that either internal or external stimulation
activates parts of the parietal, occipital, and temporal cortex.
more emphasis on the importance of the pons.
ANSWER
19. The activation-synthesis hypothesis puts much
The arousal develops into a hallucinatory perception, with no
sensory input from area V1 to override it. This idea, like the

Module 9.3  ■  In Closing

Our Limited Self-Understanding

Without minimizing how much we do understand about sleep, cupy so much of our time underscores a point about the biology
it is noteworthy how many basic questions remain. What is the of behavior: We evolved tendencies to behave in certain ways
function of REM sleep? Does dreaming have a function, or is it that lead to survival and reproduction. The behavior can serve its
just an accident? Our lack of knowledge about activities that oc- function even when we do not fully understand that function.

Summary
1. One important function of sleep is to conserve energy at 4. According to the activation-synthesis hypothesis, dreams
a time when the individual would be less efficient. are the brain’s attempts to make sense of the information
Animal species vary in their sleep per day depending on reaching it, based mostly on haphazard input originating
their feeding habits and how much danger they face while in the pons.  292
asleep.  288 5. According to the clinico-anatomical hypothesis, dreams
2. In addition to saving energy, sleep serves other functions, originate partly with external stimuli but mostly from the
including enhancement of memory.  290 brain’s own motivations, memories, and arousal. The
3. REM sleep occupies the greatest percentage of sleep in stimulation often produces peculiar results because it
individuals and species that sleep the most total does not have to compete with normal visual input and
hours.  291 does not get organized by the prefrontal cortex.  292

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
activation-synthesis clinico-anatomical hypothesis  292
hypothesis  292

Thought Question

Why would it be harder to deprive someone of just NREM


sleep than just REM sleep?
294 Chapter 9 Wakefulness and Sleep

chapter 9 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note-taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.

Animations
The Retina nnn (Figure x.x) Also available—
■ Cellular Clock
■ Electroencephalography
■ Pathways of Sleep and Waking
© 2013 Cengage Learning

■ Learning Module: Sleep Patterns

Sleep Rhythms
9.3 Why Sleep? Why REM? Why Dreams? 295

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Dement, W. C. (1992). The sleepwatchers. Stanford, CA: Stanford Alumni Association. Fascinat-
ing, entertaining account of sleep research by one of its leading pioneers.
Foster, R. G., & Kreitzman, L. (2004). Rhythms of life. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Nontechnical discussion of research on circadian rhythms.
Moorcroft, W. H. (2003). Understanding sleep and dreaming. New York: Kluwer. Excellent review
of the psychology and neurology of sleep and dreams.
Refinetti, R. (2005). Circadian physiology (2nd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Marvelous sum-
mary of research on circadian rhythms and the relevance to human behavior.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, including sites on sleep, dreams, and sleep disorders.
Gail R. Hill, Bimbimbie Ornithological Services
Internal Regulation 10
Chapter Outline Main Ideas
Module 10.1 ​Temperature Regulation 1. Many physiological and behavioral processes maintain a
Homeostasis and Allostasis near constancy of certain body variables. They anticipate
Controlling Body Temperature needs as well as react to them.
In Closing: Combining Physiological and Behavioral 2. Mammals and birds maintain constant body temperature
Mechanisms as a way of staying ready for rapid muscle activity at any
temperature of the environment. They use both
Module 10.2 ​Thirst
behavioral and physiological processes to maintain
Mechanisms of Water Regulation
temperature.
Osmotic Thirst
Hypovolemic Thirst and Sodium-Specific Hunger 3. Thirst mechanisms respond to the osmotic pressure and
In Closing: The Psychology and Biology of Thirst total volume of the blood.
4. Hunger and satiety are regulated by many factors,
Module 10.3 ​Hunger including taste, stomach distension, the availability of
Digestion and Food Selection glucose to the cells, and chemicals released by the fat
Short- and Long-Term Regulation of Feeding cells. Many brain peptides help regulate feeding and
Brain Mechanisms satiety.
Eating Disorders
In Closing: The Multiple Controls of Hunger
Interactive Exploration and Study

W
hat is life? You could define life in many ways de-
pending on whether your purpose is medical, legal,
philosophical, or poetic. Biologically, the necessary
condition for life is a coordinated set of chemical reactions. Not
all chemical reactions are alive, but all life has well-regulated
chemical reactions.
Every chemical reaction in a living body takes place in
a water solution at a rate that depends on the identity and
concentration of molecules in the water and the tempera-
OPPOSITE: All life on Earth requires water, and animals drink it
ture of the solution. Our behavior is organized to keep the
wherever they can find it. right chemicals in the right proportions and at the right
temperature.
297
Module 10.1

Temperature Regulation

H
ere’s an observation that puzzled biologists for years: they gain insulation and prevent a rapid drop in body
When a small male garter snake emerges from temperature (Shah, Shine, Hudson, & Kearney, 2003).
hibernation in early spring, it emits female pheromones ■ The Japanese giant hornet sometimes invades bee colonies,

for the first day or two. The pheromones attract larger males that kills one or more bees, and takes them to feed to its larvae.
swarm all over him, trying to copulate. Presumably, the tendency When one of these hornets invades a hive of Japanese
to release female pheromones must have evolved to provide the honeybees, the bees form a tight swarm of more than 500,
small male some advantage. But what? Biologists speculated surrounding the hornet in a tiny ball. Why? The combined
about ways in which this pseudo-mating experience might help body heat of all those bees raises the temperature to a level
the small male attract real females. The truth is simpler: A male that kills the hornet, although bees can survive it (Ono,
that has just emerged from hibernation is so cold that it has Igarashi, Ohno, & Sasaki, 1995).
trouble slithering out of its burrow. The larger males emerged ■ Migratory birds do most of their migratory flying at night.
from hibernation earlier and already had a chance to warm Why? The nights are cooler. A bird flying in midday
themselves in a sunny place. When the larger males swarm all
over the smaller male, they warm him and increase his activity
level (Shine, Phillips, Waye, LeMaster, & Mason, 2001).
Here are more behaviors that make sense in terms of tem-
perature regulation:
■ Have you ever noticed gulls, ducks, or other large birds
standing on one leg (Figure 10.1)? Why do they do that,
when balancing on two legs would seem easier? The answer
is they stand this way in cold weather. Tucking a leg under
the body keeps it warm (Ehrlich, Dobkin, & Wheye, 1988).
■ Vultures sometimes defecate onto their own legs. Are

they just careless slobs? No. They defecate onto their


legs on hot days so that the evaporating excretions will
cool their legs (Ehrlich et al., 1988).
© F1online digitale Bildagentur GmbH/Alamy

■ Toucans are tropical birds with huge bills (Figure

10.2). For many years biologists puzzled about the


function of those huge, clumsy bills. The answer is
temperature regulation (Tattersall, Andrade, & Abe,
2009). While flying on hot days, a toucan directs more
blood flow to the beak, where the passing air cools
it. At night the toucan tucks its bill under a wing to
prevent undue loss of heat.
■ Most lizards live solitary lives, but Australian thick-
Figure 10.1  ​Why do birds sometimes stand on one foot?
tailed geckos sometimes form tight huddles. Why?
Like many other puzzling behaviors, this one makes sense in
They live in an environment with rapid temperature terms of temperature regulation. Holding one leg next to the body
fluctuations. They huddle only when the environ- keeps it warm.
mental temperature is falling rapidly. By huddling,

298
10.1 Temperature Regulation 299

©A. Blank/NAS/Photo Researchers


© hironai/Shutterstock.com

Figure 10.3  ​Difficulties of temperature regulation for a


Figure 10.2  ​Why do toucans have such huge bills? newborn rodent
They use their bills to radiate heat when they need to cool the A newborn rat has no hair, thin skin, and little body fat. If left
body. They cover the bill at night to decrease heat loss. exposed to the cold, it becomes inactive.

would overheat and frequently have to stop for a drink, mostat. When the temperature in the house drops below the
often in places where fresh water is difficult to find. minimum, the thermostat triggers the furnace to provide heat.
■ Decades ago, psychologists found that infant rats ap- When the temperature rises above the maximum, the thermo-
peared deficient in certain aspects of learning, eating, stat turns on the air conditioner.
and drinking. Later results showed that the real problem Similarly, homeostatic processes in animals trigger phys-
was temperature control. Researchers generally test ani- iological and behavioral activities that keep certain variables
mals at room temperature, about 20°–23° C (68°–73° F), within a set range. In many cases, the range is so narrow
which is comfortable for adult humans but dangerously that we refer to it as a set point, a single value that the body
cold for an isolated baby rat (Figure 10.3). In a warmer works to maintain. For example, if calcium is deficient in
room, infant rats show abilities that we once assumed your diet and its concentration in the blood begins to fall
required more brain maturity (Satinoff, 1991). below the set point of 0.16 g/L (grams per liter), storage
■ Certain studies found that female rats learned best dur-
deposits in your bones release additional calcium into the
ing their fertile period (estrus). In other studies, they blood. If the calcium level in the blood rises above 0.16 g/L,
learned best a day or two before their fertile period (pro- you store part of the excess in your bones and excrete the
estrus). The difference depends on temperature. Rats rest. Similar mechanisms maintain constant blood levels of
in estrus do better in a cooler environment, presumably water, oxygen, glucose, sodium chloride, protein, fat, and
because they are generating so much heat on their own. acidity (Cannon, 1929). Processes that reduce discrepan-
Rats in proestrus do better in a warmer environment cies from the set point are known as negative feedback.
(Rubinow, Arseneau, Beverly, & Juraska, 2004). Much of motivated behavior can be described as negative
feedback: Something causes a disturbance, and behavior
The point is that temperature affects behavior in many proceeds until it relieves the disturbance.
ways that we easily overlook. Temperature regulation is more The body’s set points change from time to time (Mros-
important than you might have guessed. ovsky, 1990). For example, many animals (including most hu-
mans) increase their body fat in fall and decrease it in spring.
Your body maintains a higher temperature during the day than
Homeostasis and Allostasis at night, even if room temperature stays constant. To describe
Physiologist Walter B. Cannon (1929) introduced the term these dynamic changes, researchers use the term allostasis
homeostasis (HO-mee-oh-STAY-sis) to refer to tempera- (from the Greek roots meaning “variable” and “standing”),
ture regulation and other biological processes that keep body which means the adaptive way in which the body changes its
variables within a fixed range. The process resembles the ther- set points depending on the situation (McEwen, 2000). As you
mostat in a house with heating and cooling systems. Someone will see throughout this chapter, much of that control depends
sets the minimum and maximum temperatures on the ther- on cells in the hypothalamus.
300 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

Stop & Check drink enough to replace the water you lose by sweating. If you
sweat without drinking, you start becoming dehydrated (low
1. How does the idea of allostasis differ from homeostasis? on water). You then protect your body water by decreasing
ing it as circumstances change. your sweat, despite the risk of overheating (Tokizawa, Yasu-
ANSWER hara, Nakamura, Uchida, Crawshaw, & Nagashima, 2010).
an adjustment of that range, increasing it or decreas-
tain body variables within a fixed range. Allostasis is Several physiological mechanisms increase your body heat
1. Homeostasis is a set of processes that keep cer- in a cold environment. One is shivering. Any muscle contrac-
tions, such as those of shivering, generate heat. Second, de-
creased blood flow to the skin prevents the blood from cooling
too much. A third mechanism works well for most mammals,
though not humans: When cold, they fluff out their fur to in-
Controlling Body Temperature crease insulation. (We humans also fluff out our “fur” by erect-
ing the tiny hairs on our skin—”goose bumps.” Back when our
If you were to list your strongest motivations in life, you might remote ancestors had a fuller coat of fur, that mechanism did
not think to include temperature regulation, but it has a high some good.)
priority biologically. An average young adult expends about We also use behavioral mechanisms, just as poikilother-
2,600 kilocalories (kcal) per day. Where do you suppose all mic animals do. In fact, we prefer to rely on behavior when
that energy goes? It is not to muscle movements or mental we can. The more we regulate our temperature behaviorally,
activity. Most of it goes to basal metabolism, the energy used the less energy we need to spend physiologically (Refinetti &
to maintain a constant body temperature while at rest. Main- Carlisle, 1986). Finding a cool place on a hot day is much bet-
taining your body temperature requires about twice as much ter than sweating (Figure 10.4). Finding a warm place on a
energy as do all other activities combined (Burton, 1994). cold day is much smarter than shivering. Here are a few other
Amphibians, reptiles, and most fish are poikilothermic behavioral mechanisms of temperature regulation:
(POY-kih-lo-THER-mik, from Greek roots meaning “varied
heat”). That is, their body temperature matches the tempera- ■ Put on more clothing or take it off. This human strategy
ture of their environment. A few large fish, including sharks accomplishes what other mammals accomplish by fluffing
and tuna, are exceptions to this rule, maintaining their core out or sleeking their fur.
body temperature well above that of the surrounding water ■ Become more active to get warmer or less active to avoid

most of the time (Bernal, Donley, Shadwick, & Syme, 2005). overheating.
Poikilothermic animals lack physiological mechanisms of ■ To get warm, huddle with others. If you are waiting at a

temperature regulation, such as shivering and sweating. The bus stop on a cold day, you might feel shy about suggest-
informal term is “coldblooded,” but that term is misleading be- ing to a stranger that you hug each other to keep warm.
cause poikilothermic animals remain warm most of the day Other species have no such inhibitions (Figure 10.5).
by choosing an appropriate location. A desert lizard moves Spectacled eiders (in the duck family) spend their win-
between sunny areas, shady areas, and burrows to maintain a ters in the Arctic Ocean, which is mostly covered with
fairly steady body temperature. However, behavioral methods ice. When more than 150,000 eiders crowd together, they
do not enable animals to maintain the same degree of con- not only keep warm but also maintain a big hole in the
stancy that mammals and birds have. ice where they can dive for fish (Weidensaul, 1999).
Mammals and birds are homeothermic (from Greek roots
meaning “same heat”), except that certain species become poiki-
lothermic during hibernation. Homeothermic animals use phys-
iological mechanisms to maintain a nearly constant body tem-
perature despite changes in the temperature of the environment.
Homeothermy is costly, especially for small animals. An animal
generates heat in proportion to its total mass, but it radiates heat
in proportion to its surface area. A small mammal or bird, such
as a mouse or hummingbird, has a high surface-to-volume ratio
AP Photo/Sun-Journal, Ken Love

and therefore radiates heat rapidly. Such animals need much fuel
each day to maintain their body temperature.
To cool ourselves when the air is warmer than body tem-
perature, we have only one physiological mechanism—evapo-
ration. Humans sweat to expose water for evaporation. For
species that don’t sweat, the alternatives are licking themselves
and panting. As water evaporates, it cools the body. However,
Figure 10.4  ​One way to cope with the heat
if the air is humid as well as hot, the moisture does not evap- On a hot day, wouldn’t you do the same?
orate. Furthermore, you endanger your health if you cannot
10.1 Temperature Regulation 301

Canada where even the underground temperature ap-


proaches –40° C (which is also –40° F). How do they do
it? Some insects and fish stock their blood with glycerol
and other antifreeze chemicals at the start of the winter
(Liou, Tocilj, Davies, & Jia, 2000). Wood frogs actually do
freeze, but they have several mechanisms to reduce the
damage. They start by withdrawing most fluid from their
organs and blood vessels and storing it in extracellular
spaces. Therefore, ice crystals have room to expand
when they form, without tearing blood vessels or cells.
© Arcticphoto/Alamy

Also, the frogs have chemicals that cause ice crystals to


form gradually, not in chunks. Finally, they have such ex-
traordinary blood-clotting capacity that they quickly repair
any blood vessels that do rupture (Storey & Storey,
1999).
Figure 10.5  ​Behavioral regulation of body temperature As you may have heard, some people have had their
An emperor penguin chick is poorly insulated against Antarctic
bodies frozen after death in hopes that scientists will dis-
winter temperatures, but when chicks huddle together, they pool
cover a cure for their disease and a way to bring a frozen
their heat. The cold ones on the outside push their way inward,
and the warm ones on the inside drift outward. The process is so
body back to life. If you had enough money, would you
effective that a cluster of penguin chicks has to move frequently choose this route to possible life after death?
to prevent melting a hole in the ice. At present, the chances don’t look good. The wood
frogs that survive after freezing begin by dehydrating their
organs and blood vessels. Unless you underwent similar
dehydration—before dying!—ice crystals will tear up blood
Applications and Extensions vessels and cell membranes throughout your body. Repair-
ing all those membranes sounds close to impossible.  ■
Surviving in Extreme Cold
If the atmospheric temperature drops below 0° C (32° F),
you maintain your body temperature by shivering, shift- The Advantages of Constant
ing blood flow away from the skin, and so forth. However, High Body Temperature
a poikilothermic animal, which by definition takes the
As mentioned, we spend about two thirds of our total energy
temperature of its environment, is vulnerable. If its body
maintaining body temperature (basal metabolism). A poikilo-
temperature drops below the freezing point of water, ice
thermic animal, with a much lower level of basal metabolism,
crystals form. Because water expands when it freezes,
needs far less fuel. If we didn’t maintain a constant, high body
ice crystals would tear apart blood vessels and cell
temperature, we could eat less and spend less effort finding
membranes, killing the animal.
food. Given the substantial costs of maintaining our body tem-
perature, it must provide an important advantage, or we would
not have evolved these mechanisms. What is that advantage?
For the answer, think back to Chapter 8: As the water gets
colder, a fish has to recruit more and more fast-twitch muscle
fibers to remain active, at the risk of rapid fatigue. Birds and
mammals keep their muscles warm at all times, regardless of
air temperature, and therefore stay constantly ready for vigor-
ous activity. In other words, we eat a great deal to support our
AP Photo/Jean-Marc Bouju

high metabolism so that even when the weather is cold, we can


still run as fast and far as we need to.
Why did mammals evolve a body temperature of 37° C
(98° F) instead of some other value? From the standpoint of
muscle activity, we gain an advantage by being as warm as pos-
Companies will freeze a dead body with the prospect that sible. A warmer animal has warmer muscles and therefore runs
future technologies can restore the person to life. faster with less fatigue than a cooler animal. When a reptile has
a choice of environments at different temperatures, it usually
Amphibians and reptiles avoid that risk by burrowing chooses to warm itself to 37º–38° C (Wagner & Gleeson, 1997).
or finding other sheltered locations. However, some frogs, If warmer is better, why not heat ourselves to an even
fish, and insects survive through winters in northern higher temperature? First, maintaining a higher temperature
302 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

requires more fuel and energy. Second, and more importantly, nant women are advised to avoid hot baths and anything else
beyond about 40° or 41° C, proteins begin to break their that might overheat a developing fetus.
bonds and lose their useful properties. Birds’ body tempera-
tures are in fact about 41° C (105° F). Stop & Check
It is possible to evolve proteins that are stable at higher 2. What is the primary advantage of maintaining a constant
temperatures; indeed, odd microscopic animals called ther- high body temperature?
mophiles survive in boiling water. However, to do so, they
need many extra chemical bonds to stabilize their proteins. 3. Why did mammals evolve a temperature of 37° C (98° F)
The enzymatic properties of a protein depend on its flexibility, instead of some other temperature?
so making proteins rigid enough to withstand high tempera- above 37° C (98° F).
tures makes them inactive at more moderate temperatures ANSWERS
However, proteins lose stability at temperatures much
(Feller, 2010). In short, our body temperature of 37° C is a warm as possible and therefore as fast as possible.
trade-off between the advantages of high temperature for weather. 3. Animals gain an advantage in being as
rapid movement and the disadvantages of high temperature ready for rapid, prolonged muscle activity even in cold
for protein stability. 2. A constant high body temperature keeps the animal
Reproductive cells require a cooler environment than the
rest of the body (Rommel, Pabst, & McLellan, 1998). Birds
lay eggs and sit on them, instead of developing them inter-
nally, because the birds’ internal temperature is too hot for an
embryo. Similarly, in most male mammals, the scrotum hangs Brain Mechanisms
outside the body, because sperm production requires a cooler The physiological changes that defend body temperature—such
temperature than the rest of the body. A man who wears his as shivering, sweating, and changes in blood flow to the skin—
undershorts too tight keeps his testes too close to the body, depend on areas in and near the hypothalamus (Figure 10.6),
overheats them, and produces fewer healthy sperm cells. Preg- mainly the anterior hypothalamus and the preoptic area,

Third
Anterior ventricle
commissure
Pineal body
Hypothalamus
Mamillary
Pituitary body

Paraventricular nucleus Dorsal


of hypothalamus hypothalamus

Anterior commissure

Lateral hypothalamus Dorsomedial


(behind plane of view) hypothalamus
Posterior hypothalamus
Anterior hypothalamus
Preoptic area

Suprachiasmatic Mamillary body


nucleus

Ventromedial Arcuate nucleus


Optic chiasm hypothalamus
Figure 10.6  ​Major subdivisions of the
hypothalamus and pituitary
Anterior pituitary Posterior
pituitary (After Nieuwenhuys, Voogd, & vanHuijzen, 1988)
10.1 Temperature Regulation 303

which is just anterior to the anterior hypothalamus. (It is Fever


called preoptic because it is near the optic chiasm, where the Bacterial and viral infections generally cause fever, an increase in
optic nerves cross.) Because of the close relationship between body temperature. The fever is not part of the illness; it is part
the preoptic area and the anterior hypothalamus, researchers of the body’s defense against the illness. When bacteria, viruses,
often treat them as a single area, the preoptic area/anterior fungi, or other intruders invade the body, they mobilize leukocytes
hypothalamus, or POA/AH. The POA/AH and a couple (white blood cells) to attack them. The leukocytes release small
other hypothalamic areas send output to the hindbrain’s ra- proteins called cytokines that attack the intruders. Cytokines
phe nucleus, which controls the physiological mechanisms also stimulate the vagus nerve, which sends signals to the hypo-
(Yoshida, Li, Cano, Lazarus, & Saper, 2009). thalamus (Ek et al., 2001; Leon, 2002), increasing the release of
The POA/AH monitors body temperature partly by chemicals called prostaglandins. Stimulation of a particular kind
monitoring its own temperature (D. O. Nelson & Prosser, of prostaglandin receptor in one nucleus of the hypothalamus is
1981). If an experimenter heats the POA/AH, an animal necessary for fever. If you didn’t have those receptors, illnesses
pants or sweats, even in a cool environment. If the same would not give you a fever (Lazarus et al., 2007). Two children
brain area is cooled, the animal shivers, even in a warm have been found with mutations of a particular gene, resulting
room. An animal also reacts to a heated or cooled POA/ in a failure to develop fevers, even in the presence of pneumonia
AH by pressing a lever or doing other work for cold air or (Hanada, et al., 2009).
hot air reinforcements (Satinoff, 1964). That is, the animal A fever represents an increased set point for body temper-
acts as if it feels hot when its hypothalamus is hot, and it ature. Just as you shiver or sweat when your body temperature
acts as if it feels cold when its hypothalamus is cold. Given goes below or above its usual 37° C, when you have a fever
that the hypothalamus is well insulated on the interior of of, say, 39° C, you shiver or sweat whenever your temperature
the head, this mechanism makes sense. If the hypothalamus deviates from that level. Moving to a cooler room does not
is hot or cold, the rest of the interior of the body probably lower your fever. Your body just works harder to keep its tem-
is, too. Cells of the POA/AH also receive input from tem- perature at the feverish level.
perature receptors in the skin and spinal cord. The animal Because newborn rabbits have an immature hypothalamus,
shivers most vigorously when both the POA/AH and the they do not shiver in response to infections. If they are given
other receptors are cold. It sweats or pants most vigorously a choice of environments, however, they select a spot warm
when both are hot. enough to raise their body temperature (Satinoff, McEwen, &
The POA/AH is not the only brain area that detects Williams, 1976). That is, they develop a fever by behavioral
temperature, but it is the primary area for controlling physi- means. Fish and reptiles with an infection also choose a warm-
ological mechanisms of temperature regulation, such as enough environment, if they can find one, to produce a feverish
sweating or shivering. Separate populations of cells within body temperature (Kluger, 1991). Again, the point is that fever
the POA/AH and a couple other hypothalamic areas regu- is something the animal does to fight an infection.
late different aspects of temperature regulation, such as Does fever do any good? Certain types of bacteria grow
shivering and changes in blood flow. Therefore, tiny local- less vigorously at high temperatures than at normal mamma-
ized damage can impair one aspect of temperature regula- lian body temperatures. Also, fever enhances activity of the
tion and not others (McAllen, Tanaka, Ootsuka, & McKin- immune system (Skitzki, Chen, Wang, & Evans, 2007). Other
ley, 2010). After damage to all of the POA/AH, mammals things being equal, developing a moderate fever probably in-
can still regulate body temperature but only by the same be- creases an individual’s chance of surviving a bacterial infec-
havioral mechanisms that a lizard might use, such as seek- tion (Kluger, 1991). However, a fever above about 39° C (103°
ing a warmer or colder location (Satinoff & Rutstein, 1970; F) in humans does more harm than good, and a fever above
Van Zoeren & Stricker, 1977). 41° C (109° F) is life-threatening (Rommel et al., 1998).

Stop & Check


Stop & Check
4. What evidence do we have that the POA/AH controls body
temperature? 6. What evidence indicates that fever is an adaptation to
fight illness?
5. How can an animal regulate body temperature after dam-
age to the POA/AH? ANSWER
tion.
increases the probability of surviving a bacterial infec-
warmer or cooler place. more, a moderate fever inhibits bacterial growth and
ANSWERS
temperature through behavior, such as by finding a raise their temperature to a feverish level. Further-
physiological control of temperature. 5. It can regulate mammals with infections use behavioral means to
shivering or sweating. Also, damage there impairs evated temperature. Also, fish, reptiles, and immature
4. Direct cooling or heating of the POA/AH leads to 6. The body will shiver or sweat to maintain its el-
304 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

Module 10.1  ■  In Closing

Combining Physiological and Behavioral Mechanisms

One theme of this module has been the redundancy of mecha- If one mechanism fails, another mechanism comes to your res-
nisms. Your body has various physiological mechanisms to cue. It is not, however, a true redundancy in the sense of two
maintain constant body temperature, including shivering, mechanisms doing exactly the same thing. Each of your mecha-
sweating, and changes in blood flow. You also rely on behavioral nisms of temperature regulation solves a different aspect of the
mechanisms, such as finding a cooler or warmer place, adding or problem in a different way. We shall see this theme again in the
removing clothing, and so forth. Redundancy reduces your risk: discussions of thirst and hunger.

Summary
1. It is easy to overlook the importance of temperature energy is needed to maintain body temperature. Mam-
regulation. Many seemingly odd animal behaviors make malian body temperature of 37° C is a compromise
sense as ways to heat or cool the body.  298 between these competing considerations.  301
2. Homeostasis is a tendency to maintain a body variable 5. The preoptic area and anterior hypothalamus (POA/
near a set point. Temperature, hunger, and thirst are AH) are critical for temperature control. Cells there
almost homeostatic, but the set point changes in varying monitor both their own temperature and that of the skin
circumstances.  299 and spinal cord.  302
3. A high body temperature enables a mammal or bird 6. Even homeothermic animals rely partly on behavioral
to move rapidly and without fatigue even in a cold mechanisms for temperature regulation, especially in
environment.  301 infancy and after damage to the POA/AH.  303
4. From the standpoint of muscle activity, the higher the 7. A moderate fever helps an animal combat an
body temperature, the better. However, as temperatures infection.  303
exceed 41° C, protein stability decreases, and more
key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
allostasis  299 homeothermic  300 preoptic area/anterior hypo-
basal metabolism  300 negative feedback  299 thalamus (POA/AH)  303
cytokines  303 poikilothermic  300 set point  299
homeostasis  299

Thought Question
Speculate on why birds have higher body temperatures than
mammals.
Module 10.2

Thirst

W
ater constitutes about 70% of the mammalian body. Stop & Check
Because the concentration of chemicals in water
7. If you lacked vasopressin, would you drink like a beaver or
determines the rate of all chemical reactions in the
like a gerbil? Why?
body, the water must be regulated within narrow limits. The
body also needs enough fluid in the circulatory system to you would need to drink an equal amount to replace it.
maintain normal blood pressure. People sometimes survive ANSWER
more like a beaver. You would excrete much fluid, so
for weeks without food, but not without water. 7. If you lacked vasopressin, you would have to drink

Mechanisms of Water
Regulation Osmotic Thirst
Different species have different strategies for maintaining water. We distinguish two types of thirst. Eating salty foods causes
Beavers and other species that live in rivers or lakes drink plenty osmotic thirst, and losing fluid by bleeding or sweating induces
of water, eat moist foods, and excrete dilute urine. In contrast, hypovolemic thirst.
gerbils and other desert animals don’t need to drink at all. They The combined concentration of all solutes (molecules in so-
gain water from their food and they have many adaptations to lution) in mammalian body fluids remains at a nearly constant
avoid losing water, including the ability to excrete dry feces and level of 0.15 M (molar). (Molarity is a measure of the number of
concentrated urine. Unable to sweat, they avoid the heat of the particles per unit of solution, regardless of the size of each par-
day by burrowing under the ground. Their highly convoluted ticle. A 1.0 M solution of sugar and a 1.0 M solution of sodium
nasal passages minimize water loss when they exhale. chloride have the same number of molecules per liter.) This fixed
We humans vary our strategy depending on circum- concentration of solutes can be regarded as a set point, similar
stances. If you cannot find enough to drink or if the water to the set point for temperature. Any deviation activates mecha-
tastes bad, you conserve water by excreting more concentrated nisms that restore the concentration of solutes to the set point.
urine and decreasing your sweat, somewhat like a gerbil, al- Osmotic pressure is the tendency of water to flow across
though not to the same extreme. Your posterior pituitary (Fig- a semipermeable membrane from the area of low solute con-
ure 10.6) releases the hormone vasopressin that raises blood centration to the area of higher concentration. A semiper-
pressure by constricting blood vessels. (The term vasopressin meable membrane is one through which water can pass but
comes from vascular pressure.) The increased pressure helps solutes cannot. The membrane surrounding a cell is almost a
compensate for the decreased blood volume. Vasopressin is semipermeable membrane because water flows across it freely
also known as antidiuretic hormone (ADH) because it en- and various solutes flow either slowly or not at all between
ables the kidneys to reabsorb water from urine and therefore the intracellular fluid inside the cell and the extracellular fluid
make the urine more concentrated. (Diuresis means “urina- outside it. Osmotic pressure occurs when solutes are more
tion.”) You also increase secretion of vasopressin while you are concentrated on one side of the membrane than on the other.
sleeping to preserve body water at a time when you cannot If you eat something salty, sodium ions spread through the
drink (Trudel & Bourque, 2010). blood and the extracellular fluid but do not cross the mem-
In most cases, our strategy is closer to that of beavers: We branes into cells. The result is a higher concentration of solutes
drink more than we need and excrete the excess. (However, if (including sodium) outside the cells than inside. The resulting
you drink extensively without eating, as many alcoholics do, osmotic pressure draws water from the cells into the extracel-
you may excrete enough body salts to harm yourself.) Most lular fluid. Certain neurons detect their own loss of water and
of our drinking is with meals or in social situations, and most then trigger osmotic thirst, which helps restore the normal
people seldom experience intense thirst. state (Figure 10.7). The kidneys also excrete more concen-
305
306 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

(a) Greater concentration of (b) Water flows out of the


solutes (green dots) outside cell, equalizing the solute
the cell than inside. concentration and shrinking
the cell.

Figure 10.7  ​The consequence of


a difference in osmotic pressure
(a) Suppose a solute such as NaCl
is more concentrated outside the
cell than inside. (b) Water flows by
osmosis out of the cell until the
concentrations are equal. Neurons
in certain brain areas detect their
own dehydration and trigger thirst.
(© Argosy Publishing Inc.)

trated urine to rid the body of excess sodium and maintain as


much water as possible.
How does the brain detect osmotic pressure? It gets part
of the information from receptors around the third ventri-
cle (Figure 10.8). Of all brain areas, those around the third
ventricle have the leakiest blood–brain barrier (Simon,
2000). A weak blood–brain barrier would be harmful for
most neurons, but it helps cells monitor the contents of the
blood. The areas important for detecting osmotic pressure
and the salt content of the blood include the OVLT (orga-
num vasculosum laminae terminalis) and the subfornical Subfornical
organ (SFO) (Hiyama, Watanabe, Okado, & Noda, 2004). organ
The OVLT receives input from receptors in the brain itself
and from receptors in the digestive tract, enabling the brain
to anticipate an osmotic need before the rest of the body OVLT Third ventricle
experiences it (Bourque, 2008).
Receptors in the OVLT, the subfornical organ, the stom-
ach, and elsewhere relay their information to several parts of
the hypothalamus, including the supraoptic nucleus and the Figure 10.8  ​The brain’s receptors for osmotic pressure and
paraventricular nucleus (PVN), which control the rate at blood volume
These neurons are in areas surrounding the third ventricle of the
which the posterior pituitary releases vasopressin. Receptors
brain, where no blood–brain barrier prevents blood-borne chemi-
also relay information to the lateral preoptic area and sur-
cals from entering the brain. (Based in part on DeArmond, Fusco, &
rounding parts of the hypothalamus, which control drinking Dewey, 1974; Weindl, 1973)
(Saad, Luiz, Camargo, Renzi, & Manani, 1996).
After osmotic pressure triggers thirst, how do you know
when to stop drinking? You do not wait until water has re-
stored normal osmotic pressure for the receptors in the brain.
Stop & Check
The water you drink has to be absorbed through the digestive
system and then pumped through the blood to the brain. That 8. Would adding salt to the body’s extracellular fluids in-
process takes 15 minutes or so, and if you continued drink- crease or decrease osmotic thirst?
ing for that long, you would drink far more than you need.
The body monitors swallowing and detects the distension ANSWER
cells into the extracellular spaces.
of the stomach and upper part of the small intestine. Those
osmotic thirst because it would draw water from the

messages limit drinking to not much more than you need at a


8. Adding salt to the extracellular fluids would increase

given time (Stricker & Hoffmann, 2007).


10.2 Thirst 307

Hypovolemic Thirst and Sodium- is offered both water and salt, it alternates between them to
yield an appropriate mixture. It shows a strong craving for
Specific Hunger salty tastes. This preference, known as sodium-specific hun-
Suppose you lose a significant amount of body fluid by bleeding, ger, develops automatically as soon as the need exists (Rich-
diarrhea, or sweating. Although your body’s osmotic pressure ter, 1936). In contrast, specific hungers for other vitamins and
stays the same, you need fluid. Your heart has trouble pumping minerals have to be learned by trial and error (Rozin & Kalat,
blood to the head, and nutrients do not flow as easily as usual 1971). You may have noticed this phenomenon yourself. A
into your cells. Your body will react with hormones that constrict woman around the time of menstruation, or anyone who has
blood vessels—vasopressin and angiotensin II. When blood vol- sweated heavily, finds that salty snacks taste especially good.
ume drops, the kidneys release the enzyme renin, which splits Sodium-specific hunger depends partly on hormones
a portion off angiotensinogen, a large protein in the blood, to (Schulkin, 1991). When the body’s sodium reserves are low,
form angiotensin I, which other enzymes convert to angiotensin the adrenal glands produce the hormone aldosterone (al-
II. Like vasopressin, angiotensin II constricts the blood vessels, DOSS-ter-one), which causes the kidneys, salivary glands,
compensating for the drop in blood pressure (Figure 10.9). and sweat glands to retain salt (Verrey & Beron, 1996). Al-
Angiotensin II also helps trigger thirst, in conjunction dosterone and angiotensin II together change the properties
with receptors that detect blood pressure in the large veins. of taste receptors on the tongue, neurons in the nucleus of
However, this thirst is different from osmotic thirst, because the tractus solitarius (part of the taste system), and neurons
you need to restore lost salts and not just water. This kind of elsewhere in the brain to increase salt intake (Krause & Sakal,
thirst is known as hypovolemic (HI-po-vo-LEE-mik) thirst, 2007). Note that aldosterone indicates low sodium and an-
meaning thirst based on low volume. When angiotensin II giotensin II indicates low blood volume. Either one by itself
reaches the brain, it stimulates neurons in areas adjoining the produces only a small effect on salt intake, but their combined
third ventricle (Fitts, Starbuck, & Ruhf, 2000; Mangiapane effect is a massive increase in a preference for salt, sometimes
& Simpson, 1980; Tanaka et al., 2001). Those neurons send producing a preference for salt over sugar or anything else
axons to the hypothalamus, where they release angiotensin II (Geerling & Loewy, 2008). Table 10.1 summarizes the differ-
as their neurotransmitter (Tanaka, Hori, & Nomura, 2001). ences between osmotic thirst and hypovolemic thirst.
That is, the neurons surrounding the third ventricle both re- Stop & Check
spond to angiotensin II and release it. As in many other cases,
the connection between a neurotransmitter and its function 9. Who would drink more pure water—someone with osmot-
is not arbitrary. The brain uses a chemical that was already ic thirst or someone with hypovolemic thirst?
performing a related function elsewhere in the body.
Whereas an animal with osmotic thirst needs water, one
of a solution containing salts.
ANSWER
ter. Someone with hypovolemic thirst would drink more
with hypovolemic thirst can’t drink much pure water. If it did, 9. Someone with osmotic thirst would drink more wa-
it would dilute its body fluids. It therefore increases its pref-
erence for slightly salty water (Stricker, 1969). If the animal

Module 10.2
Low blood Kidneys release Proteins in blood Angiotensin I is Angiotensin II constricts
volume renin into blood form angiotensin I converted to blood vessels and stimulates
angiotensin II cells in subfornical organ
to increase drinking

Figure 10.9  ​Hormonal response to hypovolemia (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Table 10.1  Osmotic and Hypovolemic Thirst

Type of Thirst Stimulus Best Relieved by Drinking Receptor Location Hormone Influences
Osmotic High solute concentration Water OLVT, a brain area adjoining Accompanied by
outside cells causes loss of the third ventricle vasopressin secretion to
water from cells conserve water
Hypovolemic Low blood volume Water containing solutes 1. Receptors measuring blood Increased by angiotension II
pressure in the veins
2. Subfornical organ, a brain
area adjoining the third
ventricle

© Cengage Learning 2013


308 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

Module 10.2  ■  In Closing

The Psychology and Biology of Thirst

You may have thought that temperature regulation happens au- body water partly by the behavior of drinking but also by hor-
tomatically and that water regulation depends on your behav- mones that alter kidney activity. If your kidneys cannot regulate
ior. You can see now that the distinction is not entirely correct. your water and sodium adequately, your brain gets signals to
You control your body temperature partly by automatic means, change your drinking or sodium intake. In short, keeping your
such as sweating or shivering, but also partly by behavioral body’s chemical reactions going depends on both skeletal and
means, such as choosing a warm or cool place. You control your autonomic controls.

Summary
1. Different mammalian species have evolved different ways 3. Loss of blood volume causes hypovolemic thirst. Animals
of maintaining body water, ranging from frequent with hypovolemic thirst drink more water containing
drinking (beavers) to extreme conservation of fluids solutes than pure water.  307
(gerbils). Humans alter their strategy depending on the 4. Hypovolemic thirst is triggered by the hormone angio-
availability of acceptable fluids.  305 tensin II, which increases when blood pressure
2. An increase in the osmotic pressure of the blood draws falls.  307
water out of cells, causing osmotic thirst. Neurons in the 5. Loss of sodium salts from the body triggers sodium-
OVLT, an area adjoining the third ventricle, detect specific cravings.  307
changes in osmotic pressure and send information to
hypothalamic areas responsible for vasopressin secretion
and drinking.  305

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
aldosterone  307 lateral preoptic area  306 sodium-specific hunger  307
angiotensin II  307 osmotic pressure  305 subfornical organ (SFO)  306
antidiuretic hormone osmotic thirst  305 supraoptic nucleus  306
(ADH)  305 OVLT  306 vasopressin  305
hypovolemic thirst  307 paraventricular nucleus (PVN)  306

Thought Questions
1. An injection of concentrated sodium chloride triggers 3. Many women crave salt during pregnancy. Why?
osmotic thirst, but an injection of equally concentrat-
ed glucose does not. Why not?
2. If all the water you drank leaked out through a tube
connected to the stomach, how would your drinking
change?
Module 10.3

Hunger

D
ifferent species use different eating strategies. A snake each day to increase its body weight by 10% and then loses
or crocodile might have a huge meal and then eat that amount at night (Harrison, 2008; Sharbaugh, 2001).
nothing more for months (Figure 10.10). Bears eat as For comparison, imagine a 50 kg (110 lb) person gaining 5 kg
much as they can whenever they can. It is a sensible strategy (11 lb) during the day and then shivering it off at night.
because bears’ main foods—fruits and nuts—are available in Humans eat more than we need for today, unlike small
large quantities for only short times. Bears’ occasional feasts birds, but we do not stuff ourselves like bears—not as a rule,
tide them over through times of starvation. You might think anyway. Choosing which food to eat and how much is an
of it as survival of the fattest. (Sorry about that one.) important decision. We use a wide array of learned and un-
A small bird, at the other extreme, eats only what it needs learned mechanisms to help in the process.
at the moment. The advantage of restraint is that low weight
helps it fly away from predators and even a few extra milli-
grams might make a difference (Figure 10.11). However, in
Digestion and Food Selection
some climates, a bird needs to store a substantial amount to get Examine the digestive system, as diagramed in Figure 10.12. Its
through the night. Tiny chickadees manage to survive through function is to break food into smaller molecules that the cells
Alaska winters. Every night, a chickadee finds a hollowed tree can use. Digestion begins in the mouth, where enzymes in the
or other nesting site that provides as much insulation as pos- saliva break down carbohydrates. Swallowed food travels down
sible, and it lowers its body temperature into a state almost the esophagus to the stomach, where it mixes with hydrochloric
like hibernation. Still, it has to shiver throughout the night to acid and enzymes that digest proteins. The stomach stores food
prevent its body from freezing, and all that shivering requires for a time, and then a round sphincter muscle opens at the end of
energy. During Alaskan winters, a chickadee eats enough the stomach to release food to the small intestine.
Gunter Ziesler/Bruce Coleman Inc.

© Arco Images GmbH/Alamy

Figure 10.10  ​A python swallowing a gazelle Figure 10.11  ​A great tit, a small European bird
The gazelle weighed about 50% more than the snake. Many rep- Ordinarily, when food is abundant, tits eat just what they need
tiles eat huge but infrequent meals. Their total intake over a year each day and maintain very low fat reserves. When food is harder
is far less than that of a mammal. We mammals need far more to find, they eat all they can and live off fat reserves between
fuel because we use so much more energy, mainly for maintaining meals. During an era when their predators were scarce, tits
basal metabolism. started putting on more fat regardless of the food supplies.

309
310 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

Humans are a partial exception to this rule. Many adults


have enough lactase levels to consume milk and other dairy
products throughout life. However, nearly all the people in
China and surrounding countries lack the gene that enables
adults to metabolize lactose, as do varying numbers of people in
Salivary glands other parts of the world (Flatz, 1987). People who are lactose in-
Mouth tolerant can consume a little milk, and larger amounts of cheese
and yogurt, which are easier to digest. If they overeat dairy prod-
ucts, the result depends on the type of bacteria they have in their
Esophagus digestive system, but it often includes diarrhea, cramps, and gas
pains (Ingram, Mulcare, Itan, Thomas, & Swallow, 2009). Most
such people learn to limit their intake of dairy products. Figure
10.13 shows the worldwide distribution of lactose tolerance.
Within Africa, the distribution of ability to digest lactose
varies in a patchy way from place to place. Whereas Europeans
who can digest lactose in adulthood all have variants of the same
gene, people in different parts of Africa have genes different from
one another and different from Europeans, indicating that the
genes for lactose digestion evolved independently in different
Liver
places, probably within the last few thousand years, in response
Stomach to the domestication of cattle (Tishkoff et al., 2006). When
Gallbladder cow’s milk became available, the selective pressure was strong in
favor of genes enabling people to digest it.
Duodenum Pancreas

Small intestine Stop & Check


10. What genetic difference is most important for variants in
Large intestine the likelihood of drinking milk in adulthood?

digest lactose, the main sugar in milk.


ANSWER
Rectum
depends largely on a gene that controls the ability to
10. The likelihood of drinking milk in adulthood

Figure 10.12  ​The human digestive system (© Cengage


Learning 2013)

Food Selection and Behavior


The small intestine has enzymes that digest proteins, fats, Does your food selection change your behavior? Many people
and carbohydrates. It is also the site for absorbing digested have unsubstantiated beliefs in this regard. Many people, in-
materials into the bloodstream. The blood carries those chem- cluding physicians, believe that eating sugar makes children
icals to body cells that either use them or store them for later hyperactive. The best way to test this claim is to have children
use. The large intestine absorbs water and minerals and lubri- eat snacks with sugar on some days, and artificially sweetened
cates the remaining materials to pass as feces. snacks on other days, so that neither they nor their parents
and teachers know when the child has eaten sugar. Studies of
this type have found no significant effect of sugar on children’s
Consumption of Dairy Products activity level, play behaviors, or school performance (Ells et
Newborn mammals survive at first on mother’s milk. As they al., 2008; Milich & Pelham, 1986). Presumably the belief that
grow older, they stop nursing for several reasons: The milk sugar causes hyperactivity is an illusion based on people’s ten-
supply declines, the mother pushes them away, and they begin dency to remember the observations that fit their expectation
to eat other foods. Most mammals at about the age of wean- and disregard the others.
ing lose the intestinal enzyme lactase, which is necessary for Another common misconception is that eating turkey
metabolizing lactose, the sugar in milk. From then on, milk increases the body’s supply of tryptophan, which enables
consumption causes stomach cramps and gas (Rozin & Pel- the brain to make chemicals that make you sleepy. That idea
chat, 1988). Adult mammals can drink a little milk, as you probably originated from the observation that many people
may have noticed with a pet dog, but generally not much. The in the United States feel sleepy after eating a turkey dinner
declining level of lactase may be an evolved mechanism to en- on Thanksgiving. That sleepiness comes from overeating,
courage weaning at the appropriate time. not from turkey itself, which has only an average amount
10.3 Hunger 311

90% 70%
15% 50%
60%
80% 50%
European Ameri 40%
Native cans
7 8% 30%
Americans
25% Africa 20%
n Am
eric 50% 0–24% 15%
Mexican an
s 35 ? 40% 20% 10%
Americans %
25% 10–25% 0%

? 3–16%

Figure 10.13  ​Percentage of adults who are lactose tolerant


People in areas with high lactose tolerance (e.g., Scandinavia) are likely to enjoy milk and other dairy products throughout their lives.
Adults in areas with low tolerance (including much of Southeast Asia) drink less milk, if any. (© Cengage Learning 2013. Based on Flatz,
1987; Rozin & Pelchat, 1988)

of tryptophan. However, the rest of that idea is correct: In- Oral Factors
creasing tryptophan does help the brain produce melatonin, You’re a busy person, right? If you could get all the nutri-
which induces sleepiness. Other than taking tryptophan tion you need by swallowing a pill, would you do it? Once in
pills, the most reliable way to increase tryptophan in the a while you might, but not often. People like to eat. In fact,
brain is to eat a diet high in carbohydrates. Here is the expla- many people like to taste and chew even when they are not
nation: Tryptophan enters the brain by an active-transport hungry. Figure 10.14 shows a piece of 6,500-year-old chewing
protein that it shares with phenylalanine and other large gum made from birch–bark tar. The small tooth marks indi-
amino acids. When you eat carbohydrates, your body re- cate that a child or teenager chewed it. Anthropologists don’t
acts by increasing secretion of insulin, which moves sugars know how the ancient people removed the sap to make the
into storage and also moves phenylalanine into storage (in gum, and they aren’t sure why anyone would chew something
liver cells and elsewhere). By reducing the competition from that tasted as bad as this gum probably did (Battersby, 1997).
phenylalanine, this process makes it easier for tryptophan Clearly, the urge to chew is strong.
to reach the brain, inducing sleepiness (Silber & Schmitt, If necessary, could you become satiated without tasting
2010). In short, it’s mainly the dessert at your big meal that your food? In one experiment, college students consumed
induces sleepiness. lunch five days a week by swallowing one end of a rubber
On the other hand, one old belief, long dismissed as non- tube and then pushing a button to pump a liquid diet into
sense, may turn out to be partly true. That belief is that fish
is brain food. Many fish, including salmon, contain oils that
are helpful for brain functioning, and several research studies
have found that eating more fish helps some people improve
their memory and reasoning abilities (Ells et al., 2008).

Short- and Long-Term Figure 10.14  ​Chewing gum from about 4500 b.c.
Regulation of Feeding The gum, made from birch–bark tar, has small tooth marks,
indicating that a child or adolescent chewed it. (Reprinted by per-
Eating is far too important to be entrusted to just one mecha- mission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature, Plus c’est le meme
nism. Your brain gets messages from your mouth, stomach, chews, Stephen Battersby, 1997)
intestines, fat cells, and elsewhere to regulate your eating.
312 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

the stomach ( Jordan, 1969; Spiegel, 1973). (They were paid Stop & Check
for participating.) After a few days of practice, each person
established a consistent pattern of pumping in a constant 12. What is the evidence that stomach distension is suffi-
volume of the liquid each day and maintaining a constant cient for satiety?
body weight. Most found the untasted meals unsatisfying, stomach is full.
however, and reported a desire to taste or chew something ANSWER
the stomach, an animal becomes satiated when the
( Jordan, 1969). stomach and duodenum so that food cannot leave
Could you be satisfied if you tasted something without 12. If a cuff is attached to the junction between the
ingesting it? In sham-feeding experiments, everything an ani-
mal swallows leaks out of a tube connected to the esophagus
or stomach. Sham-feeding animals eat and swallow almost
continually without becoming satiated (G. P. Smith, 1998). In Fat in the duodenum releases a hormone called oleoyle-
short, taste and other mouth sensations contribute to satiety, thanolamide (OEA), which stimulates the vagus nerve, send-
but they are not sufficient. ing a message to the hypothalamus that delays the next meal
(Gaetani et al., 2010). Any kind of food in the duodenum
also releases the hormone cholecystokinin (ko-leh-SIS-teh-
Stop & Check KI-nehn) (CCK), which limits meal size in two ways (Gibbs,
Young, & Smith, 1973). First, CCK constricts the sphincter
11. What evidence indicates that taste is not sufficient for muscle between the stomach and duodenum, causing the
satiety? stomach to hold its contents and fill more quickly than usual
satiated.
(McHugh & Moran, 1985; G. P. Smith & Gibbs, 1998). In
ANSWER that way it facilitates stomach distension, the primary signal
feed chew and taste their food but do not become
11. It is not sufficient, because animals that sham- for ending a meal. Second, CCK stimulates the vagus nerve
to send signals to the hypothalamus, causing cells there to re-
lease a neurotransmitter that is a shorter version of the CCK
molecule itself (Kobett et al., 2006; G. J. Schwartz, 2000).
The process is something like sending a fax: The CCK in the
The Stomach and Intestines intestines can’t cross the blood–brain barrier, but it stimulates
Ordinarily, we end a meal before the food reaches the blood, cells to release something almost like it. As in the case of an-
much less the muscles and other cells. Usually, the main sig- giotensin and thirst, the body uses the same chemical in the
nal to end a meal is distension of the stomach. In one ex- periphery and in the brain for closely related functions.
periment, researchers attached an inflatable cuff at the con- Given that CCK helps to end a meal, could we use it to help
nection between the stomach and small intestine (Deutsch, people who are trying to lose weight? Unfortunately, no. CCK
Young, & Kalogeris, 1978). When they inflated the cuff, produces short-term effects only. It limits the size of the meal, but
food could not pass from the stomach to the duodenum. an animal that has eaten a smaller than usual meal compensates
They carefully ensured that the cuff was not traumatic to by overeating at the next meal (Cummings & Overduin, 2007).
the animal and did not interfere with feeding. The key result
was that, with the cuff inflated, an animal ate a normal-size Stop & Check
meal and then stopped. Evidently, stomach distension is
sufficient to produce satiety. 13. What are two mechanisms by which CCK increases
The stomach conveys satiety messages to the brain via satiety?
the vagus nerve and the splanchnic nerves. The vagus nerve and at its receptors, it triggers decreased feeding.
(cranial nerve X) conveys information about the stretching ANSWER
hypothalamus to release CCK as a neurotransmitter,
of the stomach walls, providing a major basis for satiety. signals from the intestines cause certain cells in the
The splanchnic (SPLANK-nik) nerves convey informa- the rate at which the stomach distends. Also, neural
tion about the nutrient contents of the stomach (Deutsch stomach and duodenum. CCK therefore increases
& Ahn, 1986). CCK, which closes the sphincter muscle between the
However, people who have had their stomach surgically 13. When the duodenum is distended, it releases
removed (because of stomach cancer or other disease) still
report satiety, so stomach distension is not necessary for sa-
tiety. Later researchers found that meals end after distension
of either the stomach or the duodenum (Seeley, Kaplan, & Glucose, Insulin, and Glucagon
Grill, 1995). The duodenum (DYOU-oh-DEE-num or Much digested food enters the bloodstream as glucose, an
dyuh-ODD-ehn-uhm) is the part of the small intestine ad- important source of energy throughout the body and nearly
joining the stomach. It is the first digestive site that absorbs a the only fuel of the brain. When the blood’s glucose level is
significant amount of nutrients. high, liver cells convert some of the excess into glycogen, and
10.3 Hunger 313

fat cells convert some of it into fat. When the blood’s glucose (Figure 10.15). Glucagon stimulates the liver to convert some
level starts to fall, the liver converts some of its glycogen back of its stored glycogen to glucose to replenish low supplies in
into glucose. In this way blood glucose levels stay fairly steady the blood.
for most people most of the time. If the insulin level stays constantly high, the body con-
However, the glucose in the blood is not equally available tinues moving blood glucose into the cells, including the
to the cells at all times. Two pancreatic hormones, insulin and liver cells and fat cells, long after a meal. Before too long,
glucagon, regulate the flow of glucose. Insulin enables glucose blood glucose drops, because glucose is leaving the blood
to enter the cells, except for brain cells, where glucose does not without any new glucose entering. In this case, despite the
need insulin to enter. When insulin levels are high, glucose en- high insulin level, hunger increases. In autumn, animals that
ters cells easily. When someone is getting ready for a meal, in- are preparing for hibernation have constantly high insulin
sulin levels rise, letting some of the blood glucose enter the cells levels. They rapidly deposit much of each meal as fat and
in preparation for the rush of additional glucose about to enter glycogen, grow hungry again, and continue gaining weight
the blood. Insulin continues to increase during and after a meal. (Figure 10.16). That weight gain is a valuable preparation
In general, high levels of insulin decrease appetite, because the for a season when the animal will have to survive off its fat
insulin enables so much glucose to enter the cells. When much reserves. Most humans also eat more in autumn than in
glucose is already entering the cells, you don’t need to eat more. other seasons, as shown in Figure 10.17 (de Castro, 2000).
As time passes after a meal, the blood glucose level falls. In the United States, we tend to blame our autumn weight
Therefore, insulin levels drop, glucose enters the cells more gain on the Halloween and Thanksgiving holidays, but the
slowly, and hunger increases (Pardal & López-Barneo, 2002) real reason may be an evolved drive to increase our reserves
in preparation for winter. (Our ancestors didn’t have good
food year-round, as we do.)
If the insulin level remains constantly low, as in people
with diabetes, blood glucose levels may be three or more times
Blood glucose
Eating
increases. Insulin the normal level, but little of it enters the cells (Figure 10.18).
release increases. People and animals with diabetes eat more food than normal
because their cells are starving (Lindberg, Coburn, & Stricker,
1984), but they excrete most of their glucose, and they lose
weight. Note that either prolonged high or prolonged low in-
sulin levels increase eating, but for different reasons and with
different effects on body weight.
Insulin helps glucose
Hunger enter cells for use or
storage. Hunger
decreases.
Blood glucose
Eating
increases. Insulin
level is high.
Blood glucose
levels decline.
Insulin levels
decrease.

Rapid return Insulin helps glucose


of hunger enter cells. Hunger
temporarily decreases.
Glucagon release
increases. Some
stored supplies are
converted to glucose,
which enters the Blood glucose
blood, slowing the levels decline but
return of hunger. insulin levels do not.

Figure 10.15  ​Insulin and glucagon feedback system Figure 10.16  ​Effects of steady high insulin levels on
When glucose levels rise, the pancreas releases the hormone in- feeding
sulin, which causes cells to store the excess glucose as fats and Constantly high insulin causes blood glucose to be stored as fats
glycogen. The entry of glucose into cells suppresses hunger and and glycogen. Because it becomes difficult to mobilize the stored
decreases eating, thereby lowering the glucose level. (© Cengage nutrients, hunger returns soon after each meal. (© Cengage Learn-
Learning 2013) ing 2013)
314 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

2100
Blood glucose
Daily consumption

Eating
2000 increases but insulin
in calories

level is low.
1900

1800

Glucose does not


g Hunger
in
r

er

ll

er

ll
enter cells; leaves in
te

te
Fa

Fa
pr
rin

m
in

in
S
m

m
Sp

urine and feces instead.


W

W
Su

Su
Hunger remains high.
Figure 10.17  ​People eat more in fall than in other seasons
Mean intake increases by more than 10%, on average, according
to people’s eating diaries. (Modified from de Castro, J. M. (2000).
Blood glucose
Eating behavior: Lessons from the real world of humans. Nutrition, levels stay high
16, 800–813.) but cells are starving.

Figure 10.18  ​People with untreated diabetes eat much but


Stop & Check lose weight
14. Why do people with very low insulin levels eat so much? Because of their low insulin levels, the glucose in their blood can-
not enter the cells, either to be stored or to be used. Consequently,
Why do people with constantly high levels eat so much?
they excrete glucose in their urine while their cells are starving.
15. What would happen to someone’s appetite if insulin lev- (© Cengage Learning 2013)
els and glucagon levels were both high?

ANSWERS
result would be decreased appetite.
normal mice, as well as humans and other species, the body’s fat
cells produce leptin: The more fat cells, the more leptin. Mice
entering the blood is free to enter all the cells. So the

with the obese gene fail to produce leptin.


the blood. If insulin levels are high also, the glucose
stored glycogen is converted to glucose, which enters
blood glucose drops. 15. When glucagon levels rise, Leptin signals your brain about your fat reserves, providing
so within a short time after a meal, the supply of a long-term indicator of whether you have been overeating or
deposit much of their glucose into fat and glycogen, undereating. Each meal also releases leptin, so the amount of
the urine and feces. Those with constantly high levels circulating leptin indicates something about short-term nutri-
constantly hungry. They pass much of their nutrition in tion as well. Animal studies show that when leptin levels are
get glucose to enter their cells, and therefore, they are high, you act as if you have plenty of nutrition. You eat less
14. Those with very low levels, as in diabetes, cannot (Campfield, Smith, Guisez, Devos, & Burn, 1995), become
more active (Elias et al., 1998), and increase the activity of your

Leptin
The mechanisms we have considered so far regulate the onset
and offset of a meal. However, we can’t expect those mecha-
nisms to be completely accurate. If you consistently eat either a
little more or less than necessary, eventually, you would be much
too heavy or much too thin. The body needs to compensate for
day-to-day mistakes by some sort of long-term regulation.
It does so by monitoring fat supplies. Researchers had long
suspected some kind of fat monitoring, but they discovered the
actual mechanism by accident. They found that mice of a partic-
ular genetic strain consistently become obese, as shown in Figure
10.19 (Y. Zhang et al., 1994). After researchers identified the
responsible gene, they found the peptide it makes, a previously
unrecognized substance that they named leptin, from the Greek Figure 10.19  ​The effects of the obese gene on body weight
Mice with this gene eat more, move around less, and gain weight.
word leptos, meaning “slender” (Halaas et al., 1995). Unlike insu-
(Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd.: Nature,
lin, which is so evolutionarily ancient that we find it throughout
“Positional cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human homo-
the animal kingdom, leptin is limited to vertebrates (Morton, logue,” Zhang et al., 1994)
Cummings, Baskin, Barsh, & Schwartz, 2006). In genetically
10.3 Hunger 315

immune system (Lord et al., 1998). (If you have enough fat sup- Stop & Check
plies, you can afford to devote energy to your immune system.
16. Why are leptin injections less helpful for most overweight
If you have no fat, you are starving and you have to conserve en-
people than for mice with the obese gene?
ergy wherever you can.) In adolescence, a certain level of leptin
triggers the onset of puberty. If your fat supply is too low to pro- ity to it.
ANSWER
vide for your own needs, you don’t have enough energy to pro- proportion to body fat. However, they have low sensitiv-
vide for a baby. On average, thinner people enter puberty later. 16. Nearly all overweight people produce leptin in
Because a mouse with the obese gene does not make leptin,
its brain reacts as if its body has no fat stores and must be starv-
ing. The mouse eats as much as possible, conserves its energy by
not moving much, and fails to enter puberty. Injections of leptin
reverse these symptoms: The mouse then eats less, becomes
Brain Mechanisms
more active, and enters puberty (Pellymounter et al., 1995). How does your brain decide when you should eat and how
As you might imagine, news of this research inspired much? Hunger depends on the contents of your stomach and
pharmaceutical companies to hope they could make a intestines, the availability of glucose to the cells, and your
fortune by selling leptin. After all, the body makes leptin body’s fat supplies, as well as your health and body tempera-
all the time, so it should not have unpleasant side effects. ture. Also, your appetite depends on more than your need for
However, researchers soon discovered that almost all over- food. Just seeing a picture of highly appealing food increases
weight people already produce plenty of leptin (Considine your appetite (Harmon-Jones & Gable, 2009). You eat with
et al., 1996). The problem is that they have become less friends and family just to be sociable. Somehow, the brain
sensitive to it. Leptin sensitivity declines during pregnancy combines many kinds of information to decide when to eat,
and in animals preparing for hibernation. In those cases, and how much. The key brain areas include several nuclei of
increased intake makes sense. Unfortunately, leptin sensi- the hypothalamus (Figure 10.6).
tivity also declines as a result of obesity (Ernst et al., 2009; As shown in Figure 10.20, many kinds of information
Tups, 2009). When consistent overeating leads to obesity, impinge onto two kinds of cells in the arcuate nucleus of the
it damages the endoplasmic reticulum (a cell constituent) hypothalamus, which is regarded as the “master area” for con-
in neurons of the hypothalamus, setting in motion a series trol of appetite (Mendieta-Zéron, López, & Diéguez, 2008).
of outcomes that lead to decreased leptin sensitivity. So far, Axons extend from the arcuate nucleus to other areas of the
the only known way to undo that effect is physical exercise, hypothalamus. This figure is tentative and incomplete, as
which increases production of certain chemicals of the im- feeding depends on many transmitters and mechanisms. Even
mune system. Those chemicals help repair the endoplasmic in this simplified form, the figure may be intimidating. Nev-
reticulum (Ropelle et al., 2010). We now see another rea- ertheless, it highlights some of the key mechanisms. Let’s go
son, among several, why exercise helps people lose weight. through them step by step.

Two kinds of neurons


in the arcuate nucleus
Ghrelin of the hypothalamus
(hunger signal) Two kinds of neurons
in the lateral nucleus
of the hypothalamus
Taste input
NPY, AgRP, & GABA
Ghrelin
Orexin/hypocretin
Leptin Hunger- (increases arousal)
(long-term motive
satiety signal)

Insulin
(intermediate-term Melanocortin Neurons in the
satiety signal) paraventricular nucleus
Satiety-motive of the hypothalamus
CCK Output to other areas, including
(short-term cerebral cortex and brainstem.
satiety signal) Glucose (short-term signal) Output increases feeding.

Figure 10.20  ​Hypothalamic transmitters of feeding


Hunger signals increase feeding by inhibiting inhibitory messages to the lateral hypothalamus. (Based on reviews by Horvath, 2005; Mino-
koshi et al., 2004)
316 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

The Arcuate Nucleus and Paraventricular


An additional pathway leads to cells in the lateral hy-
Hypothalamus pothalamus that release orexin, also known as hypocretin
The arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus has one set of neu- (L.-Y. Fu, Acuna-Goycolea, & van den Pol, 2004). We en-
rons sensitive to hunger signals and a second set sensitive to countered these neurons in Chapter 9 because a deficiency
satiety signals. In Figure 10.20, excitatory paths are noted in of orexin leads to narcolepsy. In addition to its role in wake-
green, and inhibitory paths are in red. The hunger-sensitive fulness, orexin has two roles in feeding. First, it increases an-
cells receive input from the taste pathway, and you have surely imals’ persistence in seeking food (G. Williams, Cai, Elliott,
noticed that good-tasting food stimulates your hunger. An- & Harrold, 2004). Second, orexin responds to incentives or
other input to the hunger-sensitive cells comes from axons reinforcement in general. If orexin receptors are blocked, an
releasing the neurotransmitter ghrelin (GRELL-in). This animal becomes less active and less likely to work for rein-
odd-looking word takes its name from the fact that it binds forcement of any kind (Borgland et al., 2009). Stimulation
to the same receptors as growth-hormone releasing hormone of orexin receptors increases activity and motivation. When
(GHRH). The stomach releases ghrelin during a period of you eagerly eat a hot-fudge sundae, in spite of not feeling
food deprivation, where it triggers stomach contractions. hungry, the pleasant taste activates orexin receptors that
Ghrelin also acts on the hypothalamus to decrease appetite override satiety messages from other receptors (Zheng, Pat-
and acts on the hippocampus to enhance learning (Diano et terson, & Berthoud, 2007).
al., 2006). Whereas the digestive system secretes several hor- In addition to the chemicals in Figure 10.20, several oth-
mones that signal satiety, ghrelin is the only known hunger ers contribute to the control of appetite. One consequence of
hormone. control by so many chemicals is that the control of feeding can
Signals of both short- and long-term satiety provide input go wrong in many ways. However, when an error occurs in
to the satiety-sensitive cells of the arcuate nucleus. Distension one way, the brain has many other mechanisms to compensate
of the intestines triggers neurons to release the neurotrans- for it. A closely related point is that researchers could develop
mitter CCK, a short-term signal (Fan et al., 2004). Blood drugs to control appetite by working on many routes—leptin,
glucose (a short-term signal) directly stimulates satiety cells insulin, NPY, and so forth—but changing any one circuit
in the arcuate nucleus (Parton et al., 2007) and leads to in- might be ineffective because of compensations by the oth-
creased secretion of insulin, which also stimulates the satiety ers. One of the most promising hopes for drug researchers is
cells. Body fat (a long-term signal) releases leptin, which pro- the melanocortin receptors. As shown in Figure 10.20, many
vides an additional input (Münzberg & Myers, 2005).
Much of the output from the arcuate nucleus goes to the
paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. The paraven-
tricular nucleus (PVN) inhibits the lateral hypothalamus, an
area important for eating. So the paraventricular nucleus is
important for satiety. Rats with damage in the paraventricular
nucleus eat larger than normal meals, indicating insensitivity
to the usual signals for ending a meal (Leibowitz, Hammer,
& Chang, 1981).
Axons from the satiety-sensitive cells of the arcuate
nucleus deliver an excitatory message to the paraventricular
nucleus, releasing the neuropeptide a-melanocyte stimulat-
Morley, Levine, Grace, & Kneip, 1985

ing hormone (aMSH), which is a type of chemical called a


melanocortin (Ellacott & Cone, 2004). Melanocortin recep-
tors in the paraventricular nucleus are important for limiting
food intake, and deficiencies of this receptor lead to overeating
(Balthasar et al., 2005).
Input from the hunger-sensitive neurons of the arcuate
nucleus is inhibitory to both the paraventricular nucleus and
the satiety-sensitive cells of the arcuate nucleus itself. The in- Figure 10.21  ​Effects of inhibiting the paraventricular
hibitory transmitters here are a combination of GABA (Tong, nucleus of the hypothalamus
Jones, Elmquist, & Lowell, 2008), neuropeptide Y (NPY) On the left is the digestive system of a normal rat. On the right is
(Stephens et al., 1995), and agouti-related peptide (AgRP) the digestive system of a rat that had its paraventricular hypothal-
(Kas et al., 2004). These transmitters block the satiety ac- amus chemically inhibited. The rat continued eating even though
tions of the paraventricular nucleus, in some cases provoking its stomach and intestines distended almost to the point of
extreme overeating, as tastelessly illustrated in Figure 10.21 bursting. (Yeah, this is a little bit disgusting.) (Reprinted from Brain
Research, 341/1, J. E. Morley, A. S. Levine, M. Grace, and J. Kneip,
(Billington & Levine, 1992; Leibowitz & Alexander, 1991;
“Peptide YY (PYY) a potently orexigenic agent,” 200–203, 1985, with
Morley, Levine, Grace, & Kneip, 1985). Conversely, a loss of permission of Elsevier)
NPY leads to decreased eating (Yang et al., 2009).
10.3 Hunger 317

Stop & Check


17. Name three hormones that increase satiety and one that
increases hunger.

18. Which neuropeptide from the arcuate nucleus to the para-


ventricular nucleus is most important for satiety?

stimulating hormone).
ANSWERS
increases hunger. 18. Melanocortin (or a-melanocyte
17. Insulin, CCK, and leptin increase satiety. Ghrelin

Lateral
ventricles Cerebral cortex
Corpus
callosum Basal ganglia
The Lateral Hypothalamus
Paraventricular
hypothalamus
Output from the paraventricular nucleus acts on the lat-
eral hypothalamus (Figure 10.22), which includes so
Lateral many neuron clusters and passing axons that it has been
hypothalamus compared to a crowded train station (Leibowitz & Hoebel,
1998). The lateral hypothalamus controls insulin secretion,
alters taste responsiveness, and facilitates feeding in other
Third ventricle Ventromedial hypothalamus ways. An animal with damage in this area refuses food and
water, averting its head as if the food were distasteful. The
Figure 10.22  ​The lateral hypothalamus, ventromedial animal may starve to death unless it is force-fed, but if kept
hypothalamus, and paraventricular hypothalamus alive, it gradually recovers much of its ability to eat (Figure
The side view above indicates the plane of the coronal section of
10.23). In contrast, stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus
the brain below. (After Hart, 1976)
increases the drive to eat.
Many axons containing dopamine pass through the lateral
hypothalamus, so damage to the lateral hypothalamus inter-
kinds of input converge onto the cells of the arcuate nucleus, rupts these fibers. To separate the roles of hypothalamic cells
but the input to the paraventricular nucleus is more limited. from those of passing fibers, experimenters used chemicals
Insulin, diet drugs, and other procedures affect eating largely that damage only the cell bodies, or induced lesions in very
by altering input to the melanocortin receptors (Benoit et al., young rats, before the dopamine axons reached the lateral
2002; Heisler et al., 2002). hypothalamus. The result was a major loss of feeding with-

Plain Sugar Plain Sugar Plain Sugar Plain


water water water water water water water

Stage 1. Aphagia and Stage 2. Anorexia. Rat eats a Stage 3. Adipsia. The rat Stage 4. Near-recovery. The rat eats
adipsia. Rat refuses all food small amount of palatable eats enough to stay alive, enough to stay alive, though at a
and drink; must be force- foods and drinks sweetened though at a lower-than- lower-than-normal body weight. It
fed to keep it alive. water. It still does not eat normal body weight. It still drinks plain water, but only at meal-
enough to stay alive. refuses plain water. times to wash down its food. Under
slightly stressful conditions, such as
in a cold room, the rat will return
to an earlier stage of refusing food
and water.

Figure 10.23  ​Recovery after damage to the lateral hypothalamus


At first, the rat refuses all food and drink. If kept alive for several weeks or months by force-feeding, it gradually recovers its ability to eat
and drink enough to stay alive. However, even at the final stage of recovery, its behavior is not the same as that of normal rats. (Based on
Teitelbaum & Epstein, 1962)
318 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

out loss of arousal and activity (Almli, Fisher, & Hill, 1979; Stop & Check
Grossman, Dacey, Halaris, Collier, & Routtenberg, 1978;
Stricker, Swerdloff, & Zigmond, 1978). 19. In what ways does the lateral hypothalamus facilitate
The lateral hypothalamus contributes to feeding in several feeding?
ways (Leibowitz & Hoebel, 1998) (Figure 10.24): creases secretions of insulin and digestive juices.
ANSWER
■ Axons from the lateral hypothalamus to the NTS taste, enhances cortical responses to food, and in-
(nucleus of the tractus solitarius), part of the taste 19. Activity of the lateral hypothalamus improves
pathway, alter the taste sensation and the salivation
response to the tastes. When the lateral hypothalamus
detects hunger, it sends messages to make the food
taste better. Medial Areas of the Hypothalamus
■ Axons from the lateral hypothalamus extend into sev-
Output from the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) in-
eral parts of the cerebral cortex, facilitating ingestion hibits feeding (Chee, Myers, Price, & Colmers, 2010), and
and swallowing and causing cortical cells to increase therefore damage to this nucleus leads to overeating and
their response to the taste, smell, or sight of food weight gain (Figure 10.22). Some people with a tumor in
(Critchley & Rolls, 1996). that area have gained more than 10 kg (22 lb) per month
■ The lateral hypothalamus increases the pituitary (Al-Rashid, 1971; Killeffer & Stern, 1970; Reeves & Plum,
gland’s secretion of hormones that increase insulin 1969). Rats with similar damage sometimes double or triple
secretion. their weight (Figure 10.25). Eventually, body weight levels off
■ The lateral hypothalamus sends axons to the spinal at a stable but high set point, and total food intake declines to
cord, controlling autonomic responses such as diges- nearly normal levels.
tive secretions (van den Pol, 1999). An animal with Although these symptoms have been known as the ventro-
damage to the lateral hypothalamus has trouble medial hypothalamic syndrome, damage limited to just the ven-
digesting foods. tromedial hypothalamus does not consistently increase eating

Somatosensory
Nucleus accumbens cortex (taste perception)
(control of ingestion
and swallowing)

Thalamus

Prefrontal cortex
(food-seeking behaviors)

Hypothalamus

Nucleus of the
tractus solitarius
(NTS)

Figure 10.24  ​Pathways from the lateral hypothalamus


Axons from the lateral hypothalamus modify activity in several other brain areas, changing the response to taste, facilitating ingestion and
swallowing, and increasing food-seeking behaviors. Also (not shown), the lateral hypothalamus controls stomach secretions. (© Cengage
Learning 2013)
10.3 Hunger 319

Rat with bilateral


400 VMH lesion

Body weight (grams)


Control rat

300

200

Food intake (grams)


30
Yoav Levy/Phototake

10

10 20 30 40 50 60
Operation Days

(a) (b)

Figure 10.25  ​Effects of damage to the ventromedial hypothalamus


(a) On the right is a normal rat. On the left is a rat after damage to the ventromedial hypothalamus. A brain-damaged rat may weigh up to
three times as much as a normal rat. (b) Weight and eating after damage to the ventromedial hypothalamus. Within a few days after the
operation, the rat begins eating much more than normal. (Reprinted by permission of the University of Nebraska Press from “Disturbances
in feeding and drinking behavior after hypothalamic lesions,” by P. Teitelbaum, pp. 39–69, in M. R. Jones, Ed., 1961, Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation. Copyright © 1961 by the University of Nebraska Press. Copyright © renewed 1988 by the University of Nebraska Press.)

Cerebral cortex
or body weight. To produce a large ef- Corpus callosum
fect, the lesion must extend outside the
ventromedial nucleus to invade nearby
axons, especially the ventral noradren- Thalamus
ergic bundle (Figure 10.26) (Ahlskog
& Hoebel, 1973; Ahlskog, Randall, &
Hoebel, 1975; Gold, 1973).
Rats with damage in and around
the ventromedial hypothalamus show
an increased appetite compared to
undamaged rats of the same weight
(B. M. King, 2006; Peters, Sensenig,
& Reich, 1973). Recall that rats with
damage to the paraventricular nucleus Olfactory bulb
eat large meals. In contrast, those with
damage in the ventromedial area eat Hypothalamus
normal-size meals, but they eat more Dorsal
frequently (Hoebel & Hernandez, Amygdala noradrenergic
bundle
1993). One reason is that they have Ventral
increased stomach motility and secre- noradrenergic Locus
tions, and their stomachs empty faster bundle coeruleus
than normal. The faster the stomach
empties, the sooner the animal is
Figure 10.26  ​Norepinephrine pathways in the human brain
ready for its next meal. Another rea-
Damage to the ventral noradrenergic bundle leads to overeating and weight gain. (Based on
son for their frequent meals is that the Valzelli, 1980)
damage increases insulin production
320 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

Table 10.2  Effects of Hypothalamic Lesions

Hypothalamic Area Effect of Lesion


Preoptic area Deficit in physiological mechanisms of temperature regulation
Lateral preoptic area Deficit in osmotic thirst due partly to damage to cells and partly to interruption of passing axons
Lateral hypothalamus Undereating, weight loss, low insulin level (because of damage to cell bodies); underarousal,
underresponsiveness (because of damage to passing axons)
Ventromedial hypothalamus Increased meal frequency, weight gain, high insulin level
Paraventricular nucleus Increased meal size, especially increased carbohydrate intake during the first meal of the active period of the day

© Cengage Learning 2013

(B. M. King, Smith, & Frohman, 1984), and therefore, much than food ( Johnson & Kenny, 2010). Many people show the
of each meal is stored as fat. If animals with this kind of dam- same tendency.
age are prevented from overeating, they gain weight anyway! Still, some people become obese and others do not, even
According to Mark Friedman and Edward Stricker (1976), when all have access to the same foods, so it is reasonable to
the problem is not that the rat gets fat from overeating. ask what makes some people more vulnerable than others. For
Rather, the rat overeats because it is storing so much fat. The a time, it was popular to assume that obesity was a reaction
high insulin levels keep moving blood glucose into storage, to psychological distress. True, many distressed people cheer
even when the blood glucose level is low. themselves up temporarily by eating rich foods. However, in
Table 10.2 summarizes the effects of lesions in several ar- the long run mood has only a weak relationship to weight
eas of the hypothalamus. gain. One study found obesity in 19% of people with a his-
tory of depression and in 15% of those who had never suffered
depression (McIntyre, Konarski, Wilkins, Soczynska, & Ken-
Stop & Check nedy, 2006).
Another possible factor is prenatal environment. A study
20. In what way does eating increase after damage in and
in rats found that if a mother consumed a high-fat diet during
around the ventromedial hypothalamus? After damage to
pregnancy, her babies developed a larger than average lateral
the paraventricular nucleus?
hypothalamus and produced more than the average amount
thalamus eat larger meals. of orexin and other transmitters that facilitate increased eat-
ANSWER
damage to the paraventricular nucleus of the hypo- ing (Chang, Gasinskaya, Karatayev, & Leibowitz, 2008).
pothalamus eat more frequent meals. Animals with These changes persisted throughout life. In short, exposure
20. Animals with damage to the ventromedial hy- to a high-fat diet before birth predisposes the offspring to
increased appetite and body weight. This example illustrates
epigenetic effects, as described in Chapter 1: An experience
can alter the expression of the genes.

Eating Disorders Genetics and Body Weight


Obesity has become a serious problem in more and more You have probably noticed that most thin parents have thin
countries. Simultaneously, other people suffer from anorexia, children, and most heavy parents have heavy children. A Dan-
in which they refuse to eat enough to survive, or bulimia, in ish study found that the weights of 540 adopted children cor-
which they alternate between eating too much and eating too related more strongly with that of their biological relatives
little. Evidently, our homeostatic or allostatic mechanisms are than with that of their adoptive relatives (Stunkard et al.,
not fully doing their job. 1986). That result has generally been taken as evidence for a
The increasing prevalence of obesity obviously relates to genetic influence, but it could just as easily be evidence for the
the increased availability of our diet and our sedentary life- effects of prenatal environment.
style. It is possible (in fact, easy) to make rats obese by giv- In some cases, obesity can be traced to the effects of a single
ing them what researchers call a “cafeteria,” which is really an gene. The most common of these is a mutated gene for the re-
all-you-can-eat buffet consisting of chocolate, cheese, salami, ceptor to melanocortin, one of the neuropeptides responsible
peanut butter, marshmallows, and other tasty, high-calorie for hunger. People with a mutation in that gene overeat and be-
foods (Geiger et al., 2009). It is hard for rats to pass up come obese from childhood onward (Mergen, Mergen, Ozata,
these treats, and hard for us, too. When rats become obese Oner, & Oner, 2001). People with a variant form of one gene
on this regimen, they tend to lose interest in rewards other called FTO weigh 3 kg (6–7 lb) more than other people, on
10.3 Hunger 321

average, and have about a two-thirds greater probability of quence is that people are relieved from thinking of them-
becoming obese (Frayling et al., 2007). However, these single- selves as morally guilty for being overweight. A possible
gene mutations are uncommon, accounting for only a small negative consequence is that some may decide to give up.
percentage of obesity cases (Mutch & Clément, 2006). The most practical consequence is that insurance companies
Syndromal obesity is obesity that results from a medical will now pay treatment providers to help obese patients . . . if
condition. Some people with severe, early-onset obesity have the providers have evidence that their treatment is safe and
deletions of part of a chromosome. If the deletion includes effective.
genes for leptin receptors, insulin receptors, or other key ele- Dieting by itself is not reliably effective, largely because
ments in regulation of eating, the outcome includes obesity as most people don’t stick to the diet for long. You will hear advo-
well as other problems (Bochukova et al., 2010). Prader-Willi cates of a particular diet plan state that many people on their
syndrome is a genetic condition marked by mental retarda- plan lost a significant amount of weight. That statement may
tion, short stature, and obesity. People with this syndrome be true, but it means little unless we know how many other
have blood levels of ghrelin four to five times higher than av- people tried that plan and failed to lose weight. We also need
erage (Cummings et al., 2002). Ghrelin, you will recall, is a to know how many people who lost weight gained it back. Ac-
peptide related to food deprivation. The fact that people with cording to one review of the literature, about as many people
Prader-Willi syndrome overeat and still produce high ghrelin gain weight on a diet as lose, and few maintain a significant
levels suggests that their problem relates to an inability to turn weight loss for years (Mann et al., 2007). Many psychologists
off ghrelin release. now recommend small changes in diet (“eat a little less than
Most cases of obesity relate to the combined influences usual”) on the expectation that more people will stick to this
of many genes and the environment. Consider the Native diet, and making a small change is better than failing to make
American Pima of Arizona and Mexico. Most are seriously a large change (Stroebele et al., 2008).
overweight, apparently because of several genes (Norman The most successful treatments require a change of life-
et al., 1998). However, obesity was uncommon among them style, including increased exercise as well as decreased eating.
in the early 1900s, when their diet consisted of desert plants That combination does help people lose weight, although only
that ripen in the brief rainy season. The Pima apparently 20%–40% keep the weight off for at least 2 years (Powell, Cal-
evolved a strategy of eating all they could when food was avail- vin, & Calvin, 2007).
able because it would have to carry them through periods of Particularly important advice is to reduce or eliminate
scarcity. They also evolved a tendency to conserve energy by the intake of soft drinks. Researchers have found that peo-
limiting their activity. Now, with a more typical U.S. diet that ple who consume at least one soft drink per day are more
is equally available at all times, the strategy of overeating and likely than others to be overweight, and if they are not al-
inactivity is maladaptive. In short, their weight depends on the ready overweight, they are more likely than others to become
combination of genes and environment. Neither one by itself overweight (Dhingra et al., 2007; Liebman et al., 2006). One
has this effect. reason is that nearly all soft drinks are sweetened with fruc-
How might genes affect weight gain? Differences in hun- tose, a sugar that does not increase insulin or leptin nearly
ger or digestion are one possibility, but exercise is another. One as much as other sugars do (Teff et al., 2004). Therefore, if
study found that mildly obese people spent more time sitting you drink something with fructose, you gain calories with-
and less time moving about, both while they were obese and out feeling satiety.
after they had lost weight ( J. A. Levine et al., 2005). Evidently, Diet soft drinks do not contain fructose, but they pose
their sedentary habits were a lifelong trait, perhaps genetic in a different problem. In one study, rats mostly ate the usual
origin, rather than a reaction to being overweight. laboratory diet, but sometimes one group ate naturally sweet-
ened yogurt while the other group ate yogurt sweetened with
saccharin (noncaloric). Overall, the rats eating the nonca-
Stop & Check loric (“diet”) yogurt gained more weight. The interpretation
21. Why did the Pima begin gaining weight in the mid-1900s? is complex: Ordinarily, rats, like people, learn to calibrate the
calories in their food. They learn that when they eat sweets,
ANSWER
able throughout the year. they gain a good deal of energy, and so they learn either to
seasonally available to a calorie-rich diet that is avail- limit their intake of sweets or to compensate by eating less
21. They shifted from a diet of local plants that were of something else. Rats that consumed noncaloric sweeteners
lost this tendency. They learned that taste is a poor predictor
of energy, and so they overate other foods and stopped com-
pensating afterward. They also became less active (Swithers
& Davidson, 2008).
Weight Loss If diet and exercise fail to help someone lose weight, an-
In the United States, obesity is now officially classified as a other option is weight-loss drugs. For years, the most effective
disease, and never mind the fact that we don’t have a clear combination was “fen–phen”: Fenfluramine increases the re-
definition of what we mean by disease. One positive conse- lease of serotonin and blocks its reuptake. Phentermine blocks
322 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

reuptake of norepinephrine and dopamine and therefore pro- of large quantities of very tasty high-calorie foods that were
longs their activity. The fen–phen combination produces brain less abundant in previous eras.
effects similar to those of a completed meal (Rada & Hoebel, On average, people with bulimia show a variety of bio-
2000). Unfortunately, fenfluramine often produces medical chemical abnormalities, including increased production of
complications, so it has been withdrawn from use. A replace- ghrelin, a hormone associated with increased appetite (Mon-
ment drug, sibutramine (Meridia), which blocks reuptake of teleone, Serritella, Scognamiglio, & Maj, 2010). The bio-
serotonin and norepinephrine, decreases meal size and binge chemistry is probably a result of the binges and purges, rather
eating (Appolinario et al., 2003). The drug orlistat (Xenical) than a cause. After therapy that decreases the symptoms of
prevents the intestines from absorbing up to 30% of fats in the bulimia, the ghrelin and other body chemicals return toward
diet. Approximately half of people using orlistat have at least normal levels (Tanaka et al., 2006).
5% weight loss 2 years later (Powell et al., 2007). A side effect In important ways bulimia resembles drug addiction
is intestinal discomfort from the large globs of undigested fats. (Hoebel, Rada, Mark, & Pothos, 1999). Eating tasty foods
Also, the bowel movements are thick with fat. activates the same brain areas as addictive drugs, such as
Finally, if someone with extreme obesity fails to respond the nucleus accumbens. Drug addicts who cannot get drugs
to other treatments, an option is gastric bypass surgery, in sometimes overeat as a substitute, and food-deprived people
which part of the stomach is removed or sewed off so that or animals become more likely than others to use drugs.
food cannot enter. Remember that stomach distension is a Researchers examined rats that were food deprived for 12
major contributor to satiety. By decreasing stomach size, the hours a day, including the first 4 hours of their wakeful period,
surgery makes it possible for a smaller meal to produce sati- and then offered a very sweet, syrupy sugar solution. Over sev-
ety. The most common result is that someone goes from being eral weeks on this regimen, the rats drank more and more each
“morbidly obese” to just “obese,” and that is a meaningful ben- day, especially during the first hour of availability each day.
efit. However, 10%–20% of people experience serious side ef- The intake released dopamine and opioid (opiate-like) com-
fects, including infections, bowel obstruction, leakage of food, pounds in the brain, similar to the effects of highly addictive
and nutritional deficiencies (Powell et al., 2007). Surgery is drugs (Colantuoni et al., 2001, 2002). It also increased the lev-
worth considering only in severe cases of obesity. els of dopamine type 3 receptors in the brain—again, a trend
resembling that of rats that receive morphine (Spangler et al.,
2004). If they were then deprived of this sweet liquid, they
showed withdrawal symptoms, including head shaking, teeth
Stop & Check chattering, and tremors. An injection of morphine relieved
22. In one study, rats eating the less-caloric yogurt gained these symptoms. In short, the rats showed clear indications
more weight than those eating the more-caloric type. of an addiction to big doses of sugar (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel,
What explanation was proposed? 2008). Similarly, we can regard bulimic cycles of dieting and
binge eating as an addiction.
stopped compensating after eating other sweets.
ANSWER
more sweets mean more energy and therefore
22. The rats unlearned their usual calibration that
Stop & Check
23. Researchers have found that people with bulimia nervosa
have elevated ghrelin levels. Why are those levels prob-
ably not the cause of bulimia?
Bulimia Nervosa
Bulimia nervosa is a condition in which people alternate be- 24. What evidence from rats suggests that bulimia resembles
tween binges of overeating and periods of strict dieting. Many, an addiction?
but not all, induce themselves to vomit. About 95% of people like the symptoms of morphine withdrawal.
with bulimia also suffer from depression, anxiety, or other ANSWERS
the sweet diet with head shaking and teeth chattering,
emotional problems (Hudson et al., 2007). In the United eat more and more, and they react to deprivation of
States, about 1.5% of women and 0.5% of men develop buli- tween food deprivation and a very sweet diet gradually
mia at some time in life. It has become more common over the regardless of current diet. 24. Rats that alternate be-
years. That is, bulimia is more common among young people produced constantly high ghrelin, it should remain high
today than it ever was in their parents’ generation and more toward normal levels. If their genetics or other factors
common in their parents’ generation than in their grandpar- 23. As people recover from bulimia, the ghrelin returns
ents’. The increase is presumably due to the ready availability
10.3 Hunger 323

Module 10.3  ■  In Closing

The Multiple Controls of Hunger

The brain areas that control eating monitor blood glucose, makes a mistake, another part can counteract it. We notice peo-
stomach distension, duodenal contents, body weight, fat cells, ple who choose a poor diet or eat the wrong amount. Perhaps
hormones, and more. Because the system is so complex, it can we should be even more impressed by how many people eat ap-
produce errors in many ways. However, the complexity of the propriately. The regulation of eating succeeds not in spite of its
system also provides a kind of security: If one part of the system complexity but because of it.

Summary
  1. The ability to digest a food is one major determinant of   8. Axons from the two kinds of neurons in the arcuate
preference for that food. For example, people who nucleus send competing messages to the paraventricu-
cannot digest lactose generally do not like to eat dairy lar nucleus, releasing neuropeptides that are specific
products.  309 to the feeding system. The paraventricular nucleus
  2. Widespread beliefs that sugar causes hyperactivity and inhibits the lateral nucleus of the hypothala-
that turkey causes sleepiness are unfounded. However, mus.  316
research does support the idea that eating fish enhances   9. The lateral nucleus of the hypothalamus facilitates
some people’s memory and reasoning.  310 feeding by axons that enhance taste responses elsewhere
  3. People and animals eat partly for the sake of taste. in the brain and increase the release of insulin and
However, a sham-feeding animal, which tastes its foods digestive juices.  316
but does not absorb them, eats far more than nor- 10. The ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus and
mal.  311 the axons passing by it influence eating by regulating
  4. Factors controlling hunger include distension of the stomach emptying time and insulin secretion. Animals
stomach and intestines, secretion of CCK by the with damage in this area eat more frequently than
duodenum, and the availability of glucose and other normal because they store much of each meal as
nutrients to the cells.  312 fat and then fail to mobilize their stored fats for
current use.  318
  5. Appetite depends partly on the availability of glucose and
other nutrients to the cells. The hormone insulin increases 11. Obesity is partly under genetic control, although
the entry of glucose to the cells, including cells that store no single gene accounts for many cases of obesity.
nutrients for future use. Glucagon mobilizes stored fuel The effects of genes depend on what foods are avail-
and converts it to glucose in the blood. Thus, the com- able. Genes also influence activity levels.  320
bined influence of insulin and glucagon determines how 12. Dieting is seldom an effective means of long-term
much glucose is available at any time.  312 weight loss. Dieting combined with exercise is
  6. Fat cells produce a peptide called leptin, which provides more effective, although at best it helps less than
the brain with a signal about weight loss or gain and half of people. Reducing consumption of soft drinks
therefore corrects day-to-day errors in the amount of is highly recommended. In more severe cases of
feeding. Deficiency of leptin production leads to obesity obesity, people consider weight-loss drugs or
and inactivity. However, leptin deficiency is rare among surgery.  321
humans.  314 13. Bulimia nervosa is characterized by alternation between
  7. The arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus receives undereating and overeating. It has been compared to
signals of both hunger and satiety. Good-tasting foods addictive behaviors.  322
and the transmitter ghrelin stimulate neurons that
promote hunger. Glucose, insulin, leptin, and CCK
stimulate neurons that promote satiety.  315
324 Chapter 10 Internal Regulation

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
agouti-related peptide glucagon  313 neuropeptide Y (NPY)  316
(AgRP)  316 insulin  313 sham-feeding  312
arcuate nucleus  316 lactase  310 splanchnic nerves  312
bulimia nervosa  322 lactose  310 vagus nerve  312
cholecystokinin (CCK)  312 lateral hypothalamus  317 ventromedial hypothalamus
duodenum  312 leptin  314 (VMH)  318
ghrelin  316 melanocortin  316

Thought Question

For most people, insulin levels tend to be higher during the hungry a few hours after a daytime meal but not so quickly
day than at night. Use this fact to explain why people grow at night.

chapter 10 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
10.3 Hunger 325

Videos

© 2013 Cengage Learning Profile of Bulimia Nervosa

Animations
The Retina nnn (Figure x.x) Also available—
Hypothalamic Control of Feeding
© 2013 Cengage Learning

■ Pathways from the Lateral Hypothalamus

Thirst

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Gisolfi, C. V., & Mora, F. (2000). The hot brain: Survival, temperature, and the human body. Cam-
bridge, MA: MIT Press. Discusses research on temperature regulation.
Widmaier, E. P. (1998). Why geese don’t get obese (and we do). New York: Freeman. Lighthearted
and often entertaining discussion of the physiology of eating, thirst, and temperature regulation.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, including sites concerning the hypothalamus and sugar addiction.
Anup and Manoj Shah/www.shahimages.com/Shah Images
Reproductive Behaviors 11
Chapter Outline 5. Much about men’s and women’s sexual behavior,
including mate choice, could be the product of
Module 11.1 ​Sex and Hormones
evolutionary selection. However, current data do not
Organizing Effects of Sex Hormones
enable us to determine how much is built-in and how
Activating Effects of Sex Hormones
much is determined by our experiences.
Parental Behavior
In Closing: Reproductive Behaviors and Motivations 6. Hormones contribute to the development of sexual
identity and orientation.
Module 11.2 ​Variations in Sexual Behavior
Evolutionary Interpretations of Mating Behavior
Gender Identity and Gender-Differentiated Behaviors
Sexual Orientation
In Closing: We Are Not All the Same
Interactive Exploration and Study

W
Main Ideas hat good is sex? Well, yes, of course: We enjoy it.
Presumably we evolved to enjoy it because sexual
1. Sex chromosomes influence development mainly, but not activity sometimes leads to reproduction, which
entirely, by controlling production of sex hormones such passes on the genes. You evolved from a long line of ancestors
as testosterone and estradiol. who engaged in sexual activity at least once.
2. Sex hormones exert organizing and activating effects on But why did we evolve to reproduce sexually instead of in-
the genitals and the brain. Organizing effects occur dividually? In some species of reptiles, a female sometimes has
during a sensitive period and last indefinitely. Activating offspring by herself, using only her own genes and none from
effects are temporary. a male (Booth, Johnson, Moore, Schal, & Vargo, 2011). In
many ways, reproduction would be easier without sex. What
3. In mammals, organizing effects of hormones influence advantage does sex provide?
the external genitals and the hypothalamus. The You might suggest the advantage of having a partner while
difference between masculine and feminine appearance of you rear children. In humans, that kind of cooperation is usu-
the external genitals depends on the amount of ally helpful. However, many species reproduce sexually even
testosterone during an early sensitive period. though the male doesn’t help at all with the young, and in some
4. Parental behavior depends on both hormones and fish species, neither sex cares for the young—they just release
experience. their sperm and eggs in the same place and then depart.
Biologists’ explanation is that sexual reproduction in-
creases variation and thereby enables quick evolutionary ad-
aptations to changes in the environment. Certain invertebrates
reproduce sexually when they live in a complex and changing
environment, but reproduce nonsexually when they live in a
constant environment (Becks & Agrawal, 2010). Sex also cor-
rects errors: If you have a disadvantageous mutation in one
gene and your mate has a disadvantageous mutation in a differ-
ent gene, your children could have a normal copy of both genes.
In this chapter, we consider many questions about sexual
OPPOSITE: Humans may be the only species that plans
parenthood, but all species have a strong biological drive that
reproduction that we often ignore or take for granted. We also
leads to parenthood. consider some of the ways in which being biologically male or
female influences our behavior.
327
Module 11.1

Sex and Hormones

B
eing male or female influences many aspects of your are more abundant in males) that increase the growth of
life. For humans and other mammals, it all begins with the testes, causing them to produce more androgens and
your genes. Females have two X chromosomes, so forth. That positive feedback cannot go on forever, but
whereas males have an X and a Y chromosome. Biologists it lasts for a period of early development. Androgens also
used to believe that the chromosomes determine sexual cause the primitive Wolffian ducts, precursors for other
differentiation entirely through hormones. Let’s examine that male reproductive structures, to develop into seminal vesicles
story, and then see how it is incomplete. (saclike structures that store semen) and the vas deferens (a
Male and female mammals start with the same anatomy duct from the testis into the penis). The testes also produce
during an early stage of prenatal development. Both have a Müllerian inhibiting hormone (MIH), which causes the Mül-
set of Müllerian ducts (precursors to female internal struc- lerian ducts to degenerate. The final result is the develop-
tures) and a set of Wolffian ducts (precursors to male inter- ment of a penis and scrotum. Because females do not have
nal structures), as well as undifferentiated gonads that are the SRY gene, their gonads develop into ovaries instead of
on their way to becoming either testes or ovaries. The male’s testes, and their Wolffian ducts degenerate. Because their
Y chromosome includes the SRY (sex-determining region ovaries do not produce MIH, females’ Müllerian ducts de-
on the Y chromosome) gene, which causes those primitive velop and mature into oviducts, uterus, and the upper va-
gonads to develop into testes, the sperm-producing organs. gina. Figure 11.1 shows how the primitive unisex structures
The developing testes produce androgens (hormones that develop into male or female external genitals.

Female Male
Fallopian Seminal
tube vesicle

Gonad
Wolffian Prostate
duct
Ovary Uterus Vas
Müllerian deferens
duct
Vagina Urethra Testis

Epididymis
Urethra

Undifferentiated condition in fetus

Figure 11.1  ​Differentiation of human genitals


We begin life with undifferentiated structures, as shown in the center. The gonad shown in blue for the fetus develops into either the ova-
ries, as shown on the left, or the testes, as shown on the right. The Müllerian ducts of the fetus develop into a female’s uterus, oviducts,
and the upper part of the vagina. The Wolffian ducts of the fetus develop into a male’s seminal vesicles (which store semen) and vas
deferens, a duct from the testis into the penis. The Müllerian ducts degenerate in males, and the Wolffian ducts degenerate in females.
(Based on Netter, 1983)

328
11.1 Sex and Hormones 329

From then on, the male’s testes produce more androgens (Blood capillary)
than estrogens (hormones that are more abundant in females).
The female’s ovaries produce more estrogens than androgens. Hormone binds to
Androgens and estrogens are steroid hormones, containing four membrane receptor
carbon rings, derived from cholesterol, as in Figure 11.2. We are
often warned about the risks of excessive cholesterol, but a mod-
erate amount is necessary for generating these important hor-
mones. Steroids exert their effects in three ways (Nadal, Díaz, (Membrane)
& Valverde, 2001). First, they bind to membrane receptors, like
neurotransmitters, exerting rapid effects. Second, they enter cells
and activate certain kinds of proteins in the cytoplasm. Third,
they bind to receptors that bind to chromosomes, where they (Cytoplasm)
activate or inactivate certain genes (Figure 11.3).
Androgens and estrogens are categories of chemicals;
neither androgen nor estrogen is a specific chemical itself.
(Nucleus)
The most widely known androgen is testosterone. The most
prominent type of estrogen is estradiol. Progesterone, an-
other predominantly female hormone, prepares the uterus for
the implantation of a fertilized ovum and promotes the main- Hormone activates
tenance of pregnancy. kinase protein in
cytoplasm
(Receptor
molecule)

Backbone of all
steroid molecules

CH2OH

C === O
Hormone activates or
HO OH
inactivates certain genes.
Cortisol

O
OH Figure 11.3  ​Routes of action for steroid hormones
Steroid hormones such as estrogens and androgens bind to mem-
brane receptors, activate proteins in the cytoplasm, and activate
Testosterone or inactivate certain genes. (Revised from Starr & Taggart, 1989)
(an androgen)

O
OH Androgens promote the development of typically mascu-
line features, such as facial hair. Estrogens promote typically
Estradiol female features, such as breast development. Androgens and
(an estrogen) estrogens also influence activity in many brain areas and alter
the pattern of which neurons survive during early develop-
HO CH3 ment (Forger et al., 2004; Morris, Jordan, & Breedlove, 2004).
C === O
Certain brain areas are relatively larger in men, on average,
and others relatively larger in women, as Figure 11.4 shows
(Cahill, 2006; J. M. Goldstein et al., 2001). These differences
Progesterone relate to gender and not to brain size. When researchers com-
pare men and women who have the same overall brain vol-
ume, many of the patterns shown in Figure 11.4 still emerge
O (Luders, Gaser, Narr, & Toga, 2009). However, remember
that these are averages. Most individuals show male-typical
Figure 11.2  ​Steroid hormones
Note the similarity between testosterone and estradiol.
patterns in some ways and female-typical patterns in others.
(© Cengage Learning 2013) For many years, biologists assumed that hormones ac-
count for all the biological differences between males and
330 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Figure 11.4  ​Men’s and women’s brains


Areas in red are, on average, larger in women
relative to the total mass of the brain. Areas in
blue are, on average, larger in men relative to
the total mass. (Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
7, 477–484, from Cahill, L. (2006). Reprinted by
permission of Macmillan Publishing Ltd.)
Anterior Posterior

females. Later research demonstrated that some differences variety of mammals and birds, were surprised to find that the
depend directly on control by the X and Y chromosomes in- answer was almost always no. But hormones injected early in
dependently of hormones (Arnold, 2009). At least three genes life have much stronger effects.
on the Y chromosome (found only in men) are active in spe- Biologists distinguish between the organizing and ac-
cific brain areas, and at least one gene on the X chromosome tivating effects of sex hormones (Arnold, 2009). Organiz-
is active only in the female brain (Arnold, 2004; Carruth, Rei- ing effects produce long-lasting structural effects. The most
sert, & Arnold, 2002; Vawter et al., 2004). In both humans prominent organizing effects occur during a sensitive stage
and nonhumans, the Y chromosome has many sites that alter of early development—shortly before and after birth in rats
the expression of genes on other chromosomes (Lemos, Ara- and well before birth in humans—determining whether the
ripe, & Hartl, 2008). In short, genes on the X and Y chromo- body develops female or male anatomy. The surge of hor-
somes produce sex differences in addition to those that we can mones at puberty also produces long-lasting effects, such
trace to androgens and estrogens. as breast development in women, facial hair in men, and
male–female differences in the anatomy of certain parts of
the hypothalamus (Ahmed et al., 2008). Activating effects
Stop & Check are more temporary, when a hormone increases some activ-
1. What does the SRY gene do? ity that lasts only while the hormone is present. Activating
effects occur at any time in life. The distinction between the
2. How do sex hormones affect neurons? two kinds of effects is not absolute, as a hormone can pro-
inactivate particular genes.
duce a combination of temporary and longer-lasting effects
ANSWERS (Arnold & Breedlove, 1985; C. L. Williams, 1986). Still, the
certain proteins in the cell’s cytoplasm, and activate or
steroids, bind to receptors on the membrane, activate distinction is often useful.
toward the male pattern. 2. Sex hormones, which are Let’s consider organizing effects during an early sensi-
duces testosterone and MIH to direct development tive period, when hormones determine whether an embryo
mammal to develop into a testis, which then pro- develops a male or female anatomy. You might imagine that
chromosome) causes the undifferentiated gonad of a testosterone produces male anatomy and estradiol produces
1. The SRY gene (sex-determining region on the Y female anatomy. No. Differentiation of the external genitals
and several aspects of brain development depend mainly on
the level of testosterone. A high level of testosterone causes
the external genitals to develop the male pattern, and a low
Organizing Effects level leads to the female pattern. Estradiol produces impor-
tant effects on the internal organs, but it has little effect on the
of Sex Hormones external genitals.
If we injected estrogens into adult males and androgens into The human sensitive period for genital formation is
adult females, could we make males act like females or females about the third and fourth months of pregnancy (Money
act like males? Researchers of the mid-1900s, working with a & Ehrhardt, 1972). In rats, testosterone begins masculin-
11.1 Sex and Hormones 331

izing the external genitals during the last several days of Stop & Check
pregnancy and first few days after birth and then continues
masculinizing them at a declining rate for the next month 3. What would be the genital appearance of a mammal
(Bloch & Mills, 1995; Bloch, Mills, & Gale, 1995; E. C. exposed to high levels of both androgens and estrogens
Davis, Shryne, & Gorski, 1995; Rhees, Shryne, & Gor- during early development? What if it were exposed to low
ski, 1990). A female rat that is injected with testosterone levels of both?
shortly before or after birth is partly masculinized, just as 4. From the standpoint of protecting a male fetus’s sexual
if her own body had produced the testosterone (I. L. Ward development, what are some drugs that a pregnant woman
& Ward, 1985). Her clitoris grows larger than normal, and should avoid?
her behavior is partly masculinized. She approaches sexu-
ally receptive females (Woodson & Balleine, 2002), mounts ANSWERS
these chemicals.
them, and makes copulatory thrusting movements rather depend on both quantities and timing of exposure to
than arching her back and allowing males to mount her. produce mild abnormalities. Obviously, the results
In short, early testosterone promotes the male pattern and Even aspirin and the chemicals lining bottles and cans
inhibits the female pattern (Gorski, 1985; J. D. Wilson, these drugs interfere with male sexual development.
George, & Griffin, 1981).
juana, haloperidol, phthalates, and cocaine because

Injecting a genetic male with estrogens produces little


levels. 4. Pregnant women should avoid alcohol, mari-
of androgens and is nearly independent of estradiol
effect on his external anatomy. However, he develops the ment depends mostly on the presence or absence
female-typical pattern of anatomy and behavior if he ge- low levels of both will appear female. Genital develop-
netically lacks androgen receptors, if he is castrated (de- female hormones will appear male. One exposed to
prived of his testes), or if he is exposed to substances 3. A mammal exposed to high levels of both male and
that block testosterone effects. Drugs that tend to femi-
nize or demasculinize early development include alcohol,
marijuana, haloperidol (an antipsychotic drug), phthalates
(chemicals common in many manufactured products), and
cocaine (Ahmed, Shryne, Gorski, Branch, & Taylor, 1991; Sex Differences in the Hypothalamus
Dalterio & Bartke, 1979; Hull, Nishita, Bitran, & Dalte- In addition to controlling differences in the external genitals,
rio, 1984; Raum, McGivern, Peterson, Shryne, & Gorski, sex hormones early in life influence development in parts of
1990; Swan et al., 2010). To a slight extent, even aspirin the hypothalamus, amygdala, and other brain areas (Shah et
interferes with the male pattern of development (Amateau al., 2004). For example, one area in the anterior hypothala-
& McCarthy, 2004). Although estradiol does not signifi- mus, known as the sexually dimorphic nucleus, is larger
cantly alter a male’s external anatomy, estradiol and several in males than in females and contributes to control of male
related compounds do produce abnormalities of the pros- sexual behavior. Parts of the female hypothalamus generate a
tate gland—the gland that stores sperm and releases it dur- cyclic pattern of hormone release, as in the human menstrual
ing intercourse. Some of those estradiol-like compounds cycle. The male hypothalamus cannot, and neither can the hy-
are now prevalent in the linings of plastic bottles and pothalamus of a female who was exposed to extra testosterone
cans, so almost everyone is exposed to them (Timms, early in life. Typical female rats have a characteristic way of
Howdeshell, Barton, Richter, & vom Saal, 2005). In short, holding food and dodging from other rats that might try to
male development is vulnerable to many sources of inter- take it away. A female rat that was either deprived of estrogens
ference. or exposed to extra testosterone in infancy pivots around the
The overall mechanism of early sexual differentiation midpoint of her trunk, like males, instead of around her pelvis,
has been described by saying that nature’s “default setting” like other females (Field, Whishaw, Forgie, & Pellis, 2004).
is to make every mammal a female. Add early testosterone In rodents, testosterone exerts much of its organizing
and the individual becomes a male; without testosterone, it effect through a surprising route: After it enters a neuron in
develops as a female, regardless of the amount of estradiol early development, it is converted to estradiol! Testosterone
or other estrogens. That generalization is an overstatement. and estradiol are chemically very similar, as you can see in
A genetic female that lacks estradiol during the early sensi- Figure 11.2. In organic chemistry, a ring of six carbon atoms
tive period develops approximately normal female external containing three double bonds is an aromatic compound. An
anatomy but does not develop normal sexual behavior. Even enzyme found in the brain can aromatize testosterone into es-
if she is given estradiol injections as an adult, she shows lit- tradiol. Other androgens that cannot be aromatized into es-
tle sexual response toward either male or female partners trogens are less effective in masculinizing the hypothalamus.
(Bakker, Honda, Harada, & Balthazart, 2002). So estradiol Drugs that prevent testosterone from being aromatized to es-
contributes to female development, including certain as- tradiol block the organizing effects of testosterone on sexual
pects of brain differentiation, even if it is not important for development and thereby impair male sexual behavior and
external anatomy. fertility (Gerardin & Pereira, 2002; Rochira et al., 2001).
332 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Why, then, does a female rodent’s own estradiol fail to mas- such as dolls and toy tea sets. Some children have a stronger
culinize her hypothalamus? During the early sensitive period, preference for boys’ or girls’ toys than others do, and their prefer-
immature mammals have a protein called alpha-fetoprotein, ences tend to be consistent over time. Those who show the great-
which is not present in adults (Gorski, 1980; MacLusky & est preference for boys’ toys and activities at age 3½ usually show
Naftolin, 1981). Alpha-fetoprotein in rodents binds with the greatest amount of typical boys’ activities at age 8, and they
estradiol and prevents it from entering cells, where it could tend to be the most physically active at age 12. Similarly, those
produce masculinizing effects. Because testosterone does not with the greatest preference for girls’ toys and activities at 3½
bind to alpha-fetoprotein, it can enter neurons, where en- usually show the greatest preference for typical girls’ activities at
zymes convert it into estradiol. That is, testosterone is a way later ages (Golombok et al., 2008; Mattocks et al., 2010).
of getting estradiol to its receptors when estradiol circulating Much of this pattern results from socialization, as most
in the blood is inactivated. parents give their sons and daughters different sets of toys.
This explanation of testosterone’s effects makes sense of an However, socialization need not be the whole story. Indeed,
otherwise puzzling fact: Injecting a large amount of estradiol it may be that parents give those toys because previous gen-
actually masculinizes a female rodent’s development. The rea- erations found that boys and girls typically differ in their in-
son is that normal amounts are bound to alpha-fetoprotein, terests from the start. In one study, infants 3–8 months old
but a larger amount exceeds the capacity of alpha-fetoprotein (too young to walk, crawl, or do much with a toy) sat in front
and therefore enters the cells and masculinizes them. of pairs of toys, where researchers could monitor eye move-
ments. The girls looked at dolls more than they looked at toy
trucks. The boys looked at both about equally (Alexander,
Stop & Check Wilcox, & Woods, 2009). (Note that the children had not
seen the trucks move, so at this point the trucks were sim-
5. How would the external genitals appear on a genetic fe-
ply unknown objects.) This study suggests a predisposition
male rat that lacked alpha-fetoprotein?
for boys and girls to prefer different types of toys, although
have in fact demonstrated (Bakker et al., 2006). we should consider an alternative explanation: Girls mature
ANSWER
masculinized by her own estradiol, as researchers faster than boys, and perhaps it was harder for boys at this age
5. A female that lacked alpha-fetoprotein would be to show a preference, whatever that preference may have been.
In two studies male monkeys played with balls and toy
cars more than female monkeys did, whereas the females
played more with dolls (Alexander & Hines, 2002; Hassett,
Siebert, & Wallen, 2008). Figure 11.5 summarizes the results
Sex Differences in Childhood Behavior from one of those studies. Monkeys’ preferences were not as
As prenatal hormones influence the structure of the male or strong as most children’s, but it is noteworthy that the sexes
female brain, do they also contribute to differences in behav- differed at all in their first encounters with these toys. Other
ior? In the second module of this chapter we shall consider in- studies found that prenatal injections of testosterone into
fluences on sexual behavior and sexual orientation, but at this female monkey fetuses led to increased masculine-type play
point let’s consider possible influences on childhood behavior. after they were born. In those cases the focus was on sponta-
Typically, most boys play mostly with “boys’ toys” such as neous, rough-and-tumble play rather than playing with toys,
balls and toy cars, whereas most girls play mostly with “girls’ toys” but the idea is similar (Wallen, 2005).

Boys’ toys Boys’ toys


Girls’ toys Girls’ toys
Neutral toys Neutral toys

Male monkeys Female monkeys

Figure 11.5  ​Toy choices by male and female monkeys


Male monkeys spent more time than female monkeys did with “boys’ toys” and the females spent more time than the males with “girls’
toys.” (Based on data from G. M. Alexander & M. Hines (2002). “Sex differences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman primates” (Cerco-
pithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.)
11.1 Sex and Hormones 333

Finally, two studies correlated chemicals in the mother’s monal secretions. For example, when doves court each other,
blood during pregnancy with their children’s choices of toys each stage of their behavior initiates hormonal changes that
years later. Researchers took blood samples from pregnant alter the birds’ readiness for the next sequence of behaviors
women, measuring testosterone (some of which would enter (C. Erickson & Lehrman, 1964; Lehrman, 1964; Martinez-
the fetus). When the daughters reached age 31/2, researchers ob- Vargas & Erickson, 1973).
served their toy play. The girls who had been exposed to higher
testosterone levels in prenatal life showed slightly elevated
preferences for boys’ toys (Hines et al., 2002). These girls were Rodents
anatomically normal, and we have no reason to believe that the For rodents, as for other mammals, sex hormones facili-
parents treated girls differently based on how much testoster- tate sexual activity. Testosterone is essential for male sexual
one had been present in prenatal life. In another study, research- arousal (Hull & Dominguez, 2007). A combination of estra-
ers measured phthalate levels in pregnant women. Phthalates diol and progesterone is the most effective combination for
inhibit testosterone production. U.S. law bans phthalates from females (Matuszewich, Lorrain, & Hull, 2000). Arousal also
children’s toys, but pregnant women are exposed to phthalates depends on previous experience. Sexually experienced rats are
from other sources, including perfumes, hair spray, and food aroused more easily because the effects of previous experience
packaging. Researchers measured phthalate levels in pregnant sensitize the response to future stimuli (Dominguez, Brann,
women’s urine samples and compared results to the sons’ toy Gil, & Hull, 2006).
use at ages 3 to 6. On average, sons of women with high phthal- Sex hormones activate sexual behavior partly by enhanc-
ate levels showed less interest in typical boys’ toys and more in- ing sensations. Estrogens increase the sensitivity of the puden-
terest in typical girls’ toys (Swan et al., 2010). In summary, these dal nerve, which transmits tactile stimulation from the vagina
studies suggest that prenatal hormones, especially testosterone, and cervix to the brain (Komisaruk, Adler, & Hutchison,
alter the brain in ways that influence differences between boys 1972). Testosterone increases sensitivity in the penis (Etgen,
and girls in their activities and interests. Chu, Fiber, Karkanias, & Morales, 1999). Sex hormones also
Do these studies imply that prenatal hormones determine bind to receptors that increase responses in parts of the hy-
toy preferences, regardless of rearing? No. Prenatal hormones pothalamus, including the ventromedial nucleus, the medial
combine forces with rearing experiences. When a child shows preoptic area (MPOA), and the anterior hypothalamus.
a preference for a certain kind of toy, even if it is just a small Erection depends partly on the fact that testosterone in-
preference, parents tend to provide more of that kind of toy creases the release of nitric oxide (NO), which increases blood
and more opportunities to strengthen that preference. Psy- flow to the penis. (As mentioned in Chapter 3, nitric oxide
chologists call this a “multiplier effect” (Dickens & Flynn, also increases blood flow in the brain.) The drug sildenafil
2001). Furthermore, if most boys in the past have preferred (Viagra) increases male sexual ability by prolonging the effects
one kind of toy and most girls preferred another, parents start of nitric oxide (Rowland & Burnett, 2000).
with this presupposition and provide those toys from the start. Testosterone and estradiol prime the MPOA and several
other brain areas to release dopamine. MPOA neurons re-
lease dopamine strongly during sexual activity, and the more
Stop & Check dopamine they release, the more likely the male is to copulate
6. What evidence most directly links children’s toy play to
(Putnam, Du, Sato, & Hull, 2001). Castrated male rats pro-
prenatal hormones?
duce normal amounts of dopamine in the MPOA, but they
do not release it in the presence of a receptive female, and they
toys less than the average for other boys. do not attempt to copulate (Hull, Du, Lorrain, & Matusze-
ANSWER
had higher phthalate exposure tend to play with boys’ wich, 1997).
than the average for other girls. Boys whose mothers In moderate concentrations, dopamine stimulates mostly
during pregnancy tend to play with boys’ toys more type D1 and D5 receptors, which facilitate erection of the penis
6. Girls whose mothers had higher testosterone levels in the male (Hull et al., 1992) and sexually receptive postures
in the female (Apostolakis et al., 1996). In higher concentra-
tions, dopamine stimulates type D2 receptors, which leads
to orgasm (Giuliani & Ferrari, 1996; Hull et al., 1992). The
sudden burst of dopamine in several brain areas at the time
Activating Effects of orgasm resembles the “rush” that addictive drugs produce
(Holstege et al., 2003). Whereas dopamine stimulates sexual
of Sex Hormones activity, the neurotransmitter serotonin inhibits it, in part by
At any time in life, not just during a sensitive period, current blocking dopamine release (Hull et al., 1999). Many popular
levels of testosterone or estradiol exert activating effects, tem- antidepressant drugs increase serotonin activity, and one of
porarily modifying behavior. Changes in hormonal secretions their side effects is to decrease sexual arousal and orgasm.
influence sexual behavior within 15 minutes (Taziaux, Keller, Researchers found what appeared to be a major difference
Bakker, & Balthazart, 2007). Behaviors can also influence hor- between male and female rats in their sexual motivation: After
334 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

rats have had sexual relations in a particular cage, males work Anders & Watson, 2006). Also, both men and women with
hard to return to that cage, but females generally do not. Then high testosterone levels are more likely than average to seek
the researchers varied the procedure. A male rat was confined additional sex partners, even after they marry or establish a
to that cage, but the female was free to enter or leave at any long-term relationship (M. McIntyre et al., 2006; van Anders,
time. She could therefore control the timing of when their Hamilton, & Watson, 2007).
sexual activity started and stopped. Under these conditions, Overall, these results say that high testosterone levels are
females developed a clear preference for that cage (Paredes associated with seeking multiple partners, to some extent for
& Vazquez, 1999). Evidently, female rats find sex reinforcing both men and women. It is tempting to assume that testos-
only if they get to decide when it occurs. (The rumor is that terone causes the drive for multiple partners, but the data are
the same trend may be true for other species as well.) correlational, and we should hesitate about a cause and effect
conclusion. The alternative interpretation is that some other
influence leads to an interest in multiple partners, and the
Stop & Check variety of partners increases testosterone production. It has
been shown that when women think about sex or anticipate
7. By what mechanism do testosterone and estradiol affect
having sex, their testosterone levels increase temporarily (van
the hypothalamic areas responsible for sexual behavior?
Anders, Brotto, Farrell, & Yule, 2009; van Anders, Hamilton,
sensitivity in the genital area. Schmidt, & Watson, 2007). So, the relationship between tes-
ANSWER
to be ready to release dopamine. They also increase tosterone and sexual interest may go both directions.
7. Testosterone and estradiol prime hypothalamic cells
Sexual
Testosterone
interest

Decreases in testosterone levels generally decrease male


Humans sexual activity. For example, castration (removal of the testes)
Although humans are less dependent on current sex hormone generally decreases a man’s sexual interest and activity (Carter,
levels than other species are, hormones do alter people’s sexual 1992). However, low testosterone is not the usual basis for im-
arousal. They also affect brain systems with functions not di- potence, the inability to have an erection. The most common
rectly related to sex. For example, testosterone decreases pain cause is impaired blood circulation, especially in older men.
and anxiety, and estrogens probably do, too (Edinger & Frye, Other common causes include neurological problems, reac-
2004). Decreases of sex hormones—for example, in men being tions to drugs, and psychological tension (Andersson, 2001).
treated for prostate cancer—lead to impairments of memory Testosterone reduction has sometimes been tried as a
(Bussiere, Beer, Neiss, & Janowsky, 2005). Estrogens directly means of controlling sex offenders, including exhibitionists,
stimulate parts of the prefrontal cortex that are important for rapists, child molesters, and those who commit incest. One
working memory—that is, memory for what one is doing at major practical problem is getting sex offenders to continue
the moment (Wang, Hara, Janssen, Rapp, & Morrison, 2010). taking drugs that block testosterone (Hughes, 2007). An-
In short, sex hormones serve functions that go beyond sexual other drawback is the side effects of testosterone deprivation,
behavior itself. including weight gain, diabetes, and depression (Giltay &
Gooren, 2009).
Testosterone
Among men, levels of testosterone correlate positively with
sexual arousal and the drive to seek sexual partners. Research- Stop & Check
ers found that, on average, married men and men living with
8. What is the explanation for why married men tend to have
a woman in a committed relationship have lower testosterone
lower testosterone levels than single men of the same
levels than single, unpaired men of the same age (M. McIntyre
age?
et al., 2006). The apparently obvious interpretation was that
once a man established a lasting relationship, he no longer levels.
ANSWER
needed to work so hard to seek a sexual partner, and his tes- to get married than are men with higher testosterone
tosterone levels dropped. However, that study did not tell us 8. Men with lower testosterone levels are more likely
which came first, the committed relationship or the lower tes-
tosterone level. Another study found that men’s testosterone
levels did not change after marriage. Instead, men with lower
testosterone levels were more likely to marry than were men Estradiol and Related Hormones
with high testosterone levels (van Anders & Watson, 2006). A woman’s hypothalamus and pituitary interact with the
Similar studies found that single women had higher testoster- ovaries to produce the menstrual cycle, a periodic variation
one levels than women with a long-term partner, either ho- in hormones and fertility over the course of about 28 days
mosexual or heterosexual (van Anders & Goldey, 2010; van (Figure 11.6). After the end of a menstrual period, the ante-
11.1 Sex and Hormones 335

rior pituitary releases follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH), inhibits the further release of LH. Toward the end of the
which promotes the growth of a follicle in the ovary. The fol- menstrual cycle, the levels of LH, FSH, estradiol, and proges-
licle nurtures the ovum (egg cell) and produces several types terone all decline. If the ovum is not fertilized, the lining of the
of estrogen, including estradiol. Toward the middle of the uterus is cast off (menstruation), and the cycle begins again.
menstrual cycle, the follicle builds up more and more recep- If the ovum is fertilized, the levels of estradiol and progester-
tors to FSH, so even though the actual concentration of FSH one increase gradually during pregnancy. One consequence of
in the blood is decreasing, its effects on the follicle increase. As high estradiol and progesterone levels is fluctuating activity
a result, the follicle produces increasing amounts of estradiol. at the serotonin 3 (5HT3) receptor, which is responsible for
The increased release of estradiol causes an increased release nausea (Rupprecht et al., 2001). Pregnant women often ex-
of FSH as well as a sudden surge in the release of luteinizing perience nausea because of the heightened activity of that re-
hormone (LH) from the anterior pituitary (see the top graph ceptor. Figure 11.7 summarizes the interactions between the
in Figure 11.6). FSH and LH combine to cause the follicle to pituitary and the ovary. Increased sensitivity to nausea may
release an ovum. be an evolved adaptation to minimize the risk of eating some-
The remnant of the follicle (now called the corpus luteum) thing harmful to the fetus.
releases the hormone progesterone, which prepares the uterus Birth-control pills prevent pregnancy by interfering with
for the implantation of a fertilized ovum. Progesterone also the usual feedback cycle between the ovaries and the pituitary.
pituitary hormones
(arbitrary units)
Blood levels of

LH

FSH
(arbitrary units)
Blood levels
of steroids

Estradiol

Progesterone

Thickness
of uterine
lining
1 5 10 15 20 25 28
Days

Menstruation Follicular Luteal


phase phase
Periovulatory Midluteal
period phase

Figure 11.6  ​Blood levels of four hormones during the human menstrual cycle
Note that estrogen and progesterone are both at high levels during the midluteal phase but drop sharply at menstruation. (© Cengage
Learning 2013)
336 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Ovum

FSH and LH
becomes
FSH

fertilized

Progesterone
or is
Estradiol
FSH

discarded

Estradiol
iol
Estrad Ovum
Follicle Follicle
Corpus
luteum
Ovary Ovary Ovary Ovary

End of menstruation Follicular phase Ovulation

Figure 11.7  ​Interactions between the pituitary and the ovary


FSH from the pituitary stimulates a follicle of the ovary to develop and produce estradiol, releasing a burst of FSH and LH from the
pituitary. Those hormones cause the follicle to release its ovum and become a corpus luteum. The corpus luteum releases progesterone
while the ovary releases estradiol. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

The most widely used birth-control pill, the combination pill, periovulatory period than if they watch it at other times (Slob,
containing estrogen and progesterone, prevents the surge of Bax, Hop, Rowland, & van der Werff ten Bosch, 1996).
FSH and LH that would otherwise release an ovum. The Another study used a method that is, shall we say, not com-
estrogen–progesterone combination also thickens the mucus mon among laboratory researchers. The researchers studied
of the cervix, making it harder for a sperm to reach the egg, erotic lap dancers, who earn tips by dancing between a man’s legs,
and prevents an ovum, if released, from implanting in the rubbing up against his groin, while wearing, in most cases, just
uterus. Thus, the pill prevents pregnancy in many ways. Note, a bikini bottom. Lap dancers recorded the times of their men-
however, that it does not protect against sexually transmitted strual periods and the amount of tip income they received each
diseases such as AIDS or syphilis. “Safe sex” must go beyond night. Lap dancers who were taking contraceptive pills (which
the prevention of pregnancy. keep hormone levels about constant through the month) earned
Changes in hormones over the menstrual cycle also alter about the same amount from one day to another. Those not
women’s sexual interest. The periovulatory period, consisting taking contraceptive pills received the largest tips 9 to 15 days
of the days around the middle of the menstrual cycle, is the time after menstruation, which is a time of increasing estrogen levels
of maximum fertility and high estrogen levels. According to (G. Miller, Tybur, & Jordan, 2007). A likely hypothesis is that
two studies, women not taking birth-control pills initiate more the women felt and acted sexier at this time.
sexual activity (either with a partner or by masturbation) dur- Sex hormones also influence women’s attention to sex-
ing the periovulatory period than at other times of the month related stimuli. Women in one study were asked to look at
(D. B. Adams, Gold, & Burt, 1978; Udry & Morris, 1968) (Fig- facial photos on a screen and classify each as male or female
ure 11.8). According to another study, women rate an erotic as quickly as possible. They made the classifications more
video as more pleasant and arousing if they watch it during the quickly when they were in their periovulatory period than at
11.1 Sex and Hormones 337

Women taking birth-control pills with fertility move women’s mate preferences toward men
Women using intrusive methods who look and act more masculine.
Women using nonintrusive methods
Estimated time of ovulation

0.3 Stop & Check


9. At what time in a woman’s menstrual cycle do her estra-
(mean number per day)

diol levels increase? When are they lowest?


Autosexual activities

0.2
during and just after menstruation.
ANSWER
to the middle of the menstrual cycle. They are lowest
9. Estrogen levels increase during the days leading up

0.1

Oxytocin
0 In addition to the sex hormones, the pituitary hormone oxy-
tocin is also important for reproductive behavior. Oxytocin
Female-initiated heterosexual activities

stimulates contractions of the uterus during delivery of a baby,


and it stimulates the mammary gland to release milk.
0.3
(mean number per day)

Sexual pleasure also releases oxytocin, especially at orgasm


(M. R. Murphy, Checkley, Seckl, & Lightman, 1990). People
typically experience a state of relaxation shortly after orgasm as a
0.2 result of oxytocin release. In animal studies, rats show increased
exploration of potentially dangerous places—that is, decreased
anxiety—after orgasm. Blocking the release of oxytocin pre-
vents that effect, so oxytocin is apparently responsible for the
0.1 calmness and lack of anxiety after orgasm (Waldherr & Neu-
mann, 2007). Strong release of oxytocin facilitates formation of
–15 –10 –5 0 pair bonds between mating partners (Kosfeld, Heinrichs, Zak,
Days before menstruation Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2005). It is also apparently related to the
formation of a pair bond between mother and infant. A study
Figure 11.8  ​Female-initiated sexual activities
found that women who had the highest oxytocin levels during
The top graph shows autosexual activities (masturbation and
pregnancy spent the most time gazing at, vocalizing to, touch-
sexual fantasies); the bottom graph shows female-initiated activi-
ties with a male partner. “Intrusive” birth-control methods are dia-
ing, and pleasurably interacting with their infants after delivery
phragm, foam, and condom; “nonintrusive” methods are IUD and (Feldman, Welle, Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, 2007).
vasectomy. Women other than pill users initiate sex more often Oxytocin also facilitates other social behaviors. When
when their estrogen levels peak. (Adams, Gold, & Burt, 1978) people inhale a nasal spray containing oxytocin, as compared
to a placebo, they become more accurate at recognizing fa-
miliar faces (Rimmele, Hediger, Heinrichs, & Klaver, 2009).
They are also quicker to recognize blurry words on a screen, if
those words refer to pleasant social relationship words, such
other times of the cycle (Macrae, Alnwick, Milne, & Schlo- as love or kissing (Unkelbach, Guastella, & Forgas, 2008).
erscheidt, 2002). In another study, women were presented Also consider effects on the “Trust Game.” If you are play-
with a computer that enabled them to modify pictures of ing this game, someone hands you some money. You can give
men’s faces to make each one look more feminine or more some or all of it to another person, and if so the amount you
masculine. When they were asked specifically to show the give triples in value. That person can then return to you what-
face of the man they would prefer for a “short-term sexual ever amount he or she chooses. So you have to decide how
relationship,” women preferred more masculine-looking faces much you trust that person. What effect would you expect
around the time of ovulation than they did at other times from oxytocin? It depends. People who are given oxytocin give
(Penton-Voak et al., 1999). When women were asked to view more money to people who seem trustworthy, but not to peo-
videotapes of two men and choose one for a short-term re- ple who seem competitive and aggressive (Mikolajczak et al.,
lationship, women around the time of ovulation were more 2010). People sometimes call oxytocin a “love hormone,” but
likely to choose a man who seemed athletic, competitive, and as you can see, that term is misleading. It might enhance your
assertive and who did not describe himself as having a “nice attraction toward someone you already love, but it doesn’t
personality” (Gangestad, Simpson, Cousins, Garver-Apgar, make you love or trust everyone. Testosterone, incidentally,
& Christensen, 2004). In short, the hormones associated decreases trust (Bos, Terburg, & van Honk, 2010).
338 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Stop & Check pressin is important for social behavior in many species, partly
by facilitating olfactory recognition of other individuals (Tobin
10. What behavioral change occurs after orgasm, and which
et al., 2010). Male prairie voles, which secrete much vasopres-
hormone is responsible?
sin, establish long-term pair bonds with females and help rear
lease of the pituitary hormone oxytocin. their young. A male meadow vole, with much lower vasopressin
ANSWER
10. Anxiety decreases after orgasm because of re- levels, mates with a female and then virtually ignores her (Figure
11.10). Imagine a male meadow vole in a long, narrow cage. At
one end, he can sit next to a female with which he has just mated.
(She is confined there.) At the other end, he can sit next to a dif-
ferent female. Will he choose his recent mate (showing loyalty)
Parental Behavior or the new female (seeking variety)? The answer: neither. He sits
A female mammal’s behavior changes in many ways when she right in the middle, by himself, as far away as he can get from
becomes a mother. In addition to nursing and caring for the both females. However, these little social isolates changed their
young, she eats and drinks more than usual, and becomes less behavior after researchers found a way to increase activity of the
fearful and more aggressive, especially in defense of her young. genes responsible for vasopressin in the voles’ hypothalamus.
Although the role of hormones is less central for humans, it is Suddenly, they showed a strong preference for a recent mate
critical for maternal behavior in other species. After a mother rat and, if placed into the same cage, they even helped her take care
delivers her babies, she increases her secretion of estradiol and of her babies (M. M. Lim et al., 2004). Whether the female was
prolactin, while decreasing production of progesterone (Numan surprised, we don’t know. This result is a strong example of alter-
& Woodside, 2010). Prolactin is necessary for milk production ing social behavior by manipulating the activity of a single gene.
and certain aspects of maternal behavior, such as retrieving the Although rodent maternal behavior depends on hormones
young when they wander away from the nest (Lucas, Ormandy, for the first few days, it becomes less dependent later. If a fe-
Binart, Bridges, & Kelly, 1998). It also inhibits sensitivity to male that has never been pregnant is left with some baby rats,
leptin, enabling the mother to eat far more than usual. she ignores them at first but gradually becomes more attentive.
In addition to secreting hormones, the female changes her (Because the babies cannot survive without parental care, the
pattern of hormone receptors. Late in pregnancy, her brain in- experimenter must periodically replace them with new, healthy
creases its sensitivity to estradiol in the areas responsible for ma- babies.) After about 6 days, the adoptive mother builds a nest,
ternal behavior (Rosenblatt, Olufowobi, & Siegel, 1998). The assembles the babies in the nest, licks them, and does every-
hormonal changes increase the mothers’ attention to their young thing else that normal mothers do, except nurse them. This
after delivery. Hormones increase activity in the medial preoptic experience-dependent behavior does not require hormonal
area and anterior hypothalamus (Featherstone, Fleming, & Ivy, changes and occurs even in rats that had their ovaries removed
2000), areas that are necessary for rats’ maternal behavior ( J. R. (Mayer & Rosenblatt, 1979; Rosenblatt, 1967). That is, hu-
Brown, Ye, Bronson, Dikkes, & Greenberg, 1996) (Figure 11.9). mans are not the only species in which a mother can adopt
We have already encountered the preoptic area/anterior hypo- young without first going through pregnancy.
thalamus, or POA/AH, because of its importance for tempera- An important influence from being with babies is that the
ture regulation, thirst, and sexual behavior. It’s a busy little area. mother becomes accustomed to their odors. Infant rats release
Another key hormone is vasopressin, synthesized by the hy- chemicals that stimulate the mother’s vomeronasal organ, which
pothalamus and secreted by the posterior pituitary gland. Vaso- responds to pheromones (see Chapter 7). We might imagine that

Figure 11.9  ​Brain development and maternal behavior in mice


The mouse on the left shows normal maternal behavior. The one on the right has a genetic mutation that impairs the development of the
preoptic area and anterior hypothalamus. (Reprinted from Cell, 86/2, Brown, J. R., Ye, H., Bronson, R. T., Dikkes, P., & Greenberg, M. E., “A
defect in nurturing in mice lacking the immediate early gene fosB,” 297–309, 1996, with permission of Elsevier.)
11.1 Sex and Hormones 339

Figure 11.10  ​Effects of vasopressin on social and


Hypothalamus mating behaviors
(a) Prairie voles (top) form long-term pair bonds. Staining of
their brain shows much expression of the hormone vaso-
pressin in the hypothalamus. A closely related species,
meadow voles (bottom), show no social attachments.
Their brains have lower vasopressin levels, as indi-
cated by less staining in the hypothalamus. (Reprinted
by permission from “Enhanced partner preference in a
promiscuous species by manipulating the expression of a
single gene,” by Lim, M. M., Wang, Z., Olazabal, D. E., Ren,
X., Terwillinger, E. F., & Young, L. J., Nature, 429, 754–757.
Copyright 2004 Nature Publishing Group/Macmillan
Hypothalamus
Magazines Ltd.)
(b)

evolution would have equipped infants with pheromones that tention to an infant. However, hormonal changes are not neces-
elicit maternal behavior, but actually, their pheromones stimu- sary for human parental behavior. After all, many people adopt
late aggressive behaviors that interfere with maternal behavior children and become excellent parents.
(Sheehan, Cirrito, Numan, & Numan, 2000). For a mother that
has just gone through pregnancy, this interference does not mat-
ter because her hormones primed her medial preoptic area so Stop & Check
strongly that it overrides competing impulses. A female without 11. What factors are responsible for maternal behavior
hormonal priming, however, rejects the young until she has be- shortly after rats give birth? What factors become more
come familiar with their smell (Del Cerro et al., 1995). important in later days?
Why do mammals need two mechanisms for maternal
behavior—one hormone-dependent and one not? In the early ANSWER
hormone levels begin to drop.
phase, hormones compensate for the mother’s lack of familiar- with the young maintains maternal behavior after the
ity with the young. In the later phase, experience maintains the that would tend to make her reject them. Experience
maternal behavior even though the hormones start to decline with the young decreases the vomeronasal responses
(Rosenblatt, 1970).
lactin and estradiol. A few days later, her experience

Are hormones important for human parental behavior?


pends on a surge in the release of the hormones pro-
11. The early stage of rats’ maternal behavior de-
Hormonal changes are necessary for a woman to nurse a baby,
and oxytocin levels correlate with several aspects of motherly at-

Module 11.1  ■  In Closing

Reproductive Behaviors and Motivations

A mother rat licks her babies all over shortly after their birth, gously, sexual behavior in general serves the function of passing
and that stimulation is essential for their survival. Why does she on our genes, but we engage in sexual behavior just because it
do it? Presumably, she does not understand that licking will feels good. We evolved a tendency to enjoy the sex act. The same
help them. She licks because they are covered with a salty fluid principle holds for hunger, thirst, and other motivations: We
that tastes good to her. If she has access to other salty fluids, she evolved tendencies to enjoy acts that have, in general, increased
stops licking her young (Gubernick & Alberts, 1983). Analo- our ancestors’ probability of surviving and reproducing.
340 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Summary
1. Male and female behaviors differ because of sex hor- 5. In adulthood, sex hormones activate sex behaviors, partly
mones that activate particular genes. Also, certain genes by facilitating activity in the medial preoptic area and
on the X and Y chromosomes exert direct effects on anterior hypothalamus. The hormones prime cells to
brain development.  328 release dopamine in response to sexual arousal.  333
2. Organizing effects of a hormone, exerted during a 6. A woman’s menstrual cycle depends on a feedback cycle
sensitive period, produce relatively permanent alterations that increases and then decreases the release of several
in anatomy and physiology.  330 hormones. Although women can respond sexually at any
3. In the absence of sex hormones, an infant mammal time in their cycle, on average, they show increased sexual
develops female-looking external genitals. The addition interest when estrogen levels are increasing.  334
of testosterone shifts development toward the male 7. The pituitary hormone oxytocin is important for sexual
pattern. Extra estradiol, within normal limits, does not pleasure, delivery of a baby, and milk production. Its
determine whether the individual looks male or female. release after orgasm decreases anxiety. It also increases
However, estradiol and other estrogens modify develop- attention to anything associated with favorable social or
ment of the brain and internal sexual organs.  330 sexual relationships.  337
4. During early development in rodents, testosterone is 8. Hormones released around the time of giving birth
converted within certain brain cells to estradiol, which facilitate maternal behavior in females of many mamma-
actually masculinizes their development. Estradiol in the lian species. Prolonged exposure to young also induces
blood does not masculinize development because it is parental behavior. Hormonal facilitation is not necessary
bound to proteins in the blood.  331 for human parental behavior.  338

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
activating effects  330 luteinizing hormone (LH)  335 sensitive period  330
alpha-fetoprotein  332 menstrual cycle  334 sexually dimorphic
androgens  328 Müllerian ducts  328 nucleus  331
estradiol  329 organizing effects  330 SRY gene  328
estrogens  329 ovaries  328 steroid hormones  329
follicle-stimulating hormone oxytocin  337 testis  328
(FSH)  335 periovulatory period  336 testosterone  329
impotence  334 progesterone  329 Wolffian ducts  328

Thought Questions
1. The pill RU-486 produces abortions by blocking the tion by estrogen create if that were the mechanism for
effects of progesterone. Why would blocking proges- mammals? Why do those problems not arise in birds?
terone interfere with pregnancy? (Hint: Think about the difference between live birth
2. The presence or absence of testosterone determines and hatching from an egg.)
whether a mammal will differentiate as a male or a 3. Antipsychotic drugs, such as haloperidol and chlor-
female. In birds, the story is the opposite: The promazine, block activity at dopamine synapses. What
presence or absence of estrogen is critical (Adkins & side effects might they have on sexual behavior?
Adler, 1972). What problems would sex determina-
Module 11.2

Variations in Sexual Behavior

P
eople vary considerably in their frequency of sexual
activity, preferred types of sexual activity, and sexual
orientation. In this module, we explore some of that
diversity, but first we consider a few differences between men
and women in general. Do men’s and women’s mating

A. Morris/Vireo: The Academy of Natural Sciences


behaviors make biological sense? If so, should we interpret
these behaviors as products of evolution? These questions
have proved to be difficult and controversial.

Evolutionary Interpretations
of Mating Behavior
Part of Charles Darwin’s theory of evolution by natural selec-
tion was that individuals whose genes help them survive will
produce more offspring, and therefore the next generation will Phalaropes are shore birds, in which the female is brilliantly col-
resemble those with these favorable genes. A second part of ored and the male is drabber. The female lays eggs and deserts
his theory, not so widely accepted at first, was sexual selec- the nest, leaving the male to attend to it.
tion: Genes that make an individual more appealing to the
other sex will increase the probability of reproduction, and
therefore the next generation will resemble those who had
these favorable genes.
Sexual selection can go only so far, however, if it starts to sible examples, although each has been controversial (Buss,
interfere with survival. A male deer with large antlers attracts 2000). Let’s examine the evidence and reasoning.
females, but being impressive wouldn’t help if the weight be-
came so great that it interfered with the deer’s movement. A
bird’s bright colors attract potential mates, but they also run Interest in Multiple Mates
the risk of attracting a predator’s attention. In many bird spe- More men than women seek opportunities for casual sexual
cies, the male is brightly colored, but the female is not, pre- relationships with many partners. Why? From the evolution-
sumably because she sits on the nest and needs to be less con- ary standpoint of spreading one’s genes, men can succeed by
spicuous. In a few species, such as phalaropes, the female is either of two strategies (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000): Be
more brightly colored, but in those species, the female lays the loyal to one woman and devote your energies to helping her
egg and deserts it, leaving the dull-colored male to sit on the and her babies, or mate with many women and hope that
nest. In species where the male and female share the nesting some of them can raise your babies without your help. No
duties, such as pigeons and doves, the male and female look one needs to be conscious of these strategies, of course. The
alike, and neither is especially gaudy. idea is that men who acted these ways in the past propagated
In humans, too, some of the differences between men and their genes, and today’s men might have inherited genes that
women may be results of sexual selection. That is, to some ex- promote these behaviors. In contrast, a woman can have no
tent women evolved based on what appeals to men, and men more than one pregnancy per 9 months, regardless of her
evolved based on what appeals to women. Certain aspects of number of sex partners. So evolution may have predisposed
behavior may also reflect different evolutionary pressures for men, or at least some men, to be more interested in multiple
men and women. Evolutionary psychologists cite several pos- mates than women are.
341
342 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

One objection is that a woman does sometimes gain from Differences in Jealousy
having multiple sex partners (Hrdy, 2000). If her husband is Traditionally, in most cultures, men have been more jealous
infertile, mating with another man could be her only way of of women’s infidelities than women have been of men’s infi-
reproducing. Also, another sexual partner may provide aid of delities. From an evolutionary standpoint, why might men be
various sorts to her and her children. In addition, she has the more jealous than women? If a man is to pass on his genes—
possibility of “trading up,” abandoning her first mate for a bet- the key point in evolution—he needs to be sure that the chil-
ter one. So the prospect of multiple mates may be more ap- dren he supports are his own. An unfaithful wife threatens
pealing to men, but it has advantages for women, too. Human that certainty. A woman knows that any children she bears are
cultures vary substantially in how well they tolerate women her own, so she does not have the same worry.
having multiple sexual partners. One way to test this interpretation of jealousy is to com-
Another objection is that researchers have no direct evi- pare cultures. Some cultures consider sexual infidelity accept-
dence that genes influence people’s preferences for one mate able for both husband and wife; some prohibit it completely
or many. We shall return to this issue later. for both; and some consider it more acceptable for the hus-
band than for the wife. However, no known society considers
it more acceptable for the wife. Should we be more impressed
What Men and Women Seek in a Mate that jealousy is always at least as strong for men as for women,
Almost all people seeking a romantic partner prefer someone and usually more, or should we be more impressed that jeal-
who is healthy, intelligent, honest, and physically attractive. ousy varies among cultures? The answer is not obvious.
Typically, women have some additional interests that are less Which would upset you more: if your partner had a brief
common for men. In particular, women are more likely than sexual affair with someone else, or if he or she became emo-
men are to prefer a mate who is likely to be a good provider tionally close to someone else? According to several studies,
(Buss, 2000). As you might guess, that tendency is strongest men say they would be more upset by the sexual infidelity,
in societies where women have no income of their own. Ac- whereas women would be more upset by the emotional infidel-
cording to evolutionary theorists, the reason is this: While a ity (Shakelford, Buss, & Bennett, 2002). However, those stud-
woman is pregnant or taking care of a small child, she needs ies dealt with hypothetical situations. Most men and women
help getting food and other requirements. Evolution would who have actually dealt with an unfaithful partner say they
have favored any gene that caused women to seek good pro- were more upset by their partner’s becoming emotionally close
viders. Related to this tendency, most women tend to be cau- to someone else than by the sexual affair (C. H. Harris, 2002).
tious during courtship. Even if a man seems interested in her,
a woman waits before concluding that he has a strong com-
mitment to her (Buss, 2001). She would not want a man who Evolved or Learned?
acts interested briefly and then leaves when she needs him. If a behavior has clear advantages for survival or reproduction
A woman is also much more likely to reject a man because and is similar across most or all cultures, can we conclude that
of his smell than a man is to reject a woman because of her it developed by evolution? Not necessarily. Of course, the brain
smell (Herz & Inzlicht, 2002). One possible reason is that evolved, just like any other organ, and of course, our behavioral
body odor relates to some of the same genes that control the tendencies are a product of evolution. But the key question is
immune system, known as the major histocompatibility com- whether evolution has micromanaged our behavior down to
plex. Research has found that a woman tends to be less sexu- such details as whether to look for a mate with high earning
ally responsive to a man whose immune genes, and therefore potential or how jealous to be of an unfaithful mate.
body odor, are too similar to her own (Garver-Apgar, Gan- Cross-cultural similarity is not necessarily good evidence
gestad, Thornhill, Miller, & Olp, 2006). Avoiding a man of for an evolved tendency. For example, people throughout the
similar odor may be a way to avoid inbreeding. world agree that 2 1 2 5 4, but we don’t assume that they
Men tend to have a stronger preference for a young part- have a gene for that belief. To establish that we evolved a ten-
ner. An evolutionary explanation is that young women are dency to act in some way, the most decisive evidence would be
likely to remain fertile longer than older women are, so a to demonstrate genes that affect the relevant behaviors. For
man can have more children by pairing with a young woman. example, if most men have genes influencing them to prefer
Curiously, male chimpanzees show no preference for young young women, then presumably, we should be able to find
females, perhaps because chimpanzee mating does not entail some men with a mutation in that gene causing them to lose
a long-term commitment. In fact, male chimps usually pre- that preference. Although this example may not be the best,
fer older (but still fertile) females, who tend to have a higher the point is that we need to be cautious about inferring what
social rank than younger females do (Muller, Thompson, & is a product of our evolution and what is learned.
Wrangham, 2006).
Men remain fertile into old age, so a woman has less need
to insist on youth. Women do prefer young partners when Conclusions
possible, but in many societies, only older men have enough Discussing these issues is difficult. Ideally, we would like to
financial resources to get married. consider the evidence and logical arguments entirely on their
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 343

scientific merits. However, when someone describes how evo- themselves as being masculine in some ways and feminine in
lutionary selection may have led men to be interested in mul- others. Psychologists have long assumed that gender depends
tiple sex partners or to be more jealous than women are, it mainly or entirely on the way people rear their children. How-
sometimes sounds like a justification for men to act that way. ever, several kinds of evidence suggest that biological factors,
No gene forces men or women to behave in any particular way. especially prenatal hormones, are important also.
Even leaving aside the social implications as far as we can,
no firm scientific consensus emerges. We need more data, es-
pecially about the effects of particular genes, before we draw Intersexes
a firm conclusion. Some people have anatomies intermediate between male and
female (Haqq & Donahoe, 1998). Individuals who appear to
be a mixture of male and female are referred to as hermaph-
Stop & Check rodites (from Hermes and Aphrodite in Greek mythology).
12. What evolutionary advantage is suggested for why women For example, some people are born with an XX chromosome
are more interested in men’s wealth and success than pattern but an SRY gene that translocated from the father’s
men are interested in women’s wealth? Y chromosome onto another chromosome. Despite their XX
chromosomes, they have either an ovary and a testis, or two
ANSWER
is not similarly dependent on a female. testes, or a mixture of testis and ovary tissue on each side.
a male partner who can provide for her. A healthy male
Others develop an intermediate appearance because of
an atypical hormone pattern. Recall that testosterone mas-
limited in her ability to get food and therefore prefers

culinizes the genitals and the hypothalamus during early de-


12. During pregnancy and early child care, a female is

velopment. A genetic male with low levels of testosterone or


a deficiency of testosterone receptors may develop a female
or intermediate appearance (Misrahi et al., 1997). A genetic
Gender Identity and Gender- female who is exposed to more testosterone than the average
Differentiated Behaviors female can be partly masculinized.
The most common cause of this condition is congenital
The coral goby is a species of fish in which the male and female adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), meaning overdevelopment of the
tend their eggs and young together. If one of them dies, the sur- adrenal glands from birth. Ordinarily, the adrenal gland has a
vivor looks for a new partner. But it does not look far. This is a negative feedback relationship with the pituitary gland. The pi-
very stay-at-home kind of fish. If it cannot easily find a partner tuitary secretes adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), which
of the opposite sex but does find an unmated member of its stimulates the adrenal gland. Cortisol, one of the hormones from
own sex—oh, well—it simply changes sex and mates with the the adrenal gland, feeds back to decrease the release of ACTH.
neighbor. Male-to-female and female-to-male
switches are equally common (Nakashima, Ku-
wamura, & Yogo, 1995).
People do not have the same flexibility
as coral gobies, but we do have variations in
sexual development. Sexual development is a
sensitive issue, so let us specify from the start:
“Different” does not mean “wrong.” People dif-
fer naturally in their sexual development just
as they do in anything else.
Gender identity is how we identify sexually
and what we call ourselves. The biological dif-
ferences between males and females are sex dif-
ferences, whereas the differences that result from
people’s thinking about themselves as male or
female are gender differences. To maintain this
Accord Alliance

useful distinction, we should resist the trend to


speak of the “gender” of dogs, fruit flies, and so
forth. Gender identity is a human characteristic.
In most cases people accept the gender This group of intersexed people gathered to provide mutual support and to pro-
test against the early surgical treatments they received. They requested that their
identity that matches their external appear-
names be used to emphasize that their intersexuality should not be considered
ance, which matches the way they were reared.
shameful. From left to right: Martha Coventry, Max Beck, David Vandertie, Kristi
However, some are dissatisfied with their Bruce, and Angela Moreno.
assigned gender, and many would describe
344 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Some people have a genetic limitation in their ability to produce Stop & Check
cortisol. Because the pituitary fails to receive much cortisol as a
feedback signal, it continues secreting more ACTH, causing the 13. What is a common cause for a genetic female (XX) to de-
adrenal gland to secrete larger amounts of its other hormones, velop a partly masculinized anatomy?
including testosterone. In a genetic male, the extra testosterone
ANSWER
ing testosterone, which masculinizes development.
causes no apparent difficulty. However, genetic females with this then produces large amounts of other hormones, includ-
condition develop various degrees of masculinization of their ues stimulating the adrenal gland. The adrenal gland
external genitals. (The ovaries and other internal organs are less receive negative feedback signals and therefore contin-
affected.) Figure 11.11 shows a structure that appears interme- ability to produce cortisol, the pituitary gland does not
diate between clitoris and penis and swellings that appear inter- 13. If a genetic female is genetically deficient in her
mediate between labia and scrotum. After birth, these children
are given medical treatments to bring their adrenal hormones
within normal levels. Some are also given surgery to alter their
external genital appearance, as we shall discuss later. Interests and Preferences of CAH Girls
People whose sexual development is intermediate, as in
Figure 11.11, are called intersexes. How common are inter- For many years, the policy was to raise most intersexed peo-
sexes? An estimated 1 child in 100 in the United States is ple as girls, on the assumption that surgery could make them
born with some degree of genital ambiguity, and 1 in 2,000 look like normal girls, and they would develop behaviors
has enough ambiguity to make its male or female status un- corresponding to the way they were reared. However, their
certain (Blackless et al., 2000). However, the accuracy of these brains were exposed to higher than normal testosterone lev-
estimates is doubtful, as hospitals and families keep the in- els during prenatal and early postnatal life compared to other
formation private. Maintaining confidentiality is of course girls. Was their behavior masculinized? As discussed in the
important, but an unfortunate consequence is that intersexed first module of this chapter, prenatal levels of testosterone
people have trouble finding others like themselves. For more correlate with girls’ toy choices. The same idea applies here. In
information, consult the website of the Intersex Society of several studies, girls with CAH were observed in a room full
North America (ISNA): http://www.isna.org/. of toys—including some that were girl typical (dolls, plates
and dishes, cosmetics kits), some that were boy typical (toy
car, tool set, gun), and some that were neutral (puzzles, cray-
ons, board games). Figure 11.12 shows the results from one
such study (Pasterski et al., 2005). Note how girls with CAH
were intermediate between the preferences of boys and girls
without CAH. When the children tested with a parent pres-
ent, again the girls with CAH were intermediate between the
other two groups.
Other studies have reported similar results and have
found that the girls exposed to the largest amount of tes-
tosterone in early development showed the largest prefer-
Image not available due to copyright restrictions ence for boys’ toys (Berenbaum, Duck, & Bryk, 2000; Nor-
denström, Servin, Bohlin, Larsson, & Wedell, 2002). You
might wonder whether the parents, knowing that these
girls had been partly masculinized in appearance, might
have encouraged “tomboyish” activities. Psychologists’ ob-
servations suggest the opposite: The parents encouraged
the girls with CAH any time they played with girl-typical
toys (Pasterski et al., 2005). A study of CAH girls in ado-
lescence found that, on average, their interests are interme-
diate between those of typical male and female adolescents.
For example, they read more sports magazines and fewer
style and glamour magazines than the average for other
Text not available due to copyright restrictions
teenage girls (Berenbaum, 1999). In adulthood, they
The genitals were masculinized by excess androgens from the show more physical aggression than most other women
adrenal gland before birth. (From Money, John and Ehrhardt, Anke do, and less interest in infants (Mathews, Fane, Con-
A., Man and Woman, Boy and Girl: Differentiation and Dimorphism way, Brook, & Hines, 2009). They are more interested in
of Gender Identity from Conception to Maturity, p. 115, Figure 6.2.
rough sports and more likely than average to be in heavily
© 1972 The Johns Hopkins University Press. Reprinted by permis-
sion of The Johns Hopkins University Press.)
male-dominated occupations such as auto mechanic and
truck driver (Frisén et al., 2009).
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 345

Testicular Feminization
Certain individuals with an XY chromosome pattern have the
genital appearance of a female. This condition is known as an-
drogen insensitivity, or testicular feminization. Although such
individuals produce normal amounts of androgens (including
testosterone), they lack the receptor that enables androgen to
activate genes in a cell’s nucleus. Consequently, the cells are in-
sensitive to androgens, and development proceeds as if the level
of testosterone and similar hormones was low. This condition
occurs in various degrees, resulting in anatomy that ranges from
Unaffected Girls a smaller than average penis to genitals like those of a normal fe-
male. In some cases, no one has any reason to suspect the person
is anything other than a normal female, until puberty. Then, in
spite of breast development and broadening of the hips, men-
struation does not begin because the body has internal testes
Girls' toys
instead of ovaries and a uterus. (The vagina is short and leads to
Boys' toys nothing but skin.) Also, pubic hair is sparse or absent, because it
Neutral toys depends on androgens in females as well as males.

Stop & Check


15. What would cause a genetic male (XY) to develop a partly
Girls with CAH
feminized external anatomy?

ance that partly or completely resembles a female.


ANSWER
one from binding to its receptors will develop an appear-
15. A genetic male with a gene that prevents testoster-

Issues of Gender Assignment and Rearing


Many girls with CAH and related conditions are born with
a nearly normal appearance, but some look as much male as
Boys female and presumably were exposed to high levels of prenatal
testosterone. Some genetic males are born with a very small
penis because of a condition called cloacal exstrophy, a defect of
Figure 11.12  ​Toy preferences by CAH girls, unaffected pelvis development (Reiner & Gearhart, 2004). Despite their
girls, and unaffected boys genital anatomy, they had typical male levels of testosterone in
CAH girls were intermediate between unaffected girls and boys. prenatal development.
These data show results when the children played alone. Results How should children with either of these conditions be
changed slightly when the mother or father was present, but in reared? Beginning in the 1950s, medical doctors began rec-
each case, the CAH girls were intermediate between the other
ommending that all intersexed people be reared as girls, using
groups. (Based on data of Byne et al., 2001)
surgery if necessary to make their genitals look more feminine
(Dreger, 1998). The reason was that it is easier to reduce an
enlarged clitoris to normal size than expand it to penis size. If
necessary, surgeons can build an artificial vagina or lengthen a
short one. After the surgery, the child looks female. Physicians
and psychologists assumed that any child who was consis-
Stop & Check
tently reared as a girl would fully accept that identity.
14. If a genetic female is exposed to extra testosterone dur- And she lives happily ever after, right? Not necessarily. Of
ing prenatal development, what behavioral effect is likely? those with cloacal exstrophy who are reared as girls, all de-
velop typical male interests, many or most eventually demand
reassignment as males, and nearly all develop sexual attraction
girls to prefer boy-typical toys.
ANSWER
toward women, not men (Reiner & Gearhart, 2004).
prenatal development is more likely than most other
14. A girl who is exposed to extra testosterone during
Girls with the CAH history also have a difficult adjustment,
especially if they were subjected to clitoris-reduction surgery. A
346 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

surgically created or lengthened vagina may be satisfactory to raise a normal female baby as a male. If the process succeeded in
a male partner, but it provides no sensation to the woman and producing an adult who was fully satisfied in the assigned role,
requires almost daily attention to prevent it from scarring over. we would know that upbringing determines gender identity. Al-
Nearly all women with a history of CAH have significant sexual though no one would perform such an experiment intentionally,
difficulties, including lack of orgasm. Many report no sexual we can learn from accidental events. In some cases, someone was
partner ever, little or no interest in sex, and little or no romantic exposed to a more-or-less normal pattern of male hormones be-
attraction to men (Frisén et al., 2009; Meyer-Bahlburg, Dolezal, fore and shortly after birth but then reared as a girl.
Baker, & New, 2008; Minto, Liao, Woodhouse, Ransley, & One kind of case was reported first in the Dominican Re-
Creighton, 2003; Nordenström et al., 2010; Zucker et al., 1996). public and then in other places, usually in communities with
Many intersexes wish they had their original “abnormal” much inbreeding. In each case, certain genetic males fail to pro-
enlarged clitoris instead of the mutilated, insensitive structure duce 5a-reductase 2, an enzyme that converts testosterone to
left to them by a surgeon. Moreover, intersexes resent being dihydrotestosterone. Dihydrotestosterone is an androgen that is
deceived. Historian Alice Dreger (1998) describes the case of more effective than testosterone for masculinizing the external
one intersex: genitals. At birth, some of these individuals look almost like a
typical female, while others have a swollen clitoris and somewhat
As a young person, [she] was told she had “twisted
“lumpy” labia. Nearly all are considered girls and reared as such.
ovaries” that had to be removed; in fact, her testes were
However, their brains had been exposed to male levels of tes-
removed. At the age of twenty, “alone and scared in the
tosterone during early development. At puberty, the testosterone
stacks of a [medical] library,” she discovered the truth of
levels increase sharply, the body makes increased amounts of a
her condition. Then “the pieces finally fit together. But
different enzyme that converts testosterone to dihydrotestoster-
what fell apart was my relationship with both my family
one, and the result is the growth of a penis and scrotum.
and physicians. It was not learning about chromosomes
Women: Imagine that at about age 12 years, your external
or testes that caused enduring trauma, it was discovering
genitals suddenly changed from female to male. Would you
that I had been told lies. I avoided all medical care for the
say, “Yep, okay, I guess I’m a boy now”? Most (but not all) of
next 18 years. . . . [The] greatest source of anxiety is not
these people reacted exactly that way. The girl-turned-boy de-
our gonads or karyotype. It is shame and fear resulting
veloped a male gender identity and directed his sexual interest
from an environment in which our condition is so
toward females (Cohen-Kettenis, 2005; Imperato-McGinley,
unacceptable that caretakers lie.” (p. 192)
Guerrero, Gautier, & Peterson, 1974). Remember, these were
How should such a child be reared? A growing number of not typical girls. Their brains had been exposed to male levels
specialists follow these recommendations (Diamond & Sig- of testosterone from prenatal life onward.
mundson, 1997): A particularly upsetting case is that of one infant boy whose
penis foreskin would not retract enough for easy urination. His
■ Be completely honest with the intersexed person and the
parents took him to a physician to circumcise the foreskin, but
family, and do nothing without their informed consent.
the physician, using an electrical procedure, set the current too
■ Identify the child as male or female based mainly on
high and accidentally burned off the entire penis. On the advice
the predominant external appearance. That is, there of respected and well-meaning authorities, the parents elected
should be no bias toward calling every intersex a to rear the child as a female, with the appropriate surgery. What
female. Those born with masculinized external geni- makes this case especially interesting is that the child had a twin
tals seldom make a successful adaptation to a female brother (whom the parents did not let the physician try to cir-
gender assignment (Houk & Lee, 2010). cumcise). If both twins developed satisfactory gender identities,
■ Rear the child as consistently as possible, but be pre- one as a girl and the other as a boy, the results would say that
pared that the person might later be sexually oriented rearing was decisive in gender identity.
toward males, females, both, or neither. Initial reports claimed that the child reared as a girl had a
■ Do not perform surgery to reduce the ambiguous female gender identity, though she also had strong tomboyish
penis/clitoris to the size of a normal clitoris. Such tendencies (Money & Schwartz, 1978). However, by about
surgery impairs the person’s erotic sensation and is at age 10, she had figured out that something was wrong and
best premature, as no one knows how the child’s sexual that “she” was really a boy. She had preferred boys’ activities
orientation will develop. If the intersexed person makes and played only with boys’ toys. She even tried urinating in a
an informed request for such surgery in adulthood, then standing position, despite always making a mess. By age 14,
it is appropriate, but otherwise it should be avoided. she insisted that she wanted to live as a boy. At that time, her
(now his) father tearfully explained the earlier events. The
child changed names and became known as a boy. At age 25,
Discrepancies of Sexual Appearance he married a somewhat older woman and adopted her chil-
The evidence from intersexes does not indisputably resolve the dren. Clearly, a biological predisposition had won out over the
roles of rearing and hormones in determining gender identity. family’s attempts to rear the child as a girl (Colapinto, 1997;
From a scientific viewpoint, the most decisive way to settle the Diamond & Sigmundson, 1997). Some years later, the story
issue would be to raise a normal male baby as a female or to ended tragically with this man’s suicide.
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 347

We should not draw universal conclusions from a single landmarks. Gay men also tend to use landmarks and are better
case. However, the point is that it was a mistake to impose than heterosexual men at remembering landmarks (Hassan &
surgery and hormonal treatments to try to force this child to Rahman, 2007). Consider this task: Experimenters repeatedly
become female. When the prenatal hormone pattern of the present a loud noise and measure the startle response. On some
brain is in conflict with a child’s appearance, no one can be trials, they present a weaker noise just before the loud noise; the
sure how that child will develop psychologically. Hormones first noise decreases the startle response to the louder one. The
don’t have complete control, but rearing patterns don’t, either. decrease is called “prepulse inhibition.” Prepulse inhibition is or-
dinarily stronger in men than in women. In this regard, homo-
sexual women are slightly shifted in the male direction compared
Stop & Check to heterosexual women (Rahman, Kumari, & Wilson, 2003).
16. When “girls” reached puberty and grew a penis and scro-
tum, what happened to their gender identity? Genetics
male. Studies of the genetics of sexual orientation have focused
ANSWER
16. Most changed their gender identity from female to mainly on twins. Early studies of the genetics of human sexual
orientation began by advertising in gay or lesbian publications
for homosexual people with twins. Then they contacted the
other twin to fill out a questionnaire. The questionnaire in-
cluded diverse items to conceal the fact that the real interest
Sexual Orientation was sexual orientation. The results showed a stronger concor-
Contrary to what biologists once assumed, homosexual behav- dance for monozygotic than dizygotic twins (Bailey & Pillard,
ior occurs in many animal species, and not just in captive ani- 1991; Bailey, Pillard, Neale, & Agyei, 1993). That is, if one
mals, those that cannot find a member of the opposite sex, or twin is homosexual, the probability for the other to be homo-
those with hormonal abnormalities (Bagemihl, 1999). If “nat- sexual is fairly high for a monozygotic twin, and less high for
ural” means “occurs in nature,” then homosexuality is natural. a dizygotic twin. However, the kind of person who answers
What accounts for differences in sexual orientation? Many an ad in a gay or lesbian magazine may not be representa-
influences appear to be important, including genetics, prenatal tive of others. A later study examined the data from all the
environment, and as-yet unidentified aspects of experience. The twins in Sweden between ages 20 and 47 (Långström, Rah-
explanations will probably be different for different people, and man, Carlström, & Lichtenstein, 2010). The Swedish study
in general different for men than for women. Whereas most men differed not only in the breadth of the sample but also in the
discover their sexual orientation early, many women are slower. behavioral criterion. Instead of asking about sexual orienta-
Feminine-type behaviors in childhood and adolescence corre- tion, the researchers asked whether someone had ever had a
late strongly with homosexual orientation in adulthood for men same-sex partner. Figure 11.13 compares the data from the
(Cardoso, 2009; Alanko et al., 2010), but early masculine-type two studies. The results do not indicate total number of people
behaviors are poor predictors of sexual orientation in women with homosexual activity or orientation. Rather, they indicate
(Udry & Chantala, 2006). A higher percentage of women than concordance—the probability of homosexual activity or orien-
men experience at least some physical attraction to both males tation in one twin, given that the other twin had already indi-
and females (Chivers, Rieger, Latty, & Bailey, 2004; Lippa, cated such activity. Although both sets of results show a higher
2006), and some women switch—once or more—between concordance for monozygotic than dizygotic twins, note the
homosexual and heterosexual orientations (Diamond, 2007). huge difference between the studies. From these data it is rea-
Switches in sexual orientation are rare in men. Although we sonable to infer a genetic contribution to sexual orientation, but
shall note certain biological correlates of female homosexuality, we can’t be sure about the size of that contribution.
the case for a biological predisposition seems stronger for men. Several studies reported a higher incidence of homosexual-
ity among the maternal than paternal relatives of homosexual
men (Camperio-Ciani, Corna, & Capiluppi, 2004; Hamer, Hu,
Behavioral and Anatomical Differences Magnuson, Hu, & Pattatucci, 1993). For example, uncles and
On average, homosexual and heterosexual people differ ana- cousins on the mother’s side were more likely to be homosexual
tomically in several ways. On average, heterosexual men are than uncles and cousins on the father’s side. These results sug-
slightly taller and heavier than homosexual men (Bogaert, gest a gene on the X chromosome, which a man necessarily re-
2010). However, let’s emphasize that term “slightly”: The dif- ceives from his mother. However, other studies have not repli-
ference on average is only 1.5 cm (about half an inch). Despite cated these results, and the current status is inconclusive (Bailey
the stereotype, a fair number of homosexual men are tall, ath- et al., 1999; Rice, Anderson, Risch, & Ebers, 1999).
letic, and masculine in appearance.
On average, people who differ in sexual orientation also
differ in several behaviors that are not directly related to sex. An Evolutionary Question
More men than women give directions in terms of distances and If certain genes promote a homosexual orientation, why hasn’t
north, south, east, or west. Women are more likely to describe evolution selected strongly against those genes, which de-
348 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

60 Stop & Check


Monozygotic
Percent concordance

50 17. For which kind of twin pair is the concordance for sexual
Dizygotic
40 orientation greatest?
30 18. It seems difficult to explain how a gene could remain at a
20 moderately high frequency in the population if most men
10 with the gene do not reproduce. How would the hypoth-
esis about inactivation by a methyl group help with the
0
US male Swedish US female Swedish explanation?
sample male sample female
sample sample men with the inactivated gene seldom reproduce.
ANSWERS
a moderately high prevalence of homosexuality, even if
Figure 11.13  ​Twin concordance for homosexuality  to that gene happens often enough, the result could be
The concordance for homosexual orientation (U.S. study) or ho- homosexuality.” If this event of attaching a methyl group
mosexual activity (Swedish study) was higher for monzygotic twins a hereditary effect, even though there is no “gene for
than for dizygotic twins. (Based on the data of Bailey & Pillard, 1991; passed to the next generation, producing evidence for
Bailey, Pillard, Neale, & Agyei, 1993; Långström, Rahman, Carlström, & tified gene, inactivating the gene. That gene could be
Lichtenstein, 2010) environment can attach a methyl group to some uniden-
cording to this hypothesis, some unknown event in the
both twins have the same sexual orientation. 18. Ac-
concordance that is greater—that is, the probability that
crease the probability of reproduction? Several possibilities common in monozygotic than dizygotic twins. It is the
are worth considering (Gavrilets & Rice, 2006). One is that
point correctly: Do not say that homosexuality is more

genes for homosexuality are maintained by kin selection, as


dizygotic twins. Note the importance of stating this
17. Monozygotic twins have higher concordance than
discussed in Chapter 1. That is, even if homosexual people do
not have children themselves, they might do a wonderful job
of helping their brothers and sisters rear children. Survey data
in the United States indicate that homosexual men are no
more likely than heterosexuals, and perhaps less likely, to help Prenatal Influences
support their relatives (Bobrow & Bailey, 2001). However, Adult hormone levels do not explain sexual orientation. On
observations in Samoa found that homosexual men are more average, homosexual and heterosexual men have nearly the
helpful than average toward their nephews and nieces (Vasey same hormone levels, and most lesbian women have about the
& VanderLaan, 2010). It is difficult to know what might have same hormone levels as heterosexual women. However, it is
been the usual pattern through human existence. possible that sexual orientation depends on testosterone levels
According to a second hypothesis, genes that produce ho- during a sensitive period of brain development (Ellis & Ames,
mosexuality in males produce advantageous effects in their sis- 1987). Animal studies have shown that prenatal or early
ters and other female relatives, increasing their probability of postnatal hormones can produce organizing effects on both
reproducing and spreading the genes. The results of one study anatomy and sexual behavior. External anatomy develops at
support this hypothesis. Homosexual men’s mothers and aunts a different time from brain anatomy, and so it is possible for
had a greater than average number of children (Camperio-Ciani hormones to alter one differently from the other.
et al., 2004). However, a common estimate is that the average The mother’s immune system may exert prenatal effects.
homosexual man has one fifth as many children as the average Several studies (though not all) report that the probability
heterosexual man. Could his female relatives have enough chil- of a homosexual orientation is slightly higher among men
dren to compensate for this decrease? It seems unlikely. who have older brothers. Younger brothers make no dif-
A third hypothesis is that certain genes lead to homosexu- ference, nor do younger or older sisters (Bogaert, 2003b;
ality in men homozygous for the gene but produce reproduc- Purcell, Blanchard, & Zucker, 2000). Furthermore, what
tive advantages in men heterozygous for the gene (Rahman matters is the number of biological older brothers. Grow-
& Wilson, 2003). A closely related idea is that several genes ing up with older stepbrothers or adopted brothers has no
produce advantages for survival or reproduction, but a combi- apparent influence. Having a biological older brother has
nation of them leads to homosexuality. an influence, even if the brothers were reared separately
A fourth idea is that homosexuality relates to the activa- (Bogaert, 2006). In short, the influence does not stem from
tion or inactivation of genes (Bocklandt, Horvath, Vilain, & social experiences. The key is how many previous times the
Hamer, 2006). As mentioned in Chapter 1, it is possible for mother gave birth to a son. The most prominent hypothesis
environmental events to attach a methyl group (CH3) to a is that a mother’s immune system sometimes reacts against
gene and inactivate it. A parent can pass the inactivation of a protein in a son and then attacks subsequent sons enough
a gene to the next generation. Conceivably, this mechanism to alter their development. That hypothesis fits with the
might produce a significant amount of homosexuality without observation that later-born homosexual men tend to be
relying on the spread of a gene that promotes homosexuality. shorter than average (Bogaert, 2003a).
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 349

Another aspect of prenatal environment relates to stress on Brain Anatomy


the mother during pregnancy. Research has shown that prena- Do brains also differ as a function of sexual orientation? The
tal stress alters sexual development in laboratory animals. In results are complex. On average, homosexual men are shifted
several experiments, rats in the final week of pregnancy had the partly in the female-typical direction for some brain struc-
stressful experience of confinement in tight Plexiglas tubes for tures but not others. Similarly, on average, homosexual wom-
more than 2 hours each day under bright lights. In some cases, en’s brains are slightly shifted in the male direction in some
they were given alcohol as well. These rats’ daughters looked and ways but not others (Rahman & Wilson, 2003). Several of
acted approximately normal. The sons, however, had normal the reported differences have no clear relationship to sexuality
male anatomy but in adulthood often responded to the pres- itself, although they may relate to other behavioral differences
ence of another male by arching their backs in the typical rat between heterosexual and homosexual people.
female posture for sex (I. L. Ward, Ward, Winn, & Bielawski, On average, the left and right hemispheres of the cere-
1994). Most males that were subjected to either prenatal stress bral cortex are of nearly equal size in heterosexual females,
or alcohol developed male sexual behavior in addition to these whereas the right hemisphere is a few percent larger in het-
female sexual behaviors, but those that were subjected to both erosexual males. Homosexual males resemble heterosexual
stress and alcohol had decreased male sexual behaviors (I. L. females in this regard, and homosexual females are inter-
Ward, Bennett, Ward, Hendricks, & French, 1999). mediate between heterosexual females and males. Also, in
Prenatal stress and alcohol may alter brain development heterosexual females the left amygdala has more widespread
through several routes. Stress releases endorphins, which can an- connections than the right amygdala, whereas in heterosexual
tagonize the effects of testosterone on the hypothalamus (O. B. males the right amygdala has more widespread connections.
Ward, Monaghan, & Ward, 1986). Stress also elevates levels of Again, homosexual males resemble heterosexual females in
the adrenal hormone corticosterone, which decreases testoster- this regard, and homosexual females are intermediate (Savic
one release (O. B. Ward, Ward, Denning, French, & Hendricks, & Lindström, 2008). The anterior commissure (Figures 4.13
2002; M. T. Williams, Davis, McCrea, Long, & Hennessy, 1999). and 14.5) is, on average, larger in heterosexual women than
The long-term effects of either prenatal stress or alcohol include in heterosexual men. In homosexual men, it is at least as large
several changes in the structure of the nervous system, making as in women, perhaps even slightly larger (Gorski & Allen,
the affected males’ anatomy closer to that of females (Nosenko & 1992). The suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) is also larger in
Reznikov, 2001; I. L. Ward, Romeo, Denning, & Ward, 1999). homosexual men than in heterosexual men (Swaab & Hof-
Although the relevance of these results to humans is un- man, 1990). However, when interpreting these and other re-
certain, they prompted investigators to examine possible ef- ported differences, we should remember two cautions (Kai-
fects of prenatal stress on humans. One approach is to ask ser, Haller, Schmitz, & Nitsch, 2009): First, we don’t know
the mothers of homosexual men whether they experienced whether these brain differences are causes or effects of sexual
any unusual stress during pregnancy. Three surveys compared orientation. Brain differences can predispose to different be-
mothers of homosexual sons to mothers of heterosexual sons. haviors, but it is also true, as discussed in Chapter 5, that
In two of the three, the mothers of homosexual sons recalled persistent behaviors can change brain anatomy. Second, it is
more than average stressful experiences during their pregnan- relatively easy to publish results showing a difference between
cies (Bailey, Willerman, & Parks, 1991; Ellis, Ames, Peckham, two groups, such as homosexual and heterosexual people,
& Burke, 1988; Ellis & Cole-Harding, 2001). However, these even if the difference was unpredicted, small, and hard to ex-
studies relied on women’s memories of pregnancies more than plain. It is less easy to publish results showing no difference.
20 years earlier. A better but more difficult procedure would Thus it is possible that the published papers overstate certain
be to measure stress during pregnancy and examine the sexual anatomical differences.
orientation of the sons many years later. The most widely cited research concerns the third in-
terstitial nucleus of the anterior hypothalamus (INAH-3),
Stop & Check
which is generally more than twice as large in heterosexual
men as in women. This area has more cells with androgen
19. By what route might having an older brother increase the receptors in men than in women (Shah et al., 2004) and
probability of male homosexuality? probably plays a role in sexual behavior, although the exact
20. How might stress to a pregnant rat alter the sexual orien- role is uncertain. Simon LeVay (1991) examined INAH-3
tation of her male offspring? in 41 people who had died between the ages of 26 and 59.
Of these, 16 were heterosexual men, 6 were heterosexual
ANSWERS
endorphin levels can block the effects of testosterone. women, and 19 were homosexual men. All of the homo-
lease of endorphins in the hypothalamus, and very high sexual men, 6 of the 16 heterosexual men, and 1 of the
the same home. 20. Evidently, stress increases the re- 6 women had died of AIDS. LeVay found that the mean
of the older brother does not depend on growing up in volume of INAH-3 was larger in heterosexual men than in
immune system in the prenatal environment. The effect
heterosexual women or homosexual men, who were about
equal in this regard. Figure 11.14 shows typical cross-
ability of male homosexuality by altering the mother’s

sections for a heterosexual man and a homosexual man. Fig-


19. Having an older brother might increase the prob-

ure 11.15 shows the distribution of volumes for the three


350 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

Figure 11.14  ​Typical sizes of interstitial nucleus 3 of the anterior hypothalamus


On average, the volume of this structure was more than twice as large in a sample of heterosexual men (left) than in a sample of homo-
sexual men (right), for whom it was about the same size as in women. (From “A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual
and homosexual men,” S. LeVay, Science, 253, pp. 1034–1037. Copyright 1991. Reprinted by permission from AAAS.)

man’s INAH-3, you could make a reasonable guess about


0.20 sexual orientation, but you could not be confident.
A later study partly replicated these trends. Researchers
found that the INAH-3 nucleus was slightly larger in het-
0.15 erosexual than homosexual men, although in this study the
homosexual men’s INAH-3 nucleus was larger than that of
heterosexual women (Byne et al., 2001). Among heterosexual
0.10 men or women, the INAH-3 nucleus was larger in those who
were HIV negative than those who were HIV positive, but
even if we look only at HIV1 men, we still find a difference
0.05
in the hypothalamus between heterosexual and homosexual
men. Figure 11.16 displays the means for the five groups. On
microscopic examination of the INAH-3, researchers found
0.00
that heterosexual men had larger neurons than homosexual
F M HM
men but about the same number. (Neither this study nor
LeVay’s earlier study included homosexual females.) Still
another study found INAH-3 to be larger in heterosexual
males than in male-to-female transsexuals—that is, people
Figure 11.15  ​Volumes of the interstitial nucleus 3 of the
anterior hypothalamus (INAH-3)
Samples are females (F), heterosexual males (M), and homosexual
males (HM). Each filled circle represents a person who died of
AIDS, and each triangle represents a person who died from other 14
causes. The one open circle represents a bisexual man who died
0.12
of AIDS. (Reprinted by permission from “A difference in hypotha-
lamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men,” by S. 0.1
LeVay, Science, 253, pp. 1034–1037. Copyright © 1991 American 0.08
Association for the Advancement of Science.) 0.06
0.04
0.02

groups. Note that the difference between heterosexual men 0


Hetero Hetero Homo Hetero Hetero
and the other two groups is fairly large, on average, and that male male male female female
the cause of death (AIDS versus other) has no clear rela- HIV– HIV+ HIV+ HIV– HIV+
tionship to the results. LeVay (1993) later examined the hy-
Figure 11.16  ​Another comparison of INAH-3
pothalamus of a homosexual man who died of lung cancer;
In this study, the mean volume for homosexual men was larger
he had a small INAH-3, like the homosexual men who died than that of women but smaller than that of heterosexual men.
of AIDS. In Figure 11.15, note also the substantial amount (Based on data of Byne et al., 2001)
of difference among individuals. If you could examine some
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 351

born as males who changed their identities to female (Garcia- anatomical difference appears before any sexual behavior,
Falgueras & Swaab, 2008). and so it is more likely a cause than a result. The same may
The meaning of these results is not clear. Do differences or may not be true in humans.
in the hypothalamus influence sexual orientation, or does
sexual activity influence the size of hypothalamic neurons?
Some brain areas do grow or shrink in adults because of hor-
mones or behavioral activities (Cooke, Tabibnia, & Breed- Stop & Check
love, 1999). Studies of nonhumans offer suggestive results. 21. In LeVay’s study, what evidence argues against the idea
About 8% of rams (male sheep) direct their sexual behavior that INAH-3 volume depends on AIDS rather than sexual
toward other males. One area of the anterior hypothalamus orientation?
was larger in female-oriented rams than in male-oriented
rams and larger in them than in females (Roselli, Larkin, ANSWER
heterosexual men who died of AIDS.
Resko, Stellflug, & Stormshak, 2004). (Whether this area of other causes had about the same size INAH-3 as
corresponds to human INAH-3 is uncertain.) This area
died of other causes. One homosexual man who died

becomes larger in male than female sheep before birth as


for heterosexual men who died of AIDS and those who
21. The average size of INAH-3 was about the same
a result of prenatal testosterone levels (Roselli, Stadelman,
Reeve, Bishop, & Stormshak, 2007). In sheep, at least, an

Module 11.2  ■  In Closing

We Are Not All the Same

When Alfred Kinsey conducted the first massive surveys of hu- How far have we come since then? People today are more
man sexual behavior, he found that most of the people he inter- aware of sexual diversity than they were in Kinsey’s time and
viewed considered their own behavior “normal,” whatever it was. generally more accepting. Still, intolerance remains common.
Many believed that sexual activity much more frequent than Biological research will not tell us how to treat one another, but
their own was excessive and abnormal and might even lead to it can help us understand how we come to be so different.
insanity (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; Kinsey, Pomeroy,
Martin, & Gebhard, 1953).

Summary
  1. In many species, males and females evolve different   5. Testicular feminization, or androgen insensitivity, is a
appearances and behaviors because of sexual selection. condition in which someone with an XY chromosome
That is, they evolve in ways that make them more pattern is partly or fully insensitive to androgens and
appealing to the other sex.  341 therefore develops a female external appearance.  345
  2. Many of the mating habits of people can be interpreted   6. People born with intermediate or ambiguous genitals
in terms of increasing the probability of passing on our are called intersexes. For many years, physicians
genes. However, it is hard to know how many of the recommended surgery to make these people look more
differences between men and women are evolutionary feminine. However, many intersexed people do not
adaptations and how many are learned.  341 develop an unambiguous female identity, and many
  3. People can develop ambiguous genitals or genitals that protest against the imposed surgery.  345
don’t match their chromosomal sex for several reasons.   7. Some children have a gene that decreases their early
One is congenital adrenal hyperplasia, in which a production of dihydrotestosterone. Such a child looks
genetic defect in cortisol production leads to overstimu- female at birth and is considered a girl but develops a
lation of the adrenal gland and therefore extra testoster- penis at adolescence. Most of these people then accept a
one production. When that condition occurs in a female male gender identity.  346
fetus, she becomes partly masculinized.  343   8. On average, homosexual people differ from hetero-
  4. On average, girls with a history of congenital adrenal sexual people in several anatomical and physiological
hyperplasia show more interest in boy-typical toys than regards, although the averages do not apply to every
other girls do, and during adolescence and young individual.  347
adulthood, they continue to show partly masculinized   9. Plausible biological explanations for homosexual orienta-
interests.  344 tion include genetics, prenatal hormones, and (in males)
352 Chapter 11 Reproductive Behaviors

reactions to the mother’s immune system. Hormone levels 11. On average, certain aspects of brain anatomy differ
in adulthood are within the normal range.  347 between homosexual and heterosexual men, although it
10. Several hypotheses have been offered for how genes is not certain whether these differences are causes or
promoting homosexuality could remain at moderate effects of the behavior.  349
frequencies in the population when most homosexual
people do not have children.  347

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
androgen insensitivity  345 gender identity  343 sexual selection  341
congenital adrenal hermaphrodite  343 testicular feminization  345
hyperplasia  343 intersex  344

Thought Question
1. On average, intersexes have IQ scores in the 110 to gator’s attention. What kind of study would be best
125 range, well above the mean for the population for deciding among these hypotheses? (For one
(Dalton, 1968; Ehrhardt & Money, 1967; Lewis, answer, see Money & Lewis, 1966.)
Money, & Epstein, 1968). One possible interpretation 2. Recall LeVay’s study of brain anatomy in heterosexual
is that a hormonal pattern intermediate between male and homosexual men. Certain critics have suggested that
and female promotes great intellectual development. one or more of the men classified as “heterosexual” might
Another possibility is that intersexuality may be more actually have been homosexual or bisexual. If so, would
common in intelligent families than in less intelligent that fact strengthen or weaken the overall conclusions?
ones or that the more intelligent families are more
likely to bring their intersexed children to an investi-

chapter 11 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
11.2 Variations in Sexual Behavior 353

Videos

© 2013 Cengage Learning


Erectile Dysfunction

Animations
The Retina nnn (Figure x.x)
© 2013 Cengage Learning

Menstrual Cycle

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Colapinto, J. (2000). As nature made him: The boy who was raised as a girl. New York: HarperCol-
lins. Describes the boy whose penis was accidentally removed.
Diamond, J. (1997). Why is sex fun? New York: Basic Books. Human sexual behavior differs from
that of other species in many ways and therefore raises many evolutionary issues, which this book
addresses. For example, why do humans have sex at times when the woman cannot become preg-
nant? Why do women have menopause? Why don’t men breast-feed their babies? And what good
are men, anyway? If you haven’t thought about such questions before, you should read this book.
LeVay, S. (2011). Gay, straight, and the reason why. New York: Oxford University Press. A scien-
tific discussion of the research concerning the factors that influence sexual orientation.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, such as The Endocrine Society and the Intersex Society of America.
© Corbis Flirt/Alamy
Emotional Behaviors 12
Chapter Outline 5. Stressful events arouse the sympathetic nervous system and
later the adrenal cortex. Prolonged or severe stress produces
Module 12.1 ​What Is Emotion?
many of the same bodily responses that illness does.
Emotions, Autonomic Arousal, and the James-Lange Theory
Brain Areas Associated with Emotion
The Functions of Emotions
In Closing: Emotions and the Nervous System
Module 12.2 ​Attack and Escape Behaviors We know the meaning [of consciousness] so long as no one
Attack Behaviors asks us to define it.
Fear and Anxiety William James (1892/1961, p. 19)
Anxiety Disorders
In Closing: Doing Something About Emotions Unfortunately, one of the most significant things ever
said about emotion may be that everyone knows what it
Module 12.3  ​Stress and Health is until they are asked to define it.
Concepts of Stress Joseph LeDoux (1996, p. 23)
Stress and the Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal Cortex Axis
Stress Control
Post-traumatic Stress Disorder

S
In Closing: Emotions and Body Reactions uppose researchers have discovered a new species—
Interactive Exploration and Study let’s call it species X—and psychologists begin testing
its abilities. They place food behind a green card and
Main Ideas nothing behind a red card and find that after a few trials, X
always goes to the green card. So we conclude that X shows
1. Emotions include cognitions, actions, and feelings. Several learning, memory, and hunger. Then researchers offer X a
kinds of evidence support the theory that emotional green card and a variety of gray cards; X still goes to the green,
feelings result from actions of the muscles or organs. so it must have color vision and not just brightness discrimi-
2. Many brain areas contribute to emotions. It is not clear nation. Next they let X touch a blue triangle that is extremely
that different emotions are localized differently in the hot. X makes a loud sound and backs away. Someone picks up
brain. the blue triangle (with padded gloves) and starts moving with
3. Aggressive and fearful behaviors represent the combined it rapidly toward X. As soon as X sees this happening, it
outcome of many biological and environmental makes the same sound, turns, and starts moving rapidly away.
influences. Shall we conclude that it feels fear?
4. The amygdala responds quickly to emotional stimuli. If you said yes, now let me add: I said this was a new spe-
Damage to the amygdala interferes with attention to cies, and so it is, but it’s a new species of robot, not animal.
information that is relevant to emotions. Do you still think X feels fear? Most people are willing to talk
about artificial learning, memory, intelligence, and motivation,
but not emotion.
If such behavior isn’t adequate evidence for emotion in a
robot, is it adequate evidence for an animal? Emotion is a diffi-
cult topic because it implies conscious feelings that we cannot
observe. Biological researchers therefore concentrate mostly
on emotional behaviors, which are observable, even if the emo-
OPPOSITE: People express emotions by facial expressions,
gestures, and postures. tional feelings are not. Still, most of us eventually hope to
learn something about the emotional experiences themselves.
355
Module 12.1

What Is Emotion?

B
y one definition, emotion includes “cognitive Walter B. Cannon
evaluations, subjective changes, autonomic and neural (1871–1945)
arousal, and impulses to action” (Plutchik, 1982, As a matter of routine I have long trusted

National Library of Medicine


p. 551). That sounds okay, but by that definition, don’t hunger unconscious processes to serve me….
and thirst count as emotions? One definition of motivation is [One] example I may cite was the inter-
“an internal process that modifies the way an organism pretation of the significance of bodily
responds to a certain class of external stimuli” (Numan & changes which occur in great emotional
Woodside, 2010). By that definition, don’t happiness, sadness, excitement, such as fear and rage. These
fear, and anger count as motivations? Distinguishing between changes—the more rapid pulse, the
motivation and emotion is difficult, and possibly not worth deeper breathing, the increase of sugar in the blood, the
the effort. Still, the term emotion provides a convenient secretion from the adrenal glands—were very diverse and
category to discuss some important, interesting topics. seemed unrelated. Then, one wakeful night, after a consider-
Regardless of how we word the definition, or whether we able collection of these changes had been disclosed, the idea
define it at all, psychologists generally agree that emotion has flashed through my mind that they could be nicely integrated
three components—cognitions (“This is a dangerous situa- if conceived as bodily preparations for supreme effort in flight
tion”), feelings (“I feel frightened”), and actions (“Run for the or in fighting.
nearest exit”). Of these, feelings are the most central to our
concept of emotion. If someone reports feeling frightened, we
attribute emotion to that person at once. However, if someone which then changes your heart rate and prompts other re-
coolly calculates, “This is a dangerous situation,” but feels no sponses. In contrast, according to the James-Lange theory
tenseness or arousal, we would be less inclined to attribute ( James, 1884), the autonomic arousal and skeletal actions
emotion. What are emotional feelings, what causes them, and come first. What you experience as an emotion is the label you
what function do they serve? give to your responses: You feel afraid because you run away;
you feel angry because you attack.

Emotions, Autonomic Arousal, Commonsense View:

and the James-Lange Theory Frightening FEAR Running away,


situation increased
Emotional situations arouse the autonomic nervous system, heart rate, etc.
which has two branches—the sympathetic and the parasym-
pathetic (Figure 12.1). Walter Cannon was the first to under- James-Lange Theory:
© Cengage Learning 2013

stand that the sympathetic nervous system prepares the body Frightening Running away, FEAR
for brief, vigorous “fight-or-flight” responses. The parasympa- situation increased
thetic nervous system increases digestion and other processes heart rate, etc.
that save energy and prepare for later events. However, each
situation evokes its own special mixture of sympathetic and
parasympathetic arousal (Wolf, 1995). For example, nausea is You might object, “How would I know to run away before
associated with sympathetic stimulation of the stomach (de- I was scared?” In a later paper, William James (1894) clarified
creasing its contractions and secretions) and parasympathetic his position. An emotion has three components: cognitions,
stimulation of the intestines and salivary glands. actions, and feelings. The cognitive aspect comes first. You
How does the autonomic nervous system relate to emo- quickly appraise something as good, bad, frightening, or what-
tions? Common sense holds that first you feel an emotion, ever. Your appraisal of the situation leads to an appropriate

356
12.1 What Is Emotion? 357

Preganglionic axons
Postganglionic axons

Cranial
nerves
Vagus (12 pairs)
nerve
Cervical
nerves
(8 pairs)

Celiac
ganglion Thoracic
nerves
(12 pairs)

Lumbar
nerves
(5 pairs)
(Most ganglia
near spinal cord) Sacral
Pelvic nerves
nerve (5 pairs)

Sympathetic outflow Parasympathetic outflow

Figure 12.1  ​The sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems


Review Chapter 4 for more information. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

action, such as running away, attacking, or sitting motionless Is Physiological Arousal Necessary
with your heart racing. When William James said that arousal
and actions led to emotions, he meant the feeling aspect of an for Emotions?
emotion. That is, People with damage to the spinal cord are paralyzed from
the level of the damage downward. People who cannot move
James-Lange Theory: their arms and legs certainly cannot attack or run away. Most
Event of them report that they feel emotions about the same as be-
fore their injury (Cobos, Sánchez, Pérez, & Vila, 2004). This
Appraisal (the cognitive aspect)
finding indicates that emotions do not require feedback from
muscle movements. However, paralysis does not affect the au-
tonomic nervous system, so it remains possible that emotional
© Cengage Learning 2013

Action (the behavioral aspect including physiology) feelings depend on feedback from autonomic responses.
In people with an uncommon condition called pure auto-
Emotional feeling (the feeling aspect) nomic failure, output from the autonomic nervous system to
the body fails, either completely or almost completely. Heart-
beat and other organ activities continue, but the nervous sys-
If a feeling is a kind of sensation, it is hard to know where tem no longer regulates them. Someone with this condition
the sensation would come from if not from some change in does not react to stressful experiences with changes in heart
the body. Nevertheless, we want to test the idea. The James- rate, blood pressure, or sweating. According to the James-
Lange theory leads to two predictions: People with weak au- Lange theory, we would expect such people to report no
tonomic or skeletal responses should feel less emotion, and emotions. In fact, they report “having” the same emotions as
causing or increasing someone’s responses should enhance an anyone else. They have little difficulty identifying what emo-
emotion. Let’s consider the evidence. tion a character in a story would probably experience (Heims,
358 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Critchley, Dolan, Mathias, & Cipolotti, 2004). However, they ask whether they are happy, people guess what the experiment
say they feel their emotions much less intensely than before is about and say what they think we want to hear. Clever re-
(Critchley, Mathias, & Dolan, 2001). Presumably, when they searchers found a way to get people to smile while concealing
report emotions, they refer to the cognitive aspect: “Yes, I’m the purpose of the study. It is a method you could easily try
angry, because this is a situation that calls for anger.” But they yourself: Hold a pen in your mouth, either with your teeth or
do not feel the anger, or if they do, they feel it weakly. Their de- with your lips, as shown in Figure 12.2. Now examine a page
creased emotional feeling is consistent with predictions from of newspaper comic strips. Mark each one 1 for very funny,
the James-Lange theory. ✓ for somewhat funny, or – for not funny. Most people rate
Here is another example: Botulinum toxin (“BOTOX”) cartoons funnier when holding a pen with their teeth—which
blocks transmission at synapses and nerve-muscle junctions. forces a smile—than when holding it with their lips—which
Physicians sometimes use it to paralyze the muscles for prevents a smile (Strack, Martin, & Stepper,
frowning and thereby remove frown lines on people’s faces. 1988). That is, the sensation of smiling in-
One result is that people become slightly slower at reading creases happiness, although only slightly. (Tell-
try it 
unhappy sentences. Ordinarily, when people read something ing a depressed person to cheer up and smile yourself
unpleasant, they frown just a bit. Evidently an inability to does not help.)
frown interferes with processing unpleasant information Researchers also found a clever way to ask
(Havas, Glenberg, Gutowski, Lucarelli, & Davidson, 2010). people to frown without saying so. They said they wanted to
A related study examined people with BOTOX injections test people’s ability to do a cognitive task and a motor task
that temporarily paralyzed all the facial muscles. These people at the same time. The cognitive task was to examine photo-
reported weaker than usual emotional responses when they graphs and rate their pleasantness or unpleasantness. For the
watched short videos (Davis, Senghas, Brandt, & Ochsner, motor task, researchers attached golf tees to each of the per-
2010). Another study found that people with brain damage son’s eyebrows and said to try to keep the tips of the golf tees
that prevents voluntary facial movements have trouble recog- touching each other. The only way to do that was to frown.
nizing other people’s emotional expressions, especially expres- People given this instruction rated the photographs as more
sions of fear (Pistoia et al., 2010). The implication of all these unpleasant than the average for people who were not induced
studies is that feeling a body change is important for feeling to frown (Larsen, Kasimatis, & Frey, 1992).
an emotion. However, although smiles and frowns slightly alter hap-
Evidence pointing away from this conclusion comes from piness, smiles are not necessary for happiness. People with a
a study of people with two kinds of brain damage. People rare condition called Möbius syndrome cannot move their fa-
with damage to the right somatosensory cortex had normal cial muscles to make a smile, as shown in Figure 12.3. They
autonomic responses to emotional music but reported little nevertheless experience happiness and amusement, although
subjective experience. People with damage to part of the pre- they have trouble making friends because other people react
frontal cortex had weak autonomic responses but normal sub- to the lack of smiling. The girl shown in the figure underwent
jective responses ( Johnson, Tranel, Lutgendorf, & Adolphs, surgery to give her an artificial smile (G. Miller, 2007b).
2009). However, it was not clear whether people’s reports of
their “emotional experience” accurately recorded the feeling as-
pect of emotion, as opposed to the cognitive aspect.

Is Physiological Arousal Sufficient


for Emotions?
According to the James-Lange theory, emotional feelings re-
sult from the body’s actions. If your heart started racing and
you started sweating and breathing rapidly, would you feel
an emotion? Well, it depends. If you had those responses be-
cause you ran a mile, you would attribute your feelings to the
exercise, not emotion. However, if they occurred spontane-
Kathleen Olson/kofoto

ously, you might indeed interpret your increased sympathetic


nervous system arousal as fear. Rapid breathing in particular
makes people worry that they are suffocating, and they experi-
ence a panic attack, marked by extreme sympathetic nervous
system arousal (Klein, 1993).
Figure 12.2  ​Effect of facial expression on emotion
What about other emotions? For example, if you find
People who hold a pen in their teeth, and who are therefore forced
yourself smiling, do you become happier? To test this hypoth-
to smile, are more likely to report amusement than are people
esis, how could we get people to smile? Yes, of course, we could with a pen in their lips, who therefore cannot smile.
tell them to smile. However, if we tell people to smile and then
12.1 What Is Emotion? 359

Figure 12.3  ​Möbius syndrome


People with this condition cannot move
their facial muscles to smile. This girl
went through surgery to give her an
artificial smile, as shown. The lack
Courtesy of Lori Thomas

of a smile before surgery did not rob


her of happiness or a sense of humor,
although it interfered with her ability to
make friends.

Overall, the results suggest that our perceptions of the Brain Areas Associated with
body’s actions contribute to our emotional feelings, as the James-
Lange theory proposed. Many psychologists therefore refer to Emotion
emotions as “embodied”—that is, they depend on responses of Do different emotions activate different brain areas? More-
the body. The theory does not insist that we can tell one emotion over, which brain areas react most strongly to emotions?
apart from another by our physiological responses.

Attempts to Localize Specific Emotions


Stop & Check
Traditionally, the limbic system—the forebrain areas sur-
1. According to the James-Lange theory, what kind of person rounding the thalamus—has been regarded as critical for
should feel no emotions? emotion (Figure 12.4). We consider one part of it, the amyg-
2. How did researchers get people to smile or frown without
dala, in more detail later in this chapter. Much of the cerebral
using those words?
cortex also reacts to emotional situations.
Whereas many brain areas respond to emotion in one way
telling them to keep the two tees touching each other. or another, it is theoretically important to know whether dif-
ANSWERS
frown by attaching golf tees to their eyebrows and then ferent brain areas respond to different emotions. For example,
to hold a pen between their teeth. They got people to does one brain area respond during happiness and another
situation.”) 2. They got people to smile by telling them during sadness? Many researchers have used PET or fMRI
nize the cognitive aspects of emotion. (“This is a sad techniques to identify the cortical areas that respond while
emotions. However, such a person might still recog-
people look at emotional pictures or listen to emotional sto-
ries. In Figure 12.5, each dot represents one research study
no perceivable changes in any organ should feel no

that found significant activation of a particular cortical area as-


1. Someone who had no muscle movements and

sociated with happiness, sadness, disgust, fear, or anger (Phan,


360 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Cingulate gyrus

Anterior thalamic nuclei

Septal nuclei

Frontal lobe Fornix

Hippocampus
Olfactory bulb

Parahippocampal gyrus
(limbic lobe)

Mamillary bodies Amygdala


Figure 12.4  ​The limbic system 
The limbic system is a group of stuctures in the interior of the brain. Here you see them as if the exterior of the brain were transparent.
(© Argosy Publishing Inc.)

Happiness
Sadness
Disgust
Figure 12.5  ​Brain areas associated with
particular emotions Fear
Each dot represents a study that found increased Anger
activity in a given brain area associated with
the emotion designated by the color of the dot.
(Reprinted from NeuroImage, 16, Phan, K. L.,
Wagner, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I., “Functional
neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of
emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI,” pages
331–348, Copyright 2002, with permission from
Elsevier.)

Wager, Taylor, & Liberzon, 2002). The frontal and temporal ular cells that responded mainly to pleasant pictures and others
cortices show many dots, and other kinds of research also that responded mainly to unpleasant pictures (Kawasaki et al.,
point to these areas as important for emotions (Kringelbach, 2005). However, no one has demonstrated cells that respond
2005). However, the most salient point of this figure is the only to a particular unpleasant emotion, such as sadness or fear.
variability of location for each emotion. The results apparently Of all emotions, only disgust seems to be associated with
depend more on the details of procedure than on which emo- the response of a particular brain area. The insular cortex,
tion was targeted. or insula, is strongly activated if you see a disgusting picture
Sometimes, physicians insert electrodes directly into the (F. C. Murphy, Nimmo-Smith, & Lawrence, 2003; M. L. Phil-
brains of patients with epilepsy to monitor their responses over lips et al., 1997) or the facial expression of someone who is feel-
time. In one study, researchers used these implanted electrodes ing disgusted (Wicker et al., 2003). That is, if you see someone
to record responses to emotional pictures. They did find partic- who looks disgusted, you feel disgusted, too. In fact, different
12.1 What Is Emotion? 361

parts of the insula respond to different types of disgusting scenes, either laughter or crying activates the right amygdala more
such as a video of a surgical operation and a video of someone than the left (Sander & Scheich, 2001). When people look at
vomiting (Harrison, Gray, Gianaros, & Critchley, 2010). faces, drawing their attention to the emotional expression in-
Locating disgust in the insula is interesting because that is creases the activity in the right temporal cortex (Narumoto,
the primary taste cortex (Figure 7.21). Disgust is literally dis- Okada, Sadato, Fukui, & Yonekura, 2001). People with dam-
gust, or bad taste. To react with disgust is to react as if some- age to the right temporal cortex have trouble identifying
thing tasted bad; we want to spit it out. One man with damage other people’s emotional expressions or even saying whether
to his insular cortex not only failed to experience disgust in two people are expressing the same emotion or different ones
daily life but also had trouble recognizing other people’s dis- (H. J. Rosen et al., 2002).
gust expressions. When he heard a retching sound, he did not In one fascinating study, people watched videotapes of
recognize that it meant nausea or vomiting. How disgusted 10 people. All 10 described themselves honestly during one
would you be if you found a cockroach in your soup? What if speech and completely dishonestly during another. The task
you saw someone whose intestines were spilling out through of the observers was to guess which of the two interviews was
a hole in the abdomen? What if you saw people with feces the honest one. The task is more difficult than it might sound,
on their hands and faces? To questions like these, this patient and most people are no more correct than chance (about 5 of
gave much lower ratings than other people do (Calder, Keane, 10). The only group tested that performed better than chance
Manes, Antoun, & Young, 2000). was a group of people with left-hemisphere brain damage
However, the insula also responds to frightening pictures (Etcoff, Ekman, Magee, & Frank, 2000). They got only 60%
(Schienle et al., 2002) and pictures of angry faces (Fusar-Poli correct—not great, but at least better than chance. Evidently,
et al., 2009), not just to items suggesting disgust. Therefore, the right hemisphere is better not only at expressing emotions
we should not too closely equate the insula with disgust. but also at detecting other people’s emotions. With the left
hemisphere out of the way, the right hemisphere was free to
do what it does best.
Stop & Check In another study, 11 patients went through a procedure
in which one hemisphere at a time was anesthetized by
3. The insula is important for which kind of emotion, and
drug injection into one of the carotid arteries, which pro-
which kind of sensation?
vide blood to the head. (This procedure, called the Wada
procedure, is sometimes used before certain kinds of brain
ANSWER
3. The insula is important for disgust and taste.
surgery.) All 11 patients had left-hemisphere language, so
they could not be interviewed with the left hemisphere
inactivated. When they were tested with the right hemi-
Contributions of the Left and Right sphere inactivated, something fascinating happened: They
could still describe any of the sad, frightening, or irritating
Hemispheres events they had experienced in life, but they remembered
Another hypothesis relates the two hemispheres of the only the facts, not the emotion. For example, one patient
brain to different categories of emotion. Activity of the left remembered a car wreck, another remembered visiting his
hemisphere, especially its frontal and temporal lobes, relates mother while she was dying, and another remembered a
to what Jeffrey Gray (1970) called the Behavioral Activa- time his wife threatened to kill him. But they denied they
tion System (BAS), marked by low to moderate autonomic had felt any significant fear, sadness, or anger. When they
arousal and a tendency to approach, which could character- described the same events with both hemispheres active,
ize either happiness or anger. Increased activity of the frontal they remembered strong emotions. So evidently, when
and temporal lobes of the right hemisphere is associated with the right hemisphere is inactive, people do not experience
the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), which increases at- strong emotions and do not even remember feeling them
tention and arousal, inhibits action, and stimulates emotions (Ross, Homan, & Buck, 1994).
such as fear and disgust (Davidson & Fox, 1982; Davidson &
Henriques, 2000; F. C. Murphy et al., 2003; Reuter-Lorenz &
Davidson, 1981). Stop & Check
The difference between the hemispheres relates to personal- 4. What are the contributions of the right hemisphere to
ity: On average, people with greater activity in the frontal cortex emotional behaviors and interpreting other people’s
of the left hemisphere tend to be happier, more outgoing, and emotions?
more fun-loving. People with greater right-hemisphere activ-
ity tend to be socially withdrawn, less satisfied with life, and ANSWER
emotions.
prone to unpleasant emotions (Knyazev, Slobodskaya, & Wil- for interpreting other people’s expressions of
son, 2002; Schmidt, 1999; Shackman, McMenamin, Maxwell,
right hemisphere is also more specialized than the left

Greischar, & Davidson, 2009; Urry et al., 2004).


with withdrawal from events and social contact. The
4. Activation of the right hemisphere is associated
The right hemisphere appears to be more responsive to
emotional stimuli than the left. For example, listening to
362 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

The Functions of Emotions spider pictures, and people shocked after snake pictures learned
If we evolved the capacity to experience and express emotions, an increased heart rate after snake pictures, even though nei-
emotions must have been adaptive for our ancestors, and ther group could consciously identify the pictures. On certain
probably for us as well. What good do emotions do? trials, participants were asked to report any perceived changes
For certain emotions, the answer is clear. Fear alerts us to in their heart rate, which were compared to measurements of
escape from danger. Anger directs us to attack an intruder. their actual heart rate. On other trials, after the stimulus, they
Disgust tells us to avoid something that might cause illness. guessed whether a shock was forthcoming. In general, those
The adaptive value of happiness, sadness, embarrassment, and who were most accurate at reporting their heart rate increases
other emotions is less obvious, although researchers have sug- were the most accurate at predicting whether they were about
gested some plausible possibilities. to get a shock (Katkin, Wiens, & Öhman, 2001). The interpre-
Also, emotions provide a useful guide when we need to tation is that people who are good at detecting their autonomic
make a quick decision. Sometimes, your “gut feeling” is use- responses may have valid gut feelings about dangers that they
ful. In one study, college students viewed a series of slides of cannot identify consciously.
snakes and spiders, each presented for just 10 ms, followed by
a masking stimulus—a random array of unrecognizable pat-
terns. Under these conditions, people cannot identify whether Emotions and Moral Decisions
they saw a snake or a spider. For each participant, one kind of We base many important decisions partly on emotional con-
stimulus—either the snakes or the spiders—was always fol- siderations—how we think one outcome or another will make
lowed by a mild shock 5.6 seconds later. Most of those shocked us feel. Consider the following moral dilemmas, of which Fig-
after spider pictures developed a bigger heart rate increase after ure 12.6 illustrates three.

(a)

(b)

Figure 12.6  ​Three moral


dilemmas
(a) Would you divert a runaway train
so it kills one person instead of five?
(b) Would you push someone off a
footbridge so a runaway train kills him
instead of five others? (c) Would you
push someone off a sinking lifeboat to
save yourself and four others?
(© Cengage Learning 2013) (c)
12.1 What Is Emotion? 363

The Trolley Dilemma. A runaway trolley is headed to- they have made the wrong choice (Berlin, Rolls, & Kischka,
ward five people on a track. The only way you can pre- 2004). You might think of impulsive decisions as emotional,
vent their death is to switch the trolley onto another but these people’s decisions often seem unemotional. For ex-
track, where it will kill one person. Would it be right to ample, if confronted with the trolley car dilemma or the other
pull the switch? dilemmas we just discussed, people with prefrontal damage
The Footbridge Dilemma. You are standing on a footbridge are more likely than average to choose the utilitarian option
overlooking a trolley track. A runaway trolley is headed of killing one to save five, even in situations where most peo-
toward five people on a track. The only way you can pre- ple find the choice emotionally unacceptable (Koenigs et al.,
vent their death is to push a heavy-set stranger off the 2007). And they make that utilitarian decision quickly and
footbridge and onto the track so that he will block the calmly.
trolley. Would it be right to push him? The most famous case of a person with prefrontal damage
is that of Phineas Gage. In 1848, an explosion sent an iron
The Lifeboat Dilemma. You and six other people are on a
rod through Gage’s prefrontal cortex. Amazingly, he survived.
lifeboat in icy waters, but it is overcrowded and starting to
During the next few months, his behavior was impulsive and
sink. If you push one of the people off the boat, the boat
he made poor decisions. These are common symptoms of
will stop sinking and the rest of you will survive. Would it
prefrontal damage. However, the reports about his behavior
be right to push someone off?
provide little detail. Over the years, with multiple retellings,
The Hospital Dilemma. You are a surgeon, and five of your people elaborated and exaggerated the meager facts available
patients will die soon unless they get organ transplants. Each (Kotowicz, 2007).
needs the transplant of a different organ. You haven’t been We know more about a modern case. Antonio Damasio
able to find organ donors for any of them. Then a nurse (1994) examined a man with prefrontal cortex damage who
bursts into your office: “Good news! A visitor to the hospital expressed almost no emotions. Nothing angered him. He was
has just arrived, who has exactly the same tissue type as all never very sad, even about his own brain damage. Nothing
five of your patients! We can kill this visitor and use the or- gave him much pleasure, not even music. Far from being bril-
gans to save the five others!” Would it be right to do so? liantly rational, he frequently made bad decisions that cost
In each of these dilemmas, you can save five people (in- him his job, his marriage, and his savings. When tested in the
cluding yourself in the lifeboat case) by killing one person. laboratory, he successfully predicted the probable outcomes of
However, although that may be true logically, the decisions various decisions. For example, when asked what would hap-
do not feel the same. Most people (though not all) say it is pen if he cashed a check and the bank teller handed him too
right to pull the switch in the trolley dilemma. Fewer say yes much money, he knew the probable consequences of returning
in the footbridge and lifeboat dilemmas. Almost no one en- it or walking away with it. But he admitted, “I still wouldn’t
dorses killing one person to save five others in the hospital know what to do” (A. R. Damasio, 1994, p. 49). He knew that
dilemma. Brain scans show that contemplating the footbridge one action would win him approval and another would get
or lifeboat dilemma activates brain areas known to respond to him in trouble, but he apparently did not anticipate that ap-
emotions, including parts of the prefrontal cortex and cingu- proval would feel good and trouble would feel bad. In a sense,
late gyrus (Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley, & Cohen, any choice requires consideration of values and emotions—
2001). Responses in the amygdala are also important. We how we think one outcome or another will make us feel. In
don’t want to act to harm someone, because we identify with Damasio’s words, “Inevitably, emotions are inseparable from
that other person and begin to feel the pain that our actions the idea of good and evil” (A. Damasio, 1999, p. 55).
might cause that other person (Pfaff, 2007). In short, when After damage to a particular part of the prefrontal
we are making a decision about right and wrong, we seldom cortex—the ventromedial prefrontal cortex—people seem
work it out rationally. One decision or the other immediately deficient in their sense of guilt, both in everyday life and in
“feels” right. After we have already decided, we try to think of laboratory situations. Consider two economic games: In the
a logical justification (Haidt, 2001). one-shot Dictator game, you are the Dictator, and you are
given some money to divide between yourself and another
person, whatever way you choose. Most people split it evenly
Decision Making After Brain Damage or almost evenly, keeping a little more than half for them-
selves. People with ventromedial prefrontal damage keep
that Impairs Emotions about 90%, on average. In the Trust game (also mentioned in
Damage to parts of the prefrontal cortex blunts people’s emo- Chapter 11), one person gets some money and has the option
tions in most regards, except for an occasional outburst of an- of giving some of it to a Trustee. If so, the amount given triples
ger. It also impairs decision making. People with such damage in value, and the Trustee can return any amount of it, such as
often make impulsive decisions without pausing to consider half, to the first person. People with ventromedial prefrontal
the consequences, including how they will feel after a possible damage give less, showing decreased trust. If they are in the
mistake. When given a choice, they frequently make a quick position of Trustee, they keep all or nearly all of the money in-
decision and then immediately sigh or wince, knowing that stead of returning it (Krajbich, Adolphs, Tranel, Denburg, &
364 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Camerer, 2009). In short, they show less than normal concern temporal lobe) are slow in processing emotional information.
for others. If most people didn’t show a reasonable amount of In this experiment, they show no nervous tension when draw-
concern for others, civilization would fall apart. ing from decks A and B, and they continue choosing those
Here is an experiment to explore further the role of emo- decks (Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Lee, 1999). In short,
tions in decisions. In the Iowa Gambling Task, people can failure to anticipate the unpleasantness of likely outcomes
draw one card at a time from four piles. They always win $100 leads to bad decisions.
in play money from decks A and B, or $50 from C and D. Of course, it is also true that emotions sometimes interfere
However, some of the cards also have penalties: with good decisions. If you were driving and suddenly started
skidding on a patch of ice, what would you do? A patient
with damage to his prefrontal cortex who happened to face
A B C D this situation calmly followed the advice he had always heard:
Take your foot off the accelerator and steer in the direction of
the skid (Shiv, Loewenstein, Bechara, Damasio, & Damasio,
© Cengage Learning 2013

Gain $100; Gain $100; Gain $50; Gain $50; 2005). Most people in this situation panic, hit the brakes, and
one-half of one-tenth of one-half of one-tenth of steer away from the skid, making a bad situation worse.
all cards also all cards also all cards also all cards also
have penalties have penalties have penalties have penalties
averaging $250 of $1250 averaging $50 of $250
Stop & Check
When you see all the payoffs laid out, you can easily deter- 5. If brain damage impairs someone’s emotions, what hap-
mine that the best strategy is to pick cards from decks C and pens to the person’s decision making?
D. In the experiment, however, people have to discover the
payoffs by trial and error. Ordinarily, as people sample from ANSWER
make them feel.
all four decks, they gradually start showing signs of nervous not quickly imagine how bad a poor decision might
tension whenever they draw a card from A or B, and they start
make impulsive decisions, evidently because they do

shifting their preference toward C and D. People with damage


5. After brain damage that impairs emotion, people

to either the prefrontal cortex or the amygdala (part of the

Module 12.1  ■  In Closing

Emotions and the Nervous System

Although we regard emotions as nebulous internal states, they different neurotransmitters, that evidence would strongly sup-
are fundamentally biological. As William James observed in port the idea of basic emotions. However, so far, researchers
the early days of psychology, emotions are “embodied”—an have found no evidence that each emotion has a specific physi-
emotional feeling requires some action and a perception of that ology, with the possible exception of disgust.
action. Studies of people with brain damage also shed light on the
Biological research sheds light on many of the central ques- functions of emotion, particularly with relation to moral behav-
tions about the psychology of emotions. For example, one issue ior and decision making. Far from being an impediment to in-
is whether people have a few “basic” emotions or continuous telligent behavior, emotional reactions are often a useful quick
dimensions along which emotions vary. If researchers found guide to appropriate actions. In short, understanding emotions
that different emotions depended on different brain areas or and understanding their biology go hand in hand.

Summary
1. According to the James-Lange theory, the feeling aspect 3. Feedback from facial movements or other actions can
of an emotion results from feedback from actions of the strengthen an emotional feeling, but they are not
muscles and organs.  356 necessary for such feelings.  358
2. Consistent with the James-Lange theory, people who 4. Emotional experiences arouse many brain areas, as
have impaired autonomic responses have weaker emo- measured by fMRI scans or EEG recordings. So far,
tional feelings, although they continue to identify the the research does not convincingly assign different
cognitive aspects of emotion.  357 emotions to different brain areas, with the possible
exception of disgust.  359
12.1 What Is Emotion? 365

5. Activation of the frontal and temporal areas of the left tion is that people decide badly because they do not
hemisphere is associated with approach and the Behav- quickly imagine their emotional reactions to possible
ioral Activation System. The corresponding areas of the consequences.  362
right hemisphere are associated with withdrawal, 7. People with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal
decreased activity, and the Behavioral Inhibition System. cortex show little concern for other people. They appar-
The right hemisphere is more effective than the left for ently lack a normal sense of guilt.  363
recognizing emotional expressions.  361
6. Brain damage that impairs emotional feelings and
responses also impairs decision making. One interpreta-

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
Behavioral Activation System James-Lange theory  356 panic attack  358
(BAS)  361 limbic system  359 pure autonomic failure  357
Behavioral Inhibition System
(BIS)  361

Thought Question
According to the James-Lange theory, we should expect peo- average emotions. What kind of people might experience
ple with pure autonomic failure to experience weaker than stronger than average emotions?
Module 12.2

Attack and Escape Behaviors

H
ave you ever watched a cat play with a rat or mouse
before killing it? It might kick, bat, toss, pick up, shake,
and carry the rodent. Is the cat sadistically tormenting Caudate
its prey? No. Most of what we call its “play” behaviors are a nuclei
compromise between attack and escape: When the rodent is
facing away, the cat approaches; if the rodent turns around Thalamus
Putamen
and bares its teeth to the cat, the cat bats it or kicks it Globus (lateral)
defensively (Pellis et al., 1988). A cat usually goes for a quick pallidus Hippocampus
kill if the rodent is small and inactive or if the cat has been (medial)
Amygdala
given drugs that lower its anxiety. The same cat withdraws
altogether if confronted with a large, menacing rodent. “Play”
occurs in intermediate situations (Adamec, Stark-Adamec, &
Livingston, 1980; Biben, 1979; Pellis et al., 1988).
Most of the vigorous emotional behaviors we observe in Central nucleus
animals fall into the categories of attack and escape, and it is
Corticomedial
no coincidence that we describe the sympathetic nervous sys- nucleus
tem as the fight-or-flight system. These behaviors and their
Lateral nucleus
corresponding emotions—anger and fear—are closely related
both behaviorally and physiologically. Basal nucleus

(a)
Attack Behaviors Corpus
callosum
Attack behavior depends on the individual as well as the situ-
ation. If a hamster intrudes into another hamster’s territory, Anterior
commissure Caudate nucleus
the home hamster sniffs the intruder and eventually attacks,
but usually not at once. Suppose the intruder leaves, and a lit-
tle later, another hamster intrudes. The home hamster attacks Putamen
faster and more vigorously than before. The first attack in-
Optic Globus pallidus
creases the probability of a second attack against any intruder chiasm
for the next 30 minutes or more (Potegal, 1994). It is as if the
first attack gets the hamster in the mood to attack again. Dur- Amygdala
ing that period, activity builds up in the corticomedial area
of the amygdala (Figure 12.7), and as it does so, it increases (b)
the hamster’s probability of attacking (Potegal, Ferris, Hebert,
Meyerhoff, & Skaredoff, 1996; Potegal, Hebert, DeCoster, & Figure 12.7  ​Location of amygdala in the human brain
The amygdala, located in the interior of the temporal lobe,
Meyerhoff, 1996). Something similar happens in people, al-
receives input from many cortical and subcortical areas. Part (a)
though we can only speculate about whether the brain mecha-
shows several nuclei of the amygdala. ([a] After Hanaway, Woolsey,
nism is the same: If you hold a toddler’s arm to prevent him or Gado, & Roberts, 1998; Nieuwenhuys, Voogd, & vanHuijzen, 1988;
her from playing with a toy, the result is sometimes screaming [b] Photo courtesy of Dr. Dana Copeland)
and other signs of anger. If you pause 30 seconds and then do

366
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 367

it again, the anger is more rapid and more intense (Potegal, 1


Robison, Anderson, Jordan, & Shapiro, 2007).
The same is true for adults. You may have noticed times Low MAOA
0.75 activity
when one person annoys you and a few minutes later you get

Composite index of
antisocial behavior
angry with someone else. You have probably been told, “If 0.5
you become angry, count to 10 before you act.” Counting to a
few thousand would work better, but the idea is correct. Ly- 0.25
ing on your back is another way to decrease anger. Research
0 High MAOA
has shown that it is easier to feel angry while standing (and activity
therefore in a position to attack) than while lying in a more
helpless position (Harmon-Jones & Peterson, 2009). As in –0.25
several cases in the first module, this finding supports the idea
that emotion is embodied: What you are doing or about to do –0.5
None Probable Severe
affects how you feel. Childhood maltreatment

Figure 12.8  ​Genes, environment, and antisocial behavior in


Heredity and Environment in Violence men
Why do some people turn to violence more readily than oth- The y axis represents a complex score combining several types of
ers do? Some environmental factors are easy to identify. Cer- measurement. Higher scores indicate more aggressive behaviors.
tainly people who were abused in childhood, people who wit- (From “Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated chil-
nessed violent abuse between their parents, and people who dren,” from Caspi, A., et al., Science, 297, 851–854. © 2002 AAAS.)
live in a violent neighborhood are at greater risk of violence
themselves. Another environmental factor is exposure to lead,
which is harmful to developing brains. Since the banning of
lead-based paints and the rise of unleaded gasoline, the preva- of their MAOA levels. In those who endured a small amount of
lence of violent crime has declined, possibly as a result of the mistreatment in childhood, the rate of antisocial behavior in-
decreased lead in the environment (Nevin, 2007). creased, but their MAOA levels did not make much difference.
What about heredity? Monozygotic twins resemble each However, among those who were seriously maltreated in child-
other more closely than dizygotic twins do with regard to vio- hood, the rate of antisocial behavior was significantly higher
lent and criminal behaviors, and adopted children resemble for those with low MAOA activity (Caspi et al., 2002). This re-
their biological parents more closely than their adoptive par- sult is fascinating because of its apparent demonstration of an
ents (Rhee & Waldman, 2002). However, various kinds of ag- interaction between genetics and environment. However, from
gressive behavior occur under different circumstances, and we a theoretical standpoint, it is not clear why decreased MAOA
cannot expect to find a single gene or set of genes that will ac- should be linked to increased aggression, or why the effect of
count for all the variations (Yeh, Coccaro, & Jacobson, 2010). this gene should depend on the environment. We can look for-
For example, researchers found one gene linked to aggressive ward to more detailed investigations of this relationship.
behavior that is common only among people of Finnish ances-
try (Bevilacqua et al., 2010).
After researchers repeatedly failed to find a strong link be- Stop & Check
tween any single gene and aggressive behavior, they explored
the possibility of interactions between heredity and environ- 6. What relationship did Caspi et al. (2002) report between
ment. Several studies have found that violence is particularly the enzyme MAOA and antisocial behavior?
enhanced in people with both a genetic predisposition and rates of antisocial behavior.
a troubled early environment (Cadoret, Yates, Troughton, ANSWER
people with lower levels of the enzyme showed higher
Woodworth, & Stewart, 1995; Caspi et al., 2002; Enoch, who suffered serious maltreatment during childhood,
Steer, Newman, Gibson, & Goldman, 2010; Widom & Brzus- ability of antisocial behavior. However, among those
towicz, 2006). Figure 12.8 illustrates the effects of genetic dif- tion of MAOA do not differ significantly in their prob-
ferences in production of the enzyme monoamine oxidase A 6. Overall, people with genes for high or low produc-
(MAOA). This enzyme breaks down the neurotransmitters
dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin, thus lowering the
available amounts. Researchers find little difference in aggres-
sive or other antisocial behavior, on average, between people
with high or low amounts of MAOA. However, the effects Hormones
of this gene apparently interact with childhood experience. Most fighting in the animal kingdom is by males competing
As the figure shows, the rate of antisocial behavior was low for mates or females defending their young. Male aggressive
among people who were treated well in childhood, regardless behavior depends heavily on testosterone, which is highest for
368 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

adult males in the reproductive season. Even in species that tosterone levels, researchers can readily measure the effects
do not have a particular season for breeding, testosterone in- of an increase. In one study, women received either testos-
creases are linked with increased striving for social dominance terone or placebo and then played the Ultimatum game. In
(Beehner et al., 2009). this game, the first person is given $10 and then must pro-
Similarly, throughout the world, men fight more often pose how to split it with a second person—$5 each, or $7
than women, commit more violent crimes, shout more insults for me and $3 for you, or whatever. The second person can
at one another, and so forth. Moreover, young adult men, who then accept the offer, or veto it, in which case neither person
have the highest testosterone levels, have the highest rate of receives anything. We might expect that giving testosterone
aggressive behaviors and violent crimes. Women’s violent acts to the first person would lead to a greedy offer, but in fact
are in most cases less severe (Archer, 2000). the women receiving testosterone offered their partner a bit
If we compare men of the same age, do men with higher more money, on average, than did those receiving a placebo
testosterone levels also commit more violent behavior? (Eisenegger, Naef, Snozzi, Heinrichs, & Fehr, 2010). That
Yes, on average, although the effects are often smaller than reaction doesn’t make sense in terms of aggression, although
most people expect (Archer, Birring, & Wu, 1998; Archer, it might make sense in terms of enhancing one’s status. That
Graham-Kevan, & Davies, 2005). Figure 12.9 shows one set is, someone who offers the other person a good share of the
of results. Note that high testosterone levels were more com- money is saying, “I’m so successful that I can afford to be
mon among men convicted of violent crimes than for those generous with you.”
convicted of less violent crimes, but also note that the differ- In another study, the women’s task was to examine photos
ences are small. According to the “triple imbalance hypoth- of faces and try to identify the expressed emotions among six
esis,” the reason testosterone’s effects are unimpressive is choices: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise.
that violence depends on other chemicals as well, especially The photos were morphed from 0% (neutral expression) to
cortisol and serotonin (van Honk, Harmon-Jones, Morgan, 100% expression of an emotion. Figure 12.10 shows the ex-
& Schutter, 2010). Cortisol, which increases under stressful ample for anger.
conditions, increases fear, and a decrease in cortisol is associ- The result was that after women received testosterone,
ated with loss of inhibitions. Therefore, aggression tends to most became less accurate at recognizing facial expressions
be highest when testosterone levels are high and cortisol lev- of anger (van Honk & Schutter, 2007). Meanwhile, other
els low. Men with high levels of both testosterone and corti- research shows that testosterone increases responses of the
sol are likely to inhibit their violent impulses. Serotonin also amygdala to photos showing angry expressions (Hermans,
tends to inhibit violent impulses. Ramsey, & van Honk, 2008). Evidently, testosterone affects
To test the effects of testosterone, correlational studies certain brain areas differently, increasing the responses of
are not ideal, because men with high testosterone levels may emotion-related areas, while decreasing the ability of the cere-
be unusual in other regards besides testosterone. Several bral cortex to identify the emotion consciously. We can specu-
studies used the idea of temporarily increasing testosterone late that the result might be increased emotional arousal and
levels in women. Because most women start with low tes- decreased ability to regulate that emotion deliberately.

Burglary Armed
Rape
robbery

Higher testosterone
Murder Drug offenses Intermediate testosterone
Lower testosterone

Figure 12.9  ​Testosterone levels for male prisoners


Testosterone levels are higher, on average, for men convicted of murder or rape than for those convicted of burglary or drug offenses.
(Based on Dabbs, Carr, Frady, & Riad, 1995)
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 369

Figure 12.10  ​Stimuli to measure people’s


ability to identify emotion
For each of six emotions, researchers prepared
views ranging from 0% to 100% expression of
the emotion. In this case, the emotion is anger.
Women identified the expression more quickly, Testosterone (51%)
on average, after a placebo injection than after
a testosterone injection. (From van Honk, J., &
Schutter, D. J. L. G. “Testosterone reduces con- Placebo (43%)
scious detection of signals serving social correc-
tion,” Psychological Science, 18, 663–667. Used
by permission of Blackwell Publishing.) 0 50 100
Morph change (%)

Stop & Check That is, serotonin’s effects combine with those of testoster-
one, as in the triple imbalance hypothesis mentioned previ-
7. How does testosterone influence emotional and cognitive ously. Serotonin activity is lower in juvenile rodents than
responses to a facial expression of anger? in adults, and aggressive behavior is higher in the juveniles
related areas of the brain. (Taravosh-Lahn, Bastida, & Delville, 2006). Serotonin re-
ANSWER lease is also below average in highly aggressive hamsters
consciously but increases the responses in emotion-
7. It decreases the ability to recognize the expression (Cervantes & Delville, 2009).
In a fascinating study, investigators measured 5-HIAA
levels in 2-year-old male monkeys living in a natural environ-
ment and then observed their behavior closely. The monkeys
Serotonin Synapses and in the lowest quartile for 5-HIAA, and therefore the lowest
serotonin turnover, were the most aggressive, had the great-
Aggressive Behavior est probability of attacking larger monkeys, and incurred
Several lines of evidence link aggressive behavior to low sero- the most injuries. Most of them died by age 6. In contrast,
tonin release. Let’s examine some of this evidence. monkeys with high serotonin turnover survived (Higley et
al., 1996). Female monkeys with low 5-HIAA levels are also
Nonhuman Animals likely to get injured and die young (Westergaard, Cleveland,
Much of the earliest evidence came from studies on mice. Luigi Trenkle, Lussier, & Higley, 2003).
Valzelli (1973) found that isolating male mice for 4 weeks in- If most monkeys with low turnover die young, why hasn’t
creased their aggressive behavior and decreased their serotonin natural selection eliminated the genes for low serotonin turn-
turnover. When neurons release serotonin, they reabsorb most over? One possibility is that evolution selects for an inter-
of it and synthesize enough to replace the amount that washed mediate amount of aggression and anxiety (Trefilov, Berard,
away. Thus, the amount present in neurons remains fairly con- Krawczak, & Schmidtke, 2000). The most fearless animals
stant, and if we examine that amount, we have little idea how get into fights and die young, but those with too much fear
much the neurons have been releasing. However, if we measure have other problems. We could say the same about humans:
the serotonin metabolites in body fluids, we gauge the turnover, People with too little fear take excessive risks—wrestling al-
which is the amount that neurons released and replaced. Re- ligators, bungee jumping with a frayed cord, things like that.
searchers estimate serotonin turnover from the concentration of Those with too much fear are withdrawn and unlikely to suc-
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), serotonin’s main me- ceed (Nettle, 2006).
tabolite, in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Measuring the amount We can also see aggressiveness as a high-risk, high-payoff
in the blood or urine is a simpler but less accurate alternative. strategy: A monkey with low 5-HIAA starts many fights and
Comparing different genetic strains of mice, Valzelli and probably dies young. However, a monkey who wins enough
his colleagues found that social isolation lowered serotonin of those fights survives and achieves a dominant status within
turnover by the greatest amount in the genetic strains that the group (Howell et al., 2007). In female monkeys, too, those
reacted with the greatest amount of fighting after social iso- with low 5-HIAA levels tend to achieve higher status in the
lation (Valzelli & Bernasconi, 1979). Social isolation does troop (Riddick et al., 2009). Under some circumstances, tak-
not decrease serotonin turnover in female mice in any ge- ing aggressive risks to achieve a dominant status might be a
netic strain, and it does not make the females aggressive. reasonable gamble.
370 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Stop & Check Linnoila, 1989; Virkkunen, Eggert, Rawlings, & Linnoila,
1996). However, although each of these relationships is sta-
8. If we want to know how much serotonin the brain has tistically reliable, the effects are not sufficiently powerful that
been releasing, what should we measure? we could use blood tests to make important decisions about
9. Given that monkeys with low serotonin turnover pick many individuals, such as which prisoners should be eligible for
fights and in most cases die young, what keeps natural parole. Furthermore, although the research points to a rela-
selection from eliminating the genes for low serotonin tionship between low serotonin turnover and highly violent
turnover? criminal or suicidal acts, the results are less clear for aggressive
behavior in the normal population (where extreme violence
ANSWERS
a low status. is uncommon). In the normal population, studies often find
serotonin turnover survive, but at the cost of accepting a weak relationship, and sometimes a relationship in the op-
and to reproduce more frequently. Monkeys with high posite direction—that is, somewhat less aggression by people
status that enables them to get more of the food
with low 5-HIAA (Coccaro & Lee, 2010).
It is possible to alter serotonin synthesis by changes in diet.
die young, many of the survivors achieve a dominant

Neurons synthesize serotonin from tryptophan, an amino


9. Although most monkeys with low serotonin turnover
been released and presumably resynthesized.
body fluids. The more 5-HIAA, the more serotonin has acid found in small amounts in proteins. Tryptophan crosses
rotonin metabolite, in the cerebrospinal fluid or other the blood–brain barrier by an active transport channel that it
8. We can measure the concentration of 5-HIAA, a se- shares with phenylalanine and other large amino acids. Thus,
a diet high in other amino acids impairs the brain’s ability to
synthesize serotonin. One study found that many young men
on such a diet showed an increase in aggressive behavior a few
Humans hours after eating (Moeller et al., 1996). Considering these
Many studies have found low serotonin turnover in people results, it would seem prudent for anyone with aggressive or
with a history of violent behavior, including people con- suicidal tendencies to reduce consumption of aspartame (Nu-
victed of arson and other violent crimes (Virkkunen, Nuu- traSweet, which is 50% phenylalanine) and maize (American
tila, Goodwin, & Linnoila, 1987) and people who attempt corn), which is high in phenylalanine and low in tryptophan
suicide by violent means, as illustrated in Figure 12.11 (Lytle, Messing, Fisher, & Phebus, 1975).
(G. L. Brown et al., 1982; Edman, Åsberg, Levander, & Schal- Much of the variation in serotonin activity, and therefore
ling, 1986; Mann, Arango, & Underwood, 1990; Pandey et violence, relates to genetics. People vary in the gene that con-
al., 1995; Roy, DeJong, & Linnoila, 1989; Sher et al., 2006; trols tryptophan hydroxylase, the enzyme that converts tryp-
Spreux-Varoquaux et al., 2001). Follow-up studies on people tophan into serotonin. People with less active forms of this
released from prison have found that those with lower sero- enzyme are more likely than average to report frequent an-
tonin turnover had a greater probability of further convictions ger and aggression (Hennig, Reuter, Netter, Burk, & Landt,
for violent crimes (Virkkunen, DeJong, Bartko, Goodwin, & 2005; Rujescu et al., 2002) and more likely to make violent
suicide attempts (Abbar et al., 2001).

How Do We Explain Serotonin Effects?


If some treatment suddenly lowered your serotonin level,
would you at once become violent? When researchers have
5–HIAA in picomoles per ml

100
used drugs or diet to suppress serotonin levels, some people
felt depressed, others became more aggressive or impulsive,
and those with previous drug problems reported a craving
for drugs (Kaplan, Muldoon, Manuck, & Mann, 1997; Van
der Does, 2001; S. N. Young & Leyton, 2002). In short, se-
rotonin’s role is not specific to aggression. A better hypothesis
is that high levels of serotonin inhibit a variety of impulses,
and low levels remove inhibitions. Then the resulting behav-
Never Attempted Attempted ior depends on what had been inhibited, which varies from
attempted once again one person to another. In an interesting study that illustrated
suicide inhibition, people ate a diet rich in other amino acids but lack-
ing tryptophan. That diet temporarily reduced their serotonin
Figure 12.11  ​Levels of 5-HIAA in the CSF of depressed
levels. Then they had to learn a response to avoid a loud buzz-
people
ing noise and a loss of money. People low in serotonin learned
Measurements for the suicide-attempting groups were taken after
the first attempt. Low levels of 5-HIAA indicate low serotonin turn-
the response, but they differed from the placebo group in this
over. (Based on results of Roy, DeJong, & Linnoila, 1989) regard: Ordinarily, when people receive punishments, such as
a loss of money, they become inhibited. They become inactive
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 371

or slow to respond in a variety of situations. The people low


in serotonin failed to show that kind of generalized inhibition
(Crockett, Clark, & Robbins, 2009).
The serotonin-aggression relationship is complex in an-
other way also: Although most studies imply that serotonin
inhibits aggressive behavior, the brain releases serotonin dur-
ing aggressive behavior (van der Vegt et al., 2003). Apparently,
a low level of serotonin activity prior to aggravation magnifies
the response when serotonin is suddenly released at the start
of an aggressive encounter (Nelson & Trainor, 2007).

Stop & Check


10. What change in diet can alter the production of sero-
tonin?

brain.
ANSWER
acids that compete with tryptophan for entry to the
proteins high in phenylalanine and other large amino
sumption of tryptophan or decrease consumption of
10. To raise production of serotonin, increase con-

Fear and Anxiety


Joe McBride/Getty Images

What is the “right” amount of anxiety? It depends. If you are


sitting with family or friends at a restaurant, a low level of
anxiety is appropriate. If you are walking alone at night and
you hear footsteps approaching you, it is time to turn up your
anxiety level. People’s choices of activities depend in part on how easily they
Nevertheless, even among people in the same situation, develop anxiety.
some show much more anxiety than others do, partly for ge-
netic reasons (Aleman, Swart, & van Rijn, 2008; Chen et al.,
2006; Lonsdorf et al., 2009, 2010; Wray et al., 2009). Both
experiences and genetics modify activity in the amygdala, one post-traumatic stress disorder, who are certainly known for
of the main areas for regulating anxiety. their intense anxiety, show a much enhanced startle reflex
(Grillon, Morgan, Davis, & Southwick, 1998).
Fear, Anxiety, and the Amygdala
Do we have any built-in, unlearned fears? Yes, at least one: Studies of Rodents
Even newborns are frightened by loud noises. The response Psychologists measure variations in the startle reflex as a gauge
to an unexpected loud noise, known as the startle reflex, is of fear or anxiety. In research with nonhumans, psychologists
extremely fast: Auditory information goes first to the cochlear first measure the normal response to a loud noise. Then they re-
nucleus in the medulla and from there directly to an area in peatedly pair a stimulus, such as a light, with shock. Finally, they
the pons that commands tensing the muscles, especially the present the light just before the loud noise and determine how
neck muscles. Tensing the neck muscles is important because much the light increases the startle response. A control group
the neck is so vulnerable to injury. (Chapter 5 discussed how is tested with a stimulus that has not been paired with shock.
woodpeckers protect their necks while pecking a tree.) Infor- Results of such studies consistently show that after animals
mation reaches the pons within 3 to 8 ms after a loud noise, have learned to associate a stimulus with shock, that stimulus
and the full startle reflex occurs in less than two tenths of a becomes a fear signal, and presenting the fear signal just before
second (Yeomans & Frankland, 1996). a sudden loud noise enhances the startle response. Conversely,
Although you don’t have to learn to fear loud noises, your a stimulus previously associated with pleasant stimuli or the
current mood or situation modifies your reaction. Your startle absence of danger becomes a safety signal that decreases the
reflex is more vigorous if you are already tense. People with startle reflex (Schmid, Koch, & Schnitzler, 1995).
372 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Investigators have determined that the


amygdala (Figures 12.7 and 12.12) is impor-
tant for enhancing the startle reflex. Many cells Thalamus
in the amygdala, especially in the basolateral
and central nuclei, get input from pain fibers
as well as vision or hearing, so the circuitry is
well suited to establishing conditioned fears
(Uwano, Nishijo, Ono, & Tamura, 1995).
Some cells in the amygdala respond strongly to
rewards, others to punishments, and still oth-
ers to surprises in either direction (Belova, Pa-
ton, Morrison, & Salzman, 2007).
Visual
Output from the amygdala to the hypo- Auditory cortex O O cortex
thalamus controls autonomic fear responses,
such as increased blood pressure. The amygdala Basolateral amygdala
also has axons to areas of the prefrontal cortex Midbrain
Central amygdala
that control approach and avoidance responses Pons
(Garcia, Vouimba, Baudry, & Thompson, 1999;
Lacroix, Spinelli, Heidbreder, & Feldon, 2000).
Medulla
Additional axons extend to midbrain areas Spinal cord
that relay information to the pons to control
the startle reflex (LeDoux, Iwata, Cicchetti, & Figure 12.12  ​Amygdala and learned fears
Reis, 1988; Zhao & Davis, 2004). Figure 12.12 The central amygdala receives sensory input from the lateral and basolateral amyg-
shows the connections. dala. It sends output to the central gray area of the midbrain, which relays informa-
If a rat has damage to the amygdala, it still tion to a nucleus in the pons responsible for the startle reflex. Damage anywhere
shows a normal startle reflex, but signals before along the route from amygdala to pons interferes with learned fears that modify the
the noise do not modify the reflex. In one study, startle reflex. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
rats were repeatedly exposed to a light followed
by shock and then tested for their responses to a loud noise. is true for humans. If you are attacked or if you have other trau-
Intact rats showed a moderate startle reflex to the loud noise matic experiences, you become more fearful in a wide variety of
and an enhanced response if the light preceded the noise. In situations. It is as if your brain has decided, “This is a dangerous
contrast, rats with damage in the path from the amygdala to world. I need to be alert for new threats.” This long-term, gener-
the hindbrain showed the same startle reflex with or without alized emotional arousal depends on a brain area called the bed
the light (Hitchcock & Davis, 1991). nucleus of the stria terminalis (Duvarci, Bauer, & Paré, 2009;
Do these results indicate that amygdala damage destroys Toufexis, 2007). The stria terminalis is a set of axons that con-
fear? Not necessarily. An alternative explanation is that the nect this nucleus to the amygdala, as shown in Figure 12.13.
rats have trouble interpreting or understanding stimuli with
emotional consequences. The same issue arises with humans, Bed nucleus of Corpus callosum
as we shall see. stria terminalis
An odd parasite has evolved a way to exploit the conse- Fornix
quences of amygdala damage (Berdoy, Webster, & Macdon-
ald, 2000). Toxoplasma gondii is a protozoan that infects many
mammals but reproduces only in cats. Cats excrete the para-
site’s eggs in their feces, thereby releasing them into the ground.
Rats that burrow in the ground can become infected with the Olfactory bulb
parasite. When the parasite enters a rat, it migrates to the brain
where it apparently damages the amygdala. The rat then fear-
lessly approaches a cat, guaranteeing that the cat will eat the rat Stria teminalis
and that the parasite will find its way back into a cat!
The amygdala is important for learning what to fear (Anto- Amygdala Hippocampus
niadis, Winslow, Davis, & Amaral, 2007; Kwon & Choi, 2009;
Figure 12.13  ​The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis
Wilensky, Schafe, Kristensen, & LeDoux, 2006). That is not
The bed nucleus is critical for long-term adjustments of anxiety,
the only type of fear conditioning. If a rat has received shocks
whereas the amygdala is responsible for fear of individual items.
after a particular stimulus in a particular cage, it learns to fear The stria terminalis is a set of axons connecting its bed nucleus
the stimulus (by changes in the amygdala) but it also learns to to the amygdala. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
fear the cage… and new cages… and new situations. The same
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 373

Stop & Check sion unless the person is afraid of you! Consequently, you
recognize an angry expression faster if it is directed toward
11. What brain mechanism enables the startle reflex to be so you and a fearful expression faster if it is directed to the side
fast? (Adams & Kleck, 2005). The amygdala, however, responds
12. How could a researcher use the startle reflex to deter- more strongly to a fearful face directed toward you (Adams
mine whether some stimulus causes fear? et al., 2003) (Figure 12.14). That is, the amygdala responds
more strongly to the expression that is harder to interpret.
ANSWERS
yond its usual level, then the stimulus produced fear. Presumably the arousal indicates that it is working harder
noise. If the stimulus increases the startle reflex be- to make sense of the stimulus.
muscles. 12. Present the stimulus before giving a loud
to cells in the pons that trigger a tensing of neck
11. Loud noises activate a path from the cochlea Individual Differences in Amygdala
Response and Anxiety
Most people’s tendency toward anxiety generally remains
Studies of Monkeys fairly consistent over time. Most infants with an “inhibited”
temperament develop into shy, fearful children and then into
The effect of amygdala damage in monkeys was described in shy adults who show an enhanced amygdala response to the
classic studies early in the 1900s and is known as the Klüver- sight of any unfamiliar face (Beaton et al., 2008; Schwartz,
Bucy syndrome, from the names of the primary investigators. Wright, Shin, Kagan, & Rauch, 2003). One study found a
Monkeys showing this syndrome are tame and placid. They strong relationship between amygdala activation and fearful-
attempt to pick up lighted matches and other objects that they ness. College students carried a beeper for 28 days. It beeped
ordinarily avoid. They display less than the normal fear of at unpredictable times each day, calling for the student to re-
snakes or of larger, more dominant monkeys (Kalin, Shelton, cord his or her emotional state at the moment. A year later
Davidson, & Kelley, 2001). the students came into a laboratory for the second part of the
However, not all monkeys with amygdala damage react study, in which an fMRI recorded their amygdala response to
with the full Klüver-Bucy syndrome. The most prominent very brief presentations of frightening pictures. The amygdala
effect is an alteration of monkeys’ social behaviors, although responses correlated highly with the number of unpleasant
the exact results vary depending on the age of the monkeys, emotions they had recorded the previous year (Barrett, Bliss-
the social situation, and the exact location of the damage. Moreau, Duncan, Rauch, & Wright, 2007). Presumably they
Some monkeys with damage to the amygdala are withdrawn recorded so many unpleasant emotions because they were bio-
and fearful, but others are friendly and fearless (Bauman, logically predisposed to react strongly.
Toscano, Mason, Lavenex, & Amaral, 2006; Emery et al., In a study of Israeli soldiers, researchers first measured their
2001; Kalin, Shelton, & Davidson, 2004; Machado et al., amygdala responses to briefly flashed unpleasant photos, at the
2008; Rosvold, Mirsky, & Pribram, 1954). Among intact
monkeys, those with a more vigorously reactive amygdala
tend to show the greatest fear in response to a noise or an
intruder (Oler et al., 2010).

Response of the Human Amygdala


0.16
to Visual Stimuli 0.14
% Signal change

Studies using fMRI show that the human amygdala responds 0.12
strongly when people look at photos that arouse fear or pho- 0.1
tos of faces showing fear. To a lesser extent it also responds to 0.08
faces showing happiness or sadness (Fusar-Poli et al., 2009). 0.06
Instructing people to pay attention to pleasant stimuli in- 0.04
creases the amygdala’s responses to them (Cunningham, Van 0.02
Bavel & Johnsen, 2008). 0
Contrary to what we might guess, the amygdala re- Anger Anger Fear Fear
sponds most strongly when a facial expression is a bit am- direct averted direct averted
biguous or difficult to interpret. Consider angry and fright- Figure 12.14  ​Amygdala response and direction of gaze
ened faces. As a rule, it is easy to interpret an angry face The amygdala responds more strongly to an angry face directed
looking straight at you, but a fearful face looking straight away from the viewer and to a frightened face directed toward the
at you is more puzzling. Frightened people almost always viewer. (From Adams, R. B. et al. “Effects of gaze on amygdala sensi-
stare at whatever is frightening them, and so you will al- tivity to anger and fear faces,” Science, 2003, 300:1536. Reprinted
most never see someone stare at you with a fearful expres- with permission from AAAS/Science Magazine.)
374 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

time of the soldiers’ induction into the army. Later they mea- 1
sured the soldiers’ responses to combat stress. Those with the High support group
greatest amygdala response at the start reported the greatest

Mean blink amplitude


0.8
amount of combat stress (Admon et al., 2009). Again, it ap-
pears that amygdala response is closely related to fear reactivity. 0.6
Fear reactivity, in turn, affects much of life—even, accord- Low support group
ing to one study, political attitudes. People were asked a series 0.4
of questions about their support for use of military force, po- Note how responses decline
0.2 after repetition of the sound.
lice powers, the death penalty, gun ownership, and so forth.
Researchers also measured each person’s responses to sudden
0
loud noises, repeated numerous times. As shown in Figure 1–3 2–4 3–5 4–6 5–7
12.15, those showing high support for military and police Three-event clusters of startle stimuli
action showed a greater startle response to the loud noises,
which we know indicates amygdala activity (Oxley et al., Figure 12.15  ​Fear responses and political attitudes
2008). The interpretation is that people with a highly reactive On average, people who show a stronger startle response to loud
amygdala are likely to perceive dangers, and therefore to sup- noises tend to favor greater reliance on military and police pow-
port strong protection against those dangers. (This relation- ers. (From Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing,
ship, of course, says nothing about whether the high support M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Political attitudes vary
or low support group is correct. It just indicates that when we with physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667–1670. Reprinted by
permission from the American Association for the Advancement of
are arguing about policy, our brain physiology influences our
Science.)
decision, just as facts and logic do.)

Stop & Check


It is possible to study damage limited to the amygdala only
13. What evidence indicates that amygdala activity corre- in people with the rare genetic disorder Urbach-Wiethe disease.
sponds to the effort needed for interpreting emotional They suffer skin lesions, and many also accumulate calcium
information? in the amygdala until it wastes away. Much of the research on
this condition deals with a woman known by her initials, SM.
14. What can we predict about someone if we know the
SM describes herself as fearless, and she certainly acts that
strength of that person’s amygdala responses to upset-
way. When she viewed 10 clips from the scariest movies the
ting pictures or loud noises?
researchers could find, she reported feeling only excitement, no
to favor strong reliance on military and police power. fear. Researchers took her to an exotic pet store. In spite of in-
ANSWERS
show strong responses to stressful experiences, and sisting that she hates snakes and spiders, she was happy to hold
many negative emotional experiences during a day, to a snake (Figure 12.16), and the staff repeatedly had to restrain
with a highly reactive amygdala are likely to report her from touching or poking the tarantulas and venomous
derstand a fearful face looking to the side. 14. People snakes they had. When the researchers took her to a “haunted
looking to the side. People usually find it easier to un- house,” she led the way without hesitation, venturing down
face directed at the viewer, rather than a similar face
dark hallways. When people dressed as monsters jumped out,
other people in the group screamed, but SM laughed, started
13. The amygdala responds more strongly to a fearful

poking one of the monsters out of curiosity, and scared the


monster! Her fearlessness is dangerous to her. She has been
held up at gun point and knife point and has been physically
Damage to the Human Amygdala assaulted repeatedly. Evidently she plunges into dangerous sit-
With laboratory animals, researchers can intentionally dam- uations without the caution other people would show. When
age the amygdala to see the effects. With humans, they have to she describes these events, she remembers feeling angry, but
rely on damage that occurs spontaneously. For example, some not afraid (Feinstein, Adolphs, Damasio, & Tranel, 2011).
people suffer a stroke that damages the amygdala and sur- Here is another example of her fearlessness: Suppose you
rounding areas, at least in one hemisphere. They are impaired are standing, and a person you don’t know approaches you,
in some ways and not others. When they examine emotional face to face. How close could that person come before you
pictures, they can classify them as pleasant vs. unpleasant began to feel uncomfortable? Most people stand about 0.7 m
about as well as anyone else. However, they experience little (2 feet) away from another person, but SM’s preferred distance
arousal from viewing unpleasant pictures (Berntson, Bechara, is about half that. When a man unknown to her, instructed by
Damasio, Tranel, & Cacioppo, 2007). That is, they continue the experimenters, approached her so close that their noses
to experience the cognitive aspect of unpleasant emotions, but touched, with eye-to-eye contact, she showed and reported no
not the feeling aspect. discomfort (Kennedy, Gläscher, Tyszka, & Adolphs, 2009).
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 375

Happy Sad

Figure 12.16  ​SM, a woman with amygdala damage, holds a


snake at an exotic pet store
Surprised Disgusted
Although she said she hates snakes, she was curious to hold this
one and wanted to touch the others, including venomous ones.
(From Feinstein, J. S., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., & Tranel, D. (2011).
The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear.
Current Biology, 21, 34–38 with permission from Elsevier)

SM and other people with Urbach-Wiethe disease of-


ten fail to recognize the emotional expressions in faces, es- Angry Afraid
pecially expressions of fear or disgust (Boucsein, Weniger, Figure 12.17  ​Drawings by SM, who has a damaged
Mursch, Steinhoff, & Irle, 2001). Even when they recognize amygdala
an expression as fear or disgust, they rate it as less intense She at first declined to draw a fearful expression because, she
than other people do, and they are less likely than average said, she could not imagine it. When urged to try, she remem-
to remember a photo of an emotional expression if they see bered that frightened people are often depicted with their hair on
the same photo an hour later (Siebert, Markowitsch, & Bar- end, at least in cartoons. (From “Fear and the human amygdala,”
tel, 2003). by R. Adolphs, D. Tranel, H. Damasio, and A. Damasio, Journal of
When SM was asked to draw faces showing certain emo- Neuroscience, 15, pp. 5879–5891. Copyright © 1995 by Oxford
tions (Figure 12.17), she made good drawings of most expres- University Press. Reprinted by permission.)
sions but had trouble drawing a fearful expression, saying that
she did not know what such a face would look like. When the
researcher urged her to try, she drew someone crawling away
with hair on end, as cartoonists often indicate fear (Adolphs, tract her attention the way they do for other people (Kennedy
Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1995). & Adolphs, 2010). When researchers asked her to look at the
Why do SM and others with amygdala damage have eyes, she quickly recognized fearful expressions (Adolphs et
trouble identifying facial expressions of fear? At first, the as- al., 2005). Seeing the eyes is particularly important for recog-
sumption was that someone with amygdala damage doesn’t nizing fear. People express happiness with the mouth, but fear
feel fear and therefore can’t understand it in others. But then mainly with the eyes (Morris, deBonis, & Dolan, 2002; Vuil-
Ralph Adolphs and his colleagues observed that SM focuses leumier, 2005). Figure 12.18 shows only the whites of the eyes
almost entirely on the nose and mouth of each photograph. of people expressing fear (left) and happiness (right). Most
Also, in everyday life, she seldom makes eye contact, looking people recognize the fear expression from the eyes alone, but
at the mouth instead (Spezio, Huang, Castelli, & Adolphs, not the happy expression (Whalen et al., 2004).
2007). Suppose you are looking at a computer screen, and a These observations suggest an alternative interpretation
face is flashed briefly on the screen, located such that your eyes of the function of the amygdala. Instead of being responsible
are fixated on the mouth. Almost instantaneously, you would for feeling fear or other emotions, perhaps it is responsible for
move your gaze to focus on the eyes, and you would be espe- detecting emotional information and directing other brain ar-
cially impelled to do so if you saw at first glance that the face eas to pay attention to it in the proper way. The distinction
was showing fear (Gamer & Büchel, 2009). She has no reluc- between these interpretations is difficult to test. As is often
tance to make eye contact, but someone’s eyes simply don’t at- the case, good research points the way for further research.
376 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

0.5 Windhaber, & Katschnig, 2003). Twin studies suggest a ge-


Fear
Happy netic predisposition, although no single gene has been identified
0.4 (Hettema, Neale, & Kendler, 2001; Kim, Lee, Yang, Hwang,
& Yoon, 2009). Curiously, panic disorder occurs in about 15%

% Signal change
of people with joint laxity, commonly known as being “double-
0.3
jointed” (able to bend the fingers backward farther than usual).
Even when people with joint laxity do not have panic disorder,

Science Magazine/AAAS
0.2 they tend to have stronger fears than most other people do
(Bulbena et al., 2004; Bulbena, Gago, Sperry, & Bergé, 2006).
0.1 The research so far links panic disorder to some abnor-
malities in the hypothalamus and not necessarily the amyg-
dala. Panic disorder is associated with decreased activity of
0
Fear Happy Eye whites the neurotransmitter GABA and increased levels of orexin.
Orexin, as discussed in Chapters 9 and 10, is associated with
maintaining wakefulness and activity. We might not have
guessed that it would also be associated with anxiety, but
Figure 12.18  ​Eye expressions for fear and happiness
The eye whites alone enable most people to guess that the
apparently it is, and drugs that block orexin receptors block
person on the left was feeling afraid. (From “Human amygdala panic responses ( Johnson et al., 2010).
responsivity to masked fearful eye whites,” by P. J. Whalen et al.
(2004). Science, 306, 2061. Reprinted by permission from AAAS/
Science magazine.)
Pharmacological Relief from Anxiety
People with excessive anxiety sometimes seek relief through
medications. A variety of studies indicate that anxiety is in-
Ralph Adolphs creased by the transmitters orexin and CCK (cholecystoki-
Will a better understanding of the social nin) in the amygdala or hippocampus (C. Becker et al., 2001;
brain lead to a better understanding of Frankland, Josselyn, Bradwejn, Vaccarino, & Yeomans, 1997).
social behavior? And can such knowledge So far, no drugs based on orexin or CCK have been approved.
ultimately be used to help our species However, many drugs are available to increase activity of the
negotiate and survive in the vastly com- transmitter GABA, which inhibits anxiety.
Ralph Adolphs

plex social world it has helped create? To The most common anti-anxiety drugs (“anxiolytic drugs”)
approach such questions, social neuro- are the benzodiazepines (BEN-zo-die-AZ-uh-peens), such
scientists will need to establish dialogues as diazepam (trade name Valium), chlordiazepoxide (Lib-
with other disciplines in the social and behavioral sciences, rium), and alprazolam (Xanax). Benzodiazepines bind to the
and to be highly sensitive to the public consequences of the GABAA receptor, which includes a site that binds GABA as
data they generate. (Adolphs, personal communication) well as sites that modify the sensitivity of the GABA site (Fig-
ure 12.19). (The brain also has other kinds of GABA recep-
tors, such as GABAB, with different behavioral effects.)
Stop & Check At the center of the GABAA receptor is a chloride chan-
nel. When open, it permits chloride ions (Cl–) to cross the
15. Why do people with amygdala damage have trouble recog- membrane into the neuron, hyperpolarizing the cell. (That is,
nizing expressions of fear? the synapse is inhibitory.) Surrounding the chloride channel
pressions of fear depend almost entirely on the eyes. are four units, each containing one or more sites sensitive to
ANSWER GABA. Benzodiazepines bind to additional sites on three of
15. They focus their vision on the nose and mouth. Ex-
those four units (labeled a in Figure 12.19). When a benzo-
diazepine molecule attaches, it neither opens nor closes the
chloride channel but twists the receptor so that the GABA
Anxiety Disorders binds more easily (Macdonald, Weddle, & Gross, 1986). Ben-
zodiazepines thus facilitate the effects of GABA.
Most psychological disorders include increased anxiety as one Benzodiazepines exert their anti-anxiety effects in the
of the symptoms. In generalized anxiety disorder, phobia, and amygdala, hypothalamus, midbrain, and several other areas. A
panic disorder, the only major symptom is increased anxiety. minute amount of benzodiazepines injected directly into a rat’s
Panic disorder is characterized by frequent periods of anxiety amygdala decreases learned shock-avoidance behaviors (Pesold
and occasional attacks of rapid breathing, increased heart rate, & Treit, 1995), relaxes the muscles, and increases social ap-
sweating, and trembling—that is, extreme arousal of the sym- proaches to other rats (S. K. Sanders & Shekhar, 1995). Ben-
pathetic nervous system. It is more common in women than in zodiazepines also decrease the responses in a rat’s brain to the
men and far more common in adolescents and young adults smell of a cat. Ordinarily, that smell triggers an apparently built-
than in older adults (Shen et al., 2007; Swoboda, Amering, in fear (McGregor, Hargreaves, Apfelbach, & Hunt, 2004).
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 377

Receptors for
benzodiazepines

α α α
α β
α β α β GABA receptor

α GABA molecule

Benzodiazepine
molecule

Neuron membrane

Cl– Cl–

GABA receptor Cross-section Benzodiazepines facilitate


GABA binding. GABA
twists the receptor to
open a chloride channel.

Figure 12.19  ​The GABAA receptor complex


Of its four receptor sites sensitive to GABA, the three a sites are also sensitive to benzodiazepines. (Based on Guidotti, Ferrero, Fujimoto,
Santi, & Costa, 1986)

Benzodiazepines produce a variety of additional effects,


including the possibility of addiction (Tan et al., 2010).
When they reach the thalamus and cerebral cortex, they
induce sleepiness, block epileptic convulsions, and impair
memory (Rudolph et al., 1999). The mixture of effects is
a problem. For example, you might want to reduce your
anxiety without becoming sleepy, and presumably, you don’t
want to impair your memory. Researchers hope to develop
National Institute of Mental Health

drugs with more specific and limited effects (Korpi & Sink-
konen, 2006).

Applications and Extensions


Figure 12.20  ​Two rats that were given the same
Alcohol as an Anxiety Reducer amount of alcohol
The rat on the right was later given the experimental drug
Alcohol promotes the flow of chloride ions through the Ro15-4513. Within 2 minutes, its performance and coordina-
GABAA receptor complex by binding strongly at a special tion improved significantly.
site found on only certain kinds of GABAA receptors
(Glykys et al., 2007). Alcohol influences the brain in
other ways as well, but the effects on GABA are respon-   Could Ro15-4513 be useful as a “sobering-up” pill or
sible for alcohol’s anti-anxiety and intoxicating effects. as a treatment to help people who want to stop drinking
Drugs that block the effects of alcohol on the GABAA alcohol? Hoffman-LaRoche, the company that discovered
receptor complex also block most of alcohol’s behav- it, concluded that the drug would be too risky. People who
ioral effects. One experimental drug, known as Ro15- relied on the pill might think they were sober and try to
4513, is particularly effective in this regard (Suzdak et drive home when they were still impaired. Furthermore,
al., 1986). Besides affecting the GABAA receptor com- giving such a pill to alcoholics would probably backfire.
plex, Ro15-4513 blocks the effects of alcohol on motor Because alcoholics drink to get drunk, a pill that de-
coordination, its depressant action on the brain, and its creased their feeling of intoxication would probably in-
ability to reduce anxiety (H. C. Becker, 1988; Hoffman, crease their drinking. Ro15-4513 reduces but does not
Tabakoff, Szabó, Suzdak, & Paul, 1987; Ticku & Kulkarni, eliminate alcohol’s effects, especially with large amounts
1988) (Figure 12.20). of alcohol (Poling, Schlinger, & Blakely, 1988).  ■
378 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Stop & Check This process has been demonstrated for both rats (Monfils,
Cowansage, Klann, & LeDoux, 2009) and humans. Here is the
16. What would be the effect of benzodiazepines on someone human study (Schiller et al., 2010): Imagine you watch as a se-
who had no GABA? ries of red squares and blue squares appear. When you see a red
benzodiazepines.
square, nothing happens, but when you see a blue square, 38%
ANSWER of the time you receive a mildly painful shock. Before long, you
so a person without GABA would have no response to
16. Benzodiazepines facilitate the effects of GABA, show distinct signs of anxiety at the sight of that blue square. A
day later, you return to the laboratory and you see a blue square,
just once, with no shock, just enough to give you a strong re-
minder of the experience. Later you undergo extinction train-
ing, with many presentations of both the red and blue squares,
Relearning as Relief from Anxiety without shock. If you get this extinction training about 10 min-
To the extent that anti-anxiety drugs provide relief, the relief is utes after the reminder, it is highly effective, and your learned
temporary. If you have a long-term problem of excessive anxiety, fear virtually disappears, long term. If you receive the extinction
you probably shouldn’t try to solve it with daily benzodiazepines. training 6 hours after the reminder, or after no reminder, the ex-
If your fear is based on a particular traumatic experience, an al- tinction suppresses the fear temporarily, but it may return later.
ternative is to try to extinguish the learned fear. Suppose, for sake A related approach uses propanolol, a drug that interferes
of illustration, that you once almost drowned in the ocean, and with protein synthesis at certain synapses in the amygdala. Sup-
now you are terribly afraid to go near it. A reasonable approach pose you learn a fear of some stimulus. Later you are exposed to
is to expose you to your feared object, perhaps a little at a time, that stimulus under the influence of propanolol. Exposure awak-
in hopes of extinction (in the classical-conditioning sense). First ens the memory and makes the memory trace labile, but propa-
you wade through a puddle, then set foot into a pond, then a big- nolol evidently blocks the reconsolidation. The result is a much
ger pond, and you work up through lakes until you are ready to weaker emotional response, although you can still describe the
face the ocean. Clinical psychologists generally use that approach experience in words (Kindt, Soeter, & Vervliet, 2009). Psychia-
to relieve phobias, with good success. The problem is, extinction trists have successfully applied this method to post-traumatic
training suppresses original learning or overhangs it with new stress disorder by asking people to describe their traumatic ex-
learning, but does not eliminate it. Young children sometimes perience under the influence of propanolol. The result was a per-
fully extinguish a learned reaction, but adults seldom do, and the sisting decrease in fear intensity (Brunet et al., 2008).
original fear might return, especially after a time of stress (Go-
golla, Caroni, Lüthi, & Herry, 2009).
How could we extinguish a learned fear more fully? In gen-
Stop & Check
eral, it is easier to extinguish a learned response immediately
after original learning than it is later. After time has passed, the 17. Why is extinction more effective a few minutes after a
learning becomes stronger. Psychologists say it has consolidated. brief reminder of the original learning?
Ordinarily, if you have a traumatic experience, no one is there to
extinguish the learning in the next few minutes. However, if an
solidated or extinguished.
ANSWER
event strongly revives the original experience, that connection
learning into a labile state from which it can be recon-
17. The reminder brings the representation of the
again becomes temporarily labile (unconsolidated) and avail-
able for either reconsolidation or highly effective extinction.

Module 12.2  ■  In Closing

Doing Something About Emotions

It is hard to foresee future developments, but suppose research- And what about anxiety? Suppose research enables us to
ers make sudden advances in linking emotional behaviors to modulate people’s anxiety precisely without undesirable side
physiological measurements. Imagine if we could take a blood effects. Would it be a good idea to use these methods to as-
sample—measuring 5-HIAA or whatever—plus an fMRI scan sure that everyone had the “right” anxiety level—not too
and a few other measurements and then predict which people much, not too little? Future research will give us new options
will commit violent crime. What would we want to do with that and opportunities. Deciding what to do with them is another
information, if anything? matter.
12.2 Attack and Escape Behaviors 379

summary 7. According to studies using fMRI, the human amygdala


responds strongly to fear stimuli and any other stimuli
1. An experience that gradually provokes an attack leaves the
that evoke strong emotional processing. It responds
individual more ready than usual to attack again.  366
most strongly when the processing is effortful.  373
2. Aggressive behavior relates to both genetic and environ-
8. People with damage to the amygdala fail to focus their
mental influences. Some studies indicate that one gene
attention on stimuli with important emotional content.
increases aggressive behavior mainly among people who
One woman with damage limited to the amygdala
had abusive experiences in childhood.  367
seems almost entirely fearless.  374
3. Differences in testosterone levels correlate weakly with
9. Damage to the amygdala impairs recognition of fear
variations in aggressive behavior. Aggressive behavior
expressions largely because of lack of attention to the
depends on a combination of chemicals, with testoster-
eyes.  375
one increasing the probability and both cortisol and
serotonin decreasing it.  367 10. Panic disorder is associated with increased orexin
release and decreased GABA release in the hippocam-
4. Low serotonin turnover is associated with an increased
pus.  376
likelihood of impulsive behavior, sometimes including
violence. Monkeys with low serotonin turnover get into 11. Anti-anxiety drugs decrease fear by facilitating the
many fights and in most cases die young. However, binding of the neurotransmitter GABA to the GABAA
those that survive have a high probability of achieving a receptors, especially in the amygdala.  376
dominant status.  369 12. A behavioral approach to reducing anxiety is to
5. Researchers measure enhancement of the startle reflex reawaken a learned fear and then apply extinction
as an indication of anxiety or learned fears.  371 procedures while the memory is in a labile
state.  378
6. The amygdala is critical for increasing or decreasing the
startle reflex on the basis of learned information.  372

Key Terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
bed nucleus of the stria GABAA receptor  376 panic disorder  376
terminalis  372 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid startle reflex  371
benzodiazepines  376 (5-HIAA)  369 turnover  369

Thought Questions
1. Much of the play behavior of a cat can be analyzed 2. People with amygdala damage approach other people
into attack and escape components. Is the same true indiscriminately instead of trying to choose people
for children’s play? who look friendly and trustworthy. What might be a
possible explanation?
Module 12.3

Stress and Health

I
n the early days of scientific medicine, physicians made has argued, many of our crises are prolonged, such as advanc-
little allowance for the relation of personality or emotions ing in a career, paying a mortgage, or caring for a relative with
to health and disease. If someone became ill, the cause had a chronic health problem. If a long-term, almost inescapable
to be structural, like a virus or bacterium. Today, behavioral issue activates the general adaptation syndrome, the result can
medicine emphasizes the effects on health of diet, smoking, be exhaustion.
exercise, stressful experiences, and other behaviors. We accept Selye’s concept of stress included any change in one’s life,
the idea that emotions and other experiences influence people’s such as either getting fired from your job or getting pro-
illnesses and patterns of recovery. This view does not imply moted. Bruce McEwen (2000, p. 173) proposed an alterna-
mind-body dualism. Stress and emotions are brain activities, tive definition that is better for most purposes: “events that
after all. are interpreted as threatening to an individual and which elicit
physiological and behavioral responses.” Although this defi-
nition differs from Selye’s, the idea remains that many kinds
Concepts of Stress of events can be stressful, and the body reacts to all kinds of
The term stress, like the term emotion, is hard to define or stress in similar ways.
quantify. Hans Selye (1979) defined stress as the nonspecific
response of the body to any demand made upon it. When
Selye was in medical school, he noticed that patients with Stress and the Hypothalamus-
a wide variety of illnesses have much in common: They de-
velop a fever, they lose their appetite, they become inactive,
Pituitary-Adrenal Cortex Axis
they are sleepy most of the day, and their immune systems Stress activates two body systems. One is the sympathetic
become more active. Later, when doing laboratory research, nervous system, which prepares the body for brief emergency
he found that rats exposed to heat, cold, pain, confinement, responses—”fight or flight.” The other is the HPA axis—the
or the sight of a cat responded to these dissimilar stimuli in hypothalamus, pituitary gland, and adrenal cortex. Activation
similar ways, including increased heart rate, breathing rate, of the hypothalamus induces the anterior pituitary gland to
and adrenal secretions. Selye inferred that any threat to the secrete adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), which in
body, in addition to its specific effects, activated a general- turn stimulates the human adrenal cortex to secrete cortisol,
ized response to stress, which he called the general adapta- which enhances metabolic activity and elevates blood levels of
tion syndrome. The initial stage, which he called alarm, is sugar and other nutrients (Figure 12.21). Many researchers
characterized by increased activity of the sympathetic ner- refer to cortisol as a “stress hormone” and use measurements
vous system, readying the body for brief emergency activ- of cortisol level as an indication of someone’s recent stress
ity. During the second stage, resistance, the sympathetic re- level. Compared to the autonomic nervous system, the HPA
sponse declines, but the adrenal cortex secretes cortisol and axis reacts more slowly, but it becomes the dominant response
other hormones that enable the body to maintain prolonged to prolonged stressors, such as living with an abusive parent
alertness, fight infections, and heal wounds. After intense, or spouse.
prolonged stress, the body enters the third stage, exhaustion. Stress that releases cortisol helps the body mobilize its
During this stage, the individual is tired, inactive, and vul- energies to fight a difficult situation, but the effects depend
nerable because the nervous system and immune systems no on amount and duration. Brief or moderate stress improves
longer have the energy to sustain their heightened responses attention and memory formation (Krugers, Hoogenraad, &
(Sapolsky, 1998). Groc, 2010). It improves performance on relatively simple
Stress-related illnesses and psychiatric problems are wide- tasks, although it impairs performance that requires complex,
spread in industrial societies, possibly because of changes in flexible thinking (Arnsten, 2009). Stress also enhances activ-
the type of stresses that we face. As Robert Sapolsky (1998) ity of the immune system, helping it fight illnesses (Benschop
380
12.3 Stress and Health 381

■ B cells, which mature mostly in the bone marrow, secrete


Hypothalamus antibodies, which are Y-shaped proteins that attach
to particular kinds of antigens, just as a key fits a lock.
Releasing factor Every cell has surface proteins called antigens (antibody-
generator molecules), and your body’s antigens are as
unique as your fingerprints. The B cells recognize the
Anterior
“self ” antigens, but when they find an unfamiliar antigen,
pituitary
they attack the cell. This kind of attack defends the body
against viruses and bacteria. It also causes rejection of
organ transplants, unless physicians take special steps to
minimize the attack. After the body has made antibodies
ACTH against a particular intruder, it “remembers” the intruder
(through blood)
and quickly builds more of the same kind of antibody if
it encounters that intruder again.
■ T cells mature in the thymus gland. Several kinds of

T cells attack intruders directly (without secreting


Adrenal antibodies), and some help other T cells or B cells to
cortex multiply.
■ Natural killer cells, another kind of leukocytes, attack

tumor cells and cells that are infected with viruses.


Whereas each B or T cell attacks a particular kind of
Cortisol foreign antigen, natural killer cells attack all intruders.
In response to an infection, leukocytes and other cells
Figure 12.21  ​The hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal cortex produce small proteins called cytokines (e.g., interleukin-1,
axis or IL-1) that combat infections and also communicate with
Prolonged stress increases secretion of the adrenal hormone corti- the brain to elicit appropriate behaviors (Maier & Watkins,
sol, which elevates blood sugar and increases metabolism. These 1998). Cytokines are the immune system’s way of telling the
changes help the body sustain prolonged activity but at the expense brain that the body is ill. They trigger the hypothalamus to
of decreased immune system activity. (© Cengage Learning 2013) produce fever, sleepiness, lack of energy, lack of appetite, and
loss of sex drive. The immune system also reacts to infection
by increased production of prostaglandins, additional chemi-
cals that promote sleepiness. In other words, cytokines and
prostaglandins are responsible for what Selye called the gen-
et al., 1995). However, prolonged stress impairs memory and eral adaptation syndrome.
immune activity. To see why, we start with an overview of the Note also that what we usually consider symptoms are
immune system. actually part of the body’s way of fighting the illness. Most
people think of fever and sleepiness as something the illness
did to them, but in fact, fever and sleepiness are strategies
The Immune System that evolved for fighting the illness. As discussed in Chapter
The immune system consists of cells that protect the body 10, a moderate fever helps fight many infections. Sleep and
against viruses, bacteria, and other intruders. The immune inactivity are ways of conserving energy so that the body
system is like a police force: If it is too weak, the “criminals” can devote more energy to its immune attack against the
(viruses and bacteria) run wild and create damage. If it be- intruders.
comes too strong and unselective, it starts attacking “law-abiding
citizens” (the body’s own cells). When the immune system
attacks normal cells, we call the result an autoimmune disease. Stop & Check
Myasthenia gravis and rheumatoid arthritis are examples of
18. What kind of cell releases cytokines?
autoimmune diseases.
19. What behavioral changes do cytokines stimulate?
Leukocytes
The most important elements of the immune system are the ANSWERS
and increased sleepiness.
leukocytes, commonly known as white blood cells (Kiecolt- produce fever, decreased hunger, decreased sex drive,
Glaser & Glaser, 1993; O’Leary, 1990).
release cytokines. 19. Cytokines stimulate neurons to

We distinguish several types of leukocytes, including


18. Leukocytes, which are part of the immune system,

B cells, T cells, and natural killer cells (Figure 12.22):


382 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Bacteria
enter through Antigen
punctured
skin
Natural killer Natural killer cells attack tumors
cell and some other intruders, injecting
Bacteria trigger chemicals that kill them.
an inflammatory
response
Tumor
cell

B Some B cells
B cell attaches
memory differentiate
to a bacterium,
cells into memory cells
leaving the B cell prepared to attack
bacterium's
the same antigen
antigen
exposed

Secretions of cytokines
Helper T cell Helper
causes this T cell
B cell to divide
Plasma cell
Circulating antibodies
attach to the antigen
Some B cells and mark it for destruction
become plasma cells
that secrete antibodies
specific to this antigen

Figure 12.22  ​Immune system responses to a bacterial infection


B cells bind to bacteria and produce antibodies against the bacteria. When a helper T cell attaches to the B cell, it stimulates the B cell
to generate copies of itself, called B memory cells, that immunize the body against future invasions by the same kind of bacteria.
(© Cengage Learning 2013)

symptoms resembling illness, including fever, sleepiness, and


Effects of Stress on the Immune System decreased appetite. The same is true for people who are un-
The nervous system has more control than we might have der great stress (Maier & Watkins, 1998). Many of the symp-
guessed over the immune system. The study of this relation- toms of depression, such as loss of interest and loss of appe-
ship, called psychoneuroimmunology, deals with the ways tite, are similar to those of illness and are probably related to
experiences alter the immune system and how the immune the increased cytokines found in depressed people (Dantzer,
system in turn influences the central nervous system (Ader, O’Connor, Freund, Johnson, & Kelley, 2008). In short, if you
2001). have been under much stress and start to feel ill, one possibil-
Stress affects the immune system in several ways. In re- ity is that your symptoms are reactions to the stress itself.
sponse to a stressful experience, the nervous system activates A prolonged stress response is as draining on the body
the immune system to increase its production of natural killer as a prolonged illness would be (Segerstrom & Miller, 2004;
cells and the secretion of cytokines (Segerstrom & Miller, Zorrilla et al., 2001). A likely hypothesis is that prolonged
2004). The immune system evolved to protect against the increase of cortisol directs energy toward increasing metabo-
stress you might get from an injury, but in prosperous coun- lism and therefore detracts energy from synthesizing proteins,
tries today it responds more often to such events as taking including the proteins of the immune system. For example, in
exams in college (L. Y. Liu et al., 2002), giving a public lecture 1979 at the Three Mile Island nuclear power plant, a major
(Dickerson, Gable, Irwin, Aziz, & Kemeny, 2009), or seeing accident was barely contained. The people who continued to
photos of sick or injured people (Schaller, Miller, Gervais, live in the vicinity during the next year had lower than normal
Yager, & Chen, 2010). levels of B cells, T cells, and natural killer cells. They also com-
The elevated cytokine levels help combat infections, but plained of emotional distress and showed impaired perfor-
they also trigger the brain to produce the same symptoms mance on a proofreading task (A. Baum, Gatchel, & Schaef-
as if one were ill. Rats subjected to inescapable shocks show fer, 1983; McKinnon, Weisse, Reynolds, Bowles, & Baum,
12.3 Stress and Health 383

1989). A study of research scientists in Antarctica found that (Beehner, Bergman, Cheney, Seyfarth, & Whitten, 2005). In
a 9-month period of cold, darkness, and social isolation re- humans, resilience in the face of stress correlates with stronger
duced T cell functioning to about half of normal levels (Tin- connections between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex
gate, Lugg, Muller, Stowe, & Pierson, 1997). (Kim & Whalen, 2009; St. Jacques, Colcos, & Cabeza, 2009).
In one study, 276 volunteers filled out an extensive ques- People have found many ways to control their stress re-
tionnaire about stressful life events before being injected with sponses. Possibilities include special breathing routines, exer-
a moderate dose of common cold virus. (The idea was that cise, meditation, and distraction, as well as, of course, trying to
those with the strongest immune responses could fight off the deal with the problem that caused the stress. Social support
cold, but others would succumb.) People who reported brief is one of the most powerful methods of coping with stress,
stressful experiences were at no more risk for catching a cold and researchers have demonstrated its effectiveness by brain
than were people who reported no stress. However, for people measurements as well as people’s self-reports. In one study,
who reported stress lasting longer than a month, the longer happily married women were given moderately painful shocks
it lasted, the greater the risk of illness (S. Cohen et al., 1998). to their ankles. On various trials, they held the hand of their
Prolonged stress can also harm the hippocampus. Stress husband, a man they did not know, or no one. Holding the
releases cortisol, and cortisol enhances metabolic activity husband’s hand reduced the response indicated by fMRI in
throughout the body. When metabolic activity is high in the several brain areas, including the prefrontal cortex. Holding
hippocampus, its cells become more vulnerable. Toxins or the hand of an unknown man reduced the response a little,
overstimulation are then more likely than usual to damage on average, but not as much as holding the husband’s hand
or kill neurons in the hippocampus (Sapolsky, 1992). Rats (Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006). In short, as expected,
exposed to high stress—such as being restrained in a mesh brain responses correspond to people’s self-reports that social
wire retainer for 6 hours a day for 3 weeks—show shrink- support from a loved one helps reduce stress.
age of dendrites in the hippocampus and impairments in the
kinds of memory that depend on the hippocampus (Kleen,
Sitomer, Killeen, & Conrad, 2006). They also show atrophy
of other brain areas (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009). High cortisol
Post-traumatic Stress Disorder
levels may also be responsible for the deterioration of the hip- People have long recognized that many soldiers returning
pocampus and decline of memory that occur in many older from battle are prone to continuing anxieties and distress.
people (Cameron & McKay, 1999). In the past, people called this condition battle fatigue or shell
shock. Today, they call it post-traumatic stress disorder
(PTSD). PTSD occurs in some people who have endured
Stop & Check terrifying experiences, such as a life-threatening attack or
watching someone get killed. The symptoms, lasting at least
20. How do the effects of prolonged stress mimic the effects
a month after the event, include frequent distressing recollec-
of illness?
tions (flashbacks) and nightmares about the traumatic event,
21. How does prolonged stress damage the hippocampus? avoidance of reminders of it, and vigorous reactions to noises
and other stimuli (Yehuda, 2002).
ANSWERS
damage by toxins or overstimulation. However, not all people who endure traumas develop
metabolic activity, they become more vulnerable to
PTSD. For example, investigators in one study examined
218 people admitted to a hospital emergency ward after se-
body. When neurons in the hippocampus have high

vere automobile accidents. All showed about similar stress


which enhances metabolic activity throughout the
appetite. 21. Stress increases the release of cortisol,
combat illness, such as fever, inactivity, and loss of responses at the time and 1 week later, but the responses
Cytokines tell the brain to initiate responses to declined over time in some and increased in others so that
20. Prolonged stress increases release of cytokines. about one sixth of them met the criteria for PTSD 4 months
after the accident (Shalev et al., 2000). The ones developing
PTSD had not been in consistently worse wrecks than the
others. Evidently, they were more vulnerable to PTSD. Other
studies have confirmed that the people showing the greatest
Stress Control distress shortly after a traumatic event are not necessarily the
Individuals vary in their reactions to a stressful experience. ones who later develop PTSD (Harvey & Bryant, 2002).
Studies with mice have identified genes that relate to being What accounts for differences in vulnerability? Most
more vulnerable or more resilient (Krishnan et al., 2007). In- PTSD victims have a smaller than average hippocampus
dividual differences also relate to life circumstances. In baboon (Stein, Hanna, Koverola, Torchia, & McClarty, 1997). It
troops, the entry of a new adult male into a troop is stressful might seem natural to assume that severe stress elevated the
to females, because he may attack either them or their ba- cortisol secretion and that the high cortisol levels damaged the
bies. However, a female who has a male “friend” to defend her hippocampus. However, PTSD victims show lower than nor-
(possibly the father of her babies) shows less stress response mal cortisol levels both immediately after the traumatic event
384 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

and weeks later (Delahanty, Raimonde, & Spoonster, 2000; 2008). Apparently, the amygdala, which is so important for
Yehuda, 1997). The low levels suggest another hypothesis: emotional processing, is essential for the extreme emotional
Perhaps people with low cortisol levels are ill-equipped to impact that produces PTSD.
combat stress and therefore more vulnerable to the damaging
effects of stress and more prone than other people to PTSD.
To determine whether certain people are predisposed to Stop & Check
PTSD, investigators examined men who developed PTSD dur-
ing war. First, they confirmed earlier reports that most PTSD 22. How do the cortisol levels of PTSD victims compare to
victims had a smaller than average hippocampus. Then they those of other people?
found cases in which the PTSD victim had an identical twin 23. What evidence indicates that a smaller than average hip-
who had not been in battle and who did not have PTSD. The pocampus makes people more vulnerable to PTSD?
results showed that the twin without PTSD also had a smaller
than average hippocampus (Gilbertson et al., 2002). Presum- ANSWERS
he or she does not have PTSD.
ably, both twins had a smaller than average hippocampus from also has a smaller than average hippocampus, even if
the start, which increased their susceptibility to PTSD. pus. For those who have an identical twin, the twin
One further point about PTSD: A study compared Viet- PTSD victims have a smaller than average hippocam-
nam War veterans who suffered injuries that produced vari-
cortisol levels in response to stress. 23. On average,

ous kinds of brain damage. Of those whose damage included


levels in contrast to most people, who show elevated
22. People with PTSD have lower than normal cortisol
the amygdala, none suffered PTSD. Of those with damage
elsewhere in the brain, 40% suffered PTSD (Koenigs et al.,

Module 12.3  ■  In Closing

Emotions and Body Reactions

Research on stress and health provides an interesting kind of same reactions that an infection would. Research has also im-
closure. Decades ago, Hans Selye argued that any stressful event proved our understanding of the predispositions behind post-
leads to the general adaptation syndrome, marked by fever and traumatic stress disorder and makes it possible to foresee a new
other signs of illness. We now see why: The body reacts to pro- era of advances in psychosomatic medicine. Emotional states,
longed stress by activating the adrenal cortex and the immune which once seemed too ephemeral for scientific study, are now
system, and the resulting increase in cytokines produces the part of mainstream biology.

summary
1. Hans Selye introduced the idea of the general adaptation 5. Because stress causes release of cytokines, it can lead to
syndrome, which is the way the body responds to all fever, sleepiness, and other symptoms that resemble those
kinds of illness and stress.  380 of illness.  382
2. Brief stress activates the sympathetic nervous system. 6. The high cortisol levels associated with prolonged stress
More prolonged stress activates the hypothalamus- damage cells in the hippocampus, thereby impairing
pituitary-adrenal cortex axis. The adrenal cortex releases memory.  383
cortisol, which increases metabolism.  380 7. Successful methods of coping with stress, such as social
3. Although brief stress enhances the immune response and support, produce measurable effects in brain responses as
facilitates memory formation, prolonged stress drains the well as in people’s self-reports.  383
body of the resources it needs for other purposes.  382 8. After a severely trying event, some people but not others
4. Stress activates the immune system, helping to fight develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Evidently,
viruses and bacteria. The immune system releases people with a smaller than average hippocampus and
cytokines, which stimulate the hypothalamus to initiate lower than average cortisol levels are predisposed to
activities to combat illness.  382 PTSD.  383
12.3 Stress and Health 385

Key Terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
adrenocorticotropic hormone cortisol  380 leukocyte  381
(ACTH)   380 cytokine  381 post-traumatic stress disorder
antibody  381 general adaptation syndrome  380 (PTSD)  383
antigen  381 HPA axis  380 psychoneuroimmunology  382
behavioral medicine  380 immune system  381 stress  380

Thought Question
If someone were unable to produce cytokines, what would be
the consequences?

chapter 12 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
386 Chapter 12 Emotional Behaviors

Videos
Also available—
■ Emotional Memory
■ Suspicion and Trust

© 2013 Cengage Learning


■ Health and Stress
■ The Limbic System

Facial Analysis

Animations
The Retina nnn (Figure x.x) Also available—
■ Sympathetic and Parasympathetic Nervous
© 2013 Cengage Learning

System
■ Amygdala and Fear Conditioning

■ GABA Synapse

■ CNS Depressants

■ Hypothalamus and Pituitary


Cells of the Immune System
12.3 Stress and Health 387

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt Brace. A neurologist’s ac-
count of the connection between emotion and consciousness, full of interesting examples.
McEwen, B. S., with Lasley, E. N. (2002). The end of stress as we know it. Washington, DC: Joseph
Henry Press. Readable review by one of the leading researchers.
Pfaff, D. W. (2007). The neuroscience of fair play. New York: Dana Press. Exploration of how the
physiology of emotions, especially the amygdala, relates to moral behavior.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, such as one concerning stress.
Source: Tim Davis/CORBIS
The Biology of Learning
and Memory
13
Chapter Outline Main Ideas
Module 13.1 ​Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain 1. To understand the physiology of learning, we must
Functioning answer two questions: What changes occur in a single
Localized Representations of Memory cell during learning, and how do changed cells work
Types of Memory together to produce adaptive behavior?
The Hippocampus 2. Psychologists distinguish among several types of memory,
The Basal Ganglia dependent on different brain areas.
Other Types of Amnesia
3. Learning requires changes that facilitate or decrease the
Other Brain Areas in Memory
activity at particular synapses.
In Closing: Different Types of Memory
Module 13.2 ​Storing Information in the Nervous System
Learning and the Hebbian Synapse
Single-Cell Mechanisms of Invertebrate Behavior Change
Long-Term Potentiation in Vertebrates
Improving Memory
In Closing: The Physiology of Memory
Interactive Exploration and Study

S
uppose you type something on your computer and
then save it. A year later, you come back, click the cor-
rect filename, and retrieve what you wrote. How did
the computer remember what to do?
That question has two parts. First, how do the physical
properties of silicon chips enable them to alter their proper-
ties when you type certain keys? Second, how does the wiring
diagram take the changes in individual silicon chips and con-
vert them into a useful activity?
Similarly, when we try to explain how you remember some
experience, we deal with two questions. First, how does a pat-
tern of sensory information alter the properties of certain
neurons? Second, after neurons change their properties, how
does the nervous system produce the behavioral changes that
we call learning or memory?
We begin this chapter by considering how the various
brain areas interact to produce learning and memory. In the
OPPOSITE: Learning produces amazingly complex behaviors. second module, we turn to the detailed physiology of how ex-
perience changes neurons and synapses.
389
Module 13.1

Learning, Memory, Amnesia,


and Brain Functioning

S
uppose you lost your ability to form long-lasting which stimulated the dog to salivate (UCR). After many such
memories. You remember what just happened but pairings, the sound alone (CS) stimulated the dog to salivate
nothing earlier. You feel as if you awakened from a (CR). In that case and many others, the CR resembles the
long sleep only a second ago. So you write on a sheet of UCR, but in some cases, it does not. For example, if a rat ex-
paper, “Just now, for the first time, I have suddenly become periences a CS paired with shock, the shock elicits screaming
conscious!” A little later, you forget this experience, too. As and jumping, but the CS elicits a freezing response.
far as you can tell, you have just now emerged into In instrumental conditioning (also known as operant
consciousness after a long sleeplike period. You look at this conditioning), an individual’s response leads to a reinforcer or
sheet of paper on which you wrote about becoming punishment (Figure 13.1b). A reinforcer is any event that in-
conscious, but you don’t remember writing it. How odd! creases the future probability of the response. A punishment
You must have written it when in fact you were not is an event that suppresses the frequency of the response. For
conscious! Irritated, you cross off that statement and write example, when a rat enters one arm of a maze and finds Froot
anew, “NOW I am for the first time conscious!” And a Loops cereal (a potent reinforcer for a rat), its probability of
minute later, you cross that one off and write it again. entering that arm again increases. If it receives a shock instead,
Eventually, someone finds this sheet of paper on which you the probability decreases. The primary difference between
have repeatedly written and crossed out statements about classical and instrumental conditioning is that in instrumen-
suddenly feeling conscious for the first time. tal conditioning the individual’s response determines the out-
Sound far-fetched? It really happened to a patient who de- come (reinforcer or punishment), whereas in classical condi-
veloped severe memory impairments after encephalitis dam- tioning the CS and UCS occur at certain times regardless of
aged his temporal cortex (B. A. Wilson, Baddeley, & Kapur, the individual’s behavior. (The behavior is useful, however, in
1995). Life without memory means no sense of existing across preparing for the UCS.)
time. Your memory is almost synonymous with your sense of Some cases of learning are difficult to label as classical
“self.” or instrumental. For example, after a male songbird hears
the song of his own species during his first few months, he
imitates it the following year. The song that he heard was not
Localized Representations paired with any other stimulus, so it doesn’t look like classical
of Memory conditioning. He learned the song without reinforcers or pun-
ishments, so we can’t call it instrumental conditioning either.
What happens in the brain during learning and memory? That is, animals have specialized methods of learning other
One early idea was that a connection grew between two brain than classical and instrumental conditioning. Also, the way
areas. Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov pioneered the inves- animals (including people) learn varies from one situation to
tigation of what we now call classical conditioning (Figure another. For example, in most situations, learning occurs only
13.1a), in which pairing two stimuli changes the response to if the CS and UCS, or response and reinforcer, occur close to-
one of them (Pavlov, 1927). The experimenter starts by pre- gether in time. But if you eat something, especially something
senting a conditioned stimulus (CS), which initially elicits no unfamiliar, and get sick later, you learn a strong aversion to
response of note, and then presents the unconditioned stim- the taste of that food, even if taste and illness are separated by
ulus (UCS), which automatically elicits the unconditioned hours (Rozin & Kalat, 1971; Rozin & Schull, 1988).
response (UCR). After some pairings of the CS and the UCS
(perhaps just one or two, perhaps many), the individual be-
gins making a new, learned response to the CS, called a con- Lashley’s Search for the Engram
ditioned response (CR). In his original experiments, Pavlov Pavlov proposed that classical conditioning reflects a strength-
presented a dog with a sound (CS) followed by meat (UCS), ened connection between a CS center and a UCS center in the
390
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 391

At first

Food Automatically Salivation


Followed by elicits

Conditioned Unconditioned Unconditioned


stimulus (CS) stimulus (UCS) response (UCR)

After some number of repetitions

Salivation

Conditioned Conditioned
stimulus (CS) response (CR)
(a) Classical conditioning

Froot
Loops

Reinforcement

Increased
probability
of same response

Shock

Response:
Rat enters one arm Punishment
of the maze

Shift to
different
(b) Instrumental conditioning response

Figure 13.1  ​Classical conditioning and instrumental conditioning


(a) In classical conditioning, two stimuli (CS and UCS) are presented at certain times regardless of what the learner does. (b) In instru-
mental conditioning, the learner’s behavior controls the presentation of reinforcer or punishment. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

brain. That strengthened connection lets any excitation of the sentation of what has been learned. (A connection between two
CS center flow to the UCS center, evoking the unconditioned brain areas would be a possible example of an engram.)
response (Figure 13.2). Karl Lashley set out to test this hypoth- Lashley reasoned that if learning depends on new or
esis. Lashley was searching for the engram—the physical repre- strengthened connections between two brain areas, a knife cut
392 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

UCS

UCR CS

(a)
Figure 13.3  ​View of rat brain from above, showing cuts
that Lashley made in various rats
No cut or combination of cuts interfered with a rat’s memory of a
maze. (Adapted from Lashley, 1950)
UCS

UCR CS
Lashley also tested whether any portion of the cerebral
cortex is more important than others for learning. He trained
rats on mazes before or after he removed large portions of the
cortex. The lesions impaired performance, but the deficit de-
pended more on the amount of brain damage than on its loca-
tion. Learning and memory apparently did not rely on a single
(b) cortical area. Lashley therefore proposed two principles about
the nervous system:
Figure 13.2  ​Pavlov’s proposal to explain learning
(a) Initially, the UCS excites the UCS center, which excites the UCR ■ equipotentiality—all parts of the cortex contribute
center. The CS excites the CS center, which elicits no response of equally to complex behaviors such as learning, and any
interest. (b) After training, excitation in the CS center flows to the part of the cortex can substitute for any other.
UCS center, thus eliciting the same response as the UCS.
■ mass action—the cortex works as a whole, and more
(© Cengage Learning 2013)
cortex is better.
Note, however, another interpretation of Lashley’s results:
Maze learning and visual discrimination learning are complex
somewhere in the brain should interrupt that connection and tasks. A rat finding its way to food attends to visual and tactile
abolish the learned response. He trained rats on mazes and stimuli, the location of its body, the position of its head, and
a brightness discrimination task and then made deep cuts in any other available cues. This example of learning depends on
varying locations in their cerebral cortices (Lashley, 1929, 1950) many cortical areas, but different areas could be contributing
(Figure 13.3). However, no knife cut significantly impaired the in different ways.
rats’ performances. Evidently, the types of learning that he stud- Eventually, researchers found that Lashley’s conclusions
ied did not depend on connections across the cortex. rested on two unnecessary assumptions: (a) that the cerebral
cortex is the best or only place to search for an engram, and
(b) that all kinds of memory are physiologically the same. As
Karl S. Lashley we shall see, investigators who discarded these assumptions
(1890–1958)
reached different conclusions.
Psychology is today a more fundamental
science than neurophysiology. By this I
mean that the latter offers few principles The Modern Search for the Engram
McGraw-Hill Education

from which we may predict or define the


normal organization of behavior, whereas Richard F. Thompson and his colleagues used a simpler task
the study of psychological processes fur- than Lashley’s and sought the engram of memory not in the
nishes a mass of factual material to which cerebral cortex but in the cerebellum. Thompson and col-
the laws of nervous action in behavior must conform. (Lashley, leagues studied classical conditioning of eyelid responses in
1930, p. 24) rabbits. They presented first a tone (CS) and then a puff of
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 393

air (UCS) to the cornea of the rabbit’s eye. At first, a rabbit not have prevented learning. Still, one could imagine that
blinked at the airpuff but not at the tone. After repeated pair- learning occurred in the LIP and an earlier area. That is, we
ings, classical conditioning occurred and the rabbit blinked have no reason to insist that learning occur in one location
at the tone also. Investigators recorded the activity in various alone. However, consider the results of this study: Single-cell
brain cells to determine which ones changed their responses recordings found that classical conditioning of the eyeblink
during learning. response was accompanied by increased responses in both the
Thompson set out to determine the location of learning. LIP and the medial geniculate nucleus (the auditory portion of
Imagine a sequence of brain areas from the sensory receptors the thalamus), which provides a major input to the LIP. How-
to the motor neurons controlling the muscles: ever, after an auditory conditioned stimulus, the activity in-
creased in the LIP 10–20 ms before it increased in the medial
A B C D E F geniculate nucleus (Halverson, Lee, & Freeman, 2010). Evi-
© Cengage Learning 2013
dently the increased activity in the medial geniculate nucleus
represents feedback from the LIP, and learning itself must rely
If we damage any one of those areas, learning will be im- on the LIP alone.
paired, but we can’t be sure that learning occurred in the dam- The mechanisms for this type of conditioning are
aged area. For example, if the learning occurs in area D, damage probably similar in humans. According to PET scans on
in A, B, or C will prevent learning by blocking the input to D. young adults, when pairing a stimulus with an airpuff
Damage in E or F will prevent learning by blocking the output produces a conditioned eyeblink, activity increases in the
from D. Thompson and colleagues reasoned as follows: Suppose cerebellum, red nucleus, and several other areas (Logan &
the learning occurs in D. If so, then D has to be active at the time Grafton, 1995). People who have damage in the cerebel-
of the learning, and so do all the areas leading up to D (A, B, and lum have weaker conditioned eyeblinks, and the blinks
C). However, learning should not require areas E and beyond. are less accurately timed relative to the onset of the air-
If area E were blocked, nothing would relay information to the puff (Gerwig et al., 2005). The cerebellum is critical for
muscles, so we would see no response, but learning could occur many other instances of classical conditioning also, but
nevertheless, and we could see evidence of it later. only if the delay between the onset of the CS and the
Thompson identified one nucleus of the cerebellum, the onset of the UCS is short (Pakaprot, Kim, & Thomp-
lateral interpositus nucleus (LIP), as essential for learning. son, 2009). As mentioned in Chapter 8, the cerebellum
At the start of training, those cells showed little response to is specialized for timing brief intervals, on the order of
the tone, but as learning proceeded, their responses increased a couple of seconds or less. Also, the results differ in the
(R. F. Thompson, 1986). When the investigators temporar- case of trace conditioning, in which the CS (such as a tone)
ily suppressed that nucleus in an untrained rabbit, either by ends before the onset of the UCS, and the animal has to
cooling the nucleus or by injecting a drug into it, and then associate a memory trace of the CS with the UCS. In that
presented the CSs and UCSs, the rabbit showed no responses case, learning depends on the basal ganglia as well as the
during the training. Then they waited for the LIP to recover cerebellum (Flores & Disterhoft, 2009).
and continued training. At that point, the rabbit began to
learn, but it learned at the same speed as animals that had re-
sStop & Check
ceived no previous training. Evidently, while the LIP was sup-
pressed, the training had no effect. 1. Thompson found a localized engram, whereas Lashley did
But does learning actually occur in the LIP, or does this not. What key differences in procedures or assumptions
area just relay the information to a later area where learning were probably responsible for their different results?
occurs? In the next experiments, investigators suppressed ac- 2. What evidence indicates that the red nucleus is neces-
tivity in the red nucleus, a midbrain motor area that receives sary for performance of a conditioned response but not
input from the cerebellum. When the red nucleus was sup- for learning the response?
pressed, the rabbits again showed no responses during train-
ing. However, as soon as the red nucleus had recovered from nucleus was inactivated.
ANSWERS
the cooling or drugs, the rabbits showed strong learned re- further training, so learning occurred while the red
sponses to the tone (R. E. Clark & Lavond, 1993; Krupa, show conditioned responses at once, without any
Thompson, & Thompson, 1993). In other words, suppressing soon as the red nucleus recovers, the animal can
the red nucleus temporarily prevented the response but did nucleus is necessary for the response. However, as
not prevent learning. Evidently, learning did not require activ- conditioned responses during the training, so the red
ity in the red nucleus or any area after it. The researchers con-
inactivated during training, the animal makes no

cluded, therefore, that the learning occurred in the LIP. Figure


of the cerebral cortex. 2. If the red nucleus is
learning. Also, he looked in the cerebellum instead
13.4 summarizes these experiments. 1. Thompson studied a different, simpler type of
How did they know that learning didn’t depend on some
area before the LIP? If it did, then suppressing the LIP would
394 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

Thalamus

Corpus callosum

Cerebral cortex

Lateral
interpositus
nucleus of
cerebellum

Sixth cranial nerve,


Red nucleus controlling the nictitating
membrane (part of the
rabbit’s system of eyelids)

Responses while LIP is suppressed Responses while red nucleus is suppressed


100 100
80 80

% CRs 60 % CRs 60
40 40
20 20
0 0
Session 1 2 3 4 5 6 Session 1 2 3 4 5 6

Responses in intact animal Responses after LIP recovers activity Responses after red nucleus recovers activity
100 100 100
80 80 80
60 60 60
% CRs % CRs % CRs
40 40 40
20 20 20
0 0 0
Session 1 2 3 4 5 6 Session 1 2 3 4 Session 1 2 3 4

Figure 13.4  ​Localization of an engram


Temporarily inactivating the lateral interpositus nucleus blocked all indications of learning. After the inactivation wore off, the rabbits
learned as slowly as rabbits with no previous training. Temporarily inactivating the red nucleus blocked responses during the period of
inactivation, but the learned response appeared as soon as the red nucleus recovered. (Based on the results of Clark & Lavond, 1993;
Krupa, Thompson, & Thompson, 1993)

Types of Memory
memories form quickly. Old people can recall events from
Decades ago, psychologists expected to find laws of learning their childhood, so we also see that some memories last a life-
or memory that would apply to all situations. Gradually, they time. Hebb could not imagine a chemical process that is fast
became aware of important differences among types of learn- enough to account for immediate memory yet stable enough
ing and memory. Researchers continue to explore exactly what to provide permanent memory. He therefore proposed a dis-
are the best distinctions to draw, and studies of brain damage tinction between short-term memory of events that have just
make important contributions to this pursuit. occurred and long-term memory of events from further back.
Several types of evidence supported this idea:
Short-Term and Long-Term Memory ■ Short-term memory and long-term memory differ in
Donald Hebb (1949) reasoned that no one mechanism could their capacity. If you hear a series of numbers or letters,
account for all the phenomena of learning. You can immedi- such as DZLAUV, you can probably repeat no more
ately repeat something you just heard, so it is clear that some than about seven of them, and with other kinds of mate-
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 395

rial, your maximum is even less. Long-term memory has James L. McGaugh
a vast, difficult-to-estimate capacity. Memory is perhaps the most critical capac-
ity that we have as humans. Memory is not
■ Short-term memory depends on rehearsal. For ex-
simply a record of experiences; it is the ba-
ample, if you read the letter sequence DZLAUV and sis of our knowledge of the world, our skills,
then something distracts you, your chance of repeat- our hopes and dreams and our ability to in-

James L. McGaugh
ing the letters declines rapidly (Peterson & Peterson, teract with others and thus influence our
1959). You can recall long-term memories that you destinies. Investigation of how the brain en-
haven’t thought about in years. ables us to bridge our present existence
■ With short-term memory, once you have forgotten
with our past and future is thus essential for
understanding human nature. Clearly, the most exciting challenge
something, it is lost. With long-term memory, a hint
of science is to determine how brain cells and systems create our
might help you reconstruct something you thought
memories.
you had forgotten. For example, try naming all
your high school teachers. After you have named all
you can, you can name still more if someone shows
you photos and tells you the teachers’ initials.
and it certainly depends on more than the time necessary to
Based on these distinctions, researchers proposed that all synthesize some new proteins.
information initially entered a short-term storage, where it The second problem is that a “consolidated” memory is
stayed until the brain had time to consolidate it into long- not solid forever. As mentioned in Chapter 12, a memory re-
term memory. If anything interrupted the rehearsal before awakened by a reminder becomes labile—that is, changeable
consolidation took place, the information was simply lost. or vulnerable. If the reminder is followed by a similar expe-
rience, the memory is reconsolidated—that is, strengthened
Our Changing Views of Consolidation again—by a process that requires protein synthesis. Giving
a reminder and then administering a drug that blocks pro-
Later studies made the distinction between short-term and tein synthesis substantially weakens the memory (Nader &
long-term memory increasingly problematic. First, many Hardt, 2009). New experiences during the reconsolidation
short-term memories are not simply temporary stores on their process can modify the memory, also. Suppose someone says,
way to being long-term memories. When you watch a soccer “Remember the time we went to Disneyland?” You reply, “Oh,
or hockey match, you remember the score until it changes, yes.” If the other person then elaborates on one part of your
perhaps an hour later. Rehearsing that score for an hour Disneyland experience, you update and modify your memory
doesn’t turn it into a long-term memory. to highlight that aspect, probably at the expense of other de-
Furthermore, consolidation isn’t what we used to think tails that might fade in your memory.
it was. The original idea was that the brain held onto some-
thing in short-term memory for whatever time it needed
to do what it had to do in order to establish a long-term
memory—mainly, synthesize new proteins (Canal & Gold, Stop & Check
2007). Once formed, the long-term memory was supposed 3. How do epinephrine and cortisol enhance memory storage?
to be permanent. This idea failed in two ways. First, the time
needed for consolidation varies enormously. If you are trying ANSWER
campus.
to memorize facts that you consider boring, you might have memories by stimulating the amygdala and hippo-
to work at it for hours. But if your romantic partner whis- 3. Epinephrine and cortisol both enhance emotional
pers something emotionally exciting to you, or if someone
warns you about the venomous snake that got loose in your
dormitory, how long will it take you to establish that mem-
ory? Emotionally significant memories form quickly. Why?
Remember from Chapter 12 that stressful or emotionally Working Memory
exciting experiences increase the secretion of epinephrine To replace the concept of short-term memory, A. D. Baddeley
(adrenaline) and cortisol. Small to moderate amounts of and G. J. Hitch introduced the term working memory to refer
cortisol activate the amygdala and hippocampus, where they to the way we store information while we are working with it. A
enhance the storage and consolidation of recent experiences common test of working memory is the delayed response task,
(Cahill & McGaugh, 1998). The amygdala in turn stimulates which requires responding to something that you saw or heard
the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, which are both impor- a short while ago. For example, imagine that while you stare at
tant for memory storage. However, prolonged stress, which a central fixation point, a light flashes briefly at some point to-
releases even more cortisol, impairs memory (deQuervain, ward the periphery, in any direction. You have to continue star-
Roozendaal, & McGaugh, 1998; Newcomer et al., 1999). ing at that central point for a few seconds until you hear a beep,
The main point here is that consolidation can be fast or slow, and then look toward the place where you remember seeing the
396 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

light. This task can be modified for use with monkeys and other third lunch and ate most of it. A few minutes later, he said he
species. During the delay, the learner has to store a representa- would like to “go for a walk and get a good meal” (Rozin, Dow,
tion of the stimulus, and much research points to the prefron- Moscovitch, & Rajaram, 1998). Other patients with amnesia
tal cortex as an important location for this storage. During the also forget that they have just eaten, although when they start
delay, certain cells in the prefrontal cortex as well as the parietal to eat again, they remark on not enjoying the food as much as
cortex increase their activity, and different cells become active usual (Higgs, Williamson, Rotshtein, & Humphreys, 2008).
depending on the direction the eye movement will need to take However, even in severe cases, no one loses all kinds of
(Chafee & Goldman-Rakic, 1998). The increase in activity memory equally. A patient who forgets that he ate lunch a few
does not necessarily take the form of repeated action potentials; minutes ago still remembers how to eat with a knife and fork,
another possibility is that cells store extra calcium, increasing for example, and what different foods taste like, and how to
their readiness to respond to new signals when the time comes cook them. Studies on amnesia help clarify the distinctions
(Mongillo, Barak, & Tsodyks, 2008). among different kinds of memory and enable us to explore the
Damage to the prefrontal cortex impairs performance, mechanisms of memory.
and the deficit can be amazingly precise, depending on the
exact location of the damage. For example, after damage in a
particular spot, a monkey might be unable to remember that People with Hippocampal Damage
the light had been directly to the left of fixation, despite being In 1953, Henry Molaison, known in most research reports as
able to see that location and despite being able to remember H. M., was suffering about 10 minor epileptic seizures per day
a light in any other location. After damage in a different spot, and a major seizure about once a week, despite trying every
a monkey might not be able to remember light in some other available antiepileptic drug. Eventually, he agreed to try a des-
location (Sawaguchi & Iba, 2001). perate measure. A surgeon who had experimented with vari-
Many older people have impairments of working mem- ous forms of lobotomy for mental illness had come to believe
ory, probably because of changes in the prefrontal cortex. that removing the medial temporal lobe would relieve epi-
Studies on aged monkeys find decreases in the number of lepsy. (Although it does in occasional cases, the surgeon was
neurons and the amount of input in certain parts of the wrong to believe this was a general rule.) The neurosurgeon
prefrontal cortex (D. E. Smith, Rapp, McKay, Roberts, & removed the hippocampus and some nearby structures of the
Tuszynski, 2004). Older humans with declining memory medial temporal cortex from both of H. M.’s hemispheres.
show declining activity in the prefrontal cortex, but those Researchers knew almost nothing about the hippocampus at
with intact memory show greater activity than young adults the time, and no one knew what to expect after the surgery.
(A. C. Rosen et al., 2002; Rossi et al., 2004). Presumably, We now know that various parts of the hippocampus are ac-
the increased activity means that the prefrontal cortex is tive during both the formation of memories and later recall
working harder in these older adults to compensate for im- (Eldridge, Engel, Zeineh, Bookheimer, & Knowlton, 2005).
pairments elsewhere in the brain. Furthermore, stimulant Although the operation reduced H. M.’s epilepsy to no more
drugs that enhance activity in the prefrontal cortex improve than two major seizures per year, he suffered a severe memory
the memory of aged monkeys (Castner & Goldman-Rakic, impairment (Milner, 1959; Penfield & Milner, 1958; Scoville
2004). Such treatments have potential for treating people & Milner, 1957). Figure 13.5 shows the normal anatomy of
with failing memory. the hippocampus and the damage in H. M.

Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia


After the surgery, H. M.’s intellect and language abilities
Stop & Check remained intact, and his personality remained the same ex-
4. What is the primary brain location for working memory, cept for emotional placidity (Eichenbaum, 2002). However,
and what is one hypothesis for how it stores temporary he suffered massive anterograde amnesia (inability to form
information? memories for events that happened after brain damage). He
also suffered a retrograde amnesia (loss of memory for events
ANSWER
responses. that occurred before the brain damage). Initially, research-
tion by elevated calcium levels, which potentiate later ers said his retrograde amnesia was confined to 1 to 3 years
cording to one hypothesis, it stores temporary informa- before the surgery. Later, they found it was more extensive.
4. The prefrontal cortex is the primary location. Ac-
H. M. is representative of many people who have suffered
amnesia after damage to the hippocampus and surrounding
structures of the medial temporal lobe. All show both antero-
grade and retrograde amnesia, with the retrograde amnesia
being most severe for the time leading up to the damage. For
The Hippocampus example, amnesic patients can usually tell where they lived as
Amnesia is memory loss. One patient ate lunch and, 20 a child and where they lived as a teenager but might not be
minutes later, ate a second lunch, apparently having forgot- able to say where they lived 3 years ago (Bayley, Hopkins, &
ten the first meal. Another 20 minutes later, he started on a Squire, 2006).
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 397

Cerebral cortex
Corpus
callosum

Thalamus

Olfactory bulb
Horizontal section
Thalamus through left
Olfactory tract
© Cengage Learning 2013

posterior cerebral
cortex

Dr. Dana Copeland


Mamillary body
Hippocampus
Left temporal lobe
Hippocampus

(a) (b)
David Amaral and Suzanne Corkin

Location of the missing hippocampus


(c)

Figure 13.5  ​The hippocampus


(a) Location of the hippocampus in the interior of the temporal lobe. The left hippocampus is closer to the viewer than the rest of this
plane; the right hippocampus is behind the plane. The dashed line marks the location of the temporal lobe, which is not visible in the
midline. (b) Photo of a human brain from above. The top part of the left hemisphere has been cut away to show how the hippocampus
loops over (dorsal to) the thalamus, posterior to it, and then below (ventral to) it. (c) MRI scan of H. M.’s brain, showing absence of the
hippocampus. The three views show coronal planes at successive locations, anterior to posterior.

Intact Working Memory Impaired Storage of Long-Term Memory


Despite H. M.’s huge deficits in forming long-term memo- Although H. M. showed normal working memory, as soon
ries, his short-term or working memory remained intact. In as he was distracted, the memory was gone. For several years
one test, Brenda Milner (1959) asked him to remember the after his operation, whenever he was asked his age and the
number 584. After a 15-minute delay without distractions, he date, he answered “27” and “1953.” After a few years, he started
recalled it correctly, explaining, “It’s easy. You just remember guessing wildly, generally underestimating his age by 10 years
8. You see, 5, 8, and 4 add to 17. You remember 8, subtract it or more and missing the date by as many as 43 years (Corkin,
from 17, and it leaves 9. Divide 9 in half and you get 5 and 4, 1984). He could read the same magazine repeatedly without
and there you are, 584. Easy.” A moment later, after his atten- losing interest. Often, he told someone about a childhood in-
tion had shifted to another subject, he had forgotten both the cident and then, a minute or two later, told the same person
number and the complicated line of thought he had associated the same story again (Eichenbaum, 2002). In 1980, he moved
with it. Most other patients with severe amnesia also show to a nursing home. Four years later, he could not say where he
normal working memory, given a lack of distraction (Shrager, lived or who cared for him. Although he watched the news on
Levy, Hopkins, & Squire, 2008). television every night, he could recall only a few fragments of
398 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

events since 1953. Over the years, many new words entered Then the researchers let the patients devise their own labels.
the English language, such as jacuzzi and granola. H. M. re- Each patient had to look at one shape at a time and describe it
garded them as nonsense (Corkin, 2002). so that another person, who was looking at the 12 shapes unla-
You might wonder whether he was surprised at his beled, would know which one the patient was looking at. At first,
own appearance in a photo or mirror. Yes and no. When the descriptions were slow and uninformative. For the shape at
asked his age or whether his hair turned gray, he replied the upper right of Figure 13.6, one patient said, “The next one
that he did not know. When shown a photo of himself with looks almost . . . the opposite of somebody kind uh . . . slumped
his mother, taken long after his surgery, he recognized his down, on the ground, with the same type of . . .” Eventually, he
mother but not himself. However, when he saw himself in said it looked like someone sleeping with his knees bent. By the
the mirror, he showed no surprise (Corkin, 2002). He had, fourth trial, he quickly labeled that shape as “the siesta guy,” and
of course, seen himself daily in the mirror over all these he continued saying the same thing from then on, even in later
years. He also had the context of knowing that the person sessions on later days (Duff, Hengst, Tranel, & Cohen, 2006).
in the mirror must be himself, whereas the person in the
photo could be anyone.
Severe Impairment of Episodic Memory
H. M. formed a few weak semantic (factual) memories for
H. M. had severe impairment of episodic memories, memories
new information he encountered repeatedly (Corkin, 2002;
of single personal events. He could not describe any experience
O’Kane, Kensinger, & Corkin, 2004). For example, when
that he had after his surgery. His retrograde amnesia was also
he was given first names and asked to fill in appropriate last
greatest for episodic memories. Although he could describe
names, his replies included some who became famous after
facts that he learned before his operation, he could relate few
1953, such as these:
personal experiences. Another patient, K. C., suffered wide-
H. M.’s Answer spread brain damage after a motorcycle accident, with scattered
damage in the hippocampus and other locations, leading to
Elvis Presley
an apparently complete loss of episodic memories. He cannot
Martin Luther King
describe a single event from any time of his life, although he
Billy Graham
remembers many facts that he knew before the damage. When
Fidel Castro
he looks at old family pictures in a photo album, he identifies
Lyndon Johnson
the people and sometimes the places, but he cannot remember
He provided even more names when he was given addi- anything about the events that happened in the photos (Rosen-
tional information: baum et al., 2005). Although his brain damage is so diffuse that
we cannot be sure which part of the damage is responsible for
H. M.’s Answer his memory loss, the observations do tell us that the brain treats
Famous artist, Pablo Picasso episodic memories differently from other memories.
born in Spain. . . How would memory loss affect people’s ability to imagine
the future? If you try to imagine a future event, you call upon
One study found an interesting qualification to the usual your memory of similar experiences and modify them. Studies
rule that patients with amnesia cannot learn new information. using fMRI show that describing past events and imagining fu-
The investigators showed a series of shapes with unrelated la- ture events activate mostly the same areas, including the hippo-
bels, as shown in Figure 13.6. As expected, amnesic patients campus (Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007). People with amnesia
made no progress toward learning the labels for each shape. are just as impaired at imagining the future as they are at describ-

Ocean Sunset Target Finger Crocodile Dollar

Yard Student Traffic Giant Broom Wing

Figure 13.6  ​Displays for a Memory Test of Amnesic Patients


Although they could not remember the arbitrary labels that an experimenter gave to each object (as shown), they did remember the de-
scriptions that they devised themselves. (From Duff, M. C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2006). Development of shared information in
communication despite hippocampal amnesia. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 140–146. Used by permission, Macmillan Publishing Ltd.)
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 399

ing the past. For example, here is part of one patient’s attempt a motor skill or habit in words, and you might not even recog-
to imagine a visit to a museum, with prompts by a psychologist nize it as a memory. For example, H. M. learned to read words
(Hassabis, Kumaran, Vann, & Maguire, 2007, p. 1727): written backward, as they would be seen in a mirror, although
he was surprised at this skill, as he did not remember having
Patient: [pause] There’s not a lot, as it happens.
tried it before (Corkin, 2002). Patient K. C. has a part-time
Psychologist: So what does it look like in your imagined
job at a library and has learned to use the Dewey decimal sys-
scene?
tem in sorting books, although he does not remember when
Patient: Well, there’s big doors. The openings would be
or where he learned it (Rosenbaum et al., 2005).
high, so the doors would be very big with brass
Here is another example of procedural memory: In the
handles, the ceiling would be made of glass, so
video game Tetris, geometrical forms such as and fall
there’s plenty of light coming through. Huge room,
from the top, and the player must move and rotate them to fill
exit on either side of the room, there’s a pathway
available spaces at the bottom of the screen. Normal people
and map through the center and on either side
improve their skill over a few hours and readily describe the
there’d be the exhibits. [pause] I don’t know what
game and its strategy. After playing the same number of hours,
they are. There’d be people. [pause] To be honest
patients with amnesia cannot describe the game and say they
there’s not a lot coming. . . . My imagination isn’t . . .
don’t remember playing it. Nevertheless, they improve, slowly.
well, I’m not imagining it, let’s put it that way. . . .
Moreover, when they are about to fall asleep, they report see-
I’m not picturing anything at the moment.
ing images of little piles of blocks falling and rotating (Stick-
gold, Malia, Maguire, Roddenberry, & O’Connor, 2000).
Better Implicit than Explicit Memory They are puzzled and wonder what these images mean!
Nearly all patients with amnesia show better implicit than ex- In summary, people with amnesia, including H. M., gener-
plicit memory. Explicit memory is deliberate recall of informa- ally show the following pattern:
tion that one recognizes as a memory, also known as declara-
■ Normal working memory
tive memory. If you have an explicit or declarative memory of
■ Severe anterograde amnesia for declarative memory—
something, you can state it in words. Implicit memory is an
influence of experience on behavior, even if you do not recog- that is, difficulty forming new declarative memories,
nize that influence. For example, you might be talking to some- especially episodic memories.
one about sports while other people nearby are carrying on a ■ Some degree of retrograde amnesia—that is, loss of

conversation about the latest movies. If asked, you could not old memories—mainly limited to episodic memories.
say what the others were talking about, but sud- ■ Better implicit than explicit memory.

denly, you comment for no apparent reason, “I ■ Nearly intact procedural memory.
wonder what’s on at the movies?” To experience try it
implicit memory, try the Online Try It Yourself yourselfonline
exercise “Implicit Memories.”
Stop & Check
Another example of implicit memory: As an experiment,
three hospital workers agreed to act in special ways toward 5. What is the difference between anterograde and retro-
a patient with amnesia. One was as pleasant as possible. grade amnesia?
The second was neutral. The third was stern, refused all re- 6. Which types of memory are least impaired in people with
quests, and made the patient perform boring tasks. After 5 amnesia?
days, the patient was asked to look at photos of the three
workers and try to identify them or say anything he knew memory, implicit memory, and procedural memory.
ANSWERS
about them. He said he did not recognize any of them. Then with amnesia are generally least impaired on working
he was asked which one he would approach as a possible memories of events after brain damage. 6. People
friend or which one he would ask for help. He was asked brain damage; anterograde amnesia is failing to store
this question repeatedly—it was possible to ask repeatedly 5. Retrograde amnesia is forgetting events before
because he never remembered being asked before—and he
usually chose the photo of the “friendly” person and never
chose the “unfriendly” person in spite of the fact that the
unfriendly person was a beautiful woman, smiling in the Theories of the Function
photograph (Tranel & Damasio, 1993). He could not say
why he chose to avoid her. of the Hippocampus
Exactly how does the hippocampus contribute to memory?
Intact Procedural Memory Some of the research comes from patients with damage to the
Procedural memory, the development of motor skills and hippocampus, but to get better control over both the anatomy
habits, is a special kind of implicit memory. As with other ex- and the environment, researchers also conduct research on
amples of implicit memory, you might not be able to describe laboratory animals.
400 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory Larry R. Squire


Although patients with hippocampal damage acquire new Memory is personal and evocative, inter-
skills, they have enormous trouble learning new facts. Larry twined with emotion, and it provides us with
Squire (1992) proposed that the hippocampus is critical for a sense of who we are…. There has been a
declarative memory, especially episodic memory. How could revolution in our understanding of what
memory is and what happens in the brain
we test this hypothesis with nonhumans, who cannot “declare”
when we learn and remember. At the begin-
anything? What could they do that would be the equivalent

Larry Squire
ning of the 21st century, one has the sense
of declarative or episodic memory? Researchers have devel- that memory may be the first mental faculty
oped many clever approaches (Crystal, 2009). Here is one that will be understandable in terms of mol-
example: A rat digs food out of five piles of sand, each with a ecules, cells, brain systems, and behavior. Yet, even with all the
different odor. Then it gets a choice between two of the odors progress, there can be no doubt that the study of the brain is still
and is rewarded if it goes toward the one it smelled first. In- a young science, rich with opportunity for the student and begin-
tact rats learn to respond correctly, apparently demonstrating ning scientist. This is a good time to hear about the promise and
memory of not only what they smelled but also when they excitement of neuroscience. The best is yet to come. (Squire, per-
smelled it. Memory of a specific event qualifies as episodic, at sonal communication)
least by a broad definition. Rats with hippocampal damage
do poorly on this task (Fortin, Agster, & Eichenbaum, 2002;
Kesner, Gilbert, & Barua, 2002).
In the delayed matching-to-sample task, an animal sees farther apart (Morgan, MacEvoy, Aguirre, & Epstein, 2011).
an object (the sample) and then, after a delay, gets a choice All of these results suggest a major role for the hippocampus
between two objects, from which it must choose the one that in spatial memory.
matches the sample. In the delayed nonmatching-to-sample Researchers conducted PET scans on the brains of Lon-
task, the procedure is the same except that the animal must don taxi drivers as they answered questions such as, “What’s
choose the object that is different from the sample (Figure the shortest legal route from the Carlton Tower Hotel to
13.7). In both cases, the animal must remember which object the Sherlock Holmes Museum?” (London taxi drivers are
was present on this occasion, thereby showing what we might well trained and answer with impressive accuracy.) Answer-
call a declarative memory, perhaps an episodic memory. Hip- ing these route questions activated their hippocampus much
pocampal damage strongly impairs performance in most cases more than did answering nonspatial questions. MRI scans
(Zola et al., 2000). also revealed that the taxi drivers have a larger than average
posterior hippocampus and that the longer they had been taxi
The Hippocampus and Spatial Memory drivers, the larger their posterior hippocampus (Maguire et
A second hypothesis focuses on spatial memories. Electrical al., 2000). This surprising result suggests actual growth of
recordings indicate that many neurons in a rat’s hippocampus the adult human hippocampus in response to spatial learning
are tuned to particular spatial locations, responding best when experiences.
an animal is in a particular place (O’Keefe & Burgess, 1996) Consider a couple of ways to test spatial memory in
or looking in a particular direction (Dudchenko & Taube, nonhumans. From a central point, a radial maze has sev-
1997; Rolls, 1996). When people perform spatial tasks, such eral arms—typically eight—some or all of which have a bit
as imagining the best route between one house and another, of food at the end (Figure 13.8). A rat’s best strategy is to
fMRI results show enhanced activity in the hippocampus explore each arm once and only once, remembering where it
(Kumaran & Maguire, 2005). An fMRI study with college has already gone. In a variation of the task, a rat might have
students recorded their responses to photos of familiar cam- to learn that the arms with a rough floor never have food
pus sights. Buildings close to each other on campus produced or that the arms pointing toward the window never have
more similar hippocampal responses than did those that are food. Thus, a rat can make a mistake either by entering a
never-correct arm or by entering a cor-
rect arm twice.
Rats with damage to the hippo-
Delay campus gradually learn not to enter the
never-correct arms, but even after much
training they often enter a correct arm
twice. That is, they forget which arms
they have already tried ( Jarrard, Oka-
ichi, Steward, & Goldschmidt, 1984;
Olton & Papas, 1979; Olton, Walker, &
Monkey lifts sample object to get food. Food is under the new object.
Gage, 1978). With people, psychologi-
cal researchers use a virtual radial maze
Figure 13.7  ​A delayed nonmatching-to-sample task (© Cengage Learning 2013) that the person can navigate on a com-
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 401

(a) (b) (c)

Figure 13.9  ​The Morris water maze


Robert Folz/Visuals Unlimited

An intact rat learns by trial and error. In each case the line traces
the path a rat took to the platform, marked by a circle. On the
fifth trial (A), the rat stayed mainly near the edge and never found
the platform. On the 34th trial (B), it found the platform in 35
seconds. On the 71st trial (C), it went directly to the platform in 6
seconds. (de Bruin, J. P. C., Winkels, W. A. M., & de Brabander, J. M.
Figure 13.8  ​A radial maze (1997). Response learning of rats in a Morris water maze: Involve-
A rat that reenters one arm before trying other arms has made an ment of the medial prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Brain Research,
error of spatial working memory. 85, 47–55.)

puter screen. On this task, people with damage to the hip- the winter, when other food is unavailable. Pinyon jays live
pocampus are slow to learn which arms are never correct, and at lower elevations, bury less food, and depend on it less to
they frequently visit one arm several times before trying all the survive the winter. Scrub jays and Mexican jays, living at still
others (Goodrich-Hunsaker & Hopkins, 2010). That is, the lower altitudes, depend even less on stored food. Of these four
human data resemble those of rats. species, the Clark’s nutcrackers have the largest hippocampus
Another test of spatial memory is the Morris water maze, and perform best on tests of spatial memory. Pinyon jays are
in which a rat swims through murky water to find a rest plat- second best in both respects. On nonspatial tasks, such as color
form that is just under the surface (Figure 13.9). (Rats swim memory, size of hippocampus does not correlate with perfor-
as little as they can. Humans are among the very few land mance (Basil, Kamil, Balda, & Fite, 1996; Olson, Kamil, Balda,
mammals that swim recreationally.) A rat with hippocampal & Nims, 1995) (Figure 13.10). In short, species comparisons
damage slowly learns to find the platform if it always starts show a link between the hippocampus and spatial memory.
from the same place and the rest platform is always in the
same place. However, if it has to start from a different location Hippocampus and Contextual Memory
or if the rest platform occasionally moves from one location A third hypothesis relates to learning about context. You
to another, the rat is disoriented (Eichenbaum, 2000; P. Liu could try this yourself: Describe something that you experi-
& Bilkey, 2001). enced yesterday or today. Then describe something you expe-
If a rat already learned to find the platform before damage rienced months or years ago. How do your two narratives dif-
to the hippocampus, the damage leaves the rat exploring the fer? Your recent narrative probably includes more detail about
water haphazardly, like a rat that had never been in the water where you were, who else was there, where people were sitting,
maze before. It ignores landmarks, including a beacon of light perhaps what some of them were wearing, exactly what cer-
pointing to the platform. Researchers observed that the rat tain people said, maybe the weather outside,
acts as if it not only forgot where the platform was but also and other important and not so important de-
forgot that there even was a platform (R. E. Clark, Broadbent, tails. When you describe something from long
try it 
& Squire, 2007). ago, you remember the highlights or “gist” of the yourself
As with the radial maze, researchers sometimes test people event but fewer details.
with a virtual water maze that they navigate on a computer. The hippocampus is important for remembering details
People with a rare condition called acute transient global amnesia and context. A recent memory, which generally depends on the
have a temporary dysfunction of the hippocampus. If they are hippocampus, includes much detail. As time passes, memory
tested soon after onset of the condition, they are slow to learn becomes less detailed, less dependent on the hippocampus, and
the correct route in a virtual water maze (Bartsch et al., 2010). more dependent on the cerebral cortex (Takehara-Nishiuchi
Interesting evidence for the role of the hippocampus in & McNaughton, 2008). The same is true of rats: When rats
spatial memory comes from comparisons of closely related are trained to do something, and then tested again after a short
species that differ in their spatial memory. Clark’s nutcracker, delay, they remember the response best if they are tested in the
a member of the jay family, lives at high altitudes in western same location. That is, their memory depends on the context.
North America. During the summer and fall, it buries seeds As time passes, the context matters less and less, and to the ex-
in thousands of locations and then digs them up to survive tent that rats remember the response, they remember it equally
402 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

Size of Hippocampus
Habitat Relative to Rest of Brain Spatial Memory Color Memory
Clark’s nutcracker Lives high in mountains; Largest Best Slightly worse
stores food in summer and
relies on finding it to survive
the winter.

Pinyon jay Lives at fairly high altitude; Second largest Second best Slightly better
depends on stored food to
survive the winter.

Scrub jay Stores some food but less Smaller Less good Slightly worse
dependent on it.

Mexican jay Stores some food but less Smaller Less good Slightly better
dependent on it.

Figure 13.10  ​Hippocampus and spatial memory in jays


Of four related species, the species that relies most heavily on stored food to get through the winter has the largest hippocampus and
performs best on laboratory tests of spatial memory. Size of hippocampus does not relate consistently to nonspatial memory. (Based on
results of Basil, Kamil, Balda, & Fite, 1996; Olson, Kamil, Balda, & Nims, 1995) (© Tom Vezo/The Wildlife Collection)

well in a different location. Rats with damage to the hippocam- when they were in room A, they had to dig in flowerpot X
pus, if they learn something at all, show no difference between instead of Y to find food, but in room B they had to dig in
testing in the familiar place and some other place. Their mem- flowerpot Y instead of X, regardless of location of the flow-
ory doesn’t depend on context, presumably because they do not erpots within each room. As already described, most cells in
remember it (Winocur, Moscovitch, & Sekeres, 2007). the hippocampus become active only in a particular location
In humans, recalling a recent memory (which usually in- within a room or other setting. In this experiment, most of
cludes details and context) activates the hippocampus. Recall- those “place” cells responded much more strongly to their pre-
ing an old factual memory may or may not activate the hip- ferred place if the correct kind of flowerpot was in that place
pocampus, but episodic memories, because they necessarily (Komorowski, Manns, & Eichenbaum, 2009).
include some context details, do activate the hippocampus. We have considered three hypotheses of the role of the
This hypothesis is well suited to dealing with the observation hippocampus and found evidence to support each. The views
that people with hippocampal damage have particular trouble are not necessarily in conflict, as the hippocampus probably
with episodic memories. contributes in more than one way. It is also possible that
Single-cell recordings in rats confirm the idea that the hip- researchers will find a way to combine all three hypotheses
pocampus responds to context. In one study, rats learned that into one.
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 403

Stop & Check


7. Suppose a rat is in a radial maze in which six arms have
food once per day, and two other arms never have food.
What kind of mistake does a rat with hippocampal dam-
age make?

8. According to the context hypothesis, why does hippocam- Figure 13.11  ​The “weather” task of probabilistic learning
pal damage impair episodic memory? Each picture provides information that enables a partly accurate
prediction, but together they are highly accurate. (© Cengage
includes some context and detail. Learning 2013)
ANSWERS
membering context, and episodic memory necessarily
8. The hippocampus is especially important for re-
entered today, and it enters a single arm repeatedly.
never correct, it seems to forget which arms it has
7. Although it learns not to enter the arms that are
to any one picture, you could guess correctly most of the time,
but by attending to all three, you could increase your accuracy.
On this task, most normal people quickly adopt a strat-
egy of responding based on one of the pictures, and therefore
getting the correct answer most of the time but not always.
The Basal Ganglia We detect this strategy by the pattern of errors. That strategy
The hippocampus is not responsible for all learning and mem- is based on declarative memory—quickly noticing, for ex-
ory. It is most important for episodic memory—the kind of ample, that a light bulb in the first position usually indicates
memory that develops from a single experience. After dam- rain. However, after many repetitions, gradually people start
age to the hippocampus, learning still occurs, but it occurs doing better, eventually approaching 100% correct, without
gradually over repeated experiences, and it is often the kind of necessarily being able to say what strategy they are following.
memory that is hard to put into words. Gradually the basal ganglia learned the pattern and estab-
For example, imagine the following: You are playing bas- lished a habit. If we test people with Parkinson’s disease, who
ketball, guarding player #22, who sometimes shoots from the have impairments of the basal ganglia, many of them perform
outside and sometimes tries to drive around you to the bas- about the same as normal people at first, because they have
ket. You could guard more effectively if you knew that player’s an intact hippocampus. However, even after many trials, they
likely choice at this moment. If you play against this player continue basing their answers on one picture, and they do not
long enough, you learn what to expect. He or she might be show the gradual improvement that requires the basal gan-
more likely to shoot from the outside when behind on the glia. On other kinds of complex learning tasks, if they don’t
score, less likely if the last outside shot missed, more likely form an explicit, declarative memory, they don’t improve at
late in the game, less likely if player #31 is also in the lineup, all (Moody, Chang, Vanek, & Knowlton, 2010). That is, they
and so forth. None of these cues by itself is an accurate predic- don’t learn habits and implicit memories.
tor. That is, an episodic memory won’t help, because whether People with amnesia from hippocampal damage perform
your opponent shot from the inside or outside on one play randomly on the weather task for many trials, because they
doesn’t provide you with reliable information. However, you form no declarative memories and they cannot remember
might gradually pick up a pattern. When you do, you might that any particular symbol is usually a signal for one type of
say afterward that you just had a “hunch” your opponent was weather or the other. In fact, even after many trials, they can-
going to shoot from outside, and you guarded accordingly— not describe the task or the instructions. However, if they
and therefore won the game, let’s suppose. continue long enough, they show gradual improvement,
Gradual learning like that depends on the basal ganglia. based on habits supported by the basal ganglia (Bayley, Fras-
We could call it implicit learning or habit learning. Let’s con- cino, & Squire, 2005; Shohamy, Myers, Kalanithi, & Gluck,
sider another example, one that many researchers have used. 2008). If the signals switch, so that the signal that formerly
On each trial, you are given three or four pieces of informa- predicted a high chance of rain now predicts a high chance of
tion, typically pictures (Figure 13.11), and your task is to use sun, they are very slow to switch their responses (Shohamy,
that information to predict the weather to be either sunny or Myers, Hopkins, Sage, & Gluck, 2009). When normal peo-
rainy. By trial and error, you quickly discover that none of the ple try to learn a complex task under conditions of extreme
pictures is completely accurate, but each is partly accurate. distraction, they too learn slowly, like people with a damaged
(This task resembles the actual task of weather forecasting.) hippocampus. Their gradual learning under these conditions
For example, perhaps when the first picture is a light bulb in- depends on the basal ganglia (Foerde, Knowlton, & Poldrack,
stead of a candle, it usually rains. A butterfly instead of a fish 2006).
in the second position, and a sailboat instead of a plane in the Together, these results do suggest that the hippocampus is
third position also indicate probable rain. By paying attention more important for declarative memory and the basal ganglia
404 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

more important for procedural memory. However, psycholo- tions or merely the fact that the patient’s past life was, on the
gists no longer believe in a strict separation between hippo- whole, more pleasant than the present.
campal tasks and basal ganglia tasks. Nearly all tasks activate The tendency to confabulate produces a fascinating influ-
both areas (Albouy et al., 2008), and it is possible to shift ence on the strategies for studying. Suppose you have to learn
from one type of memory to the other, even on the same task. a long list of three-word sentences such as: “Medicine cured
hiccups” and “Tourist desired snapshot.” Would you simply
reread the list many times? Or would you alternate between
Stop & Check reading the list and testing yourself?
9. If you learn a skill (e.g., predicting the weather) as a Medicine cured ____________________________.
procedural habit, instead of learning the same skill as a
Tourist desired _____________________________.
declarative memory, how will the outcome differ?
Almost everyone learns better the second way. Complet-
ANSWER
memory. ing the sentences forces you to be more active and calls your
performance than someone learning a declarative
attention to the items you have not yet learned. Korsakoff ’s
patients, however, learn much better the first way, by reading
of cues, you might reach a higher level of accurate

the list over and over. The reason is, when they test themselves,
if the task requires simultaneous attention to a variety
be unable to describe what you have learned. However,
basal ganglia), you will learn more slowly and probably they confabulate. (“Medicine cured headache. Tourist desired
9. If you learn it as a procedural habit (based on the passport.”) Then they remember their confabulation instead of
the correct answer (Hamann & Squire, 1995).

Stop & Check

Other Types of Amnesia 0. On what kind of question is someone with Korsakoff’s


1
syndrome most likely to confabulate?
Different kinds of brain damage produce different types of
amnesia. Here we briefly consider two more examples: Kor- at one time.
ANSWER
sakoff ’s syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease. confabulations are usually statements that were true
answer, such as questions about themselves. Their

Korsakoff’s Syndrome
on questions for which they would expect to know the
10. Patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome confabulate
Korsakoff ’s syndrome, also known as Wernicke-Korsakoff
syndrome, is brain damage caused by prolonged thiamine de-
ficiency. Severe thiamine deficiency occurs mostly in chronic
alcoholics who go for weeks at a time on a diet of nothing
but alcoholic beverages, lacking in vitamins. The brain needs Alzheimer’s Disease
thiamine (vitamin B1) to metabolize glucose, its primary fuel. Another cause of memory loss is Alzheimer’s (AHLTZ-
Prolonged thiamine deficiency leads to a loss or shrinkage of hime-ers) disease. Daniel Schacter (1983) reported playing
neurons throughout the brain. One of the areas most affected golf with an Alzheimer’s patient who remembered the rules
is the dorsomedial thalamus, the main source of input to the and jargon of the game correctly but kept forgetting how
prefrontal cortex. The symptoms of Korsakoff ’s syndrome many strokes he took. On five occasions, he teed off, waited
are similar to those of people with damage to the prefrontal for the other player to tee off, and then teed off again, hav-
cortex, including apathy, confusion, and memory loss. They ing forgotten his first shot. As with other amnesic patients,
also overlap those of hippocampal damage, with major im- Alzheimer’s patients have better procedural than declarative
pairment of episodic memory and sparing of implicit memory. memory. They learn new skills but then surprise themselves
A distinctive symptom of Korsakoff ’s syndrome is con- with their good performance because they don’t remember
fabulation, in which patients fill in memory gaps with doing it before (Gabrieli, Corkin, Mickel, & Growdon, 1993).
guesses. They seldom confabulate on semantic questions such Their memory and alertness vary substantially from time
as “What is the capital of Russia?” or nonsense questions to time, suggesting that many of their problems result from
such as “Who is Princess Lolita?” They confabulate mainly on malfunctioning neurons, rather than the death of neurons
questions about episodic memory, such as “What did you do (Palop, Chin, & Mucke, 2006). Increased arousal improves
last weekend?” (Borsutzky, Fujiwara, Brand, & Markowitsch, memory, and people who drink 3 to 5 cups of coffee per day
2008; Schnider, 2003). Usually, the confabulated answer was are less likely than average to develop Alzheimer’s (Eskelinen,
true at some time in the past but not now, such as, “I went Ngandu, Tuomilehto, Soininen, & Kivipelto, 2009).
dancing,” or “I visited with my children.” Most of the confabu- Alzheimer’s disease gradually progresses to more serious
lated answers are more pleasant than the currently true an- memory loss, confusion, depression, restlessness, hallucinations,
swers (Fotopoulou, Solms, & Turnbull, 2004). That tendency delusions, sleeplessness, and loss of appetite. It occasionally
may reflect the patient’s attempt to maintain pleasant emo- strikes people younger than age 40 but becomes more common
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 405

Image not available due to copyright restrictions

Text not available due to copyright restrictions

with age, affecting almost 5% of people between ages 65 and 74


and almost 50% of people over 85 (Evans et al., 1989).
The first major clue to the cause of Alzheimer’s was the fact
that people with Down syndrome (a type of mental retardation) al-
most invariably get Alzheimer’s disease if they survive into middle
age (Lott, 1982). People with Down syndrome have three copies
of chromosome 21 rather than the usual two. That fact led investi-
gators to examine chromosome 21, where they found a gene linked
to many cases of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (Goate et al.,
1991; Murrell, Farlow, Ghetti, & Benson, 1991). Later research- (a) (b)
ers found two more genes linked to early-onset Alzheimer’s. For
Figure 13.13  ​Neuronal degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease
cases with onset of symptoms after age 60 to 65 (vastly more com- (a) A cell in the prefrontal cortex of a normal human; (b) cells from
mon than early-onset cases), one gene has a significant influence, the same area of cortex in Alzheimer’s disease patients at various
and several other genes appear to be related in one population or stages of deterioration. (© Cengage Learning 2013. After “Dendritic
another (Bertram & Tanzi, 2008). However, the genes linked to changes,” by A. B. Scheibel, p. 70. In B. Reisberg, Ed., Alzheimer’s
the late-onset variety increase the risk only slightly, as opposed to Disease, 1983. Free Press.)
the genes for early onset that increase it strongly. About half of all
patients with late-onset Alzheimer’s disease have no known rela-
tives with the disease (St George-Hyslop, 2000).
Although genes do not completely control Alzheimer’s phate groups to attach to tau proteins. The altered tau can’t
disease, understanding the genes shed light on the disease bind to its usual targets within axons, and so it starts spread-
itself. The genes controlling early-onset Alzheimer’s disease ing into the cell body and dendrites. The attack of tau from
cause a protein called amyloid-b to accumulate both inside within dendrites adds to the attack by amyloid-b, magnify-
and outside neurons (LaFerla, Green, & Oddo, 2007). The ing the damage. Researchers hypothesize that altered tau
impact varies among cells (Abramov et al., 2009; Busche et also increases the production of amyloid-b, causing a vicious
al., 2008), but the net effect is to damage dendritic spines, de- cycle (Ittner & Götz, 2011). The altered tau is principally re-
crease synaptic input, and decrease plasticity (Wei, Nguyen, sponsible for tangles, structures formed from degeneration
Kessels, Hagiwara, Sisodia, & Malinow, 2010). within neurons (Figure 13.14). The pattern of amyloid, tau,
As amyloid damages axons and dendrites, the damaged and other chemicals varies from one Alzheimer’s patient to
structures cluster into structures called plaques (Selkoe, another, and it may be useful to distinguish several subtypes
2000). As the plaques accumulate, the cerebral cortex, hip- of patients (Iqbal & Grundke-Iqbal, 2010).
pocampus, and other areas atrophy (waste away), as Figures At this point, no drug is highly effective for Alzheimer’s
13.12 and 13.13 show. disease, although many new possibilities are under investiga-
In addition to amyloid-b, a second problem relates to the tion (Roberson & Mucke, 2006). The most common treat-
tau protein in the intracellular support structure of axons ment is to give drugs that stimulate acetylcholine receptors or
(Davies, 2000). High levels of amyloid-b cause more phos- prolong acetylcholine release. The result is increased arousal.
406 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

cating that their episodic memories were intact, as well as their


Dr. M. Goedert/Science Photo Library/Photo Researchers

speech and their willingness to cooperate. What was lacking


was their ability to elaborate on a memory spontaneously (Ber-
Plaque
ryhill, Phuong, Picasso, Cabeza, & Olson, 2007). Ordinarily,
when most of us recall an event, one thing reminds us of an-
other, and we start adding one detail after another, until we
have said all that we know. In people with parietal lobe damage,
Tangle that process of associating one piece with another is impaired.
People with damage in the anterior and inferior regions of
the temporal lobe suffer semantic dementia, a loss of seman-
tic memory. One patient while riding down a road saw some
Figure 13.14  ​Cerebral cortex of an Alzheimer’s patient sheep and asked what they were. The problem wasn’t that he
Plaques and tangles result from amyloid-b and abnormal tau couldn’t remember the word sheep. It was as if he had never
protein. (From Taylor, Hardy, & Fischbeck, 2002) seen a sheep before. When another person saw a picture of a
zebra, she called it a horse but then pointed at the stripes and
asked what “those funny things” were. She hadn’t merely lost
An interesting possibility is curcumin, a component of tur- the word zebra but had lost the concept of zebra. Such patients
meric, a spice in Indian curries. Research with animals shows cannot remember the typical color of common fruits and veg-
that curcumin inhibits amyloid-b deposits and phosphate at- etables or the appearance of various animals. Don’t think of the
tachment to tau proteins. Research on possible human appli- anterior and inferior temporal lobe as the sole point of storage
cations has just begun (Hamaguchi, Ono, & Tamada, 2010). for semantic memory. These areas store some of the informa-
tion and serve as a “hub” for communicating with other brain
areas to bring together a full concept (Patterson, Nestor, &
What Patients with Amnesia Teach Us Rogers, 2007). Serious deficits in semantic memory occur only
The study of patients with amnesia reveals that people do not after bilateral damage. People with damage to the temporal
lose all aspects of memory equally. A patient with great difficulty cortex in just one hemisphere perform approximately normally
establishing new memories may remember events from long ago, (Ralph, Cipoloti, Manes, & Patterson, 2010).
and someone with greatly impaired factual memory may learn Parts of the prefrontal cortex, shown in Figure 13.15, are
new skills. Evidently, people have several somewhat independent important for learning about rewards and punishments. The
kinds of memory that depend on different brain areas. basal ganglia also learn about the reward values of various ac-
tions, but they learn slowly, based on the average reward over
a long period of time. The prefrontal cortex responds more
Stop & Check quickly, based on the most recent events. If you are confronted
with an opportunity to make a response, cells in the ventro-
11. How does amyloid-b relate to tau? medial prefrontal cortex respond based on the reward to be
where they add to the damage that amyloid-b caused.
expected, based on past experience. Cells in the orbitofrontal
ANSWER cortex respond based on how that reward compares to other
therefore they spread to cell bodies and dendrites,
tau proteins no longer attach to their usual sites, and possible choices. For example, a $2 reward might be good or
phosphate attachment to tau proteins. The altered bad depending on whether other choices offer a $1 or $5 re-
and outside neurons. Within neurons, it increases ward (Frank & Claus, 2006). Cells in the orbitofrontal cortex
11. The protein amyloid-b accumulates both inside are also important for “self control.” If you have a choice be-
tween a small reward now and a larger one later, you try to
restrain your impulse to take the immediate reward. If the or-
bitofrontal cortex is damaged or temporarily inactivated, you
Other Brain Areas in Memory become more likely to take the immediate reward (Figner et
al., 2010; Sellitto, Ciaramelli, & De Pellegrino, 2010). Chil-
Most of this module has focused on the hippocampus and the dren have trouble restraining their impulses, because the pre-
basal ganglia. Chapter 12 mentioned the importance of the frontal cortex is slow to mature.
amygdala for fear memories. Other brain areas are important
for learning and memory, too. In fact, most of the brain con-
tributes in one way or another. Stop & Check
Investigators asked two patients with parietal lobe damage
to describe various events from their past. When tested this 1
2. Which brain area records the expected gains and losses
way, their episodic memory appeared sparse, almost devoid of associated with possible actions?
details. However, the investigators asked follow-up questions,
ANSWER
12. The prefrontal cortex.
such as, “Where were you?” and “Who else was there at the
time?” Then these patients answered in reasonable detail, indi-
13.1 Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning 407

Anterior cingulate cortex

Orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex

Ventromedial prefrontal cortex

Figure 13.15  ​Three areas important for making decisions


These areas respond to the expected outcome of a decision relative to other possible outcomes and to discrepancies between actual
and expected outcomes. The orbitofrontal cortex gets its name because it is close to the orbit of the eye. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Module 13.1  ■  In Closing

Different Types of Memory

“Overall intelligence,” as measured by an IQ test, is a convenient one but not another. Even memory is composed of separate
fiction. It is convenient because, under most circumstances, abilities, and it is possible to lose one type or aspect of memory
people who are good at one kind of intellectual task are also without impairing others. The study of amnesia shows how the
good at other kinds, so an overall test score makes useful predic- brain operates as a series of partly independent mechanisms
tions. However, it is a fiction because different kinds of abilities serving specific purposes.
rely on different brain processes, and it is possible to damage

summary
  1. Ivan Pavlov suggested that learning depends on the   3. Psychologists distinguish between short-term memory
growth of a connection between two brain areas. Karl and long-term memory. Short-term memory holds only
Lashley showed that learning does not depend on new a small amount of information and retains it only briefly
connections across the cerebral cortex.  390 unless it is constantly rehearsed.  394
  2. Richard Thompson found that some instances of   4. Working memory, a modern alternative to the concept
classical conditioning take place in small areas of the of short-term memory, stores information that one is
cerebellum.  392 currently using.  395
408 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

  5. People with damage to the hippocampus have great   8. Patients with Korsakoff ’s syndrome often fill in their
trouble forming new long-term declarative memories, memory gaps with confabulations, which they then
especially episodic memories. However, they still show remember as if they were true.  404
implicit memory, they still store short-term memories,   9. Alzheimer’s disease is a progressive disease, most
and they still form new procedural memories.  396 common in old age, characterized by impaired memory
  6. Several theories about the hippocampus focus on its and attention. It is related to deposition of amyloid-b
role in declarative memory, spatial memory, and protein in the brain. The accumulating amyloid leads to
memory for details and context.  399 abnormalities of the tau protein, causing additional
  7. Whereas the hippocampus is important for rapid difficulties.  404
storage of an event, the basal ganglia learn gradually. 10. Other brain areas are important for elaborating episodic
Gradual learning is important for developing habits and memories, for semantic memories, and for memories of
for seeing complex patterns that may not be evident on the reward or punishment values of various possible
a single trial.  403 actions.  406

Key Terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
Alzheimer’s disease  404 delayed response task  395 punishment  390
amnesia  396 engram  391 radial maze  400
amyloid-b  405 episodic memory  398 reconsolidation  395
anterograde amnesia  396 equipotentiality  392 reinforcer  390
classical conditioning  390 explicit memory  399 retrograde amnesia  396
conditioned response (CR)  390 implicit memory  399 semantic dementia  406
conditioned stimulus (CS)  390 instrumental conditioning  390 short-term memory  394
confabulation  404 Korsakoff ’s syndrome  404 tau protein  405
consolidation  395 lateral interpositus nucleus (LIP)  393 unconditioned response
declarative memory  399 long-term memory  394 (UCR)  390
delayed matching-to-sample task  400 mass action  392 unconditioned stimulus
delayed nonmatching-to-sample Morris water maze  401 (UCS)  390
task  400 procedural memory  399 working memory  395

Thought Question
Lashley sought to find the engram, the physiological repre- someone have to demonstrate before you could conclude
sentation of learning. In general terms, how would you rec- that a particular change in the nervous system was really an
ognize an engram if you saw one? That is, what would engram?
Module 13.2

Storing Information
in the Nervous System

I
f you walk through a field, are the footprints that you leave I feel as though I were in the bathroom at school.
“memories”? How about the mud that you pick up on your I see myself at the corner of Jacob and Washington in South
shoes? If the police wanted to know who walked across Bend, Indiana.
that field, a forensics expert could check your shoes to answer I remember myself at the railroad station in Vanceburg,
the question. And yet we would not call these physical traces Kentucky; it is winter and the wind is blowing outside, and
memories in the usual sense. I am waiting for a train.
Similarly, when a pattern of activity passes through Penfield (1955; Penfield & Perot, 1963) suggested
the brain, it leaves a path of physical changes, but not every that each neuron stores a particular memory, like a
change is a memory. The task of finding how the brain stores videotape of one’s life. However, brain stimulation
memories is a little like searching for the proverbial needle in rarely elicited a memory of a specific event. Usually, it
a haystack, and researchers have explored many avenues that evoked vague sights and sounds, or recollections of
seemed promising for a while but now seem fruitless. common experiences such as “seeing a bed” or “hear-
ing a choir sing ‘White Christmas.’” Stimulation al-
most never elicited memories of doing anything—just
Applications and Extensions of seeing and hearing. Also, some patients reported
events that they had never actually experienced, such
Blind Alleys and Abandoned Mines as being chased by a robber or seeing Christ descend
Textbooks, this one included, concentrate mostly on suc- from the sky. In short, the stimulation produced some-
cessful research that led to our current understanding of thing more like a dream than a memory.
a field. You may get the impression that science pro- 2. G. A. Horridge (1962) apparently demonstrated that de-
gresses smoothly, with each investigator contributing to capitated cockroaches can learn. First he cut the connec-
the body of knowledge. However, if you look at old jour- tions between a cockroach’s head and the rest of its body.
nals or textbooks, you will find discussions of many Then he suspended the cockroach so that its legs dangled
“promising” or “exciting” findings that we disregard to- just above a surface of water. An electrical circuit was ar-
day. Scientific research does not progress straight from ranged as in Figure 13.16 so that the roach’s leg received
ignorance to enlightenment. It explores one direction af- a shock whenever it touched the water. Each experimental
ter another, a little like a rat in a complex maze, abandon- roach was paired with a control roach that got a leg shock
ing the dead ends and pursuing arms that lead further. whenever the first roach did. Only the experimental roach
The problem with the maze analogy is that an investiga- had any control over the shock, however. This kind of ex-
tor seldom runs into a wall that clearly identifies the end of periment is known as a “yoked-control” design.
a route. A better analogy is a prospector digging for gold, Over 5 to 10 minutes, roaches in the experimental
never certain whether to abandon an unprofitable spot or to group increased a response of tucking the leg under the
keep digging just a little longer. Many once-exciting lines of body to avoid shocks. Roaches in the control group did
research in the physiology of learning are now of little more not, on average, change their leg position as a result of
than historical interest. Here are three examples. the shocks. Thus, the changed response apparently
1.  Wilder Penfield sometimes performed brain surgery for se- qualifies as learning and not as an accidental byproduct
vere epilepsy on conscious patients who had only scalp an- of the shocks.
esthesia. When he applied a brief, weak electrical stimulus These experiments initially seemed a promising way to
to part of the brain, the patient could describe the experi- study learning in a simple nervous system (Eisenstein &
ence that the stimulation evoked. Stimulation of the tempo- Cohen, 1965). Unfortunately, decapitated cockroaches
ral cortex sometimes evoked vivid descriptions such as: learn slowly, and the results vary sharply from one individ-

409
410 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

Donald O. Hebb
(1904–1985)
Modern psychology takes completely for
Stimulator granted that behavior and neural function
are perfectly correlated…. There is no sep-

Mary Ellen Hebb


arate soul or life force to stick a finger into
the brain now and then and make neural
cells do what they would not otherwise….
It is quite conceivable that some day the
Water assumption will have to be rejected. But it
is important also to see that we have not reached that day yet….
One cannot logically be a determinist in physics and chemistry
and biology, and a mystic in psychology. (Hebb, 1946, p. xiii)

Figure 13.16  ​Learning in a headless cockroach?


The decapitated cockroach, suspended just above the water,
receives a shock whenever its hind leg touches the water. A
Learning and the Hebbian
cockroach in the control group gets a shock whenever the first Synapse
roach does regardless of its own behavior. According to some
reports, the experimental roach learned to keep its leg out of Research on the physiology of learning began with Ivan Pav-
the water. (From G. A. Horridge, “Learning of Leg Position by the lov’s concept of classical conditioning. Although, as we con-
Ventral Nerve Cord in Headless Insects.” Proceedings of the sidered earlier, that theory led Karl Lashley to an unsuccessful
Royal Society of London, B, 157, 1962, 33–52. Copyright © search for connections in the cerebral cortex, it also stimulated
1962 The Royal Society of London. Reprinted by permission of Donald Hebb to propose a mechanism for change at a synapse.
the Royal Society of London and G. A. Horridge.) Hebb suggested that when the axon of neuron A “repeatedly
or persistently takes part in firing [cell B], some growth process or
metabolic change takes place in one or both cells” that increases
ual to another, limiting the usefulness of the results. After the subsequent ability of axon A to excite cell B (Hebb, 1949, p.
a handful of studies, interest in this line of research faded. 62). In other words, an axon that has successfully stimulated cell
3. In the 1960s and early 1970s, several investigators pro- B in the past becomes even more successful in the future.
posed that each memory is coded as a specific mole- Consider how this process relates to classical condition-
cule, probably RNA or protein. The boldest test of that ing. Suppose axon A initially excites cell B slightly, and axon
hypothesis was an attempt to transfer memories chemi- C excites B more strongly. If A and C fire together, their com-
cally from one individual to another. James McConnell bined effect on B may produce an action potential. You might
(1962) reported that, when planaria (flatworms) canni- think of axon A as the CS and axon C as the UCS. Pairing
balized other planaria that had been classically condi- activity in axons A and C increases the future effect of A on B.
tioned to respond to a light, they apparently “remem- A synapse that increases in effectiveness because of simul-
bered” what the cannibalized planaria had learned. At taneous activity in the presynaptic and postsynaptic neurons is
least they learned the response faster than planaria called a Hebbian synapse. In Chapter 6, we encountered exam-
generally do. ples of this type of synapse. In the development of the visual sys-
Inspired by that report, other investigators trained tem, if an axon from the left eye consistently fires at the same time
rats to approach a clicking sound for food (Babich, as one from the right eye, a neuron in the visual cortex increases
Jacobson, Bubash, & Jacobson, 1965). After the rats its response to both of them. Such synapses may also be critical
were well trained, the experimenters ground up their for many kinds of associative learning. Neuroscientists have dis-
brains, extracted RNA, and injected it into untrained covered much about the mechanisms of Hebbian synapses.
rats. The recipient rats learned to approach the clicking
sound faster than rats in the control group did. Stop & Check
That report led to a wealth of studies on the transfer 1
3. How can a Hebbian synapse account for the basic phe-
of training by brain extracts. In some of these experi- nomena of classical conditioning?
ments, rats that received brain extracts from a trained
group showed apparent memory of the task, whereas ANSWER
conditioned response.
those that received extracts from an untrained group did of the postsynaptic cell, which we can regard as a
not (Dyal, 1971; Fjerdingstad, 1973). The results were On later trials, it will produce a bigger depolarization
inconsistent and unreplicable, however, even within a
strengthens the response of the cell to the CS axon.
single laboratory (L. T. Smith, 1975). Many laboratories
produces an action potential and in the process
a weaker (CS) axon with a stronger (UCS) axon
failed to find any hint of a transfer effect. By the mid- 13. In a Hebbian synapse, pairing the activity of
1970s, most biological psychologists saw no point in
continuing this research. ■
13.2 Storing Information in the Nervous System 411

Single-Cell Mechanisms of Eric R. Kandel


Invertebrate Behavior Change The questions posed by higher cognitive
processes such as learning and memory
If we are going to look for a needle in a haystack, a good strat- are formidable, and we have only begun to
egy is to look in a small haystack. Therefore, many research- explore them. Although elementary as-
ers have turned to studies of invertebrates. Vertebrate and in- pects of simple forms of learning have
vertebrate nervous systems are organized differently, but the been accessible to molecular analysis in

Eric Kandel
chemistry of the neuron, the principles of the action potential, invertebrates, we are only now beginning
and the neurotransmitters and their receptors are the same. If to know a bit about the genes and proteins
involved in more complex, hippocampus
we identify the physical basis of learning and memory in an in-
based learning processes of mammals.
vertebrate, we have at least a hypothesis of what might work in
vertebrates. (Biologists have long used this strategy for study-
ing genetics, embryology, and other biological processes.)

Aplysia’s gills with a brief jet of seawater, it withdraws at first,


Aplysia as an Experimental Animal but after many repetitions, it stops responding. The decline
Aplysia, a marine invertebrate related to the common slug, has in response is not due to muscle fatigue because, even after
been a popular animal for studies of the physiology of learn- habituation has occurred, direct stimulation of the motor
ing (Figure 13.17). Compared to vertebrates, it has fewer neu- neuron produces a full-size muscle contraction (Kupfermann,
rons, many of which are large and easy to study. Moreover, Castellucci, Pinsker, & Kandel, 1970). We can also rule out
unlike vertebrates, Aplysia neurons are virtually identical from changes in the sensory neuron. The sensory neuron still gives
one individual to another so that different investigators can a full, normal response to stimulation; it merely fails to ex-
study the properties of the same neuron. cite the motor neuron as much as before (Kupfermann et al.,
One commonly studied behavior is the withdrawal re- 1970). We are therefore left with the conclusion that habitu-
sponse: If someone touches the siphon, mantle, or gill of an ation in Aplysia depends on a change in the synapse between
Aplysia (Figure 13.18), the animal vigorously withdraws the the sensory neuron and the motor neuron (Figure 13.19).
irritated structure. Investigators have traced the neural path
from the touch receptors through other neurons to the motor
neurons that direct the response. Using this neural pathway, in- Sensitization in Aplysia
vestigators have studied changes in behavior as a result of expe- If you experience an unexpected, intense pain, you tempo-
rience. In 2000, Eric Kandel won a Nobel prize for this work. rarily react more strongly than usual to other strong, sudden
stimuli. This phenomenon is sensitization, an increase in re-
sponse to mild stimuli as a result of exposure to more intense
Habituation in Aplysia stimuli. Similarly, a strong stimulus almost anywhere on Ap-
Habituation is a decrease in response to a stimulus that is lysia’s skin intensifies a later withdrawal response to a touch.
presented repeatedly and accompanied by no change in other Researchers traced sensitization to changes at identified
stimuli. For example, if your clock chimes every hour, you synapses (Cleary, Hammer, & Byrne, 1989; Dale, Schacher, &
gradually respond less and less. If we repeatedly stimulate an Kandel, 1988; Kandel & Schwartz, 1982). Strong stimulation

Tactile stimulus
Mantle shelf
Marty Snyderman/Visuals Unlimited

Gill

Figure 13.18  ​Touching an Aplysia causes a withdrawal


response
Figure 13.17  ​Aplysia, a marine mollusk The sensory and motor neurons controlling this reaction have
A full-grown animal is a little larger than the average human hand. been identified and studied. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
412 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

Branch to Muscle that


other neurons retracts gill

Sensory Motor
neuron neuron

Gill
Siphon After habituation, no No fatigue
change in number of of the muscle
action potentials after
a stimulation In habituation,
decreased release
of neurotransmitter
at this synapse

Figure 13.19  ​Habituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia


Touching the siphon causes gill withdrawal. After many repetitions, the response habituates (declines) because of decreased transmis-
sion at the synapse between the sensory neuron and the motor neuron. (Redrawn from “Neuronal Mechanisms of Habituation and Disha-
bituation of the Gill-Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia,” by V. Castellucci, H. Pinsker, I. Kupfermann, and E. Kandel, Science, 167, pp. 1745–1748.
Copyright © 1970 by AAAS. Used by permission of AAAS and V. Castellucci.)

on the skin excites a facilitating interneuron that releases serotonin this: One or more axons connected to a dendrite bombard it
(5-HT) onto the presynaptic terminals of many sensory neu- with a rapid series of stimuli. The burst of intense stimulation
rons. Serotonin blocks potassium channels in these membranes. leaves some of the synapses potentiated (more responsive to
The result is that after later action potentials, the membrane takes new input of the same type) for minutes, days, or weeks.
longer than usual to repolarize (because potassium is slow to flow LTP shows three properties that make it an attractive can-
out of the cell). Therefore, the presynaptic neuron continues re- didate for a cellular basis of learning and memory:
leasing its neurotransmitter for longer than usual. Repeating this
■ specificity—If some of the synapses onto a cell have been
process causes the sensory neuron to synthesize new proteins
highly active and others have not, only the active ones
that produce long-term sensitization (C. H. Bailey, Giustetto,
become strengthened.
Huang, Hawkins, & Kandel, 2000). This research shows how it
■ cooperativity—Nearly simultaneous stimulation by
is possible to explain one example of behavioral plasticity in terms
of molecular events. Later studies explored mechanisms of classi- two or more axons produces LTP much more strongly
cal and instrumental conditioning in Aplysia. than does repeated stimulation by just one axon.
■ associativity—Pairing a weak input with a strong input

enhances later response to the weak input, as illustrated


Stop & Check in Figure 13.20. In this regard, LTP matches what we
would expect of Hebbian synapses. In some cases, a
14. When serotonin blocks potassium channels on the pre-
synapse that was almost completely inactive before LTP
synaptic terminal, what is the effect on transmission?
becomes effective afterward (Kerchner & Nicoll, 2008).
ANSWER
rotransmitters, producing an increased response.
The opposite change, long-term depression (LTD), a
prolonged decrease in response at a synapse, occurs for axons
potential and therefore prolongs the release of neu-

that have been less active than others, such as axon 3 in Figure
14. Blocking potassium channels prolongs the action

13.20 (Collingridge, Peineau, Howland, & Wang, 2010). You


can think of this as a compensatory process. As one synapse
strengthens, another weakens (Royer & Paré, 2003). If learn-
Long-Term Potentiation ing produced only a strengthening of synapses, then every
time you learned something, your brain would get more and
in Vertebrates more active, burning more and more fuel!
Since the work of Sherrington and Cajal, most neuroscientists
have assumed that learning depends on changes at synapses,
and the work on Aplysia supports that idea. The first evidence Biochemical Mechanisms
for a similar process among vertebrates came from studies of Determining how LTP or LTD occurs has been a huge re-
neurons in the rat hippocampus (Bliss & Lømo, 1973). The search challenge because each neuron has many tiny synapses,
phenomenon, known as long-term potentiation (LTP), is sometimes in the tens of thousands. Isolating the chemical
13.2 Storing Information in the Nervous System 413

Axon 1
Strong stimulation

Axon 3
Synapse No stimulation
strengthened
Axon 2
Weak stimulation

Synapse not strengthened,


probably weakened

Synapse
strengthened

Simultaneous strong activation


of two axons

4
Prolonged enhanced
response
measured in mV
EPSP in Axon 2,

Baseline
2 response
Figure 13.20  ​Associativity in long-term
LTP
potentiation
0
The response to axon 2 is initially weak. Then it is
briefly paired with rapid stimulation in axon 1, leading
to strong depolarization of the dendrite. Afterward,
the response to axon 2 (as well as axon 1) is en-
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
hanced. (© Argosy Publishing Inc.)
Time (min)

changes at any one synapse takes an enormous amount of acid (abbreviated AMPA). The NMDA receptor is also ordi-
creative research. We shall discuss LTP in the hippocampus, narily excited only by glutamate, but it can respond to a drug
where it occurs most readily and where its mechanisms have called N-methyl-D-aspartate (abbreviated NMDA).
been most extensively studied. Both are ionotropic receptors. That is, when they are
stimulated, they open a channel to let ions enter the post-
AMPA and NMDA Synapses synaptic cell. The AMPA receptor is a typical ionotropic
In a few cases, LTP depends on changes at GABA synapses receptor that opens sodium channels. The NMDA recep-
(Nugent, Penick, & Kauer, 2007), but in most cases, it de- tor, however, is different: Its response to the transmitter
pends on changes at glutamate synapses. The brain has several glutamate depends on the degree of polarization across the
types of receptors for glutamate, its most abundant transmit- membrane. When glutamate attaches to an NMDA recep-
ter. Neuroscientists identify different dopamine receptors by tor while the membrane is at its resting potential, the ion
number, such as D1 and D2, and different GABA receptors channel is usually blocked by magnesium ions. (Magnesium
by letter, such as GABAA. For glutamate, they named the dif- ions, positively charged, are attracted to the negative charge
ferent receptors after certain drugs that stimulate them. Here inside the cells but do not fit through the NMDA channel.)
we are interested in two types of glutamate receptors, called The NMDA channel opens only if the magnesium leaves,
AMPA and NMDA. The AMPA receptor is excited by and the surest way to detach the magnesium is to depolarize
the neurotransmitter glutamate, but it can also respond to a the membrane, decreasing the negative charge that attracts
drug called a-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic it (Figure 13.21).
414 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

Axon
releases
glutamate

G G
Glutamate molecule G
Magnesium ion
attached to
blocking channel
receptor
G G Mg

AMPA NMDA
receptor receptor
(Nothing enters
usually)

Na+
enters
Dendrite

Figure 13.21  ​The AMPA and NMDA receptors before LTP


Glutamate attaches to both receptors. At the AMPA receptor, it opens a channel to let sodium ions enter. At the NMDA receptor, it binds
but usually fails to open the channel, which is blocked by magnesium ions. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Suppose an axon releases glutamate repeatedly. Better yet, 2008; Minichiello, 2009; Silva, Zhou, Rogerson, Shobe, &
let’s activate two axons repeatedly, side by side on the same Balaji, 2009), as well as others. When neurons are repeatedly
dendrite. So many sodium ions enter through the AMPA activated, only those with the greatest production of these
channels that the dendrite becomes strongly depolarized. The chemicals will undergo LTP (Han et al., 2007). The final
depolarization displaces the magnesium molecules, enabling outcome varies, including these possibilities:
glutamate to open the NMDA channel. At that point, both
sodium and calcium enter through the NMDA channel (Fig- ■ The dendrite builds more AMPA receptors or moves
ure 13.22). old ones into better positions (Poncer & Malinow, 2001;
The entry of calcium is the key to maintaining LTP. Takahashi, Svoboda, & Malinow, 2003).
When calcium enters through the NMDA channel, it ac- ■ The dendrite may make more branches and spines,

tivates a protein called CaMKII (a-calcium-calmodulin- thus forming additional synapses with the same axon
dependent protein kinase II) (Lisman, Schulman, & Cline, (Engert & Bonhoeffer, 1999; Toni, Buchs, Nikonenko,
2002; Otmakhov et al., 2004). CaMKII sets in motion Bron, & Muller, 1999; Xu et al., 2009) (Figure 13.23).
a series of reactions leading to release of a protein called Recall from Chapter 5 that enriched experience also
CREB—cyclic adenosine monophosphate responsive ele- leads to increased dendritic branching.
ment-binding protein. (You can see why it’s almost always ■ Phosphate groups attach to certain AMPA receptors

abbreviated.) CREB goes to the nucleus of the cell and regu- to make them more responsive than before.
lates the expression of several genes. In some cases, the al- ■ In some cases, the neuron makes more NMDA recep-
tered gene expression lasts for months or years, long enough tors (Grosshans, Clayton, Coultrap, & Browning, 2002).
to account for long-term memory (Miller et al., 2010). The
effects of CaMKII and CREB are magnified by BDNF— Let’s summarize: When glutamate massively stimulates
brain-derived neurotrophic factor, a neurotrophin similar to AMPA receptors, the resulting depolarization enables gluta-
nerve growth factor that Chapter 5 discussed. Persisting activ- mate to stimulate nearby NMDA receptors also. Stimulation of
ity at synapses leads to action potentials that start in axons but the NMDA receptors lets calcium enter the cell, where it sets
back-propagate into the dendrites, which then release BDNF. into motion a series of changes that potentiate the dendrite’s fu-
The formation and maintenance of LTP depends on all these ture responsiveness to glutamate at AMPA receptors. After LTP
chemicals—CaMKII, CREB, and BDNF (Kuczewski et al., occurs, NMDA receptors revert to their original condition.
13.2 Storing Information in the Nervous System 415

Axon Displaced Axon


releases magnesium releases
glutamate molecule glutamate
repeatedly repeatedly

G G Mg++
G G G G
G G G
G G G
G G G G
G G
NMDA
AMPA receptor AMPA
receptor receptor

Na+ and
Much Na+ Ca++ enter Much Na+
enters enters
Dendrite, much depolarized

Figure 13.22  ​The AMPA and NMDA receptors during LTP


If one or more AMPA receptors have been repeatedly stimulated, enough sodium enters to largely depolarize the dendrite’s membrane.
Doing so displaces the magnesium ions and enables glutamate to open the NMDA receptor, through which sodium and calcium enter.
(© Cengage Learning 2013)

Figure 13.23  ​One way in which LTP occurs


In some cases, the dendrite makes new branches to the same axon, increasing the overall stimulation. (Based on Toni, Buchs, Nikonenko,
Bron, & Muller, 1999)

Once LTP has been established, it no longer depends on which synapses to strengthen? The answer is that during the
NMDA synapses. Drugs that block NMDA synapses prevent period of heavy bombardment at a synapse, chemical changes
the establishment of LTP, but they do not interfere with the at that synapse mark it for later identification by chemicals
maintenance of LTP that was already established (Gustafsson circulating through the cell (Redondo & Morris, 2011; Wang
& Wigström, 1990; Uekita & Okaichi, 2005). In other words, et al., 2009).
once LTP occurs, the AMPA receptors stay potentiated, re-
gardless of what happens to the NMDAs. Presynaptic Changes
This question might occur to you: LTP depends on chemi- The changes just described occur in the postsynaptic neuron.
cals that alter gene expression, but LTP occurs only at the syn- In many cases, LTP depends on changes in the presynaptic
apses that were highly activated. So how do the genes know neuron instead or in addition. Extensive stimulation of a post-
416 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

synaptic cell causes it to release a retrograde transmitter that ing fMRI found that progressively older events produced more
travels back to the presynaptic cell to modify it. In many cases, activity in the cerebral cortex and less in the hippocampus and
that retrograde transmitter is nitric oxide (NO). As a result, amygdala (Smith & Squire, 2009). That is, we can demonstrate
a presynaptic neuron decreases its threshold for producing a shift to the cerebral cortex both over a period of one day and
action potentials (Ganguly, Kiss, & Poo, 2000), increases its over a period of many years! At this point we understand LTP
release of neurotransmitter (Zakharenko, Zablow, & Siegel- better than we understand the gradual shift of representation
baum, 2001), expands its axon (Routtenberg, Cantallops, Zaf- to the cortex.
futo, Serrano, & Namgung, 2000), and releases its transmitter
from additional sites along its axon (Reid, Dixon, Takahashi,
Bliss, & Fine, 2004). In short, LTP reflects increased activity
by the presynaptic neuron as well as increased responsiveness
Improving Memory
by the postsynaptic neuron. Most biological research on learning and memory focuses on
basic mechanisms, in hopes of practical applications later, af-
ter we understand the process better. Is there anything we can
Stop & Check do toward improving memory?
Understanding the mechanisms of LTP may enable re-
15. Before LTP: In the normal state, what is the effect of glu-
searchers to understand what could impair or improve mem-
tamate at the AMPA receptors? At the NMDA receptors?
ory. LTP depends on production of several proteins, and en-
16. During the formation of LTP: When a burst of intense hancing production of these proteins enhances memory in
stimulation releases much more glutamate than usual at rodents (Routtenberg et al., 2000). Drugs that inhibit their
two or more incoming axons, what is the effect of the glu- production weakens memory, even if the drugs are given days
tamate at the AMPA receptors? At the NMDA receptors? after the training (Shema, Sacktor, & Dudai, 2007). Several
Which ions enter at the NMDA receptors? pharmaceutical companies are investigating drugs that might
improve learning by enhancing LTP (Farah et al., 2004).
17. After the neuron has gone through LTP: What is now the
Moderate doses of stimulant drugs enhance learning by in-
effect of glutamate at the AMPA receptors? At the NMDA
creasing arousal. Caffeine is one example, and methylphenidate
receptors?
(Ritalin) is another. Many patients with Alzheimer’s disease
usually ineffective.
take drugs that facilitate acetylcholine by blocking the enzyme
ANSWERS that degrades it (Farah et al., 2004). Other drugs recommended
of AMPA receptors. At the NMDA receptors, it is again
than before, mainly because of an increased number for memory improvement have more doubtful effects. You may
lished, glutamate stimulates the AMPA receptors more have heard claims that the herb Ginkgo biloba improves mem-
sodium enter there. 17. After LTP has been estab- ory. Drug companies face stiff regulation by the Food and Drug
excite the NMDA receptors also. Both calcium and Administration before they can market a new drug, but a com-
dendrite. This depolarization enables glutamate to pany marketing an herb or other naturally occurring substance
stimulates the AMPA receptors, thus depolarizing the does not have to do any research at all, provided that the label or
formation of LTP, the massive glutamate input strongly advertisement does not claim medical benefits. You may see ad-
because magnesium blocks them. 16. During the
vertisements for vitamin pills or other supplements that brag of
containing Ginkgo biloba. You may also notice that the ads gen-
but usually has little effect at the NMDA receptors

erally leave it to your imagination what good, if any, this supple-


15. Before LTP, glutamate stimulates AMPA receptors

ment does. Some early research on Ginkgo biloba suggested


mild benefits to a small percentage of Alzheimer’s patients or
other older adults with impaired blood flow to the brain. How-
Consolidation, Revisited ever, a more extensive study of more than 3,000 people tested
Earlier in the chapter, we encountered the concept that a short- in many ways over six years found that the substance has no
term memory can be consolidated into a long-term memory. benefits (Snitz et al., 2009).
Long-term potentiation in the hippocampus is important for Researchers using biotechnological methods have found
certain types of learning, but as time passes and learning pro- ways to alter gene expression in mice, enhancing memory in cer-
ceeds, the memory becomes less dependent on the hippocampus tain ways. However, so far the benefits come with a cost (Lehrer,
and more dependent on the cerebral cortex (Sacco & Sacchetti, 2009). Mice with increased expression of a gene that enhances
2010). This process is gradual over varying periods of time. In NMDA receptors show faster learning, but also chronic pain.
one study, fMRI showed that after people learned some associa- Mice with another variant gene learn complex mazes faster than
tions, they showed more activity in the hippocampus after 15 usual, but are worse than average at learning simple mazes. An-
minutes, and more activity in the cerebral cortex after 24 hours other type of mouse learns quickly, at the cost of learning fears
(Takashima et al., 2009). In another study, people aged 51–64 quickly and failing to unlearn the fears. Chemically improving
answered detailed questions about news events of the previous memory does not appear to be a simple matter.
30 years—the kind of events you might remember only if you By far the best way to improve learning is by the old-
were attending to news events at the time. Measurements us- fashioned way of studying better in the first place. Still, research-
13.2 Storing Information in the Nervous System 417

ers have found the biological basis for why one kind of study creased brain activity improves memory remains uncertain, but
technique works. Suppose someone tells you a miscellaneous at least we know where to look. Furthermore, it confirms the
series of facts. Later you would remember a few of them. You importance of stirring someone’s curiosity, as a study technique.
would remember more if someone engages your curiosity first
by asking questions. For example, do you know which musical
instrument was invented to sound like human singing? Take
Stop & Check
a guess. How sure are you? How curious are you to find out
whether your guess was right? If you rate your curiosity high, 1
8. At this point, what type of drug or chemical is most clearly
you will remember the correct answer (violin) much better than shown to improve memory without unacceptable side
if someone had told you that fact without first asking you to effects?
guess. The research finds that activity increases in several brain
areas while you are waiting for the answer, especially if you rate
does not support their use at this time.
ANSWER
your curiosity high. Activity also increases greatly in several
Other drugs may become helpful, but the evidence
18. Caffeine and other stimulants produce benefits.
other areas when you hear the correct answer, especially if your
guess had been wrong (Kang et al., 2009). Exactly how this in-

Module 13.2  ■  In Closing

The Physiology of Memory

In this module, we examined biochemical changes at synapses. of information, you don’t want or need to keep everything for-
After we understand these mechanisms more completely, what ever. Similarly, you might not want your brain to retain every
can we do with the information? Presumably, we will help peo- experience, even if it had unlimited storage capacity. The ideal
ple overcome or prevent memory deterioration. We can expect memory would not just record more information. It would
much better therapies for Alzheimer’s disease and so forth. faithfully record the important information and discard the
Should we also look forward to improving memory for normal rest. If we improve memory in any way, we will still want to
people? Would you like to have a supermemory? maintain that selectivity.
Maybe, but let’s be cautious. Even though you could add
memory chips to your computer to store ever-larger quantities

Summary
1. A Hebbian synapse is one that is strengthened by being membrane. The depolarization removes magnesium ions
repeatedly active when the postsynaptic neuron produces that had been blocking NMDA receptors. Glutamate is
action potentials.  410 then able to excite the NMDA receptors, opening a
2. Habituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia channel for calcium ions to enter the neuron.  413
depends on a mechanism that decreases the release of 7. When calcium enters through the NMDA-controlled
transmitter from a particular presynaptic neuron.  411 channels, it activates a protein that sets in motion a series
3. Sensitization of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia of events that build more AMPA receptors and increase
occurs when serotonin blocks potassium channels in a the growth of dendritic branches. These changes increase
presynaptic neuron and thereby prolongs the release of the later responsiveness of the dendrite to incoming
transmitter from that neuron.  411 glutamate at AMPA receptors.  414
4. Long-term potentiation (LTP) is an enhancement of 8. At many synapses, LTP relates to increased release of
response at certain synapses because of a brief but intense transmitter from the presynaptic neuron, in addition to
series of stimuli delivered to a neuron, generally by two or or instead of changes in the postsynaptic neuron.  415
more axons delivering simultaneous inputs.  412 9. Although researchers hope to develop drugs or proce-
5. If axons are active at a very slow rate, their synapses may dures to improve memory, at this point no such proce-
decrease in responsiveness—a process known as long- dure is safe and effective, with the exception of mild
term depression (LTD).  412 stimulants such as caffeine.  416
6. LTP in hippocampal neurons occurs as follows: Repeated
glutamate excitation of AMPA receptors depolarizes the
418 Chapter 13 The Biology of Learning and Memory

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
AMPA receptor  413 habituation  411 NMDA receptor  413
associativity  412 Hebbian synapse  410 retrograde transmitter  416
BDNF  414 long-term depression (LTD)  412 sensitization  411
cooperativity  412 long-term potentiation (LTP)  412 specificity  412

Thought Questions
1. If a synapse has already developed LTP once, should it 2. Dopamine facilitates activity at many AMPA
be easier or more difficult to get it to develop LTP synapses (Tye et al., 2010). How might this fact help
again? Why? explain how methylphenidate (Ritalin) improves
learning?

chapter 13 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note-taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
13.2 Storing Information in the Nervous System 419

Videos
Also available—
■ Mike, an Amnesic Patient
Tom, a Patient with Alzheimer’s

© 2013 Cengage Learning


■ Brain Food

■ Boosting your Memory

Neural Networks and Memory

Animations
The Retina nnn (Figure x.x) Also available—
© 2013 Cengage Learning

■ Learning Module: Classical Conditioning

Habituation

try it
yourself
Try It Yourself Online online
© 2013 Cengage Learning

Long-Term Potentiation

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Eichenbaum, H. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of memory. New York: Oxford University
Press. Thoughtful treatment of both the behavioral and physiological aspects of memory.
Schnider, A. (2008). The confabulating mind: How the brain creates reality. New York: Oxford. A
study of people with Korsakoff ’s syndrome and other conditions that lead to confident but false
memory reports.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, such as one about Alzheimer’s disease and another about memory loss.
Daly & Newton/Getty Images
Cognitive Functions 14
Chapter Outline 2. For most people, the left hemisphere is specialized for
language and analytical processing. The right hemisphere
Module 14.1 ​Lateralization of Function
is specialized for certain complex visuospatial tasks and
The Left and Right Hemispheres
synthetic processing.
Visual and Auditory Connections to the Hemispheres
Cutting the Corpus Callosum 3. The language specializations of the human brain are
Development of Lateralization and Handedness enormous elaborations of features that are present in
Avoiding Overstatements other primates.
In Closing: One Brain, Two Hemispheres 4. Abnormalities of the left hemisphere can lead to a great
variety of specific language impairments.
Module 14.2  ​Evolution and Physiology of Language
5. Stimuli become conscious when corresponding brain
Nonhuman Precursors of Language
activity reaches a high enough level, spreading through
How Did Humans Evolve Language?
much of the cerebral cortex.
Brain Damage and Language
Music and Language
Dyslexia
In Closing: Language and the Brain
Module 14.3  ​Conscious and Unconscious Processes
and Attention
The Mind–Brain Relationship
Brain Activity Associated with Consciousness
Attention
In Closing: Attending to Attention and Being Conscious of
Consciousness
Interactive Exploration and Study

B
iological explanations of vision, hearing, and move-
Main Ideas ment are fairly detailed. Research progresses, mainly
1. The left and right hemispheres of the brain communicate because researchers can measure the stimuli and be-
primarily through the corpus callosum, although other haviors reasonably well. Language, thought, and attention are
smaller commissures also exchange some information harder to measure, and therefore harder to study. Neverthe-
between the hemispheres. After damage to the corpus less, they have been integral topics for neuroscience since its
callosum, each hemisphere has access to information only earliest days, beginning with Paul Broca’s report in the 1860s
from the opposite half of the body and from the opposite that speech depends on part of the left frontal cortex.
visual field. Although research on the biology of cognition is difficult,
many of the results are fascinating. After damage to the cor-
pus callosum, which connects the two hemispheres, people
act as if they have two fields of awareness—separate “minds,”
you might say. With damage to certain areas of the left hemi-
sphere, people lose their language abilities, while remaining
unimpaired in other ways. People with damage to parts of the
OPPOSITE: Language may have evolved from our tendency to
make gestures.
right hemisphere ignore the left side of their body and the left
side of the world. Studies of such people offer clues about how
the brain operates and raise stimulating questions.

421
Module 14.1

Lateralization of Function

S
ymmetry is common in nature. The sun, stars, and
planets are nearly symmetrical, as are most animals and
plants. When an atom undergoes radioactive decay, it
emits identical rays in exactly opposite directions. However,
the human brain is asymmetrical. The left hemisphere has
somewhat different functions from the right hemisphere.
Why? Presumably, assigning different functions to the two
hemispheres provides some advantage. This module explores
the distinctions between hemispheres.

The Left and Right Hemispheres


The left hemisphere of the cerebral cortex is connected to skin
receptors and muscles mainly on the right side of the body. The (a)
right hemisphere is connected to skin receptors and muscles Corpus callosum
mainly on the left side. As an exception to this rule, both hemi-
spheres control the trunk muscles and facial muscles. The left
hemisphere sees only the right half of the world. The right hemi-
sphere sees only the left half of the world. Each hemisphere gets
auditory information from both ears but slightly stronger infor-
mation from the contralateral ear. Taste and smell, however, are
uncrossed. Each hemisphere gets taste information from its own
side of the tongue (Aglioti, Tassinari, Corballis, & Berlucchi,
2000; Pritchard, Macaluso, & Eslinger, 1999) and smell infor-
mation from the nostril on its own side (Herz, McCall, & Cahill,
1999; Homewood & Stevenson, 2001).
Why all vertebrates (and many invertebrates) evolved so
that each hemisphere controls the contralateral (opposite)
side of the body, no one knows. At any rate, the left and right
hemispheres of the cerebral cortex exchange information
through a set of axons called the corpus callosum (Figure
14.1; see also Figures 4.10 and 4.13) and through the anterior
commissure, the hippocampal commissure, and a couple of (b)
other small commissures. Information that initially enters one
hemisphere crosses quickly so that both hemispheres have ac- Figure 14.1  ​Two views of the corpus callosum
cess to the information. The corpus callosum is a large set of axons conveying information
between the two hemispheres. (a) A sagittal section through the
The two hemispheres are not mirror images of each other.
human brain. (b) A dissection (viewed from above) in which gray
In most humans, the left hemisphere is specialized for language. matter has been removed to expose the corpus callosum.
The functions of the right hemisphere are more difficult to sum- (© Cengage Learning 2013)
marize, as we shall see later. Such division of labor between the

422
14.1 Lateralization of Function 423

two hemispheres is known as lateralization. If you had no cor- the left side. In rabbits and other species with eyes far to the
pus callosum, your left hemisphere could react only to informa- side of the head, the left eye connects to the right hemisphere,
tion from the right side of your body, and your right hemisphere and the right eye connects to the left. Human eyes are not con-
could react only to information from the left. Because of the nected to the brain in this way. Both of your eyes face forward.
corpus callosum, however, each hemisphere receives information You see the left side of the world almost as well with your
from both sides. Only after damage to the corpus callosum (or to right eye as with your left eye.
one hemisphere) do we see clear evidence of lateralization. Figure 14.2 illustrates the connections from the eyes to
the human brain. Light from the right half of the visual field
(what is visible at any moment) strikes the left half of each
Visual and Auditory Connections retina, and light from the left visual field strikes the right half
of each retina. The left half of each retina connects to the left
to the Hemispheres hemisphere, which therefore sees the right visual field. Simi-
Before we can discuss lateralization in any detail, let’s consider larly, the right half of each retina connects to the right hemi-
how the eyes connect to the brain. The hemispheres connect sphere, which sees the left visual field. A small vertical strip
to the eyes such that each hemisphere gets input from the op- down the center of each retina, covering about 5 degrees of
posite half of the visual world. That is, the left hemisphere visual arc, connects to both hemispheres (Innocenti, 1980;
sees the right side of the world, and the right hemisphere sees Lavidor & Walsh, 2004). In Figure 14.2, note how half of the
Left visual field Right visual field

Olfactory
bulbs
Left Right Optic
retina retina nerves
(cut)

Optic
chiasm
© Cengage Learning 2013

Courtesy of Dr. Dana Copeland

Blood
To left Optic To right vessels
hemisphere chiasm hemisphere
of brain of brain

(a) (b)

Figure 14.2  ​Connections from the eyes to the human brain


(a) The left hemisphere connects to the left half of each retina and thus gets visual input from the right half of the world. The opposite is
true of the right hemisphere. (b) At the optic chiasm, axons from the right half of the left retina cross to the right hemisphere, and axons
from the left half of the right retina cross to the left hemisphere.
424 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

axons from each eye cross to the opposite side of the brain at ity cannot cross the corpus callosum, so it remains within one
the optic chiasm (literally, the “optic cross”). hemisphere.) A surprising bonus is that the seizures become
Right visual field d left half of each retina d left less frequent. Evidently, epileptic activity rebounds back and
hemisphere forth between the hemispheres and prolongs seizures. If it
can’t bounce back and forth across the corpus callosum, a sei-
Left visual field d right half of each retina d right
zure may not develop at all.
hemisphere
How does severing the corpus callosum affect other as-
pects of behavior? People who have undergone surgery to the
The auditory system is organized differently. Each ear sends
corpus callosum, referred to as split-brain people, maintain
the information to both sides of the brain, because any brain
their intellect and motivation, and they still walk without dif-
area that contributes to localizing sounds must compare input
ficulty. They also use the two hands together on familiar tasks
from both ears. However, each hemisphere does pay more at-
such as tying shoes. However, if they are asked to pretend they
tention to the ear on the opposite side (Hugdahl, 1996).
are hitting a golf ball, threading a needle, or attaching a fish-
hook to a line, they struggle with any task that is not familiar
Stop & Check to them (Franz, Waldie, & Smith, 2000).
Split-brain people can use their two hands independently in
1. The left hemisphere of the brain is connected to the right a way that other people cannot. For example, try drawing  with
eye in rabbits. In humans, the left hemisphere is con- your left hand while simultaneously drawing  with your right
nected to the left half of each retina. Explain the reason hand. Most people find this task difficult, but split-brain people
for this species difference. do it with ease. Or try drawing circles with both hands simulta-
2. In humans, light from the right visual field strikes the neously, but one of them just a little faster than
_____ half of each retina, which sends its axons to the the other (not twice as fast). Most people find this
_____ hemisphere of the brain. task difficult; split-brain people spontaneously
try it 
draw the circles at different speeds (Kennerley, yourself
ANSWERS
sees the right visual field. 2. left . . . left. Diedrichsen, Hazeltine, Semjen, & Ivry, 2002).
the eyes point straight ahead and half of each eye The difficulty of simultaneously moving your left hand
head and sees only the right visual field. In humans, one way and your right hand a different way reflects a cogni-
1. In rabbits, the right eye is far to the side of the
tive difficulty more than a motor limitation. It is hard to draw
a  with one hand and a  with the other, but if you carefully
draw both of them and then try to trace over the  with one
hand and a  with the other, you will find it
easier. Evidently, it is difficult to plan two ac-
Cutting the Corpus Callosum tions at once unless you have clear targets to
try it 
Damage to the corpus callosum prevents the two hemi- direct your movements. Split-brain people have yourself
spheres from exchanging information. Occasionally, sur- no trouble planning two actions at once.
geons sever the corpus callosum as a treatment for severe Research by Roger Sperry and his students (Nebes, 1974)
epilepsy, a condition characterized by repeated episodes of revealed behavioral effects when stimuli were limited to one side
excessive synchronized neural activity. Epilepsy can result of the body. In a typical experiment, a split-brain person stared
from a mutation in a gene controlling the GABA recep- straight ahead as the experimenter flashed words or pictures on
tor (Baulac et al., 2001), from trauma or infection in the either side of a screen, too briefly for the person to move his or
brain, brain tumors, or exposure to toxic substances. Often, her eyes (Figure 14.3). Information going to one hemisphere
the cause is not known. About 1% to 2% of all people have could not cross to the other, because of the damage to the corpus
epilepsy. The symptoms vary depending on the location and callosum. The person could then point with the left hand to what
type of brain abnormality. the right hemisphere saw and could point with the right hand to
Antiepileptic drugs block sodium flow across the mem- what the left hemisphere saw. The person could talk about what
brane or enhance the effects of GABA. More than 90% of the left hemisphere saw, but not what the right hemisphere saw.
epileptic patients respond well enough to live a normal life. (In most people the left hemisphere controls speech.) The two
However, if someone continues having frequent seizures de- halves of the brain had different information, and they could not
spite medication, physicians consider surgically removing the communicate with each other.
focus, the point in the brain where the seizures begin. The According to fMRI data and other methods, the left hemi-
location of the focus varies from one person to another. sphere is dominant for speech production in more than 95% of
Removing the focus is not an option if someone has several right-handers and nearly 80% of left-handers (McKeever, Seitz,
foci. Therefore, the idea arose to cut the corpus callosum to Krutsch, & Van Eys, 1995). Not many left-handers have com-
prevent epileptic seizures from crossing from one hemisphere plete right-hemisphere dominance for speech. A more com-
to the other. One benefit is that, as predicted, the person’s epi- mon pattern is mixed left- and right-hemisphere dominance. In
leptic seizures affect only half the body. (The abnormal activ- contrast to speech production, speech comprehension is more
14.1 Lateralization of Function 425

HATBAND

(a) (b) (c)

Figure 14.3  ​Effects of damage to the corpus callosum


(a) When the word hatband is flashed on a screen, (b) a woman with a split brain can report only what her left hemisphere saw—”band.”
(c) However, with her left hand, she can point to a hat, which is what the right hemisphere saw. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

equally divided. The left hemisphere understands speech bet- sphere. The same person viewing a display in the left visual
ter than the right hemisphere, but for most people, the right field (right hemisphere) usually cannot name or describe it.
hemisphere understands speech reasonably well, except with However, a small amount of information travels between the
complex grammar (Beeman & Chiarello, 1998). hemispheres through several smaller commissures, as shown
A split-brain person can name an object after viewing it in Figure 14.4, and some split-brain people get enough in-
briefly in the right visual field and therefore the left hemi- formation to describe objects in part (Berlucchi, Mangun,

Corpus callosum Thalamus

Hippocampus

Anterior commissure

Hippocampal commissures

Corpus callosum

Thalamus

Anterior commissure
Figure 14.4  ​The anterior
and hippocampal commissures
These commissures exchange information Hippocampus
between the two hemispheres, as does the larger
corpus callosum. (Based on Nieuwenhuys, Voogd,
& vanHuijzen, 1988, and others)
426 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

& Gazzaniga, 1997; Forster & Corballis, 2000). A patient enough. In one study, researchers asked a split-brain person
who cannot name something points to it correctly with the to identify photos after viewing them briefly in one visual field
left hand, even while saying, “I don’t know what it was.” (Of or the other. They formed photos by morphing pictures of the
course, a split-brain person who watches the left hand point split-brain person himself, and pictures of another familiar
out an object can then name it.) person. When he saw a picture in the right visual field (left
Is it an advantage for just one hemisphere to control speech? hemisphere), he was more likely to say it was himself. When
Possibly. Many people who have bilateral control of speech stut- he saw it in the left visual field (right hemisphere), he usually
ter (Fox et al., 2000), although not all people who stutter have thought it was the other person (Turk et al., 2002).
bilateral control of speech. Perhaps having two speech centers In other situations, the hemispheres learn to cooperate. A
produces competing messages to the speech muscles. split-brain person who was tested with the apparatus shown
in Figure 14.3 used an interesting strategy to answer a yes–no
question about what he saw in the left visual field. Suppose
Stop & Check
an experimenter flashes a picture in the left visual field and
asks, “Was it green?” The left (speaking) hemisphere takes
3. Can a split-brain person name an object after feeling it a guess: “Yes.” That guess might be correct. If not, the right
with the right hand? With the left hand? Explain. hemisphere, which knows the correct answer, makes the face
4. After a split-brain person sees something in the left visual frown. (Both hemispheres control facial muscles on both sides
field, how can he or she identify the object? of the face.) The left hemisphere, feeling the frown, says, “Oh,
I’m sorry, I meant ‘no.’”
ANSWERS
could point to the correct answer with the left hand. In another experiment, a split-brain person saw two words
something in the left visual field, a split-brain person flashed at once, one on each side. He was then asked to draw a
right hemisphere, which cannot speak. 4. After seeing picture of what he had read. Each hemisphere saw a full word,
people. The left hand sends its information to the but the two words could combine to make a different word.
hemisphere, which is dominant for language in most
For example,
left. The right hand sends its information to the left
after feeling it with the right hand but not with the Left Visual Field Right Visual Field
(Right Hemisphere) (Left Hemisphere)
3. A split-brain person can describe something

hot dog
honey moon
Split Hemispheres: Competition sky scraper
and Cooperation rain bow
Each hemisphere of a split-brain person processes informa- With the right hand, he almost always drew what he had
tion independently of the other. In the first weeks after sur- seen in the right visual field, such as dog or moon. However,
gery, the hemispheres act like separate people sharing one with the left hand, he sometimes drew a literal combination of
body. One split-brain person repeatedly took items from the the two words. For example, after seeing hot and dog, he drew
grocery shelf with one hand and returned them with the other an overheated dog, not a wiener on a bun, and after seeing sky
(Reuter-Lorenz & Miller, 1998). and scraper, he drew a sky and a scraper (Figure 14.5). The
Another person—specifically, his left hemisphere—de- right hemisphere, which predominantly controls the left hand,
scribed his experience as follows (Dimond, 1979): drew what it saw in the left visual field (hot or sky). Ordinarily,
If I’m reading, I can hold the book in my right hand; it’s a the left hemisphere doesn’t control the left hand, but through
lot easier to sit on my left hand, than to hold it with both the bilateral mechanisms of the medial corticospinal pathway
hands. . . . You tell your hand—I’m going to turn so many (described in Chapter 8), it can move the left hand clumsily
pages in a book—turn three pages—then somehow the and, evidently, enough to add what it saw in the right visual
left hand will pick up two pages and you’re at page 5, or field (dog or scraper). However, neither hemisphere could
whatever. It’s better to let it go, pick it up with the right combine the words into one concept (Kingstone & Gazza-
hand, and then turn to the right page. With your right niga, 1995).
hand, you correct what the left has done. (p. 211)
Such conflicts are more common soon after surgery than The Right Hemisphere
later. The corpus callosum does not heal, but the brain learns Suppose you watch videotapes of people talking about them-
to use the smaller connections between the left and right selves. Each person speaks twice, once telling the truth and once
hemispheres (Myers & Sperry, 1985). The left hemisphere lying. Could you guess which version was the truth? The average
somehow suppresses the right hemisphere’s interference and score for MIT undergraduates was 47% correct, a bit worse than
takes control in some situations. However, even then, the the 50% they should have had by random guessing. Most other
hemispheres show differences of opinion if we test carefully groups did equally badly, except for one group of people who got
14.1 Lateralization of Function 427

Jerre Levy
Despite the quite amazing progress
of the last half century in neuroscien-
tific understanding, we are still, in my
SCRAPER-SKY view, as distant now as ever in know-
ing what questions to ask about how
and why brains make minds. It is sim-
ply evading the issue to say, as some

Jerre Levy
philosophers do, that our mental ex-
Figure 14.5  ​A split-brain person draws with the left hand  periences are just the inside view of
He saw the word sky in the left visual field and scraper in the right the stuff we measure on the outside. Why is the inside view
visual field. His left hemisphere controlled the left hand enough so utterly different from our external measurements? Even if
to draw a scraper, and his right hemisphere controlled it enough we specified all the critical spatiotemporal neural dynamics
to draw a sky. (From “Subcortical Transfer of Higher Order Informa- that were necessary and sufficient for a given mental experi-
tion: More Illusory Than Real?” by A. Kingstone and M. S. Gazzaniga, ence, this would not tell us why those dynamics give rise to
1995. Neuropsychology, 9, pp. 321–328. Copyright © 1995 Ameri- any experience at all… Nature will answer if we ask the right
can Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission.) questions. (Levy, personal communication)

60% correct—still not a great score, but at least better than ran- ample, examine the faces in Figure 14.6. Each of these combines
dom (Etcoff, Ekman, Magee, & Frank, 2000). Who do you sup- half of a smiling face with half of a neutral face. Which looks
pose they were? They were people with left-hemisphere brain happier to you: face (a) or face (b)? Most people choose face (a),
damage! They only poorly understood the speech, but they were with the smile on the viewer’s left (Heller & Levy, 1981; Hopt-
adept at reading gestures and facial expressions. As mentioned man & Levy, 1988; Levy, Heller, Banich, & Burton, 1983). Simi-
in Chapter 12, the right hemisphere is better than the left at per- larly, a frown on the viewer’s left looks sadder than
ceiving the emotions in people’s gestures and tone of voice, such a frown on the viewer’s right (Sackeim, Putz, Vin-
as happiness or sadness (Adolphs, Damasio, & Tranel, 2002). If giano, Coleman, & McElhiney, 1988). Remem-
the left hemisphere is damaged (and therefore prevented from ber, what you see in your left visual field stimu- yourself try it 
interfering with the right hemisphere), the right hemisphere is lates your right hemisphere first.
free to make reliable judgments. In contrast, people with damage
in parts of the right hemisphere
speak in a monotone voice,
do not understand other peo-
ple’s emotional expressions, and
usually fail to understand hu-
mor and sarcasm (Beeman &
Chiarello, 1998).
The right hemisphere is
dominant for recognizing emo-
tions in others, including both
pleasant and unpleasant emo-
tions (Narumoto, Okada, Sa-
dato, Fukui, & Yonekura, 2001).
In a split-brain person, the right
hemisphere does better than
the left at recognizing whether
two photographs show the
same or different emotions
(Stone, Nisenson, Eliassen, &
Gazzaniga, 1996). Moreover, (a) (b)
according to Jerre Levy and her
colleagues’ studies of brain-in- Figure 14.6  ​Half of a smiling face combined with half of a neutral face
tact people, when the left and Which looks happier to you—(a) the one with a smile on your left or (b) the one with a smile on
your right? Your answer suggests which hemisphere of your brain is dominant for interpreting
right hemispheres perceive dif-
emotional expressions. (Reprinted from Brain and Cognition, 2/4, Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich,
ferent emotions in someone’s M. T., Burton, L. A., “Asymmetry of perception in free viewing of chimeric faces,” 404–419, 1983,
face, the response of the right with permission from Elsevier.)
hemisphere dominates. For ex-
428 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

The right hemisphere also appears more adept than the Another task, and again it’s a laboratory task with little ob-
left at comprehending spatial relationships. For example, one vious application to everyday life: On each trial in this experi-
young woman with damage to her posterior right hemisphere ment, you respond to a signal with a handgrip. Right before the
had trouble finding her way around, even in familiar areas. To signal, a picture flashes very briefly on one side of the screen or
reach a destination, she needed directions with specific visual de- the other, so briefly (subliminally) that you can’t identify it con-
tails, such as, “Walk to the corner where you see a building with sciously. This subliminal signal indicates whether you can earn
a statue in front of it. Then turn left and go to the corner that a large or a small reward with your handgrip on this trial. Even
has a flagpole and turn right. . . .” Each of these directions had though the signal is subliminal, you will grasp more strongly
to include an unmistakable feature. If the instruction was “go to when it signals a large reward… but only if it flashes to the same
the city government building—that’s the one with a tower,” she hemisphere that controls the hand you are using on this trial
might go to a different building that happened to have a tower (Schmidt, Palminteri, Lafargue, & Pessiglione, 2010).
(Clarke, Assal, & deTribolet, 1993). Here is something you can try yourself: Tap with your
How can we best describe the difference in functions be- right hand as many times as you can in a short period of time.
tween the hemispheres? Rest and repeat with your left hand. Then repeat with each
According to Robert Ornstein (1997), the left hemisphere hand while talking. The Online Try It Yourself activity
focuses more on details and the right hemisphere more on “Hemisphere Control” will keep track of your totals. For
overall patterns. For example, in one study, people with intact most right-handers and many left-handers, talking decreases
brains examined visual stimuli such as the one in Figure 14.7, the tapping rate with the right hand more than
in which many repetitions of a small letter compose a different with the left hand (Kinsbourne & McMurray,
large letter. When they were asked to identify the small letters 1975). Evidently, it is more difficult to do two try it
(in this case, B), activity increased in the left hemisphere, but things at once when both activities depend on yourself online
when they were asked to identify the large overall letter (H), the same hemisphere.
activity increased in the right hemisphere (Fink et al., 1996).

B B Stop & Check


B B 5. Which hemisphere is dominant for the following in most
B B people: speech, emotional inflection of speech, interpret-
B B ing other people’s emotional expressions, spatial relation-
BB B B B BB ships, perceiving overall patterns?
B B hemisphere is dominant for all the other items listed.
ANSWER
B B 5. The left hemisphere is dominant for speech. The right
B B
B B
Figure 14.7  ​Analytical vs. holistic perception
When people are told to name the large composite letter, they Development of Lateralization
have more activity in the right hemisphere. When told to name
the small component letters, they have more activity in the left
and Handedness
hemisphere. (Based on Fink, Halligan, et al., 1996) Because most people’s language depends primarily on the left
hemisphere, it is natural to ask whether the hemispheres differ
anatomically. If so, do they differ before speech develops? How
does handedness relate to hemispheric dominance for speech?
Hemispheric Specializations
in Intact Brains
Anatomical Differences Between
Even in people without brain damage, careful testing dem-
onstrates differences between the hemispheres. Suppose you the Hemispheres
smell something with just one nostril, so the information goes The human brain is specialized to attend to language sounds.
primarily to one hemisphere (in this case, the ipsilateral one). If you listen to a repeated syllable (“pack pack pack pack . . .”)
Further suppose that it’s an unfamiliar odor that you can’t and then suddenly the vowel sound changes (“. . . pack pack
name. Twelve seconds later you smell something again with pack peck . . .”), the change catches your attention and evokes
either the same nostril or the other one, and you have to say larger electrical responses measured on your scalp. Changing
whether it is the same as the previous smell. You will be more from pack to peck also increases the evoked response from a
accurate if you smelled the two substances with the same nos- baby, even a premature infant (Cheour-Luhtanen et al., 1996).
tril, and therefore the same hemisphere (Yeshurun, Dudai, & Evidently, humans attend to language sounds from the start.
Sobel, 2008). (Yes, you’re right, this is the kind of thing that Do the hemispheres differ from the start? Norman Ge-
seldom happens in everyday life.) schwind and Walter Levitsky (1968) found that one section
14.1 Lateralization of Function 429

Anterior

Anterior Posterior Ventricles

Planum
temporale
Planum
Location of cut temporale

Left Right
Posterior

Figure 14.8  ​Horizontal section through a human brain


This cut, just above the surface of the temporal lobe, shows the planum temporale, an area critical for speech comprehension. It is larger
in the left hemisphere than in the right hemisphere. (From “Human Brain: Left-Right Asymmetries in Temporal Speech Region,” by
N. Geschwind and W. Levitsky, 1968, Science, 161, pp. 186–187. Copyright © 1968 by AAAS and N. Geschwind. Reprinted by permission.)

of the temporal cortex, called the planum temporale (PLAY- say whether the materials felt the same or different. The
num tem-poh-RAH-lee), is larger in the left hemisphere for 5-year-olds did equally well with one hand or with two. The
65% of people (Figure 14.8). Sandra Witelson and Wazir Pal- 3-year-olds made 90% more errors with two hands than with
lie (1973) examined the brains of infants who died before age one (Galin, Johnstone, Nakell, & Herron, 1979). The likely
3 months and found that the left planum temporale was larger interpretation is that the corpus callosum matures sufficiently
in 12 of 14. between ages 3 and 5 to facilitate the comparison of stimuli
Smaller but still significant differences are found between between the two hands.
left and right hemispheres of chimpanzees, bonobos, and go- Other kinds of tasks show continuing maturation of the
rillas (Hopkins, 2006). Chimpanzees with a larger left than corpus callosum in 5- and 6-year-olds. Did you ever play with
right planum temporale generally show a preference for using an Etch-A-Sketch toy? You can rotate two wheels, one with
their right hand, as most humans do (Hopkins, Russell, & each hand. One wheel moves a line up or down, and the other
Cantalupo, 2007). Evidently, the specialization we see in the moves it left or right. Five- and six-year-olds have great trou-
human brain built upon specializations already present in our ble with this toy, partly because their corpus callosum is not
apelike ancestors of long ago. mature enough to integrate the actions of the two hands. In
contrast, consider the task of tapping keys with one hand or
two whenever a stimulus appears on the screen. Adults and
Maturation of the Corpus Callosum older children are slower to respond with two hands than
The corpus callosum gradually grows and thickens as myelin with one, presumably because the message to one hand inter-
increases around certain axons during childhood and adoles- feres with the message to the other hand. Children younger
cence (Luders, Thompson, & Toga, 2010). The corpus callo- than 6 years respond just as fast with two hands as with one,
sum also matures by discarding many axons. At an early stage, again suggesting that they do not yet have a mature corpus
the brain generates far more axons than it will have at maturity callosum (Franz & Fahey, 2007).
(Ivy & Killackey, 1981; Killackey & Chalupa, 1986). The rea-
son is that any two neurons connected by the corpus callosum
need to have corresponding functions. For example, a neuron
in the left hemisphere that responds to light in the center of the
fovea should be connected to a right-hemisphere neuron that
responds to light in the same location. During early embryonic
development, the genes cannot specify exactly where those two
© Judith Collins/Alamy Limited

neurons will be. Therefore, many connections are made across


the corpus callosum, but only those axons that happen to con-
nect very similar cells survive (Innocenti & Caminiti, 1980).
Because the connections take years to develop their ma-
ture adult pattern, certain behaviors of young children resem-
ble those of split-brain adults. In one study, 3- and 5-year-
old children were asked to feel two fabrics, either with one Etch-A-Sketch toy
hand at two times or with two hands at the same time, and
430 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

Development Without a Corpus Callosum left-hemisphere dominance for speech, but some have right-
Rarely, the corpus callosum forms incompletely or not at all, hemisphere dominance or a mixture of left and right (Basso
possibly for genetic reasons. People born without a corpus & Rusconi, 1998). The same is true for people who were left-
callosum are unlike people who have it cut later in life. First, handed in early childhood but forced to switch to writing
whatever prevented formation of the corpus callosum un- right-handed (Siebner et al., 2002). Many left-handers who
doubtedly affects brain development in other ways. Second, have partial right-hemisphere control of speech are also partly
the absence or near absence of the corpus callosum induces reversed for spatial perception, showing more than the usual
the remaining brain areas to develop differently. amount of left-hemisphere contribution. A few left-handers
People born without a corpus callosum perform more have right-hemisphere dominance for both language and spa-
slowly or less accurately than average on tasks that re- tial perception (Flöel et al., 2001).
quire cooperation between the two hemispheres—Etch- Hand preference relates to some other asymmetries in
A-Sketch, for example (Mueller, Marion, Paul, & Brown, brain and behavior. Suppose you are hiking through the
2009). However, they perform reasonably well on many woods when you come to a fork in the path. Other things
tasks where split-brain people fail. They verbally describe being equal, which direction do you choose? You might
what they feel with either hand and what they see in either imagine that you choose randomly, but most people show a
visual field. They also feel objects with the two hands and tendency to pick one direction more than the other. In one
say whether they are the same or different (Paul et al., 2007). study, people wore a device on their belt that counted the
How do they do so? They do not use their right hemisphere number of times they turned left or right over 3 days. On
for speech (Lassonde, Bryden, & Demers, 1990). Rather, average, right-handers turned mostly to the left, and left-
each hemisphere develops pathways connecting it to both handers turned mostly to the right (Mohr, Landis, Bracha,
sides of the body, enabling the left (speaking) hemisphere & Brugger, 2003).
to feel both the left and right hands. Also, the brain’s other Might this tendency relate to some other observations?
commissures become larger than usual, including the ante- In most races—car races, horse races, ice skating races, and
rior commissure (Figures 4.12 and 14.4), which connects so forth—the track is set up for turning to the left (counter-
the anterior parts of the cerebral cortex, and the hippocam- clockwise). In baseball, players run the bases to the left (coun-
pal commissure, which connects the left and right hippo- terclockwise). Watch little children when they run around in
campi (Figure 14.4). The extra development of these other circles on a playground. Do you see most of them running
commissures partly compensates for the lack of a corpus counterclockwise?
callosum. However, the amount of information they convey
varies from one person to another, and so do the behavioral Avoiding Overstatements
deficits.
The research on left-brain/right-brain differences is excit-
ing, but it sometimes leads to unscientific assertions. Occa-
sionally, you may hear a person say something like, “I don’t
Stop & Check
do well in science because it is a left-brain subject and I am
6. A child born without a corpus callosum can name some- a right-brain person.” That kind of statement is based on
thing felt with the left hand, but an adult who suffered two reasonable premises and a doubtful one. The scientific
damage to the corpus callosum cannot. What are two ideas are that (a) the hemispheres are specialized for differ-
likely explanations? ent functions and (b) certain tasks evoke greater activity in
one hemisphere or the other. The doubtful premise is that
any individual habitually relies mostly on one hemisphere.
and other commissures grow larger than usual.
ANSWER
What evidence do you suppose someone has for believ-
tions with the left hand, and the anterior commissure
hemisphere develops more than the usual connec-
6. In children born without a corpus callosum, the left ing, “I am a right-brain person”? Did he or she undergo an
MRI or PET scan to determine which hemisphere was larger
or more active? Not likely. Generally, when people say, “I am
right-brained,” their only evidence is that they perform well on
Hemispheres, Handedness, creative tasks or poorly on logical tasks. (Saying, “I am right-
brained” sometimes implies that because I do poorly on logi-
and Language Dominance cal tasks, therefore, I am creative. Unfortunately, illogical is not
For more than 95% of right-handed people, the left hemi- the same as creative.) In fact, you use both hemispheres for all
sphere is strongly dominant for speech (McKeever et al., but the simplest tasks. Most tasks require cooperation by both
1995). Left-handers are more variable. Most left-handers have hemispheres.
14.1 Lateralization of Function 431

Module 14.1  ■  In Closing

One Brain, Two Hemispheres

Imagine you are a split-brain person. Someone asks you—that sphere is saying or wish it would just shut up for a while. This
is, your left hemisphere, the talking side of you—a question to must be an unsettling experience, too.
which you honestly reply that you do not know. Meanwhile, Do split-brain people really have two minds, two conscious-
your left hand points to the correct answer. It must be an unset- nesses? At times, they seem to. At least, there are times when
tling experience. one side answers a question and the other cannot. We cannot
Now imagine life from the standpoint of that right hemi- get into someone else’s head to know what it is like. Indeed, each
sphere. You sit there mute, while that other hemisphere is jab- hemisphere does not know what, if anything, the other hemi-
bering away. Sometimes, you disagree with what that hemi- sphere is experiencing.

summary
1. The corpus callosum is a set of axons connecting the two 6. The right hemisphere is dominant for the emotional
hemispheres of the brain.  422 inflections of speech and for interpreting other people’s
2. The left hemisphere controls speech in most people, and emotional expressions in either speech or facial expression.
each hemisphere controls mostly the hand on the In vision, it attends mostly to overall patterns in contrast to
opposite side, sees the opposite side of the world, and the left hemisphere, which is better for details.  426
feels the opposite side of the body.  422 7. The left and right hemispheres differ anatomically even
3. In humans, the left visual field projects onto the right half during infancy. Young children have some trouble
of each retina, which sends axons to the right hemisphere. comparing information from the left and right hands
The right visual field projects onto the left half of each because the corpus callosum is not fully mature.  428
retina, which sends axons to the left hemisphere.  423 8. A child born without a corpus callosum does not show
4. After damage to the corpus callosum, each hemisphere all the same deficits as an adult who sustains damage to
answers questions about the information that reaches it the corpus callosum.  430
directly. Each slowly answers a few questions about 9. The brain of a left-handed person is not simply a mirror
information on the other side if it crosses the anterior image of a right-hander’s brain. Most left-handers have
commissure or one of the other small commissures.  424 left-hemisphere or mixed dominance for speech; few have
5. Although the two hemispheres of a split-brain person are strong right-hemisphere dominance for speech.  430
sometimes in conflict, they find ways to cooperate and
cue each other.  426

Key Terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
anterior commissure  430 focus  424 planum temporale  429
corpus callosum  422 lateralization  423 split-brain people  424
epilepsy  424 optic chiasm  424 visual field  423

Thought Question
When a person born without a corpus callosum moves the of the other hand involuntarily. What possible explanation
fingers of one hand, he or she also is likely to move the fingers can you suggest?
Module 14.2

Evolution and Physiology


of Language

N
early all animals communicate through visual, auditory,
tactile, or chemical (pheromonal) displays. Human
language stands out from other forms of communication
because of its productivity—its ability to improvise new
combinations of signals to represent new ideas. Did we evolve
this ability out of nothing or from a precursor already present in
other species? Why did we evolve language, whereas other
species did not? And what brain specializations make language
possible?

Nonhuman Precursors
of Language
Evolution rarely creates something totally new. Bat wings are
modified arms, and porcupine quills are modified hairs. We
would expect human language to be a modification of some-
thing we can detect in our closest relatives, chimpanzees. Figure 14.9  ​An attempt to teach chimpanzees language
One of the Premacks’s chimps, Elizabeth, reacts to colored plastic
chips that read “Not Elizabeth banana insert—Elizabeth apple
Common Chimpanzees wash.” (Photo courtesy of Ann Premack)
After many unsuccessful attempts to teach chimpanzees to
speak, researchers achieved better results by teaching them
American Sign Language or other visual systems (B. T. Gard- ■ The chimpanzees used their symbols mainly to re-
ner & Gardner, 1975; Premack & Premack, 1972) (Figure quest, seldom to describe.
14.9). In one version, chimps learned to press keys bearing
symbols to type messages on a computer (Rumbaugh, 1977), Nevertheless, the chimpanzees showed at least moderate
such as “Please machine give apple” or to another chimpanzee, understanding. For example, the chimp Washoe, trained in
“Please share your chocolate.” sign language, usually answered “Who” questions with names,
Is the use of symbols really language? Not everything that “What” questions with objects, and “Where” questions with
we can translate as a series of words is really language. For places, even when she used the wrong symbol for a name, ob-
example, when you insert your ATM card and enter your ject, or place (Van Cantfort, Gardner, & Gardner, 1989).
four-digit PIN, you don’t really understand those four dig-
its to mean, “Please machine give money.” Similarly, when a Bonobos
chimpanzee presses four symbols on a machine, it may not
Amid widespread skepticism about chimpanzee language,
understand them to mean, “Please machine give apple.” The
surprising results emerged from studies of an endangered spe-
chimps’ use of symbols had features that raised doubts about
cies, Pan paniscus, known as the bonobo or the pygmy chim-
calling it language (Rumbaugh, 1990; Terrace, Petitto, Sand-
panzee (a misleading name because they are practically the
ers, & Bever, 1979):
same size as common chimpanzees).
■ The chimpanzees seldom used symbols in new, original Bonobos’ social order resembles humans’ in several regards.
combinations. That is, their symbol use was short on Males and females form strong, sometimes lasting, personal at-
productivity. tachments. They often copulate face to face. The female is sexu-

432
14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 433

ally responsive on almost any day


and not just during her fertile period.
The males contribute significantly to
infant care. Adults often share food.
They stand comfortably on their
hind legs. In short, they resemble hu-
mans more than other primates do.
In the mid-1980s, Sue Savage-
Rumbaugh, Duane Rumbaugh,
and their associates tried to teach
a female bonobo named Matata
to press symbols that lit when
touched. Each symbol represents
a word (Figure 14.10). Although
Matata made little progress, her
infant son Kanzi learned just
by watching her. When given a
chance to use the symbol board, he
quickly excelled. Later, researchers
noticed that Kanzi understood a
fair amount of spoken language.
For example, whenever anyone
said the word “light,” Kanzi would
Figure 14.10  ​Language tests for Kanzi, a bonobo (Pan paniscus)
flip the light switch. By age 51/2, He listens to questions through earphones and points to answers on a board. The experiment-
he understood about 150 Eng- er with him does not hear the questions. (From Georgia State University’s Language Research
lish words and responded to such Center, operated with the Yerkes Primate Center of Emory. Photo courtesy of Duane Rumbaugh.)
unfamiliar spoken commands
as “Throw your ball in the river”
and “Go to the refrigerator and get out a tomato” (Savage- to explain the cut that he had received on his hand an
Rumbaugh, 1990; Savage-Rumbaugh, Sevcik, Brakke, & hour earlier.
Rumbaugh, 1992). Kanzi and his younger sister developed ■ They frequently make original, creative requests, such

language comprehension comparable to that of a typical 2- as asking one person to chase another.
to 21/2-year-old child (Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1993):
Why have Kanzi and Mulika developed more impressive
■ They understand more than they can produce. skills than other chimpanzees? Perhaps bonobos have more
■ They use symbols to name and describe objects even language potential than common chimpanzees. A second ex-
when they are not requesting them. planation is that Kanzi and Mulika began language training
■ They request items that they do not see, such as when young. A third reason pertains to the method of train-
“bubbles” (I want to play with the bubble-blower). ing: Perhaps learning by observation and imitation promotes
■ They occasionally use the symbols to describe past better understanding than the formal training methods of
events. Kanzi once pressed the symbols “Matata bite” previous studies (Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1992).

Duane Rumbaugh, Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, and


chimpanzee Austin
Chimpanzees and bonobos are outstanding teachers of psychol-
ogy. They never presume that we, as their students, know a damn
thing about who they are. And they certainly aren’t impressed with
our degrees. Consequently, they are able to teach all manner of
Duane Rumbaugh

important things about what it means to be human and to be


ape—that is, if we as students are quiet, listen carefully, and let
them tell us as only they can.
434 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

Stop & Check used their claws to pull up the chain until they reached the al-
7. What are three likely explanations for why bonobos made
mond. Alex and another language-trained parrot repeatedly told
more language progress than common chimpanzees?
the experimenter, “Want nut.” When she declined to bring it to
them, they gave up (Pepperberg, 2004) (Figure 14.12).
instead of formal training techniques. What do we learn from studies of nonhuman language
ANSWER
training at an earlier age. They learned by imitation abilities? At a practical level, we gain insights into how best
than common chimpanzees. The bonobos started to teach language to those who do not learn it easily, such as
7. Bonobos may be more predisposed to language people with brain damage or children with autism. At a more
theoretical level, these studies indicate that human language
evolved from precursors present in other species. These stud-
ies also point out the ambiguity of our concept: We cannot
Nonprimates decide whether chimpanzees or parrots have language unless
What about nonprimate species? Spectacular results have we define language more precisely.
been reported for Alex, an African gray parrot (Figure 14.11).
Parrots are, of course, famous for imitating sounds. Irene Pep- How Did Humans Evolve
perberg was the first to argue that parrots can use sounds
meaningfully. She kept Alex in a stimulating environment and Language?
taught him by saying a word many times and offering rewards Although a few other species can learn a little language after
if Alex approximated the same sound. Here is an excerpt early much training, humans stand out by learning it easily. How
in training (Pepperberg, 1981, p. 142): did we evolve this ability? Most theories fall into two catego-
Pepperberg: Pasta! (Takes pasta.) Pasta! (Alex stretches ries: (a) we evolved it as a byproduct of overall brain develop-
from his perch, appears to reach for pasta.) ment or (b) we evolved it as a specialization.
Alex: Pa!
Pepperberg: Better . . . what is it? Language: Byproduct of Intelligence,
Alex: Pah-ah. or Specialized Adaptation?
Pepperberg: Better! The simplest view is that as humans evolved big brains, language
Alex: Pah-ta. developed as an accidental byproduct of intelligence. In its sim-
plest form, this hypothesis faces serious problems.
Pepperberg: Okay, here’s the pasta.
Good try.
Although pasta was used in this exam-
ple, Pepperberg generally used toys. For ex-
ample, if Alex said “paper,” “wood,” or “key,”
she would give him what he asked for. In no
case did she reward him with food for say-
ing “paper” or “wood.”
In one test, Alex viewed a tray of 12 ob-
jects and correctly answered 39 of 48 ques-
tions such as“What color is the key?” (answer:
“green”) and “What object is gray?” (answer:
“circle”). Many of his incorrect answers were
almost correct. In one case, he was asked the
color of the block and he responded with the
color of the rock (Pepperberg, 1993). He also
answered questions of the form “How many
blue keys?” in which he had to count the blue
keys among objects of two shapes and two
colors (Pepperberg, 1994).
Relying on language is not always help-
ful. Pepperberg put Alex and three other gray
Figure 14.11  ​Language tests for Alex
parrots on perches; each had a chain of large
Alex conversed about objects in simple English—for example, answering, “What color
plastic links from the perch to an almond on
is the circle?” He received no food rewards. (Courtesy of Irene Pepperburg/The Alex
the bottom. (Almonds are favorite foods for Foundation)
parrots.) The parrots untrained in language
14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 435

Irene Pepperberg/The Alex Foundation

Irene Pepperberg/The Alex Foundation

(a) (b)

Figure 14.12  ​A gray parrot with a reasoning task


Two parrots not trained in language pulled up chain links to reach the treat. Two with language training persisted in saying, “Want nut,”
apparently expecting help.

People with Normal Intelligence


In another test, experimenters presented sentences and
but Impaired Language
asked whether each sentence was correct and, if not, how to
If language is a product of overall brain size, then anyone with
improve it. They made many errors and odd corrections. For
a full-size brain and normal overall intelligence should have
example:
normal language. However, not all do. In one family, 16 of
30 people over three generations show severe language defi- Original Item Attempted Correction
cits despite normal intelligence in other regards. Because of
The boy eats three cookie. The boys eat four cookie.
a particular dominant gene, the affected people have serious
troubles in pronunciation and many other aspects of language Despite the language difficulties, these people behave nor-
(Fisher, Vargha-Khadem, Watkins, Monaco, & Pembrey, mally and intelligently in most regards. Evidently, language
1998; Gopnik & Crago, 1991; Lai, Fisher, Hurst, Vargha- requires more than just a large brain and overall intelligence.
Khadem, & Monaco, 2001). When they speak, their brains
show activity in posterior regions instead of the frontal cortex, People with Mental Retardation but Relatively
as in other people (Vargha-Khadem, Gadian, Copp, & Mish- Spared Language
kin, 2005). What about the reverse? Could someone with mental re-
They have trouble with even simple grammatical rules, as tardation have good language? Psychologists would have
shown in the following dialogue about making plurals: answered “no,” until they discovered Williams syndrome,
affecting about 1 person in 20,000. Despite mental retarda-
Experimenter Respondent
tion in most regards, many people with Williams syndrome
This is a wug; these are . . . How should I know? speak grammatically and fluently. The cause is a deletion of
  [Later] These are wug. several genes from chromosome 7 (Korenberg et al., 2000),
This is a zat; these are . . . These are zacko. leading to decreased gray matter, especially in visual process-
This is a sas; these are . . . These are sasss. ing areas (Kippenhan et al., 2005; Meyer-Lindenberg et al.,
  [Not sasses] 2004; Reiss et al., 2004). Affected people are poor at tasks re-
436 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

well, or at least close to normal, in certain regards. One is music,


such as the ability to clap a complex rhythm and memorize songs
(Levitin & Bellugi, 1998). Another is friendliness and the ability
to interpret facial expressions, such as relaxed or worried, serious
or playful, flirtatious or uninterested (Tager-Flusberg, Boshart,
& Baron-Cohen, 1998). Their fascination with faces probably
relates to the fact that their fusiform cortex—an area sensitive
to faces (as described in Chapter 6)—is about twice as large as
normal (Golarai et al., 2010). However, many also have bouts
of severe anxiety or quarrelsome irritability that interfere with
AP Photo/The Hutchinson News, Lindsey Bauman

social relationships (Martens, Wilson, & Reutens, 2008).


Their most surprising skill is language. Although their
language abilities develop more slowly than average, some in-
dividuals have remarkably good language, considering their
impairments in other regards. Figure 14.13 shows the result
when a young woman with Williams syndrome was asked to
draw an elephant and describe it. Contrast her almost poetic
description to the unrecognizable drawing.
Let’s not overstate the case. People with Williams syn-
drome do not handle language perfectly (Martens, Wilson,
People with Williams syndrome have a characteristic appearance,
& Reutens, 2008; Meyer-Lindenberg, Mervis, & Berman,
as well as a special set of behavioral strengths and weaknesses.
2006). Their grammar is awkward, like that of someone
who learned a second language late in life (Clahsen & Al-
lated to numbers, visuospatial skills (e.g., copying a drawing), mazen, 1998; Karmiloff-Smith et al., 1998). If shown a pic-
and spatial perception (e.g., finding their way home). When ture of an unfamiliar object and told its name, they are as
asked to estimate the length of a bus, three people with Wil- likely to think the name refers to part of the object as to the
liams syndrome answered “30 inches,” “3 inches or 100 inches object itself (Stevens & Karmiloff-Smith, 1997). They use
maybe,” and “2 inches, 10 feet” (Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, fancy words when a common word would work better, such
Lai, & St George, 2000). Throughout life, they require con- as “I have to evacuate the glass” instead of “empty” or “pour
stant supervision and cannot hold even simple jobs. out” the glass (Bellugi et al., 2000). Still, observations of
In spite of overall mental retardation, with a mean IQ score Williams syndrome indicate that language is not simply a
around 50–60, many people with Williams syndrome perform byproduct of overall intelligence.

And what an elephant is, it is one of the


animals. And what the elephant does, it
lives in the jungle. It can also live in the
zoo. And what it has, it has long gray
ears, fan ears, ears that can blow in the
wind. It has a long trunk that can pick
up grass, or pick up hay . . . If they’re
in a bad mood it can be terrible . . . If
the elephant gets mad it could stomp; it
could charge, like a bull can charge.
They have long big tusks. They can
damage a car . . . it could be dangerous.
When they’re in a pinch, when they’re
in a bad mood it can be terrible. You
don’t want an elephant as a pet. You
want a cat or a dog or a bird . . .

Figure 14.13  ​A young woman with Williams syndrome draws and describes an elephant
The investigator added the labels on the drawing based on what the woman said she was drawing. (From “Williams Syndrome: An Unusual
Neuropsychological Profile,” by U. Bellugi, P. O. Wang, and T. L. Jernigan, S. H. Broman and J. Grafman, Eds., Atypical Cognitive Deficits in
Developmental Disorders. Copyright © 1994 Lawrence Erlbaum. Reprinted by permission.)
14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 437

Language as a Specialization A Sensitive Period for Language Learning


If language is not just a byproduct of overall intelligence, it If humans are specially adapted to learn language, perhaps we
must have evolved as a specialized brain mechanism. Noam are adapted to learn best during a sensitive period early in life,
Chomsky (1980) and Steven Pinker (1994) proposed that just as sparrows learn their song best during an early period.
humans have a language acquisition device, a built-in One way to test this hypothesis is to see whether people learn a
mechanism for acquiring language. Most children develop second language best if they start young. The consistent result is
language so quickly and easily that it seems they must have that adults are better than children at memorizing the vocabu-
been biologically “prepared” for this learning. Also, deaf lary of a second language, but children have a great advantage
children quickly learn sign language, and if no one teaches on learning the pronunciation and grammar. There is no sharp
them a sign language, they invent one of their own and teach cutoff for learning a second language; starting at age 2 is better
it to one another (Goldin-Meadow, McNeill, & Singleton, than 4, 4 is better than 6, and 13 is better than 16 (Hakuta,
1996; Goldin-Meadow & Mylander, 1998). Bialystok, & Wiley, 2003; Harley & Wang, 1997; Weber-Fox
Researchers have begun to explore the genetic basis & Neville, 1996). However, people who start learning a second
of this preparation for language. Remember that family language beyond age 12 or so almost never reach the level of a
whose members have such trouble with pronunciation and true native speaker (Abrahamsson & Hyltenstam, 2009). Also,
basic grammar? Their problem stems from a mutation in a learning a second language from the start is very different from
gene designated FOXP2 (Lai et al., 2001). Although both learning one later. The first module of this chapter noted that
humans and chimpanzees have that gene, it differs in two the left hemisphere is dominant for language. People who grow
places, resulting in proteins with different amino acids at up in a bilingual home, speaking two languages from the start,
two sites. That gene produces a multitude of effects, partly are an exception. In most cases, they show substantial bilateral
on brain development, but also on structures of the jaw and activity during speech, for both languages. Also, the language
throat that are important for speech (Konopka et al., 2009). areas of their temporal and frontal cortex grow thicker than
If researchers altered this gene in chimpanzees, what would average (Mechelli et al., 2004). People who learn a second lan-
happen? Presumably language depends on more than just guage after age 6 or so activate just the left hemisphere for both
one gene, but the result would be interesting. That study languages (Hull & Vaid, 2007). Many people guess that a bilin-
hasn’t been done, but researchers did alter the FOXP2 gene gual person might rely on the left hemisphere for one language
in mice. The effects included changes in vocalizations and and the right hemisphere for the other. That guess is wrong, as
increased dendritic branching and synaptic plasticity in the the second language depends on the same brain areas as the first
basal ganglia (Enard et al., 2009). (Perani & Abutalebi, 2005).
So back to the original question: How and why did hu- Another way to test the sensitive-period idea is to study
mans evolve language? The fossil record cannot answer a people who learned no language during early childhood. The
question like this, and we are left with speculations. One clearest data come from studies of deaf children whose parents
is that language relates to the long period of dependency concentrated unsuccessfully on teaching them spoken language
in childhood. Social interactions among people, including and lip-reading, and then eventually gave up on this effort and
those between parents and children, favored the evolution introduced sign language. Children who began while still young
of language, and overall intelligence may be a byproduct of learned sign language much better than those who started later
language development more than language is a byproduct of (Harley & Wang, 1997). A child who learns English early can
intelligence (Deacon, 1992, 1997). learn sign language later, and a deaf child who learns sign lan-
guage early can learn English later (except for poor pronuncia-
Stop & Check tion), but a child who learns no language while young is per-
8. What evidence argues against the hypothesis that lan- manently impaired at learning any kind of language (Mayberry,
guage evolution depended simply on the overall evolution Lock, & Kazmi, 2002). This observation strongly supports the
of brain and intelligence? importance of learning language in early childhood.

9. Describe tasks that people with Williams syndrome do


Stop & Check
poorly and those that they do well.
10. What is the strongest evidence in favor of a sensitive pe-
ANSWERS
pressions, social behaviors, some aspects of music. riod for language learning?
Relatively good: language, interpretation of facial ex-
numbers, visual-motor skills, and spatial perception. proficient at it.
ANSWER
nevertheless develop nearly normal language. 9. Poor: language either while they were young) do not become
language. Also, some people are mentally retarded but guage until later in life (and who did not learn spoken
8. Some people have normal brain size but very poor 10. Deaf children who are not exposed to sign lan-
438 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

Brain Damage and Language When people with brain damage suffer impaired language
production, we call it Broca’s aphasia, or nonfluent aphasia,
Another way to study specializations for language is to exam-
regardless of the exact location of damage. People with Broca’s
ine the role of various brain areas. Much of our knowledge has
aphasia also have comprehension deficits when the meaning
come from studies of people with brain damage.
of a sentence depends on prepositions, word endings, or un-
usual word order—in short, when the sentence structure is
Broca’s Aphasia (Nonfluent Aphasia) complicated.
In 1861, French surgeon Paul Broca treated the gangrene of a
patient who had been mute for 30 years. When the man died Difficulty in Language Production
5 days later, Broca did an autopsy and found a lesion in the People with Broca’s aphasia are slow and awkward with all
left frontal cortex. Over the next few years, Broca examined forms of expression, including speaking, writing, and gestur-
the brains of additional patients with aphasia (language im- ing, as well as sign language for the deaf (Cicone, Wapner,
pairment). In nearly all cases, he found damage that included Foldi, Zurif, & Gardner, 1979; Neville et al., 1998; Petitto et
this same area, which is now known as Broca’s area (Figure al., 2000). So Broca’s aphasia relates to language, not just the
14.14). The usual cause was a stroke (an interruption of blood vocal muscles.
flow to part of the brain). Broca published his results in 1865, When people with Broca’s aphasia speak, they omit most
slightly later than papers by other French physicians, Marc pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions, auxiliary (helping)
and Gustave Dax, who also pointed to the left hemisphere as verbs, quantifiers, and tense and number endings. At least,
the seat of language abilities (Finger & Roe, 1996). Broca re- those are the results for people speaking English. People
ceived the credit, however, because his description was more with aphasia use more word endings if they speak German,
detailed and more convincing. This discovery, the first demon- Italian, or other languages in which word endings are more
stration of the function of a particular brain area, paved the critical than they are in English (Blackwell & Bates, 1995).
way for modern neurology. Prepositions, conjunctions, helping verbs, and so forth are
We now know that speaking activates much of the brain, known as the closed class of grammatical forms because a
mostly in the left hemisphere, and not just Broca’s area language rarely adds new prepositions, conjunctions, and
(Wallesch, Henriksen, Kornhuber, & Paulson, 1985) (Figure the like. In contrast, new nouns and verbs (the open class)
14.15). Damage limited to Broca’s area produces only minor
or brief language impairment. Serious deficits result from
extensive damage that extends into other areas as well. Most
cases result from a stroke, but similar deficits result from dis-
eases causing gradual atrophy to Broca’s area and surrounding
areas (S. M. Wilson et al., 2010).

Broca’s area Wernicke’s area

Figure 14.15  ​Brain activity during speech for a normal


adult
This study inferred brain activity from patterns of blood flow. Red
indicates the highest level of activity, followed by yellow, green,
Sylvian or
lateral fissure
and blue. The areas in red showed the greatest increase in activ-
ity during speech. Note the increased activity in many brain areas,
especially on the left side. (Reprinted from Brain and Language,
25/2, Wallesch, Henriksen, Kornhuber, & Paulson, “Observations on
regional cerebral blood flow in cortical and subcortical structures
Figure 14.14  ​Two areas important for language during language production in normal man,” pp. 224–233, 1985,
(© Cengage Learning 2013) with permission from Elsevier.)
14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 439

enter a language frequently. People with Broca’s aphasia Broca’s Area One Step at a Time
seldom use the closed-class words. They find it difficult to Brain damage studies give us only general information about
repeat a phrase such as “No ifs, ands, or buts,” although they what Broca’s area does. More detailed information comes
can successfully repeat, “The general commands the army.” from the rare opportunities to record from individual cells.
Furthermore, patients who cannot read aloud “To be or not Occasionally physicians expose someone’s brain to explore
to be” can read “Two bee oar knot two bee” (H. Gardner & options for treating severe epilepsy. Although the scalp is
Zurif, 1975). Clearly, the trouble is with the word mean- anesthetized, the brain is awake. In a few cases, researchers
ings, not just pronunciation. implanted electrodes to record activity in Broca’s area while
Why do people with Broca’s aphasia omit the grammati- the person listened to sentences or processed them in other
cal words and endings? Perhaps they have suffered damage to ways. The cells that responded first made the same response
a “grammar area” in the brain, but here is another possibility: regardless of what, if anything, the person was supposed to do
When speaking is a struggle, people leave out the weakest ele- with the word. Evidently these cells had something to do with
ments. Many people who are in great pain speak as if they understanding the word. A second group of cells responded a
have Broca’s aphasia (Dick et al., 2001). bit later, and responded more strongly if the instruction was
to change the tense (walk → walked) or number (rock →
Problems in Comprehending Grammatical rocks). A third group, with the latest response, was active in
Words and Devices preparation for saying the word. Those cells responded most
People with Broca’s aphasia have trouble understanding the strongly to long words that required more effort to speak
same kinds of words that they omit when speaking, such as (Sahin, Pinker, Cash, Schomer, & Halgren, 2009). These re-
prepositions and conjunctions. They often misunderstand sults suggest that cells in Broca’s area go through at least three
sentences with complex grammar, such as “The girl that the boy stages in controlling speech.
is chasing is tall” (Zurif, 1980). However, most English sen-
tences follow the subject-verb-object order, and their meaning
is clear even without the prepositions and conjunctions. You Stop & Check
can demonstrate this for yourself by taking a paragraph and
11. What kind of words are Broca’s patients least likely to
deleting its prepositions, conjunctions, articles, helping verbs,
use?
pronouns, and word endings to see how it might appear to
someone with Broca’s aphasia. Here is an example, taken from 12. What kind of words do Broca’s patients have the most
earlier in this section. Note how understandable it is despite trouble understanding?
the deletions:
the closed-class words.
ANSWERS
In 1861, the French surgeon Paul Broca treated
the same kind of words they have trouble producing—

the gangrene of a patient who had been mute for 30 years.


verbs. 12. They have the most trouble understanding
tence, such as prepositions, conjunctions, and helping
When the man died 5 days later, Broca did an autopsy and words that are meaningful only in the context of a sen-
found a lesion in the left frontal cortex. Over the next few 11. They have the greatest trouble with “closed-class”
years, Broca examined the brains of additional patients
with aphasia (language impairment). In nearly all cases, he
found damage that included this same area, which is now
known as Broca’s area. The usual cause was a stroke (an
interruption of blood flow to part of the brain). Wernicke’s Aphasia (Fluent Aphasia)
In 1874, Carl Wernicke (pronounced WER-nih-kee by
Still, people with Broca’s aphasia have not totally lost most English speakers, although the German pronuncia-
their knowledge of grammar. For example, they generally tion is VAYR-nih-keh), a 26-year-old junior assistant in a
recognize that something is wrong with the sentence “He German hospital, discovered that damage in part of the left
written has songs,” even if they cannot say how to improve it temporal cortex produced a different kind of language im-
(Wulfeck & Bates, 1991). In many ways, their comprehen- pairment. Although patients could speak and write, their
sion resembles that of normal people who are distracted. If language comprehension was poor. Damage in and around
you listen to someone speaking rapidly with a heavy accent Wernicke’s area (Figure 14.14), located near the audi-
in a noisy room, while you are trying to do something else at tory cortex, produces Wernicke’s aphasia, characterized
the same time, you catch bits and pieces of what the speaker by poor language comprehension and impaired ability to
says and try to guess the rest. Even when we hear a sentence remember the names of objects. It is also known as fluent
clearly, we sometimes ignore the grammar. If you hear “The aphasia because the person can still speak smoothly. As
dog was bitten by the man,” you might assume it was the with Broca’s aphasia, the symptoms and brain damage vary.
dog that did the biting (Ferreira, Bailey, & Ferraro, 2002). We use the term Wernicke’s aphasia, or fluent aphasia, to
Patients with Broca’s aphasia just rely on inferences more describe a certain pattern of behavior, independent of the
often than others do. location of damage.
440 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

The typical characteristics of Wernicke’s aphasia are as W: Yeah, something like that.
follows: T: Okay, and what is it? You wear it around your waist,
1. Articulate speech. In contrast to people with Broca’s and you cook . . .
aphasia, those with Wernicke’s aphasia speak fluently, W: Cook. Umm, umm, see I can’t remember.
except when pausing to try to think of the name of
something. T: It’s an apron.
2. Difficulty finding the right word. People with Wernicke’s W: Apron, apron, that’s it, apron.
aphasia have anomia (ay-NOME-ee-uh), difficulty T: (Holding another picture) That you wear when you’re
recalling the names of objects. They make up names getting ready for bed after a shower.
(e.g., “thingamajig”), substitute one name for another, W: Oh, I think that he put under different, something dif-
and use roundabout expressions such as “the thing that ferent. We had something, you know, umm, you know.
we used to do with the thing that was like the other one.” T: A different way of doing it?
When they do manage to find some of the right words,
they arrange them improperly, such as, “The Astros W: No, umm . . . umm . . . (Pause)
listened to the radio tonight” (instead of “I listened to T: It’s actually a bathrobe.
the Astros on the radio tonight”) (R. C. Martin & W: Bathrobe. Uh, we didn’t call it that, we called it some-
Blossom-Stach, 1986). thing else.
3. Poor language comprehension. People with Wernicke’s T: Smoking jacket?
aphasia have trouble understanding spoken and written W: No, I think we called it, uh . . .
speech and—in the case of deaf people—sign language
(Petitto et al., 2000). Although many sentences are clear T: Lounging . . .?
enough without the prepositions, word endings, and W: No, no, something, in fact, we called it just . . . (Pause)
grammar that confuse Broca’s aphasics, few sentences T: Robe?
make sense without nouns and verbs (which trouble W: Robe. Or something like that.
Wernicke’s patients).
The following conversation is between a woman with The patient still knows the names of objects and recog-
Wernicke’s aphasia and a speech therapist trying to teach nizes them when she hears them; she just has trouble find-
her the names of some objects. (The Duke University De- ing them for herself. In some ways, her speech resembles
partment of Speech Pathology and Audiology provided this that of a student called upon to speak in a foreign language
dialogue.) class after poorly studying the vocabulary list.
Although Wernicke’s area and surrounding areas are im-
Therapist: (Holding picture of an apron) Can you name portant, language comprehension also depends on the con-
that one? nections to other brain areas. For example, reading the word
Woman: Um . . . you see I can’t, I can I can barely do; he lick activates not only Wernicke’s area but also the part of
would give me sort of umm. . . the motor cortex responsible for tongue movements. Read-
ing throw activates the part of the premotor cortex control-
T: A clue?
ling hand movements (Willems, Hagoort, & Casasanto,
W: That’s right . . . just a like, just a . . . 2010). Apparently when you think about an action word,
T: You mean, like, “You wear that when you wash dishes or you imagine doing it. Table 14.1 contrasts Broca’s aphasia
when you cook a meal . . .”? and Wernicke’s aphasia.

Table 14.1  Broca’s Aphasia and Wernicke’s Aphasia

Type Pronunciation Content of Speech Comprehension

Broca’s aphasia Poor Mostly nouns and verbs; omits prepositions and Impaired if the meaning depends on
other grammatical connectives complex grammar
Wernicke’s aphasia Unimpaired Grammatical but often nonsensical; has trouble Seriously impaired
finding the right word, especially names of objects

© Cengage Learning 2013


14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 441

Stop & Check Stop & Check


13. Describe the speech production of people with 15. In what way do musical compositions vary depending on
Wernicke’s aphasia. the language spoken by the composer?

14. Describe the speech comprehension of people with the composer.


ANSWER
Wernicke’s aphasia. rhythms that are common in the language spoken by
15. Musical compositions tend to follow the same
nicke’s aphasia understand little speech.
ANSWERS
therefore make little sense. 14. People with Wer-
grammatically but omit most nouns and verbs and
13. People with Wernicke’s aphasia speak fluently and

Dyslexia
Dyslexia is a specific impairment of reading in someone
Music and Language with adequate vision, adequate motivation, and adequate
overall cognitive skills. It is more common in boys than girls
Language occurs in every human culture, and no other and has been linked to at least four genes that produce defi-
species develops language as we know it. Exactly the same cits in hearing or cognition (Galaburda, LoTurco, Ramus,
could be said for music. Language and music have many Fitch, & Rosen, 2006). Dyslexia is especially common in
parallels, including the ability of both to evoke strong emo- English because it has so many words with odd spellings.
tions. Broca’s area is strongly activated when orchestral (Consider phlegm, bivouac, khaki, yacht, choir, physique, and
musicians sight-read music, as well as when they perform gnat.) However, dyslexia occurs in all languages and always
difficult visuospatial tasks (Sluming, Brooks, Howard, pertains to a difficulty converting symbols into sounds
Downes, & Roberts, 2007). The parallels between language (Ziegler & Goswami, 2005). A study comparing readers of
and music are sufficient to suggest that they arose together. English and Chinese found that normal readers activated
That is, whatever evolutionary processes helped us develop somewhat different brain areas, presumably because English
language also enabled us to develop music. letters represent sounds, whereas a Chinese symbol repre-
Consider some of the parallels (Patel, 2008): sents a whole syllable or word. However, English-speaking
■ Trained musicians and music students tend to be bet- dyslexics and Chinese-speaking dyslexics were remarkably
ter than average at learning a second language. similar in showing decreased activation in several brain ar-
eas while reading (Hu, et al., 2010).
■ In both language and music, we alter the timing and
As a rule, people with dyslexia are more likely to have
volume to add emphasis or to express emotion.
a bilaterally symmetrical cerebral cortex, whereas in other
■ English speakers average about 0.5 to 0.7 seconds be-
people, the planum temporale and certain other areas are
tween one stressed syllable and another in speech and larger in the left hemisphere (Galaburda, Sherman, Rosen,
prefer music with about 0.5 to 0.7 seconds between Aboitiz, & Geschwind, 1985; Hynd & Semrud-Clikeman,
beats. 1989; Jenner, Rosen, & Galaburda, 1999). Several brain
■ Greek and Balkan languages have less regular rhythms areas in the parietal and temporal cortex have less than av-
than English, and much of the music written by speak- erage gray matter in children with dyslexia and show less
ers of those languages has irregularly spaced beats. arousal during reading (Hoeft et al., 2006; Gabrieli, 2009).
■ English usually stresses the first syllable of a word or
Similar differences are apparent in 5- and 6-year-olds who
phrase, whereas French more often stresses the final have a family history of dyslexia (Raschle, Chang, & Gaab,
syllable. Similarly, French composers more often than 2011). Evidently these brain features represent a predispo-
English composers make the final note of a phrase sition toward dyslexia rather than a result of failure to read.
longer than the others. Reading is a complicated skill that requires seeing subtle
differences as abode vs. adobe, hearing subtle differences as
■ English vowels vary in duration more than French
symphony vs. sympathy, and connecting the sound patterns
vowels do. For example, compare the vowels in tourist
to the visual symbols. In fact, even understanding spoken
or pirate. English composers, on average, have more
language requires a combination of vision and hearing.
variation in note length from one note to the next.
Non-deaf people do far more lip-reading than they realize.
These similarities and others suggest that we use the Consider your experience when you see a foreign film that
language areas of the brain when we compose music, and is dubbed badly or a film in which the soundtrack is slightly
we prefer music that resembles our language in rhythms off from the picture.
and tones (Ross, Choi, & Purves, 2007). You could think of In the often confusing literature about dyslexia, one
music as an alternative method of communication. point that stands out is that different people have different
442 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

kinds of reading problems, and no one explanation works sense words and saying whether they were the same. They
for all. Most (but not all) have auditory problems, a smaller were impaired only when they had to look at a nonsense
number have impaired control of eye movements, and word on the screen and then say whether it was the same as
some have both ( Judge, Caravolas, & Knox, 2006). Some a nonsense word they heard (Snowling, 1980).
researchers distinguish between dysphonetic dyslexics and Many people with dyslexia also have abnormalities in
dyseidetic dyslexics (Flynn & Boder, 1991), although many their attention (Facoetti, Corradi, Ruffino, Gori, & Zorzi,
people with dyslexia do not fit neatly into either category 2010). Here is a demonstration. Fixate your eyes on the
(Farmer & Klein, 1995). Dysphonetic dyslexics have trou- central dot in each display below and, without moving your
ble sounding out words, so they try to memorize each word eyes left or right, try to read the middle letter of each three-
as a whole, and when they don’t recognize a word, they letter display:
guess based on context. For example, they might read the
word laugh as “funny.” Dyseidetic readers sound out words NOE •
well enough, but they fail to recognize a word as a whole.
They read slowly and have particular trouble with irregu- • TWC
larly spelled words.
WSH •
The most severe cases of dyseidetic dyslexia result from
brain damage that restricts the field of vision. People who • EYO
see only one letter at a time have many short eye movements,
CTN •
very slow reading, and particular difficulty with long words.
In one study, normal people viewed words on a computer • ONT
screen while a device monitored their eye movements and
blurred every letter on the screen except the one the viewer HCW •
focused on. The result was very slow reading (Rayner & • OHW
Johnson, 2005).
Most but not all people with dyslexia have auditory IEY •
problems (Caccappolo-van Vliet, Miozzo, & Stern, 2004). • WCI
Brain scans have shown that dyslexics’ brains, on average,
show less than normal responses to speech sounds, espe- HNO •
cially consonants (Helenius, Salmelin, Richardson, Lei- • SIY
nonen, & Lyytinen, 2002; McCrory, Frith, Brunswick, &
Price, 2000). Many people with dyslexia have particular
trouble detecting the temporal order of sounds, such as Most people find it easier to read the letters close to the
noticing the difference between beep-click-buzz and beep- fixation point, but some people with dyslexia are unusually
buzz-click (Farmer & Klein, 1995; Kujala et al., 2000; Na- adept at identifying letters well to the right of their fixa-
garajan et al., 1999). They also have much difficulty mak- tion point. When they focus on a word, they are worse than
ing Spoonerisms—that is, trading the first consonants of average at reading it but better than average at perceiving
two words, such as listening to “dear old queen” and saying letters 5 to 10 degrees to the right of it (Geiger, Lettvin, &
“queer old dean” or hearing “way of life” and replying “lay of Zegarra-Moran, 1992; Lorusso et al., 2004). That kind of
wife” (Paulesu et al., 1996). Doing so, of course, requires attentional focus could certainly confuse attempts at read-
close attention to sounds and their order. Many people ing (De Luca, Di Page, Judica, Spinelli, & Zoccolotti, 1999).
with dyslexia have trouble with other temporal order tasks Figure 14.16 shows the mean results for normal readers and
as well, such as tapping a regular rhythm with the fingers for people with dyslexia.
(Wolff, 1993). For people with this abnormality, an effective treatment
However, the problem cannot be simply impaired hear- might be to teach them to attend to just one word at a time.
ing. Many deaf or partly deaf people can read, and people Some children and adults with dyslexia have been told to
with dyslexia have no trouble carrying on a conversation place over the page that they are reading a sheet of paper
(which would be difficult if their hearing were seriously with a window cut out of it that is large enough to expose
impaired). The problem must be something more specific, just one word. In 3 months, 15 dyslexic children improved
such as paying attention to certain aspects of sound or con- their reading skills by 1.22 grade levels (Geiger, Lettvin, &
necting sound to vision. In one study, dyslexics performed Fahle, 1994). Four dyslexic adults also made spectacular
normally at watching nonsense words flashed on the screen progress; one advanced from a third-grade to a tenth-grade
and saying whether they were the same or different. (For ex- reading level in 4 months (Geiger et al., 1992). After about
ample, brap-brap would be the same and sond-snod would be the first 3 weeks of practice, they no longer needed the spe-
different.) They were also normal at listening to two non- cial cutout sheet of paper.
14.2 Evolution and Physiology of Language 443

Ordinary readers
Dyslexic readers
100%

Correct identification
of letters
50

–10° 0° 10°
Left Distance from fixation point Right

Figure 14.16  ​Identification of letters at various distances from the fixation point
Normal readers identify a letter most accurately when it is closest to the fixation point, and their accuracy drops for letters farther
away. Many people with dyslexia are more accurate than normal readers are in identifying letters 5 to 10 degrees to the right of
fixation. (Reprinted from “Task-Determined Strategies of Visual Process,” by G. Geiger, J. Y. Lettvin, & U. Zagarra-Moran, 1992, Cognitive
Brain Research, 1, pp. 39–52, 1992, with kind permission of Elsevier Science-NL, Sara Burgerhartstraat 25, 1055 KV Amsterdam, The
Netherlands.)

One final twist: Of the four adults with dyslexia who went Stop & Check
through this process, three decided that they would rather re-
16. What usually gives the most problems to a person with
turn to being dyslexic! While dyslexic, they could attend to
dyslexia—vision, hearing, or connecting vision to hearing?
several tasks at once, such as talking to someone, listening to
news on the radio, creating a work of art, and so forth. When necting visual stimuli to sounds.
ANSWER
they learned to read one word at a time, they found themselves 16. Generally, the greatest problem arises with con-
able to perform only one task at a time, and they missed their
old way of life. In short, their reading skills were tied to their
overall attentional strategies.

Module 14.2  ■  In Closing

Language and the Brain

Perhaps the best summary of dyslexia is also the best summary many ways for many reasons. Language is not simply a byprod-
of language impairments in general: Language and reading are uct of overall intelligence, but it is hardly independent of other
sufficiently complicated that people can become impaired in intellectual functions either.

Summary
1. Chimpanzees can learn to communicate through gestures more language progress than common chimpanzees
or nonvocal symbols, although their output does not because of species differences, early onset of training, and
closely resemble human language. Bonobos have made different training methods.  432
444 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

2. An African gray parrot has shown surprising language second language in early childhood differs in many ways
abilities, with a brain organized differently from that of from learning it later.  437
primates.  434 6. People with Broca’s aphasia (nonfluent aphasia) have
3. The hypothesis that language emerged as a byproduct of difficulty speaking and writing. They find prepositions,
overall intelligence or brain size faces major problems: conjunctions, and other grammatical connectives
Some people have full-size brains and otherwise normal especially difficult. They also fail to understand speech
intelligence but impaired language, and people with when its meaning depends on complex grammar.  438
Williams syndrome have nearly normal language despite 7. People with Wernicke’s aphasia have trouble understand-
mental retardation.  434 ing speech and recalling the names of objects.  439
4. People are specialized to learn language easily, partly 8. Music has many parallels with language. Composers
because of one gene that differs between humans and usually write music with rhythm patterns that resemble
chimpanzees.  437 the rhythm of speech in their own language.  441
5. The best evidence for a sensitive period for language 9. Dyslexia (reading impairment) has many forms. The
development is the observation that deaf children learn main problem is usually in converting visual signals into
sign language much better if they start early than if their auditory information or attending to the right aspects of
first opportunity comes later in life. Also, learning a a visual display.  441
key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
anomia  440 Broca’s area  438 Wernicke’s aphasia (fluent
aphasia  438 dyslexia  441 aphasia)  439
Broca’s aphasia (nonfluent language acquisition device  437 Wernicke’s area  439
aphasia)  438 productivity  432 Williams syndrome  435

Thought Questions
1. Most people with Broca’s aphasia suffer from partial person can still see and speak. What is a possible
paralysis on the right side of the body. Most people neurological explanation? That is, can you imagine a
with Wernicke’s aphasia do not. Why? pattern of brain damage that might produce this
2. In a syndrome called word blindness, a person loses the result?
ability to read (even single letters), although the
Module 14.3

Conscious and Unconscious


Processes and Attention

I
n Chapter 1, we introduced the mind–body problem: In a The alternative to dualism is monism, the belief that the
universe composed of matter and energy, why is there such a universe consists of only one kind of substance. Various forms
thing as consciousness? And how does it relate to the brain? of monism are possible, grouped into the following categories:
Now armed with more understanding of the brain, it is time to
return to those questions (even if we can’t answer them). ■ materialism: the view that everything that exists is mate-
rial, or physical. According to one version of this view
(“eliminative materialism”), mental events don’t exist at
The Mind–Brain Relationship all, and any folk psychology based on minds and mental
Suppose you say, “I became frightened because I saw a man activity is fundamentally mistaken. However, most of us
with a gun.” A neuroscientist says, “You became frightened find it difficult to believe that our minds are figments of
because of increased electrochemical activity in the central our imagination! A more plausible version is that we will
amygdala of your brain.” If both statements are right, what is eventually find a way to explain all psychological experi-
the connection between them? ences in purely physical terms.
■ mentalism: the view that only the mind really exists
Biological explanations of behavior raise the mind–body
or mind–brain problem: What is the relationship between and that the physical world could not exist unless
the mind and the brain? The most widespread view among some mind were aware of it. It is not easy to test this
nonscientists is, no doubt, dualism, the belief that mind and idea—go ahead and try!—but few philosophers or
body are different kinds of substance that exist independently. scientists take it seriously.
The French philosopher René Descartes defended dualism ■ identity position: the view that mental processes and

but recognized the vexing issue of how a mind that is not made certain kinds of brain processes are the same thing,
of material could influence a physical brain. He proposed that described in different terms. By analogy, one could
mind and brain interact at a single point in space, which he describe the Mona Lisa as an extraordinary painting,
suggested was the pineal gland, the smallest unpaired struc- or one could list the exact color and brightness of each
ture he could find in the brain (Figure 1.5). point on the painting. Although the two descriptions
Although we credit Descartes with the first ex- appear entirely different, they refer to the same object.
plicit defense of dualism, he hardly originated the A
idea. Our experiences seem so different from the 5
physical actions of the brain that most people take it 6
3
a 1
for granted that mind and brain are different. How- H. b
4
2
ever, nearly all current philosophers and neurosci- c
6
50
B
entists reject dualism. The decisive objection is that 2
4
5
3
dualism conflicts with one of the cornerstones of 1
physics, known as the law of the conservation of mat-
ter and energy: So far as we can tell, the total amount B C
of matter and energy in the universe has been fixed
since the Big Bang that originated it all. Matter can
transform into energy or energy into matter, but nei-
ther one emerges from nothing or disappears into Figure 14.17  ​René Descartes’s conception of brain and mind
Descartes understood how light from an object (the arrow) reached the
nothing. Because matter alters its course only when
retinas at the back of the eyes. The letters and numbers represent pathways
other matter or energy acts upon it, a mind that is that he imagined from the retinas to the pineal gland. (His guesses about
not composed of matter or energy could not make those pathways were wrong.) (From Descartes’ Treatises on Man)
anything happen, including muscle movements.
445
446 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

The identity position does not say that the mind is the Stop & Check
brain. It says the mind is brain activity. Just as fire is not a
“thing,” but what happens to something, mental activity is 18. What is meant by the “hard problem”?
what happens in the brain. The identity position also does not sciousness?
say brain activity “causes consciousness.” Brain activity does ANSWER
physical world. Why is there such a thing as con-
not cause consciousness any more than consciousness causes 18. The hard problem is why minds exist at all in a
brain activity. Each is the same as the other.
Can we be sure that monism is correct? No. However,
researchers adopt it as the most reasonable working hy-
pothesis, to see how much progress they can make on that Brain Activity Associated
assumption. As you have seen throughout this text, experi-
ences and brain activities appear inseparable. Stimulation of with Consciousness
any brain area provokes an experience, and any experience Although we don’t have even a good hypothesis about why
evokes brain activity, and damage to any brain area leads to brain activity is (sometimes) conscious, we might be able to
loss of some mental function. As far as we can tell, you can- discover which types of brain activity are conscious (Crick &
not have mental activity without brain activity. If you use Koch, 2004). Once we answer that question, we might be bet-
terms like mind to mean a ghostlike something that is nei- ter at identifying signs of consciousness in nonhuman animals,
ther matter nor energy, don’t underestimate the scientific infants, or brain-damaged people. Already researchers tenta-
and philosophical arguments that can be marshaled against tively use brain measurements for that purpose. Researchers
you (Dennett, 1991). used fMRI to record brain activity in a young woman who
(Does a belief in monism mean that we are lowering our was in a persistent vegetative state following a brain injury in
evaluation of minds? Maybe not. Maybe we are elevating our a traffic accident. She had neither spoken nor made any other
concept of the material world.) purposive movements. However, when she was told to imagine
playing tennis, the fMRI showed increased activity in motor
areas of her cortex, similar to what healthy volunteers showed.
Stop & Check
When she was told to imagine walking through her house, a
17. Why do nearly all scientists and philosophers reject the
different set of brain areas became active, again similar to those
idea of dualism?
of healthy volunteers (Owen et al., 2006). Follow-up studies
found another patient in a vegetative state whose brain showed
ANSWER energy. similar responses to instructions, although several other pa-
of your body, is to act on it with other matter and tients did not (K. Smith, 2007). Do these results mean that
way to influence matter and energy, including that certain patients in a vegetative state are actually conscious? The
of matter and energy. According to that law, the only answer is not certain, but the possibilities are exciting.
17. Dualism contradicts the law of the conservation One of the main problems in research is that we cannot
observe consciousness. Even defining it is treacherously dif-
ficult. For practical purposes, researchers use this operational
definition: If a cooperative person reports awareness of one
David Chalmers (1995) distinguished between what he stimulus and not another, then he or she was conscious of
calls the easy problems and the hard problem of conscious- the first and not the second. With individuals who cannot
ness. The easy problems pertain to such questions as the speak—such as infants, people with Broca’s aphasia, or non-
difference between wakefulness and sleep and what brain ac- human animals—this definition doesn’t apply. We might infer
tivity occurs during consciousness. These issues are difficult consciousness based on other criteria, but we won’t use such
scientifically but not philosophically. In contrast, the hard individuals for research.
problem concerns why consciousness exists at all. As Chalm- Using this definition, the next step is to present a given
ers (1995, p. 203) put it, “Why doesn’t all this information- stimulus under two conditions. In one condition we expect
processing go on ‘in the dark,’ free of any inner feel?” Why the observer to be conscious of it, and in the other condition
does brain activity feel like anything at all? Many scientists we expect the observer to be unconscious of it. In both cases
(Crick & Koch, 2004) and philosophers (Chalmers, 2004) the stimulus excites receptors that send a message to the brain,
agree that we cannot answer that question, at least at pres- but once the message reaches the brain presumably something
ent. We don’t even have a clear hypothesis to test. The best different happens for conscious vs. unconscious processing.
we can do is determine what brain activity is necessary or How could we present a stimulus while preventing con-
sufficient for consciousness. Maybe research on subordinate sciousness? Researchers have developed clever approaches,
questions will some day lead us to a breakthrough on the mostly based on interference. Suppose you clearly see a yellow
hard question, or maybe not. But starting with the “easy” dot. Then, although the dot remains on the screen, other dots
questions seems the best strategy. around it flash on and off. While they are flashing you may be
14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention 447

unable to see the stationary dot. This procedure is called “flash scious stimuli also produce more consistent responses from
suppression” (Kreiman, Fried, & Koch, 2002). Similarly, sup- one trial to another than do similar but unconscious stimuli
pose you see a yellow dot, and then some blue dots all around (Schurger, Pereira, Treisman, & Cohen, 2010). Becoming
it start moving rapidly. They grab your attention so strongly conscious of something means that its information takes over
that you have trouble seeing the yellow dot. In fact, it seems to more of your brain’s activity.
disappear for a few seconds, reappear for a few seconds, disap-
pear again, and so forth (Bonneh, Cooperman, & Sagi, 2001).
Stop & Check
19. In the experiment by Dehaene et al., how were the con-
Experiments Using Masking scious and unconscious stimuli similar? How were they
Many studies use masking: a brief visual stimulus is preceded different?
and followed by longer interfering stimuli. In many cases, just
the later stimulus is presented, in which case it is called back- 20. In this experiment, how did the brain’s responses differ
ward masking. In one study, researchers flashed a word on a between the conscious and unconscious stimuli?
screen for 29 milliseconds (ms). On some trials, it was pre- tern is conscious.
ceded and followed by a blank screen: ANSWERS
brain responses become synchronized when a pat-
then the activity spread to additional areas. Also,
as an unconscious stimulus but more strongly, and
GROVE became conscious, it activated the same brain areas
lowed by an interfering pattern. 20. If a stimulus
not become conscious if it was preceded and fol-

In these cases, people identified the word almost 90% of


for 29 ms). The difference was that a stimulus did
physically the same (a word flashed on the screen
the time. On other trials, the researchers flashed a word for 19. The conscious and unconscious stimuli were
the same 29 ms but preceded and followed it with masking
patterns:

SALTY Experiments Using Binocular Rivalry


Here is another way to make a stimulus unconscious. Look at
Figure 14.18, but hold it so close to your eyes that your nose
In the masking condition, people usually say they saw touches the page, right between the two circles. Better yet, look
no word at all, and almost never identify it. Using fMRI and at the two parts through a pair of tubes, such as the tubes in-
evoked potentials, the researchers found that the stimulus ini- side rolls of paper towels or toilet paper. You will see red and
tially activates the primary visual cortex for both the conscious black vertical stripes with your left eye and green and black
and unconscious conditions but activates it more strongly in horizontal stripes with your right eye. (Close one eye and then
the conscious condition (because of less interference). Also, the other to make sure you see completely different patterns
in the conscious condition, the activity spreads to additional with the two eyes.) Seeing something requires seeing where it
brain areas (Dehaene et al., 2001), including the prefrontal is, and the red vertical stripes cannot be in the same place as the
cortex and parietal cortex. Those areas apparently amplify the green horizontal stripes. Because your brain cannot perceive
signal. For people with damage to the prefrontal cortex, a vi- both patterns in the same location, your perception alternates.
sual stimulus has to last longer before it becomes conscious, For a while, you see the red and black stripes, and then gradu-
relative to other people (Del Cul, Dehaene, Reyes, Bravo, & ally, the green and black invade your consciousness. Then your
Slachevsky, 2009). perception shifts back to the red and black. For the average per-
A conscious stimulus also synchronizes responses for neu- son, each perception lasts about 2 seconds before switching to
rons in various brain areas (Eckhorn et al., 1988; Gray, König, the other, but some people switch faster or slower. These shifts,
Engel, & Singer, 1989; Melloni et al., 2007; Womelsdorf et al., known as binocular rivalry, are gradual, sweeping from one
2007). When you see something and recognize it, it evokes activ- side to another. The two images do not necessarily divide your
ity precisely synchronized in several brain areas, in the frequency attention time equally. Some people see with one
of about 30–50 Hz (cycles per second), known as gamma waves. eye longer than the other. Also, an emotionally
(Doesburg, Green, McDonald, & Ward, 2009; Fisch et al., charged image, such as a picture of a happy face,
2009). One consequence of synchronized action potentials is holds attention longer than a neutral image yourself try it 

that their synaptic inputs arrive simultaneously at their target (Yoon, Hong, Joormann, & Kang, 2009).
cells, producing maximal summation (Fell & Axmacher, 2011). The stimulus seen by each eye evokes a brain response
Overall, the data imply that consciousness of a stimulus that researchers can measure with fMRI or similar methods.
depends on the amount and spread of brain activity. Con- As the first perception fades and the stimulus seen by the
448 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

other eye replaces it, the first pattern of brain activity fades much of the brain, virtually taking over brain activity. When
also, and a different pattern replaces it. Each shift in percep- the same stimulus is unconscious, it produces weaker and
tion is accompanied by a shift in the activity over a large por- less widespread activity.
tion of the brain (Lee, Blake, & Heeger, 2005).
Both the red–black and green–black patterns you just
experienced were stationary. To make the brain responses
Stop & Check
easier to distinguish, researchers presented to one eye
a stationary stimulus and to the other eye a pattern that 21. How could someone use fMRI to determine which of two
pulsated in size and brightness, as shown in Figure 14.19. patterns in binocular rivalry is conscious at a given mo-
Then they recorded brain activity in several areas. At times ment?
when people reported consciousness of the pulsating stimu-
lus, pulsating activity at the same rhythm was prominent in ANSWER
when that pattern is conscious.
much of the brain, as shown in Figure 14.20. When people
The rhythm takes over widespread areas of the brain

reported consciousness of the stationary stimulus, the pul-


look for brain areas showing that rhythm of activity.
21. Make one stimulus pulsate at a given rhythm and
sating activity was weak (Cosmelli et al., 2004). Again, the
conclusion is that a conscious stimulus strongly activates

Figure 14.18  ​Binocular rivalry


If possible, look at the two circles through tubes, such as those from inside rolls of toilet paper or paper towels. Otherwise, touch your
nose to the paper between the two parts so that your left eye sees one pattern while your right eye sees the other. The two views will
compete for your consciousness, and your perception will alternate between them. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Frame 1 Frame 2 Frame 3 Frame 4 Frame 5 Frame 6 Frame 7

5 6 7 8 9 10
Seconds

Figure 14.19  ​Stimuli for a study of binocular rivalry


The pattern in one eye was stationary. The one in the other eye pulsated a few times per second. Researchers could then examine brain
activity to find cells that followed the rhythm of the pulsating stimulus. (Reprinted from NeuroImage, 23/1, Cosmelli et al. “Waves of con-
sciousness: Ongoing cortical patterns during binocular rivalry,” 128–140, 2004, with permission from Elsevier.)
14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention 449

RT13
RT16
RO12
LO12
LO11
LP34
10 15 20 25 30 35 40
Time (sec)

Figure 14.20  ​Brain activity during binocular rivalry


When the person reported seeing the pulsating stimulus, neurons throughout much of the brain responded vigorously at the same rhythm
as the stimulus. When the person reported the stationary stimulus, the rhythmic activity subsided. (Reprinted from NeuroImage, 23/1,
Cosmelli et al. “Waves of consciousness: Ongoing cortical patterns during binocular rivalry,” 128–140, 2004, with permission from Elsevier.)

The Fate of an Unattended Stimulus partly conscious of one stimulus and partly conscious of an-
Let’s further consider binocular rivalry. While you are attend- other? Does consciousness come in degrees?
ing to, say, the green and black stripes, your brain does not com- This is not an easy question to answer, but one study sug-
pletely discard information from the red and black stripes in gests that consciousness is a yes–no phenomenon. Research-
your other eye. Certainly, if a bright stimulus suddenly flashed ers flashed blurry words on a screen for brief fractions of a
in that eye, it would capture your attention. More interestingly, second and asked people to identify each word, if possible,
suppose a word fades onto the screen slowly, and you are to and rate how conscious they were of the word on a scale from
report the time when your attention shifts to the previously 0 to 100. People almost always rated a word either 0 or 100.
unattended eye. The word will capture your attention, causing They almost never said they were partly conscious of some-
you to shift your attention faster than you would have other- thing (Sergent & Dehaene, 2004). These results suggest that
wise. Moreover, if it is a word from your own language, or bet- consciousness is a threshold phenomenon. When a stimulus
ter yet your own name, it captures your attention faster than if activates enough neurons to a sufficient extent, the activity
it were a word from a language you do not understand ( Jiang, reverberates, magnifies, and extends over much of the brain.
Costello, & He, 2007). If a meaningful stimulus captures your If a stimulus fails to reach that level, the pattern fades away.
attention faster than a meaningless stimulus, somehow your
brain had to know it was meaningful before it became con- The Timing of Consciousness
scious! The conclusion is that much of brain activity is uncon-
scious, and even unconscious activity can influence behavior. Are you conscious of events instant by instant as they happen?
It certainly seems that way, but if there were a delay between
an event and your consciousness of it, how would you know?
You wouldn’t. Perhaps you sometimes construct a conscious
Stop & Check
experience after the event.
22. If someone is aware of the stimulus on the right in a case Consider the phi phenomenon that perceptual researchers
of binocular rivalry, what evidence indicates that the brain noted long ago: If you see a dot in one position alternating with
is also processing the stimulus on the left? a similar dot nearby, it will seem that the dot is moving back and
forth. Considering just the simplest case, imagine what happens
if you see a dot in one position and then another: • → •. You
language.
ANSWER
see a dot in one position, it appears to move, and you see it in
meaningful word than if it is a word from an unfamiliar
attention shifts to the left faster if that stimulus is a
22. If a stimulus gradually appears on the left side, the second position. Okay, but when did you see it move? When
you saw it in the first position, you didn’t know it was going to
appear in the second position. You could not perceive it as mov-
ing until after it appeared in the second position. Evidently, you
Consciousness as a Threshold perceived it as moving from one position to the second after
it appeared in the second position! In other words, the second
Phenomenon position changed your perception of what occurred before it.
In binocular rivalry, you might be aware of a pattern in one Another example: Suppose you hear a recorded word
part of your visual field and another pattern in another part, that is carefully engineered to sound halfway between dent
but each point in the visual field sees just one or the other. Is and tent. We’ll call it *ent. If you hear it in the phrase “*ent
that a general principle, or do occasions arise when you are in the fender,” it sounds like dent. If you hear it in the phrase
450 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

“*ent in the forest,” it sounds like tent. That is, later words As you deliberately shift your attention, you increase ac-
changed what you heard before them (Connine, Blasko, & tivity in the appropriate brain area. Another demonstration:
Hall, 1991). What is the current sensation in your left foot? Chances are,
before you read this question, you were not conscious of any
Stop & Check sensation in your left foot. When you directed your atten-
tion to it, activity increased in the corresponding part of the
23. In what way does the phi phenomenon imply that a new
somatosensory cortex (Lambie & Marcel, 2002). Similarly,
stimulus sometimes changes consciousness of what
when you direct your attention to a visual stimulus, your
went before it?
brain’s response to that stimulus increases, while responses
peared on the right. to other stimuli decrease (Kamitani & Tong, 2005; Wegener,
ANSWER
reason to infer that movement until after the dot ap- Freiwald, & Kreiter, 2004).
before the dot on the right, but the person had no One of psychologists’ favorite ways to study attention is
to right. The perceived movement would have occurred the Stroop effect, the difficulty of ignoring words and saying
dot on the right perceives the dot as moving from left the color of ink. In the following display, say aloud the color of
23. Someone who sees a dot on the left and then a ink of each word, ignoring the words themselves:
RED BLUE GREEN GREEN BROWN BLUE
RED PURPLE GREEN RED
Attention After all your years of learning to read words, it is hard to sup-
press that habit and respond to the color instead. However,
Attention is closely aligned with consciousness. Of all that when people successfully do so, they enhance the activity in
your eyes see at any instant, you are conscious of only those the color–vision areas of the cortex and decrease the activity
few to which you direct your attention (Huang, Treisman, & in the areas responsible for identifying words (Polk, Drake,
Pashler, 2007). For example, consider inattentional blind- Jonides, Smith, & Smith, 2008).
ness or change blindness: If something in a complex scene Directing your attention toward something requires in-
changes slowly, or changes while you blink your eyes, you creasing activity in some neurons and decreasing it in others.
probably will not notice it unless you are paying attention to Suppose, for example, you are looking for a friend in a crowd,
the particular item that changes (Henderson & but it’s a carnival crowd. Some people are dressed as clowns or
Hollingworth, 2003; Rensink, O’Regan, & wearing other gaudy attire, but your friend is wearing a plain
Clark, 1997). You can experience this phenom- try it shirt and blue jeans. You need to suppress the attention and
enon with the Online Try It Yourself exercise yourself activity that the highly unusual items would ordinarily attract
online
“Change Blindness.” (Mevorach, Hodsoll, Allen, Shalev, & Humphreys, 2010).
Deliberate, top-down direction of attention depends on parts
Brain Areas Controlling Attention of the prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex (Buschman &
Miller, 2007; Rossi, Bichot, Desimone, & Ungerleider, 2007).
Psychologists distinguish bottom-up from top-down atten- Here is an experiment demonstrating that point: On each
tion. A bottom-up process is a reaction to a stimulus. If you trial, a participant viewed a display similar to Figure 14.21,
are sitting on a park bench, gazing off into the distance, when visible for half a second. A square appeared in the center of
suddenly a deer runs past you, it grabs your attention. A top- the screen, indicating the color to which the observer should
down process is intentional. You might be looking for some- attend. On the right were several lines differing in color and
one you know in a crowd, and you have to check one face after orientation. The task was to identify the line of the indicated
another to find the one you want. color and then indicate whether or not it was vertical. If the
You can control your attention (top-down) even without central square directed attention to the same color for many
moving your eyes. To illustrate, keep your eyes fixated on the trials in a row—green, for example—the task was fairly easy.
central x in the following display. Then attend to the G at the But when the color switched from one trial to the next, the
right and gradually shift your attention clockwise around the participant had to alter attention, and the result was increased
circle. Notice how you become aware of different parts of the activity in parts of the prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex.
circle without moving your eyes. Furthermore, monkeys with damage to the prefrontal cortex
A performed this task at near-normal levels when the central
Z V cue stayed the same color many times in a row, but they suf-
R fered severely if the color changed frequently from trial to trial
W
(Rossi, Pessoa, Desimone, & Ungerleider, 2009).
B x G Your ability to resist distraction on tasks like this fluctu-
ates. That is, you might pay close attention for a while and
N K then get distracted. In another experiment, people viewed
displays like that in Figure 14.22. The task was to report
F J P the orientation (vertical or horizontal) of the line inside the
14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention 451

Figure 14.22  ​Example stimulus for experiment on


distraction
The task required attending to the circle, wherever it was. On
some trials one of the squares was red. Although irrelevant to the
task, it often attracted attention and slowed performance. (From
Leber, A. B. (2010). Neural predictors of within-subject fluctuations
in attentional control. 11458–11465.)

Stop & Check


24. What brain response was related to people’s ability to
resist distraction from an irrelevant red square among the
green squares and circle?

presentation of stimuli.
ANSWER
Figure 14.21  ​Example stimulus for experiment on of activity in part of the prefrontal cortex before the
controlling attention 24. Resistance to distraction related to the amount
The square in the center of the visual field indicated the color to
attend on the right. The participant indicated whether that line
was or was not vertical. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

Unilateral Neglect
Spectacular failure of attention often occurs in people with
damage to the right hemisphere. Many such people show spatial
green circle, ignoring the five green squares. On some trials, neglect—a tendency to ignore the left side of the body or the left
one of the squares was red instead of green. Anything that side of objects. (Damage in the left hemisphere seldom produces
is different attracts attention, and on average, people re- significant neglect of the right side.) They also generally ignore
sponded a bit more slowly on trials with a red square pres- much of what they hear in the left ear and feel in the left hand,
ent. However, the amount of delay varied from trial to trial. especially if they simultaneously feel something in the right
On trials when activity was enhanced in the middle fron- hand. They may put clothes on only the right side of the body.
tal gyrus (part of the prefrontal cortex) at the start of the If asked to point “straight ahead,” most patients with ne-
trial (before seeing the stimuli), the red square produced glect point to the right of center. If a patient with neglect is
the least distraction (Leber, 2010). This result confirms the shown a long horizontal line and asked to divide it in half,
importance of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention generally the person picks a spot well to the right of center, as
and suppressing distraction. if part of the left side wasn’t there (Richard, Honoré, Bernati,
The ability to resist distraction also varies among indi- & Rousseaux, 2004).
viduals. People with attention-deficit disorder are notorious People with intact brains generally do not hit the center of
for their vulnerability to distraction. In general, people who the line but veer 2% to 3% to the left of center. Also, if they are
habitually play action video games perform above average asked to indicate a rating of something along a scale from left to
on many tests of attention, because of greatly enhanced top- right, they show a slight tendency to prefer the left side (Nich-
down control (Mishra, Zinni, Bavelier, & Hillyard, 2011). It olls, Orr, Okubo, & Loftus, 2006). For example, on the questions
seems likely that years of playing video games improves atten- that follow, most people would rate their political views slightly
tion, although the alternative possibility remains that people more conservative on the first question than on the second:
who start with better attention abilities are the most likely to 1. Rate your political views on the following scale:
persist at video games. One experiment found that 21 hours
|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|
of playing action video games was not enough to produce a
most conservative moderate most liberal
major effect (Boot, Kramer, Simons, Fabiani, & Gratton,
2008). In addition to video games, one study found that three 2. Rate your political views on the following scale:
months of intensive training at meditation also improves cer- |—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|—|
tain aspects of attention (Lutz et al., 2009). most liberal moderate most conservative
452 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

You might try the following demonstration. Try marking the AAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAA
A A A
A A
A A A
A
A
center of the line below. Then measure it to see A A A

A
A
A A

A
how close you came. Most people miss slightly to

A
A A

A
A
A A

A A A A A A
the left. Curiously, people with extensive musical

A A A
A A
A A
training usually get within 1% of the exact center try it  A A

A A
yourself A A
(Patston, Corballis, Hogg, & Tippett, 2006). A A

A
A A

A
A
A A

A
A A
A A A
A A A A
AAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAAA A A A

Some patients with neglect also show deviations when esti- (a) (b)
mating the midpoint of a numerical range. For example, what is
• •
halfway between 11 and 19? The correct answer is, of course, 15, HHHHHHHHHH • •
H • •
but some people with neglect say “17.” Evidently, they discount H • •
• •
the lower numbers as if they were on the left side (Doricchi, H
H
• •
• •
Guariglia, Gasparini, & Tomaiuolo, 2005; Zorzi, Priftis, & Um- HHHHHHHHHH • • • • • • • • •• •
H • •
iltà, 2002). At least in Western society, many people visualize the H • •
• •
numbers as a line stretching to the right, as in the x axis of a graph. H
HHHHHHHHHH
• •
• •
All of these results vary, depending on the amount and • •

location of right hemisphere damage (Buxbaum, 2006). Peo- (c) (d)


ple with damage to the inferior right parietal cortex tend to
neglect everything to the left of their own body. People with
damage to the right superior temporal cortex neglect the left
side of objects, even if they are on the right side of the body
(Hillis et al., 2005). After damage to an axon pathway called
the right superior longitudinal fasciculus, connecting the right
posterior parietal cortex to the prefrontal cortex, people ne-
glect the left side almost always. After damage that spares
this pathway, people neglect the left side only when distracted (e)
by something on the right side (Ptak & Schnider, 2010). Figure 14.23  ​Spatial neglect
Although some neglect patients have sensory losses, in A patient with neglect identified the overall figures as E, O, and X,
many cases, the main problem is loss of attention rather than indicating that she saw the whole figures. However, when asked to
impaired sensation. One patient was shown a letter E, com- cross off the elements that composed them, she crossed off only
posed of small Hs, as in Figure 14.23(c). She identified it as a the parts on the right. (From J. C. Marshall and P. W. Halligan, “See-
big E composed of small Hs, indicating that she saw the whole ing the forest but only half the trees?” Nature, 373, pp. 521–523,
figure. However, when she was then asked to cross off all the Figure 1 [parts C and E]. © 1995 Nature.)
Hs, she crossed off only the ones on the right. When she was
shown the figures in Figure 14.23(e), she identified them as an
O composed of little Os and an X composed of little Xs. Again, Other manipulations also shift the attention. For example,
she could see both halves of both figures, but when she was some patients with neglect usually report feeling nothing with
asked to cross off all the elements, she crossed off only the ones the left hand, especially if the right hand feels something at
on the right. The researchers summarized by saying she saw the the time. However, if you cross one hand over the other as
forest but only half the trees (Marshall & Halligan, 1995). shown in Figure 14.24, the person is more likely to report feel-
Several procedures increase attention to the neglected side. ing the left hand, which is now on the right side of the body
Simply telling the person to pay attention to the left side helps (Aglioti, Smania, & Peru, 1999). Also, the person ordinarily
temporarily. So does having the person look left while at the has trouble pointing to anything in the left visual field but has
same time feeling an object with the left hand (Vaishnavi, Cal- somewhat better success if the hand was so far to the left that
houn, & Chatterjee, 2001) or hearing a sound from the left side he or she would have to move it to the right to point to the ob-
of the world (Frassinetti, Pavani, & Làdavas, 2002). Something ject (Mattingley, Husain, Rorden, Kennard, & Driver, 1998).
similar is true for unimpaired people also. Suppose you are star- Again, the conclusion is that neglect is not due to a loss of
ing straight ahead and an experimenter is flashing stimuli on the sensation but a difficulty in directing attention to the left side.
left and right sides. Your task is to identify something about each Many patients with neglect also have deficits in spatial work-
stimulus, such as whether it was on the top or bottom half of ing memory (Malhotra et al., 2005) and in shifting attention,
the screen. If someone touches you just before a visual stimulus, even when location is irrelevant. For example, one patient could
you will respond slightly faster if the touch was on the same side not listen to two sounds and say which one came first, unless
of the body as the visual stimulus (Kennett, Eimer, Spence, & the sounds were very prolonged (Cusack, Carlyon, & Robertson,
Driver, 2001). That is, a touch stimulus briefly increases atten- 2000). In short, the problems associated with neglect extend to
tion to one side of the body or the other. many kinds of attention, not just the left–right dimension.
Learning 2013)
24. When a patient with neglect sees a large letter the right, attention to the left arm increases. (© Cengage
composed of small letters, he or she can identify the the left arm. However, if the left arm crosses over or under
large letter but then neglects part of it when asked Ordinarily, someone with right hemisphere damage neglects
to cross off all the small letters. Also, someone who Figure 14.24  ​A way to reduce sensory neglect
neglects the left hand pays attention to it when it
is crossed over the right hand. 25. Simply telling
the person to attend to something on the left helps
temporarily. Having the person look to the left while
feeling something on the left side increases attention
to the felt object. Crossing the left hand over the right
increases attention to the left hand. Moving a hand far
to the left makes it easier for the person to point to
something in the left visual field because the hand will
ANSWERS
move toward the right to point at the object.
the left side in a person with spatial neglect?
25. What are several procedures that increase attention to
attention, not just sensation?
24. What is the evidence that spatial neglect is a problem in
Stop & Check
453 14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention
454 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

Module 14.3  ■  In Closing

Attending to Attention and Being Conscious of Consciousness

Before the 1970s, many psychological researchers, especially report. Most researchers agree that we have made no progress
those studying learning in rats, were not convinced that the con- toward answering the hard problem of why consciousness ex-
cept of attention was useful at all. Today, the concept of atten- ists at all. Still, progress continues at an encouraging rate on
tion is well established in cognitive psychology, although some subordinate questions, such as finding the brain activities most
still doubt that consciousness is a scientifically useful concept. important for consciousness and attention. How far we can go
Research in this area is difficult because we cannot observe con- is uncertain, but the only way to find out is to try.
sciousness itself, and we have no access to it beyond what people

Summary
  1. Dualism—the belief in a nonmaterial mind that exists   8. We are not always conscious of events instantaneously
separately from the body and influences it—conflicts as they occur. Sometimes, a later event modifies our
with the conservation of matter and energy, one of the conscious perception of a stimulus that went before
best-established principles of physics. Nearly all it.  449
neuroscientists and philosophers accept some version of   9. Attention to a stimulus is almost synonymous with
monism, the idea that mental activity is inseparable being conscious of it. Because attention is limited,
from brain activity.  445 people often fail to notice changes in a scene that occur
  2. The hard problem is the question of why consciousness gradually or between one glance and another.  450
exists at all. Most researchers agree that we cannot 10. It is possible to direct attention toward one stimulus
answer this question, at least at present.  446 and away from another deliberately.  450
  3. Researchers sometimes use brain recordings to infer 11. Deliberate, top-down attention depends on activity in
whether someone is conscious. More confident infer- the prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex.  450
ences should become possible after we better under-
12. Attention and resistance to distraction vary across time
stand what aspects of brain functioning are necessary
and vary among individuals. Long-term players of
for consciousness.  446
action video games generally show above-average
  4. To identify the brain activities associated with con- performance on attention tasks. Prolonged meditation
sciousness, researchers present the same stimulus under training may also enhance attention.  451
conditions when an observer probably will or probably
13. Damage to parts of the right hemisphere produce
will not identify it consciously.  447
spatial neglect for the left side of the body or the left
  5. When someone is conscious of a stimulus, the represen- side of objects.  451
tation of that stimulus spreads over a large portion of
14. Neglect results from a deficit in attention, not sensation.
the brain.  447
For example, someone with neglect can see an entire
  6. Many stimuli influence our behavior without being letter enough to say what it is, even though that same
conscious. Even before a stimulus becomes conscious, person ignores the left half when asked to cross out all
the brain processes the information enough to identify the elements that compose it.  452
something as meaningful or meaningless.  449
15. People with sensory neglect also have difficulties with
  7. People almost never say they were partly conscious of working memory and with shifting attention from one
something. It may be that consciousness is a threshold stimulus to another, even when the stimuli do not vary
phenomenon: We become conscious of anything that from left to right.  452
exceeds a certain level of brain activity, and we are not
conscious of other events.  449
14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention 455

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
backward masking  447 identity position  445 mind–brain problem  445
binocular rivalry  447 inattentional blindness  450 monism  445
conscious  446 masking  447 phi phenomenon  449
dualism  445 materialism  445 spatial neglect  451
hard problem  446 mentalism  445 Stroop effect  450

Thought Questions
1. Could a computer be conscious? What evidence, if any, using this definition has determined certain brain
would convince you that it was conscious? correlates of consciousness. Could we now use those
2. The operational definition of consciousness applies brain correlates to infer consciousness or its absence in
only to people willing and able to report that they are newborn infants, brain-damaged people, or nonhu-
conscious of some events and not others. Research man animals?

chapter 14 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note-taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
456 Chapter 14 Cognitive Functions

Videos
Also available—

© 2012 ABC News. All rights reserved.

trademarks of American Broadcasting


The ABC News name and design are
• Corpus Callosum
• Situated Cognition

Companies, Inc.
Evaluating Patients
with Brain Damage

Animations
Also available—
© 2013 Cengage Learning

■ Hemisphere Lateralization
■ Learning Module: Wernicke-Geschwind Model
■ Capture of Attention by a Meaningful Stimulus

Seizure Pathways

try it
yourself
Try It Yourself Online online

Also available—
■ McGurk Effect
■ Phi Phenomenon
■ Binocular Rivalry
© 2013 Cengage Learning

■ Change Blindness

Split Brain
14.3 Conscious and Unconscious Processes and Attention 457

Look for these and additional animations and Try It Yourself Online activities, which are also avail-
able among the media resources for Chapter 14:

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Baars, B. J., & Gage, N. M. (Eds.). (2007). Cognition, brain, and consciousness. San Diego, CA:
Elsevier. Review of research on brain mechanisms of attention and consciousness.
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain. New York: Viking. Review of research on biological
bases of language, especially as they relate to reading.
Ornstein, R. (1997). The right mind. New York: Harcourt Brace. Very readable description of
split-brain research and the differences between the left and right hemispheres.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter, including ones on language training in bonobos, aphasia, and dyslexia.
(Reprinted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd.: Nature, “A functional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizophrenia,” Silbersweig et al., 1995.)
Mood Disorders
and Schizophrenia
15
Chapter Outline Main Ideas
Module 15.1 ​Mood Disorders 1. Psychological disorders result from a combination of
Major Depressive Disorder environmental and biological influences, including
Antidepressant Drugs genetics.
Bipolar Disorder 2. The effectiveness of certain drugs provides a clue as to
Seasonal Affective Disorder the underlying basis of depression and schizophrenia, but
In Closing: Biology of Mood Swings many questions remain about how these drugs exert their
effects.
Module 15.2 ​Schizophrenia
Diagnosis 3. Schizophrenia may be the result of genetic or other
Genetics problems that impair early development of the brain.
The Neurodevelopmental Hypothesis
Treatments
In Closing: Many Remaining Mysteries
Interactive Exploration and Study

A
re mental illnesses really illnesses, analogous to tuber-
culosis or influenza? Or are they normal reactions to
abnormal experiences? They are not exactly either.
They are outcomes that combine biological predispositions
with experiences. To control them, we need a good under-
standing of both aspects.
Abnormal behavior comes in many varieties. The Diagnos-
tic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994) lists hundreds of
disorders. This chapter deals with mood disorders—depres-
sion and bipolar disorder—and schizophrenia. These disor-
OPPOSITE: PET scans show the brain areas that increase their ders have been the focus of a huge amount of biological re-
activation during visual and auditory hallucinations by a patient
with schizophrenia. search. Chapter 12 discussed anxiety disorders and Chapter 3
had a section about addictions.
459
Module 15.1

Mood Disorders

D
ifferent people can get to the same place by different
routes. For example, the people in a room at any
moment may have started from different cities or
different parts of a city and traveled in different ways,
although they all reached the same destination. Similarly,
people can become depressed through different routes,
including genetics, traumatic experiences, hormonal
problems, substance abuse, head injuries, brain tumors, and
other illnesses. Despite having different causes, or
combinations of causes, these people all look and act
depressed (Figure 15.1). In this module, we explore some of
the many factors that contribute to depression.

Major Depressive Disorder


Many people say they feel “depressed” when they feel sad or
discouraged. Major depression is much more intense and pro-
longed. According to the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric As-
sociation, 1994), people with a major depression feel sad and
helpless every day for weeks at a time. They have little energy,
feel worthless, contemplate suicide, have trouble sleeping, can-
not concentrate, find little pleasure, and can hardly even imag-
Bruce Ayers/Getty Images

ine being happy again.


Absence of happiness is a more reliable symptom than
increased sadness. In one study, people carried a beeper that
sounded at unpredictable times to signal them to describe
their emotional reactions at the moment. People with de-
pression reported only an average number of unpleasant ex- Figure 15.1  ​The face of depression
periences but far below the average number of pleasant ones Depression shows in a person’s face, walk, voice, and mannerisms.
(Peters, Nicolson, Berkhof, Delespaul, & deVries, 2003). In
other studies, people examined photographs or films as re-
searchers recorded their reactions. Individuals with depres-
sion reacted normally to sad or frightening depictions but enough to warrant attention) within a given year, and more
seldom smiled at the comedies or pleasant pictures (Rot- than 10% do at some point in life (Narrow, Rae, Robins,
tenberg, Kasch, Gross, & Gotlib, 2002; Sloan, Strauss, & & Regier, 2002). Reported one-year prevalence rates vary
Wisner, 2001). Additional studies found that people with among countries and ethnic groups, from less than 5% among
depression show a decreased response to happy facial ex- Chinese-Canadians to more than 15% in India (Murali, 2001;
pressions (Monk et al., 2008) and a decreased response to a Tiwari & Wang, 2006). It is hard to know how seriously to
likely reward (McFarland & Klein, 2009). take these numbers. Standards for diagnosis inevitably vary
A survey reported that about 5% of adults in the United from place to place, and psychiatrists have no laboratory tests
States have a “clinically significant” depression (i.e., serious to confirm a diagnosis.

460
15.1 Mood Disorders 461

Childhood depression is about equally common for boys .50


and girls, but beyond about age 14, depression is more com-
s/s
mon in females (Twenge & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2002). Various .40

Probability of major
depression episode
hypotheses have been advanced to explain this tendency, but
none is well established. .30 s/l
Although some people suffer from long-term depres-
sion (Klein, 2010), it is more common to have episodes of .20 l/l
depression separated by periods of normal mood. The first
episode is special in certain regards. The first episode is gen-
.10
erally longer than most of the later ones, and most patients
can identify a highly stressful event that triggered the first
episode. For later episodes, people are less and less likely to .00
0 1 2 3 4+
identify a triggering event (Post, 1992). It is as if the brain Number of stressful life events
learns how to be depressed and gets better at it (Monroe
& Harkness, 2005). In that regard it is like epilepsy and Figure 15.2  ​Genetics, stress, and depression
migraine headaches: The more often you have had an epi- The effect of the serotonin transporter gene depends on the
sode, the easier it is to start another one (Post & Silberstein, amount of stress. (Reprinted by permission from A. Caspi, et al., “In-
1994). fluence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism
in the 5-HTT gene,” Science, 301, pp. 386–389. © 2003 AAAS.)

Genetics
Studies of twins and adopted children indicate a moderate de-
gree of heritability for depression (Shih, Belmonte, & Zandi, ity of depression. For those with two long forms, stressful
2004). However, although researchers have identified several events only slightly increased the risk of depression. Those
genes linked to depression, none of the genes by itself has a with one short and one long gene were intermediate. In
large effect (Camp et al., 2005; Holmans et al., 2007). other words, the short form of the gene by itself did not lead
One reason why no gene shows a strong link to depres- to depression, but it might magnify the reaction to stressful
sion is that when we talk about depression, we are probably events (Caspi et al., 2003).
lumping together at least two distinguishable syndromes. This report provoked a great deal of excitement. However,
People with early-onset depression (before age 30) have a since then most researchers have failed to replicate the result,
high probability of other relatives with depression (Bierut finding no significant relationship between depression and the
et al., 1999; Kendler, Gardner, & Prescott, 1999; Lyons et serotonin transporter gene itself and no interaction between
al., 1998), as well as relatives with anxiety disorders, atten- effects of the gene and stress (Munafo, Durrant, Lewis, &
tion-deficit disorder, alcohol or marijuana abuse, obsessive- Flint, 2009; Risch et al., 2009).
compulsive disorder, bulimia, migraine headaches, and ir- When a result in psychology, medicine, or any other field
ritable bowel syndrome (Q. Fu et al., 2002; Hudson et al., cannot be replicated, the obvious interpretation is that the
2003). People with late-onset depression (especially after first report was wrong. Given the huge number of research-
age 45 to 50) have a high probability of relatives with cir- ers collecting studies, occasionally a random fluctuation in
culatory problems (Kendler, Fiske, Gardner, & Gatz, 2009). data suggests a relationship between variables that are in
Distinguishing between early-onset and late-onset cases fact unrelated. However, here is another possibility: If a
may lead to progress in identifying genes, and perhaps in se- study finds no significant correlation between two variables,
lecting effective therapies. perhaps one or both of the variables was poorly measured.
Still, given the difficulty so far in identifying any gene (We can’t expect a poorly measured variable to correlate
strongly linked to depression, another hypothesis arose: with anything else.) Our measurements of depression are
Perhaps the effect of a gene varies with the environment. probably good enough, but the measurements of stress are
One gene controls the serotonin transporter, a protein that more doubtful. Generally researchers ask people how many
regulates the ability of axons to reabsorb serotonin after its stressful events they have experienced, and simply count
release, to recycle it for further use. Investigators examined them. Losing a job can be extremely stressful or hardly
the serotonin transporter genes of 847 young adults, iden- stressful at all, depending on how easily someone found an
tifying two types: the “short” type and the “long” type. They equal or better job. Similarly, divorce is much more stressful
also asked each participant to report certain stressful events to some people than others. Also, the biochemical methods
over five years, including financial setbacks, loss of job, di- used to measure short vs. long forms of the gene have been
vorce, and so forth. Figure 15.2 shows the results. For peo- inaccurate in many cases (Wray et al., 2009). We should
ple with two short forms of the gene, increasing numbers of await more research with more careful measurements be-
stressful experiences led to a big increase in the probabil- fore we draw a firm conclusion.
462 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Stop & Check 1985). Only 12 people tested positive for Borna disease virus,
but all 12 were suffering from major depression or bipolar dis-
1. What evidence suggests two types of depression influ- order. These 12 were a small percentage of the 265 depressed
enced by different genes? people tested; still, none of the 105 nondepressed people had
2. What did Caspi and colleagues report to be the relation- the virus.
ship between depression and the gene controlling the Since then, thousands of people have been tested in Eu-
serotonin transporter protein? rope, Asia, and North America. The Borna virus is found in
about 5% of normal people and about one-third of people
3. What should we conclude from the fact that most re- with severe depression or schizophrenia (Bode, Ferszt, &
searchers have not been able to replicate Caspi et al.’s Czech, 1993; Bode, Riegel, Lange, & Ludwig, 1992; Nunes
finding? et al., 2008; Terayama et al., 2003). The role of this virus in
been hampered by inaccurate measurement. psychiatric disorders remains uncertain, but the results so far
ANSWERS suggest that viruses might be a predisposing factor in some
really related to depression, or the studies so far have
not increased. 3. Either variation in this gene is not cases.
absence of stressful experiences, their probability is Hormones may be another trigger for depression. Stress
experiences by becoming depressed. However, in the is accepted as an important factor in depression, and stress in-
are more likely than other people to react to stressful creases release of cortisol, as discussed in Chapter 12. About
problems. 2. People with the short form of the gene 20% of women report some degree of postpartum depres-
depression have a high probability of circulatory sion—that is, depression after giving birth—and many re-
logical disorders. Relatives of people with late-onset
searchers suspect that hormonal fluctuations are a contribut-
ing factor. Stress hormones reach a peak late in pregnancy, and
have a high risk of depression and many other psycho-

ovarian hormones go through major changes around the time


1. Relatives of people with early-onset depression

of delivery. One study found that after a drug-induced drop


in estradiol and progesterone levels, women with a history of
postpartum depression suddenly show new symptoms of de-
Other Biological Influences pression, whereas other women do not (M. Bloch et al., 2000).
Genetic differences partly explain why some people are more Among older men, a declining level of the hormone testoster-
vulnerable to depression than others are, but other factors one is associated with increased probability of depression (Al-
contribute also. A few cases of depression are linked to viral meida, Yeap, Hankey, Jamrozik, & Flicker, 2008). However,
infections. Borna disease, a viral infection of farm animals, few studies have been done that directly link hormones to de-
produces periods of frantic activity alternating with periods pression, and the relationship remains uncertain (Brummelte
of inactivity (Figure 15.3). In 1985, investigators tested 370 & Galea, 2010). We do know that the risk of postpartum de-
people for possible exposure to this virus (Amsterdam et al., pression increases in women with previous bouts of depres-

Figure 15.3  ​Symptoms of Borna disease


Animals infected with Borna disease have periods of frantic activity alternating with inactivity, much like a person with bipolar disorder.
(left) Horse with Borna disease. (right) Same horse after recovery. (Figure 2, page 174, from Bode L. and Ludwig H., (1997). “Clinical simi-
larities and close genetic relationship of human and animal Borna disease virus.” Archives of Virology (Supplement 13), 167–182. Springer-
Verlag. Photo scan by Kevin J. Nolte.)
15.1 Mood Disorders 463

sion, stressful life events, and a lack of social support—that is, cohol. Disulfiram became the drug Antabuse, sometimes
the same factors linked to major depression at any other time prescribed for people who are trying to avoid alcohol.
of life (O’Hara, 2009). The use of bromides to control epilepsy was originally
based on a theory that was all wrong (Friedlander, 1986;
Levitt, 1975). Many people in the 1800s believed that
Abnormalities of Hemispheric Dominance masturbation caused epilepsy and that bromides reduced
Studies of normal people have found a fairly strong relation- sexual drive. Therefore, they reasoned, bromides should
ship between happy mood and increased activity in the left reduce epilepsy. It turns out that bromides do relieve epi-
prefrontal cortex ( Jacobs & Snyder, 1996). Most people with lepsy but for different reasons.
depression have decreased activity in the left and increased ac- Iproniazid, the first antidepressant drug, was originally
tivity in the right prefrontal cortex, and this imbalance is stable marketed to treat tuberculosis, until physicians noticed
over years despite changes in symptoms of depression (David- that it relieved depression. Similarly, chlorpromazine, the
son, 1984; Pizzagalli et al., 2002; Vuga et al., 2006). Here’s first antipsychotic drug, was originally used for other pur-
something you can try: Ask someone to solve a cognitive prob- poses, until physicians noticed its ability to alleviate
lem, such as, “See how many words you can think of that start schizophrenia. For decades, researchers sought new
with hu-” or “Try to remember all the ingredients you’ve ever drugs entirely by trial and error. Today, researchers evalu-
seen on a pizza.” Then unobtrusively watch the person’s eye ate new potential drugs in test tubes or tissue samples
movements to see whether they gaze right or until they find one with a potential for stronger or more
left. Most people gaze to the right during verbal specific effects on neurotransmission. The result is the
tasks, but most individuals with depression gaze use of fewer laboratory animals.  ■
to the left, suggesting right-hemisphere domi- try it 
yourself
nance (Lenhart & Katkin, 1986).
Types of Antidepressants
Antidepressant drugs fall into several categories, including
Stop & Check tricyclics, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, monoamine
4. Some people offer to train you to use the right hemi-
oxidase inhibitors, and atypical antidepressants (Figure 15.4).
sphere of your brain more strongly, allegedly to increase
The tricyclics (e.g., imipramine, trade name Tofranil) operate
creativity. If they were successful, can you see any disad-
by blocking the transporter proteins that reabsorb serotonin,
vantage?
dopamine, and norepinephrine into the presynaptic neuron
after their release. The result is to prolong the presence of the
increased tendency toward depression. neurotransmitters in the synaptic cleft, where they continue
ANSWER
ity and decreased left-hemisphere activity show an stimulating the postsynaptic cell. However, the tricyclics also
4. People with predominant right-hemisphere activ- block histamine receptors, acetylcholine receptors, and certain

Antidepressant Drugs
You might assume that investigators first determine the
causes of a psychological disorder and then develop medica-
tions based on the causes. The opposite order has been more
common: First investigators find a drug that seems helpful,
and then they try to figure out how it works. Like many other
psychiatric drugs, the early antidepressants were discovered
© Jonathan Nourok/Photo Edit

by accident.

Applications and Extensions

Accidental Discoveries of Psychiatric Drugs


Nearly all of the earliest psychiatric drugs were discov- Figure 15.4  ​Antidepressant pills
ered by accident. Disulfiram, for example, was originally Tricyclic drugs block the reuptake of catecholamines and se-
rotonin by presynaptic terminals. Selective serotonin reuptake
used in the manufacture of rubber. Someone noticed that
inhibitors, such as Prozac, have similar effects but are limited to
workers in a certain rubber factory avoided alcohol and
serotonin. MAOIs block an enzyme that breaks down catechol-
traced the cause to disulfiram, which had altered the amines and serotonin.
workers’ metabolism so they became ill after drinking al-
464 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

sodium channels (Horst & Preskorn, 1998). As


mentioned in Chapter 9, blocking histamine pro-
duces drowsiness. Blocking acetylcholine leads to
dry mouth and difficulty urinating. Blocking so-
dium channels causes heart irregularities, among Presynaptic
terminal
other problems. People have to limit their use of
tricyclic drugs to minimize these side effects.
The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors MAOIs block the enzyme
(SSRIs) are similar to tricyclics but specific to the MAO, prevent it from
MAO breaking transmitters
neurotransmitter serotonin. For example, fluox- into inactive metabolites
etine (trade name Prozac) blocks the reuptake Transmitter Metabolite
of serotonin. SSRIs produce milder side effects
than the tricyclics, but their effectiveness is about
the same. Other common SSRIs include sertra-
line (Zoloft), fluvoxamine (Luvox), citalopram
Tricyclic drugs and
(Celexa), and paroxetine (Paxil or Seroxat). Sev- Reuptake SSRIs block reuptake
eral newer drugs are serotonin norepinephrine Release
reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs), such as duloxetine
(Cymbalta) and venlafaxine (Effexor). As you Postsynaptic cell
might guess, they block reuptake of serotonin and
norepinephrine.
The monoamine oxidase inhibitors
(MAOIs) (e.g., phenelzine, trade name Nardil) Figure 15.5  ​Routes of action of antidepressants
block the enzyme monoamine oxidase (MAO), Tricyclics block the reuptake of dopamine, norepinephrine, and serotonin. SSRIs
a presynaptic terminal enzyme that metabolizes specifically block the reuptake of serotonin. SNRIs block reuptake of serotonin
catecholamines and serotonin into inactive forms. and norepinephrine. MAOIs block the enzyme MAO, which converts dopamine,
When MAOIs block this enzyme, the presynaptic norepinephrine, or serotonin into inactive chemicals. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
terminal has more of its transmitter available for
release. Generally, physicians prescribe tricyclics
or SSRIs first and then try MAOIs with people who did not taking St. John’s wort decreases the effectiveness of other
respond to the other drugs (Thase, Trivedi, & Rush, 1995). drugs you might be taking—including other antidepressant
People taking MAOIs must avoid foods containing tyra- drugs, cancer drugs, and AIDS drugs (He, Yang, Li, Du, &
mine—including cheese, raisins, and many others—because Zhou, 2010; Moore et al., 2000).
a combination of tyramine and MAOIs increases blood pres-
sure. Figure 15.5 summarizes the mechanisms of tricyclics,
SSRIs, and MAOIs. Stop & Check
The atypical antidepressants are a miscellaneous 5. What are the effects of tricyclic drugs?
group—everything other than the types just discussed (Horst
& Preskorn, 1998). One example is bupropion (Wellbutrin), 6. What are the effects of SSRIs?
which inhibits reuptake of dopamine and to some extent nor- 7. What are the effects of MAOIs?
epinephrine but not serotonin.
In addition, many people use St. John’s wort, an herb. availability of these transmitters.
ANSWERS
Because it is marketed as a nutritional supplement instead catecholamines and serotonin. The result is increased
of a drug, the U.S. Food and Drug Administration does not 7. MAOIs block the enzyme MAO, which breaks down
regulate it, and its purity varies from one bottle to another. 6. SSRIs selectively inhibit the reuptake of serotonin.
It has the advantage of being less expensive than antidepres- thereby producing unpleasant side effects.
sant drugs. An advantage or disadvantage, depending on your acetylcholine receptors, and certain sodium channels,
point of view, is that it is available without prescription. Peo-
catecholamines. They also block histamine receptors,

ple can get it easily but often take inappropriate amounts. Its
5. Tricyclic drugs block reuptake of serotonin and

effectiveness appears to be about the same as that of standard


antidepressant drugs (Kasper, Caraci, Forti, Drago, & Agug-
lia, 2010). However, it has a potentially dangerous side effect: How Do Antidepressants Work?
All mammals have a liver enzyme that breaks down plant tox- Understanding how antidepressants work should shed some
ins. St. John’s wort increases the effectiveness of that enzyme. light on the causes of depression. The commonly used anti-
Increasing the breakdown of toxins sounds like a good thing, depressants increase the presence of serotonin or other neu-
but the enzyme also breaks down most medicines. Therefore, rotransmitters at the synapse, and so it might seem that the
15.1 Mood Disorders 465

problem in depression is too little of the neurotransmitters. Apparently BDNF by itself does not automatically elevate
However, the story is not that simple. So far as we can tell mood, but it helps by facilitating new learning that builds new
from blood metabolites, people with depression have approxi- synapses and removes many old ones. That mode of action
mately normal levels of release of neurotransmitters. In fact, explains why antidepressants help people in depression—who
some studies show that people with depression have an in- might profit from substituting new thoughts for old ones—
crease in serotonin release (Barton et al., 2008). Furthermore, but fail to elevate mood for normal people (Castrén & Ran-
it is possible to decrease serotonin levels suddenly by consum- tamäki, 2010).
ing all the amino acids except tryptophan, the precursor to se- Although this story may seem convincing, the conclu-
rotonin. For most people, this decrease in serotonin does not sion remains tentative, as a few antidepressant drugs improve
provoke any feelings of depression (Neumeister et al., 2004, mood without demonstrable effects on BDNF (Basterzi et al.,
2006). 2008; Matrisciano et al., 2009). Perhaps antidepressants work
Furthermore, given that different drugs act in different in more than one way.
ways on different neurotransmitters, wouldn’t you expect
some of them to be more effective than others? So far as we
can tell, all of them are about equal in effectiveness (Montgom- Stop & Check
ery et al., 2007). Two studies examined patients who failed 8. In what way does the time course of antidepressants con-
to respond to an antidepressant drug within a few weeks. In flict with the idea that they improve mood by increasing
one study the psychiatrists added a second drug, and in the neurotransmitter levels?
other study they switched patients from one drug to the other.
The result was that some patients who did not respond to the 9. As opposed to an interpretation in terms of neurotrans-
first drug did respond after a few weeks on the new regimen mitter levels, what is an alternative explanation for the
(Rush et al., 2006; Trivedi et al., 2006). Should we conclude, benefits of antidepressant drugs?
as the researchers did, that adding a drug or switching drugs synapses, and new learning in the hippocampus.
helped? No. Unfortunately, neither study included a control ANSWERS
which gradually promotes growth of new neurons, new
group that stayed on the first drug for the additional time. In 9. Antidepressant drugs increase production of BDNF,
short, we have no clear evidence that any antidepressant drug ioral benefits develop gradually over 2 to 3 weeks.
produces any different effects from any other. and other neurotransmitters quickly, but their behav-
The time course of effects poses an additional threat to 8. Antidepressants produce their effects on serotonin
any explanation in terms of neurotransmitters: Antidepres-
sant drugs produce their effects on neurotransmitters in the
synapses within minutes to hours, depending on the drug, but
people need to take the drugs for 2 or more weeks before they
experience any mood elevation (Stewart et al., 1998). This de- How Effective Are Antidepressants?
lay of benefits strongly suggests that increasing the levels of So far we have considered explanations of how antidepres-
neurotransmitters at synapses does not explain the benefits of sants work. How sure are we that they do work? Not everyone
the drugs. Perhaps the neurotransmitter effects are not even is convinced (Kirsch, 2010), and at least we have to say that
relevant. the effectiveness is limited.
Today, much research attention focuses on neurotroph- In most cases, depression occurs in episodes. That is, even
ins. As discussed in Chapter 5, neurotrophins aid in the sur- without treatment, many people recover within a few months.
vival, growth, and connections of neurons. Most people with Furthermore, giving someone a medication produces an ex-
depression have lower than average levels of a neurotrophin pectation of improvement, thereby enhancing the probability
called brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) that is im- of recovery, even if the medication itself is ineffective. To test
portant for synaptic plasticity, learning, and proliferation of the effectiveness of an antidepressant drug, researchers need
new neurons in the hippocampus (Martinowich, Manji, & to compare its effects to those of a placebo (a pharmacologi-
Lu, 2007; Sen, Duman, & Sanacora, 2008). As a result of low cally inactive substance).
BDNF, most people with depression have a smaller than aver- Figure 15.6 summarizes the results of many experi-
age hippocampus, impaired learning, and reduced production ments in which people were randomly assigned to receive
of new hippocampal neurons. Prolonged use of antidepres- antidepressant drugs or placebos. The horizontal axis rep-
sant drugs generally increases BDNF production and im- resents the mean amount of improvement on the Hamilton
proves learning and formation of new neurons. This process Depression Rating Scale. The pink triangles represent patients
takes weeks (Drzyzga, Marcinowska, & Obuchowicz, 2009; receiving the drug in a study, and the gray circles represent
Vetencourt et al., 2008). That is, the time course for BDNF patients receiving a placebo. The size of the triangle or circle
and changes in the hippocampus matches the time course for is proportional to the number of patients in a group. Many
behavioral recovery. Procedures that block neuron production people respond well on placebos, either because of sponta-
also block the behavioral benefits of antidepressant drugs (Ai- neous recovery over time or because of the expectation
ran et al., 2007). that a pill induced. Younger patients are particularly likely
466 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

to respond to placebos (Bridge, Birmaher, Iyengar, Barbe, term benefits, reducing the likelihood of a relapse months
& Brent, 2009). For patients with mild to moderate depres- or years after the end of treatment (Bortolotti, Menchetti,
sion, the results for placebo groups overlap those for drug Bellini, Montaguti, & Berardi, 2008; Imel, Malterer, McKay
groups, and the differences between the groups are, on aver- & Wampold, 2008).
age, too small to be of much clinical significance. Only for Would a combination of antidepressant drugs and psy-
people with severe depression do the drugs show a mean- chotherapy work better than either one alone? On average,
ingful advantage (Kirsch et al., 2008). Another independent people who improve while receiving both treatments improve
analysis of the research confirmed that the drugs show no more than people receiving either one alone. However, the
clear benefit over placebos for people with mild to moderate percentage of people showing improvement increases only
depression (Fournier et al., 2010). Furthermore, even at the slightly with combined treatment (de Maat et al., 2008; Hol-
most severe levels of depression, antidepressants help some lon et al., 2005). That is, it is not the case that many people
people and not others (Uhr et al., 2008). respond better to one treatment than the other. Evidently,
An alternative to antidepressant drugs is psychotherapy. many people with mild to moderate depression improve with
Reviews of the research literature find that antidepressant only a placebo, another group improves with either antide-
drugs and psychotherapy are about equally effective for pressants or psychotherapy, a few respond better to one or the
treating all levels of depression, from mild to severe, with other, and the remainder—one-third to one-half, by most es-
three exceptions: First, the drugs work better for dysthymia, timates—do not respond well to either one (Friedman et al.,
a long-term, almost life-long condition of unhappy mood. 2009; Hollon, Thase, & Markowitz, 2002; Thase et al., 1997).
Nearly all of the research studies examined short-term The effects of antidepressants and those of psychotherapy
therapies, and it may be that brief psychotherapy is ineffec- overlap more than we might have guessed. Brain scans show
tive for such a long-term condition. Second, antidepressants that antidepressants and psychotherapy increase metabolism
are generally ineffective for patients who had suffered abuse in the same brain areas (Brody et al., 2001; S. D. Martin et
or neglect during early childhood or patients with multiple al., 2001). That similarity should not be terribly surprising if
psychological disorders. Those patients usually respond we accept the mind–body monism position. If mental activ-
better to psychotherapy (Asarnow et al., 2009; Nemeroff et ity is the same thing as brain activity, then changing some-
al., 2003). Third, psychotherapy is more likely to have long- one’s thoughts should indeed change brain chemistry.

Drug Clinically
significant
Placebo difference

2.0

1.5
Improvement (d)

1.0

0.5

0.0
16 20 24 28 32
Initial severity

Figure 15.6  ​Mean improvement from depression by people taking antidepressants or placebos
Pink triangles represent people taking medications in a particular study. Gray circles represent people taking placebos. The size of the
triangle or circle is proportional to the number of people in the study. (From Kirsch, 2008)
15.1 Mood Disorders 467

Stop & Check


10. As depression becomes more severe, what happens to
the percentage of patients showing improvement while
taking antidepressant drugs or placebos?

11. What is an advantage of psychotherapy over antidepres-


sant drugs?

neglect in childhood.
ANSWERS
multiple disorders or people who suffered abuse or
effects and appear to be less effective for people with
Also, antidepressant drugs produce unpleasant side
than people who respond to antidepressant drugs.
well to psychotherapy have a lower risk of later relapse
placebos show improvement. 11. People who respond
taking antidepressant drugs, but fewer patients taking
who improve remains about the same for patients
10. For more severe cases, the percentage of patients

Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT)


Many people with depression do not respond well to either
drugs or psychotherapy. What options are available for them?
One possibility, despite its stormy history, is treatment through
an electrically induced seizure, known as electroconvulsive
Najlah Feanny/Stock, Boston, Inc.

therapy (ECT). ECT originated with the observation that for


people with both epilepsy and schizophrenia, as symptoms of
one disorder increase, symptoms of the other often decrease
(Trimble & Thompson, 1986). In the 1930s, Ladislas Meduna
tried to relieve schizophrenia by inducing convulsions. Soon,
other physicians were doing the same, inducing seizures with
a large dose of insulin. Insulin shock is a dreadful experience, Figure 15.7  ​Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT)
In contrast to an earlier era, ECT today is administered with
however, and difficult to control. An Italian physician, Ugo Cer-
muscle relaxants or anesthetics to minimize discomfort and only
letti, after years of experimentation with animals, developed a
if the patient gives informed consent.
method of inducing seizures with an electric shock through the
head (Cerletti & Bini, 1938). Electroconvulsive therapy is quick,
and most patients awaken calmly without remembering it.
When ECT proved to be not very effective with schizo-
phrenia, you might guess that psychiatrists would abandon it. memory loss, the other serious drawback to ECT is the high
Instead, they tried it for other mental hospital patients, de- risk of relapsing into another episode of depression within a
spite having no theoretical basis. ECT did indeed relieve de- few months (Riddle & Scott, 1995). After ECT has relieved
pression in many cases. However, its misuse during the 1950s depression, the usual strategy is to try to prevent a relapse by
earned it a bad reputation, as some patients were given ECT means of drugs, psychotherapy, or periodic ECT treatments
hundreds of times without their consent. (Swoboda, Conca, König, Waanders, & Hansen, 2001).
When antidepressant drugs became available in the late More than half a century after the introduction of ECT,
1950s, the use of ECT declined abruptly. However, it made a no one is yet sure how it relieves depression, but like antide-
partial comeback in the 1970s. ECT today is used only with pressant drugs, ECT increases the proliferation of new neu-
informed consent, usually for patients with severe depression rons in the hippocampus (Perera et al., 2007). It also alters
who have not responded to antidepressant drugs (Reisner, the expression of at least 120 genes in the hippocampus and
2003). It is usually applied every other day for about 2 weeks. frontal cortex alone (Altar et al., 2004).
Patients are given muscle relaxants or anesthetics to minimize A similar treatment is repetitive transcranial magnetic
discomfort and the possibility of injury (Figure 15.7). stimulation. An intense magnetic field is applied to the scalp,
The most common side effect of ECT is memory loss, but stimulating the axons near the surface of the brain. This pro-
limiting the shock to the right hemisphere reduces the memory cedure is moderately effective against depression, although
loss. In any case, the memory impairment lasts no more than a its mechanism of behavioral effect is not known (Ridding &
few months, not forever (Reisner, 2003). Besides the threat of Rothwell, 2007).
468 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Altered Sleep Patterns the very early morning, but by that time he or she would have
Almost everyone with depression has sleep problems, and received seven or eight hours of sleep. This procedure relieves
the sleep problems generally precede the mood changes. One depression for at least a week in most patients and often lon-
study identified teenagers who reported almost daily prob- ger (Riemann et al., 1999).
lems in falling asleep or staying asleep. Within the next 6 to 7 Researchers cannot yet explain how sleep deprivation or
years, more than half of these young people developed depres- rescheduling produces mood benefits. A better understanding
sion (Roane & Taylor, 2008). might lead to other treatments for depression.
The usual sleep pattern for a depressed person resembles
the sleep of healthy people who travel a couple of time zones Stop & Check
west and have to go to bed later than usual: They fall asleep
12. How can one decrease the memory loss associated with
but awaken early, unable to get back to sleep, and they enter
ECT?
REM sleep within 45 minutes after going to sleep, as Figure
15.8 illustrates. In addition, people who are depressed have 13. What change in sleep habits sometimes relieves
more than the average number of eye movements per minute depression?
during REM sleep. Many of their relatives show these same
sleep patterns, and the relatives who show these patterns are ANSWERS
go to bed earlier sometimes relieves depression.
more likely to become depressed themselves than are relatives
memory loss. 13. Getting people with depression to

who sleep normally (Modell, Ising, Holsboer, & Lauer, 2005).


12. ECT over just the right hemisphere produces less

In short, altered sleep is a lifelong trait of people who are pre-


disposed to depression.
Surprisingly, although most people feel worse after a sleep-
less night, a night of total sleep deprivation is the quickest Other Therapies
known method of relieving depression (Ringel & Szuba, 2001). Each of the currently available treatments for depression has
However, the benefit is brief, as the depression usually returns its pros and cons, and some people with depression do not re-
after the next night’s sleep. Also, while sleep deprivation helps al- spond well to any of them. The search continues for new and
leviate depression, it increases sensitivity to pain (Kundermann, improved treatments.
Hemmeter-Spernal, Huber, Krieg, & Lautenbacher, 2008). One promising possibility is a program of regular, non-
A more practical solution is to alter the sleep schedule, go- strenuous exercise, such as brisk walking for half an hour or
ing to bed earlier than usual. The person might still awaken in more per day (Leppämäki, Partonen, & Lönnqvist, 2002).

37.5° Normal person


Body temperature

37°C Depressed person

36.5°

11 A.M. 3 P.M. 7 P.M. 11 P.M. 3 A.M. 7 A.M.

Normal people Depressed people Normal people


going to sleep going to sleep OR going to sleep
at normal time at normal time late

REM REM
sleep sleep
Stage 1 Stage 1

Stage 4 Stage 4

Figure 15.8  ​Circadian rhythms and depression


Most people with depression have their circadian rhythms advanced by several hours. They sleep as if they had gone to bed later than
they actually did. (Bottom graphs from Sleep by J. Allan Hobson, ©1989, 1995 by J. Allan Hobson. Reprinted by permission of Henry Holt
and Company, LLC.)
15.1 Mood Disorders 469

Many studies have shown that active people are less likely
than sedentary people to become depressed. However, most
of these studies are correlational in nature and do not sup-
port a cause-and-effect conclusion. A few controlled experi-
ments have yielded inconclusive results (Teychenne, Ball, &
Salmon, 2010). Still, exercise increases blood flow to the brain
and provides other benefits, without the costs or risks of other
treatments (Hillman, Erickson, & Kramer, 2008; Hunsberger
et al., 2007). More research is necessary, but in the meantime,
exercise is a good recommendation.

Bipolar Disorder
Depression can be either unipolar or bipolar. People with
unipolar disorder vary between normality and one pole—
depression. People with bipolar disorder, formerly known as
manic-depressive disorder, alternate between two poles—de-
pression and its opposite, mania. Mania is characterized by
restless activity, excitement, laughter, self-confidence, ram-
bling speech, and loss of inhibitions. People with mania be- Figure 15.9  ​PET scans for a patient with bipolar disorder
Horizontal planes through three levels of the brain are shown
come dangerous to themselves and others. Figure 15.9 shows
for each day. On May 17 and May 27, when the patient was
the brain’s increase in glucose use during mania and its de-
depressed, brain metabolic rates were low. On May 18, when the
crease during depression (Baxter et al., 1985). patient was in a cheerful, hypomanic mood, the brain metabolic
People who have full-blown episodes of mania are said to rate was high. Red indicates the highest metabolic rate, followed
have bipolar I disorder. People with bipolar II disorder have by yellow, green, and blue. (Reprinted by permission from Macmil-
milder manic phases, called hypomania, characterized by agi- lan Publishers Ltd: Nature, A functional neuroanatomy of hallucina-
tation or anxiety. In addition to the mood swings, most people tions in schizophrenia, Silbersweig et al., 1995.)
with bipolar disorder have attention deficits, poor impulse
control, and impairments of verbal memory (Quraishi & Fran-
gou, 2002). Diagnoses of bipolar disorder have been increasing
since the 1990s, especially among teenagers and young adults
(Moreno et al., 2007). It is now estimated that about 1% of peo- patients. It was indeed helpful, although investigators eventu-
ple will have bipolar I disorder at some time in life, another 1% ally realized that lithium was the effective agent, not uric acid.
will have bipolar II disorder, and 2% to 3% will have “subthresh- Lithium stabilizes mood, preventing a relapse into either
old” bipolar disorder—a minor case not quite strong enough for mania or depression. The dose must be regulated carefully, as
a diagnosis of bipolar disorder (Merikangas et al., 2007). a low dose is ineffective and a high dose is toxic (Schou, 1997).
Two other effective drugs are valproate (trade names Depak-
ene, Depakote, and others) and carbamazepine. If these drugs
Genetics are not fully effective, physicians sometimes supplement them
A genetic predisposition for bipolar disorder is supported by with antidepressant drugs or antipsychotic drugs—the ones
the usual types of evidence—twin studies and adoption stud- also prescribed for schizophrenia. Antidepressant drugs are
ies. In addition, researchers have located two genes that ap- risky, as they sometimes provoke a switch from depression to
pear to increase the probability of bipolar II disorder (Nwulia mania. Antipsychotic drugs can be helpful, but they also pro-
et al., 2007). They have also demonstrated that some of the duce unpleasant side effects.
same genes that predispose to major depression also predis- Lithium, valproate, and carbamazepine have many effects
pose to bipolar disorder (Liu et al., 2011). However, the genes on the brain. A good research strategy is to assume that they
merely increase the risk. None of the genes shows a strong relieve bipolar disorder because of some effect they have in
relationship to the disorder. common. One effect they share is that they decrease the num-
ber of AMPA type glutamate receptors in the hippocampus
(Du et al., 2008). Excessive glutamate activity is responsible
Treatments for some aspects of mania. Also, the drugs that are effective
The first successful treatment for bipolar disorder, and still against bipolar disorder block the synthesis of a brain chemi-
the most common one, is lithium salts. Lithium’s benefits cal called arachidonic acid, which is produced during brain in-
were discovered accidentally by an Australian investigator, J. flammation (S. I. Rapoport & Bosetti, 2002). Bipolar patients
F. Cade, who believed uric acid might relieve mania and de- show an increased expression of genes associated with inflam-
pression. Cade mixed uric acid (a component of urine) with mation (Padmos et al., 2008). The effects of arachidonic acid
a lithium salt to help it dissolve and then gave the solution to are also counteracted by omega-3 fatty acids, such as those in
470 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Most depressed people


Normal

Body temperature
37.5°
People with seasonal
affective disorder
37°C

36.5°

11 A.M. 3 P.M. 7 P.M. 11 P.M. 3 A.M. 7 A.M.

Figure 15.10  ​Circadian rhythms for major depression and seasonal affective disorder (SAD)
Patients with SAD are phase-delayed whereas most other patients with depression are phase-advanced. (© Cengage Learning 2013)

seafood, and epidemiological studies suggest that people who SAD differs from other types of depression in many ways.
eat at least a pound (0.45 kg) of seafood per week have a de- For example, patients with SAD have phase-delayed sleep and
creased risk of bipolar disorder (Noaghiul & Hibbeln, 2003). temperature rhythms—becoming sleepy and wakeful later
Another possible treatment relates to sleep. Patients with than normal—unlike most other patients with depression,
bipolar disorder during the depressed phase tend to stay in whose rhythms are phase-advanced (Teicher et al., 1997)
bed for many hours. During the manic phase, they awaken (Figure 15.10). Also, SAD is seldom as severe as major de-
early, reach their activity peak earlier in the day than most pression. Many people with SAD have a mutation in one of
people, and get relatively little sleep (Salvatore et al., 2008). the genes responsible for regulating the circadian rhythm, as
Preliminary studies suggest that getting people to maintain a discussed in Chapter 9 ( Johansson et al., 2003).
consistent sleeping schedule in a dark, quiet room reduces the It is possible to treat SAD with very bright lights (e.g.,
intensity of mood swings (Wehr et al., 1998). 2,500 lux) for an hour or more each day. The bright light treat-
ment is effective in the morning, afternoon, or evening (East-
man, Young, Fogg, Liu, & Meaden, 1998; Lewy et al., 1998;
Stop & Check Terman, Terman, & Ross, 1998). Although its benefits are as
14. What are two common treatments for bipolar disorder? yet unexplained, they are substantial. Bright light is less ex-
pensive than the other antidepressant therapies and produces
ANSWER
ate and carbamazepine. its benefits more rapidly, often within 1 week (Kripke, 1998).
lithium salts and certain anticonvulsant drugs—valpro-
14. The common treatments for bipolar disorder are

Stop & Check


15. What are the advantages of bright light treatment com-
Seasonal Affective Disorder pared to antidepressant drugs?
One more form of depression is seasonal affective disorder
(SAD)—depression that recurs during a particular season,
benefits more quickly.
ANSWER
15. It is cheaper, has no side effects, and produces its
such as winter. SAD is most prevalent near the poles, where
the winter nights are long (Haggarty et al., 2002).

Module 15.1  ■  In Closing

Biology of Mood Swings

There is nothing abnormal about feeling sad or happy if some- come depressed, the mood persists for months, and even the
thing unusually bad or good has just happened to you. For best of news provides little cheer. A bipolar patient in a manic
people with major depression or bipolar disorder, mood be- state has boundless energy and self-confidence that no contra-
comes largely independent of events. A traumatic experience diction can deter. Studying these states has great potential to
might trigger a bout of depression, but once someone has be- inform us about the brain states that correspond to moods.
15.1 Mood Disorders 471

Summary
  1. People with major depression find that almost nothing new neurons, synapses, and learning in the hippocam-
makes them happy. In most cases, depression occurs as a pus. Most antidepressant drugs produce a gradual
series of episodes.  460 increase in BDNF, and therefore enhance synaptic
  2. Depression has a genetic predisposition, but no one plasticity in the hippocampus. The effects on BDNF
gene has a strong effect by itself.  461 may be the main reason for the drugs’ benefits.  465
  3. Uncommonly, depression can be a reaction to a virus, or   8. Antidepressant drugs are ineffective for many people.
possibly to hormonal changes.  462 For depressed patients with mild to moderate depres-
sion, antidepressants are not significantly more effective
  4. Depression is associated with decreased activity in the
than placebos.  465
left hemisphere of the cortex.  463
  9. Psychotherapy is about as effective as antidepressant
  5. Several kinds of antidepressant drugs are in wide use.
drugs for patients with all levels of severity. Psychother-
Tricyclics block reuptake of serotonin and catechol-
apy is more likely than antipsychotic drugs to produce
amines. SSRIs block reuptake of serotonin. SNRIs
long-lasting benefits that prevent or delay a relapse after
block reuptake of both serotonin and norepinephrine.
the end of treatment.  466
MAOIs block an enzyme that breaks down catechol-
amines and serotonin. Atypical antidepressants are a 10. Other therapies for depression include electroconvulsive
miscellaneous group with diverse effects.  463 therapy, altered sleep patterns, and exercise.  467
  6. Antidepressants alter synaptic activity quickly, but their 11. People with bipolar disorder alternate between depres-
effects on behavior require at least 2 weeks. Although sion and mania. Effective therapies include lithium salts
different drugs affect different neurotransmitters, they and certain other drugs. A consistent sleep schedule is
all appear to be about equally effective. It is possible that also recommended.  469
their well-known effects on neurotransmitters are not 12. Seasonal affective disorder is marked by recurrent
the main reason for their effects on behavior.  464 depression during one season of the year. Exposure to
  7. Most people with depression have a deficiency of the bright lights is usually effective in treating it.  470
neurotrophin BDNF, which promotes development of

key terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
atypical antidepressants  464 major depression  460 selective serotonin reuptake
bipolar disorder  469 mania  469 inhibitors (SSRIs)  464
bipolar I disorder  469 monoamine oxidase inhibitors serotonin norepinephrine
bipolar II disorder  469 (MAOIs)  464 reuptake inhibitors
electroconvulsive therapy postpartum depression  462 (SNRIs)  464
(ECT)  467 seasonal affective disorder (SAD)  470 tricyclics  463
lithium  469 unipolar depression  469

Thought Questions
1. Some people have suggested that ECT relieves 2. Certain people suffer from what they describe as
depression by causing people to forget the events “post-Christmas depression,” a feeling of letdown after
that caused it. What evidence opposes this all the excitement of the holiday season. What other
hypothesis? explanation can you offer?
Module 15.2

Schizophrenia

H
ere is a conversation between two people diagnosed emotional and intellectual aspects of experience: The person’s
with schizophrenia (Haley, 1959, p. 321): emotional expression or lack of it seems unconnected with
current experiences. For example, someone might giggle or cry
for no apparent reason or show no reaction to bad news. Not
: Do you work at the air base?
A
all patients show this detachment of emotion from intellect,
B: You know what I think of work. I’m 33 in June, do you but the term lives on.
mind? Diagnosis of schizophrenia is difficult. In most areas of
A: June? medicine, a physician can confirm a diagnosis with a lab test
B: 33 years old in June. This stuff goes out the window of some sort. Psychiatry has no dependable lab tests. Psychia-
after I live this, uh—leave this hospital. So I can’t get trists rely on behavioral observations, and many cases leave
my vocal cords back. So I lay off cigarettes. I’m in a room for uncertainty.
spatial condition, from outer space myself. . . . According to the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Asso-
ciation, 1994), to be diagnosed with schizophrenia, someone
A: I’m a real spaceship from across.
must have deteriorated in everyday functioning (work, inter-
B: A lot of people talk that way, like crazy, but “Believe It personal relations, self care, etc.) for at least 6 months, and
or Not,” by Ripley, take it or leave it—alone—it’s in must show at least two of the following, that are not attribut-
the Examiner, it’s in the comic section, “Believe It or able to other disorders:
Not,” by Ripley, Robert E. Ripley, believe it or not, but
we don’t have to believe anything, unless I feel like it. ■ Delusions (unjustifiable beliefs, such as “Beings from
Every little rosette—too much alone. outer space are controlling my actions”)
A: Yeah, it could be possible. ■ Hallucinations (false sensory experiences, such as

B: I’m a civilian seaman. hearing voices when alone)


■ Disorganized speech (rambling or incoherent)
A: Could be possible. I take my bath in the ocean.
■ Grossly disorganized behavior
B: Bathing stinks. You know why? ‘Cause you can’t quit
when you feel like it. You’re in the service. ■ Weak or absent signs of emotion, speech, and

socialization
People with schizophrenia say and do things that other
people (including other people with schizophrenia) find dif- Each of these is a judgment call. Sometimes a statement
ficult to understand. The causes of the disorder are not well that appears to be a delusion (“People are persecuting me”)
understood, but they include a large biological component. is actually true, or at least defensible. Many healthy people
have heard a voice when they knew they were alone, at least
once or twice. The term “grossly disorganized behavior” en-
compasses a wide variety of possibilities. The symptoms vary
Diagnosis so greatly that you could easily find several people diagnosed
Schizophrenia was originally called dementia praecox, which with schizophrenia who have almost nothing in common
is Latin for “premature mental deterioration.” In 1911, Eugen (Andreasen, 1999).
Bleuler introduced the term schizophrenia. Although the term The first four items on the list—delusions, hallucinations,
is Greek for “split mind,” it is not related to dissociative identity disorganized speech, and disorganized behavior—are called
disorder (previously known as multiple personality disorder), positive symptoms (behaviors that are present that should
in which someone alternates among different personalities. be absent). Weak or absent emotion, speech, and socialization
What Bleuler meant by schizophrenia was a split between the are negative symptoms (behaviors that are absent that should

472
15.2 Schizophrenia 473

Nancy C. Andreasen Stop & Check


Being a scientist and a clinician is a
16. Why are hallucinations considered a positive symptom?
double privilege. We actually get paid to
spend our time asking both scientific A “positive” symptom is not a “good” symptom.
ANSWER
and clinical questions that everyone because they are present when they should be absent.
would like to ask and have answered,
Nancy Andreason

16. Hallucinations are considered a positive symptom


and people grant us the trust of sharing
their most intimate thoughts and expe-
riences with us.

be present). Negative symptoms are usually stable over time Applications and Extensions
and difficult to treat.
It is also useful to distinguish cognitive symptoms. The Differential Diagnosis of Schizophrenia
cognitive symptoms are limitations of thought and reasoning In the rules for diagnosing schizophrenia, did you notice
that are common in schizophrenia, even if they are not central the expression “not attributable to other disorders”?
to the diagnosis. Overall intelligence varies considerably, but Even if someone’s symptoms match the description of
on average, IQ scores are a few points below those of the rest schizophrenia perfectly, it is important to make a differ-
of the population (Woodberry, Giuliano, & Seidman, 2008). ential diagnosis—that is, one that rules out other condi-
The most typical type of thought disorder of schizophrenia tions with similar symptoms. Here are a few conditions
is a difficulty understanding and using abstract concepts. that sometimes resemble schizophrenia:
Related symptoms include deficits in attention and working
memory (Hanlon et al., 2005). Mood disorder with psychotic features: People with de-

Which of the various symptoms, if any, is the primary prob- pression frequently have delusions, especially delusions
lem? According to Nancy Andreasen (1999), a leading investiga- of guilt or failure. Some report hallucinations also.
tor of schizophrenia, the main problem is disordered thoughts Substance abuse: Many of the positive symptoms

that result from abnormal interactions between the cortex and of schizophrenia can develop from prolonged use of
the thalamus and cerebellum. The disordered thinking may lead amphetamine, methamphetamine, cocaine, LSD, or
to the hallucinations, delusions, and other symptoms. phencyclidine (“angel dust”). Someone who stops
One way to test this idea is to see whether we could make taking the drugs is likely, though not certain, to
normal, healthy people talk or behave in incoherent ways if we recover from these symptoms. Substance abuse is
overtaxed their working memory. Imagine yourself in the fol- more likely than schizophrenia to produce visual hal-
lowing study. The researcher shows a series of pictures for 30 lucinations.
seconds each, and you are supposed to tell a short story about Brain damage: Damage or tumors in the temporal or

each one. If you see the same picture a second time, you should prefrontal cortex often produce some of the symp-
tell a totally new story about it, unlike your first one. Further- toms of schizophrenia.
more, on some trials, you have an additional task to burden your
memory while you are trying to tell a story: A series of letters Undetected hearing deficits: Sometimes, someone

who is starting to have trouble hearing thinks that


appears on the screen, one at a time. You should pay attention
everyone else is whispering and starts to worry,
to every second letter. Whenever it is the same as the last letter
“They’re whispering about me!” Delusions of persecu-
that you paid attention to, you should press a key. For example,
tion can develop.
D L K F R F B L M T J T X H Q U B R B N Huntington’s disease: The symptoms of Hunting-

ton’s disease include hallucinations, delusions, and


disordered thinking, as well as motor symptoms. An
Attend to every Press on these, because Do not press here. uncommon type of schizophrenia, catatonic schizo-
second letter. same as previous Same as previous phrenia, includes motor abnormalities, so a mixture
attended letter. nonattended letter. of psychological and motor symptoms could represent
Most people’s speech becomes less clear when they per- either schizophrenia or Huntington’s disease.
form this memory task while trying to tell a story. If it is the Nutritional abnormalities: Niacin deficiency can

second presentation of a picture, requiring them to avoid what produce hallucinations and delusions (Hoffer, 1973),
they said the first time and tell a totally new story, the mem- and so can a deficiency of vitamin C or an allergy to
ory task causes even greater interference, and their speech milk proteins (not the same as lactose intolerance).
becomes incoherent, somewhat like schizophrenic speech Some people who cannot tolerate wheat gluten or
(Kerns, 2007). The implication is that memory impairment other proteins react with hallucinations and delusions
could be the central symptom. (Reichelt, Seim, & Reichelt, 1996).  ■
474 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Demographic Data chemical themselves (Brewer et al., 2007; K. Smith,


Worldwide, about 1% of people suffer from schizophrenia at Thompson, & Koster, 1969).
some point in life (Narrow et al., 2002; Perälä et al., 2007). ■ Most people with schizophrenia and many of their

The estimate rises or falls depending on how many mild cases unaffected relatives have deficits in pursuit eye move-
we include. Since the mid-1900s, the reported prevalence ments—the ability to keep their eyes on a moving
of schizophrenia has been declining in many countries (Su- target (Keefe et al., 1997; Sereno & Holzman, 1993).
visaari, Haukka, Tanskanen, & Lönnqvist, 1999; Torrey &
Miller, 2001). Is schizophrenia actually less common, or are
psychiatrists just diagnosing it differently? This is not an easy Stop & Check
question to answer. However, even when it is diagnosed today,
it appears to be less severe than it often used to be. Perhaps 17. Has the reported prevalence of schizophrenia been in-
our society is doing something to prevent schizophrenia with- creasing, decreasing, or staying the same?
out knowing what. prevalence.
Schizophrenia occurs in all ethnic groups and all parts of ANSWER
17. Schizophrenia has been decreasing in reported
the world. However, it is significantly more common in cities
than in rural areas, for reasons unknown (Kelly et al., 2010).
Also it is 10 to 100 times more common in the United States
and Europe than in most Third World countries (Torrey,
1986). Part of that discrepancy could be due to differences Genetics
in recordkeeping, but other possibilities exist, including social
support and diet. A diet high in sugar and saturated fat, as is Huntington’s disease (Chapter 8) can be called a genetic dis-
common in prosperous countries, aggravates schizophrenia, ease: By examining part of chromosome 4, one can predict
whereas a diet rich in fish alleviates it (Peet, 2004). Omega-3 with almost perfect accuracy who will develop the disease
fatty acids, abundant in seafood, increase production of and who will not. At one time, many researchers believed that
BDNF, increase production of new cells in the hippocampus, schizophrenia might be a genetic disease in the same sense.
and block apoptosis and other neural damage (V. R. King et However, accumulating evidence indicates that although
al., 2006; Venna, 2008). schizophrenia has a genetic basis, it does not depend on any
Lifetime prevalence of schizophrenia is more common for single gene.
men than women by a ratio of about 7:5. On average, it is also
more severe in men and has an earlier onset—usually in the Twin Studies
teens or early 20s for men and the mid to late 20s for women
(Aleman, Kahn, & Selten, 2003). The more closely you are biologically related to someone with
Researchers have documented several unexplained oddi- schizophrenia, the greater your own probability of schizo-
ties about schizophrenia. The points that follow do not fit phrenia, as shown in Figure 15.11 (Gottesman, 1991). One
neatly into any currently prominent theory. They indicate how of the most important points in Figure 15.11, confirmed by
many mysteries remain: other studies (Cardno et al., 1999), is that monozygotic twins
have a much higher concordance (agreement) for schizophre-
■ Schizophrenia is significantly less common than aver- nia than do dizygotic twins. Furthermore, twin pairs who
age among people with type 1 (juvenile-onset) diabetes, are really monozygotic, but thought they weren’t, are more
although it is more common than average in people with concordant than twin pairs who thought they were, but re-
type 2 (adult-onset) diabetes ( Juvonen et al., 2007). ally aren’t (Kendler, 1983). That is, being monozygotic is more
■ People with schizophrenia have an increased risk of critical than being treated as monozygotic.
colon cancer but below average probability of respira- The high concordance for monozygotic twins has long
tory cancer or brain cancer (Hippisley-Cox, Vinogra- been taken as strong evidence for a genetic influence. How-
dova, Coupland, & Parker, 2007; Roppel, 1978). ever, note two limitations:
■ People with schizophrenia seldom develop rheuma-
■ Monozygotic twins have only about 50% concordance,
toid arthritis or allergies (Goldman, 1999; Rubinstein, not 100%. Monozygotic twins could differ because a gene
1997). is activated in one individual and suppressed in another
■ Women who have a schizophrenic breakdown during (Tsujita et al., 1998), or they could differ because of
pregnancy usually give birth to daughters. However, environmental influences.
those who have a breakdown shortly after giving birth ■ In Figure 15.11, note the greater similarity between
usually give birth to sons (M. A. Taylor, 1969). dizygotic twins than between siblings. Dizygotic twins
■ Many people with schizophrenia have a characteristic have the same genetic resemblance as siblings but
body odor, attributed to the chemical trans-3-methyl- greater environmental similarity, including prenatal
2-hexenoic acid, and decreased ability to smell that environment.
15.2 Schizophrenia 475

Percent developing schizophrenia 40

Percent schizophrenia or similar


Genetic risk
General population 1% Not
30
Husband or wife of schizophrenic person 2%

Cousins of patient 2% 20

Uncles/aunts 2%
10
Nephews/nieces 4%

Grandchildren 5% 0
Normal Moderately Severely
dysfunctional dysfunctional
Half-siblings 6%
Adopting family
Children 13%
Figure 15.12  ​Probability of schizophrenia or similar
Children of schizophrenic mothers,
conditions in adopted children
adopted by nonschizophrenic mothers 17%
The probability was higher for children of a mother with schizo-
Siblings 9% phrenia, but growing up in a dysfunctional family magnified that
risk. (Based on data from Wynne et al., 2006)
DZ twins 17%

Parents 6%

MZ twins 48% land found a high probability of schizophrenia or related


Children of two conditions among children who had a biological mother with
schizophrenic parents 46% schizophrenia and a severely disordered adopting family. The
genetic risk itself or the disordered family itself had less effect,
Figure 15.11  ​Probabilities of developing schizophrenia as shown in Figure 15.12 (Wynne et al., 2006).
People with a closer genetic relationship to someone with schizo-
phrenia have a higher probability of developing it themselves.
(Based on data from Gottesman, 1991) Efforts to Locate a Gene
The strongest evidence for a genetic influence would be to
locate a gene that is consistently linked with schizophrenia.
Adopted Children Who Develop Researchers working with various populations have identi-
fied more than a dozen genes that appear to be more common
Schizophrenia in people with schizophrenia. One that has attracted much
When an adopted child develops schizophrenia, the disor- interest, called DISC1 (disrupted in schizophrenia 1), con-
der is more common in the person’s biological relatives than trols production of dendritic spines (Hayashi-Takagi et al.,
adopting relatives. One Danish study found schizophrenia in 2010) and the generation of new neurons in the hippocampus
12.5% of the immediate biological relatives and none of the (Duan et al., 2007). Other genes linked to schizophrenia in
adopting relatives (Kety et al., 1994). Note in Figure 15.11 several studies are important for brain development (Hall et
that children of a mother with schizophrenia have a moder- al., 2006; Stefansson et al., 2009), control of transmission at
ately high probability of schizophrenia, even if adopted by glutamate synapses (Dickman & Davis, 2009), and connec-
mentally healthy parents. tions between the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex
These results suggest a genetic basis, but they are also con- (Esslinger et al., 2009). However, researchers have not had
sistent with a prenatal influence. Consider a pregnant woman great success at replicating the results from one population to
with schizophrenia. True, she passes her genes to her child, another. A study of nearly 2,000 patients with schizophrenia
but she also provides the prenatal environment. Many women and a control group found no statistically significant relation-
with schizophrenia smoke, drink, use other drugs, and eat a ship between schizophrenia and any of the 14 genes that pre-
less than desirable diet during pregnancy. A disproportionate vious studies identified as linked to schizophrenia (Sanders
number have complications during pregnancy and delivery et al., 2008).
( Jablensky, Morgan, Zubrick, Bower, & Yellachich, 2005). If In a way, these results should not be surprising. If schizo-
some of their children develop schizophrenia, we cannot be phrenia depended on a single gene, it would be hard for that
sure that the influence is genetic. gene to remain in 1% of the population, given the natural
Studies on adopted children also support a role for en- selection pressures against it. People with schizophrenia
vironmental influences. A study of adopted children in Fin- die younger than other people, on average (Saha, Chant, &
476 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

McGrath, 2007). More importantly, on average they have fewer The Neurodevelopmental
than half as many children as other people do, and their broth-
ers and sisters do not compensate by having more children
Hypothesis
than average (Bundy, Stahl, & MacCabe, 2011). Any gene for According to the neurodevelopmental hypothesis now
schizophrenia should decline rapidly in prevalence, it seems. popular among researchers, schizophrenia begins with ab-
If schizophrenia has a genetic basis but we can’t find normalities in the prenatal (before birth) or neonatal (new-
any gene with a consistent link, and any gene that leads to born) development of the nervous system, based on either
schizophrenia can’t be passed down through many genera- genetics or other influences. These early problems leave
tions, what is going on? A prominent hypothesis is that many the developing brain vulnerable to other disturbances later
cases of schizophrenia arise from new mutations. Ordinarily, in life, including but not limited to highly stressful experi-
it would be ridiculous to suggest that a condition affecting ences. The eventual results are mild abnormalities in brain
1% of the population could depend on new mutations. Mu- anatomy and major disorders of behavior (Fatemi & Folsom,
tations just aren’t that common. But proper brain develop- 2009; Weinberger, 1996).
ment depends on hundreds of genes. A mutation in one gene The supporting evidence is that (a) several kinds of pre-
is a rare event, but a mutation in any of several hundred is natal or neonatal difficulties are linked to later schizophrenia;
not so rare. An even more likely possibility is deletion of a (b) people with schizophrenia have minor brain abnormalities
gene, a fairly common error in reproduction (International that apparently originate early in life; and (c) it is plausible
Schizophrenia Consortium, 2009). Researchers examined that abnormalities of early development could impair behav-
the chromosomes of people with and without schizophrenia ior in adulthood.
and found genetic microdeletions and microduplications (i.e.,
elimination or duplication of parts of a gene) in 5% of the
control group, 15% of people with schizophrenia, and 20% of Prenatal and Neonatal Environment
people with onset of schizophrenia before age 18 (Walsh et The risk of schizophrenia is elevated among people who had
al., 2008). Those microdeletions and microduplications were problems that could have affected their brain development,
distributed over a great many genes. Thus, the hypothesis is including poor nutrition of the mother during pregnancy,
that a new mutation or deletion of any of a large number of premature birth, low birth weight, and complications during
genes disrupts brain development and increases the probabil- delivery (Ballon, Dean, & Cadenhead, 2007). The risk is also
ity of schizophrenia. As fast as natural selection weeds out elevated if the mother was exposed to extreme stress, such as
those mutations or deletions, new ones arise to replace them. the sudden death of a close relative, early in her pregnancy
One observation supporting this idea is that schizophrenia (Khashau et al., 2008). None of these influences by itself ac-
is somewhat more common among children of older fathers counts for many cases of schizophrenia, although together
(Byrne, Agerbo, Ewald, Eaton, & Mortensen, 2003; Mala- their influence is greater (Cannon, Jones, & Murray, 2002).
spina et al., 2002). Women are born with all the eggs they will Schizophrenia has also been linked to head injuries in early
ever have, but men continue making new sperm throughout childhood (AbdelMalik, Husted, Chow, & Bassett, 2003),
life, and the possibility of mutations accumulates over time. although we do not know whether the head injuries led to
We need not assume that all cases of schizophrenia have a schizophrenia or early symptoms of schizophrenia increased
genetic predisposition. Others may depend on prenatal envi- the risk of head injuries.
ronment or other influences on brain development. If a mother is Rh-negative and her baby is Rh-positive,
the baby’s Rh-positive blood factor may trigger an immuno-
Stop & Check logical rejection by the mother. The response is weak with
the woman’s first Rh-positive baby but stronger in later preg-
18. The fact that adopted children who develop schizophrenia
nancies, and it is more intense with boy than girl babies. Sec-
usually have biological relatives with schizophrenia im-
ond- and later-born boy babies with Rh incompatibility have
plies a probable genetic basis. What other interpretation
an increased risk of hearing deficits, mental retardation, and
is possible?
several other problems, and about twice the usual probability
19. Does the hypothesis of new mutations conflict with the of schizophrenia (Hollister, Laing, & Mednick, 1996).
results showing that an aberrant form of the gene DISC1 Another suggestion of prenatal influences comes from
is often linked to schizophrenia? the season-of-birth effect: the tendency for people born in
winter to have a slightly (5% to 8%) greater probability of
ANSWERS
the mutation is more certain to cause schizophrenia. developing schizophrenia than people born at other times of
lead to schizophrenia, the DISC1 gene could be one where the year. This tendency is particularly pronounced in latitudes
tions in many genes can, according to the hypothesis,
far from the equator (Davies, Welham, Chant, Torrey, & Mc-
Grath, 2003; Torrey, Miller, et al., 1997).
even if the child is adopted early. 19. No. Although muta-

What might account for this effect? One possibility is


ment through prenatal environment as well as genetics,
18. A biological mother can influence her child’s develop-
complications of delivery or early nutrition ( Jablensky et al.,
2005). Another is viral infection. Influenza and other viral
15.2 Schizophrenia 477

epidemics are most common in the fall. Therefore, the reason- phrenia than in the general population (Leweke et al., 2004;
ing goes, many pregnant women become infected in the fall Niebuhr et al., 2008; Yolken et al., 2001).
with a virus that impairs a crucial stage of brain development In short, some cases of schizophrenia may develop as a re-
in a baby who will be born in the winter. A virus that affects sult of infections. This mechanism is an alternative or supple-
the mother might or might not cross the placenta into the fe- ment to genetics and other influences. Evidently, a variety of
tus’s brain, but the mother’s cytokines do cross, and excessive influences can lead to similar outcomes in schizophrenia.
cytokines can impair brain development (Zuckerman, Re-
havi, Nachman, & Weiner, 2003). Animal studies show that
some of the effects of cytokines on brain development appear Stop & Check
mild at first but gradually impair brain development as the
20. What does the season-of-birth effect suggest about a
individual approaches adulthood (Vuillermot, Weber, Feldon,
possible cause of schizophrenia?
& Meyer, 2010). The mother’s infection also causes a fever,
which can damage the fetal brain. A fever of just 38.5° C winter.
ANSWER
(101° F) slows the division of fetal neurons (Laburn, 1996). fall impair brain development of a baby born in the
(Exercise during pregnancy does not overheat the abdomen influenza or other infections of the mother during the
and is not dangerous to the fetus. Hot baths and saunas may who were born in the winter. One interpretation is that
be risky, however.) When mice are infected with influenza schizophrenia is slightly more common among people
during pregnancy, their offspring develop a number of behav- 20. The season-of-birth effect is the observation that
ioral abnormalities, including deficient exploration and defi-
cient social reactions to other mice (Shi, Fatemi, Sidwell, &
Patterson, 2003).
Researchers examined the records of tens of thousands
of people in Scotland, England, and Denmark over sev- Mild Brain Abnormalities
eral decades. They found increased schizophrenia rates In accord with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis, some
among people born 2 to 3 months after major influenza (though not all) people with schizophrenia show mild abnor-
epidemics, such as the one in the autumn of 1957 (Ad- malities of brain anatomy that vary from one individual to an-
ams, Kendell, Hare, & Munk-Jørgensen, 1993). Other other. On average, people with schizophrenia have less than
studies retrieved blood samples that hospitals had taken average gray matter and white matter, and larger than average
from pregnant women and stored for decades. Research- ventricles—the fluid-filled spaces within the brain (Meyer-
ers found increased incidence of influenza virus among Lindenberg, 2010; Wolkin et al., 1998; Wright et al., 2000)
mothers whose children eventually developed schizophre- (Figure 15.13)
nia (A. S. Brown et al., 2004; Buka et al., 2001). Rates Figure 15.14 summarizes 15 studies, including a total of
of schizophrenia are also increased among offspring of 390 people with schizophrenia. Brain areas marked in yel-
mothers who had rubella (German measles), herpes, and low showed decreased volume in the most studies, those in
other infections during pregnancy (A. S. Brown et al., various shades of red showed decreases in fewer studies, and
2001; Buka et al., 2008). those in gray appeared normal in all studies (Honea, Crow,
Certain childhood infections
may also relate to schizophrenia. Ventricles
The parasite Toxoplasma gondii
(discussed also in Chapter 12
in the context of anxiety and the
amygdala) reproduces only in cats,
but it can infect humans and other
species also. If it infects the brain
of an infant or child, it impairs
E. F. Torrey & M. F. Casanova/NIMH

brain development and leads to


memory disorder, hallucinations,
and delusions (Torrey & Yolken,
2005). People who develop schizo-
phrenia in adulthood are more
likely than other people to have
had a pet cat in childhood (Torrey,
Rawlings, & Yolken, 2000). Blood Figure 15.13  ​Coronal sections for identical twins
tests have found antibodies to the The twin on the left has schizophrenia; the twin on the right does not. The ventricles (near the
Toxoplasma parasite in a higher center of each brain) are larger in the twin with schizophrenia.
percentage of people with schizo-
478 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

frontal cortex (Park, Holzman, & Goldman-Rakic, 1995)


Left Right 69 (Methods 15.1).
57 At a microscopic level, the most reliable finding is that cell
43
bodies are smaller than normal, especially in the hippocampus
31
Image not available due to copyright restrictions and prefrontal cortex (Pierri, Volk, Auh, Sampson, & Lewis,
21
2001; Rajkowska, Selemon, & Goldman-Rakic, 1998; Sele-
15
11
mon, Rajkowska, & Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Weinberger, 1999).
7 Lateralization also differs from the normal pattern. In
most people, the left hemisphere is slightly larger than the
 ​ right, especially in the planum temporale of the temporal lobe,
but in people with schizophrenia, the right planum temporale
is equal or larger (Kasai et al., 2003; Kwon et al., 1999). People
with schizophrenia have lower than normal overall activity in
Text not available due to copyright restrictions
the left hemisphere (Gur & Chin, 1999) and are more likely
than other people to be left-handed (Satz & Green, 1999). All
these results suggest a subtle change in brain development.
The reasons behind the brain abnormalities are not certain.
Most researchers have been careful to limit their studies to pa-
tients with schizophrenia who have never taken, or who have
not recently taken, antipsychotic drugs, so the deficits are not a
result of treatments for schizophrenia. However, many people
Passingham, & Mackay, 2005). Note that the strongest defi- with schizophrenia use alcohol, marijuana, and other drugs,
cits were in the left temporal and frontal areas of the cortex. and it is likely that some of the brain abnormalities result from
Note also that most cortical areas showed mild abnormalities heavy drug use (Rais et al., 2008; Sullivan et al., 2000).
in at least one or two studies. The thalamus, which is in the The results are inconsistent as to whether the brain
interior of the brain and therefore not shown in Figure 15.14, damage associated with schizophrenia is progressive—that
is also smaller than average for people with schizophrenia is, whether it increases over time. The brain damage asso-
(Harms et al., 2007). ciated with Parkinson’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and
The areas with consistent signs of abnormality include Alzheimer’s disease gets worse as the person ages. Brain ab-
some that mature slowly, such as the dorsolateral prefron- normalities are found in young people shortly after a diag-
tal cortex (Berman, Torrey, Daniel, & Weinberger, 1992; nosis of schizophrenia (Lieberman et al., 2001), and many
Fletcher et al., 1998; Gur, Cowell, et al., 2000). The abnor- studies find that the brain abnormalities are no greater in
malities include weaker than average connections from the older patients (Andreasen et al., 1990; Censits, Ragland,
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to other brain areas, and less Gur, & Gur, 1997; Russell, Munro, Jones, Hemsley, & Mur-
than normal activity in this area during tasks requiring at- ray, 1997; Selemon et al., 1995). However, other studies
tention and memory (Lynall et al., 2010; van den Heuvel, show a moderate degree of increased brain loss as patients
Mandl, Stam, Kahn, & Pol, 2010; Weiss et al., 2009). As you age (Cahn et al., 2002; Hulshoff et al., 2001; Mathalon, Sul-
might predict, people with schizophrenia perform poorly at livan, Lim, & Pfefferbaum, 2001; Rais et al., 2008). Never-
tasks that depend on the prefrontal cortex (Goldberg, Wein- theless, the brains of people with schizophrenia do not show
berger, Berman, Pliskin, & Podd, 1987; Spindler, Sullivan, the signs that accompany neuron death—proliferation of
Menon, Lim, & Pfefferbaum, 1997). Most patients with glia cells and activation of the genes responsible for repair
schizophrenia show deficits of memory and attention simi- after injury (Arnold, 2000; Benes, 1995; K. O. Lim et al.,
lar to those of people with damage to the temporal or pre- 1998). Possibly, the neurons are shrinking without dying.

Methods 15.1

The Wisconsin Card Sorting Task


Neuropsychologists use many behavioral tests to measure the the person is supposed to sort them by a different rule, such
functioning of the prefrontal cortex. One is the Wisconsin as number. Shifting to a new rule requires suppressing the
Card Sorting Task. A person is handed a shuffled deck of old one and evokes activity in the prefrontal cortex (Konishi
cards that differ in number, color, and shape of ­objects— et al., 1998). People with damage to the prefrontal cortex
for example, three red circles, five blue triangles, four green can sort by whichever rule is first, but then they have trouble
squares. First the person is asked to sort them by one rule, shifting to a new rule. People with schizophrenia have the
such as separate them by color. Then the rule changes, and same difficulty. (So do children.)
15.2 Schizophrenia 479

In any case, most of the damage is apparent early, and later Treatments
changes are relatively small.
Before antipsychotic drugs became available in the mid-1950s,
most people with schizophrenia were confined to mental hos-
Early Development and Later pitals with little hope of recovery. Today, mental hospitals are
Psychopathology far less crowded because of drugs and outpatient treatment.
One question may have struck you. How can we reconcile the
idea of abnormalities in early development with the fact that the Antipsychotic Drugs and Dopamine
disorder is usually diagnosed after age 20? The time course may In the 1950s, psychiatrists discovered that chlorpromazine
not be as puzzling as it seems at first (Weinberger, 1996). Most (trade name Thorazine) relieves the positive symptoms of
of the people who develop schizophrenia in adulthood had schizophrenia for most, though not all, patients. Researchers
shown other problems since childhood, including deficits in later discovered other antipsychotic, or neuroleptic, drugs
attention, memory, and impulse control (Keshavan, Diwadkar, (drugs that tend to relieve schizophrenia and similar condi-
Montrose, Rajarethinam, & Sweeney, 2005). Furthermore, the tions) in two chemical families: the phenothiazines (FEE-
prefrontal cortex, an area that shows consistent signs of deficit no-THI-uh-zeens), which include chlorpromazine, and the
in schizophrenia, is one of the slowest brain areas to mature. butyrophenones (BYOO-tir-oh-FEE-noans), which in-
In one study, researchers damaged this area in infant monkeys clude haloperidol (trade name Haldol). Behavioral benefits
and tested the monkeys later. At age 1 year, the monkeys’ behav- of any of these drugs develop gradually over a month or more.
ior was nearly normal, but by age 2 years, it had deteriorated Symptoms generally return after cessation of treatment.
markedly (P. S. Goldman, 1971, 1976). That is, the effects of As Figure 15.16 illustrates, each of these drugs blocks do-
the brain damage grew worse over age. Presumably, the effects pamine synapses. For each drug, researchers determined the
of brain damage were minimal at age 1 year because the dor- mean dose prescribed for patients with schizophrenia (dis-
solateral prefrontal cortex doesn’t do much at that age anyway. played along the horizontal axis) and the amount needed to
Later, when it should begin assuming important functions, the block dopamine receptors (displayed along the vertical axis).
damage begins to make a difference (Figure 15.15). As the figure shows, the drugs that are most effective against
schizophrenia (and therefore used in the smallest doses) are
the most effective at blocking dopamine receptors (Seeman,
Stop & Check Lee, Chau-Wong, & Wong, 1976).
21. If schizophrenia is due to abnormal brain development, That finding inspired the dopamine hypothesis of schizo-
why do behavioral symptoms not become apparent until phrenia, which holds that schizophrenia results from excess
later in life? activity at dopamine synapses in certain brain areas. Although
the concentration of dopamine in the brain is no higher than
ANSWER
little anyway. normal, the turnover is elevated, especially in the basal ganglia
early in life, when the prefrontal cortex is contributing
(Kumakura et al., 2007). That is, neurons release dopamine
at a faster than average rate and synthesize more to replace
area’s development might not produce any symptoms

the molecules that they do not reabsorb. Elevated dopamine


reach maturity; therefore, early disruption of this
21. Parts of the prefrontal cortex are very slow to
release also occurs in people showing the first symptoms of
schizophrenia (Howes et al., 2009).

Infant brain Age Age


damage one year two years

Little apparent Clear effect of


effect of lesion; lesion; performs
performs almost much worse than
as well as undamaged
undamaged monkeys do
Damage to monkeys do
dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex

Figure 15.15  ​Delayed effects of brain damage in infant monkeys


After damage to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, monkeys are unimpaired at age 1 year but impaired later, when this area ordinarily
matures. Researchers speculate that similar damage in humans might produce behavioral deficits not apparent until adulthood. (Based
on P. S. Goldman, 1976)
480 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Spiroperidol
10–10

Dosage necessary to block dopamine receptors Benperidol

Trifluperidol
Pimozide
Fluphenazine
(moles per liter)

Droperidol
10–9
Haloperidol
Thiothixene
Trifluperazine
Moperone
Prochlorperazine
10–8 Molindone
Thioridazine
Clozapine
Trazodone
Chlorpromazine
Promazine

10–7
1000 100 10 1 0.1
Mean clinical dose for controlling schizophrenia (mg per day)

Figure 15.16  ​Dopamine-blocking effects of antipsychotic drugs


Drugs are arranged along the horizontal axis in terms of the average daily dose prescribed for patients with schizophrenia. (Horizontal lines
indicate common ranges.) Larger doses are to the left and smaller doses are to the right so that more effective drugs are to the right. Along
the vertical axis is a measurement of the amount of each drug required to achieve a certain degree of blockage of postsynaptic dopamine
receptors. Larger doses are toward the bottom and smaller doses are toward the top so that the drugs on top are more effective. (From
“Antipsychotic Drug Doses and Neuroleptic/Dopamine Receptors,” by P. Seeman, T. Lee, M. Chau-Wong, and K. Wong, Nature, 261, 1976,
pp. 717–719. Copyright © 1976 Macmillan Magazines Limited. Reprinted by permission of Nature and Phillip Seeman.)

Further support for the dopamine hypothesis comes from ■ Second count: IBZM binds to all D2 receptors
the fact that large, repeated use of amphetamine, methamphet- (because AMPT eliminated production of
amine, or cocaine induces substance-induced psychotic disor- dopamine).
der, characterized by hallucinations and delusions, the positive ■ Second count minus first count equals the number of

symptoms of schizophrenia. Each of these drugs increases or D2 receptors bound to dopamine at the first count.
prolongs the activity at dopamine synapses. LSD, which also The researchers found that people with schizophrenia had
produces psychotic symptoms, is best known for its effects on about twice as many D2 receptors occupied as normal (Abi-
serotonin synapses, but it also stimulates dopamine synapses. Dargham et al., 2000). Another study found that among pa-
Researchers set out to measure the number of dopamine tients with schizophrenia, the greater the amount of D2 recep-
receptors occupied at a given moment. They used a radioac- tor activation in the prefrontal cortex, the greater the cognitive
tively labeled drug, IBZM, that binds to dopamine type D2 impairment (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2002).
receptors. Because IBZM binds only to receptors that dopa-
mine did not already bind, measuring the radioactivity counts
the number of vacant dopamine receptors. Then the research-
ers used a second drug, AMPT, that blocks all synthesis of do- Stop & Check
pamine and again used IBZM to count the number of vacant
23. The ability of traditional antipsychotic drugs to re-
D2 receptors. Because AMPT had prevented production of
lieve schizophrenia correlates strongly with what effect on
dopamine, all D2 receptors should be vacant at this time, so
neurotransmitters?
the researchers got a count of the total. Then they subtracted
the first count from the second count, yielding the number of synapses.
ANSWER
D2 receptors occupied by dopamine at the first count: strongly with how well they block activity at dopamine

First count: IBZM binds to all D2 receptors not already


22. Their ability to relieve schizophrenia correlates

attached to dopamine.
15.2 Schizophrenia 481

Role of Glutamate tipsychotic drug by itself, it increases the effects of other anti-
psychotic drugs, especially with regard to negative symptoms
Abnormalities of dopamine transmission need not be the (Heresco-Levy et al., 1999; Heresco-Levy & Javitt, 2004).
whole story for schizophrenia. According to the glutamate Studies on laboratory mice found that extra glycine decreases
hypothesis of schizophrenia, the problem relates in part to the behavioral responses to phencyclidine (Yee et al., 2006).
deficient activity at glutamate synapses, especially in the pre-
frontal cortex. In many brain areas, dopamine inhibits glu-
tamate release, or glutamate stimulates neurons that inhibit
Stop & Check
dopamine release. Therefore, increased dopamine would pro-
duce the same effects as decreased glutamate. The antipsy- 23. What drugs induce mainly the positive symptoms of
chotic effects of drugs that block dopamine are compatible schizophrenia? What drug can induce both positive and
with either the excess-dopamine hypothesis or the deficient- negative symptoms?
glutamate hypothesis. 24. Why are the effects of antipsychotic drugs equally com-
Schizophrenia is associated with lower than normal re- patible with the dopamine hypothesis and the glutamate
lease of glutamate and fewer than normal receptors in the hypothesis?
prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (Akbarian et al., 1995;
Ibrahim et al., 2000; Tsai et al., 1995). Similar abnormali- ing glutamate.
ANSWERS
ties occur in people known to be at high risk for developing increasing dopamine are similar to those of decreas-
schizophrenia, because of their family background and early rons that inhibit dopamine. Therefore, the effects of
behaviors (Valli et al., 2011). Mice with a deficiency of glu- cells in many areas, and glutamate stimulates neu-
tamate receptors show some abnormal behaviors, including negative symptoms. 24. Dopamine inhibits glutamate
increased anxiety, impaired memory, and impaired social be- delusions. Phencyclidine induces both positive and
haviors (Belforte et al., 2010).
duces positive symptoms, such as hallucinations and

Further support for the glutamate hypothesis comes from


23. Repeated use of amphetamine, cocaine, or LSD in-

the effects of phencyclidine (PCP) (“angel dust”), a drug that


inhibits the NMDA glutamate receptors. At low doses, it pro-
duces intoxication and slurred speech. At larger doses, it pro-
duces both positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia, New Drugs
including hallucinations, thought disorder, loss of emotions, The brain has several dopamine pathways with different func-
and memory loss. PCP is an interesting model for schizophre- tions. The drugs that block dopamine synapses produce their
nia in other regards also (Farber, Newcomer, & Olney, 1999; benefits by acting on neurons in the mesolimbocortical sys-
Olney & Farber, 1995): tem, a set of neurons that project from the midbrain tegmen-
tum to the limbic system. However, the drugs also block dopa-
■ PCP and the related drug ketamine produce little if any mine neurons in the mesostriatal system that projects to the basal
psychotic response in preadolescents. Just as the symp- ganglia (Figure 15.18). The effect on the basal ganglia produces
toms of schizophrenia usually begin to emerge well after tardive dyskinesia (TARD-eev dis-kih-NEE-zhee-uh), char-
puberty, so do the psychotic effects of PCP and ketamine. acterized by tremors and other involuntary movements that de-
■ LSD, amphetamine, and cocaine produce temporary velop gradually and to varying degrees among different patients
schizophrenic symptoms in almost anyone, and the (Kiriakakis, Bhatia, Quinn, & Marsden, 1998).
effects are not much worse in people with a history
of schizophrenia than in anyone else. However, PCP
produces severe effects for someone who has recovered Glycine Glutamate
binding site binding site
from schizophrenia, including a long-lasting relapse.
It might seem that the best test of the glutamate hypothesis
would be to administer glutamate itself. However, recall from
Chapter 5 that strokes kill neurons by overstimulating gluta-
mate synapses. Increasing overall brain glutamate would be
risky. However, drugs that stimulate particular kinds of metabo-
tropic glutamate receptors have shown much promise in treating
schizophrenia (González-Maeso et al., 2008; Patil et al., 2007).
Furthermore, the NMDA glutamate receptor has a pri-
mary site that is activated by glutamate and a secondary site
that is activated by glycine (Figure 15.17). Glycine by itself
Figure 15.17  ​An NMDA glutamate receptor
does not activate the receptor, but it increases the effectiveness
NMDA glutamate receptors have a primary binding site for gluta-
of glutamate. Thus, an increase in glycine can increase the ac- mate and a secondary binding site for glycine. Glycine increases
tivity at NMDA synapses without overstimulating glutamate the effect of glutamate. (© Cengage Learning 2013)
throughout the brain. Although glycine is not an effective an-
482 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Basal ganglia
they avoid tardive dyskinesia, they produce other side effects,
Mesostriatal including weight gain and impairment of the immune system.
system
All things considered, the atypical antipsychotics do not im-
prove overall quality of life more than the older drugs (Cross-
Prefrontal ley & Constante, 2010; P. B. Jones et al., 2006).
cortex Compared to drugs like haloperidol, the second-generation
antipsychotics have less effect on dopamine type D2 receptors
but more strongly antagonize serotonin type 5-HT2 receptors
(Kapur et al., 2000; Meltzer, Matsubara, & Lee, 1989; Mrzljak
et al., 1996; Roth, Willins, Kristiansen, & Kroeze, 1999). They
also increase the release of glutamate (Melone et al., 2001). In
short, schizophrenia is neither a one-gene disorder nor a one-
neurotransmitter disorder.
Mesolimbocortical
system
Ventral
tegmental area
Substantia
nigra

Figure 15.18  ​Two major dopamine pathways


Overactivity of the mesolimbocortical system is linked to the
symptoms of schizophrenia. The path to the basal ganglia is as-
sociated with tardive dyskinesia, a movement disorder. (Adapted
from Valzelli, 1980)

Once tardive dyskinesia emerges, it can last long after


someone quits the drug (Kiriakakis et al., 1998). Conse-
quently, the best strategy is to prevent it from starting. Cer-
tain new drugs called second-generation antipsychotics, or
atypical antipsychotics, alleviate schizophrenia without pro-
Figure 15.19  ​PET scans of a patient with schizophrenia
ducing movement problems (Figure 15.19). The most com-
These PET scans of a patient with schizophrenia (a) taking clozap-
mon of these drugs are clozapine, amisulpride, risperidone,
ine and (b) during a period off the drug demonstrate that cloza­
olanza­pine, and aripiprazole. They are more effective than pine increases brain activity in many brain areas. Red indicates
older drugs at treating the negative symptoms of schizophre- the highest activity, followed by yellow, green, and blue. (Hank
nia, and they are now used more widely ( J. M. Davis, Chen, Morgan/Science Source/Photo Researchers)
& Glick, 2003; Edlinger et al., 2005). Unfortunately, although
15.2 Schizophrenia 483

Module 15.2  ■  In Closing

Many Remaining Mysteries

A great deal about abnormal psychology remains unknown. 100%? Also, why are the treatments for both depression and
One of the most fundamental questions is whether it even makes schizophrenia highly successful for some people and not at all for
sense to distinguish among different disorders. Some of the others? Perhaps you can name additional puzzles.
drugs originally approved for schizophrenia are often effective in Research is a little like reading a good mystery novel that
relieving depression or bipolar disorder. Antidepressant drugs presents a mixture of important clues and irrelevant informa-
help relieve anxiety disorders. Drugs intended for bipolar disor- tion. In research on schizophrenia, we have an enormous
der help many people with attention-deficit disorder. If the same amount of information, but also major gaps and occasional
treatments work for different disorders, maybe those disorders points that don’t seem to fit. The final chapter of our mystery
are not so different after all (Dean, 2011). Another major mys- novel on schizophrenia isn’t complete. However, although re-
tery is why concordance for schizophrenia in monozygotic twins searchers have not yet solved the mystery, it should also be clear
is only about 50%. If they share their genes and presumably that they have made progress. It will be fascinating to see what
nearly the same environment, why isn’t concordance nearly develops in future research.

summary
  1. Positive symptoms of schizophrenia (behaviors that are and frontal lobes. They also show cognitive deficits that
not present in most other people) include hallucina- make sense if their frontal and temporal lobes are less
tions, delusions, inappropriate emotions, bizarre than fully functional.  477
behaviors, and thought disorder.  472   8. Parts of the prefrontal cortex are very slow to mature. It
  2. Negative symptoms (normal behaviors absent that is plausible that early disruption of those areas might
should be present) include deficits of social interaction, produce behavioral symptoms that become manifest as
emotional expression, and speech.  472 schizophrenia in young adults.  479
  3. Studies of twins and adopted children imply a genetic   9. According to the dopamine hypothesis, schizophre-
predisposition to schizophrenia. However, the adoption nia is due to excess dopamine activity. Drugs that
studies do not distinguish between the roles of genetics block dopamine synapses reduce the positive
and prenatal environment.  474 symptoms of schizophrenia, and drugs that
  4. So far, researchers have not located any gene that is increase dopamine activity induce the positive
strongly linked with schizophrenia in general. A symptoms.  479
promising hypothesis is that schizophrenia results from 10. According to the glutamate hypothesis, part of the
new mutations or deletions of any of the hundreds of problem is deficient glutamate activity. Phencyclidine,
genes that are important for brain development.  475 which blocks NMDA glutamate synapses, produces
  5. According to the neurodevelopmental hypothesis, either both positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia,
genes or difficulties early in life impair brain develop- especially in people predisposed to schizophre-
ment in ways that increase vulnerability to later insults nia.  481
and predispose to behavioral abnormalities beginning in 11. Prolonged use of antipsychotic drugs may produce
early adulthood.  476 tardive dyskinesia, a movement disorder. Second-gen-
  6. The probability of schizophrenia is slightly higher than eration antipsychotic drugs relieve both positive and
average for those who were subjected to difficulties before negative symptoms without producing tardive dyskine-
or at the time of birth or during early infancy.  476 sia. However, these drugs apparently do not improve
overall quality of life any better than the original
  7. Some people with schizophrenia show mild abnormali-
drugs do.  481
ties of brain development, especially in the temporal
484 Chapter 15 Mood Disorders and Schizophrenia

Key Terms
Terms are defined in the module on the page number indicated. They’re also presented in alphabetical order with definitions in the
book’s Subject Index/Glossary, which begins on page 561. Interactive flashcards and crossword puzzles are among the online re-
sources available to help you learn these terms and the concepts they represent.
antipsychotic (neuroleptic) drugs  479 glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia  472
butyrophenones  479 schizophrenia  481 season-of-birth effect  476
chlorpromazine  474 hallucinations  472 second-generation
concordance  479 mesolimbocortical system  481 antipsychotics  482
delusions  472 negative symptoms  472 substance-induced psychotic
differential diagnosis  473 neurodevelopmental hypothesis  476 disorder  480
DISC1  475 phencyclidine (PCP)  481 tardive dyskinesia  481
dopamine hypothesis of phenothiazines  479
schizophrenia  479 positive symptoms  472

Thought Question
On average, people who use much marijuana are more likely while the prevalence of schizophrenia has remained steady or
than others to develop schizophrenia. However, over the last decreased. What would be a reasonable conclusion about the
several decades, the use of marijuana has increased substantially relationship between marijuana use and schizophrenia?

chapter 15 Interactive Exploration and Study

The Psychology CourseMate for this text brings chapter topics to life with interactive
learning, study, and exam preparation tools, including quizzes and flashcards for the Key
Concepts that appear throughout each module, as well as an interactive media-rich eBook version
of the text that is fully searchable and includes highlighting and note-taking capabilities and inter-
active versions of the book’s Stop & Check quizzes and Try It Yourself Online activities. The site
also features Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, videos, and animations to help you better
understand concepts—logon and learn more at www.cengagebrain.com, which is your gateway to
all of this text’s complimentary and premium resources, including the following:

Virtual Biological Psychology Labs


Explore the experiments that led to modern-day understanding of
biopsychology with the Virtual Biological Psychology Labs, featuring
© 2013 Cengage Learning

a realistic lab environment that allows you to conduct experiments and


evaluate data to better understand how scientists came to the conclu-
sions presented in your text. The labs cover a range of topics, including
perception, motivation, cognition, and more. You may purchase access
at www.cengagebrain.com, or login at login.cengagebrain.com if an
access card was included with your text.
15.2 Schizophrenia 485

Videos
Also available—
Magnetic Stimulation to the Brain

Andre, a Portrait of Schizophrenia

© 2013 Cengage Learning


Emilie, a Portrait of Bipolar

Animations
Also available—
■ Saccades and Schizophrenia  xxx
© 2013 Cengage Learning

Antidepressants

Suggestions for Further Exploration


Books
Andreasen, N. C. (2001). Brave new brain. New York: Oxford University Press. Excellent discus-
sion of biological research on psychiatric disorders by one of the leading researchers dealing with
schizophrenia.
Kirsch, I. (2010). The Emperor’s New Drugs. New York: Basic Books. A highly skeptical discussion
of the effectiveness or ineffectiveness of antidepressant drugs.
Websites
The Psychology CourseMate for this text provides regularly updated links to relevant online re-
sources for this chapter concerning depression and schizophrenia.
Brief, Basic Chemistry
A
Main Ideas in nature.) Figure A.1, the periodic table, lists each of these
elements. Of these, only a few are important for life on
1. All matter is composed of a limited number of elements
Earth. Table A.1 shows the elements commonly found in
that combine in endless ways.
the human body.
2. Atoms, the component parts of an element, consist of
Note that each element has a one- or two-letter abbre-
protons, neutrons, and electrons. Most atoms can gain or
viation, such as O for oxygen, H for hydrogen, and Ca for
lose electrons, or share them with other atoms.
calcium. These are internationally accepted symbols that fa-
3. The chemistry of life is predominantly the chemistry of
cilitate communication among chemists who speak different
carbon compounds.
languages. For example, element number 19 is called potas-
sium in English, potassio in Italian, kālijs in Latvian, and
draslík in Czech. But chemists in all countries use the symbol
K (from kalium, the Latin word for “potassium”). Similarly,
the symbol for sodium is Na (from natrium, the Latin word
Introduction for “sodium”), and the symbol for iron is Fe (from the Latin
To understand certain aspects of biological psychology, par- word ferrum).
ticularly the action potential and the molecular mechanisms A compound is represented by the symbols for the ele-
of synaptic transmission, you need to know a little about ments that compose it. For example, NaCl stands for sodium
chemistry. If you have taken a high school or college course chloride (common table salt). H2O, the symbol for water, in-
and remember the material reasonably well, you should have dicates that water consists of two parts of hydrogen and one
no trouble with the chemistry in this text. If your knowledge part of oxygen.
of chemistry is pretty hazy, this appendix will help. (If you
plan to take other courses in biological psychology, you should
study as much biology and chemistry as possible.)   The Elements That Compose
Table A.1 Almost All of the Human Body
Percentage by Weight
Element Symbol in Human Body
Elements and Compounds
Oxygen O 65
If you look around, you will see an enormous variety of Carbon C 18
materials—dirt, water, wood, plastic, metal, cloth, glass, your Hydrogen H 10
own body. Every object is composed of a small number of ba- Nitrogen N 3
sic building blocks. If a piece of wood catches fire, it breaks Calcium Ca 2
down into ashes, gases, and water vapor. The same is true of Phosphorus P 1.1
your body. An investigator could take those ashes, gases, and Potassium K 0.35
water and break them down by chemical and electrical means Sulfur S 0.25
into carbon, oxygen, hydrogen, nitrogen, and a few other ma- Sodium Na 0.15
terials. Eventually, however, the investigator arrives at a set of Chlorine Cl 0.15
materials that cannot be broken down further: Pure carbon or Magnesium Mg 0.05
pure oxygen, for example, cannot be converted into anything Iron Fe Trace
simpler, at least not by ordinary chemical means. (High-power Copper Cu Trace
bombardment with subatomic particles is another story.) The Iodine I Trace
matter we see is composed of elements (materials that cannot Fluorine F Trace
be broken down into other materials) and compounds (mate- Manganese Mn Trace
rials made up by combining elements). Zinc Zn Trace
Chemists have found 92 elements in nature, and they Selenium Se Trace
have constructed more in the laboratory. (Actually, one of Molybdenum Mo Trace
the 92—technetium—is so rare as to be virtually unknown

486
Periodic Table of the Elements
Alkali
Period Metals Noble Gases

1 18
IA Alkaline VIIA
Earth
1 Metals Halogens 2
1 H He
hydrogen 2 13 14 15 16 17 helium
1.008 IIA IIIA IVA VA VIA VIIA 4.003

3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
2 Li Be B C N O F Ne
lithium beryllium boron carbon nitrogen oxygen fluorine neon
6.941 9.012 10.81 12.011 14.007 16.0 18.999 20.179 

11 12 Transition Elements 13 14 15 16 17 18
3 Na Mg Al Si P S Cl Ar
sodium magnesium 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 aluminum silicon phosphorous sulfur chlorine argon
22.99 24.305 IIIB IVB VB VIB VIIB VIIIB VIIIB VIIIB IB IIB 26.982 28.085 30.974 32.060 35.453 39.948

19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36
4 K Ca Sc Ti V Cr Mn Fe Co Ni Cu Zn Ga Ge As Se Br Kr
potassium calcium scandium titanium vanadium chromium manganese iron cobalt nickel copper zinc gallium germanium arsenic selenium bromine krypton
39.098 40.08 44.955 47.90 50.941 51.996 54.938 55.847 58.933 58.70 63.546 65.38 69.72 72.59 74.922 78.96 79.904 83.80

37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54
5 Rb Sr Y Zr Nb Mo Tc Ru Rh Pd Ag Cd In Sn Sb Te I Xe
rubidium strontium yttrium zirconium niobium molybdenum technetium ruthenium rhodium palladium silver cadmium indium tin antimony tellurium iodine xenon
85.468 87.62 88.906 91.22 92.906 95.940 (97) 101.07 102.905 106.40 107.868 112.41 114.82 118.69 121.75 127.60 126.904 131.30

55 56 57 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86
6 Cs Ba La Hf Ta W Re Os Ir Pt Au Hg Tl Pb Bi Po At Rn
cesium barium lanthanum hafnium tantalum tungsten rhenium osmium iridum platinum gold mercury thallium lead bismuth polonium astatine radon
132.905 137.33 138.906 178.49 180.948 183.85 186.207 190.20 192.22 195.09 196.967 200.59 204.37 207.20 208.980 (209) (210) (222)

87 88 89 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118
7 Fr Ra Ac Rf Db Sg Bh Hs Mt Ds Rg Cn Uut Uuq Uup Uuh Uus Uuo
francium radium actinium rutherfordium dubnium seaborgium bohrium hassium meitnerium darmstadtium roentgenium copernicium ununtrium ununquadium ununpentium ununhexium ununseptium ununoctium
(223) 226.025 (227) (261) (262) (266) (264) (269) (268) (271) (272) (285) (284) (289) (288) (292) (?) (?)

Inner Transition Elements

58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71
Lanthanides Ce Pr Nd Pm Sm Eu Gd Tb Dy Ho Er Tm Yb Lu
6 cerium praseodymium neodymium prometheum samarium europium gadolinium terbium dysprosium holmium erbium thulium ytterbium lutetium
140.12 140.908 144.24 (145) 150.40 151.96 157.25 158.925 162.50 164.93 167.26 168.934 173.04 174.97

90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103


Actinides Th Pa U Np Pu Am Cm Bk Cf Es Fm Md No Lr
7 thorium protactinium uranium neptunium plutonium americium curium berkelium californium einsteinium fermium mendelevium nobelium lawrencium

Elements and Compounds


232.038 231.036 238.029 (237) (244) (243) (247) (247) (251) (254) (257) (258) (255) (260)

Key atomic number 1


H symbol of element
element name hydrogen
1.008 atomic weight

Figure A.1  The periodic table of chemistry


It is called “periodic” because certain properties show up at periodic intervals. For example, the column
from lithium down consists of metals that readily form salts. The column at the far right consists of
gases that do not readily form compounds. Elements 112–118 have only tentative names and symbols.

487
488 Appendix A Brief, Basic Chemistry

Atoms and Molecules Sodium Chloride


A block of iron can be chopped finer and finer until it is di- ion ion
vided into tiny pieces that cannot be broken down any further.
Na+ Cl–
These pieces are called atoms. Every element is composed of
atoms. A compound, such as water, can also be divided into
tinier and tinier pieces. The smallest possible piece of a com-
pound is called a molecule. A molecule of water can be fur-
ther decomposed into two atoms of hydrogen and one atom
of oxygen, but when that happens the compound is broken
and is no longer water. A molecule is the smallest piece of a –
+ +

compound that retains the properties of the compound. + – +
An atom is composed of subatomic particles, includ- + – – +
+
ing protons, neutrons, and electrons. A proton has a posi- – –
tive electrical charge, a neutron has a neutral charge, and an + – +
electron has a negative charge. The nucleus of an atom—its
center—contains one or more protons plus a number of neu- Part of a sodium crystal
trons. Electrons are found in the space around the nucleus.
Because an atom has the same number of protons as electrons, Figure A.2  The crystal structure of sodium chloride
the electrical charges balance out. (Ions, which we will soon Each sodium ion is surrounded by chloride ions, and each ­chloride
consider, have an imbalance of positive and negative charges.) ion is surrounded by sodium ions; no ion is bound to any other
single ion in particular.
The difference between one element and another is in
the number of protons in the nucleus of the atom. Hydro-
gen has just one proton, for example, and oxygen has eight.
The number of protons is the atomic number of the element; some pairs of atoms share electrons with each other, forming
in the periodic table it is recorded at the top of the square a covalent bond. For example, two hydrogen atoms bind, as
for each element. The number at the bottom is the element’s shown in Figure A.3, and two hydrogen atoms bind with an
atomic weight, which indicates the weight of an atom relative oxygen atom, as shown in Figure A.4. Atoms that are attached
to the weight of one proton. A proton has a weight of one by a covalent bond cannot move independently of one another.
unit, a neutron has a weight just trivially greater than one, and
an electron has a weight just trivially greater than zero. The
atomic weight of the element is the number of protons in the
atom plus the average number of neutrons. For example, most H •• H
H• + •H
hydrogen atoms have one proton and no neutrons; a few at-
oms per thousand have one or two neutrons, giving an average H H H2
atom atom molecule
atomic weight of 1.008. Sodium ions have 11 protons; most
also have 12 neutrons, and the atomic weight is slightly less Figure A.3  Structure of a hydrogen molecule
than 23. (Can you figure out the number of neutrons in the A hydrogen atom has one electron; in the compound the two
average potassium atom? Refer to Figure A.1.) atoms share the two electrons equally.

Ions and Chemical Bonds


An atom that has gained or lost one or more electrons is called
an ion. For example, if sodium and chloride come together, the
sodium atoms readily lose one electron each and the chloride
atoms gain one each. The result is a set of positively charged
sodium ions (indicated Na1) and negatively charged chloride
O
ions (Cl2). Potassium atoms, like sodium atoms, tend to lose
an electron and to become positively charged ions (K1); cal- H
cium ions tend to lose two electrons and gain a double positive
charge (Ca11). H
Because positive charges attract negative charges, sodium
ions attract chloride ions. When dry, sodium and chloride form
a crystal structure, as Figure A.2 shows. (In water solution, the Figure A.4  Structure of a water molecule
The oxygen atom shares a pair of electrons with each hydrogen
two kinds of ions move about haphazardly, occasionally attract-
atom. Oxygen holds the electrons more tightly, making the oxygen
ing one another but then pulling apart.) The attraction of posi- part of the molecule more negatively charged than the hydrogen
tive ions for negative ions forms an ionic bond. In other cases, part of the molecule.
instead of transferring an electron from one atom to another,
Reactions of Carbon Atoms 489

Reactions of Carbon Atoms Ringed structures are common in organic chemistry. To simplify
the diagrams chemists often omit the hydrogen atoms. You can
Living organisms depend on the enormously versatile com- simply assume that each carbon atom in the diagram has four
pounds of carbon. Because of the importance of these com- covalent bonds and that all the bonds not shown are with hy-
pounds for life, the chemistry of carbon is known as organic drogen atoms. To further simplify the diagrams, chemists often
chemistry. omit the carbon atoms themselves, showing only the carbon-to-
Carbon atoms form covalent bonds with hydrogen, oxy- carbon bonds. For example, the two molecules shown in the pre-
gen, and a number of other elements. They also form covalent vious diagram might be rendered as follows:
bonds with other carbon atoms. Two carbon atoms may share
from one to three pairs of electrons. Such bonds can be indi-
cated as follows:
C C Two atoms share one pair of electrons.
If a particular carbon atom has a bond with some atom other
CC Two atoms share two pairs of electrons.
than hydrogen, the diagram shows the exception. For example,
C;C Two atoms share three pairs of electrons.
in each of the two molecules diagrammed below, one carbon
Each carbon atom ordinarily forms four covalent bonds, either has a bond with an oxygen atom, which in turn has a bond
with other carbon atoms, with hydrogen atoms, or with other with a hydrogen atom. All the bonds that are not shown are
atoms. Many biologically important compounds include long carbon–hydrogen bonds.
chains of carbon compounds linked to one another, such as:

H H H H H H H H H

H C C C OH H C C C C C C H O H O H

H H H
Figure A.5 illustrates some carbon compounds that are criti-
cal for animal life. Purines and pyrimidines form the central
Note that each carbon atom has a total of four bonds, count-
structure of DNA and RNA, the chemicals responsible for
ing each double bond as two. In some molecules, the carbon
heredity. Proteins, fats, and carbohydrates are the primary
chain loops around to form a ring:
types of fuel that the body uses. Figure A.6 displays the chem-
H H H H ical structures of seven neurotransmitters that are extensively


C C C C discussed in this text.


H H
H—C—H H—C—H H—C C—H Chemical Reactions in the Body
H H
A living organism is an immensely complicated, coordinated

C C C C
set of chemical reactions. Life requires that the rate of each


H H H H reaction be carefully regulated. In many cases one reaction

CH2OH
NH2 O O
H H H
N N CH3
N
HO OH H OH
N O H OH
N N

Adenine Thymine Glucose


(a purine) (a pyrimidine) (a carbohydrate)

H H O H O H O H O
+H N C C N C C
N C C N C C
H R H R H R H R O–

(a protein)
Figure A.5  Structures of some important
biological molecules O H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H
The R in the protein represents a point of at- C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C C H
tachment for various chains that differ from one
HO H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H H
amino acid to another. Actual proteins are much
longer than the chemical shown here. Stearic acid
(a fat)
490 Appendix A Brief, Basic Chemistry

O Adenosine

CH3 C—O—CH2 CH2N(CH3)3 Acetylcholine NH2


C
N C N
HO CH
—CH2CH2NH2 Dopamine HC C N
Phosphates
N
HO
– O– O–
O
Ribose
–O
OH P O P O P O CH2 O
HO —CHCH2NH2 Norepinephrine O O O C H H C
H C C H
HO
OH OH
OH AMP
HO —CHCH2NH—CH3 Epinephrine ADP

HO ATP
Figure A.7  ATP, composed of adenosine, ribose, and three
phosphates
ATP can lose one phosphate group to form ADP (adenosine
HO —CH2CH2NH2 Serotonin diphosphate) and then lose another one to form AMP (adenosine
(5-hydroxytryptamine)
N monophosphate). Each time it breaks off a phosphate group, it
H releases energy.

OH O
form the bonds and a large amount of energy is released when
C—CH2—CH2—CH—C Glutamate they break. ATP can break off one or two of its three phos-
OH NH3+OH phates to provide energy.

O Summary
NH2—CH2—CH2—CH2—C GABA (γ-amino-butyric acid)
1. Matter is composed of 92 elements that combine to form
OH an endless variety of compounds. 486
Figure A.6  Chemical structures of seven abundant 2. An atom is the smallest piece of an element. A molecule
­neurotransmitters is the smallest piece of a compound that maintains the
properties of the compound. 488
produces a chemical that enters into another reaction, which 3. The atoms of some elements can gain or lose an electron,
produces another chemical that enters into another reaction, thus becoming ions. Positively charged ions attract
and so forth. If any one of those reactions is too rapid com- negatively charged ions, forming an ionic bond. In some
pared to the others, the chemical it produces will accumulate cases two or more atoms may share electrons, thus
to possibly harmful levels. If a reaction is too slow, it will not forming a covalent bond. 488
produce enough product and the next reaction will be stalled. 4. The principal carrier of energy in the body is a chemical
Enzymes are proteins that control the rate of chemical called ATP. 490
reactions. Each reaction is controlled by a particular enzyme.
Enzymes are a type of catalyst. A catalyst is any chemical that
facilitates a reaction among other chemicals without being al- Terms
tered itself in the process.
atom 488
atomic number 488
The Role of ATP atomic weight 488
The body relies on ATP (adenosine triphosphate) as its ATP (adenosine triphosphate) 490
main way of sending energy where it is needed (Figure A.7). compound 486
Much of the energy derived from food goes into forming ATP covalent bond 488
molecules that eventually provide energy for the muscles and element 486
other body parts. enzyme 490
ATP consists of adenosine bound to ribose and three ion 488
phosphate groups (PO3). Phosphates form high-energy cova- ionic bond 488
lent bonds. That is, a large amount of energy is required to molecule 488
Society for Neuroscience Policies
on the Use of Animals and Human
Subjects in Neuroscience Research
B
Policy on the Use of Animals purpose of this document is to outline the policy that guides
that relationship. Compliance with the following policy will be
in Neuroscience Research an important factor in determining the suitability of research
The Policy on the Use of Animals in Neuroscience Research af- for presentation at the Annual Meeting or for publication in
fects a number of the Society for Neuroscience’s functions that The Journal of Neuroscience, and in situations where the Soci-
involve making decisions about animal research conducted by ety is asked to provide public and active support for a member
individual members. These include the scheduling of scientific whose use of animals in research has been questioned.
presentations at the Annual Meeting, the review and publica-
tion of original research papers in The Journal of Neuroscience, General Policy
and the defense of members whose ethical use of animals in
research is questioned by antivivisectionists. The responsibility Neuroscience research uses complicated, often invasive meth-
for implementing the policy in each of these areas will rest with ods, each of which is associated with different problems, risks,
the relevant administrative body (Program Committee, Publi- and specific technical considerations. An experimental method
cations Committee, Editorial Board, and Committee on Ani- that would be deemed inappropriate for one kind of research
mals in Research, respectively), in consultation with Council. may be the method of choice for another kind of research. It is
therefore impossible for the Society to define specific policies
and procedures for the care and use of all research animals and
Introduction for the design and conduct of every neuroscience experiment.
The Society for Neuroscience, as a professional society for The U.S. Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and
basic and clinical researchers in neuroscience, endorses and Use of Laboratory Animals (PHS Policy) and the Guide for the
supports the appropriate and responsible use of animals as Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (the Guide) describe a
experimental subjects. Knowledge generated by neurosci- set of general policies and procedures designed to ensure the
ence research on animals has led to important advances in humane and appropriate use of live vertebrate animals in all
the understanding of diseases and disorders that affect the forms of biomedical research. The Society finds the policies
nervous system and in the development of better treatments and procedures set forth in the PHS Policy and the Guide
that reduce suffering in humans and animals. This knowledge to be both necessary and sufficient to ensure a high standard
also makes a critical contribution to our understanding of of animal care and use and adopts them as its official “Policy
ourselves, the complexities of our brains, and what makes us on the Use of Animals in Neuroscience Research” (Society
human. Continued progress in understanding how the brain Policy). All Society members are expected to conduct their
works and further advances in treating and curing disorders of animal research in compliance with the Society Policy and are
the nervous system require investigation of complex functions required to verify that they have done so when submitting ab-
at all levels in the living nervous system. Because no adequate stracts for presentation at the Annual Meeting or manuscripts
alternatives exist, much of this research must be done on ani- for publication in The Journal of Neuroscience. Adherence to
mal subjects. The Society takes the position that neuroscien- the Society Policy is also an important step toward receiv-
tists have an obligation to contribute to this progress through ing help from the Society in responding to questions about a
responsible and humane research on animals. member’s use of animals in research. A complete description
Several functions of the Society are related to the use of of the Society’s policy and procedures for defending members
animals in research. A number of these involve decisions about whose research comes under attack is given in the Society’s
research conducted by individual members of the Society, in- Handbook for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience Research.
cluding the scheduling of scientific presentations at the Annual
Meeting, the review and publication of original research papers
in The Journal of Neuroscience, and the defense of members Local Committee Review
whose ethical use of animals in research is questioned by anti- An important element of the Society Policy is the establish-
vivisectionists. Each of these functions, by establishing explicit ment of a local committee that is charged with reviewing and
support of the Society for the research of individual members, approving all proposed animal care and use procedures. In ad-
defines a relationship between the Society and its members. The dition to scientists experienced in research involving animals
491
492 Appendix B Society for Neuroscience Policies on the Use of Animals and Human Subjects in Neuroscience Research

and a veterinarian, the membership of this local committee OPRR Public Health Service Policy on Humane Care and Use
should include an individual who is not affiliated with the of Laboratory Animals (revised Sept. 1986). Office for
member’s institution in any other way. In reviewing a pro- Protection from Research Risks, NIH, 6100 Executive
posed use of animals, the committee should evaluate the ad- Blvd., Suite 3B01-MSC 7507, Rockville, MD 20892-7507.
equacy of institutional policies, animal husbandry, veterinary Preparation and Maintenance of Higher Mammals During
care, and the physical plant. Specific attention should be paid Neuroscience Experiments. Report of a National Institutes
to proposed procedures for animal procurement, quarantine of Health Workshop. (March 1991). NIH Publication
and stabilization, separation by species, disease diagnosis and No. 91-3207, National Eye Institute, Bldg. 31, Rm. 6A47,
treatment, anesthesia and analgesia, surgery and postsurgical Bethesda, MD 20892.
care, and euthanasia. The review committee also should ensure Seventh Title of the Regulations of the General Law of
that procedures involving live vertebrate animals are designed Health, Regarding Health Research. In: Laws and Codes
and performed with due consideration of their relevance to of Mexico. (1995). Published in the Porrua Collection
human or animal health, the advancement of knowledge, or (12th updated ed., pp. 430–431). Porrua Publishers,
the good of society. This review and approval of a member’s Mexico.
use of live vertebrate animals in research by a local committee
The following principles, based largely on the PHS Policy,
is an essential component of the Society Policy. Assistance in
can be a useful guide in the design and implementation of ex-
developing appropriate animal care and use procedures and
perimental procedures involving laboratory animals.
establishing a local review committee can be obtained from
Animals selected for a procedure should be of an appro-
the documents listed here and from the Society.
priate species and quality and the minimum number required
to obtain valid results.
Other Laws, Regulations, and Policies Proper use of animals, including the avoidance or minimi-
In addition to complying with the policy described above, zation of discomfort, distress, and pain, when consistent with
Regular Members (i.e., North American residents) of the So- sound scientific practices, is imperative.
ciety must also adhere to all relevant national, state, or local Procedures with animals that may cause more than mo-
laws and/or regulations that govern their use of animals in mentary or slight pain or distress should be performed with
neuroscience research. Thus, U.S. members must observe the appropriate sedation, analgesia, or anesthesia. Surgical or
U.S. Animal Welfare Act (as amended in 1985) and its imple- other painful procedures should not be performed on unanes-
menting regulations from the U.S. Department of Agricul- thetized animals paralyzed by chemical agents.
ture. Canadian members must abide by the Guide to the Care Postoperative care of animals shall be such as to minimize
and Use of Experimental Animals, and members in Mexico discomfort and pain and, in any case, shall be equivalent to ac-
must comply with the Reglamento de la Ley General de Salud cepted practices in schools of veterinary medicine.
en Materia de Investigacion para la Salud of the Secretaria de Animals that would otherwise suffer severe or chronic
Salud (published on Jan. 6, 1987). Similarly, in addition to pain or distress that cannot be relieved should be painlessly
complying with the laws and regulations of their home coun- killed at the end of the procedure or, if appropriate, during the
tries, Foreign Members of the Society should adhere to the procedure. If the study requires the death of the animal, the
official Society Policy outlined here. animal must be killed in a humane manner.
Living conditions should be appropriate for the species
Recommended References and contribute to the animals’ health and comfort. Normally,
the housing, feeding, and care of all animals used for biomedi-
“Anesthesia and paralysis in experimental animals.” (1984).
cal purposes must be directed by a veterinarian or other scien-
Visual Neuroscience, 1, 421–426.
tist trained and experienced in the proper care, handling, and
The Biomedical Investigator’s Handbook for Researchers Using
use of the species being maintained or studied. In any case,
Animal Models. (1987). Foundation for Biomedical
appropriate veterinary care shall be provided.
Research, 818 Connecticut Ave., N.W., Suite 303,
Exceptions to these principles require careful consid-
Washington, D.C. 20006.
eration and should only be made by an appropriate review
Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (7th ed.
group such as an institutional animal care and use committee.
1996). NRC (National Research Council), Institute of
Laboratory Animal Resources, National Academy of
Sciences, 2101 Constitution Ave., N.W., Washington,
D.C. 20418.
Policy on the Use of
Guide to the Care and Use of Experimental Animals (2nd ed., Human Subjects in
vol. 1). (1993). Canadian Council on Animal Care, 350
Albert St., Suite 315, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1R 1B1.
Neuroscience Research
Handbook for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience Research. Experimental procedures involving human subjects must have
(1991). Society for Neuroscience, 11 Dupont Circle, N.W., been conducted in conformance with the policies and prin-
Suite 500, Washington, D.C. 20036. ciples contained in the Federal Policy for the Protection of
Policy on the Use of Human Subjects in Neuroscience Research 493

Human Subjects (United States Office of Science and Tech- Federal Policy for the Protection of Human Subjects; Notices
nology Policy) and in the Declaration of Helsinki. When pub- and Rules. ( June 18, 1991). Federal Register. 56 (117)
lishing a paper in The Journal of Neuroscience or submitting an 28002–28007.
abstract for presentation at the Annual Meeting, authors must http://www.apa.org/science/anguide.html
sign a statement of compliance with this policy. This Website presents the ethical guidelines adopted by
the American Psychological Association. They are largely
Recommended References similar to those of the Neuroscience Society.
Declaration of Helsinki. (Adopted in 1964 by the 18th World
Medical Assembly in Helsinki, Finland, and revised by the
29th World Medical Assembly in Tokyo in 1975.) (1984).
In A.C. Varga (Ed.), The Main Issue in Bioethics Revised
Edition. New York: Paulist Press, 1984.
References
Numbers in parentheses following entries indicate event construction and elaboration. Neuropsycho- Ahmed, I. I., Shryne, J. E., Gorski, R. A., Branch,
the chapter in which a reference is cited. logia, 45, 1363–1377. (13) B. J., & Taylor, A. N. (1991). Prenatal ethanol
Abbar, M., Couret, P., Bellivier, F., Leboyer, M., Ader, R. (2001). Psychoneuroimmunology. Current and the prepubertal sexually dimorphic nucleus
Boulenger, J. P., Castelhau, D., et al. (2001). Sui- Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 94–98. of the preoptic area. Physiology & Behavior, 49,
cide attempts and the tryptophan hydroxylase (12) 427–432. (11)
gene. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 268–273. (12) Adkins, E. K., & Adler, N. T. (1972). Hormonal Aimone, J. B., Wiles, J., & Gage, F. H. (2006). Poten-
Abbott, N. J., Rönnback, L., & Hansson, E. (2006). control of behavior in the Japanese quail. Journal tial role for adult neurogenesis in the encoding of
Astrocyte-endothelial interactions at the blood– of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 81, time in new memories. Nature Neuroscience, 9,
brain barrier. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 27–36. (11) 723–727. (5)
41–53. (2) Adler, E., Hoon, M. A., Mueller, K. L., Chan- Airaksinen, M. S., & Saarma, M. (2002). The
AbdelMalik, P., Husted, J, Chow, E. W. C., & Bas- drashekar, J., Ryba, N. J. P., & Zuker, C. S. GDNF family: Signalling, biological functions
sett, A. S. (2003). Childhood head injury and (2000). A novel family of mammalian taste re- and therapeutic value. Nature Reviews Neurosci-
expression of schizophrenia in multiply affected ceptors. Cell, 100, 693–702. (7) ence, 3, 383–394. (5)
families. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 231– Admon, R., Lubin, G., Stern, O., Rosenberg, K., Airan, R. D., Meltzer, L. A., Roy, M., Gong, Y.,
236. (15) Sela, L., Ben-Ami, H., et al. (2009). Human vul- Chen, H., & Deisseroth, K. (2007). High-speed
Abi-Dargham, A., Rodenhiser, J., Printz, D., Zea- nerability to stress depends on amygdala’s predis- imaging reveals neurophysiological links to be-
Ponce, Y., Gil, R., Kegeles, L. S., et al. (2000). position and hippocampal plasticity. Proceedings havior in an animal model of depression. Science,
Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by of the National Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.), 106, 317, 819–823. (15)
dopamine in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the 14120–14125. (12) Akbarian, S., Kim, J. J., Potkin, S. G., Hagman, J. O.,
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, 8104– Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Tranel, D. (2002). Tafazzoli, A., Bunney, W. E., Jr., et al. (1995).
8109. (15) Neural systems for recognition of emotional Gene expression for glutamic acid decarboxylase
Abrahamsen, B., Zhao, J., Asante, C. O., Cendan, prosody: A 3-D lesion study. Emotion, 2, 23–51. is reduced without loss of neurons in prefrontal
C. M., Marsh, S., Martinez-Barbera, J. P., et al. (14) cortex of schizophrenics. Archives of General Psy-
(2008). The cell and molecular basis of mechani- Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Buchanan, T. W. (2005). chiatry, 52, 258–266. (15)
cal, cold, and inflammatory pain. Science, 321, Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory Alanko, K., Santtila, P., Harlaar, N., Witting, K.,
702–705. (7) for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nature Varjonen, M., Jern, P., et al. (2010). Common ge-
Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of Neuroscience, 8, 512–518. (12) netic effects of gender atypical behavior in child-
onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, hood and sexual orientation in adulthood: A
Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. A. (1995). Fear and the human amygdala. Journal study of Finnish twins. Archives of Sexual Behav-
Language Learning, 59, 249–306. (14) of Neuroscience, 15, 5879–5891. (12) ior, 39, 81–92. (11)
Abramov, E., Dolev, I., Fogel, H., Ciccotosto, G. D., Agarwal, N., Pacher, P., Tegeder, I., Amaya, F., Con- Albanese, M.-C., Duerden, E. G., Rainville, P., &
Ruff, E., & Slutsky, I. (2009). Amyloid-b as a stantin, C. E., Brenner, G. J., et al. (2007). Can- Duncan, G. H. (2007). Memory traces of pain in
positive endogenous regulator of release proba- nabinoids mediate analgesia largely via peripher- human cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 4612–
bility at hippocampal synapses. Nature Neurosci- al type 1 cannabinoid receptors in nociceptors. 4620. (7)
ence, 12, 1567–1576. (13) Nature Neuroscience, 10, 870–879. (7) Albouy, G., Sterpenich, V., Balteau, E., Vandewalle,
Adamec, R. E., Stark-Adamec, C., & Livingston, Aglioti, S., Smania, N., Atzei, A., & Berlucchi, G. G., Desseilles, M., Dang-Vu, T., et al. (2008).
K. E. (1980). The development of predatory ag- (1997). Spatio-temporal properties of the pat- Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved
gression and defense in the domestic cat (Felis tern of evoked phantom sensations in a left index in consolidation of motor sequence memory.
catus): 3. Effects on development of hunger be- amputee patient. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, Neuron, 58, 261–272. (13)
tween 180 and 365 days of age. Behavioral and 867–872. (5) Albright, T. D., Jessell, T. M., Kandel, E. R., & Pos-
Neural Biology, 30, 435–447. (12) Aglioti, S., Smania, N., & Peru, A. (1999). Frames ner, M. I. (2001). Progress in the neural sciences
Adams, D. B., Gold, A. R., & Burt, A. D. (1978). of reference for mapping tactile stimuli in brain- in the century after Cajal (and the mysteries that
Rise in female-initiated sexual activity at ovula- damaged patients. Journal of Cognitive Neurosci- remain). Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
tion and its suppression by oral contraceptives. ence, 11, 67–79. (14) ences, 929, 11–40. (2)
New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 1145– Aglioti, S., Tassinari, G., Corballis, M. C., & Berluc- Aldrich, M. S. (1998). Diagnostic aspects of narco-
1150. (11) chi, G. (2000). Incomplete gustatory localization lepsy. Neurology, 50(Suppl. 1), S2–S7. (9)
Adams, R. B., Jr., Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., Am- as shown by analysis of taste discrimination after Aleman, A., Kahn, R. S., & Selten, J. P. (2003). Sex
bady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2003). Effects of gaze callosotomy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, differences in the risk of schizophrenia. Archives
on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. 238–245. (7, 14) of General Psychiatry, 60, 565–571. (15)
Science, 300, 1536. (12) Ahlskog, J. E., & Hoebel, B. G. (1973). Overeating Aleman, A., Swart, M., & van Rijn, S. (2008). Brain
Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of and obesity from damage to a noradrenergic sys- imaging, genetics and emotion. Biological Psychol-
direct and averted gaze on the perception of fa- tem in the brain. Science, 182, 166–169. (10) ogy, 79, 58–69. (12)
cially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5, 3–11. Ahlskog, J. E., Randall, P. K., & Hoebel, B. G. Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differ-
(12) (1975). Hypothalamic hyperphagia: Dissocia- ences in response to children’s toys in nonhuman
Adams, W., Kendell, R. E., Hare, E. H., & Munk- tion from hyperphagia following destruction of primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sebaeus). Evolu-
Jørgensen, P. (1993). Epidemiological evidence noradrenergic neurons. Science, 190, 399–401. tion and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479. (11)
that maternal influenza contributes to the aetiol- (10) Alexander, G. M., Wilcox, T., & Woods, R. (2009).
ogy of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychia- Ahmed, E. I., Zehr, J. L., Schulz, K. M., Lorenz, Sex differences in infants’ visual interest in toys.
try, 163, 522–534. (15) B. H., DonCarlos, L. L., & Sisk, C. L (2008). Pu- Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 427–433. (11)
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). bertal hormones modulate the addition of new Allen, H. L., Estrada, K., Lettre, G., Berndt, S. I.,
Remembering the past and imagining the future: cells to sexually dimorphic brain regions. Nature Weedon, M. N., Rivadeneira, F., et al. (2010).
Common and distinct neural substrates during Neuroscience, 11, 995–997. (11) Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci

494
References 495

and biological pathways affect human height. Na- Andersen, T. S., Tiippana, K., & Sams, M. (2004). in young men: Empirical findings and a meta-
ture, 467, 832–838. (1) Factors influencing audiovisual fission and fu- analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 24, 411–420. (12)
Allen, J. S., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Bruss, J., sion illusions. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, Archer, J., Graham-Kevan, N., & Davies, M. (2005).
& Zhang, W. (2003). Sexual dimorphism and 301–308. (4) Testosterone and aggression: A reanalysis of
asymmetries in the gray-white composition of the Andersson, K.-E. (2001). Pharmacology of penile Book, Starzyk, and Quinsey’s (2001) study. Ag-
human cerebrum. NeuroImage, 18, 880–894. (4) erection. Pharmacological Reviews, 53, 417– gression and Violent Behavior, 10, 241–261. (12)
Alleva, E., & Francia, N. (2009). Psychiatric vulner- 450. (11) Arnold, A. P. (2004). Sex chromosomes and brain
ability: Suggestions from animal models and role Andreasen, N. C. (1988). Brain imaging: Applica- gender. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 701–708.
of neurotrophins. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral tions in psychiatry. Science, 239, 1381–1388. (4) (1, 11)
Reviews, 33, 525–536. (5) Andreasen, N. C. (1999). A unitary model of Arnold, A. P. (2009). The organizational-activational
Allison, T., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1976). Sleep in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, hypothesis as the foundation for a unified theory
mammals: Ecological and constitutional corre- 781–787. (15) of sexual differentiation of all mammalian tissues.
lates. Science, 194, 732–734. (9) Andreasen, N. C., Swayze, V. W., II, Flaum, M., Hormones and Behavior, 55, 570–578. (11)
Almeida, O. P., Yeap, B. B., Hankey, G. J., Jamrozik, Yates, W. R., Arndt, S., & McChesney, C. Arnold, A. P., & Breedlove, S. M. (1985). Organiza-
K., & Flicker, L. (2008). Low free testosterone (1990). Ventricular enlargement in schizo- tional and activational effects of sex steroids on
concentration as a potentially treatable cause of phrenia evaluated with computed tomographic brain and behavior: A reanalysis. Hormones and
depressive symptoms in older men. Archives of scanning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, Behavior, 19, 469–498. (11)
General Psychiatry, 65, 283–289. (15) 1008–1015. (15) Arnold, S. E. (2000). Cellular and molecular neuro-
Almli, C. R., Fisher, R. S., & Hill, D. L. (1979). Lat- Andresen, J. M., Gayán, J., Cherny, S. S., Brockle- pathology of the parahippocampal region in
eral hypothalamus destruction in infant rats pro- bank, D., Alkorta-Aranburu, G., Addis, E. A., et schizophrenics. Annals of the New York Academy
duces consummatory deficits without sensory al. (2007). Replication of twelve association of Sciences, 911, 275–292. (15)
neglect or attenuated arousal. Experimental Neu- studies for Huntington’s disease residual age of Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signaling pathways
rology, 66, 146–157. (10) onset in large Venezuelan kindreds. Journal of that impair prefrontal cortex structure and
Al-Rashid, R. A. (1971). Hypothalamic syndrome Medical Genetics, 44, 45–50. (8) function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 410–
in acute childhood leukemia. Clinical Pediatrics, Andrew, D., & Craig, A. D. (2001). Spinothalamic 422. (12)
10, 53–54. (10) lamina I neurons selectively sensitive to hista- Arvidson, K., & Friberg, U. (1980). Human taste:
Altar, C. A., Laeng, P., Jurata, L. W., Brockman, J. A., mine: A central neural pathway for itch. Nature Response and taste bud number in fungiform
Lemire, A., Bullard, J., et al. (2004). Electrocon- Neuroscience, 4, 72–77. (7) papillae. Science, 209, 807–808. (7)
vulsive seizures regulate gene expression of dis- Andrews, T. J., Halpern, S. D., & Purves, D. (1997). Asarnow, J. R., Emslie, G., Clarke, G., Wagner,
tinct neurotrophic signaling pathways. Journal of Correlated size variations in human visual cortex, K. D., Spirito, A., Vitiello, B., et al. (2009). Treat-
Neuroscience, 24, 2667–2677. (15) lateral geniculate nucleus, and optic tract. Journal ment of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor-
Alter, M. D., Rubin, D. B., Ramsey, K., Halpern, R., of Neuroscience, 17, 2859–2868. (6) resistant depression in adolescents: Predictors
Stephan, D. A., Abbott, L. F., et al. (2008). Varia- Angulo, M. C., Kozlov, A. S., Charpak, S., & Audi- and moderators of treatment response. Journal of
tion in the large-scale organization of gene ex- nat, E. (2004). Glutamate released from glial the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy-
pression levels in the hippocampus relates to cells synchronizes neuronal activity in the hippo- chiatry, 48, 330–339. (15)
stable epigenetic variability in behavior. PLoS campus. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6920–6927. Ascherio, A., Weisskopf, M. G., O’Reilly, E. J., Mc-
ONE, 3, e3344. (1) (2) Cullough, M., Calle, E. E., Rodriguez, C., et al.
Alto, L. T., Howton, L. A., Conner, J. M., Hollis, Ankney, C. D. (1992). Sex differences in relative (2004). Coffee consumption, gender, and Parkin-
E. R. III, Blesch, A., & Tuszynski, M. H. (2009). brain size: The mismeasure of women, too? Intel- son’s disease mortality in the cancer prevention
Chemotropic guidance facilitates axonal regen- ligence, 16, 329–336. (4) study II cohort: The modifying effects of estro-
eration and synapse formation after spinal cord Antanitus, D. S. (1998). A theory of cortical neu- gen. American Journal of Epidemiology, 160, 977–
injury. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1106–1113. (5) ron-astrocyte interaction. Neuroscientist, 4, 154– 984. (8)
Amateau, S. K., & McCarthy, M. M. (2004). Induc- 159. (2) Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1955). Two types of
tion of PGE2 by estradiol mediates developmen- Antoniadis, E. A., Winslow, J. T., Davis, M., & ocular motility occurring in sleep. Journal of Ap-
tal masculinization of sex behavior. Nature Neu- Amaral, D. G. (2007). Role of the primate amyg- plied Physiology, 8, 1–10. (9)
roscience, 7, 643–650. (11) dala in fear-potentiated startle. Journal of Neuro- Ashmore, L. J., & Sehgal, A. (2003). A fly’s eye view
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnos- science, 27, 7386–7396. (12) of circadian entrainment. Journal of Biological
tic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th Apostolakis, E. M., Garai, J., Fox, C., Smith, C. L., Rhythms, 18, 206–216. (9)
ed.). Washington, DC: Author. (15) Watson, S. J., Clark, J. H., et al. (1996). Dopami- Aston-Jones, G., Chen, S., Zhu, Y., & Oshinsky,
Amiry-Moghaddam, M., & Ottersen, O. P. (2003). nergic regulation of progesterone receptors: M. L. (2001). A neural circuit for circadian
The molecular basis of water transport in the brain. Brain D5 dopamine receptors mediate induction regulation of arousal. Nature Neuroscience, 4,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 991–1001. (2) of lordosis by D1-like agonists in rats. Journal of 732–738. (9)
Amsterdam, J. D., Winokur, A., Dyson, W., Herzog, Neuroscience, 16, 4823–4834. (11) Athos, E. A., Levinson, B., Kistler, A., Zemansky, J.,
S., Gonzalez, F., Rott, R., et al. (1985). Borna Appolinario, J. C., Bacaltchuk, J., Sichieri, R., Claudi- Bostrom, A., Freimer, N., et al. (2007). Dichot­
disease virus. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, no, A. M., Godoy-Matos, A., Morgan, C., et al. omy and perceptual distortions in absolute pitch
1093–1096. (15) (2003). A randomized, double-blind, placebo- ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Anaclet, C., Parmentier, R., Ouk, K., Guidon, G., controlled study of sibutramine in the treatment ences, USA, 104, 14795–14800. (7)
Buda, C., Sastre, J.-P., et al. (2009). Orexin/ of binge-eating disorder. Archives of General Psy- Avena, N. M., Rada, P., & Hoebel, B. G. (2008).
hypocretin and histamine: Distinct roles in the chiatry, 60, 1109–1116. (10) Evidence for sugar addiction: Behavioral and
control of wakefulness demonstrated using Araneda, R. C., Kini, A. D., & Firestein, S. (2000). neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive
knock-out mouse models. Journal of Neurosci- The molecular receptive range of an odorant re- sugar intake. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re-
ence, 29, 14423–14438. (9) ceptor. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1248–1255. (7) views, 32, 20–39. (10)
Andersen, J. L., Klitgaard, H., & Saltin, B. (1994). Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression be- Avissar, M., Furman, A. C., Saunders, J. C., & Par-
Myosin heavy chain isoforms in single fibres tween heterosexual partners: A meta-analytic sons, T. D. (2007). Adaptation reduces spike-
from m. vastus lateralis of sprinters: Influence of review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 651–680. (12) count reliability, but not spike-timing precision,
training. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 151, Archer, J., Birring, S. S., & Wu, F. C. W. (1998). The of auditory nerve responses. Journal of Neurosci-
135–142. (8) association between testosterone and aggression ence, 27, 6461–6472. (7)
496 References

Azrin, N. H., Sisson, R. W., Meyers, R., & Godley, protects the developing female mouse brain from Basil, J. A., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., & Fite, K. V.
M. (1982). Alcoholism treatment by disulfiram masculinization and defeminization by estrogens. (1996). Differences in hippocampal volume
and community reinforcement therapy. Journal of Nature Neuroscience, 9, 220–226. (11) among food storing corvids. Brain, Behavior and
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, Bakker, J., Honda, S.-I., Harada, N., & Balthazart, J. Evolution, 47, 156–164. (13)
13, 105–112. (3) (2002). The aromatase knock-out mouse pro- Basso, A., & Rusconi, M. L. (1998). Aphasia in left-
Babich, F. R., Jacobson, A. L., Bubash, S., & Jacob- vides new evidence that estradiol is required dur- handers. In P. Coppens, Y. Lebrun, & A. Basso
son, A. (1965). Transfer of a response to naive ing development in the female for the expression (Eds.), Aphasia in atypical populations (pp. 1–34).
rats by injection of ribonucleic acid extracted of sociosexual behaviors in adulthood. Journal of Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (14)
from trained rats. Science, 149, 656–657. (13) Neuroscience, 22, 9104–9112. (11) Basterzi, A. D., Yazici, K., Aslan, E., Delialioglu,
Babiloni, C., Brancucci, A., Pizzella, V., Romani, Ballard, P. A., Tetrud, J. W., & Langston, J. W. B. T., Acar, S. T., & Yazici, A. (2008). Effects of
G. L., Tecchio, F., Torquati, K., et al. (2005). Con- (1985). Permanent human Parkinsonism due to fluoxetine and venlafaxine on serum brain de-
tingent negative variation in the parasylvian cortex 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine rived neurotrophic factor levels in depressed pa-
increases during expectancy of painful sensorimo- (MPTP). Neurology, 35, 949–956. (8) tients. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology &
tor events: A magnetoencephalographic study. Be- Ballon, J. S., Dean, K. A., & Cadenhead, K. S. biological psychiatry, 33, 281–285. (15)
havioral Neuroscience, 119, 491–502. (7) (2007). Obstetrical complications in people at Battersby, S. (1997). Plus c’est le même chews. Na-
Babkoff, H., Caspy, T., Mikulincer, M., & Sing, risk for developing schizophrenia. Schizophrenia ture, 385, 679. (10)
H. C. (1991). Monotonic and rhythmic influ- Research, 98, 307–311. (15) Baulac, S., Huberfeld, G., Gourfinkel-An, I., Mitro-
ences: A challenge for sleep deprivation research. Balthasar, N., Dalgaard, L. T., Lee, C. E., Yu, J., Fu- poulou, G., Beranger, A., Prud’homme, J.-F., et
Psychological Bulletin, 109, 411–428. (9) nahashi, H., Williams, T., et al. (2005). Diver- al. (2001). First genetic evidence of GABAA re-
Backlund, E.-O., Granberg, P.-O., Hamberger, B., gence of melanocortin pathways in the control of ceptor dysfunction in epilepsy: A mutation in the
Sedvall, G., Seiger, A., & Olson, L. (1985). food intake and energy expenditure. Cell, 123, g2-subunit gene. Nature Genetics, 28, 46–48.
Transplantation of adrenal medullary tissue to 493–505. (10) (14)
striatum in Parkinsonism. In A. Björklund & U. Banks, W. P., & Isham, E. A. (2009). We infer rather Baum, A., Gatchel, R. J., & Schaeffer, M. A. (1983).
Stenevi (Eds.), Neural grafting in the mammalian than perceive the moment we decided to act. Psy- Emotional, behavioral, and physiological effects of
CNS (pp. 551–556). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (8) chological Science, 20, 17–21. (8) chronic stress at Three Mile Island. Journal of Con-
Bäckman, J., & Alerstam, T. (2001). Confronting Barbour, D. L., & Wang, X. (2003). Contrast tuning sulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 565–582. (12)
the winds: Orientation and flight behavior of in auditory cortex. Science, 299, 1073–1075. (7) Bauman, M. D., Toscano, J. E., Mason, W. A.,
roosting swifts, Apus apus. Proceedings of the Roy- Bargary, G., Barnett, K. J., Mitchell, K. J., & Newell, Lavenex, P., & Amaral, D. G. (2006). The expres-
al Society of London. Series B—Biological Sciences, F. N. (2009). Colored-speech synaesthesia is trig- sion of social dominance following neonatal le-
268, 1081–1087. (9) gered by multisensory, not unisensory, percep- sions of the amygdala or hippocampus in rhesus
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Develop- tion. Psychological Science, 20, 529–533. (7) monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neurosci-
ments in the concept of working memory. Neuro- Barinaga, M. (1996). Finding new drugs to treat ence, 120, 749–760. (12)
psychology, 8, 485–493. (13) stroke. Science, 272, 664–664. (5) Bautista, D. M., Siemens, J., Glazer, J. M., Tsuruda,
Baer, J. S., Sampson, P. D., Barr, H. M., Connor, Barnea, G., O’Donnell, S., Mancia, F., Sun, X., P. R., Basbaum, A. I., Stucky, C. L., et al. (2007).
P. D., & Streissguth, A. P. (2003). A 21-year lon- Nemes, A., Mendelsohn, M., et al. (2004). Odor- The menthol receptor TRPM8 is the principal
gitudinal analysis of the effects of prenatal alco- ant receptors on axon termini in the brain. Sci- detector of environmental cold. Nature, 448,
hol exposure on young adult drinking. Archives of ence, 304, 1468. (7) 204–208. (7)
General Psychiatry, 60, 377–385. (3) Barnes, B. M. (1996, September/October). Sang Bautista, D. M., Sigal, Y. M., Milstein, A. D., Garrison,
Bagemihl, B. (1999). Biological exuberance. New froid. The Sciences, 36(5), 13–14. (9) J. L., Zorn, J. A., Tsuruda, P. R., et al. (2008). Pun-
York: St. Martin’s Press. (11) Barnett, K. J., Finucane, C., Asher, J. E., Bargary, G., gent agents from Szechuan peppers excite sensory
Baghdoyan, H. A., Spotts, J. L., & Snyder, S. G. Corvin, A. P., Newell, F. N., et al. (2008). Familial neurons by inhibiting two-pore potassium chan-
(1993). Simultaneous pontine and basal fore brain patterns and the origins of individual differences nels. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 772–779. (7)
microinjections of carbachol suppress REM sleep. in synaesthesia. Cognition, 106, 871–893. (7) Baxter, L. R., Phelps, M. E., Mazziotta, J. C.,
Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 229–242. (9) Barrett, L. F., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S. L., Schwartz, J. M., Gerner, R. H., Selin, C. E., et al.
Bailey, C. H., Giustetto, M., Huang, Y.-Y., Hawkins, Rauch, S. L., & Wright, C. I. (2007). The amyg- (1985). Cerebral metabolic rates for glucose in
R. D., & Kandel, E. R. (2000). Is heterosynaptic dala and the experience of affect. Social Cognitive mood disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry,
modulation essential for stabilizing Hebbian & Affective Neuroscience, 2, 73–83. (12) 42, 441–447. (15)
plasticity and memory? Nature Reviews Neurosci- Barton, D. A., Esler, M. D., Dawood, T., Lambert, Bayley, P. J., Frascino, J. C., & Squire, L. R. (2005).
ence, 1, 11–20. (13) E. A., Haikerwal, D., Brenchley, C., et al. (2008). Robust habit learning in the absence of aware-
Bailey, J. M., & Pillard, R. C. (1991). A genetic study Elevated brain serotonin turnover in patients ness and independent of the medial temporal
of male sexual orientation. Archives of General with depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, lobe. Nature, 436, 550–553. (13)
Psychiatry, 48, 1089–1096. (11) 65, 38–46. (15) Bayley, P. J., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2006).
Bailey, J. M., Pillard, R. C., Dawood, K., Miller, Barton, R. A., & Harvey, P. H. (2000). Mosaic evo- The fate of old memories after medial temporal
M. B., Farrer, L. A., Trivedi, S., et al. (1999). A lution of brain structure in mammals. Nature, lobe damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 13311–
family history study of male sexual orientation 405, 1055–1058. (4) 13317. (13)
using three independent samples. Behavior Genet- Bartoshuk, L. M. (1991). Taste, smell, and pleasure. Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (2001). Shape-coding in
ics, 29, 79–86. (11) In R. C. Bolles (Ed.), The hedonics of taste (pp. IT cells generalizes over contrast and mirror re-
Bailey, J. M., Pillard, R. C., Neale, M. C., & Agyei, Y. 15–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (7) versal but not figure-ground reversal. Nature
(1993). Heritable factors influence sexual orien- Bartoshuk, L. M., Gentile, R. L., Moskowitz, H. R., Neuroscience, 4, 937–942. (6)
tation in women. Archives of General Psychiatry, & Meiselman, H. L. (1974). Sweet taste induced Bean, B. P. (2007). The action potential in mamma-
50, 217–223. (11) by miracle fruit (Synsephalum dulcificum). Physiol- lian central neurons. Nature Reviews Neurosci-
Bailey, J. M., Willerman, L., & Parks, C. (1991). A ogy & Behavior, 12, 449–456. (7) ence, 8, 451–465. (2)
test of the maternal stress theory of human male Bartsch, T., Schönfeld, R., Müller, F. J., Alfke, K., Beaton, E. A., Schmidt, L. A., Schulkin, J., Antony,
homosexuality. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20, Leplow, B., Aldenhoff, J., et al. (2010). Focal le- M. M., Swinson, R. P., & Hall, G. B. (2008). Dif-
277–293. (11) sions of human hippocampal CA1 neurons in ferent neural responses to stranger and person-
Bakker, J., De Mees, C., Douhard, Q., Balthazart, J., Ga- transient global amnesia impair place memory. ally familiar faces in shy and bold adults. Behav-
bant, P., Szpirer, J., et al. (2006). Alpha-fetoprotein Science, 328, 1412–1415. (13) ioral Neuroscience, 122, 704–709. (12)
References 497

Beauchamp, M. S., & Ro, T. (2008). Neural sub- file of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of Berman, K. F., Torrey, E. F., Daniel, D. G., & Wein-
strates of sound-touch synesthesia after a tha- strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive berger, D. R. (1992). Regional cerebral blood
lamic lesion. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 13696– Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.), 7–29. (14) flow in monozygotic twins discordant and con-
13702. (7) Belova, M. A., Paton, J. J., Morrison, S. E., & Salz- cordant for schizophrenia. Archives of General
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A. R., & Lee, man, C. D. (2007). Expectation modulates neu- Psychiatry, 49, 927–934. (15)
G. P. (1999). Different contributions of the hu- ral responses to pleasant and aversive stimuli in Bernal, D., Donley, J. M., Shadwick, R. E., & Syme,
man amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cor- primate amygdala. Neuron, 55, 970–984. (12) D. A. (2005). Mammal-like muscles power
tex to decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience, Benedetti, F., Amanzio, M., Vighetti, S., & Asteg- swimming in a cold-water shark. Science, 437,
19, 5473–5481. (12) giano, G. (2006). The biochemical and neuroen- 1349–1352. (10)
Beck, S., Richardson, S. P., Shamin, E. A., Dang, N., docrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. Bernstein, J. J., & Gelderd, J. B. (1970). Regenera-
Schubert, M., & Hallett, M. (2008). Short intra- Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12014–12022. (7) tion of the long spinal tracts in the goldfish. Brain
cortical and surround inhibition are selectively Benedetti, F., Arduino, C., & Amanzio, M. (1999). Research, 20, 33–38. (5)
reduced during movement initiation in focal Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by Berntson, G. G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel,
hand dystonia. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, target-directed expectations of analgesia. Journal D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Amygdala contri-
10363–10369. (5) of Neuroscience, 19, 3639–3648. (7) bution to selective dimensions of emotion. So-
Becker, C., Thiébot, M.-H., Touitou, Y., Hamon, M., Benes, F. M. (1995). Is there a neuroanatomic basis cial Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 2, 123–
Cesselin, F., & Benoliel, J.-J. (2001). Enhanced for schizophrenia? An old question revisited. The 129. (12)
cortical extracellular levels of cholecystokinin-like Neuroscientist, 1, 104–115. (15) Berridge, C. W., Stellick, R. L., & Schmeichel, B. E.
material in a model of anticipation of social de- Benes, F. M., Turtle, M., Khan, Y., & Farol, P. (1994). (2005). Wake-promoting actions of medial basal
feat in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 262– Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocam- forebrain b2 receptor stimulation. Behavioral
269. (12) pal formation occurs in the human brain during Neuroscience, 119, 743–751. (9)
Becker, H. C. (1988). Effects of the imidazobenzo- childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Archives Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1995). The
diazepine Ro15-4513 on the stimulant and de- of General Psychiatry, 51, 477–484. (5) mind of an addicted brain: Neural sensitization
pressant actions of ethanol on spontaneous loco- Benoit, S. C., Air, E. L., Coolen, L. M., Strauss, R., of wanting versus liking. Current Directions in
motor activity. Life Sciences, 43, 643–650. (12) Jackman, A., Clegg, D. J., et al. (2002). The cata- Psychological Science, 4, 71–76. (3)
Becks, L., & Agrawal, A. F. (2010). Higher rates of bolic action of insulin in the brain is mediated by Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is
sex evolve in spatially heterogeneous environ- melanocortins. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact,
ments. Nature, 468, 89–92. (11) 9048–9052. (10) reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Re-
Beebe, D. W., & Gozal, D. (2002). Obstructive sleep Benschop, R. J., Godaert, G. L. R., Geenen, R., search Reviews, 28, 309–369. (3)
apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a com- Brosschot, J. F., DeSmet, M. B. M., Olff, M., et al. Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R.,
prehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway (1995). Relationships between cardiovascular & Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic
obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral and immunologic changes in an experimental memory: Bilateral damage causes impaired free
deficits. Journal of Sleep Research, 11, 1–16. (9) stress model. Psychological Medicine, 25, 323– recall of autobiographical memory. Journal of
Beehner, J. C., Bergman, T. J., Cheney, D. L., Sey- 327. (12) Neuroscience, 27, 14415–14423. (13)
farth, R. M., & Whitten, P. L. (2005). The effect Berdoy, M., Webster, J. P., & Macdonald, D. W. Berson, D. M., Dunn, F. A., & Takao, M. (2002). Pho-
of new alpha males on female stress in free- (2000). Fatal attraction in rats infected with totransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the
ranging baboons. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1211– Toxoplasma gondii. Proceedings of the Royal Society circadian clock. Science, 295, 1070–1073. (9)
1221. (12) of London, B, 267, 1591–1594. (12) Bertram, L., & Tanzi, R. E. (2008). Thirty years of
Beehner, J. C., Gesquirere, L., Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, Berenbaum, S. A. (1999). Effects of early androgens Alzheimer’s disease genetics: The implications of
D. L., Alberts, S. C., & Altmann, J. (2009). Testos- on sex-typed activities and interests in adoles- systematic meta-analyses. Nature Reviews Neuro-
terone related to age and life-history stages in male cents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hor- science, 9, 768–778. (13)
baboons and geladas. Hormones and Behavior, 56, mones and Behavior, 35, 102–110. (11) Beuming, T., Kniazeff, J., Bergmann, M. L., Shi, L.,
472–480. (12) Berenbaum, S. A., Duck, S. C., & Bryk, K. (2002). Gracia, L., Raniszewska, K., et al. (2008). The
Beeman, M. J., & Chiarello, C. (1998). Complemen- Behavioral effects of prenatal versus postnatal an- binding sites for cocaine and dopamine in the do-
tary right- and left-hemisphere language compre- drogen excess in children with 21-hydroxylase- pamine transporter overlap. Nature Neuroscience,
hension. Current Directions in Psychological Sci- deficient congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal 11, 780–789. (3)
ence, 7, 2–8. (14) of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 85, 727– Bevilacqua, L., Doly, S., Kaprio, J., Yuan, Q., Tik-
Behl, C. (2002). Oestrogen as a neuroprotective 733. (11) kanen, R., Paunio, T., et al. (2010). A population-
hormone. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 433– Berger, R. J., & Phillips, N. H. (1995). Energy con- specific HTR2B stop codon predisposes to se-
442. (14) servation and sleep. Behavioural Brain Research, vere impulsivity. Nature, 468, 1061–1066. (12)
Behrens, M., Foerster, S., Staehler, F., Raguse, J.-D., 69, 65–73. (9) Biben, M. (1979). Predation and predatory play be-
& Meyerhof, W. (2007). Gustatory expression Berger-Sweeney, J., & Hohmann, C. F. (1997). Be- haviour of domestic cats. Animal Behaviour, 27,
pattern of the human TAS2R bitter receptor havioral consequences of abnormal cortical de- 81–94. (12)
gene family reveals a heterogenous population of velopment: Insights into developmental disabili- Biernaskie, J., Chernenko, G., & Corbett, D. (2004).
bitter responsive taste receptor cells. Journal of ties. Behavioural Brain Research, 86, 121–142. (5) Efficacy of rehabilitative experience declines with
Neuroscience, 27, 12630–12640. (7) Bergmann, O., Bhardwaj, R. D., Bernard, S., time after focal ischemic brain injury. Journal of
Békésy, G.—See von Békésy, G. Zdunek, S., Barnabé-Heider, F., Walsh, S., et al. Neuroscience, 24, 1245–1254. (5)
Belforte, J. E., Zsiros, V., Sklar, E. R., Jiang, Z., Yu, (2009). Evidence for cardiomyocyte renewal in Bierut, L. J., Heath, A. C., Bucholz, K. K., Dinwiddie,
G., Li, Y., et al. (2010). Postnatal NMDA recep- humans. Science, 324, 98–102. (5) S. H., Madden, P. A. F., Statham, D. J., et al. (1999).
tor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers Berlin, H. A., Rolls, E. T., & Kischka, U. (2004). Major depressive disorder in a community-based
schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Nature Neurosci- Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and rein- twin sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56,
ence, 13, 76–83. (15) forcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofron- 557–563. (15)
Bella, S. D., Giguere, J. F., & Peretz, I. (2009). Sing- tal cortex lesions. Brain, 127, 1108–1126. (12) Bilgüvar, K., Öztürk, A. K., Louvi, A., Kwan, K. Y.,
ing in congenital amusia. Journal of the Acoustical Berlucchi, G., Mangun, G. R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. Choi, M., Tatli, B., et al. (2010). Whole-exome
Society of America, 126, 414–424. (7) (1997). Visuospatial attention and the split sequencing identifies recessive WDR62 muta-
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & brain. News in Physiological Sciences, 12, 226– tions in severe brain malformations. Nature, 467,
St George, M. (2000). I. The neurocognitive pro- 231. (14) 207–210. (5)
498 References

Billington, C. J., & Levine, A. S. (1992). Hypotha- Bochukova, E. G., Huang, N., Keogh, J., Henning, Booth, W., Johnson, D. H., Moore, S., Schal, C., &
lamic neuropeptide Y regulation of feeding and E., Purmann, C., Blaszczyk, K., et al. (2010). Vargo, E. L. (2011). Evidence for viable, non-
energy metabolism. Current Opinion in Neurobi- Large, rare chromosomal deletions associated clonal but fatherless Boa constrictors. Biology
ology, 2, 847–851. (10) with severe early-onset obesity. Nature, 463, Letters, 7, 253–256. (11)
Bimler, D., & Kirkland, J. (2009). Colour-space dis- 666–670. (10) Borgland, S. L., Chang, S.-J., Bowers, M. S.,
tortion in women who are heterozygous for colour Bocklandt, S., Horvath, S., Vilain, E., & Hamer, Thompson, J. L., Vittoz, N., Floresco, S. B., et al.
deficiency. Vision Research, 49, 536–543. (6) D. H. (2006). Extreme skewing of X chromo- (2009). Orexin A/hypocretin-1 selectively pro-
Binder, G. K., & Griffin, D. E. (2001). Interfer­on- some inactivation in mothers of homosexual motes motivation for positive reinforcers. Journal
g-mediated site-specific clearance of alphavirus men. Human Genetics, 118, 691–694. (11) of Neuroscience, 29, 11215–11225. (10)
from CNS neurons. Science, 293, 303–306. (2) Bode, L., Ferszt, R., & Czech, G. (1993). Borna Borodinsky, L. N., Root, C. M., Cronin, J. A., Sann,
Bird, A. (2007). Perceptions of epigenetics. Nature, disease virus infection and affective disorders S. B., Gu, X., & Spitzer, N. C. (2004). Activity-
447, 396–398. (1) in man. Archives of Virology (Suppl. 7), 159– dependent homeostatic specification of transmit-
Björnsdotter, M., Löken, L., Olausson, H., Vallbo, 167. (15) ter expression in embryonic neurons. Nature,
Å., & Wessberg, J. (2009). Somatotopic organi- Bode, L., & Ludwig, H. (1997). Clinical similarities 429, 523–530. (3)
zation of gentle touch processing in the posterior and close genetic relationship of human and ani- Borsutzky, S., Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., & Markow-
insular cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9314– mal Borna disease virus. Archives of Virology itsch, H. J. (2008). Confabulations in alcoholic
9320. (7) (Suppl. 13), 167–182. (15) Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia, 46, 3133–
Blackless, M., Charuvastra, A., Derryck, A., Fausto- Bode, L., Riegel, S., Lange, W., & Ludwig, H. 3143. (13)
Sterling, A., Lauzanne, K., & Lee, E. (2000). (1992). Human infections with Borna disease Bortolotti, B., Menchetti, M., Bellini, F., Montagu-
How sexually dimorphic are we? Review and virus: Seroprevalence in patients with chronic ti, M. B., & Berardi, D. (2008). Psychological
synthesis. American Journal of Human Biology, diseases and healthy individuals. Journal of Medi- interventions for major depression in primary
12, 151–166. (11) cal Virology, 36, 309–315. (15) care: A meta-analytic review of randomized
Blackwell, A., & Bates, E. (1995). Inducing agram- Boettiger, C. A., Mitchell, J. M., Tavares, V. C., Robert- controlled trials. General Hospital Psychiatry,
matic profiles in normals: Evidence for the selec- son, M., Joslyn, G., D’Esposito, M., et al. (2007). 30, 293–302. (15)
tive vulnerability of morphology under cognitive Immediate reward bias in humans: Fronto-parietal Bos, P. A., Terburg, D., & van Honk, J. (2010). Tes-
resource limitation. Journal of Cognitive Neurosci- networks and a role for the catechol-O-methyl- tosterone decreases trust in socially naïve hu-
ence, 7, 228–257. (14) transferase 158val/val genotype. Journal of Neuro- mans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Blake, R., & Hirsch, H. V. B. (1975). Deficits in bin- science, 27, 14383–14391. (3) ences (U.S.A.), 107, 9991–9995. (11)
ocular depth perception in cats after alternating Bogaert, A. F. (2003a). The interaction of fraternal Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and
monocular deprivation. Science, 190, 1114– birth order and body size in male sexual orienta- environmental influences on human psychological
1116. (6) tion. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 381–384. (11) differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54, 4–45. (1)
Blake, R., Palmeri, T. J., Marois, R., & Kim, C.-Y. Bogaert, A. F. (2003b). Number of older brothers Boucsein, K., Weniger, G., Mursch, K., Steinhoff,
(2005). On the perceptual reality of synesthetic and sexual orientation: New tests and the attrac- B. J., & Irle, E. (2001). Amygdala lesion in tem-
color. In L. C. Robertson & N. Sagiv (Eds.), Syn- tion/behavior distinction in two national proba- poral lobe epilepsy subjects impairs associative
esthesia (pp. 47–73). Oxford, England: Oxford bility samples. Journal of Personality and Social learning of emotional facial expressions. Neuro-
University Press. (7) Psychology, 84, 644–652. (11) psychologia, 39, 231–236. (12)
Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. Bogaert, A. F. (2006). Biological versus nonbiologi- Bourque, C. W. (2008). Central mechanisms of os-
(1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle cal older brothers and men’s sexual orientation. mosensation and systemic osmoregulation. Na-
sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 635–640. (7) Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, ture Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 519–531. (10)
Blankenburg, F., Ruff, C. C., Deichmann, R., Rees, USA, 103, 10771–10774. (11) Boutrel, B., Franc, B., Hen, R., Hamon, M., &
G., & Driver, J. (2006). The cutaneous rabbit il- Bogaert, A. F. (2010). Physical development and Adrien, J. (1999). Key role of 5-HT1B receptors
lusion affects human primary sensory cortex so- sexual orientation in men and women: An analy- in the regulation of paradoxical sleep as evi-
matotopically. PLoS Biology, 4, 459–466. (7) sis of NATSAL-2000. Archives of Sexual Behav- denced in 5-HT1B knock-out mice. Journal of
Bliss, T. V. P., & Lømo, T. (1973). Long-lasting po- ior, 39, 110–116. (11) Neuroscience, 19, 3204–3212. (9)
tentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate Boivin, D. B., Duffy, J. F., Kronauer, R. E., & Bowles, S. (2006). Group competition, reproductive
area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimu- Czeisler, C. A. (1996). Dose-response relation- leveling, and the evolution of human altruism.
lation of the perforant path. Journal of Physiology ships for resetting of human circadian clock by Science, 314, 1569–1572. (1)
(London), 232, 331–356. (13) light. Nature, 379, 540–542. (9) Bowles, S., & Posel, B. (2005). Genetic related-
Bloch, G. J., & Mills, R. (1995). Prepubertal testos- Bonath, B., Noesselt, T., Martinez, A., Mishra, J., ness predicts South African migrant workers’
terone treatment of neonatally gonadectomized Schwiecker, K., Heinze, H.-J., et al. (2007). Neu- remittances to their families. Nature, 434,
male rats: Defeminization and masculinization ral basis of the ventriloquist illusion. Current Bi- 380–383. (1)
of behavioral and endocrine function in adult- ology, 17, 1697–1703. (4) Bowmaker, J. K. (1998). Visual pigments and mo-
hood. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 59, Bonifati, V., Rizzu, P., van Baren, M. J., Schaap, O., lecular genetics of color blindness. News in Physi-
187–200. (11) Breedlove, G. J., Krieger, E., et al. (2003). Muta- ological Sciences, 13, 63–69. (6)
Bloch, G. J., Mills, R., & Gale, S. (1995). Prepuber- tions in the DJ-x gene associated with autosomal Bowmaker, J. K., & Dartnall, H. J. A. (1980). Vi-
tal testosterone treatment of female rats: Defem- recessive early-onset Parkinsonism. Science, 299, sual pigments of rods and cones in a human
inization of behavioral and endocrine function in 256–259. (8) retina. Journal of Physiology (London), 298,
adulthood. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re- Bonneh, Y. S., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). 501–511. (6)
views, 19, 177–186. (11) Motion-induced blindness in normal observers. Branco, T., Clark, B. A., & Häusser, M. (2010).
Bloch, M., Schmidt, P. J., Danaceau, M., Murphy, J., Nature, 411, 798–801. (14) Dendritic discrimination of temporal input se-
Nieman, L., & Rubinow, D. R. (2000). Effects of Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, quences in cortical neurons. Science, 329, 1671–
gonadal steroids in women with a history of M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video 1675. (3)
postpartum depression. American Journal of Psy- game playing on attention, memory, and executive Brandt, T. (1991). Man in motion: Historical and
chiatry, 157, 924–930. (15) control. Acta Psychologia, 129, 387–398. (514) clinical aspects of vestibular function. Brain, 114,
Bobrow, D., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Is male homo- Booth, F. W., & Neufer, P. D. (2005, January/Febru- 2159–2174. (7)
sexuality maintained via kin selection? Evolution ary). Exercise controls gene expression. American Brans, R. G. H., Kahn, R. S., Schnack, H. G., van
and Human Behavior, 22, 361–368. (11) Scientist, 93, 28–35. (8) Baal, G. C. M., Posthuma, D., van Haren, N. E.
References 499

M., et al. (2010). Brain plasticity and intellectual Brown, A. S., Begg, M. D., Gravenstein, S., Schaefer, rus and risk of psychosis among adult offspring.
ability are influenced by shared genes. Journal of C. A., Wyatt, R. J., Bresnahan, M., et al. (2004). Biological Psychiatry, 63, 809–815. (15)
Neuroscience, 30, 5519–5524. (5) Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the Buka, S. L., Tsuang, M. T., Torrey, E. F., Klebanoff,
Brasted, P. J., Watts, C., Robbins, T. W., & Dunnett, etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General M. A., Bernstein, D., & Yolken, R. H. (2001).
S. B. (1999). Associative plasticity in striatal Psychiatry, 61, 774–780. (15) Maternal infections and subsequent psychosis
transplants. Proceedings of the National Academy Brown, A. S., Cohen, P., Harkavy-Friedman, J., Ba- among offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry,
of Sciences, USA, 96, 10524–10529. (8) bulas, V., Malaspina, D., Gorman, J. M., et al. 58, 1032–1037. (15)
Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Guadry, (2001). Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormali- Bulbena, A., Gago, J., Martin-Santos, R., Porta, M.,
F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., et al. (1998). Disso- ties, and adult schizophrenia. Biological Psychia- Dasquens, J., & Berrios, G. E. (2004). Anxiety
ciated pattern of activity in visual cortices and try, 49, 473–486. (15) disorder & joint laxity: A definitive link. Neurol-
their projections during human rapid eye move- Brown, C. E., Li, P., Boyd, J. D., Delaney, K. R., & ogy, Psychiatry and Brain Research, 11, 137–140.
ment sleep. Science, 279, 91–95. (8, 9) Murphy, T. H. (2007). Extensive turnover of (12)
Braus, H. (1960). Anatomie des Menschen, 3. dendritic spines and vascular remodeling in cor- Bulbena, A., Gago, J., Sperry, L., & Bergé, D. (2006).
Band: Periphere Leistungsbahnen II. Centrales tical tissues recovering from stroke. Journal of The relationship between frequency and intensi-
Nervensystem, Sinnesorgane. 2. Auflage [Hu- Neuroscience, 27, 4101–4109. (5) ty of fears and a collagen condition. Depression
man anatomy: Vol. 3. Peripheral pathways II. Brown, G. L., Ebert, M. H., Goyer, P. F., Jimerson, and Anxiety, 23, 412–417. (12)
Central nervous system, sensory organs (2nd D. C., Klein, W. J., Bunney, W. E., et al. (1982). Bundgaard, M. (1986). Pathways across the verte-
ed.)]. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. (4) Aggression, suicide, and serotonin: Relationships brate blood–brain barrier: Morphological view-
Breiter, H. C., Aharon, I., Kahneman, D., Dale, A., & of CSF amine metabolites. American Journal of points. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional imaging of neural Psychiatry, 139, 741–746. (12) ences, 481, 7–19. (2)
responses to expectancy and experience of mone- Brown, J., Babor, T. F., Litt, M. D., & Kranzler, Bundy, H., Stahl, B. H., & MacCabe, J. H. (2011).
tary gains and losses. Neuron, 30, 619–639. (3) H. R. (1994). The type A/type B distinction. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fer-
Bremmer, F., Kubischik, M., Hoffmann, K.-P., & Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 708, tility of patients with schizophrenia and their
Krekelberg, B. (2009). Neural dynamics of sac- 23–33. (3) unaffected relatives. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavi-
cadic suppression. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, Brown, J. R., Ye, H., Bronson, R. T., Dikkes, P., & ca, 123, 98–106. (15)
12374–12383. (6) Greenberg, M. E. (1996). A defect in nurturing Burman, D. D., Lie-Nemeth, T., Brandfonbrener, A.
Brewer, W. J., Wood, S. J., Pantelis, C., Berger, G. E., in mice lacking the immediate early gene fos B. G., Parisi, T., & Meyer, J. R. (2009). Altered fin-
Copolov, D. L., & McGorry, P. D. (2007). Olfac- Cell, 86, 297–309. (11) ger representations in sensorimotor cortex of
tory sensitivity through the course of psychosis: Brown, T. P., Rumsby, P. C., Capleton, A. C., Rush- musicians with focal dystonia: Precentral cortex.
Relationships to olfactory identification, symp- ton, L., & Levy, L. S. (2006). Pesticides and Par- Brain Imaging and Behavior, 3, 10–23. (5)
tomatology and the schizophrenia odour. Psychi- kinson’s disease—Is there a link? Environmental Burr, D. C., Morrone, M. C., & Ross, J. (1994). Se-
atry Research, 149, 97–104. (15) Health Perspectives, 114, 156–164. (8) lective suppression of the magnocellular visual
Breysse, N., Carlsson, T., Winkler, C., Björklund, Bruck, M., Cavanagh, P., & Ceci, S. J. (1991). Forty- pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature,
A., & Kirik, D. (2007). The functional impact of something: Recognizing faces at one’s 25th re- 371, 511–513. (6)
the intrastriatal dopamine neuron grafts in Par- union. Memory & Cognition, 19, 221–228. (6) Burrell, B. (2004). Postcards from the brain museum.
kinsonian rats is reduced with advancing disease. Brummelte, S., & Galea, L. A. M. (2010). Depres- New York: Broadway Books. (4)
Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 5849–5856. (8) sion during pregnancy and postpartum: Contri- Burton, H., Snyder, A. Z., Conturo, T. E., Akbudak,
Bridge, H., Thomas, O., Jbabdi, S., & Cowey, A. bution of stress and ovarian hormones. Progress E., Ollinger, J. M., & Raichle, M. E. (2002).
(2008). Changes in connectivity after visual cor- in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychia- Adaptive changes in early and late blind: A fMRI
tical brain damage underlie altered visual func- try, 34, 766–776. (15) study of Braille reading. Journal of Neurophysiol-
tion. Brain, 131, 1433–1444. (6) Brunet, A., Orr, S. P., Tremblay, J., Robertson, K., ogy, 87, 589–607. (5)
Bridge, J. A., Birmaher, B., Iyengar, S., Barbe, R. P., Nader, K., & Pitman, R. K. (2008). Effect of Burton, R. F. (1994). Physiology by numbers. Cam-
& Brent, D. A. (2009). Placebo response in ran- post-retrieval propanolol on psychophysiologic bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. (10)
domized controlled trials of antidepressants for responding during subsequent script-driven Busche, M. A., Eichhoff, G., Adelsberger, H.,
pediatric major depressive disorder. American traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress dis- Abramowski, D., Wiederhold, K.-H., Haass, C.,
Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 42–49. (15) order. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 503– et al. (2008). Clusters of hyperactive neurons near
Bridgeman, B., & Staggs, D. (1982). Plasticity in 506. (12) amyloid plaques in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s
human blindsight. Vision Research, 22, 1199– Bruno, R. M., & Sakmann, B. (2006). Cortex is disease. Science, 321, 1686–1689. (13)
1203. (6) driven by weak but synchronously active thalamo- Buschman, T. J., & Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down
Brisbare-Roch, C., Dingemanse, J., Koberstein, R., cortical synapses. Science, 312, 1622–1627. (3) versus bottom-up control of attention in the pre-
Hoever, P., Aissoui, H., Flores, S., et al. (2007). Buchen, L. (2010). Illuminating the brain. Nature, frontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science,
Promotion of sleep by targeting the orexin sys- 465, 26–28. (4) 315, 1860–1862. (14)
tem in rats, dogs and humans. Nature Medicine, Buck, L., & Axel, R. (1991). A novel multigene fam- Buss, D. M. (1994). The strategies of human mat-
13, 150–155. (9) ily may encode odorant receptors: A molecular ing. American Scientist, 82, 238–249. (1)
Brody, A. L., Saxena, S., Stoesssel, P., Gillies, L. A., basis for odor recognition. Cell, 65, 175–187. (7) Buss, D. M. (2000). Desires in human mating. An-
Fairbanks, L. A., Alborzian, S., et al. (2001). Re- Buckholtz, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Cowan, R. L., nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907,
gional brain metabolic changes in patients with Woodward, N. D., Li, R., Ansari, M. S., et al. 39–49. (11)
major depression treated with either paroxetine (2010). Dopaminergic network differences in Buss, D. M. (2001). Cognitive biases and emotional
or interpersonal therapy. Archives of General Psy- human impulsivity. Science, 329, 532. (3) wisdom in the evolution of conflict between the
chiatry, 58, 631–640. (15) Buell, S. J., & Coleman, P. D. (1981). Quantitative sexes. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Brooks, D. C., & Bizzi, E. (1963). Brain stem elec- evidence for selective dendritic growth in normal 10, 219–223. (11)
trical activity during deep sleep. Archives Ital- human aging but not in senile dementia. Brain Bussiere, J. R., Beer, T. M., Neiss, M. B., & Janowsky,
iennes de Biologie, 101, 648–665. (9) Research, 214, 23–41. (5) J. S. (2005). Androgen deprivation impairs mem-
Brouwer, G. J., & Heeger, D. J. (2009). Decoding Buka, S. L., Cannon, T. D., Torrey, E. F., Yolken, ory in older men. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119,
and reconstructing color from responses in hu- R. H., & Collaborative Study Group on the Pre- 1429–1437. (11)
man visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, natal Origins of Severe Psychiatric Disorders. Butler, M. P., Turner, K. W., Park, J. H., Schoomer,
13992–14003. (6) (2008). Maternal exposure to herpes simplex vi- E. E., Zucker, I., & Gorman, M. R. (2010). Sea-
500 References

sonal regulation of reproduction: Altered role of experience of disgust following brain injury. Na- perience moderate to intense pain. Nature, 392,
melatonin under naturalistic conditions in ham- ture Neuroscience, 3, 1077–1078. (12) 390–394. (7)
sters. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, Caldwell, J. A., Mu, Q., Smith, J. K., Mishory, A., Capela, J. P., Carmo, H., Remião, F., Bastos, M. L.,
2867–2874. (9) Caldwell, J. L., Peters, G., et al. (2005). Are indi- Meisel, A., & Carvalho, F. (2009). Molecular and
Buxbaum, L. J. (2006). On the right (and left) vidual differences in fatigue vulnerability related cellular mechanisms of ecstasty-induced neuro-
track: Twenty years of progress in studying to baseline differences in cortical activation? Be- toxicity: An overview. Molecular Neurobiology,
hemispatial neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, havioral Neuroscience, 119, 694–707. (9) 39, 210–271. (3)
23, 184–201. (14) Calignano, A., LaRana, G., Giuffrida, A., & Pio- Cardno, A. G., Marshall, E. J., Coid, B., Macdonald,
Byl, N. N., McKenzie, A., & Nagarajan, S. S. (2000). melli, D. (1998). Control of pain initiation by A. M., Ribchester, T. R., Davies, N. J., et al. (1999).
Differences in somatosensory hand organization endogenous cannabinoids. Nature, 394, 277– Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders. Ar-
in a healthy flutist and a flutist with focal hand 281. (3) chives of General Psychiatry, 56, 162–168. (15)
dystonia: A case report. Journal of Hand Therapy, Cameron, H. A., & McKay, R. D. G. (1999). Re- Cardoso, F. L. (2009). Recalled sex-typed behavior
13, 302–309. (5) storing production of hippocampal neurons in in childhood and sports’ preferences in adult-
Byne, W., Tobet, S., Mattiace, L. A., Lasco, M. S., old age. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 894–897. (12) hood of heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual
Kemether, E., Edgar, M. A., et al. (2001). The Cameron, N. M., Champagne, F. A., Parent, C., men from Brazil, Turkey, and Thailand. Archives
interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypo- Fish, E. W., Ozaki-Kuroda, K., & Meaney, M. J. of Sexual Behavior, 38, 726–736. (11)
thalamus: An investigation of variation with sex, (2005). The programming of individual differ- Carlsson, A. (2001). A paradigm shift in brain re-
sexual orientation, and HIV status. Hormones ences in defensive responses and reproductive search. Science, 294, 1021–1024. (3)
and Behavior, 40, 86–92. (11) strategies in the rat through variations in mater- Carpenter, G. A., & Grossberg, S. (1984). A neural
Byrne, M., Agerbo, E., Ewald, H., Eaton, W. W., & nal care. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, theory of circadian rhythms: Aschoff ’s rule in
Mortensen, P. B. (2003). Parental age and risk of 29, 843–865. (5) diurnal and nocturnal mammals. American Jour-
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, Camp, N. J., Lowry, M. R., Richards, R. L., Plenk, nal of Physiology, 247, R1067–R1082. (9)
673–678. (15) A. M., Carter, C., Hensel, C. H., et al. (2005). Carreiras, M., Seghier, M. L., Baquero, S., Estévez,
Cabeza, R., Anderson, N. D., Locantore, J. K., & Genome-wide linkage analyses of extended A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T., et al. (2009). An
McIntosh, A. R. (2002). Aging gracefully: Com- Utah pedigrees identifies loci that influence re- anatomical signature for literacy. Nature, 461,
pensatory brain activity in high-performing older current, early-onset major depression and anxi- 983–986. (5)
adults. NeuroImage, 17, 1394–1402. (5) ety disorders. American Journal of Medical Ge- Carruth, L. L., Reisert, I., & Arnold, A. P. (2002). Sex
Caccappolo-van Vliet, E., Miozzo, M., & Stern, Y. netics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 135B, chromosome genes directly affect brain sexual dif-
(2004). Phonological dyslexia: A test case for 85–93. (15) ferentiation. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 933–934. (11)
reading models. Psychological Science, 15, 583– Campbell, S. S., & Tobler, I. (1984). Animal sleep: Carter, C. S. (1992). Hormonal influences on human
590. (14) A review of sleep duration across phylogeny. sexual behavior. In J. B. Becker, S. M. Breedlove, &
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Wood- Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 8, 269– D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinology (pp.
worth, G., & Stewart, M. A. (1995). Genetic- 300. (9) 131–142). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (11)
environmental interaction in the genesis of ag- Camperio-Ciani, A., Corna, F., & Capiluppi, C. Cash, S. S., Halgren, E., Dehghani, N., Rosssetti, A.
gressivity and conduct disorders. Archives of (2004). Evidence for maternally inherited factors O., Thesen, T., Wang, C. M., et al. (2009). The
General Psychiatry, 52, 916–924. (12) favouring male homosexuality and promoting human K-complex represents an isolated cortical
Cahill, L. (2006). Why sex matters for neuroscience. female fecundity. Proceedings of the Royal Society down-state. Science, 324, 1084–1087. (9)
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 477–484. (11) of London, B, 271, 2217–2221. (11) Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin,
Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of Campfield, L. A., Smith, F. J., Guisez, Y., Devos, R., J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in
emotional arousal and lasting declarative memo- & Burn, P. (1995). Recombinant mouse OB pro- the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Sci-
ry. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 294–299. (13) tein: Evidence for a peripheral signal linking adi- ence, 297, 851–854. (12)
Cahn, W., Hulshoff, H. E., Lems, E. B. T. E., van posity and central neural networks. Science, 269, Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A.,
Haren, N. E. M., Schnack, H. G., van der Lin- 546–552. (10) Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influ-
den, J. A., et al. (2002). Brain volume changes in Canal, C. E., & Gold, P. E. (2007). Different tempo- ence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a
first-episode schizophrenia. Archives of General ral profiles of amnesia after intra-hippocampus polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301,
Psychiatry, 59, 1002–1010. (15) and intra-amygdala infusions of anisomycin. Be- 386–389. (15)
Cai, D. J., Mednick, S. A., Harrison, E. M., Kanady, havioral Neuroscience, 121, 732–741. (13) Cassia, V. M., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a
J. C., & Mednick, S. C. (2009). REM, not incu- Canepari, M., Rossi, R., Pellegrino, M. A., Orell, nonspecific bias toward top-heavy patterns ex-
bation, improves creativity by priming associative R. W., Cobbold, M., Harridge, S., et al. (2005). plain newborns’ face preference? Psychological Sci-
networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Effects of resistance training on myosin function ence, 15, 379–383. (6)
Sciences (U.S.A.), 106, 10130–10134. (9) studies by the in vitro motility assay in young and Castellucci, V. F., Pinsker, H., Kupfermann, I., & Kan-
Cai, D. J., & Rickard, T. C. (2009). Reconsidering older men. Journal of Applied Physiology, 98, del, E. (1970). Neuronal mechanisms of habitua-
the role of sleep for motor memory. Behavioral 2390–2395. (8) tion and dishabituation of the gill-withdrawal re-
Neuroscience, 123, 1153–1157. (9) Cannon, M., Jones, P. B., & Murray, R. M. (2002). flex in Aplysia. Science, 167, 1745–1748. (13)
Cai, D. J., Shuman, T., Gorman, M. R., Sage, J. R., & Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: His- Castner, S. A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2004). En-
Anagnostaras, S. G. (2009). Sleep selectively en- torical and meta-analytic review. American Jour- hancement of working memory in aged monkeys
hances hippocampus-dependent memory in nal of Psychiatry, 159, 1080–1092. (15) by a sensitizing regimen of dopamine D1 recep-
mice. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 713–719. (9) Cannon, W. B. (1929). Organization for physiologi- tor stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 24,
Cajal, S. R. (1937). Recollections of my life. Mem- cal homeostasis. Physiological Reviews, 9, 399– 1446–1450. (13)
oirs of the American Philosophical Society, 8. 431. (10) Castrén, E., & Rantamäki, T. (2010). The role of
(Original work published 1901–1917) (2) Cannon, W. B. (1945). The way of an investigator. BDNF and its receptors in depression and anti-
Caldara, R., & Seghier, M. L. (2009). The fusiform New York: Norton. (inside cover) depressant drug action: Reactivation of develop-
face area responds automatically to statistical Cao, M., & Guilleminault, C. (2010). Families with mental plasticity. Developmental Neurobiology,
regularities optimal for face categorization. Hu- sleepwalking. Sleep Medicine, 11, 726–734. (9) 70, 289–296. (15)
man Brain Mapping, 30, 1615–1625. (6) Cao, Y. Q., Mantyh, P. W., Carlson, E. J., Gillespie, Catalano, S. M., & Shatz, C. J. (1998). Activity-
Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manes, F., Antoun, N., & A.-M., Epstein, C. J., & Basbaum, A. I. (1998). dependent cortical target selection by thalamic
Young, A. W. (2000). Impaired recognition and Primary afferent tachykinins are required to ex- axons. Science, 281, 559–562. (5)
References 501

Catania, K. C. (2006). Underwater “sniffing” by semi- Chaudhari, N., Landin, A. M., & Roper, S. D. Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy. Cam-
aquatic mammals. Nature, 444, 1024–1025. (7) (2000). A metabotropic glutamate receptor vari- bridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. (1)
Catchpole, C. K., & Slater, P. J. B. (1995). Bird song: ant functions as a taste receptor. Nature Neurosci- Cicone, N., Wapner, W., Foldi, N. S., Zurif, E., &
Biological themes and variations. Cambridge, Eng- ence, 3, 113–119. (7) Gardner, H. (1979). The relation between ges-
land: Cambridge University Press. (1) Chaudhri, N., Sahuque, L. L., & Janak, P. H. ture and language in aphasic communication.
Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Senso- (2009). Ethanol seeking triggered by environ- Brain and Language, 8, 342–349. (14)
rimotor learning configures the human mirror mental context is attenuated by blocking dopa- Clahsen, H., & Almazen, M. (1998). Syntax and
system. Current Biology, 17, 1527–1531. (8) mine D1 receptors in the nucleus accumbens morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition,
Catterall, W. A. (1984). The molecular basis of neu- core and shell in rats. Psychopharmacology, 207, 68, 167–198. (14)
ronal excitability. Science, 223, 653–661. (2) 303–314. (3) Clark, D. A., Mitra, P. P., & Wang, S. S.-H. (2001).
Censits, D. M., Ragland, J. D., Gur, R. C., & Gur, Chee, M. J. S., Myers, M. G. Jr., Price, C. J., & Colm- Scalable architecture in mammalian brains. Na-
R. E. (1997). Neuropsychological evidence sup- ers, W. F. (2010). Neuropeptide Y suppresses ture, 411, 189–193. (4)
porting a neurodevelopmental model of schizo- anorexigenic output from the ventromedial nu- Clark, R. E., Broadbent, N. J., & Squire, L. R. (2007).
phrenia: A longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Re- cleus of the hypothalamus. Journal of Neurosci- The hippocampus and spatial memory: Findings
search, 24, 289–298. (15) ence, 30, 3380–3390. (10) with a novel modification of the water maze. Jour-
Cepeda-Benito, A., Davis, K. W., Reynoso, J. T., & Chee, M. W. L., Tan, J. C., Zheng, H., Parimal, S., nal of Neuroscience, 27, 6647–6654. (13)
Harraid, J. H. (2005). Associative and behavioral Weissman, D. H., Zagorodnov, V., et al. (2008). Clark, R. E., & Lavond, D. G. (1993). Reversible
tolerance to the analgesic effects of nicotine in Lapsing during sleep deprivation is associated lesions of the red nucleus during acquisition
rats: Tail-flick and paw-lick assays. Psychophar- with distributed changes in brain activation. Jour- and retention of a classically conditioned behav-
macology, 180, 224–233. (3) nal of Neuroscience, 28, 5519–5528. (9) ior in rabbits. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107,
Cerletti, U., & Bini, L. (1938). L’Elettro-shock. Ar- Cheer, J. F., Wassum, K. M., Heien, M. L. A. V., 264–270. (13)
chivio Generale di Neurologia e Psichiatria e Psicoa- Phillips, P. E. M., & Wightman, R. M. (2004). Clark, W. S. (2004). Is the zone-tailed hawk a mim-
nalisi, 19, 266–268. (15) Cannabinoids enhance subsecond dopamine ic? Birding, 36, 494–498. (1)
Cervantes, M. C., & Delville, Y. (2009). Serotonin release in the nucleus accumbens of awake Clarke, S., Assal, G., & deTribolet, N. (1993). Left
5-HT1A and 5-HT3 receptors in an impulsive- rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 4393– hemisphere strategies in visual recognition, topo-
aggressive phenotype. Behavioral Neuroscience, 4400. (3) graphical orientation and time planning. Neuro-
123, 589–598. (12) Chen, L. M., Friedman, R. M., & Roe, A. W. (2003). psychologia, 31, 99–113. (14)
Chabris, C. F., & Glickman, M. E. (2006). Sex dif- Optical imaging of a tactile illusion in area 3b of Clarkson, A. N., Huang, B. S., MacIsaac, S. E.,
ferences in intellectual performance: Analysis of the primary somatosensory cortex. Science, 302, Mody, I., & Carmichael, S. T. (2010). Reducing
a large cohort of competitive chess players. Psy- 881–885. (7) excessive GABA-mediated tonic inhibition pro-
chological Science, 17, 1040–1046. (4) Chen, Z.-Y., Jing, D., Bath, K. G., Ieraci, A., Khan, motes functional recovery after a stroke. Nature,
Chafee, M. V., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1998). T., Siao, C.-J., et al. (2006). Genetic variant 468, 305–309. (5)
Matching patterns of activity in primate prefron- BDNF (Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxi- Clayton, E. C., & Williams, C. L. (2000). Glutama-
tal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a ety-related behavior. Science, 314, 140–143. (12) tergic influences on the nucleus paragigantocel-
spatial working memory task. Journal of Neuro- Cheour-Luhtanen, M., Alho, K., Sainio, K., Rinne, lularis: Contribution to performance in avoid-
physiology, 79, 2919–2940. (13) T., Reinikainen, K., Pohjavuoir, M., et al. (1996). ance and spatial memory tasks. Behavioral Neuro-
Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing up to the problem of The ontogenetically earliest discriminative re- science, 114, 707–712. (9)
consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2, sponse of the human brain. Psychophysiology, 33, Cleary, L. J., Hammer, M., & Byrne, J. H. (1989). In-
200–219. (1, 14) 478–481. (14) sights into the cellular mechanisms of short-term
Chalmers, D. L. (2004). How can we construct a Chiang, M.-C., Barysheva, M., Shattuck, D. W., sensitization in Aplysia. In T. J. Carew &
science of consciousness? In M. S. Gazzaniga Lee, A. D., Madsen, S. K., Avedissian, C., et al. D. B. Kelley (Eds.), Perspectives in neural systems
(Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (3rd ed.) (pp. (2009). Genetics of brain fiber architecture and and behavior (pp. 105–119). New York: Liss. (13)
1111–1119). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (14) intellectual performance. Journal of Neuroscience, Cleary, M., Moody, A. D., Buchanan, A., Stewart,
Chalmers, D. (2007). Naturalistic dualism. In M. 29, 2212–2224. (4) H., & Dutton, G. N. (2009). Assessment of a
Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell Chiueh, C. C. (1988). Dopamine in the extrapyra- computer-based treatment for older amblyopes:
companion to consciousness (pp. 359–368). Mal- midal motor function: A study based upon the The Glasgow Pilot Study. Eye, 23, 124–131. (6)
den, MA: Blackwell. (1) MPTP-induced primate model of Parkinsonism. Clelland, C. D., Choi, M., Romberg, C., Clemenson,
Chan, C. S., Guzman, J. N., Ilijic, E., Mercer, J. N., Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 515, G. D. Jr., Fragniere, A., Tyers, P., et al. (2009). A
Rick, C., Tkatch, T., et al. (2007). “Rejuvenation” 226–248. (8) functional role for adult hippocampal neurogen-
protects neurons in mouse models of Parkinson’s Chivers, M. L., Rieger, G., Latty, E., & Bailey, J. M. esis in spatial pattern separation. Science, 325,
disease. Nature, 447, 1081–1086. (8) (2004). A sex difference in the specificity of sexual 210–213. (5)
Chang, E. F., & Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Environ- arousal. Psychological Science, 15, 736–744. (11) Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Cooperation between non-
mental noise retards auditory cortical develop- Cho, K. (2001). Chronic “jet lag” produces temporal kin in animal societies. Nature, 462, 51–57. (1)
ment. Science, 300, 498–502. (7) lobe atrophy and spatial cognitive deficits. Nature Clutton-Brock, T. H., O’Riain, M. J., Brotherton,
Chang, G.-Q., Gaysinskaya, V., Karatayev, O., & Neuroscience, 4, 567–568. (9) P. N. M., Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A. S., et
Leibowitz, S. F. (2008). Maternal high-fat diet Choi, D.-S., Cascini, M.-G., Mailliard, W., Young, al. (1999). Selfish sentinels in cooperative mam-
and fetal programming: Increased proliferation H., Paredes, P., McMahon, T., et al. (2004). The mals. Science, 284, 1640–1644. (1)
of hypothalamic peptide-producing neurons that type I equilibrative nucleoside transporter regu- Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J.
increase risk for overeating and obesity. Journal of lates ethanol intoxication and preference. Nature (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the
Neuroscience, 28, 12107–12119. (10) Neuroscience, 7, 855–861. (3) neural response to threat. Psychological Science,
Changeux, J.-P. (2010). Nicotine addiction and nic- Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New 17, 1032–1039. (12)
otinic receptors: Lessons from genetically modi- York: Columbia University Press. (14) Cobos, P., Sánchez, M., Pérez, N., & Vila, J. (2004).
fied mice. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 389– Chuang, H., Prescott, E. D., Kong, H., Shields, S., Effects of spinal cord injuries on the subjective
401. (3) Jordt, S.-E., Basbaum, A. I., et al. (2001). Brady- component of emotions. Cognition and Emotion,
Chao, M. V. (2010). A conversation with Rita Levi- kinin and nerve growth factor release the capsa- 18, 281–287. (12)
Montalcini. Annual Review of Physiology, 72, icin receptor from Ptdlns(4, 5)P2-mediated inhi- Coccaro, E. F., & Lee, R. (2010). Cerebrospinal fluid
1–13. (5) bition. Nature, 411, 957–962. (7) 5-hydroxyindolacetic acid and homovanillic acid:
502 References

Reciprocal relationships with impulsive aggres- Coppola, D. M., Purves, H. R., McCoy, A. N., & Cravchik, A., & Goldman, D. (2000). Neurochemi-
sion in human subjects. Journal of Neural Trans- Purves, D. (1998). The distribution of oriented cal individuality. Archives of General Psychiatry,
mission, 117, 241–248. (12) contours in the real world. Proceedings of the Na- 57, 1105–1114. (3)
Coderre, T. J., Katz, J., Vaccarino, A. L., & Melzack, tional Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 4002– Cremers, C. W. R. J., & van Rijn, P. M. (1991). Ac-
R. (1993). Contribution of central neuroplastic- 4006. (6) quired causes of deafness in childhood. Annals
ity to pathological pain: Review of clinical and Corcoran, A. J., Barber, J. R., & Conner, W. E. of the New York Academy of Sciences, 630, 197–
experimental evidence. Pain, 52, 259–285. (7) (2009). Tiger moth jams bat sonar. Science, 325, 202. (7)
Cohen, D., & Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2004). Reduction 325–327. (7) Crick, F. C., & Koch, C. (2004). A framework for
of single-neuron firing uncertainty by cortical en- Corkin, S. (1984). Lasting consequences of bilateral consciousness. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cog-
sembles during motor skill learning. Journal of medial temporal lobectomy: Clinical course and nitive neurosciences (3rd ed., pp. 1133–1143).
Neuroscience, 24, 3574–3582. (8) experimental findings in H. M. Seminars in Neu- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (14)
Cohen, L. G., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, A., Cor- rology, 4, 249–259. (13) Crick, F., & Mitchison, G. (1983). The function of
well, B., Faiz, L., Dambrosia, J., et al. (1997). Corkin, S. (2002). What’s new with the amnesic pa- dream sleep. Nature, 304, 111–114. (9)
Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in tient H. M.? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J.
blind humans. Nature, 389, 180–183. (5) 153–159. (13) (2001). Neuroanatomical basis for first- and
Cohen, S., Frank, E., Doyle, W. J., Skoner, D. P., Corkin, S., Rosen, T. J., Sullivan, E. V., & Clegg, second-order representations of bodily states.
Rabin, B. S., & Swaltney, J. M., Jr. (1998). Types R. A. (1989). Penetrating head injury in young Nature Neuroscience, 4, 207–212. (12)
of stressors that increase susceptibility to the adulthood exacerbates cognitive decline in later Critchley, H. D., & Rolls, E. T. (1996). Hunger
common cold in healthy adults. Health Psycholo- years. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 3876–3883. (5) and satiety modify the responses of olfactory
gy, 17, 214–223. (12) Cosmelli, D., David, O., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, and visual neurons in the primate orbito-frontal
Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2005). Gender change in 46, J., Garnero, L., Renault, B., et al. (2004). Waves cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 75, 1673–
XY persons with 5a-reductase-2 deficiency and of consciousness: Ongoing cortical patterns dur- 1686. (10)
17b-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase-3 deficiency. ing binocular rivalry. NeuroImage, 23, 128–140. Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., & Robbins, T. W. (2009).
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 399–410. (11) (14) Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral
Cohen-Tannoudji, M., Babinet, C., & Wassef, M. Coss, R. G., Brandon, J. G., & Globus, A. (1980). inhibition and aversion: Acute tryptophan deple-
(1994). Early determination of a mouse somato- Changes in morphology of dendritic spines on tion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in
sensory cortex marker. Nature, 368, 460–463. (5) honeybee calycal interneurons associated with humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 11993–
Colantuoni, C., Rada, P., McCarthy, J., Patten, C., cumulative nursing and foraging experiences. 11999. (12)
Avena, N. M., Chadeayne, A., et al. (2002). Evi- Brain Research, 192, 49–59. (5) Crossin, K. L., & Krushel, L. A. (2000). Cellular
dence that intermittent, excessive sugar intake Costa, M., Braun, C., & Birbaumer, N. (2003). Gen- signaling by neural cell adhesion molecules of the
causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obesity der differences in response to pictures of nudes: immunoglobulin family. Developmental Dynam-
Research, 10, 478–488. (10) A magnetoencephalography study. Biological Psy- ics, 218, 260–279. (5)
Colantuoni, C., Schwenker, J., McCarthy, J., Rada, chology, 63, 129–147. (4) Crossley, N. A., & Constante, M. (2010). Efficacy of
P., Ladenheim, B., Cadet, J.-L., et al. (2001). Ex- Costa, V. D., Lang, P. J., Sabatinelli, D., Versace, F., atypical v. typical antipsychotics in the treatment
cessive sugar intake alters binding to dopamine & Bradley, M. M. (2010). Emotional imagery: of early psychosis: meta-analysis. British Journal
and mu-opioid receptors in the brain. NeuroRe- Assessing pleasure and arousal in the brain’s re- of Psychiatry, 196, 434–439. (15)
port, 12, 3549–3552. (10) ward circuitry. Human Brain Mapping, 31, Crystal, J. D. (2009). Elements of episodic-like
Colapinto, J. (1997, December 11). The true story 1446–1457. (3) memory in animal models. Behavioural Processes,
of John/Joan. Rolling Stone, pp. 54–97. (11) Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., 80, 269–277. (13)
Collingridge, G. L., Peineau, S., Howland, J. G., & Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., Cummings, D. E., Clement, K., Purnell, J. Q.,
Wang, Y. T. (2010). Long-term depression in the et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in Vaisse, C., Foster, K. E., Frayo, R. S., et al. (2002).
CNS. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 459– autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuro- Elevated plasma ghrelin levels in Prader-Willi
473. (13) science, 108, 848–865. (4) syndrome. Nature Medicine, 8, 643–644. (10)
Colom, R., Haier, R. J., Head, K., Álvarez-Linera, J., Cowart, B. J. (2005, Spring). Taste, our body’s gusta- Cummings, D. E., & Overduin, J. (2007). Gastroin-
Quiroga, M. A., Shih, P. C., et al. (2009). Gray tory gatekeeper. Cerebrum, 7(2), 7–22. (7) testinal regulation of food intake. Journal of Clini-
matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial Cowey, A., & Stoerig, P. (1995). Blindsight in mon- cal Investigation, 117, 13–23. (10)
intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. Intelli- keys. Nature, 373, 247–249. (6) Cunningham, W. A., Van Bavel, J. J., & Johnsen, I. R.
gence, 37, 124–135. (4) Cox, J. J., Reimann, F., Nicholas, A. K., Thornton, (2008). Adaptive flexibility: Evaluative process-
Conn, P. M., & Parker, J. V. (2008). Winners and G., Roberts, E., Springell, K., et al. (2006). An ing goals shape amygdala activity. Psychological
losers in the animal-research war. American Sci- SCNxA channelopathy causes congenital in- Science, 19, 152–160. (12)
entist, 96, 184–186. (1) ability to experience pain. Nature, 304, 115– Cusack, R., Carlyon, R. P., & Robertson, I. H.
Connine, C. M., Blasko, D. G., & Hall, M. (1991). 117. (7) (2000). Neglect between but not within auditory
Effects of subsequent sentence context in audi- Craig, A. D., Krout, K., & Andrew, D. (2001). objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12,
tory word recognition: Temporal and linguistic Quantitative response characteristics of thermo- 1056–1065. (14)
constraints. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, receptive and nociceptive lamina I spinothalamic Cutler, W. B., Preti, G., Krieger, A., Huggins, G. R.,
234–250. (14) neurons in the cat. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, Garcia, C. R., & Lawley, H. J. (1986). Human
Considine, R. V., Sinha, M. K., Heiman, M. L., Kri- 1459–1480. (7) axillary secretions influence women’s menstrual
auciunas, A., Stephens, T. W., Nyce, M. R., et al. Craig, A. M., & Boudin, H. (2001). Molecular het- cycles: The role of donor extract from men. Hor-
(1996). Serum immunoreactive-leptin concen- erogeneity of central synapses: Afferent and target mones and Behavior, 20, 463–473. (7)
trations in normal-weight and obese humans. regulation. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 569–578. (3) Cuzon, V. C., Yeh, P. W. L., Yanagawa, Y., Obata, K.,
New England Journal of Medicine, 334, 292–295. Crair, M. C., Gillespie, D. C., & Stryker, M. P. & Yeh, H. H. (2008). Ethanol consumption dur-
(10) (1998). The role of visual experience in the devel- ing early pregnancy alters the disposition of tan-
Cooke, B. M., Tabibnia, G., & Breedlove, S. M. opment of columns in cat visual cortex. Science, gentially migrating GABAergic interneurons in
(1999). A brain sexual dimorphism controlled by 279, 566–570. (6) the fetal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 28,
adult circulating androgens. Proceedings of the Crair, M. C., & Malenka, R. C. (1995). A critical 1854–1864. (5)
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 96, 7538– period for long-term potentiation at thalamocor- Czeisler, C. A., Johnson, M. P., Duffy, J. F., Brown,
7540. (11) tical synapses. Nature, 375, 325–328. (6) E. N., Ronda, J. M., & Kronauer, R. E. (1990).
References 503

Exposure to bright light and darkness to treat mate spinothalamic tract neurons. Nature Neuro- De Luca, M., Di Page, E., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., &
physiologic maladaptation to night work. New science, 12, 544–546. (7) Zoccolotti, P. (1999). Eye movement patterns in
England Journal of Medicine, 322, 1253–1259. (9) Davidson, S., Zhang, X., Yoon, C. H., Khasabov, linguistic and non-linguistic tasks in develop-
Czeisler, C. A., Weitzman, E. D., Moore-Ede, S. G., Simone, D. A., & Giesler, G. J., Jr. (2007). mental surface dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 37,
M. C., Zimmerman, J. C., & Knauer, R. S. The itch-producing agents histamine and cow- 1407–1420. (14)
(1980). Human sleep: Its duration and organi- hage activate separate populations of primate de Maat, S., Dekker, J., Schoevers, R., van Aalst,
zation depend on its circadian phase. Science, spinothalamic tract neurons. Journal of Neurosci- G., Gijsbers-van Wijk, C., Hendriksen, M., et
210, 1264–1267. (9) ence, 27, 10007–10014. (7) al. (2008). Short psychodynamic supportive
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Carr, T. S., Frady, R. L., & Riad, Davies, G., Welham, J., Chant, D., Torrey, E. F., & psychotherapy, antidepressants, and their com-
J. K. (1995). Testosterone, crime, and misbehav- McGrath, J. (2003). A systematic review and bination in the treatment of major depression:
ior among 692 male prison inmates. Personality meta-analysis of Northern hemisphere season of A mega-analysis based on three randomized
and Individual Differences, 18, 627–633. (12) birth effects in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bul- clinical trials. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 565–
Dale, N., Schacher, S., & Kandel, E. R. (1988). letin, 29, 587–593. (15) 574. (15)
Long-term facilitation in Aplysia involves in- Davies, P. (2000). A very incomplete comprehensive De Wall, C. N., Mac Donald, G., Webster, G. D.,
crease in transmitter release. Science, 239, 282– theory of Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of the New Masten, C. L., Baumeister, R. F., Powell, C., et al.
285. (13) York Academy of Sciences, 924, 8–16. (13) (2010). Acetaminophen reduces social pain: Be-
Dale, P. S., Harlaar, N., Haworth, C. M. A., & Plo- Davies, P. (2006). The Goldilocks enigma. Boston, havioral and neural evidence. Psychological Sci-
min, R. (2010). Two by two: A twin study of MA: Houghton Mifflin. (1) ence, 21, 931–937. (7)
second-language acquisition. Psychological Sci- Davis, E. C., Shryne, J. E., & Gorski, R. A. (1995). A Deacon, T. W. (1992). Brain-language coevolution.
ence, 21, 635–640. (1) revised critical period for the sexual differentia- In J. A. Hawkins & M. Gell-Mann (Eds.), The
Dalterio, S., & Bartke, A. (1979). Perinatal exposure tion of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the pre- evolution of human languages (pp. 49–83). Read-
to cannabinoids alters male reproductive func- optic area in the rat. Neuroendocrinology, 62, ing, MA: Addison-Wesley. (14)
tion in mice. Science, 205, 1420–1422. (11) 579–585. (11) Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species. New
Dalton, K. (1968). Ante-natal progesterone and in- Davis, J. I., Senghas, A., Brandt, F., & Ochsner, K. N. York: Norton (4, 14)
telligence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, (2010). The effects of BOTOX injections on emo- Deadwyler, S. A., Porrino, L., Siegel, J. M., &
1377–1382. (11) tional experience. Emotion, 10, 433–440. (12) Hampson, R. E. (2007). Systemic and nasal de-
Dalton, P., Doolittle, N., & Breslin, P. A. (2002). Davis, J. M., Chen, N., & Glick, I. D. (2003). A me- livery of orexin-A (hypocretin-1) reduces the ef-
Gender-specific induction of enhanced sensitivi- ta-analysis of the efficacy of second-generation fects of sleep deprivation on cognitive perfor-
ty to odors. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 199–200. (7) antipsychotics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, mance in nonhuman primates. Journal of Neuro-
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens. 553–564. (15) science, 27, 14239–14247. (9)
New York: Harcourt Brace. (12) Davis, K. D., Kiss, Z. H. T., Luo, L., Tasker, R. R., Dean, C. E. (2011). Psychopharmacology: A house
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Lozano, A. M., & Dostrovsky, J. O. (1998). divided. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology &
Putnam’s Sons. (12) Phantom sensations generated by thalamic mi- Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1–10. (15)
Damsma, G., Pfaus, J. G., Wenkstern, D., Phillips, crostimulation. Nature, 391, 385–387. (5) DeArmond, S. J., Fusco, M. M., & Dewey, M. M.
A. G., & Fibiger, H. C. (1992). Sexual behavior Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (new ed.). Ox- (1974). Structure of the human brain. New York:
increases dopamine transmission in the nucleus ford, England: Oxford University Press. (1) Oxford University Press. (10)
accumbens and striatum of male rats: A com- Dawson, T. M., & Dawson, V. L. (2003). Molecular DeCoursey, P. (1960). Phase control of activity in a
parison with novelty and locomotion. Behavioral pathways of neurodegeneration in Parkinson’s rodent. Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantita-
Neuroscience, 106, 181–191. (3) disease. Science, 302, 819–822. (8) tive biology, 25, 49–55. (9)
Dantzer, R., O’Connor, J. C., Freund, G. G., John- Dawson, T. M., Gonzalez-Zulueta, M., Kusel, J., & DeFelipe, C., Herrero, J. F., O’Brien, J. A., Palmer,
son, R. W., & Kelley, K. W. (2008). From inflam- Dawson, V. L. (1998). Nitric oxide: Diverse ac- J. A., Doyle, C. A., Smith, A. J. H., et al. (1998).
mation to sickness and depression: When the tions in the central and peripheral nervous sys- Altered nociception, analgesia and aggression in
immune system subjugates the brain. Nature Re- tem. The Neuroscientist, 4, 96–112. (3) mice lacking the receptor for substance P. Nature,
views Neuroscience, 9, 46–57. (12) Day, S. (2005). Some demographic and socio- 392, 394–397. (7)
Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species. New York: cultural aspects of synesthesia. In L. C. Robert- Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., LeBihan, D.,
D. Appleton. (1) son & N. Sagiv (Eds.), Synesthesis (pp. 11–33). Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., et al. (2001). Cerebral
Davalos, D., Grutzendler, J., Yang, G., Kim, J. V., Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. (7) mechanisms of word masking and unconscious
Zuo, Y., Jung, S., et al. (2005). ATP mediates de Bruin, J. P. C., Winkels, W. A. M., & de Braband- repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 752–
rapid microglial response to local brain injury in er, J. M. (1997). Response learning of rats in a 758. (14)
vivo. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 752–758. (2) Morris water maze: Involvement of the medial Del Cerro, M. C. R., Perez Izquierdo, M. A.,
Davidson, R. J. (1984). Affect, cognition, and hemi- prefrontal cortex. Behavioral Brain Research, 85, Rosenblatt, J. S., Johnson, B. M., Pacheco, P.,
spheric specialization. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & 47–55. (13) & Komisaruk, B. R. (1995). Brain 2-deoxy-
R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, & be- de Castro, J. M. (2000). Eating behavior: Lessons glucose levels related to maternal behavior-
havior (pp. 320–365). Cambridge, England: from the real world of humans. Nutrition, 16, inducing stimuli in the rat. Brain Research,
Cambridge University Press. (15) 800–813. (10) 696, 213–220. (11)
Davidson, R. J., & Fox, N. A. (1982). Asymmetrical de Jong, W. W., Hendriks, W., Sanyal, S., & Nevo, Del Cul, A., Dehaene, S., Reyes, P., Bravo, E., &
brain activity discriminates between positive and E. (1990). The eye of the blind mole rat (Spalax Slachevsky, A. (2009). Causal role of prefrontal
negative affective stimuli in human infants. Sci- ehrenbergi): Regressive evolution at the molecu- cortex in the threshold for access to conscious-
ence, 218, 1235–1237. (12) lar level. In E. Nevo & O. A. Reig (Eds.), Evolu- ness. Brain, 132, 2531–2540. (14)
Davidson, R. J., & Henriques, J. (2000). Regional tion of subterranean mammals at the organismal Delahanty, D. L., Raimonde, A. J., & Spoonster, E.
brain function in sadness and depression. In and molecular levels (pp. 383–395). New York: (2000). Initial posttraumatic urinary cortisol lev-
J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emo- Liss. (9) els predict subsequent PTSD symptoms in mo-
tion (pp. 269–297). London: Oxford University de la Rocha, J., Marchetti, C., Schiff, M., & Reyes, tor vehicle accident victims. Biological Psychiatry,
Press. (12) A. D. (2008). Linking the response properties of 48, 940–947. (12)
Davidson, S., Zhang, X., Khasabov, S. G., Simone, cells in auditory cortex with network architec- Deliagina, T. G., Orlovsky, G. N., & Pavlova, G. A.
D. A., & Giesler, G. J. Jr. (2009). Relief of itch by ture: Cotuning versus lateral inhibition. Journal (1983). The capacity for generation of rhythmic
scratching: State-dependent inhibition of pri- of Neuroscience, 28, 9151–9163. (7) oscillations is distributed in the lumbosacral spi-
504 References

nal cord of the cat. Experimental Brain Research, nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 708, Dierks, T., Linden, D. E. J., Jandl, M., Formisano, E.,
53, 81–90. (8) 119–128. (3) Goebel, R., Lanfermann, H., et al. (1999). Acti-
Dement, W. (1972). Some must watch while some Devor, M. (1996). Pain mechanisms. The Neurosci- vation of Heschl’s gyrus during auditory halluci-
must sleep. San Francisco: Freeman. (9) entist, 2, 233–244. (7) nations. Neuron, 22, 615–621. (4)
Dement, W., & Kleitman, N. (1957a). Cyclic varia- DeYoe, E. A., Felleman, D. J., Van Essen, D. C., & Dijk, D.-J., & Archer, S. N. (2010). PERIOD3, cir-
tions in EEG during sleep and their relation to McClendon, E. (1994). Multiple processing cadian phenotypes, and sleep homeostasis. Sleep
eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. streams in occipitotemporal visual cortex. Na- Medicine Reviews, 14, 151–160. (9)
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiol- ture, 371, 151–154. (6) Dijk, D.-J., & Cajochen, C. (1997). Melatonin and
ogy, 9, 673–690. (9) Dhingra, R., Sullivan, L., Jacques, P. F., Wang, T. J., the circadian regulation of sleep initiation, con-
Dement, W., & Kleitman, N. (1957b). The relation Fox, C. S., Meigs, J. B., et al. (2007). Soft drink solidation, structure, and the sleep EEG. Journal
of eye movements during sleep to dream activi- consumption and risk of developing cardiometa- of Biological Rhythms, 12, 627–635. (9)
ty: An objective method for the study of dream- bolic risk factors and the metabolic syndrome in Dijk, D.-J., Neri, D. F., Wyatt, J. K., Ronda, J. M.,
ing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 339– middle-aged adults in the community. Circula- Riel, E., Ritz-deCecco, A., et al. (2001). Sleep,
346. (9) tion, 116, 480–488. (10) performance, circadian rhythms, and light-dark
Dement, W. C. (1990). A personal history of sleep Di Lorenzo, P. M., Chen, J.-Y., & Victor, J. D. (2009). cycles during two space shuttle flights. Ameri-
disorders medicine. Journal of Clinical Neurophys- Quality time: Representation of a multidimen- can Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative,
iology, 7, 17–47. (9) sional sensory domain through temporal coding. and Comparative Physiology, 281, R1647–
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Bos- Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9227–9238. (7) R1664. (9)
ton, MA: Little, Brown. (1, 14) Di Lorenzo, P. M., Leshchinskiy, S., Moroney, D. N., Dilks, D. D., Serences, J. T., Rosenau, B. J., Yantis,
deQuervain, D. J.-F., Roozendaal, B., & McGaugh, & Ozdoba, J. M. (2009). Making time count: S., & McCloskey, M. (2007). Human adult corti-
J. L. (1998). Stress and glucocorticoids impair Functional evidence for temporal coding of taste cal reorganization and consequent visual distor-
retrieval of long-term spatial memory. Nature, sensation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 14–25. (2) tion. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9585–9594. (5)
394, 787–790. (13) Diamond, L. M. (2007). A dynamical systems ap- Diller, L., Packer, O. S., Verweij, J., McMahon,
Derégnaucourt, S., Mitra, P. P., Fehér, O., Pytte, C., proach to the development and expression of fe- M. J., Williams, D. R., & Dacey, D. M. (2004). L
& Tchernichovski, O. (2005). How sleep affects male same-sex sexuality. Perspectives on Psycho- and M cone contributions to the midget and
the developmental learning of bird song. Nature, logical Science, 2, 142–161. (11) parasol ganglion cell receptive fields of macaque
433, 710–716. (9) Diamond, M., & Sigmundson, H. K. (1997). Man- monkey retina. Journal of Neuroscience, 24,
DeSimone, J. A., Heck, G. L., & Bartoshuk, L. M. agement of intersexuality: Guidelines for dealing 1079–1088. (6)
(1980). Surface active taste modifiers: A com- with persons with ambiguous genitalia. Archives DiMarzo, V., Fontana, A., Cadas, H., Schinelli, S.,
parison of the physical and psycho-physical of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 151, 1046– Cimino, G., Schwartz, J.-C., et al. (1994). Forma-
properties of gymnemic acid and sodium lauryl 1050. (11) tion and inactivation of endogenous cannabinoid
sulfate. Chemical Senses, 5, 317–330. (7) Diamond, M. C., Scheibel, A. B., Murphy, G. M., & anandamide in central neurons. Nature, 372,
DeSimone, J. A., Heck, G. L., Mierson, S., & Harvey, T. (1985). On the brain of a scientist: 686–691. (3)
DeSimone, S. K. (1984). The active ion transport Albert Einstein. Experimental Neurology, 88, Dimond, S. J. (1979). Symmetry and asymmetry in
properties of canine lingual epithelia in vitro. 198–204. (4) the vertebrate brain. In D. A. Oakley & H. C.
Journal of General Physiology, 83, 633–656. (7) Diano, S., Farr, S. A., Benoit, S. C., McNay, E. C., da Plotkin (Eds.), Brain, behaviour, and evolution
Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szath- Silva, I., Horvath, B., et al. (2006). Ghrelin con- (pp. 189–218). London: Methuen. (14)
mari, A., Mottolese, C., & Sirigu, A. (2009). trols hippocampal spine synapse density and Dinstein, I., Hasson, U., Rubin, N., & Heeger, D. J.
Movement intention after parietal cortex stimu- memory performance. Nature Neuroscience, 9, (2007). Brain areas selective for both observed
lation in humans. Science, 324, 811–813. (8) 381–388. (10) and executed movements. Journal of Neurophysi-
Detre, J. A., & Floyd, T. F. (2001). Functional MRI Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J. C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., ology, 98, 1415–1427. (8)
and its applications to the clinical neurosciences. Mesquita, A. R., Cerqueira, J. J., et al. (2009). Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Humphreys, K., Minshew,
Neuroscientist, 7, 64–79. (4) Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganiza- N., Behrmann, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Nor-
Deutsch, D., Henthorn, T., Marvin, E., & Xu, H. S. tion and affects decision making. Science, 325, mal movement selectivity in autism. Neuron, 66,
(2006). Absolute pitch among American and 621–625. (12) 461–469. (8)
Chinese conservatory students: Prevalence dif- Dichgans, J. (1984). Clinical symptoms of cerebellar Doesburg, S. M., Green, J. J., McDonald, J. J., &
ferences, and evidence for a speech-related criti- dysfunction and their topodiagnostic signifi- Ward, L. M. (2009). Rhythms of consciousness:
cal period. Journal of the Acoustical Society of cance. Human Neurobiology, 2, 269–279. (8) Binocular rivalry reveals large-scale oscillatory
America, 119, 719–722. (7) Dick, D. M., Johnson, J. K., Viken, R. J., & Rose, R. network dynamics mediating visual perception.
Deutsch, J. A., & Ahn, S. J. (1986). The splanchnic J. (2000). Testing between-family associations in PLoS One, 4, e6142. (14)
nerve and food intake regulation. Behavioral and within-family comparisons. Psychological Science, Domhoff, G. W. (in press). The neural substrate for
Neural Biology, 45, 43–47. (10) 11, 409–413. (3) dreaming: Is it a subsystem of the default net-
Deutsch, J. A., Young, W. G., & Kalogeris, T. J. Dick, F., Bates, E., Wulfeck, B., Utman, J. A., work? Consciousness and Cognition. (90)
(1978). The stomach signals satiety. Science, 201, Dronkers, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2001). Dominguez, J. M., Brann, J. H., Gil, M., & Hull, E.
165–167. (10) Language deficits, localization, and grammar: M. (2006). Sexual experience increases nitric
DeValois, R. L., Albrecht, D. G., & Thorell, L. G. Evidence for a distributive model of language oxide synthase in the medial preoptic area of
(1982). Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in breakdown in aphasic patients and neurological- male rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 1389–
macaque visual cortex. Vision Research, 22, 545– ly intact individuals. Psychological Review, 108, 1394. (11)
559. (6) 759–788. (14) Doricchi, F., Guariglia, P., Gasparini, M., & Tomai-
Devane, W. A., Dysarz, F. A., III, Johnson, M. R., Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability uolo, F. (2005). Dissociation between physical
Melvin, L. S., & Howlett, A. C. (1988). Determi- estimates versus large environmental effects: The and mental number line bisection in right hemi-
nation and characterization of a cannabinoid re- IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review, 108, sphere brain damage. Nature Neuroscience, 8,
ceptor in rat brain. Molecular Pharmacology, 34, 346–369. (11) 1663–1665. (14)
605–613. (3) Dickerson, S. S., Gable, S. L., Irwin, M. R., Aziz, N., Doty, R. L., Applebaum, S., Zusho, H., & Settle,
Devor, E. J., Abell, C. W., Hoffman, P. L., Tabakoff, & Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Social-evaluative R. G. (1985). Sex differences in odor identifica-
B., & Cloninger, C. R. (1994). Platelet MAO threat and proinflammatory cytokine regulation. tion ability: A cross-cultural analysis. Neuropsy-
activity in Type I and Type II alcoholism. An- Psychological Science, 20, 1237–1244. (12) chologia, 23, 667–672. (7)
References 505

Dowling, J. E. (1987). The retina. Cambridge, MA: Duvarci, S., Bauer, E. P., & Paré, D. (2009). The bed Eichenbaum, H. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of
Harvard University Press. (6) nucleus of the stria terminalis mediates inter- memory. New York: Oxford University Press. (13)
Dowling, J. E., & Boycott, B. B. (1966). Organiza- individual variations in anxiety and fear. Journal Eidelberg, E., & Stein, D. G. (1974). Functional recov-
tion of the primate retina. Proceedings of the Royal of Neuroscience, 29, 10357–10361. (12) ery after lesions of the nervous system. Neurosci-
Society of London, B, 166, 80–111. (6) Dyal, J. A. (1971). Transfer of behavioral bias: Real- ences Research Program Bulletin, 12, 191–303. (5)
Downar, J., Mikulis, D. J., & Davis, K. D. (2003). ity and specificity. In E. J. Fjerdingstad (Ed.), Eippert, F., Finsterbusch, J., Binget, U., & Büchel, C.
Neural correlates of the prolonged salience of Chemical transfer of learned information (pp. 219– (2009). Direct evidence for spinal cord involve-
painful stimulation. NeuroImage, 20, 1540– 263). New York: American Elsevier. (13) ment in placebo analgesia. Science, 326, 404. (7)
1551. (7) Earnest, D. J., Liang, F.-Q., Ratcliff, M., & Cassone, Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Wil-
Downing, P. E., Chan, A. W.-Y., Peelen, M. V., V. M. (1999). Immortal time: Circadian clock liams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An
Dodds, C. M., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Domain properties of rat suprachiasmatic cell lines. Sci- fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302,
specificity in visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16, ence, 283, 693–695. (9) 290–292. (7)
1453–1461. (6) Eastman, C. I., Hoese, E. K., Youngstedt, S. D., & Eisenegger, C., Naef, M., Snozzi, R., Heinrichs, M.,
Doyon, J., Korman, M., Morin, A., Dostie, V., Tahar, Liu, L. (1995). Phase-shifting human circadian & Fehr, E. (2010). Prejudice and truth about the
A. H., Benali, H., et al. (2009). Contribution of rhythms with exercise during the night shift. effect of testosterone on human bargaining. Na-
night and day sleep vs. simple passage of time to Physiology & Behavior, 58, 1287–1291. (9) ture, 463, 356–359. (12)
the consolidation of motor sequence and visuo- Eastman, C. I., Young, M. A., Fogg, L. F., Liu, L., & Eisenstein, E. M., & Cohen, M. J. (1965). Learning
motor adaptation learning. Experimental Brain Meaden, P. M. (1998). Bright light treatment of in an isolated prothoracic insect ganglion. Ani-
Research, 195, 15–26. (9) winter depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, mal Behaviour, 13, 104–108. (13)
Dreger, A. D. (1998). Hermaphrodites and the medi- 55, 883–889. (15) Ek, M., Engblom, D., Saha, S., Blomqvist, A.,
cal invention of sex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Eaves, L. J., Martin, N. G., & Heath, A. C. (1990). Jakobsson, P.-J., & Ericsson-Dahlstrand, A.
University Press. (11) Religious affiliation in twins and their parents: (2001). Pathway across the blood–brain barri-
Drewnowski, A., Henderson, S. A., Shore, A. B., & Testing a model of cultural inheritance. Behavior er. Nature, 410, 430–431. (10)
Barratt-Fornell, A. (1998). Sensory responses to Genetics, 20, 1–22. (1) Elbert, T., Candia, V., Altenmüller, E., Rau, H.,
6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) or sucrose solu- Eccles, J. C. (1964). The physiology of synapses. Ber- Sterr, A., Rockstroh, B., et al. (1998). Alteration
tions and food preferences in young women. An- lin: Springer-Verlag. (3) of digital representations in somatosensory cor-
nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 855, Ecker, A. S., Berens, P., Keliris, G. A., Bethge, M., tex in focal hand dystonia. Neuroreport, 9, 3571–
797–801. (7) Logothetis, N. K., & Tolias, A. S. (2010). Decor- 3575. (5)
Drzyzga, L. R., Marcinowska, A., & Obuchowicz, related neuronal firing in cortical microcircuits. Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B.,
E. (2009). Antiapoptotic and neurotrophic ef- Science, 327, 584–587. (6) & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representa-
fects of antidepressants: A review of clinical and Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Jordan, W., Brosch, M., tion of the fingers of the left hand in string play-
experimental studies. Brain Research Bulletin, 79, Kruse, W., Munk, M., et al. (1988). Coherent os- ers. Science, 270, 305–307. (5)
248–257. (15) cillations: A mechanism of feature linking in the Eldridge, L. L., Engel, S. A., Zeineh, M. M.,
Du, J., Creson, T. K., Wu, L.-J., Ren, M., Gray, N. A., visual cortex? Biological Cybernetics, 60, 121– Bookheimer, S. Y., & Knowlton, B. J. (2005). A
Falke, C., et al. (2008). The role of hippocampal 130. (14) dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes
GluR1 and GluR2 receptors in manic-like behav- Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism. New in the human hippocampus. Journal of Neurosci-
ior. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 68–79. (15) York: Basic Books. (5) ence, 25, 3280–3286. (13)
Duan, X., Chang, J. H., Ge, S., Faulkner, R. L., Kim, Edinger, J. D., McCall, W. V., Marsh, G. R., Radtke, Elias, C. F., Lee, C., Kelly, J., Aschkenazi, C., Ahima,
J. Y., Kitabatake, Y., et al. (2007). Disrupted-in- R. A., Erwin, C. W., & Lininger, A. (1992). Peri- R. S., Couceyro, P. R., et al. (1998). Leptin acti-
schizophrenia 1 regulated integration of newly odic limb movement variability in older DIMS vates hypothalamic CART neurons projecting to
generated neurons in the adult brain. Cell, 130, patients across consecutive nights of home moni- the spinal cord. Neuron, 21, 1375–1385. (10)
1146–1158. (15) toring. Sleep, 15, 156–161. (9) Ellacott, K. L. J., & Cone, R. D. (2004). The central
Ducci, F., & Goldman, D. (2008). Genetic ap- Edinger, K. L., & Frye, C. A. (2004). Testosterone’s melanocortin system and the integration of
proaches to addiction: Genes and alcohol. Addic- analgesic, anxiolytic, and cognitive-enhancing ef- short- and long-term regulators of energy ho-
tion, 103, 1414–1428. (3) fects may be due in part to actions of its 5a- meostasis. Recent Progress in Hormone Research,
Ducommun, C. Y., Michel, C. M., Clarke, S., Adri- reduced metabolites in the hippocampus. Behav- 59, 395–408. (10)
ani, M., Seeck, M., Landis, T., et al. (2004). Cor- ioral Neuroscience, 118, 1352–1364. (11) Elliott, T. R. (1905). The action of adrenalin. Jour-
tical motion deafness. Neuron, 43, 765–777. (7) Edlinger, M., Hausmann, A., Kemmler, G., Kurz, nal of Physiology (London), 32, 401–467. (3)
Dudchenko, P. A., & Taube, J. S. (1997). Correla- M., Kurzhaler, I., Walch, T., et al. (2005). Trends Ellis, L., & Ames, M. A. (1987). Neurohormonal
tion between head direction cell activity and spa- in the pharmacological treatment of patients functioning and sexual orientation: A theory of
tial behavior on a radial arm maze. Behavioral with schizophrenia over a 12 year observation homosexuality–heterosexuality. Psychological
Neuroscience, 111, 3–19. (13) period. Schizophrenia Research, 77, 25–34. (15) Bulletin, 101, 233–258. (11)
Duelli, R., & Kuschinsky, W. (2001). Brain glu- Edman, G., Åsberg, M., Levander, S., & Schalling, Ellis, L., Ames, M. A., Peckham, W., & Burke, D.
cose transporters: Relationship to local energy D. (1986). Skin conductance habituation and ce- (1988). Sexual orientation of human offspring
demand. News in Physiological Sciences, 16, rebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxy-indoleacetic acid in may be altered by severe maternal stress during
71–76. (2) suicidal patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, pregnancy. Journal of Sex Research, 25, 152–
Duff, M. C., Hengst, J., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. 43, 586–592. (12) 157. (11)
(2006). Development of shared information in Ehrhardt, A. A., & Money, J. (1967). Progestin- Ellis, L., & Cole-Harding, S. (2001). The effects of
communication despite hippocampal amnesia. induced hermaphroditism: IQ and psychosexual prenatal stress, and of prenatal alcohol and nico-
Nature Neuroscience, 9, 140–146. (13) identity in a study of ten girls. Journal of Sex Re- tine exposure, on human sexual orientation.
Dunbar, G. L., Sandstrom, M. I., Rossignol, J., & search, 3, 83–100. (11) Physiology & Behavior, 74, 213–226. (11)
Lescaudron, L. (2006). Neurotrophic enhancers Ehrlich, P. R., Dobkin, D. S., & Wheye, D. (1988). Ellis-Behnke, R. G., Liang, Y.-X., You, S.-W., Tay,
as therapy for behavioral deficits in rodent mod- The birder’s handbook. New York: Simon & D. K. C., Zhang, S., So, K.-F., et al. (2006). Nano
els of Huntington’s disease: Use of gangliosides, Schuster. (10) neuro knitting: Peptide nanofiber scaffold for
substituted pyrimidines, and mesenchymal stem Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal brain repair and axon regeneration with func-
cells. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Re- system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews tional return of vision. Proceedings of the National
views, 5, 63–79. (8) Neuroscience, 1, 41–50. (13) Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 5054–5059. (5)
506 References

Ells, L. J., Hillier, F. C., Shucksmith, J., Crawley, H., learning or after retrieval in rats. Journal of Neuro- centration of 0.05%. Journal of Sleep Research, 12,
Harbige, L., Shield, J., et al. (2008). A systematic physiology, 26, 12914–12920. (9) 265–274. (9)
review of the effect of dietary exposure that could Eskelinen, M. H., Ngandu, T., Tuomilehto, J., Soin- Fan, P. (1995). Cannabinoid agonists inhibit the ac-
be achieved through normal dietary intake on inen, H., & Kivipelto, M. (2009). Midlife coffee tivation of 5-HT3 receptors in rat nodose gan-
learning and performance of school-aged chil- and tea drinking and the risk of late-life demen- glion neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73,
dren of relevance to UK schools. British Journal of tia: A population-based CAIDE study. Journal of 907–910. (3)
Nutrition, 100, 927–936. (10) Alzheimer’s Disease, 16, 85–91. (13) Fan, W., Ellacott, K. L. J., Halatchev, I. G., Takahashi,
Elston, G. N. (2000). Pyramidal cells of the frontal Esser, S. K., Hill, S., & Tononi, G. (2009). Break- K., Yu, P., & Cone, R. D. (2004). Cholecystokinin-
lobe: All the more spinous to think with. Journal down of effective connectivity during slow wave mediated suppression of feeding involves the
of Neuroscience, 20, RC95: 1–4. (4) sleep: Investigating the mechanism underlying a brainstem melanocortin system. Nature Neurosci-
Emery, N. J., Capitanio, J. P., Mason, W. A., Macha- cortical gate using large-scale modeling. Journal ence, 7, 335–336. (10)
do, C. J., Mendoza, S. P., & Amaral, D. G. (2001). of Neurophysiology, 102, 2096–2111. (9) Fantz, R. L. (1963). Pattern vision in newborn in-
The effects of bilateral lesions of the amygdala on Esslinger, C., Walter, H., Kirsch, P., Erk, S., Schnell, fants. Science, 140, 296–297. (6)
dyadic social interactions in rhesus monkeys K., Arnold, C., et al. (2009). Neural mechanisms Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual agnosia. Cambridge, MA:
(Macaca mulatta). Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, of a genome-wide supported psychosis variant. MIT Press. (6)
515–544. (12) Science, 324, 605. (15) Farah, M. J., Illes, J., Cook-Deegan, R., Gardner, H.,
Emmorey, K., Allen, J. S., Bruss, J., Schenker, N., & Etcoff, N. L., Ekman, P., Magee, J. J., & Frank, M. G. Kandel, E., King, P., et al. (2004). Neurocogni-
Damasio, H. (2003). A morphometric analysis of (2000). Lie detection and language comprehen- tive enhancement: What can we do and what
auditory brain regions in congenitally deaf sion. Nature, 405, 139. (12, 14) should we do? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5,
adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci- Etgen, A. M., Chu, H.-P., Fiber, J. M., Karkanias, 421–425. (13)
ences, USA, 100, 10049–10054. (7) G. B., & Morales, J. M. (1999). Hormonal inte- Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka,
Enard, W., Gehre, S., Hammerschmidt, K., Hölter, gration of neurochemical and sensory signals J. N. (1998). What is “special” about face percep-
S. M., Blass, T., Somel, M., et al. (2009). A hu- governing female reproductive behavior. Behav- tion? Psychological Review, 105, 482–498. (6)
manized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ioural Brain Research, 105, 93–103. (11) Farber, N. B., Newcomer, J. W., & Olney, J. W.
ganglia circuits in mice. Cell, 137, 961–971. (14) Euston, D. R., Tatsuno, M., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Glycine agonists: What can they teach us
Eng, M. Y., Schuckit, M. A., & Smith, T. L. (2005). (2007). Fast-forward playback of recent memory about schizophrenia? Archives of General Psychia-
The level of response to alcohol in daughters of sequences in prefrontal cortex during sleep. Sci- try, 56, 13–17. (15)
alcoholics and controls. Drug and Alcohol Depen- ence, 318, 1147–1150. (9) Farivar, R. (2009). Dorsal-ventral integration in ob-
dence, 79, 83–93. (3) Evans, D. A., Funkenstein, H. H., Albert, M. S., ject recognition. Brain Research Reviews, 61,
Engert, F., & Bonhoeffer, T. (1999). Dendritic spine Scherr, P. A., Cook, N. R., Chown, M. J., et al. 144–153. (6)
changes associated with hippocampal long-term (1989). Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease in a Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence
synaptic plasticity. Nature, 399, 66–70. (13) community population of older persons. Journal for a temporal processing deficit linked to dys-
Enoch, M.-A., Steer, C. D., Newman, T. K., Gibson, of the American Medical Association, 262, 2551– lexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2,
N., & Goldman, D. (2010). Early life stress, 2556. (13) 460–493. (14)
MAOA, and gene–environment interactions Evarts, E. V. (1979). Brain mechanisms of move- Fatemi, S. H., & Folsom, T. D. (2009). The neuro-
predict behavioral disinhibition in children. ment. Scientific American, 241(3), 164–179. (8) developmental hypothesis of schizophrenia,
Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 9, 65–74. (12) Evenson, K. R., Foraker, R. E., Morris, D. L., & Ro- revisited. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 528–548.
Epping-Jordan, M. P., Watkins, S. S., Koob, G. F., & samond, W. D. (2009). A comprehensive review (15)
Markou, A. (1998). Dramatic decreases in brain of prehospital and in-hospital delay times in Featherstone, R. E., Fleming, A. S., & Ivy, G. O.
reward function during nicotine withdrawal. Na- acute stroke care. International Journal of Stroke, (2000). Plasticity in the maternal circuit: Effects
ture, 393, 76–79. (3) 4, 187–199. (5) of experience and partum condition on brain as-
Erickson, C., & Lehrman, D. (1964). Effect of cas- Eyre, J. A., Taylor, J. P., Villagra, F., Smith, M., & trocyte number in female rats. Behavioral Neuro-
tration of male ring doves upon ovarian activity Miller, S. (2001). Evidence of activity-dependent science, 114, 158–172. (11)
of females. Journal of Comparative and Physiologi- withdrawal of corticospinal projections during hu- Feeney, D. M., & Sutton, R. L. (1988). Catechol-
cal Psychology, 58, 164–166. (11) man development. Neurology, 57, 1543–1554. (8) amines and recovery of function after brain dam-
Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Facoetti, A., Corradi, N., Ruffino, M., Gori, S., & age. In D. G. Stein & B. A. Sabel (Eds.), Pharma-
Chaddock, L., Heo, S., McLaren, M., et al. Zorzi, M. (2010). Visual spatial attention and cological approaches to the treatment of brain and
(2010). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is speech segmentation are both impaired in pre- spinal cord injury (pp. 121–142). New York: Ple-
associated with age-related decline in hippo- schoolers at familial risk for developmental dys- num Press. (5)
campal volume. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, lexia. Dyslexia, 16, 22–239. (14) Feeney, D. M., Sutton, R. L., Boyeson, M. G., Hov-
5368–5375. (5) Fadda, F. (2000). Tryptophan-free diets: A physio- da, D. A., & Dail, W. G. (1985). The locus coeru-
Erickson, R. P. (1982). The across-fiber pattern logical tool to study brain serotonin function. leus and cerebral metabolism: Recovery of func-
theory: An organizing principle for molar neural News in Physiological Sciences, 15, 260–264. (3) tion after cerebral injury. Physiological Psychology,
function. Contributions to Sensory Physiology, 6, Fadool, D. A., Tucker, K., Perkins, R., Fasciani, G., 13, 197–203. (5)
79–110. (7) Thompson, R. N., Parsons, A. D.. et al. (2004). Feinstein, J. S., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., & Tranel,
Erickson, R. P., diLorenzo, P. M., & Woodbury, Kv1.3 channel gene-targeted deletion produces D. (2011). The human amygdala and the induc-
M. A. (1994). Classification of taste responses in “super-smeller mice” with altered glomeruli, in- tion and experience of fear. Current Biology, 21,
brain stem: Membership in fuzzy sets. Journal of teracting scaffolding proteins, and biophysics. 34–38. (12)
Neurophysiology, 71, 2139–2150. (7) Neuron, 41, 389–404. (7) Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., &
Ernst, M. B., Wunderlich, C. M., Hess, S., Paehler, Fagen, Z. M., Mitchum, R., Vezina, P., & McGehee, Levine, A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocri-
M., Mesaros, A., Koralov, S. B., et al. (2009). En- D. S. (2007). Enhanced nicotinic receptor func- nological foundation of human affiliation. Psy-
hanced stat3 activation in POMC neurons pro- tion and drug abuse vulnerability. Journal of Neu- chological Science, 18, 965–970. (11)
vokes negative feedback inhibition of leptin and roscience, 27, 8771–8778. (3) Fell, J., & Axmacher, N. (2011). The role of phase
insulin signaling in obesity. Journal of Neurosci- Falleti, M. G., Maruff, P., Collie, A., Darby, D. G., & synchronization in memory processes. Nature
ence, 29, 11582–11593. (10) McStephen, M. (2003). Qualitative similarities Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 105–118. (14)
Eschenko, O., Mölle, M., Born, J., & Sara, S. J. in cognitive impairment associated with 24 h of Feller, G. (2010). Protein stability and enzyme ac-
(2006). Elevated sleep spindle density after sustained wakefulness and a blood alcohol con- tivity at extreme biological temperatures. Journal
References 507

of Physics—Condensed Matter, 22, article Fisch, L, Privman, E., Ramot, M., Harel, M., Nir, Y., Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006).
323101. (10) Kipervasser, S., et al. (2009). Neural “ignition”: Modulation of competing memory systems by dis-
Fendrich, R., Wessinger, C. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. Enhanced activation linked to perceptual aware- traction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
(1992). Residual vision in a scotoma: Implica- ness in human ventral stream visual cortex. Neu- ences, USA, 103, 11778–11783. (13)
tions for blindsight. Science, 258, 1489–1491. (6) ron, 64, 562–574. (14) Fogel, S. M., Nader, R., Cote, K. A., & Smith, C. T.
Feng, J., Fouse, S., & Fan, G. (2007). Epigenetic Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., (2007). Sleep spindles and learning potential. Be-
regulation of neural gene expression and neuro- Monaco, A. P., & Pembrey, M. E. (1998). Locali- havioral Neuroscience, 121, 1–10. (9)
nal function. Pediatric Research, 61 (5), Part 2, sation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and Földy, C., Neu, A., Jones, M. V., & Soltesz, I. (2006).
58R–63R. (1) language disorder. Nature Genetics, 18, 168–170. Presynaptic activity-dependent modulation of
Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an ac- (14) cannabinoid type 1 receptor-mediated inhibition
tion video game reduces gender differences in Fitts, D. A., Starbuck, E. M., & Ruhf, A. (2000). of GABA release. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18, 850– Circumventricular organs and ANGII-induced 1465–1469. (3)
855. (4) salt appetite: Blood pressure and connectivity. Foltz, E. I., & White, L. E. Jr. (1962). Pain “relief ” by
Fentress, J. C. (1973). Development of grooming in American Journal of Physiology, 279, R2277– frontal cingulumotomy. Journal of Neurosurgery,
mice with amputated forelimbs. Science, 179, R2286. (10) 19, 89–100. (7)
704–705. (8) Fitzgerald, P. B., Brown, T. L., & Daskalakis, Z. J. Foo, H., & Mason, P. (2009). Analgesia accompany-
Ferreira, F., Bailey, K. G. D., & Ferraro, V. (2002). (2002). The application of transcranial magnetic ing food consumption requires ingestion of he-
Good-enough representations in language com- stimulation in psychiatry and neurosciences re- donic foods. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 13053–
prehension. Current Directions in Psychological search. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 324– 13062. (7)
Science, 11, 11–15. (14) 340. (4) Forger, N. G., & Breedlove, S. M. (1987). Motoneu-
Fettiplace, R. (1990). Transduction and tuning in Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Fennema-Notestine, ronal death during human fetal development. Jour-
auditory hair cells. Seminars in the Neurosciences, C., McEvoy, L. K., Hagler, D. J., Holland, D., et nal of Comparative Neurology, 234, 118–122. (5)
2, 33–40. (7) al. (2009). One-year brain atrophy evident in Forger, N. G., Rosen, G. J., Waters, E. M., Jacob, D.,
Field, E. F., Whishaw, I. Q., Forgie, M. L., & Pellis, healthy aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, Simerly, R. B., & de Vries, G. J. (2004). Deletion
S. M. (2004). Neonatal and pubertal, but not 15223–15231. (5) of Bax eliminates sex differences in the mouse
adult, ovarian steroids are necessary for the de- Fjerdingstad, E. J. (1973). Transfer of learning in forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of
velopment of female-typical patterns of dodging rodents and fish. In W. B. Essman & S. Nakajima Sciences, USA, 101, 13666–13671. (11)
to protect a food item. Behavioral Neuroscience, (Eds.), Current biochemical approaches to learning Foroni, F., & Semin, G. R. (2009). Language that
118, 1293–1304. (11) and memory (pp. 73–98). Flushing, NY: Spec- puts you in touch with your bodily feelings. Psy-
Figner, B., Knoch, D., Johnson, E. J., Krosch, A. R., trum. (13) chological Science, 20, 974–980. (8)
Lisanby, S. H., Fehr, E., et al. (2010). Lateral pre- Flatz, G. (1987). Genetics of lactose digestion in hu- Forster, B., & Corballis, M. C. (2000). Interhemi-
frontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal mans. Advances in Human Genetics, 16, 1–77. (10) spheric transfer of colour and shape information
choice. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 538–539. (13) Fleet, W. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1986). The fatigue in the presence and absence of the corpus callo-
Filosa, J. A., Bonev, A. D., Straub, S. V., Meredith, effect in hemispatial neglect. Neurology, 36(Sup- sum. Neuropsychologia, 38, 32–45. (14)
A. L., Wilkerson, M. K., Aldrich, R. W., et al. pl. 1), 258. (5) Forsyth, R. J., Salorio, C. F., & Christensen, J. R.
(2006). Local potassium signaling couples neu- Fletcher, P. C., McKenna, P. J., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. (2010). Modeling early recovery patterns after
ronal activity to vasodilation in the brain. Nature M., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Brain ac- paediatric traumatic brain injury. Archives of Dis-
Neuroscience, 9, 1397–1403. (2) tivations in schizophrenia during a graded memo- ease in Childhood, 95, 266–270. (5)
Fils-Aime, M.-L., Eckardt, M. J., George, D. T., ry task studied with functional neuroimaging. Ar- Fortin, N. J., Agster, K. L., & Eichenbaum, H. B.
Brown, G. L., Mefford, I., & Linnoila, M. (1996). chives of General Psychiatry, 55, 1001–1008. (15) (2002). Critical role of the hippocampus in
Early-onset alcoholics have lower cerebrospinal Fletcher, R., & Voke, J. (1985). Defective colour vi- memory for sequences of events. Nature Neuro-
fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid levels than late- sion. Bristol, England: Hilger. (6) science, 5, 458–462. (13)
onset alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, Flöel, A., Knecht, S., Lohmann, H., Deppe, M., Fotopoulou, A., Solms, M., & Turnbull, O.
53, 211–216. (3) Sommer, J., Dräger, B., et al. (2001). Language (2004). Wishful reality distortions in confab-
Finch, C. E. (2009). Update on slow aging and neg- and spatial attention can lateralize to the same ulation: A case report. Neuropsychologia, 47,
ligible senescence—A mini-review. Gerontology, hemisphere in healthy humans. Neurology, 57, 727–744. (13)
55, 307–313. (1) 1018–1024. (14) Fournier, J. C., DeRubeis, R. J., Hollon, S. D., Dimi-
Fine, I., Smallman, H. S., Doyle, P., & MacLeod, Flor, H., Elbert, T., Knecht, S., Wienbruch, C., Pan- djian, S., Amsterdam, J., Shelton, R. C., et al.
D. I. A. (2002). Visual function before and after tev, C., Birbaumer, N., et al. (1995). Phantom- (2010). Antidepressant drug effects and depres-
the removal of bilateral congenital cataracts in limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical re- sion severity. Journal of the American Medical As-
adulthood. Vision Research, 42, 191–210. (6) organization following arm amputation. Nature, sociation, 303, 47–53. (15)
Fine, I. Wade, A. R., Brewer, A. A., May, M. G., 375, 482–484. (5) Fox, P. T., Ingham, R. J., Ingham, J. C., Zamarripa,
Goodman, D. F., Boynton, G. M., et al. (2003). Florence, S. L., & Kaas, J. H. (1995). Large-scale F., Xiong, J.-H., & Lancaster, J. L. (2000). Brain
Long-term deprivation affects visual perception reorganization at multiple levels of the somato- correlates of stuttering and syllable production:
and cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 915–916. (6) sensory pathway follows therapeutic amputation A PET performance-correlation analysis. Brain,
Finger, S., & Roe, D. (1996). Gustave Dax and the of the hand in monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 123, 1985–2004. (14)
early history of cerebral dominance. Archives of 15, 8083–8095. (5) Frangou, S., Chitins, X., & Williams, S. C. R. (2004).
Neurology, 53, 806–813. (14) Flores, L. C., & Disterhoft, J. F. (2009). Caudate Mapping IQ and gray matter density in healthy
Fink, G., Sumner, B. E. H., Rosie, R., Grace, O., & nucleus is critically involved in trace eyeblink young people. NeuroImage, 23, 800–805. (4)
Quinn, J. P. (1996). Estrogen control of central conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 14511– Frank, M. J., & Claus, E. D. (2006). Anatomy of a
neurotransmission: Effect on mood, mental state, 14520. (13) decision: Striato-orbitofrontal interactions in re-
and memory. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiolo- Flynn, J. M., & Boder, E. (1991). Clinical and elec- inforcement learning, decision making, and re-
gy, 16, 325–344. (14) trophysiological correlates of dysphonetic and versal. Psychological Review, 113, 300–326. (13)
Fink, G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, dyseidetic dyslexia. In J. F. Stein (Ed.), Vision and Frank, M. J., Samanta, J., Moustafa, A. A., & Sher-
C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). visual dyslexia (pp. 121–131). Vol. 13 of J. R. man, S. J. (2007). Hold your horses: Impulsivity,
Where in the brain does visual attention select the Cronly-Dillon (Ed.), Vision and visual dysfunc- deep brain stimulation, and medication in Par-
forest and the trees? Nature, 382, 626–628. (14) tion. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. (14) kinsonism. Science, 318, 1309–1312. (8)
508 References

Frank, R. A., Mize, S. J. S., Kennedy, L. M., de los chosocial adaptation in women with congenital Galin, D., Johnstone, J., Nakell, L., & Herron, J.
Santos, H. C., & Green, S. J. (1992). The effect adrenal hyperplasia due to CYP21A2 deficiency. (1979). Development of the capacity for tactile
of Gymnema sylvestre extracts on the sweetness Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, information transfer between hemispheres in
of eight sweeteners. Chemical Senses, 17, 461– 94, 3432–3439. (11) normal children. Science, 204, 1330–1332. (14)
479. (7) Fritsch, G., & Hitzig, E. (1870). Über die elek- Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G.
Frankland, P. W., Josselyn, S. A., Bradwejn, J., Vac- trische Erregbarkeit des Grosshirns [Concerning (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cor-
carino, F. J., & Yeomans, J. S. (1997). Activation the electrical stimulability of the cerebrum]. Ar- tex. Brain, 119, 593–609. (8)
of amygdala cholecystokinin B receptors potenti- chiv für Anatomie Physiologie und Wissenschaftli- Gamer, M., & Büchel, C. (2009). Amygdala activa-
ates the acoustic startle response in the rat. Jour- che Medicin, 300–332. (8) tion predicts gaze toward fearful eyes. Journal of
nal of Neuroscience, 17, 1838–1847. (12) Fu, L.-Y., Acuna-Goycolea, C., & van den Pol, A. N. Neuroscience, 29, 9123–9126. (12)
Franz, E. A., & Fahey, S. (2007). Developmental (2004). Neuropeptide Y inhibits hypocretin/ Gan, W.-B., Kwon, E., Feng, G., Sanes, J. R., & Lich-
change in interhemispheric communication. Psy- orexin neurons by multiple presynaptic and post- tman, J. W. (2003). Synaptic dynamism mea-
chological Science, 18, 1030–1031. (14) synaptic mechanisms: Tonic depression of the sured over minutes to months: Age-dependent
Franz, E. A., Waldie, K. E., & Smith, M. J. (2000). hypothalamic arousal system. Journal of Neurosci- decline in an autonomic ganglion. Nature Neuro-
The effect of callosotomy on novel versus familiar ence, 24, 8741–8751. (10) science, 6, 956–960. (5)
bimanual actions: A neural dissociation between Fu, Q., Heath, A. C., Bucholz, K. K., Nelson, E., Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evo-
controlled and automatic processes? Psychological Goldberg, J., Lyons, M. J., et al. (2002). Shared lution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic
Science, 11, 82–85. (14) genetic risk of major depression, alcohol depen- pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23,
Frassinetti, F., Pavani, F., & Làdavas, E. (2002). dence, and marijuana dependence. Archives of 573–644. (11)
Acoustical vision of neglected stimuli: Interac- General Psychiatry, 59, 1125–1132. (15) Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J.,
tion among spatially converging audiovisual in- Fu, W., Sugai, T., Yoshimura, H., & Onoda, N. Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N.
puts in neglect patients. Journal of Cognitive Neu- (2004). Convergence of olfactory and gustatory (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral
roscience, 14, 62–69. (14) connections onto the endopiriform nucleus in displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psy-
Frayling, T. M., Timpson, N. J., Weedon, M. N., the rat. Neuroscience, 126, 1033–1041. (7) chological Science, 15, 203–207. (11)
Zeggini, E., Freathy, R. M., Lindgren, C. M., et Fuchs, T., Haney, A., Jechura, T. J., Moore, F. R., & Ganguly, K., Kiss, L., & Poo, M. (2000). Enhance-
al. (2007). A common variant in the FTO gene is Bingman, V. P. (2006). Daytime naps in night-mi- ment of presynaptic neuronal excitability by cor-
associated with body mass index and predisposes grating birds: Behavioural adaptation to seasonal related presynaptic and postsynaptic spiking.
to childhood and adult obesity. Science, 316, sleep deprivation in the Swainson’s thrush, Catha- Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1018–1026. (13)
889–894. (10) rus ustulatus. Animal Behaviour, 72, 951–958. (9) Gao, J.-H., Parsons, L. M., Bower, J. M., Xiong, J.,
Freed, C. R., Greene, P. E., Breeze, R. E., Tsai, W.-Y., Fuller, R. K., & Roth, H. P. (1979). Disulfiram for the Li, J., & Fox, P. T. (1996). Cerebellum implicated
DuMouchel, W., Kao, R., et al. (2001). Trans- treatment of alcoholism: An evaluation in 128 in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather
plantation of embryonic dopamine neurons for men. Annals of Internal Medicine, 90, 901–904. (3) than motor control. Science, 272, 545–547. (8)
severe Parkinson’s disease. New England Journal Fusar-Poli, P., Placentino, A., Carletti, F., Landi, P., Gao, X., Martin, E. R., Liu, Y., Mayhew, G.,
of Medicine, 344, 710–719. (8) Allen, P., Surguladze, S., et al. (2009). Functional Vance, J. M., & Scott, W. K. (2009). Genome-
Freedman, M. S., Lucas, R. J., Soni, B., von Schantz, atlas of emotional faces processing: A voxel- wide linkage screen in familial Parkinson dis-
M., Muñoz, M., David-Gray, Z., et al. (1999). based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic ease identifies loci on chromosomes 3 and 18.
Regulation of mammalian circadian behavior by resonance imaging studies. Journal of Psychiatry American Journal of Human Genetics, 84, 499–
non-rod, non-cone, ocular photoreceptors. Sci- and Neuroscience, 34, 418–432. (12) 504. (8)
ence, 284, 502–504. (9) Fuster, J. M. (1989). The prefrontal cortex (2nd ed.). Garcia, R., Vouimba, R.-M., Baudry, M., & Thomp-
Frese, M., & Harwich, C. (1984). Shiftwork and the New York: Raven Press. (4) son, R. F. (1999). The amygdala modulates pre-
length and quality of sleep. Journal of Occupa- Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy frontal cortex activity relative to conditioned fear.
tional Medicine, 26, 561–566. (9) between education and cognitive neuroscience. Nature, 402, 294–296. (12)
Frey, S. H., Bogdanov, S., Smith, J. C., Watrous, S., Science, 325, 280–283. (14) Garcia-Falgueras, A., & Swaab, D. F. (2008). A sex
& Breidenbach, W. C. (2008). Chronically deaf- Gabrieli, J. D. E., Corkin, S., Mickel, S. F., & Growdon, difference in the hypothalamic uncinate nucleus:
ferented sensory cortex recovers a grossly typical J. H. (1993). Intact acquisition of mirror-tracing Relationship to gender identity. Brain, 131,
organization after allogenic hand transplanta- skill in Alzheimer’s disease and in global amnesia. 3132–3146. (11)
tion. Current Biology, 18, 1530–1534. (5) Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 899–910. (13) Gardner, B. T., & Gardner, R. A. (1975). Evidence
Fried, S., Münch, T. A., & Werblin, F. S. (2002). Gaetani, S., Fu, J., Cassano, T., Dipasquale, P., Ro- for sentence constituents in the early utterances
Mechanisms and circuitry underlying direction- mano, A., Righetti, L., et al. (2010). The fat- of child and chimpanzee. Journal of Experimental
al selectivity in the retina. Nature, 420, 411– induced satiety factor oleoylethanolamide sup- Psychology: General, 104, 244–267. (14)
414. (6) presses feeding through central release of oxyto- Gardner, H., & Zurif, E. B. (1975). Bee but not be:
Friedlander, W. J. (1986). Who was “the father of cin. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8096–8101. (10) Oral reading of single words in aphasia and alex-
bromide treatment of epilepsy”? Archives of Neu- Gage, F. H. (2000). Mammalian neural stem cells. ia. Neuropsychologia, 13, 181–190. (14)
rology, 43, 505–507. (15) Science, 287, 1433–1438. (5) Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill,
Friedman, E. S., Thase, M. E., Wisniewski, S. R., Gais, S., Plihal, W., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2000). R., Miller, R. D., & Olp, J. J. (2006). Major histo-
Trivedi, M. H., Biggs, M. M., Fava, M., et al. Early sleep triggers memory for early visual dis- compatibility complex alleles, sexual responsivity,
(2009). Cognitive therapy augmentation versus crimination skills. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1335– and unfaithfulness in romantic couples. Psycho-
CT switch treatment: A STAR*D report. In- 1339. (9) logical Science, 17, 830–835. (11)
ternational Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 2, 66– Galaburda, A. M., LoTurco, J., Ramus, F., Fitch, Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures
87. (15) R. H., & Rosen, G. D. (2006). From genes to be- differ between musicians and non-musicians.
Friedman, M. I., & Stricker, E. M. (1976). The phys- havior in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuro- Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 9240–9245. (5)
iological psychology of hunger: A physiological science, 9, 1213–1217. (14) Gavrilets, S., & Rice, W. R. (2006). Genetic models
perspective. Psychological Review, 83, 409–431. Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., of homosexuality: Generating testable predic-
(10) Aboitiz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). Develop- tions. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 273,
Frisén, L., Nordenström, A., Falhammar, H., Filips- mental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with 3031–3038. (11)
son, H., Holmdahl, G., Janson, P. O., et al. cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222– Ge, S., Yang, C.-h., Hsu, K.-s., Ming, G.-l., & Song,
(2009). Gender role behavior, sexuality, and psy- 233. (14) H. (2007). A critical period for enhanced synap-
References 509

tic plasticity in newly generated neurons of the fects of alcohol. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, Gold, R. M. (1973). Hypothalamic obesity: The
adult brain. Neuron, 54, 559–566. (5) 4583–4591. (3) myth of the ventromedial hypothalamus. Science,
Geerling, J. C., & Loewy, A. D. (2008). Central regu- Gilmore, J. H., Lin, W., Prastawa, M. W., Looney, 182, 488–490. (10)
lation of sodium appetite. Experimental Physiolo- C. B., Vetsa, Y. S. K., Knickmeyer, R. C., et al. Goldberg, T. E., Weinberger, D. R., Berman, K. F.,
gy, 93, 177–209. (10) (2007). Regional gray matter growth, sexual di- Pliskin, N. H., & Podd, M. H. (1987). Further
Geier, C. F., Terwilliger, R., Teslovich, T., Velanova, K., morphism, and cerebral asymmetry in the neo- evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in
& Luna, B. (2010). Immaturities in reward pro- natal brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1255– schizophrenia? Archives of General Psychiatry, 44,
cessing and its influence on inhibitory control in 1260. (4) 1008–1014. (15)
adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1613–1629. (5) Gilmour, D., Knaut, H., Maischein, H.-M., & Golden, S. M. (2009). Does childhood use of
Geiger, B. M., Haburcak, M., Avena, N. M., Moyer, Nüsslein-Volhard, C. (2004). Towing of sensory stimulant medication as a treatment or ADHD
M. C., Hoebel, B. G., & Pothos, E. N. (2009). axons by their migrating target cells in vivo. Na- affect the likelihood of future drug abuse and
Deficits of mesolimbic dopamine neurotransmis- ture Neuroscience, 7, 491–492. (4) dependence? A literature review. Journal of
sion in rat dietary obesity. Neuroscience, 159, Giltay, E. J., & Gooren, L. J. G. (2009). Potential Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 18, 343–
1193–1199. (10) side effects of androgen deprivation treatment in 358. (3)
Geiger, G., Lettvin, J. Y., & Fahle, M. (1994). Dys- sex offenders. Journal of the American Academy of Goldin-Meadow, S., McNeill, D., & Singleton, J.
lexic children learn a new visual strategy for read- Psychiatry and the Law, 37, 53–58. (11) (1996). Silence is liberating: Removing the hand-
ing: A controlled experiment. Vision Research, Ginsberg, M. D. (2008). Neuroprotection for isch- cuffs on grammatical expression in the manual
34, 1223–1233. (14) emic stroke: Past, present and future. Neurophar- modality. Psychological Review, 103, 34–55. (14)
Geiger, G., Lettvin, J. Y., & Zegarra-Moran, O. macology, 55, 363–389. (5) Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1998). Spon-
(1992). Task-determined strategies of visual pro- Giraux, P., Sirigu, A., Schneider, F., & Dubernard, taneous sign systems created by deaf children in
cess. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 39–52. (14) J.-M. (2001). Cortical reorganization in motor two cultures. Nature, 391, 279–281. (14)
Gerardin, D. C. C., & Pereira, O. C. M. (2002). Re- cortex after graft of both hands. Nature Neurosci- Goldman, L. S. (1999). Medical illness in patients
productive changes in male rats treated perina- ence, 4, 691–692. (5) with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
tally with an aromatase inhibitor. Pharmacology Giuliani, D., & Ferrari, F. (1996). Differential behav- 60(Suppl 21), 10–15. (15)
Biochemistry and Behavior, 71, 309–313. (11) ioral response to dopamine D2 agonists by sexually Goldman, P. S. (1971). Functional development of
Gerwig, M., Hajjar, K., Dimitrova, A., Maschke, M., naive, sexually active, and sexually inactive male the prefrontal cortex in early life and the problem
Kolb, F. P., Frings, M., et al. (2005). Timing of rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 802–808. (11) of neuronal plasticity. Experimental Neurology,
conditioned eyeblink responses is impaired in Glendenning, K. K., Baker, B. N., Hutson, K. A., & 32, 366–387. (15)
cerebellar patients. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, Masterton, R. B. (1992). Acoustic chiasm V: In- Goldman, P. S. (1976). The role of experience in re-
3919–3931. (13) hibition and excitation in the ipsilateral and con- covery of function following orbital prefrontal
Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human tralateral projections of LSO. Journal of Compar- lesions in infant monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 14,
brain: Left–right asymmetries in temporal ative Neurology, 319, 100–122. (7) 401–412. (15)
speech region. Science, 161, 186–187. (14) Gloria, R., Angelos, L., Schaefer, H. S., Davis, J. M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Topography of cogni-
Ghashghaei, H. T., Lai, C., & Anton, E. S. (2007). Majeskie, M., Richmond, B. S., et al. (2009). An tion: Parallel distributed networks in primate as-
Neuronal migration in the adult brain: Are we fMRI investigation of the impact of withdrawal sociation cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
there yet? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 141– on regional brain activity during nicotine antici- 11, 137–156. (4)
151. (5) pation. Psychophysiology, 46, 681–693. (3) Goldstein, A. (1980). Thrills in response to music
Ghosh, A., Sydekum, E., Haiss, F., Peduzzi, S., Glykys, J., Peng, Z., Chandra, D., Homanics, G. E., and other stimuli. Physiological Psychology, 8,
Schneider, R., Baltes, C., et al. (2009). Functional Houser, C. R., & Mody, I. (2007). A new natu- 126–129. (7)
and anatomical reorganization of the sensory- rally occurring GABAA receptor subunit part- Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Horton, N. J., Makris,
motor cortex after incomplete spinal cord injury nership with high sensitivity to ethanol. Nature N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness, V. S., Jr., et al. (2001).
in adult rats. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12210– Neuroscience, 10, 40–48. (12) Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human
12219. (5) Goard, M., & Dan, Y. (2009). Basal forebrain activa- brain assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imag-
Gibbs, F. P. (1983). Temperature dependence of the tion enhances cortical coding of natural scenes. ing. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 490–497. (11)
hamster circadian pacemaker. American Journal Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1444–1449. (9) Golestani, N., Molko, N., Dehaene, S., LeBihan, D.,
of Physiology, 244, R607–R610. (9) Goate, A., Chartier-Harlin, M. C., Mullan, M., & Pallier, C. (2006). Brain structure predicts the
Gibbs, J., Young, R. C., & Smith, G. P. (1973). Cho- Brown, J., Crawford, F., Fidani, L., et al. (1991). learning of foreign speech sounds. Cerebral Cor-
lecystokinin decreases food intake in rats. Journal Segregation of a missense mutation in the amy- tex, 17, 575–582. (4)
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 84, loid precursor protein gene with familial Al- Golestani, N., Price, C. J., & Scott, S. K. (2011).
488–495. (10) zheimer’s disease. Nature, 349, 704–706. (13) Born with an ear for dialects? Structural plastic-
Gilbert, S. J., Henson, R. N. A., & Simons, J. S. Godfrey, K. M., Lillycrop, K. A., Burdge, G. C., ity in the expert phonetician brain. Journal of
(2010). The scale of functional specialization Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. A. (2007). Neuroscience, 31, 4213–4220. (5)
within human prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neu- Epigenetic mechanisms and the mismatch con- Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T.,
roscience, 30, 1233–1237. (4) cept of the developmental origins of health and Golding, J., & Hines, M. (2008). Developmental
Gilbertson, M. W., Shenton, M. E., Ciszewski, A., disease. Pediatric Research, 61 (5), Part 2, 5R– trajectories of sex-typed behavior in boys and
Kasai, K., Lasko, N. B., Orr, S. P., et al. (2002). 10R. (1) girls: A longitudinal general population study of
Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologi- Gogolla, N., Caroni, P., Lüthi, A., & Herry, C. children aged 2.5–8 years. Child Development,
cal vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature (2009). Perineuronal nets protect fear memories 79, 1583–1593. (11)
Neuroscience, 5, 1242–1247. (12) from erasure. Science, 325, 1258–1261. (12) Gonzalez Andino, S. L., de Peralta Menendez,
Gillette, M. U., & McArthur, A. J. (1996). Circa- Gogos, J. A., Osborne, J., Nemes, A., Mendelsohn, R. G., Khateb, A., Landis, T., & Pegna, A. J.
dian actions of melatonin at the suprachias­ M., & Axel, R. (2000). Genetic ablation and res- (2009). Electrophysiological correlates of affec-
matic nucleus. Behavioural Brain Research, 73, toration of the olfactory topographic map. Cell, tive blindsight. NeuroImage, 44, 581–589. (6)
135–139. (9) 103, 609–620. (5) González-Maeso, J., Ang, R. L., Yuen, T., Chan, P.,
Gilman, J. M., Ramchandani, V. A., Davis, M. B., Golarai, G., Hong, S., Haas, B. W., Galaburda, A. M., Weisstaub, N. V., López-Giménez, J. F., et al.
Bjork, J. M., & Hommer, D. W. (2008). Why we Mills, D. L., Bellugi, U., et al. (2010). The fusiform (2008). Identification of a serotonin/glutamate
like to drink: A functional magnetic resonance face area is enlarged in Williams syndrome. Journal receptor complex implicated in psychosis. Na-
imaging study of the rewarding and anxiolytic ef- of Neuroscience, 30, 6700–6712. (14) ture, 452, 93–97. (15)
510 References

Goodale, M. A. (1996). Visuomotor modules in the Greenough, W. T. (1975). Experiential modification Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L.
vertebrate brain. Canadian Journal of Physiology of the developing brain. American Scientist, 63, (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science, 320,
and Pharmacology, 74, 390–400. (6, 8) 37–46. (5) 1164–1165. (4)
Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Griffin, D. R., Webster, F. A., & Michael, C. R. Güler, A. D., Ecker, J. L., Lall, G. S., Haq, S., Alti-
Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation (1960). The echolocation of flying insects by mus, C. M., Liao, H.-W., et al. (2008). Melanop-
between perceiving objects and grasping them. bats. Animal Behaviour, 8, 141–154. (7) sin cells are the principal conduits for rod-cone
Nature, 349, 154–156. (6, 8) Griffiths, T. D., Uppenkamp, S., Johnsrude, I., Jo- input to non-image-forming vision. Nature, 453,
Goodrich-Hunsaker, N. J., & Hopkins, R. O. sephs, O., & Patterson, R. D. (2001). Encoding 102–105. (9)
(2010). Spatial memory deficits in a virtual radial of the temporal regularity of sound in the human Gunn, S. R., & Gunn, W. S. (2007). Are we in the
arm maze in amnesic participants with hippo- brainstem. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 633–637. (7) dark about sleepwalking’s dangers? In C. A. Read
campal damage. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124, Grillon, C., Morgan, C. A., III, Davis, M., & South- (Ed.), Cerebrum 2007: Emerging ideas in brain sci-
405–413. (13) wick, S. M. (1998). Effect of darkness on acoustic ence (pp. 71–84). New York: Dana Press. (9)
Gooley, J. J., Rajaratnam, S. M. W., Brainard, G. C., startle in Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Ameri- Guo, S.-W., & Reed, D. R. (2001). The genetics of
Kronauer, R. E., Czeisler, C. A., & Lockley, S. W. can Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 812–817. (12) phenylthiocarbamide perception. Annals of Hu-
(2010). Spectral responses of the human circa- Gritton, H. J., Sutton, B. C., Martinez, V., Sarter, man Biology, 28, 111–142. (7)
dian system depend on the irradiance and dura- M., & Lee, T. M. (2009). Interactions between Gupta, A. S., van der Meer, M. A. A., Touretzky,
tion of exposure to light. Science Translational cognition and circadian rhythms: Attentional de- D. S., & Redish, A. D. (2010). Hippocampal re-
Medicine, 2, 31ra33. (9) mands modify circadian entrainment. Behavioral play is not a simple function of experience. Neu-
Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggre- Neuroscience, 123, 937–948. (9) ron, 65, 695–705. (9)
gation of a developmental language disorder. Gross, C. G. (1999). The fire that comes from the Gur, R. E., & Chin, S. (1999). Laterality in func-
Cognition, 39, 1–50. (14) eye. The Neuroscientist, 5, 58–64. (6) tional brain imaging studies of schizophrenia.
Gorski, R. A. (1980). Sexual differentiation of the Gross, C. G., & Graziano, M. S. A. (1995). Multiple Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 141–156. (15)
brain. In D. T. Krieger & J. C. Hughes (Eds.), representations of space in the brain. The Neuro- Gur, R. E., Cowell, P. E., Latshaw, A., Turetsky, B. I.,
Neuroendocrinology (pp. 215–222). Sunderland, scientist, 1, 43–50. (4) Grossman, R. I., Arnold, S. E., et al. (2000). Re-
MA: Sinauer. (11) Grosshans, D. R., Clayton, D. A., Coultrap, S. J., & duced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter
Gorski, R. A. (1985). The 13th J. A. F. Stevenson Browning, M. D. (2002). LTP leads to rapid sur- volumes in schizophrenia. Archives of General
memorial lecture. Sexual differentiation of the face expression of NMDA but not AMPA recep- Psychiatry, 57, 761–768. (15)
brain: Possible mechanisms and implications. tors in adult rat CA1. Nature Neuroscience, 5, Gusella, J. F., & MacDonald, M. E. (2000). Molecu-
Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, 27–33. (13) lar genetics: Unmasking polyglutamine triggers
63, 577–594. (11) Grossman, S. P., Dacey, D., Halaris, A. E., Collier, T., in neurodegenerative disease. Nature Reviews
Gorski, R. A., & Allen, L. S. (1992). Sexual orien- & Routtenberg, A. (1978). Aphagia and adipsia Neuroscience, 1, 109–115. (8)
tation and the size of the anterior commissure after preferential destruction of nerve cell bodies Gustafsson, B., & Wigström, H. (1990). Basic fea-
in the human brain. Proceedings of the National in hypothalamus. Science, 202, 537–539. (10) tures of long-term potentiation in the hippo-
Academy of Sciences, USA, 89, 7199–7202. Grueter, M., Grueter, T., Bell, V., Horst, J., Laskow- campus. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 321–
(11) ki, W., Sperling, K., et al. (2007). Hereditary 333. (13)
Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis. New prosopagnosia: The first case series. Cortex, 43, Gutschalk, A., Patterson, R. D., Scherg, M., Uppen-
York: Freeman. (15) 734–749. (6) kamp, S., & Rupp, A. (2004). Temporal dynam-
Gougoux, F., Belin, P., Voss, P., Lepore, F., Lassonde, Grutzendler, J., Kasthuri, N., & Gan, W.-B. (2002). ics of pitch in human auditory cortex. NeuroIm-
M., & Zatorre, R. J. (2009). Voice perception in Long-term dendritic spine stability in the adult age, 22, 755–766. (7)
blind persons: A functional magnetic resonance cortex. Nature, 420, 812–816. (5) Gvilia, I., Turner, A., McGinty, D., & Szymusiak, R.
imaging study. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2967–2974. Gubernick, D. J., & Alberts, J. R. (1983). Maternal (2006). Preoptic area neurons and the homeo-
(5) licking of young: Resource exchange and proxi- static regulation of rapid eye movement sleep.
Granados-Fuentes, D., Tseng, A., & Herzog, E. D. mate controls. Physiology & Behavior, 31, 593– Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3037–3044. (9)
(2006). A circadian clock in the olfactory bulb 601. (11) Gwinner, E. (1986). Circannual rhythms in the con-
controls olfactory responsivity. Journal of Neuro- Guérit, J.-M. (2005). Neurophysiological patterns of trol of avian rhythms. Advances in the Study of
science, 26, 12219–12225. (9) vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neuro- Behavior, 16, 191–228. (9)
Gray, C. M., König, P., Engel, A. K., & Singer, W. psychological Rehabilitation, 15, 357–371. (9) Haas, H., & Panula, P. (2003). The role of hista-
(1989). Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex Guidotti, A., Ferrero, P., Fujimoto, M., Santi, R. M., mine and the tuberomamillary nucleus in the
exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which & Costa, E. (1986). Studies on endogenous li- nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4,
reflects global stimulus properties. Nature, 338, gands (endocoids) for the benzodiazepine/beta 121–130. (9)
334–337. (14) carboline binding sites. Advances in Biochemical Hadjikhani, N., Liu, A. K., Dale, A. M., Cavanagh,
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of Pharmacology, 41, 137–148. (12) P., & Tootell, R. B. H. (1998). Retinotopy and
introversion–extraversion. Behavioural Research Guidotti, A., Forchetti, C. M., Corda, M. G., Konkel, color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8.
Therapy, 8, 249–266. (12) D., Bennett, C. D., & Costa, E. (1983). Isolation, Nature Neuroscience, 1, 235–241. (6)
Graziadei, P. P. C., & deHan, R. S. (1973). Neuro- characterization, and purification to homogeneity Hagenbuch, B., Gao, B., & Meier, P. J. (2002).
nal regeneration in frog olfactory system. Journal of an endogenous polypeptide with agonistic ac- Transport of xenobiotics across the blood–brain
of Cell Biology, 59, 525–530. (5) tion on benzodiazepine receptors. Proceedings of barrier. News in Physiological Sciences, 17, 231–
Graziano, M. S. A., Taylor, C. S. R., & Moore, T. the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 80, 3531– 234. (2)
(2002). Complex movements evoked by micro- 3535. (12) Haggarty, J. M., Cernovsky, Z., Husni, M., Minor,
stimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron, 34, Guillery, R. W., Feig, S. L., & Lozsádi, D. A. (1998). K., Kermean, P., & Merskey, H. (2002). Seasonal
841–851. (4, 8) Paying attention to the thalamic reticular nucle- affective disorder in an Arctic community. Acta
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., us. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 28–32. (4, 9) Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 378–384. (15)
Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI Guillery, R. W., Feig, S. L., & van Lieshout, D. P. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational
investigation of emotional engagement in moral (2001). Connections of higher order visual relays tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judg-
judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108. (12) in the thalamus: A study of corticothalamic path- ment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–834. (12)
Greengard, P. (2001). The neurobiology of slow syn- ways in cats. Journal of Comparative Neurology, Haier, R. J., Colom, R., Schroeder, D. H., Condon,
aptic transmission. Science, 294, 1024–1030. (3) 438, 66–85. (6) C. A., Tang, C., Eaves, E., et al. (2009). Gray mat-
References 511

ter and intelligence factors: Is there a neuro-g? Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of Harper, S. Q., Staber, P. D., He, X., Eliason, S. L.,
Intelligence, 37, 136–144. (4) social behavior (I and II). Journal of Theoretical Martino, I. H., Mao, Q., et al. (2005). RNA in-
Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., & Al- Biology, 7, 1–16, 17–52. (1) terference improves motor and neuropathologi-
kire, M. T. (2004). Structural brain variation and Han, C. J., & Robinson, J. K. (2001). Cannabinoid cal abnormalities in a Huntington’s disease
general intelligence. NeuroImage, 23, 425–433. (4) modulation of time estimation in the rat. Behav- mouse model. Proceedings of the National Acade-
Haimov, I., & Arendt, J. (1999). The prevention and ioral Neuroscience, 115, 243–246. (3) my of Sciences, USA, 102, 5820–5825. (8)
treatment of jet lag. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 3, Han, J.-H., Kushner, S. A., Yiu, A. P., Cole, C. J., Harris, C. H. (2002). Sexual and romantic jealousy
229–240. (9) Matynia, A., Brown, R. A., et al. (2007). Neuro- in heterosexual and homosexual adults. Psycho-
Haimov, I., & Lavie, P. (1996). Melatonin—A sopo- nal competition and selection during memory logical Science, 13, 7–12. (11)
rific hormone. Current Directions in Psychological formation. Science, 316, 457–460. (13) Harris, C. R. (1999, July/August). The mystery of
Science, 5, 106–111. (9) Hanada, R., Leibbrandt, A., Hanada, T., Kitaoka, ticklish laughter. American Scientist, 87(4), 344–
Hains, B. C., Everhart, A. W., Fullwood, S. D., & S., Furuyashiki, T., Fujihara, H., et al. (2009). 351. (7)
Hulsebosch, C. E. (2002). Changes in serotonin, Central control of fever and female body tem- Harris, K. M., & Stevens, J. K. (1989). Dendritic
serotonin transporter expression and serotonin perature by RANKL/RANK. Nature, 462, spines of CA1 pyramidal cells in the rat hippo-
denervation supersensitivity: Involvement in 515–509. (10) campus: Serial electron microscopy with refer-
chronic central pain after spinal hemisection in the Hanaway, J., Woolsey, T. A., Gado, M. H., & Rob- ence to their biophysical characteristics. Journal
rat. Experimental Neurology, 175, 347–362. (5) erts, M. P., Jr. (1998). The brain atlas. Bethesda, of Neuroscience, 9, 2982–2997. (2)
Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E. (2003). Criti- MD: Fitzgerald Science Press. (12) Harrison, G. H. (2008, January). How chickadees
cal evidence: A test of the critical-period hypoth- Hanlon, F. M., Weisend, M. P., Yeo, R. A., Huang, weather winter. National Wildlife, 46(1), 14–
esis for second-language acquisition. Psychological M., Lee, R. R., Thoma, R. J., et al. (2005). A spe- 15. (10)
Science, 14, 31–38. (14) cific test of hippocampal deficit in schizophrenia. Harrison, N. A., Gray, M. A., Gianaros, P. J., &
Halaas, J. L, Gajiwala, K. S., Maffei, M., Cohen, Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 863–875. (15) Critchley, H. D. (2010). The embodiment of
S. L., Chait, B. T., Rabinowitz, D., et al. (1995). Hannibal, J., Hindersson, P., Knudsen, S. M., emotional feelings in the brain. Journal of Neuro-
Weight-reducing effects of the plasma protein Georg, B., & Fahrenkrug, J. (2001). The phot- science, 30, 12878–12884. (12)
encoded by the obese gene. Science, 269, 543– opigment melanopsin is exclusively present in Hart, B. L. (Ed.). (1976). Experimental psychobiol­
546. (10) pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypep- ogy. San Francisco: Freeman. (10)
Halaschek-Wiener, J., Amirabbasi-Beik, M., Mon- tide-containing retinal ganglion cells of the Hartline, H. K. (1949). Inhibition of activity of visual
fared, N., Pieczyk, M., Sailer, C., Kollar, A., et al. retinohypothalamic tract. Journal of Neurosci- receptors by illuminating nearby retinal areas in
(2009). Genetic variation in healthy oldest-old. ence, 21, RC191: 1–7. (9) the limulus eye. Federation Proceedings, 8, 69. (6)
PLoS ONE, 4, e6641. (1) Hanson, K. L., & Luciana, M. (2010). Neurocogni- Harvey, A. G., & Bryant, R. A. (2002). Acute stress
Haley, J. (1959). An interactional description of tive impairments in MDMA and other drug us- disorder: A synthesis and critique. Psychological
schizophrenia. Psychiatry, 22, 321–332. (15) ers: MDMA alone may not be a cognitive risk Bulletin, 128, 886–902. (12)
Hall, J., Whalley, H. C., Job, D. E., Baig, B. J., McIn- factor. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro- Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., & Maguire,
tosh, A. M., Evans, K. L., et al. (2006). A neu- psychology, 32, 337–349. (3) E. A. (2007). Patients with hippocampal amnesia
regulin 1 variant associated with abnormal corti- Haqq, C. M., & Donahoe, P. K. (1998). Regulation cannot imagine new experiences. Proceedings of
cal function and psychotic symptoms. Nature of sexual dimorphism in mammals. Physiological the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104,
Neuroscience, 9, 1477–1478. (15) Reviews, 78, 1–33. (11) 1726–1731. (13)
Hallem, E. A., Fox, A. N., Zwiebel, L. J., & Carlson, Hara, J., Beuckmann, C. T., Nambu, T., Willie, J. T., Hassan, B., & Rahman, Q. (2007). Selective sex-
J. R. (2004). Mosquito receptor for human-sweat Chemelli, R. M., Sinton, C. M., et al. (2001). Ge- ual orientation-related differences in object
odorant. Nature, 427, 212–213. (7) netic ablation of orexin neurons in mice results in location memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121,
Hallmayer, J., Faraco, J., Lin, L., Hessselson, S., narcolepsy, hypophagia, and obesity. Neuron, 30, 625–633. (11)
Winkelmann, J., Kawashima, M., et al. (2009). 345–354. (9) Hassani, O. K., Lee, M. G., Henny, P., & Jones, B. E.
Narcolepsy is strongly associated with the T-cell Hargreaves, R. (2007). New migraine and pain re- (2009). Discharge profiles of identified GAB-
receptor alpha locus. Nature Genetics, 41, 708– search. Headache, 47(Suppl 1), S26–S43. (4) Aergic in comparison to cholinergic and putative
711. (9) Hari, R. (1994). Human cortical functions revealed glutamatergic basal forebrain neurons across the
Halpern, S. D., Andrews, T. J., & Purves, D. (1999). by magnetoencephalography. Progress in Brain sleep-wake cycle. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
Interindividual variation in human visual perfor- Research, 100, 163–168. (4) 11828–11840. (9)
mance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, Harley, B., & Wang, W. (1997). The critical period Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008).
521–534. (6) hypothesis: Where are we now? In A. M. B. de- Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences
Halverson, H. E., Lee, I., & Freeman, J. H. (2010). Groot & J. F. Knoll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingual- parallel those of children. Hormones and Behav-
Associative plasticity in the medial auditory thal- ism (pp. 19–51). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (14) ior, 54, 359–364. (11)
amus and cerebellar interpositus nucleus during Harmon-Jones, E., & Gable, P. A. (2009). Neural ac- Hatcher, J. M., Pennell, K. D., & Miller, G. W.
eyeblink conditioning. Journal of Neuroscience, tivity underlying the effect of approach-motivated (2008). Parkinson’s disease and pesticides: A
30, 8787–8796. (13) positive affect on narrowed attention. Psychological toxicological perspective. Trends in Pharmacologi-
Hamaguchi, T., Ono, K., & Yamada, M. (2010). Science, 20, 406–49. (10) cal Science, 29, 322–329. (8)
Curcumin and Alzheimer’s disease. CNS Neuro- Harmon-Jones, E., & Peterson, C. K. (2009). Su- Haueisen, J., & Knösche, T. R. (2001). Involuntary
science & Therapeutics, 16, 285–297. (13) pine body position reduces neural response to motor activity in pianists evoked by music per-
Hamann, S. B., & Squire, L. R. (1995). On the acqui- anger evocation. Psychological Science, 20, 1209– ception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13,
sition of new declarative knowledge in amnesia. 1210. (12) 786–792. (8)
Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 1027–1044. (13) Harms, M. P., Wang, L., Mamah, D., Barch, D. M., Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., Gutowski, K. A., Lu-
Hamer, D. H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., & Thompson, P. A., & Csernansky, J. G. (2007). carelli, M. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2010). Cosmetic
Pattatucci, A. M. L. (1993). A linkage between Thalamic shape abnormalities in individuals with use of botulinum toxin-A affects processing of
DNA markers on the X chromosome and male schizophrenia and their nonpsychotic siblings. emotional language. Psychological Science, 21,
sexual orientation. Science, 261, 321–327. (11) Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 13835–13842. (15) 895–900. (12)
Hamilton, N. B., & Attwell, D. (2010). Do astro- Harper, L. V. (2005). Epigenetic inheritance and the Havlicek, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated
cytes really exocytose neurotransmitters? Nature intergenerational transfer of experience. Psycho- mate choice in humans: A review. Psychoneuroendo-
Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 227–238. (2) logical Bulletin, 131, 340–360. (1) crinology, 34, 497–512. (7)
512 References

Hayashi-Takagi, A., Takaki, M., Graziane, N., cal training induces functional plasticity in hu- Hill, S. Y., De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Low-
Seshadri, S., Murdoch, H., Dunlop, A. J., et al. man hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, ers, L., Shen, S., Hall, J., et al. (2001). Right
(2010). Disrupted-in-schizophrenia 1(DISC- 1377–1384. (5) amygdala volume in adolescents and young adult
1) regulates spines of the glutamate synapse Heresco-Levy, U., & Javitt, D. C. (2004). Compara- offspring from families at high risk for develop-
via Rac1. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 327–332. tive effects of glycine and D-cycloserine on per- ing alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 894–
(15) sistent negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A 905. (3)
Haydon, P. G. (2001). Glia: Listening and talking to retrospective analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 66, Hillis, A. E., Newhart, M., Heidler, J., Barker, P. B.,
the synapse. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 89–96. (15) Herskovits, E. H., & Degaonkar, M. (2005). Anat-
185–193. (2) Heresco-Levy, U., Javitt, D. C., Ermilov, M., Mordel, omy of spatial attention: Insights from perfusion
Hayes, J. E., Bartoshuk, L. M., Kidd, J. R., & Duffy, C., Silipo, G., & Lichtenstein, M. (1999). Effi­ imaging and hemispatial neglect in acute stroke.
V. B. (2008). Supertasting and PROP bitterness cacy of high-dose glycine in the treatment of en- Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 3161–3167. (14)
depends on more than the TAS2R38 gene. during negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Ar- Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F.
Chemical Senses, 33, 255–265. (7) chives of General Psychiatry, 56, 29–36. (15) (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise
Haynes, J.-D., Katsuyuki, S., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Herkenham, M. (1992). Cannabinoid receptor lo- effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews
Frith, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Reading calization in brain: Relationship to motor and Neuroscience, 9, 58–65. (15)
hidden intentions in the human brain. Current reward systems. Annals of the New York Academy Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K. J.,
Biology, 17, 323–328. (4) of Sciences, 654, 19–32. (3) Golding, J., & the Avon Longitudinal Study of
He, S. M., Yang, A. K., Li, X. T., Du, Y. M., & Zhou, Herkenham, M., Lynn, A. B., de Costa, B. R., & Parents and Children Study Team. (2002). Tes-
S. F. (2010). Effects of herbal products on the Richfield, E. K. (1991). Neuronal localization of tosterone during pregnancy and gender role be-
metabolism and transport of anticancer agents. cannabinoid receptors in the basal ganglia of the havior of preschool children: A longitudinal,
Expert Opinion on Drug Metabolism & Toxicity, rat. Brain Research, 547, 267–274. (3) population study. Child Development, 73, 1678–
6, 1195–1213. (15) Hermans, E. J., Ramsey, N. F., & van Honk, J. 1687. (11)
He, W., Yasumatsu, K., Varadarajan, V., Yamada, A., (2008). Exogenous testosterone enhances re- Hippisley-Cox, J., Vinogradova, Y., Coupland, C., &
Lem, J., Ninomiya, Y., et al. (2004). Umami taste sponsiveness to social threat in the neural cir- Parker, C. (2007). Risk of malignancy in patients
receptors are mediated by a-transducin and a- cuitry of social aggression in humans. Biological with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. Archives
gustducin. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 7674– Psychiatry, 63, 263–270. (12) of General Psychiatry, 64, 1368–1376. (15)
7680. (7) Herrero, S. (1985). Bear attacks: Their causes and Hitchcock, J. M., & Davis, M. (1991). Efferent path-
Hebb, D. O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New avoidance. Piscataway, NJ: Winchester. (7) way of the amygdala involved in conditioned fear
York: Wiley. (13) Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lin- as measured with the fear-potentiated startle
Hegdé, J., & Van Essen, D. C. (2000). Selectivity for denberger, U. (2009). Enrichment effects on paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 826–
complex shapes in primate visual area V2. Journal adult cognitive development. Psychological Science 842. (12)
of Neuroscience, 20, RC61: 1–6. (6) in the Public Interest, 9, 1–65. (5) Hiyama, T. Y., Watanabe, E., Okado, H., & Noda,
Heims, H. C., Critchley, H. D., Dolan, R., Mathias, Herz, R. S., & Inzlicht, M. (2002). Sex differences M. (2004). The subfornical organ is the primary
C. J., & Cipolotti, L. (2004). Social and motiva- in response to physical and social factors involved locus of sodium-level sensing by Nax sodium
tional functioning is not critically dependent on in human mate selection: The importance of channels for the control of salt-intake behavior.
feedback of autonomic responses: Neuropsycho- smell for women. Evolution and Human Behavior, Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 9276–9281. (10)
logical evidence from patients with pure autonom- 23, 359–364. (7, 11) Hnasko, T. S., Sotak, B. N., & Palmiter, R. D.
ic failure. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1979–1988. (12) Herz, R. S., McCall, C., & Cahill, L. (1999). Hemi- (2005). Morphine reward in dopamine-deficient
Heisler, L. K., Cowley, M. A., Tecott, L. H., Fan, W., spheric lateralization in the processing of odor mice. Nature, 438, 854–857. (3)
Low, M. J., Smart, J. L., et al. (2002). Activation pleasantness versus odor names. Chemical Senses, Hobson, J. A. (1989). Sleep. New York: Scientific
of central melanocortin pathways by fenflura- 24, 691–695. (14) American Library. (15)
mine. Science, 297, 609–611. (10) Hess, B. J. M. (2001). Vestibular signals in self- Hobson, J. A. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming:
Helenius, P., Salmelin, R., Richardson, U., Lei- orientation and eye movement control. News in Towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature
nonen, S., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Abnormal au- Physiological Sciences, 16, 234–238. (7) Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 803–813. (9)
ditory cortical activation in dyslexia 100 msec Hesse, M. D., Thiel, C. M., Stephan, K. E., & Fink, Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain
after speech onset. Journal of Cognitive Neurosci- G. R. (2006). The left parietal cortex and motor as a dream state generator: An activation-synthe-
ence, 14, 603–617. (14) intention: An event-related functional magnetic sis hypothesis of the dream process. American
Heller, W., & Levy, J. (1981). Perception and expres- resonance imaging study. Neuroscience, 140, Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348. (9)
sion of emotion in right-handers and left-handers. 1209–1221. (8) Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R.
Neuropsychologia, 19, 263–272. (14) Hettema, J. M., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cog-
Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2003). (2001). A review and meta-analysis of the ge- nitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavior-
Global transsaccadic change blindness during netic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Ameri- al and Brain Sciences, 23, 793–1121. (9)
scene perception. Psychological Science, 14, 493– can Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1568–1578. (12) Hochberg, L. R., Serruya, M. D., Friehs, G. M., Mu-
497. (14) Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come kand, J. A., Saleh, M., Caplan, A. H., et al.
Hendry, S. H. C., & Reid, R. C. (2000). The konio- from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, (2006). Neuronal ensemble control of prosthetic
cellular pathway in primate vision. Annual Re- 575–583. (8) devices by a human with tetraplegia. Nature, 442,
view of Neuroscience, 23, 127–153. (6) Higgs, S., Williamson, A. C., Rotshtein, P., & Hum- 164–171. (8)
Hennig, J., Reuter, M., Netter, P., Burk, C., & Landt, phreys, G. W. (2008). Sensory-specific satiety is Hoebel, B. G., & Hernandez, L. (1993). Basic neu-
O. (2005). Two types of aggression are differen- intake in amnesics who eat multiple meals. Psy- ral mechanisms of feeding and weight regulation.
tially related to serotonergic activity and the chological Science, 19, 623–628. (13) In A. J. Stunkard & T. A. Wadden (Eds.), Obesi-
A779C TPH polymorphism. Behavioral Neuro- Higley, J. D., Mehlman, P. T., Higley, S. B., Fer- ty: Theory and therapy (2nd ed., pp. 43–62). New
science, 119, 16–25. (12) nald, B., Vickers, J., Lindell, S. G., et al. (1996). York: Raven Press. (10)
Hennig, R., & Lømo, T. (1985). Firing patterns of Excessive mortality in young free-ranging male Hoebel, B. G., Rada, P. V., Mark, G. P., & Pothos, E.
motor units in normal rats. Nature, 314, 164– nonhuman primates with low cerebrospinal (1999). Neural systems for reinforcement and
166. (8) fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentra- inhibition of behavior: Relevance to eating, ad-
Herdener, M., Esposito, F., di Salle, F., Boller, C., tions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 537– diction, and depression. In D. Kahneman, E.
Hilti, C. C., Habermeyer, B., et al. (2010). Musi- 543. (12) Diener, & N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: Foun-
References 513

dations of hedonic psychology (pp. 560–574). New Hoover, J. E., & Strick, P. L. (1993). Multiple output tion in early retinotopic cortex. Journal of Neuro-
York: Russell Sage Foundation. (10) channels in the basal ganglia. Science, 259, 819– physiology, 103, 1501–1507. (6)
Hoeft, F., Hernandez, A., McMillon, G., Taylor- 821. (8) Hu, P., Stylos-Allan, M., & Walker, M. P. (2006).
Hill, H., Martindale, J. L., Meyler, A., et al. Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Comparative and familial Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional de-
(2006). Neural basis of dyslexia: A comparison analysis of handedness in great apes. Psychological clarative memory. Psychological Science, 17, 891–
between dyslexic and nondyslexic children equat- Bulletin, 132, 538–559. (14) 898. (9)
ed for reading ability. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., & Cantalupo, C. Hu, W., Lee, H. L., Zhang, Q., Liu, T., Geng, L. B.,
10700–10708. (14) (2007). Neuroanatomical correlates of handed- Seghier, M. L., et al. (2010). Developmental dys-
Hoffer, A. (1973). Mechanism of action of nicotinic ness for tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). lexia in Chinese and English populations: Dis-
acid and nicotinamide in the treatment of schizo- Psychological Science, 18, 971–977. (14) sociating the effect of dyslexia from language dif-
phrenia. In D. Hawkins & L. Pauling (Eds.), Or- Hoptman, M. J., & Levy, J. (1988). Perceptual asym- ferences. Brain, 133, 1694–1706. (14)
thomolecular psychiatry (pp. 202–262). San Fran- metries in left- and right-handers for cartoon and Hua, J. Y., & Smith, S. J. (2004). Neural activity and
cisco: Freeman. (15) real faces. Brain and Cognition, 8, 178–188. (14) the dynamics of central nervous system develop-
Hoffman, P. L., Tabakoff, B., Szabó, G., Suzdak, P. Horne, J. A. (1992). Sleep and its disorders in chil- ment. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 327–332. (5)
D., & Paul, S. M. (1987). Effect of an imidazo- dren. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Huang, A. L., Chen, X., Hoon, M. A., Chan-
benzodiazepine, Ro15-4513, on the incoordi- Allied Disciplines, 33, 473–487. (9) drashekar, J., Guo, W., Tränker, D., et al. (2006).
nation and hypothermia produced by ethanol Horne, J. A., & Minard, A. (1985). Sleep and sleepi- The cells and logic for mammalian sour taste de-
and pento-barbital. Life Sciences, 41, 611–619. ness following a behaviourally “active” day. Ergo- tection. Nature, 442, 934–938. (7)
(12) nomics, 28, 567–575. (9) Huang, L., Treisman, A., & Pashler, H. (2007).
Hoffmann, F., & Curio, G. (2003). REM-Schlaf Horridge, G. A. (1962). Learning of leg position by Characterizing the limits of human visual aware-
und rezidivierende Erosio corneae—eine Hy- the ventral nerve cord in headless insects. Pro- ness. Science, 317, 823–825. (14)
pothese. [REM sleep and recurrent corneal ero- ceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 157, Huang, Y.-J., Maruyama, Y., Lu, K.-S., Pereira, E.,
sion—A hypothesis.] Klinische Monatsblatter für 33–52. (13) Plonsky, I., Baur, J. E., et al. (2005). Mouse taste
Augenheilkunde, 220, 51–53. (9) Horst, W. D., & Preskorn, S. H. (1998). Mecha- buds use serotonin as a neurotransmitter. Journal
Hökfelt, T., Johansson, O., & Goldstein, M. (1984). nisms of action and clinical characteristics of of Neuroscience, 25, 843–847. (3)
Chemical anatomy of the brain. Science, 225, three atypical antidepressants: Venlafaxine, ne- Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S.
1326–1334. (3) fazodone, bupropion. Journal of Affective Disor- (2005). Interactions between number and space
Holcombe, A. O., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). Early ders, 51, 237–254. (15) in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6,
binding of feature pairs for visual perception. Na- Horvath, T. L. (2005). The hardship of obesity: A 435–448. (4)
ture Neuroscience, 4, 127–128. (4) soft-wired hypothalamus. Nature Neuroscience, 8, Hubel, D. H. (1963, November). The visual cortex of
Hollister, J. M., Laing, P., & Mednick, S. A. (1996). 561–565. (10) the brain. Scientific American, 209(5), 54–62. (6)
Rhesus incompatibility as a risk factor for schizo- Hoshi, E., & Tanji, J. (2000). Integration of target and Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1959). Receptive
phrenia in male adults. Archives of General Psy- body-part information in the premotor cortex fields of single neurons in the cat’s striate cortex.
chiatry, 53, 19–24. (15) when planning action. Nature, 408, 466–470. (8) Journal of Physiology, 148, 574–591. (6)
Hollon, S. D., Jarrett, R. B., Nierenberg, A. A., Houk, C. P., & Lee, P. A. (2010). Approach to as- Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1965). Binocular
Thase, M. E., Trivedi, M., & Rush, A. J. (2005). signing gender in 46,XX congenital adrenal hy- interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with
Psychotherapy and medication in the treatment perplasia with male external genitalia: Replacing artificial squint. Journal of Neurophysiology, 28,
of adult and geriatric depression: Which mono- dogmatism with pragmatism. Journal of Clinical 1041–1059. (6)
therapy or combined treatment? Journal of Clini- Endocrinology & Metabolism, 95, 4501–4508. Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1977). Functional
cal Psychiatry, 66, 455–468. (15) (11) architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex.
Hollon, S. D., Thase, M. E., & Markowitz, J. C. Hovda, D. A., & Feeney, D. M. (1989). Amphet- Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 198,
(2002). Treatment and prevention of depression. amine-induced recovery of visual cliff perfor- 1–59. (6)
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 3, 39– mance after bilateral visual cortex ablation in cats: Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1998). Early ex-
77. (15) Measurements of depth perception thresholds. ploration of the visual cortex. Neuron, 20, 401–
Holmans, P., Weissman, M. M., Zubenko, G. S., Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, 574–584. (5) 412. (6)
Scheftner, W. A., Crowe, R. R., DePaulo, J. R. Jr., Howard, J. D., Plailly, J., Grueschow, M., Haynes, J.- Huber, R., Ghilardi, M. F., Massimini, M., & Tono-
et al. (2007). Genetics of recurrent early-onset D., & Gottfried, J. A. (2009). Odor quality coding ni, G. (2004). Local sleep and learning. Nature,
major depression (GenRED): Final genome and categorization in human posterior piriform 430, 78–81. (9)
scan report. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 932–938. (7) Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Kessler,
248–258. (15) Howell, S., Westergaard, G., Hoos, B., Chavanne, R. C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of
Holstege, G., Georgiadis, J. R., Paans, A. M. J., Mei- T. J., Shoaf, S. E., Cleveland, A., et al. (2007). Se- eating disorders in the National Comorbidity
ners, L. C., van der Graaf, F. H. C. E., & Reinders, rotonergic influences on life-history outcomes in Survey Replication. Biological Psychiatry, 61,
A. A. T. S. (2003). Brain activation during hu- free-ranging male rhesus macaques. American 348–358. (10)
man male ejaculation. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, Journal of Primatology, 69, 851–865. (12) Hudson, J. I., Mangweth, B., Pope, H. G., Jr., De Col,
9185–9193. (11) Howes, O. D., Montgomery, A. J., Asselin, M.-C., C., Hausmann, A., Gutweniger, S., et al. (2003).
Holy, T. E., Dulac, C., & Meister, M. (2000). Re- Murray, R. M., Valli, I., Tabraham, P., et al. Family study of affective spectrum disorder. Ar-
sponses of vomeronasal neurons to natural stim- (2009). Elevated striatal dopamine function chives of General Psychiatry, 60, 170–177. (15)
uli. Science, 289, 1569–1572. (7) linked to prodromal signs of schizophrenia. Ar- Hudspeth, A. J. (1985). The cellular basis of hear-
Homewood, J., & Stevenson, R. J. (2001). Differ- chives of General Psychiatry, 66, 13–20. (15) ing: The biophysics of hair cells. Science, 230,
ences in naming accuracy of odors presented to Hrdy, S. B. (2000). The optimal number of fathers. 745–752. (7)
the left and right nostrils. Biological Psychology, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907, Huey, E. D., Garcia, C., Wassermann, E. M., Tier-
58, 65–73. (14) 75–96. (11) ney, M. C., & Grafman, J. (2008). Stimulant
Honea, R., Crow, T. J., Passingham, D., & Mackay, Hróbjartsson, A., & Gøtzsche, P. C. (2001). Is the treatment of frontotemporal dementia in 8 pa-
C. E. (2005). Regional deficits in brain volume in placebo powerless? New England Journal of Medi- tients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69, 1981–
schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of voxel-based cine, 344, 1594–1602. (7) 1982. (5)
morphometry studies. American Journal of Psy- Hsieh, P.-J., Vul, E., & Kanwisher, N. (2010). Rec- Hugdahl, K. (1996). Brain laterality—Beyond the
chiatry, 162, 2233–2245. (15) ognition alters the spatial pattern of fMRI activa- basics. European Psychologist, 1, 206–220. (14)
514 References

Hughes, J. C., & Cook, C. C. H. (1997). The efficacy moderates craving after alcohol consumption. tion and the evolutionary genetics of lactase
of disulfiram: A review of outcome studies. Ad- Health Psychology, 21, 139–146. (3) persistence. Human Genetics, 124, 579–591.
diction, 92, 381–395. (3) Hyde, J. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. (10)
Hughes, J. R. (2007). Review of medical reports on American Psychologist, 60, 581–592. (4) Innocenti, G. M. (1980). The primary visual path-
pedophilia. Clinical Pediatrics, 46, 667–682. (11) Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, A. B., way through the corpus callosum: Morphological
Hull, E. M., & Dominguez, J. M. (2007). Sexual be- & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender similarities and functional aspects in the cat. Archives Ital-
havior in male rodents. Hormones and Behavior, characterize math performance. Science, 321, iennes de Biologie, 118, 124–188. (14)
52, 45–55. (11) 494–495. (4) Innocenti, G. M., & Caminiti, R. (1980). Postna-
Hull, E. M., Du, J., Lorrain, D. S., & Matuszewich, Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., tal shaping of callosal connections from senso-
L. (1997). Testosterone, preoptic dopamine, and Winner, E., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009a). Musical ry areas. Experimental Brain Research, 38, 381–
copulation in male rats. Brain Research Bulletin, training shapes structural brain development. 394. (14)
44, 327–333. (11) Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3019–3025. (5) Inouye, S. T., & Kawamura, H. (1979). Persistence
Hull, E. M., Eaton, R. C., Markowski, V. P., Moses, Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., of circadian rhythmicity in a mammalian hypo-
J., Lumley, L. A., & Loucks, J. A. (1992). Oppo- Winner, E., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009b). The ef- thalamic “island” containing the suprachiasmatic
site influence of medial preoptic D1 and D2 re- fects of musical training on structural brain devel- nucleus. Proceedings of the National Academy of
ceptors on genital reflexes: Implications for copu- opment: A longitudinal study. Annals of the New Sciences, USA, 76, 5962–5966. (9)
lation. Life Sciences, 51, 1705–1713. (11) York Academy of Sciences, 1169,182–186. (5) International Schizophrenia Consortium. (2009).
Hull, E. M., Lorrain, D. S., Du, J., Matuszewich, Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J. P., Zatorre, R. J., Griffiths, Common polygenic variation contributes to risk
L., Lumley, L. A., Putnam, S. K., et al. (1999). T. D., Evans, A. C., & Peretz, I. (2007). Cortical of schizophenia and bipolar disorder. Nature,
Hormone-neurotransmitter interactions in the thickness in congenital amusia: When less is bet- 460, 748–752. (15)
control of sexual behavior. Behavioural Brain Re- ter than more. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, Iqbal, K., & Grundke-Iqbal, I. (2010). Alzheimer’s
search, 105, 105–116. (11) 13028–13032. (7) disease, a multifactorial disorder seeking multi-
Hull, E. M., Nishita, J. K., Bitran, D., & Dalterio, S. Hyde, K. L., & Peretz, I. (2004). Brains that are out therapies. Alzheimers & Dementia, 6, 420–424.
(1984). Perinatal dopamine-related drugs de- of tune but in time. Psychological Science, 15, (13)
masculinize rats. Science, 224, 1011–1013. (11) 356–360. (7) Isoda, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2007). Switching from
Hull, R., & Vaid, J. (2007). Bilingual language lat- Hynd, G. W., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1989). automatic to controlled action by monkey medial
eralization: A meta-analytic tale of two hemi- Dyslexia and brain morphology. Psychological frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 240–248.
spheres. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1987–2008. Bulletin, 106, 447–482. (14) (8)
(14) Ibrahim, H. M., Hogg, A. J., Jr., Healy, D. J., Harou- Ito, M. (1984). The cerebellum and neural control.
Hulshoff, H. E., Schnack, H. G., Mandl, R. C. W., tunian, V., Davis, K. L., & Meador-Woodruff, New York: Raven Press. (8)
van Haren, N. E. M., Koning, H., Collins, L., et J. H. (2000). Ionotropic glutamate receptor bind- Ittner, L. M., & Götz, J. (2011). Amyloid-b and
al. (2001). Focal gray matter density changes in ing and subunit mRNA expression in thalamic tau—a toxic pas de deux in Alzheimer’s disease.
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, nuclei in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psy- Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 67–72. (13)
1118–1125. (15) chiatry, 157, 1811–1823. (15) Ivry, R. B., & Diener, H. C. (1991). Impaired veloc-
Hunsberger, J. G., Newton, S. S., Bennett, A. H., Iggo, A., & Andres, K. H. (1982). Morphology of ity perception in patients with lesions of the cer-
Duman, C. H., Russell, D. S., Salton, S. R., et al. cutaneous receptors. Annual Review of Neurosci- ebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3,
(2007). Antidepressant actions of the exercise- ence, 5, 1–31. (7) 355–366. (8)
regulated gene VGF. Nature Medicine, 13, 1476– Ikeda, H., Stark, J., Fischer, H., Wagner, M., Drdla, Ivy, G. O., & Killackey, H. P. (1981). The ontogeny
1482. (15) R., Jäger, T., et al. (2006). Synaptic amplifier of of the distribution of callosal projection neurons
Hunt, S. P., & Mantyh, P. W. (2001). The molecular inflammatory pain in the spinal dorsal horn. Sci- in the rat parietal cortex. Journal of Comparative
dynamics of pain control. Nature Reviews Neuro- ence, 312, 1659–1662. (7) Neurology, 195, 367–389. (14)
science, 2, 83–91. (7) Ikonomidou, C., Bittigau, P. Ishimaru, M. J., Woz- Iwema, C. L., Fang, H., Kurtz, D. B., Youngentob,
Hunter, W. S. (1923). General psychology (Rev. ed.). niak, D. F., Koch, C., Genz, K., et al. (2000). S. L., & Schwob, J. E. (2004). Odorant receptor
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (4) Ethanol-induced apoptotic neurodegeneration expression patterns are restored in lesion-recov-
Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Research and fetal alcohol syndrome. Science, 287, 1056– ered rat olfactory epithelium. Journal of Neurosci-
Group. (1993). A novel gene containing a trinu- 1060. (5) ence, 24, 356–369. (7)
cleotide repeat that is expanded and unstable on Imamura, K., Mataga, N., & Mori, K. (1992). Cod- Jablensky, A. V., Morgan, V., Zubrick, S. R., Bower,
Huntington’s disease chromosomes. Cell, 72, ing of odor molecules by mitral/tufted cells in C., & Yellachich, L.-A. (2005). Pregnancy, deliv-
971–983. (8) rabbit olfactory bulb: I. Aliphatic compounds. ery, and neonatal complications in a population
Hurovitz, C. S., Dunn, S., Domhoff, G. W., & Fiss, Journal of Neurophysiology, 68, 1986–2002. (7) cohort of women with schizophrenia and major
H. (1999). The dreams of blind men and women: Imayoshi, I., Sakamoto, M., Ohtsuka, T., Takao, K., affective disorders. American Journal of Psychia-
A replication and extension of previous findings. Miyakawa, T., Yamaguchi, M., et al. (2008). try, 162, 79–91. (15)
Dreaming, 9, 183–193. (6) Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the struc- Jacobs, B., & Scheibel, A. B. (1993). A quantitative
Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D. (1957). An opponent- tural and functional integrity of the adult fore- dendritic analysis of Wernicke’s area in humans:
process theory of color vision. Psychological Review, brain. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 1153–1161. (5) I. Lifespan changes. Journal of Comparative Neu-
64, 384–404. (6) Imel, Z. E., Malterer, M. B., McKay, K. M., & rology, 327, 83–96. (5)
Hutcheson, D. M., Everitt, B. J., Robbins, T. W., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). A meta-analysis of psy- Jacobs, G. D., & Snyder, D. (1996). Frontal brain
Dickinson, A. (2001). The role of withdrawal in chotherapy and medication in unipolar depres- asymmetry predicts affective style in men. Behav-
heroin addiction: Enhances reward or promotes sion and dysthymia. Journal of Affective Disorders, ioral Neuroscience, 110, 3-6. (15)
avoidance? Nature Neuroscience, 4, 943–947. (3) 110, 197–206. (15) Jacobs, G. H., Williams, G. A., Cahill, H., & Na-
Hutchison, K. E., LaChance, H., Niaura, R., Bryan, Imperato-McGinley, J., Guerrero, L., Gautier, T., & thans, J. (2007). Emergence of novel color vision
A., & Smolen, A. (2002). The DRD4 VNTR Peterson, R. E. (1974). Steroid 5 alpha-reductase in mice engineered to express a human cone
polymorphism influences reactivity to smoking deficiency in man: An inherited form of male photopigment. Science, 315, 1723–1725. (6)
cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 134– pseudohermaphroditism. Science, 186, 1213– James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9,
143. (3) 1215. (11) 188–205. (12)
Hutchison, K. E., McGeary, J., Smolen, A., & Bryan, Ingram, C. J. E., Mulcare, C. A., Itan, Y., Thomas, James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion.
A. (2002). The DRD4 VNTR polymorphism M. G., & Swallow, D. M. (2009). Lactose diges- Psychological Review, 1, 516–529. (12)
References 515

James, W. (1961). Psychology: The briefer course. Jones, A. R., & Shusta, E. V. (2007). Blood–brain mediating fear and anxiety in the primate. Journal
New York: Harper. (Original work published barrier transport of therapeutics via receptor- of Neuroscience, 24, 5506–5515. (12)
1892) (12) mediation. Pharmaceutical Research, 24, 1759– Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., Davidson, R. J., & Kel-
Jameson, K. A., Highnote, S. M., & Wasserman, 1771. (2) ley, A. E. (2001). The primate amygdala mediates
L. M. (2001). Richer color experience in observ- Jones, C. R., Campbell, S. S., Zone, S. E., Cooper, F., acute fear but not the behavioral and physiologi-
ers with multiple photopigment opsin genes. Psy- DeSano, A., Murphy, P. J., et al. (1999). Familial cal components of anxious temperament. Journal
chonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 244–261. (6) advanced sleep-phase syndrome: A short-period of Neuroscience, 21, 2067–2074. (12)
Jäncke, L., Beeli, G., Eulig, C., & Hänggi, J. (2009). circadian rhythm variant in humans. Nature Kamitani, Y., & Tong, F. (2005). Decoding the vi-
The neuroanatomy of grapheme-color synesthe- Medicine, 5, 1062–1065. (9) sual and subjective contents of the human brain.
sia. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1287– Jones, E. G., & Pons, T. P. (1998). Thalamic and Nature Neuroscience, 8, 679–685. (14)
1293. (7) brainstem contributions to large-scale plasticity Kandel, E. R., & Schwartz, J. H. (1982). Molecular
Jarrard, L. E., Okaichi, H., Steward, O., & Gold- of primate somatosensory cortex. Science, 282, biology of learning: Modulation of transmitter
schmidt, R. B. (1984). On the role of hippocampal 1121–1125. (5) release. Science, 218, 433–443. (13)
connections in the performance of place and cue Jones, H. S., & Oswald, I. (1968). Two cases of Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein,
tasks: Comparisons with damage to hippocam- healthy insomnia. Electroencephalography and G., Mcclure, S. M., Wang, J. T., et al. (2009). The
pus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 946–954. (13) Clinical Neurophysiology, 24, 378–380. (9) wick in the candle of learning. Psychological Sci-
Jenner, A. R., Rosen, G. D., & Galaburda, A. M. Jones, P. B., Barnes, T. R. E., Davies, L., Dunn, G., ence, 8, 963–973. (13)
(1999). Neuronal asymmetries in primary visual Lloyd, H., Hayhurst, K. P., et al. (2006). Ran- Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the
cortex of dyslexic and nondyslexic brains. Annals domized controlled trial of the effect on quality human brain: A window into the functional ar-
of Neurology, 46, 189–196. (14) of life of second- vs. first-generation antipsychot- chitecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National
Jerison, H. J. (1985). Animal intelligence as en- ic drugs in schizophrenia. Archives of General Academy of Sciences, 107, 11163–11170. (6)
cephalization. Philosophical Transactions of the Psychiatry, 63, 1079–1087. (15) Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform
Royal Society of London, B, 308, 21–35. (4) Jordan, H. A. (1969). Voluntary intragastric feed- face area: A cortical region specialized for the
Ji, D., & Wilson, M. A. (2007). Coordinated memory ing. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of
replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus dur- chology, 62, 237–244. (10) the Royal Society, B, 361, 2109–2128. (6)
ing sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 100–107. (9) Jouvet, M. (1960). Telencephalic and rhombence- Kaplan, J. R., Muldoon, M. F., Manuck, S. B., &
Jiang, J., Zhu, W., Shi, F., Liu, Y., Qin, W., Li, K., et phalic sleep in the cat. In G. E. W. Wolstenholme Mann, J. J. (1997). Assessing the observed rela-
al. (2009). Thick visual cortex in the early blind. & M. O’Connor (Eds.), CIBA Foundation sympo- tionship between low cholesterol and violence-
Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 2205–2211. (5) sium on the nature of sleep (pp. 188–208). Boston: related mortality. Annals of the New York Acade-
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., & He, S. (2007). Processing of Little, Brown. (9) my of Sciences, 836, 57–80. (12)
invisible stimuli. Psychological Science, 18, 349– Joyner, A. L., & Guillemot, F. (1994). Gene target- Kapur, S., Zipusky, R., Jones, C., Shammi, C. S.,
355. (14) ing and development of the nervous system. Cur- Remington, G., & Seeman, P. (2000). A positron
Jin, X., & Costa, R. M. (2010). Start/stop signals rent Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 37–42. (4) emission tomography study of quetiapine in
emerge in nigrostriatal circuits during sequence Judge, J., Caravolas, M., & Knox, P. C. (2006). schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57,
learning. Nature, 466, 457–462. (8) Smooth pursuit eye movements and phonologi- 553–559. (15)
Johanek, L. M., Meyer, R. A., Hartke, T., Hobel- cal processing in adults with dyslexia. Cognitive Karama, S., Ad-Dab’bagh, Y., Haier, R. J., Deary, I.
mann, J. G., Maine, D. N., LaMotte, R. H., et al. Neuropsychology, 23, 1174–1189. (14) J., Lyttelton, O. C., Lepage, C., et al. (2009). Pos-
(2007). Psychophysical and physiological evi- Jueptner, M., & Weiller, C. (1998). A review of dif- itive association between cognitive ability and
dence for parallel afferent pathways mediating ferences between basal ganglia and cerebellar cortical thickness in a representative US sample
the sensation of itch. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, control of movements as revealed by functional of healthy 6 to 18 year olds. Intelligence, 37, 145–
7490–7497. (7) imaging studies. Brain, 121, 1437–1449. (8) 155. (4)
Johansson, C., Willeit, M., Smedh, C., Ekholm, J., Juvonen, H., Reunanen, A., Haukka, J., Muhonen, Kargo, W. J., & Nitz, D. A. (2004). Improvements
Paunio, T., Kieseppä, T., et al. (2003). Circadi- M., Suvisari, J., Arajärvi, R., et al. (2007). Inci- in the signal-to-noise ratio of motor cortex cells
an clock-related polymorphisms in seasonal af- dence of schizophrenia in a nationwide cohort of distinguish early versus late phases of motor
fective disorder and their relevance to diurnal patients with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Archives of skill learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5560–
preference. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 734– General Psychiatry, 64, 894–899. (15) 5569. (8)
739. (15) Kaas, J. H. (1983). What, if anything, is SI? Organi- Karlsson, M., & Frank, L. M. (2009). Awake replay
Johnson, E. L., Tranel, D., Lutgendorf, S., & zation of first somatosensory area of cortex. of remote experiences in the hippocampus. Na-
Adolphs, R. (2009). A neuroanatomical dissocia- Physiological Reviews, 63, 206–231. (7) ture Neuroscience, 12, 913–918. (9)
tion for emotion induced by music. International Kaas, J. H., Merzenich, M. M., & Killackey, H. P. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Tyler, L. K., Voice, K., Sims,
Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 24–33. (12) (1983). The reorganization of somatosensory K., Udwin, O., Howlin, P., et al. (1998). Linguis-
Johnson, L. C. (1969). Physiological and psycho- cortex following peripheral nerve damage in tic dissociations in Williams syndrome: Evaluat-
logical changes following total sleep deprivation. adult and developing mammals. Annual Review ing receptive syntax in on-line and off-line tasks.
In A. Kales (Ed.), Sleep: Physiology & pathology of Neuroscience, 6, 325–356. (5) Neuropsychologia, 36, 343–351. (14)
(pp. 206–220). Philadelphia: Lippincott. (9) Kaas, J. H., Nelson, R. J., Sur, M., Lin, C.-S., & Mer- Karnath, H. O., Rüter, J., Mandler, A., & Himmel-
Johnson, M. T. V., Kipnis, A. N., Coltz, J. D., Gupta, zenich, M. M. (1979). Multiple representations of bach, M. (2009). The anatomy of object recogni-
A., Silverstein, P., Zwiebel, F., et al. (1996). Ef- the body within the primary somatosensory cor- tion: Visual form agnosia caused by nedial oc-
fects of levodopa and viscosity on the velocity tex of primates. Science, 204, 521–523. (4) cipitotemporal stroke. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
and accuracy of visually guided tracking in Par- Kaiser, A., Haller, S., Schmitz, S., & Nitsch, C. 5854–5862. (6)
kinson’s disease. Brain, 119, 801–813. (8) (2009). On sex/gender related similarities and Karrer, T., & Bartoshuk, L. (1991). Capsaicin de-
Johnson, P. M., & Kenny, P. J. (2010). Dopamine D2 differences in fMRI language research. Brain Re- sensitization and recovery on the human tongue.
receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction search Reviews, 61, 49-59. (11) Physiology & Behavior, 49, 757–764. (7)
and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Neu- Kales, A., Scharf, M. B., & Kales, J. D. (1978). Re- Kas, M. J. H., Tiesjema, B., van Dijk, G., Garner,
roscience, 13, 635–641. (10) bound insomnia: A new clinical syndrome. Sci- K. M., Barsh, G. S., Ter Brake, O., et al. (2004).
Jonas, P., Bischofberger, J., & Sandkühler, J. (1998). ence, 201, 1039–1041. (9) Induction of brain region-specific forms of obe-
Corelease of two fast neurotransmitters at a cen- Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). sity by agouti. Journal of Neuroscience, 24,
tral synapse. Science, 281, 419–424. (3) The role of the central nucleus of the amygdala in 10176–10181. (10)
516 References

Kasai, K., Shenton, M. E., Salisbury, D. F., Hiraya- Kennard, C., Lawden, M., Morland, A. B., & Rud- adverse life events. Archives of General Psychiatry,
su, Y., Onitsuka, T., Spencer, M. H., et al. (2003). dock, K. H. (1995). Colour identification and 65, 146–152. (15)
Progressive decrease of left Heschl gyrus and pla- colour constancy are impaired in a patient with Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1993). Mind and
num temporale gray matter volume in first- incomplete achromatopsia associated with pres- immunity. In D. Goleman & J. Gurin (Eds.),
episode schizophrenia. Archives of General Psy- triate cortical lesions. Proceedings of the Royal So- Mind/body medicine (pp. 39–61). Yonkers, NY:
chiatry, 60, 766–775. (15) ciety of London, B, 260, 169–175. (6) Consumer Reports Books. (12)
Kasper, S., Caraci, F., Forti, B., Drago, F., & Aguglia, Kennaway, D. J., & Van Dorp, C. F. (1991). Free- Kilgour, A. R., de Gelder, B., & Lederman, S. J.
E. (2010). Efficacy and tolerability of Hypericum running rhythms of melatonin, cortisol, electro- (2004). Haptic face recognition and prosopagno-
extract for the treatment of mild to moderate de- lytes, and sleep in humans in Antarctica. Ameri- sia. Neuropsychologia, 42, 707–712. (6)
pression. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, can Journal of Physiology, 260, R1137–R1144. Killackey, H. P., & Chalupa, L. M. (1986). Ontoge-
747–765. (15) (9) netic change in the distribution of callosal projec-
Katkin, E. S., Wiens, S., & Öhman, A. (2001). Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Impaired tion neurons in the postcentral gyrus of the fetal
Nonconscious fear conditioning, visceral percep- fixation to eyes following amygdala damage arises rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
tion, and the development of gut feelings. Psycho- from abnormal bottom-up attention. Neuropsy- 244, 331–348. (14)
logical Science, 12, 366–370. (12) chologia, 48, 3392–3398. (12) Killeffer, F. A., & Stern, W. E. (1970). Chronic ef-
Kavanau, J. L. (1998). Vertebrates that never sleep: Kennedy, D. P., Gläscher, J., Tyszka, J. M., & fects of hypothalamic injury. Archives of Neurolo-
Implications for sleep’s basic function. Brain Re- Adolphs, R. (2009). Personal space regulation by gy, 22, 419–429. (10)
search Bulletin, 46, 269–279. (9) the human amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12, Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J.,
Kawasaki, H., Adolphs, R., Oya, H., Kovach, C., 1226–1227. (12) & Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of mirror neu-
Damasio, H., Kaufman, O., et al. (2005). Analy- Kennerley, S. W., Diedrichsen, J., Hazeltine, E., rons in human inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of
sis of single-unit responses to emotional scenes Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2002). Callosotomy Neuroscience, 29, 10153–10159. (8)
in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal patients exhibit temporal uncoupling during con- Kim, M. J., & Whalen, P. J. (2009). The structural
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1509–1518. (12) tinuous bimanual movements. Nature Neurosci- integrity of an amygdala-prefrontal pathway pre-
Kay, K. N., Naselaris, T., Prenger, R. J., & Gallant, ence, 5, 376–381. (14) dicts trait anxiety. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
J. L. (2008). Identifying natural images from hu- Kennett, S., Eimer, M., Spence, C., & Driver, J. 11614–11618. (12)
man brain activity. Nature, 452, 352–355. (4) (2001). Tactile-visual links in exogenous spatial Kim, U., Jorgenson, E., Coon, H., Leppert, M.,
Kee, N., Teixeira, C. M., Wang, A. H., & Frankland, attention under different postures: Convergent Risch, N., & Drayna, D. (2003). Positional clon-
P. W. (2007). Preferential incorporation of adult- evidence from psychophysics and ERPs. Journal ing of the human quantitative trait locus under-
generated granule cells into spatial memory net- of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 462–478. (14) lying taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide.
works in the dentate gyrus. Nature Neuroscience, Kenny, P. J., Chen, S. A., Kitamura, O., Markou, A., Science, 299, 1221–1225. (7)
10, 355–362. (5) & Koob, G. F. (2006). Conditioned withdrawal Kim, Y.-K., Lee, H.-J., Yang, J.-C., Hwang, J.-A., &
Keefe, R. S. E., Silverman, J. M., Mohs, R. C., Siever, drives heroin consumption and decreases reward Yoon, H.-K. (2009). A tryptophan hydroxylase 2
L. J., Harvey, P. D., Friedman, L., et al. (1997). sensitivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 5894– gene polymorphism is associated with panic dis-
Eye tracking, attention, and schizotypal symp- 5900. (3) order. Behavior Genetics, 39, 170–175. (12)
toms in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with Kerchner, G. A., & Nicoll, R. A. (2008). Silent syn- Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Be-
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, apses and the emergence of a postsynaptic mech- yond extinction: Erasing human fear responses
169–176. (15) anism for LTP. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuro-
Keele, S. W., & Ivry, R. (1990). Does the cerebellum 813–825. (13) science, 12, 256–258. (12)
provide a common computation for diverse Kerns, J. G. (2007). Experimental manipulation King, B. M. (2006). The rise, fall, and resurrection of
tasks? Annals of the New York Academy of Scienc- of cognitive control processes causes an in- the ventromedial hypothalamus in the regulation
es, 608, 179–207. (8) crease in communication disturbances in of feeding behavior and body weight. Physiology
Kelly, B. D., O’Callaghan, E., Waddington, J. L., Fee- healthy volunteers. Psychological Medicine, 37, & Behavior, 87, 221–244. (10)
ney, L., Browne, S., Scully, P. J., et al. (2010). 995–1004. (15) King, B. M., Smith, R. L., & Frohman, L. A. (1984).
Schizophrenia and the city: A review of litera- Kerr, J. N. D., & Denk, W. (2008). Imaging in vivo: Hyperinsulinemia in rats with ventromedial hy-
ture and prospective study of psychosis and urba- Watching the brain in action. Nature Reviews pothalamic lesions: Role of hyperphagia. Behav-
nicity in Ireland. Schizophrenia Research, 116, Neuroscience, 9, 195–205. (4) ioral Neuroscience, 98, 152–155. (10)
75–89. (15) Keshavan, M. S., Diwadkar, V. A., Montrose, D. M., King, V. R., Huang, W. L., Dyall, S. C., Curran,
Kelly, T. L., Neri, D. F., Grill, J. T., Ryman, D., Hunt, Rajarethinam, R., & Sweeney, J. A. (2005). Pre- O. E., Priestley, J. V., & Michael-Titus, A. T.
P. D., Dijk, D.-J., et al. (1999). Nonentrained cir- morbid indicators and risk for schizophrenia: A (2006). Omega-3 fatty acids improve recovery,
cadian rhythms of melatonin in submariners selective review and update. Schizophrenia Re- whereas omega-6 fatty acids worsen outcome af-
scheduled to an 18-hour day. Journal of Biological search, 79, 45–57. (15) ter spinal cord injury in the adult rat. Journal of
Rhythms, 14, 190–196. (9) Kesner, R. P., Gilbert, P. E., & Barua, L. A. (2002). Neuroscience, 26, 4672–4680. (15)
Kendler, K. S. (1983). Overview: A current per- The role of the hippocampus in meaning for the Kingstone, A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). Sub-
spective on twin studies of schizophrenia. Ameri- temporal order of a sequence of odors. Behavioral cortical transfer of higher order information:
can Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 1413–1425. (15) Neuroscience, 116, 286–290. (13) More illusory than real? Neuropsychology, 9,
Kendler, K. S. (2001). Twin studies of psychiatric Kety, S. S., Wender, P. H., Jacobson, B., Ingraham, 321–328. (14)
illness. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 1005– L. J., Jansson, L., Faber, B., et al. (1994). Mental Kinnamon, J. C. (1987). Organization and innerva-
1014. (1) illness in the biological and adoptive relatives of tion of taste buds. In T. E. Finger & W. L. Silver
Kendler, K. S., Fiske, A., Gardner, C. O., & Gatz, M. schizophrenic adoptees. Archives of General Psy- (Eds.), Neurobiology of taste and smell (pp. 277–
(2009). Delineation of two genetic pathways to chiatry, 51, 442–455. (15) 297). New York: Wiley. (7)
major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 808– Keverne, E. B. (1999). The vomeronasal organ. Sci- Kinomura, S., Larsson, J., Gulyás, B., & Roland, P.
811. (15) ence, 286, 716–720. (7) E. (1996). Activation by attention of the human
Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., & Prescott, C. A. Khashau, A. S., Abel, K. M., McNamee, R., Peder- reticular formation and thalamic intralaminar
(1999). Clinical characteristics of major depres- sen, M. G., Webb, R. T., Baker, P. N., et al. nuclei. Science, 271, 512–515. (9)
sion that predict risk of depression in relatives. (2008). Higher risk of offspring schizophrenia Kinsbourne, M., & McMurray, J. (1975). The effect
Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 322–327. (15) following antenatal maternal exposure to severe of cerebral dominance on time sharing between
References 517

speaking and tapping by preschool children. versus learned odour processing in the mouse ol- structure and cognitive map of Williams syn-
Child Development, 46, 240–242. (14) factory bulb. Nature, 450, 503–508. (7) drome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Sup-
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. Kobett, P., Paulitsch, S., Goebel, M., Stengel, A., pl.), 89–107. (14)
(1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. Phila- Schmidtmann, M., van der Voort, I. R., et al. Korman, M., Doyon, J., Doljansky, J., Carrier, J., Da-
delphia: Saunders. (11) (2006). Peripheral injection of CCK-8S induc- gan, Y., & Karni, A. (2007). Daytime sleep con-
Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & es Fos expression in the dorsomedial hypotha- denses the time course of motor memory consoli-
Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the hu- lamic nucleus in rats. Brain Research, 1117, dation. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1206–1213. (9)
man female. Philadelphia: Saunders. (11) 109–117. (10) Kornhuber, H. H. (1974). Cerebral cortex, cerebel-
Kippenhan, J. S., Olsen, R. K., Mervis, C. B., Morris, Kodituwakku, P. W. (2007). Defining the behavioral lum, and basal ganglia: An introduction to their
C. A., Kohn, P., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., et al. phenotype in children with fetal alcohol spec- motor functions. In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Wor-
(2005). Genetic contributions to human gyrifica- trum disorders: A review. Neuroscience & Biobe- den (Eds.), The neurosciences: Third study program
tion: Sulcal morphometry in Williams syndrome. havioral Reviews, 31, 192–201. (5) (pp. 267–280). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (8)
Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 7840–7846. (14) Koenigs, M., Huey, E. D., Raymont, V., Cheon, B., Korpi, E. R., & Sinkkonen, S. T. (2006). GABAA
Kiriakakis, V., Bhatia, K. P., Quinn, N. P., & Mars- Solomon, J., Wassermann, E. M., et al. (2008). Fo- receptor subtypes as targets for neuropsychiatric
den, C. D. (1998). The natural history of tardive cal brain damage protects against post-traumatic drug development. Pharmacology & Therapeutics,
dyskinesia: A long-term follow-up study of 107 stress disorder in combat veterans. Nature Neuro- 109, 12–32. (12)
cases. Brain, 121, 2053–2066. (15) science, 11, 232–237. (12) Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher,
Kirkpatrick, P. J., Smielewski, P., Czosnyka, M., Me- Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in
non, D. K., & Pickard, J. D. (1995). Near-infrared Cushman, F., Hauser, M., et al. (2007). Damage humans. Nature, 435, 673–676. (11)
spectroscopy in patients with head injury. Journal to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian mor- Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L.
of Neurosurgery, 83, 963–970. (5) al judgments. Nature, 446, 908–911. (12) (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature
Kirsch, I. (2010). The Emperor’s New Drugs. New Koepp, M. J., Gunn, R. N., Lawrence, A. D., Cun- Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635–642. (6)
York: Basic Books. (15) ningham, V. J., Dagher, A., Jones, T., et al. (1998). Kosslyn, S. M., & Thompson, W. L. (2003). When is
Kirsch, I., Deacon, B. J., Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sco- Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a early visual cortex activated during visual mental
boria, A., Moore, T. J., & Johnson, B. T. (2008). video game. Nature, 393, 266–268. (3) imagery? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 723–746. (6)
Initial severity and antidepressant benefits: A me- Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Kostrzewa, R. M., Kostrzewa, J. P., Brown, R. W.,
ta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing Nowak, P., & Brus, R. (2008). Dopamine recep-
Drug Administration. PLoS Medicine, 5, e45. (15) sounds, understanding actions: Action representa- tor supersensitivity: Development, mechanisms,
Kleen, J. K., Sitomer, M. T., Killeen, P. R., & Con- tion in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–848. (8) presentation, and clinical applicability. Neurotox-
rad, C. D. (2006). Chronic stress impairs spatial Kohn, M. (2008). The needs of the many. Nature, icity Research, 14, 121–128. (5)
memory and motivation for reward without dis- 456, 296–299. (1) Kotowicz, Z. (2007). The strange case of Phineas
rupting motor ability and motivation to explore. Komisaruk, B. R., Adler, N. T., & Hutchison, J. Gage. History of the Human Sciences, 20, 115–
Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 842–851. (12) (1972). Genital sensory field: Enlargement by 131. (12)
Klein, D. F. (1993). False suffocation alarms, spon- estrogen treatment in female rats. Science, 178, Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Activation in
taneous panics, and related conditions. Archives 1295–1298. (11) human MT/MST by static images with implied
of General Psychiatry, 50, 306–317. (11) Komorowski, R. W., Manns, J. R., & Eichenbaum, motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 48-
Klein, D. N. (2010). Chronic depression: Diagnosis H. (2009). Robust conjunctive item-lace coding 55. (6)
and classification. Current Directions in Psycho- by hippocampal neurons parallels learning what Kraemer, D. J. M., Macrae, C. N., Green, A. E., &
logical Science, 19, 96–100. (15) happens where. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, Kelley, W. M. (2005). Sound of silence activates
Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and wakefulness (Rev. 9918–9929. (13) auditory cortex. Nature, 434, 158. (7)
ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (9) Komura, Y., Tamura, R., Uwano, T., Nishijo, H., Krajbich, I., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Denburg,
Kluger, M. J. (1991). Fever: Role of pyrogens and Kaga, K., & Ono, T. (2001). Retrospective and N. L., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). Economic games
cryogens. Physiological Reviews, 71, 93–127. (10) prospective coding for predicted reward in the quantify diminished sense of guilt in patients
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1939). Preliminary analy- sensory thalamus. Nature, 412, 546–549. (4) with damage to the prefrontal cortex. Journal of
sis of functions of the temporal lobes in mon- Kondo, I., Marvizon, J. C. G., Song, B., Salgado, F., Neuroscience, 29, 2188–2192. (12)
keys. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 42, Codeluppi, S., Hua, X.-Y., et al. (2005). Inhibition Krakauer, A. H. (2005). Kin selection and cooperative
979–1000. (4) by spinal m- and d-opioid agonists of afferent- courtship in wild turkeys. Nature, 434, 69–72. (1)
Knabl, J., Witschi, R., Hösl, K., Reinold, H., Zeil- evoked substance P release. Journal of Neuroscience, Krause, E. G., & Sakal, R. R. (2007). Richter and
hofer, U. B., Ahmadi, S., et al. (2008). Reversal of 25, 3651–3660. (7) sodium appetite: From adrenalectomy to molec-
pathological pain through specific spinal GAB- Kong, J., Shepel, P. N., Holden, C. P., Mackiewicz, ular biology. Appetite, 49, 353–367. (10)
AA receptor subtypes. Nature, 451,330–334. (7) M., Pack, A. I., & Geiger, J. D. (2002). Brain gly- Kreek, M. J., Nielsen, D. A., Butelman, H. R., & La-
Knoll, J. (1993). The pharmacological basis of the cogen decreases with increased periods of wake- Forge, K. S. (2005). Genetic influences on impul-
beneficial effects of (2) deprenyl (selegiline) in fulness: Implications for homeostatic drive to sivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulner-
Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases. Journal of sleep. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 5581–5587. (9) ability to drug abuse and addiction. Nature Neu-
Neural Transmission (Suppl. 40), 69–91. (8) Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Kameyama, roscience, 8, 1450–1457. (3)
Knyazev, G. G., Slobodskaya, H. R., & Wilson, M., Nakahara, K., Sekihara, K., et al. (1998). Kreiman, G., Fried, I., & Koch, C. (2002). Single-
G. D. (2002). Psychophysiological correlates of Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex neuron correlates of subjective vision in the hu-
behavioural inhibition and activation. Personality during cognitive set shifting. Nature Neuroscience, man medial temporal lobe. Proceedings of the Na-
and Individual Differences, 33, 647–660. (12) 1, 80–84. (15) tional Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.), 99, 8378–
Ko, C.-H., Liu, G.-C., Hsiao, S., Yen, J.-Y., Yang, Konopka, G., Bomar, J. M., Winden, K., Coppola, 8383. (14)
M.-J., Lin, W.-C., et al. (2009). Brain activities G., Jonsson, Z. O., Gao, F., et al. (2009). Hu- Kreitzer, A. C., & Malenka, R. C. (2007). Endocan-
associated with gaming urge of online gaming man specific transcriptional regulation of CNS nabinoid-mediated rescue of striatal LTD and
addiction. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, development genes by FOXP2. Nature, 462, motor deficits in Parkinson’s disease models. Na-
739–747. (3) 213–217. (1, 14) ture, 445, 643–647. (8)
Kobayakawa, K., Kobayakawa, R., Matsumoto, H., Korenberg, J. R., Chen, X.-N., Hirota, H., Lai, Z., Kreitzer, A. C., & Regehr, W. G. (2001). Retrograde
Oka, Y., Imai, T., Ikawa, M., et al. (2007). Innate Bellugi, U., Burian, D., et al. (2000). VI. Genome inhibition of presynaptic calcium influx by en-
518 References

dogenous cannabinoids at excitatory synapses Kumaran, D., & Maguire, E. A. (2005). The human tive study of 45,850 twins and relatives on two
onto Purkinje cells. Neuron, 29, 717–727. (3) hippocampus: Cognitive maps or relational continents. Behavior Genetics, 30, 223–233. (1)
Kriegeskorte, N., Formisano, E., Sorger, B., & Goe- memory? Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 7254– Lalancette-Hébert, M., Gowing, G., Simard, A.,
bel, R. (2007). Individual faces elicit distinct re- 7259. (13) Weng, Y. C., & Kriz, J. (2007). Selective ablation
sponse patterns in human anterior temporal cor- Kumpik, D. P., Kacelnik, O., & King, A. J. (2010). of proliferating microglial cells exacerbates isch-
tex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci- Adaptive reweighting of auditory localization emic injury in the brain. Journal of Neuroscience,
ences (U.S.A.), 104, 20600–20605. (6) cues in response to chronic unilateral earplug- 30, 2596–2605. (5)
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofron- ging in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, Lambie, J. A., & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness
tal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. 4883–4894. (7) and the varieties of emotion experience: A theo-
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 691–702. (12) Kundermann, B., Hemmeter-Spernal, J., Huber, retical framework. Psychological Review, 109,
Kripke, D. F. (1998). Light treatment for nonsea- M. T., Krieg, J.-C., & Lautenbacher, S. (2008). 219–259. (14)
sonal depression: Speed, efficacy, and combined Effects of total sleep deprivation in major depres- Land, E. H., Hubel, D. H., Livingstone, M. S., Perry,
treatment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 49, 109– sion: Overnight improvement of mood is accom- S. H., & Burns, M. M. (1983). Colour-generating
117. (15) panied by increased pain sensitivity and aug- interactions across the corpus callosum. Nature,
Krishnan, V., Han, M.-H., Graham, D. L., Berton, mented pain complaints. Psychosomatic Medicine, 303, 616–618. (6)
O., Renthal, W., Russo, S. J., et al. (2007). Mo- 70, 92–101. (15) Landis, D. M. D. (1987). Initial junctions between
lecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and Kupfermann, I., Castellucci, V., Pinsker, H., & developing parallel fibers and Purkinje cells are
resistance to social defeat in brain reward re- Kandel, E. (1970). Neuronal correlates of ha- different from mature synaptic junctions. Journal
gions. Cell, 131, 391–404. (12) bituation and dishabituation of the gill with- of Comparative Neurology, 260, 513–525. (3)
Krishnan-Sarin, S., Krystal, J. H., Shi, J., Pittman, drawal reflex in Aplysia. Science, 167, 1743– Långström, N., Rahman, Q., Carlström, E., & Lich-
B., & O’Malley, S. S. (2007). Family history of 1745. (13) tenstein, P. (2010). Genetic and environmental
alcoholism influences naloxone-induced reduc- Kusunoki, M., Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2006). Ef- effects on same-sex sexual behavior: A popula-
tion in alcohol drinking. Biological Psychiatry, 62, fect of background colors on the tuning of color- tion study of twins in Sweden. Archives of Sexual
694–697. (3) selective cells in monkey area V4. Journal of Neuro- Behavior, 39, 75–80. (11)
Krueger, J. M., Rector, D. M., Roy, S., Van Dongen, physiology, 95, 3047–3059. (6) Lara, A. H., Kennerley, S. W., & Wallis, J. D. (2009).
H. P. A., Belenky, G., & Panksepp, J. (2008). Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1989). Motor system organi- Encoding of gustatory working memory by orbi-
Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal as- zation. In G. Adelman (Ed.), Neuroscience year tofrontal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
semblies. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 910– (pp. 107–110). Boston: Birkhäuser. (8) 765–774. (7)
919. (9) Kwon, J. S., McCarley, R. W., Hirayasu, Y., Ander- Larsen, R. J., Kasimatis, M., & Frey, K. (1992). Facili-
Krugers, H. J., Hoogenraad, C. C., & Groc, L. son, J. E., Fischer, I. A., Kikinis, R., et al. (1999). tating the furrowed brow—An unobtrusive test of
(2010). Stress hormones and AMPA receptor Left planum temporale volume reduction in the facial feedback hypothesis applied to unpleas-
trafficking in synaptic plasticity and memory. Na- schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, ant affect. Cognition & Emotion, 6, 321–338. (12)
ture Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 675–681. (12) 142–148. (15) Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelli-
Krupa, D. J., Thompson, J. K., & Thompson, R. F. Kwon, J.-T., & Choi, J. S. (2009). Cornering the fear gence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (13)
(1993). Localization of a memory trace in the engram: Long-term synaptic changes in the lat- Lashley, K. S. (1930). Basic neural mechanisms in
mammalian brain. Science, 260, 989–991. (13) eral nucleus of the amygdala after fear condition- behavior. Psychological Review, 37, 1–24. (13)
Kuba, H., Ishii, T. M., & Ohmori, H. (2006). Axonal ing. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9700–9703. (12) Lashley, K. S. (1950). In search of the engram. Sym-
site of spike initiation enhances auditory coinci- Laburn, H. P. (1996). How does the fetus cope with posia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4,
dence detection. Nature, 444, 1069–1072. (2) thermal challenges? News in Physiological Scienc- 454–482. (13)
Kubista, H., & Boehm, S. (2006). Molecular mech- es, 11, 96–100. (5, 15) Lassonde, M., Bryden, M. P., & Demers, P. (1990).
anisms underlying the modulation of exocytotic Lacroix, L., Spinelli, S., Heidbreder, C. A., & Fel- The corpus callosum and cerebral speech lateral-
noradrenaline release via presynaptic receptors. don, J. (2000). Differential role of the medial and ization. Brain and Language, 38, 195–206. (14)
Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 112, 213–242. (3) lateral prefrontal cortices in fear and anxiety. Be- Lau, H. C., Rogers, R. D., Haggard, P., & Passing-
Kuczewski, N., Porcer, C., Ferrand, N., Fiorentino, havioral Neuroscience, 114, 1119–1130. (12) ham, R. E. (2004). Attention to intention. Sci-
H., Pellegrino, C., Kolarow, R., et al. (2008). Laeng, B., & Caviness, V. S. (2001). Prosopagnosia ence, 303, 1208–1210. (8)
Backpropagating action potentials trigger den- as a deficit in encoding curved surfaces. Journal of Laurent, J.-P., Cespuglio, R., & Jouvet, M. (1974). Dè-
dritic release of BDNF during spontaneous net- Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 556–576. (6) limitation des voies ascendantes de l’activité ponto-
work activity. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 7013– Laeng, B., Svartdal, F., & Oelmann, H. (2004). Does géniculo-occipitale chez le chat [Demarcation of
7023. (13) color synesthesia pose a paradox for early-selection the ascending paths of ponto-geniculo-occipital
Kujala, T., Myllyviita, K., Tervaniemi, M., Alho, K., theories of attention? Psychological Science, 15, activity in the cat]. Brain Research, 65, 29–52. (9)
Kallio, J., & Näätänen, R. (2000). Basic auditory 277–281. (7) Lavidor, M., & Walsh, V. (2004). The nature of fo-
dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain LaFerla, F. M., Green, K. N., & Oddo, S. (2007). veal representation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
activity measurements. Psychophysiology, 37, Intracellular amyloid-b in Alzheimer’s disease. 5, 729–735. (14)
262–266. (14) Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 499–509. (13) Lazarus, M., Yoshida, K., Coppair, R., Bass, C. E.,
Kujawa, S. G., & Liberman, M. C. (2009). Adding Lahti, T. A., Leppamäki, S., Ojanen, S.-M., Haukka, Mochizuki, T., Lowell, B. B., et al. (2007). EP3
insult to injury: Cochlear nerve degeneration af- J., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Lönnqvist, J., et al. prostaglandin receptors in the median preoptic
ter “temporary” noise-induced hearing loss. Jour- (2006). Transition into daylight saving time influ- nucleus are critical for fever responses. Nature
nal of Neuroscience, 29, 14077–14085. (7) ences the fragmentation of the rest–activity cycle. Neuroscience, 10, 1131–1133. (10)
Kullmann, D. M., & Lamsa, K. P. (2007). Long- Journal of Circadian Rhythms, 4, 1. (9) Lè, A. D., Li, Z., Funk, D., Shram, M., Li, T. K., &
term synaptic plasticity in hippocampal inter- Lai, C. S. L., Fisher, S. E., Hurst, J. A., Vargha-Kha- Shaham, Y. (2006). Increased vulnerability to nic-
neurons. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 687– dem, F., & Monaco, A. P. (2001). A forkhead- otine self-administration and relapse in alcohol-
699. (3) domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and naïve offspring of rats selectively bred for high al-
Kumakura, Y., Cumming, P., Vernaleken, I., Buch- language disorder. Nature, 413, 519–523. (14) cohol intake. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1872–
holz, H.-G., Siessmeier, T., Heinz, A., et al. Lake, R. I. E., Eaves, L. J., Maes, H. H. M., Heath, 1879. (3)
(2007). Elevated [18F]fluorodopamine turnover A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Further evidence Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent,
in brains of patients with schizophrenia. Journal against the environmental transmission of indi- H. P. (2001). Early visual experience and face
of Neuroscience, 27, 8080–8087. (15) vidual differences in neuroticism from a collabora- processing. Nature, 410, 809. (6)
References 519

Leber, A. B. (2010). Neural predictors of within- Lenz, F. A., & Byl, N. N. (1999). Reorganization in of chimeric faces. Brain and Cognition, 2, 404–
subject fluctuations in attentional control. Jour- the cutaneous core of the human thalamic princi- 419. (14)
nal of Neuroscience, 30, 11458–11465. (14) pal somatic sensory nucleus (ventral caudal) in Leweke, F. M., Gerth, C. W., Koethe, D., Klosterköt-
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: patients with dystonia. Journal of Neurophysiolo- ter, J., Ruslanova, I., Krivogorsky, B., et al. (2004).
Simon & Schuster. (12) gy, 82, 3204–3212. (5) Antibodies to infectious agents in individuals
LeDoux, J. E., Iwata, J., Cicchetti, P., & Reis, D. J. Leon, L. R. (2002). Invited review: Cytokine regula- with recent onset schizophrenia. European Ar-
(1988). Different projections of the central tion of fever: Studies using gene knockout mice. chives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 254,
amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and be- Journal of Applied Physiology, 92, 2648–2655. 4–8. (15)
havioral correlates of conditioned fear. Journal of (10) Lewis, D. A. (1997). Development of the prefrontal
Neuroscience, 8, 2517–2529. (12) Leopold, D. A., Bondar, I. V., & Giese, M. A. (2006). cortex during adolescence: Insights into vulnera-
Lee, K. M., Skoe, E., Kraus, N., & Ashley, R. (2009). Norm-based face encoding by single neurons in ble neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsycho-
Selective subcortical enhancement of musical in- the monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature, 442, pharmacology, 16, 385–398. (5, 15)
tervals in musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 572–575. (6) Lewis, E. R., Everhart, T. E., & Zeevi, Y. Y. (1969).
5832–5840. (5) Leppämäki, S., Partonen, T., & Lönnqvist, J. (2002). Studying neural organization in Aplysia with the
Lee, M. G., Hassani, O. K., & Jones, B. E. (2005). Bright-light exposure combined with physical scanning electron microscope. Science, 165,
Discharge of identified orexin/hypocretin neu- exercise elevates mood. Journal of Affective Disor- 1140–1143. (3)
rons across the sleep-waking cycle. Journal of ders, 72, 572–575. (15) Lewis, T. L., & Maurer, D. (2005). Multiple sensi-
Neuroscience, 25, 6716–6720. (9) Leriche, L., Björklund, T., Breysse, N., Besret, L., tive periods in human visual development: Evi-
Lee, S.-H., Blake, R., & Heeger, D. J. (2005). Travel- Grégoire, M.-C., Carlsson, T., et al. (2009). Posi- dence from visually deprived children. Develop-
ing waves of activity in primary visual cortex dur- tron emission tomography imaging demon- mental Psychobiology, 46, 163–183. (6)
ing binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 22– strates correlation between behavioral recovery Lewis, V. G., Money, J., & Epstein, R. (1968). Con-
23. (14) and correction of dopamine neurotransmission cordance of verbal and nonverbal ability in the
Legrand, L. N., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2005, after gene therapy. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, adrenogenital syndrome. Johns Hopkins Medical
March/April). Predicting addiction. American 1544–1553. (8) Journal, 122, 192–195. (11)
Scientist, 93, 140–147. (3) LeVay, S. (1991). A difference in hypothalamic Lewy, A. J., Bauer, V. K., Cutler, N. L., Sack, R. L.,
Lehky, S. R. (2000). Deficits in visual feature bind- structure between heterosexual and homosexual Ahmed, S., Thomas, K. H., et al. (1998). Morn-
ing under isoluminant conditions. Journal of Cog- men. Science, 253, pp. 1034–1037. (11) ing vs. evening light treatment of patients with
nitive Neuroscience, 12, 383–392. (4) LeVay, S. (1993). The sexual brain. Cambridge, MA: winter depression. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Lehrman, D. S. (1964). The reproductive behavior of MIT Press. (11) 55, 890–896. (15)
ring doves. Scientific American, 211(5), 48–54. (11) Levenson, R. W., Oyama, O. N., & Meek, P. S. Li, N., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2008). Unsupervised natural
Lehrer, J. (2009). Small, furry… and smart. Nature, (1987). Greater reinforcement from alcohol for experience rapidly alters invariant object repre-
461, 862–864. (13) those at risk: Parental risk, personality risk, and sentation in visual cortex. Science, 321, 1502–
Leibniz, G. (1714). The Principles of Nature and sex. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 242– 1507. (6)
Grace, Based on Reason. (1) 253. (3) Li, R., Polat, U., Makous, W., & Bavelier, D. (2009).
Leibowitz, S. F., & Alexander, J. T. (1991). Analysis LeVere, T. E. (1975). Neural stability, sparing and Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function
of neuropeptide Y-induced feeding: Dissociation behavioral recovery following brain damage. Psy- through action video game training. Nature Neu-
of Y1 and Y2 receptor effects on natural meal pat- chological Review, 82, 344–358. (5) roscience, 12, 549–551. (6)
terns. Peptides, 12, 1251–1260. (10) LeVere, T. E., & Morlock, G. W. (1973). Nature of Liberles, S. D., & Buck, L. B. (2006). A second class
Leibowitz, S. F., Hammer, N. J., & Chang, K. visual recovery following posterior neodecortica- of chemosensory receptors in the olfactory epi-
(1981). Hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus tion in the hooded rat. Journal of Comparative thelium. Nature, 442, 645–650. (7)
lesions produce overeating and obesity in the rat. and Physiological Psychology, 83, 62–67. (5) Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl,
Physiology & Behavior, 27, 1031–1040. (10) Levi-Montalcini, R. (1987). The nerve growth fac- D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act
Leibowitz, S. F., & Hoebel, B. G. (1998). Behavioral tor 35 years later. Science, 237, 1154–1162. (5) in relation to onset of cerebral activities (readi-
neuroscience of obesity. In G. A. Bray, C. Levi-Montalcini, R. (1988). In praise of imperfection. ness potential): The unconscious initiation of a
Bouchard, & P. T. James (Eds.), Handbook of obe- New York: Basic Books. (5) freely voluntary act. Brain, 106, 623–642. (8)
sity (pp. 313–358). New York: Dekker. (10) Levin, E. D., & Rose, J. E. (1995). Acute and Lieberman, J., Chakos, M., Wu, H., Alvir, J., Hoff-
Lein, E. S., & Shatz, C. J. (2001). Neurotrophins chronic nicotine interactions with dopamine man, E., Robinson, D., et al. (2001). Longitudinal
and refinement of visual circuitry. In W. M. systems and working memory performance. An- study of brain morphology in first episode schizo-
Cowan, T. C. Südhof, & C. F. Stevens (Eds.), nals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 757, phrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 487–499. (15)
Synapses (pp. 613–649). Baltimore: Johns Hop- 245–252. (3) Liebman, M., Pelican, S., Moore, S. A., Holmes, B.,
kins University Press. (6) Levine, J. A., Lannngham-Foster, L. M., McCrady, Wardlaw, M. K., Melcher, L. M., et al. (2006).
Leinders-Zufall, T., Lane, A. P., Puche, A. C., Ma, W., S. K., Krizan, A. C., Olson, L. R., Kane, P. H., et Dietary intake-, eating behavior-, and physical
Novotny, M. V., Shipley, M. T., et al. (2000). Ultra- al. (2005). Interindividual variation in posture al- activity-related determinants of high body mass
sensitive pheromone detection by mammalian location: Possible role in human obesity. Science, index in the 2003 Wellness IN the Rockies
vomeronasal neurons. Nature, 405, 792–796. (7) 307, 584–586. (10) cross-sectional study. Nutrition Research, 26,
Lemos, B., Araripe, L. O., & Hartl, D. L. (2008). Levine, J. D., Fields, H. L., & Basbaum, A. I. (1993). 111–117. (10)
Polymorphic Y chromosomes harbor cryptic Peptides and the primary afferent nociceptor. Lim, K. O., Adalsteinsson, E., Spielman, D., Sulli-
variation with manifold functional consequences. Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 2273–2286. (3) van, E. V., Rosenbloom, M. J., & Pfefferbaum, A.
Science, 319, 91–93. (11) Levitin, D. J., & Bellugi, U. (1998). Musical abilities (1998). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic
Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, in individuals with Williams syndrome. Music imaging of cortical gray and white matter in
O. (2007). Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily Perception, 15, 357–389. (14) schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55,
self-consciousness. Science, 317, 1096–1099. (4) Levitt, R. A. (1975). Psychopharmacology. Washing- 346–352. (15)
Lenhart, R. E., & Katkin, E. S. (1986). Psychophys- ton, DC: Hemisphere. (15) Lim, M. M., Wang, Z., Olazábal, D. E., Ren, X.,
iological evidence for cerebral laterality effects in Levitzki, A. (1988). From epinephrine to cyclic Terwilliger, E. F., & Young, L. J. (2004). En-
a high-risk sample of students with subsyndro- AMP. Science, 241, 800–806. (3) hanced partner preference in a promiscuous spe-
mal bipolar depressive disorder. American Jour- Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M. T., & Burton, L. A. cies by manipulating the expression of a single
nal of Psychiatry, 143, 602–607. (15) (1983). Asymmetry of perception in free viewing gene. Nature, 429, 754–757. (11)
520 References

Lin, D. Y., Shea, S. D., & Katz, L. C. (2006). Repre- Liu, P., & Bilkey, D. K. (2001). The effect of excito- Amygdala-dependent fear conditioning in hu-
sentation of natural stimuli in the rodent main toxic lesions centered on the hippocampus or mans is modulated by the BDNF val66met
olfactory bulb. Neuron, 50, 937–949. (7) perirhinal cortex in object recognition and spatial polymorphism. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124,
Lin, J.-S., Hou, Y., Sakai, K., & Jouvet, M. (1996). memory tasks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 94– 9–15. (12)
Histaminergic descending inputs to the meso- 111. (13) Lord, G. M., Matarese, G., Howard, J. K., Baker,
pontine tegmentum and their role in the control Liu, X., Zwiebel, L. J., Hinton, D., Benzer, S., Hall, R. J., Bloom, S. R., & Lechler, R. I. (1998).
of cortical activation and wakefulness in the cat. J. C., & Rosbash, M. (1992). The period gene en- Leptin modulates the T-cell immune response
Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 1523–1537. (9) codes a predominantly nuclear protein in adult and reverses starvation-induced immunosup-
Lin, L., Faraco, J., Li, R., Kadotani, H., Rogers, W., Drosophila. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 2735– pression. Nature, 394, 897–901. (10)
Lin, X., et al. (1999). The sleep disorder canine 2744. (9) Lorincz, A., & Nusser, Z. (2010). Molecular iden-
narcolepsy is caused by a mutation in the hypocre- Liu, Y., Blackwood, D. H., Caesar, S., de Geus, E. J. tity of dendritic voltage-gated sodium channels.
tin (orexin) receptor 2 gene. Cell, 98, 365–376. (9) C., Farmer, A., Ferreira, M. A. R., et al. (2011). Science, 328, 906–909. (2)
Lindberg, N. O., Coburn, C., & Stricker, E. M. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association data Lorrain, D. S., Riolo, J. V., Matuszewich, L., & Hull,
(1984). Increased feeding by rats after subdiabe- of bipolar disorder and major depressive disor- E. M. (1999). Lateral hypothalamic serotonin
togenic streptozotocin treatment: A role for in- der. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 2–4. (15) inhibits nucleus accumbens dopamine: Implica-
sulin in satiety. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 138– Liu, Z.-W., Faraguna, U., Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., & tions for sexual refractoriness. Journal of Neuro-
145. (10) Gao, X.-B. (2010). Direct evidence for wake- science, 19, 7648–7652. (3)
Lindemann, B. (1996). Taste reception. Physiological related increases and sleep-related decreases in Lorusso, M. L., Facoetti, A., Pesenti, S., Cattaneo,
Reviews, 76, 719–766. (7) synaptic strength in rodent cortex. Journal of C., Molteni, M., & Geiger, G. (2004). Wider rec-
Lindholm, P., Voutilainen, M. H., Laurén, J., Perän- Neuroscience, 30, 8671–8675. (9) ognition in peripheral vision common to differ-
en, V.-M., Leppänen, V.-M., Andressoo, J.-O., et Livingstone, M. S. (1988, January). Art, illusion and ent subtypes of dyslexia. Vision Research, 44,
al. (2007). Novel neurotrophic factor CDNF the visual system. Scientific American, 258(1), 2413–2424. (14)
protects and rescues midbrain dopamine neu- 78–85. (6) Lott, I. T. (1982). Down’s syndrome, aging, and
rons in vivo. Nature, 448, 73–77. (8) Livingstone, M. S., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segrega- Alzheimer’s disease: A clinical review. Annals of
Lindner, A., Iyer, A., Kagan, I., & Andersen, R. A. tion of form, color, movement, and depth: Anat- the New York Academy of Sciences, 396, 15–27.
(2010). Human posterior parietal cortex plans omy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240, (13)
where to reach and what to avoid. Journal of Neu- 740–749. (6) Lotto, R. B., & Purves, D. (2002). The empirical ba-
roscience, 30, 11715–11725. (8) Ljungberg, M. C., Stern, G., & Wilkin, G. P. (1999). sis of color perception. Consciousness and Cogni-
Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1972). Human Survival of genetically engineered, adult-derived tion, 11, 609–629. (6)
information processing. New York: Academic rat astrocytes grafted into the 6-hydroxydopamine Lotze, M., Grodd, W., Birbaumer, N., Erb, M.,
Press. (7) lesioned adult rat striatum. Brain Research, 816, Huse, E., & Flor, H. (1999). Does use of a myo-
Liou, Y.-C., Tocilj, A., Davies, P. L., & Jia, Z. (2000). 29–37. (8) electric prosthesis prevent cortical reorganization
Mimicry of ice structure by surface hydroxyls Lockwood, A. H., Salvi, R. J., Coad, M. L., Towsley, and phantom limb pain? Nature Neuroscience, 2,
and water of a -helix antifreeze protein. Nature, M. L., Wack, D. S., & Murphy, B. W. (1998). The 501–502. (5)
406, 322–324. (10) functional neuroanatomy of tinnitus: Evidence Loui, P., Alsop, D., & Schlaug, G. (2009). Tone
Lippa, R. A. (2006). Is high sex drive associated for limbic system links and neural plasticity. Neu- deafness: A new disconnection syndrome? Jour-
with increased sexual attraction to both sexes? rology, 50, 114–120. (7) nal of Neuroscience, 29, 10215–10220. (7)
Psychological Science, 17, 46–52. (11) Loe, I. M., Feldman, H. M., Yasui, E., & Luna, B. Lucas, B. K., Ormandy, C. J., Binart, N., Bridges, R. S.
Lisman, J., Schulman, H., & Cline, H. (2002). The (2009). Oculomotor performance identifies un- & Kelly, P. A. (1998). Null mutation of the prolac-
molecular basis of CaMKII function in synaptic derlying cognitive deficits in attention-deficit/ tin receptor gene produces a defect in maternal
and behavioural memory. Nature Reviews Neuro- hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American behavior. Endocrinology, 139, 4102–4107. (11)
science, 3, 175–190. (13) Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, Lucas, R. J., Douglas, R. H., & Foster, R. G. (2001).
Lisman, J. E., Raghavachari, S., & Tsien, R. W. (2007). 431–440. (5) Characterization of an ocular photopigment ca-
The sequence of events that underlie quantal trans- Loewenstein, W. R. (1960, August). Biological trans- pable of driving pupillary constriction in mice.
mission at central glutamatergic synapses. Nature ducers. Scientific American, 203(2), 98–108. (7) Nature Neuroscience, 4, 621–626. (9)
Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 597–609. (3) Loewi, O. (1960). An autobiographic sketch. Per- Lucas, R. J., Freedman, M. S., Muñoz, M., Garcia-
Litt, A., Khan, U., & Shiv, B. (2010). Lusting while spectives in Biology, 4, 3–25. (3) Fernández, J.-M., & Foster, R. G. (1999). Regu-
loathing: Parallel counterdriving of wanting and Logan, C. G., & Grafton, S. T. (1995). Functional lation of the mammalian pineal by non-rod, non-
liking. Psychological Science, 21, 118–125. (3) anatomy of human eyeblink conditioning deter- cone ocular photoreceptors. Science, 284, 505–
Liu, F., Wollstein, A., Hysi, P. G., Ankra-Badu, mined with regional cerebral glucose metabolism 507. (9)
G. A., Spector, T. D., Park, D, et al. (2010). Digi- and positron-emission tomography. Proceedings Luczak, S. E., Glatt, S. J., & Wall, T. L. (2006).
tal quantification of human eye color highlights of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 92, Meta-analysis of ALDHx and ADHIB with al-
genetic association of three new loci. PLoS Genet- 7500–7504. (13) cohol dependence in Asians. Psychological Bulle-
ics, 6, e1000934. (1) Löken, L. S., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, tin, 132, 607–621. (3)
Liu, G., & Tsien, R. W. (1995). Properties of synap- F., & Olausson, H. (2009). Coding of pleasant Luders, E., Gaser, C., Narr, K. L., & Toga, A. W.
tic transmission at single hippocampal synaptic touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Na- (2009). Why sex matters: Brain size independent
boutons. Nature, 375, 404–408. (3) ture Neuroscience, 12, 547–548. (7) differences in gray matter distributions between
Liu, K., Lu, Y., Lee, J. K., Samara, R., Willenberg, Lomber, S. G., & Malhotra, S. (2008). Double dis- men and women. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
R., Sears-Kraxberger, I., et al. (2010). PTEN de- sociation of “what” and “where” processing in 14265–14270. (11)
letion enhances the regenerative ability of adult auditory cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 609– Luders, E., Narr, K. L., Thompson, P. M., Rex, D. E.,
corticospinal neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 617. (7) Jancke, L., Steinmetz, H., et al. (2004). Gender
1075–1081. (5) Long, M. A., Jutras, M. J., Connors, B. W., & Bur- differences in cortical complexity. Nature Neuro-
Liu, L. Y., Coe, C. L., Swenson, C. A., Kelly, E. A., well, R. D. (2005). Electrical synapses coordinate science, 7, 799–800. (4)
Kita, H., & Busse, W. W. (2002). School exami- activity in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Nature Luders, E., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2010).
nations enhance airway inflammation to antigen Neuroscience, 8, 61–66. (9) The development of the corpus callosum in the
challenge. American Journal of Respiratory and Lonsdorf, T. B., Weike, A. I., Golkar, A., Schal- healthy human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30,
Critical Care Medicine, 165, 1062–1067. (12) ling, M., Hamm, A. O., & Öhman, A. (2010). 10985–10990. (14)
References 521

Ludwig, M., & Leng, G. (2006). Dendritic peptides MacLusky, N. J., & Naftolin, F. (1981). Sexual dif- Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Pe-
release and peptide-dependent behaviours. Na- ferentiation of the central nervous system. Sci- tiau, C., Phillips, C., et al. (2000). Experience-
ture Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 126–136. (3) ence, 211, 1294–1303. (11) dependent changes in cerebral activation during
Luna, B., Padmanabhan, A., & O’Hearn, K. (2010). Macphail, E. M. (1985). Vertebrate intelligence: human REM sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 831–
What has fMRI told us about the development The null hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of 836. (9)
of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain the Royal Society of London, B, 308, 37–51. (4) Maquet, P., Peters, J.-M., Aerts, J., Delfiore, G., Deg-
and Cognition, 72, 101–113. (5) Macrae, C. N., Alnwick, K. A., Milne, A. B., & ueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (1996). Functional
Lund, R. D., Lund, J. S., & Wise, R. P. (1974). The Schloerscheidt, A. M. (2002). Person perception neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement
organization of the retinal projection to the dor- across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, sleep and dreaming. Nature, 383, 163–166. (8, 9)
sal lateral geniculate nucleus in pigmented and 13, 532–536. (11) Maraganore, D. M., deAndrade, M., Lesnick, T. G.,
albino rats. Journal of Comparative Neurology, Maffei, A., Nataraj, K., Nelson, S. B., & Turrigiano, Strain, K. J., Farrer, M. J., Rocca, W. A., et al.
158, 383–404. (6) G. G. (2006). Potentiation of cortical inhibition (2005). High-resolution whole-genome associa-
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. by visual deprivation. Nature, 443, 81–84. (6) tion study of Parkinson disease. American Jour-
D., Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2009). Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., nal of Human Genetics, 77, 685–693. (8)
Mental training enhances attentional stability: Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Marcar, V. L., Zihl, J., & Cowey, A. (1997). Com-
Neural and behavioral evidence. Journal of Neuro- et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural paring the visual deficits of a motion blind pa-
science, 29, 13418–13427. (14) change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceed- tient with the visual deficits of monkeys with
Lyman, C. P., O’Brien, R. C., Greene, G. C., & Pa- ings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, area MT removed. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1459–
pafrangos, E. D. (1981). Hibernation and lon- 4398–4403. (13) 1465. (6)
gevity in the Turkish hamster Mesocricetus brand- Maier, S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (1998). Cytokines for March, S. M., Abate, P., Spear, N. E., & Molina, J. C.
ti. Science, 212, 668–670. (9) psychologists: Implications of bidirectional (2009). Fetal exposure to moderate ethanol doses:
Lynall, M.-E., Bassett, D. S., Kerwin, R., McKenna, immune-to-brain communication for under- Heightened operant responsiveness elicited by
P. J., Kitzbichler, M., Muller, U., et al. (2010). standing behavior, mood, and cognition. Psycho- ethanol-related reinforcers. Alcoholism: Clinical
Functional connectivity and brain networks in logical Review, 105, 83–107. (12) and Experimental Research, 33, 1981–1993. (3)
schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 9477– Malaspina, D., Corcoran, C., Fahim, C., Berman, A., Maricich, S. M., Wellnitz, S. A., Nelson, A. M.,
9487. (15) Harkavy-Friedman, J., Yale, S., et al. (2002). Pa- Lesniak, D. R., Gerling, G. J., Lumpkin, E. A., et
Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., Goldberg, J., True, W., Lin, ternal age and sporadic schizophrenia: Evidence al. (2009). Merkel cells are essential for light-
N., Meyer, J. M., et al. (1998). A registry-based for de novo mutations. American Journal of Medi- touch responses. Science, 324, 1580–1582. (7)
twin study of depression in men. Archives of Gen- cal Genetics, 114, 299–303. (15) Mariño, G., Fernández, A. F., Cabrera, S., Lundberg,
eral Psychiatry, 55, 468–472. (15) Malhotra, P., Jäger, H. R., Parton, A., Greenwood, Y. W., Cabanillas, R., Rodríguez, F., et al. (2010).
Lytle, L. D., Messing, R. B., Fisher, L., & Phebus, L. R., Playford, E. D., Brown, M. M., et al. (2005). Autophagy is essential for mouse sense of bal-
(1975). Effects of long-term corn consumption Spatial working memory capacity in unilateral ance. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 120, 2331–
on brain serotonin and the response to electric neglect. Brain, 128, 424–435. (14) 2344. (7)
shock. Science, 190, 692–694. (12) Mallis, M. M., & DeRoshia, C. W. (2005). Circa­ Marris, E. (2006). Grey matters. Nature, 444, 808–
Macdonald, R. L., Weddle, M. G., & Gross, R. A. dian rhythms, sleep, and performance in space. 810. (1)
(1986). Benzodiazepine, ß-carboline, and bar- Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, Marshall, J. C., & Halligan, P. W. (1995). Seeing the
biturate actions on GABA responses. Advances 76(Suppl. 6), B94–B107. (9) forest but only half the trees? Nature, 373, 521–
in Biochemical Psychopharmacology, 41, 67–78. Malmberg, A. B., Chen, C., Tonegawa, S., & Bas- 523. (14)
(12) baum, A. I. (1997). Preserved acute pain and re- Marshall, J. F. (1985). Neural plasticity and recovery
Macey, P. M., Henderson, L. A., Macey, K. E., Al- duced neuropathic pain in mice lacking PKCg. of function after brain injury. International Re-
ger, J. R., Frysinger, R. C., Woo, M. A., et al. Science, 278, 279–283. (7) view of Neurobiology, 26, 201–247. (5)
(2002). Brain morphology associated with ob- Mameli, M., Halbout, B., Creton, C., Engblom, D., Martens, M. A., Wilson, S. J., & Reutens, D. C.
structive sleep apnea. American Journal of Respi- Parkitna, J. R., Spanagel, R., et al. (2009). (2008). Research review: Williams syndrome: A
ratory & Critical Care Medicine, 166, 1382– Cocaine-evoked synaptic plasticity: Persistence critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and
1387. (9) in the VTA triggers adaptations in the NAc. Na- neuroanatomical phenotype. Journal of Child Psy-
MacFarlane, J. G., Cleghorn, J. M., & Brown, G. M. ture Neuroscience, 12, 1036–1041. (3) chology and Psychiatry, 49, 576–608. (14)
(1985a, September). Circadian rhythms in chronic Mancuso, K., Hauswirth, W. W., Li, Q., Connor, Martin, E. R., Scott, W. K., Nance, M. A., Watts,
insomnia. Paper presented at the World Congress T. B., Kuchenbecker, J. A., Mauck, M. C., et al. R. L., Hubble, J. P., Koller, W. C., et al. (2001).
of Biological Psychiatry, Philadelphia. (9) (2009). Gene therapy for red-green colour blind- Association of single-nucleotide polymorphisms
MacFarlane, J. G., Cleghorn, J. M., & Brown, G. M. ness. Nature, 461, 784–787. (6) of the tau gene with late-onset Parkinson disease.
(1985b). Melatonin and core temperature Manfredi, M., Stocchi, F., & Vacca, L. (1995). Dif- Journal of the American Medical Association, 286,
rhythms in chronic insomnia. In G. M. Brown & ferential diagnosis of Parkinsonism. Journal of 2245–2250. (8)
S. D. Wainwright (Eds.), The pineal gland: Endo- Neural Transmission (Suppl. 45), 1–9. (8) Martin, G., Rojas, L. M., Ramírez, Y., & McNeil, R.
crine aspects (pp. 301–306). New York: Pergam- Mangan, M. A. (2004). A phenomenology of prob- (2004). The eyes of oilbirds (Steatornis caripen-
on Press. (9) lematic sexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behav- sis): Pushing at the limits of sensitivity. Naturwis-
MacFarquhar, L. (2009, July 27). The kindest cut. ior, 33, 287–293. (9) senschaften, 91, 26–29. (6)
The New Yorker, 85(22), 38–51. (1) Mangiapane, M. L., & Simpson, J. B. (1980). Sub- Martin, P. R., Lee, B. B., White, A. J. R., Solomon,
Machado, C. J., Emery, N. J., Capitanio, J. P., Mason, fornical organ: Forebrain site of pressor and dip- S. G., & Rütiger, L. (2001). Chromatic sensitivi-
W. A., Mendoza, S. P., & Amaral, D. G. (2008). sogenic action of angiotensin II. American Jour- ty of ganglion cells in the peripheral primate ret-
Bilateral neurotoxic amygdala lesions in rhesus nal of Physiology, 239, R382–R389. (10) ina. Nature, 410, 933–936. (6)
monkeys (Macaca mulatta): Consistent pattern Mann, J. J., Arango, V., & Underwood, M. D. (1990). Martin, R. C., & Blossom-Stach, C. (1986). Evi-
of behavior across different social contexts. Be- Serotonin and suicidal behavior. Annals of the New dence of syntactic deficits in a fluent aphasic.
havioral Neuroscience, 122, 251–266. (12) York Academy of Sciences, 600, 476–485. (12) Brain and Language, 28, 196–234. (14)
MacLean, P. D. (1949). Psychosomatic disease and Mann, T., Tomiyama, A. J., Westling, E., Lew, Martin, S. D., Martin, E., Rai, S. S., Richardson,
the “visceral brain”: Recent developments bearing A.-M., Samuels, B., & Chatman, J. (2007). Medi- M. A., Royall, R., & Eng, C. (2001). Brain blood
on the Papez theory of emotion. Psychosomatic care’s search for effective obesity treatments. flow changes in depressed patients treated with
Medicine, 11, 338–353. (12) American Psychologist, 62, 220–233. (10) interpersonal psychotherapy or venlafaxine hy-
522 References

drochloride. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, nipulation and sexual activity. Behavioral Neuro- in human brain associates with childhood abuse.
641–648. (15) science, 114, 772–782. (11) Nature Neuroscience, 12, 342–348. (1)
Martindale, C. (2001). Oscillations and analogies: Maurice, D. M. (1998). The Von Sallmann lecture McGregor, I. S., Hargreaves, G. A., Apfelbach, R., &
Thomas Young, MD, FRS, genius. American Psy- of 1996: An ophthalmological explanation of Hunt, G. E. (2004). Neural correlates of cat
chologist, 56, 342–345. (6) REM sleep. Experimental Eye Research, 66, 139– odor-induced anxiety in rats: Region-specific ef-
Martinez-Vargas, M. C., & Erickson, C. J. (1973). 145. (9) fects of the benzodiazepine midazolam. Journal
Some social and hormonal determinants of nest- May, P. R. A., Fuster, J. M., Haber, J., & Hirschman, of Neuroscience, 24, 4134–4144. (12)
building behaviour in the ring dove (Streptopelia A. (1979). Woodpecker drilling behavior: An en- McGuire, S., & Clifford, J. (2000). Genetic and en-
risoria). Behaviour, 45, 12–37. (11) dorsement of the rotational theory of impact brain vironmental contributions to loneliness in chil-
Martinowich, K., Manji, H., & Lu, B. (2007). New injury. Archives of Neurology, 36, 370–373. (5) dren. Psychological Science, 11, 487–491. (1)
insights into BDNF function in depression and Mayberry, R. I., Lock, E., & Kazmi, H. (2002). Lin- McHugh, P. R., & Moran, T. H. (1985). The stom-
anxiety. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1089–1093. (15) guistic ability and early language exposure. Na- ach: A conception of its dynamic role in satiety.
Masland, R. H. (2001). The fundamental plan of ture, 417, 38. (14) Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychol-
the retina. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 877–886. Mayer, A. D., & Rosenblatt, J. S. (1979). Hormonal ogy, 11, 197–232. (10)
(6) influences during the ontogeny of maternal be- McIntyre, M., Gangestad, S. W., Gray, P. B., Chap-
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Weg- havior in female rats. Journal of Comparative and man, J. F., Burnham, T. C., O’Rourke, M. T., et al.
ner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. Physiological Psychology, 93, 879–898. (11) (2006). Romantic involvement often reduces
(2007). Wandering minds: The default network Maze, I., Covington, H. E. III, Dietz, D. M., La men’s testosterone levels—But not always: The
and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, Plant, Q., Renthal, W., Russo, S. J., et al. (2010). moderating effect of extrapair sexual interest.
393–395. (4) Essential role of the histone methyltransferase Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
Massa, F., Mancini, G., Schmidt, H., Steindel, F., G9a in cocaine-induced plasticity. Science, 327, 642–651. (11)
Mackie, K., Angioni, C., et al. (2010). Alterations 213–216. (3) McIntyre, R. S., Konarski, J. Z., Wilkins, K., Soc-
in the hippocampal endocannabinoid system in McAllen, R. M., Tanaka, M., Ootsuka, Y., & zynska, J. K., & Kennedy, S. H. (2006). Obesity
diet-induced obese mice. Journal of Neuroscience, McKinley, M. J. (2010). Multiple thermoregula- in bipolar disorder and major depressive disor-
30, 6273–6281. (3) tory effectors with independent central controls. der: Results from a National Community Health
Massimini, M., Ferrarelli, F., Huber, R., Esser, S. K., European Journal of Applied Physiology, 109, 27– Survey on Mental Health and Well-Being. Cana-
Singh, H., & Tononi, G. (2005). Breakdown of 33. (10) dian Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 274–280. (10)
cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Sci- McBurney, D. H., & Bartoshuk, L. M. (1973). In- McKeever, W. F., Seitz, K. S., Krutsch, A. J., & Van
ence, 309, 2228–2232. (9) teractions between stimuli with different taste Eys, P. L. (1995). On language laterality in nor-
Mathalon, D. H., Sullivan, E. V., Lim, K. O., & Pfef- qualities. Physiology & Behavior, 10, 1101– mal dextrals and sinistrals: Results from the bi-
ferbaum, A. (2001). Progressive brain volume 1106. (7) lateral object naming latency task. Neuropsycholo-
changes and the clinical course of schizophrenia McCarley, R. W., & Hoffman, E. (1981). REM gia, 33, 1627–1635. (14)
in men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 148– sleep, dreams, and the activation-synthesis hy- McKemy, D. D., Neuhausser, W. M., & Julius, D.
157. (15) pothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, (2002). Identification of a cold receptor reveals a
Mathews, G. A., Fane, B. A., Conway, G. S., Brook, 904–912. (9) general role for TRP channels in thermosensa-
C. G. D., & Hines, M. (2009). Personality and McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. tion. Nature, 416, 52–58. (7)
congenital adrenal androgen exposure. Hormones (1997). Face-specific processing in the human McKinnon, W., Weisse, C. S., Reynolds, C. P.,
and Behavior, 55, 285–291. (11) fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Bowles, C. A., & Baum, A. (1989). Chronic
Matrisciano, F., Bonaccorso, S., Ricciardi, A., Scac- 9, 605–610. (6) stress, leukocyte-subpopulations, and humoral
cianoce, S., Panaccione, I., Wang, L., et al. (2009). McClintock, M. K. (1971). Menstrual synchrony response to latent viruses. Health Psychology, 8,
Changes in BDNF serum levels in patients with and suppression. Nature, 229, 244–245. (7) 839–402. (12)
major depression disorder (MDD) after 6 McConnell, J. V. (1962). Memory transfer through Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U.,
months treatment with sertraline, escitalopram, cannibalism in planarians. Journal of Neuropsy- O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et
or venlafaxine. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, chiatry, 3(Suppl. 1), 42–48. (13) al. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual
247–254. (15) McConnell, S. K. (1992). The genesis of neuronal brain. Nature, 431, 757. (14)
Matsumoto, Y., Mishima, K., Satoh, K., Tozawa, T., diversity during development of cerebral cortex. Meddis, R., Pearson, A. J. D., & Langford, G.
Mishima, Y., Shimizu, T., et al. (2001). Total Seminars in the Neurosciences, 4, 347–356. (5) (1973). An extreme case of healthy insomnia.
sleep deprivation induces an acute and transient McCrory, E., Frith, U., Brunswick, N., & Price, C. EEG and Clinical Neurophysiology, 35, 213–
increase in NK cell activity in healthy young vol- (2000). Abnormal functional activation during a 214. (9)
unteers. Sleep, 24, 804–809. (9) simple word repetition task: A PET study of Meister, M., Wong, R. O. L., Baylor, D. A., & Shatz,
Matsunami, H., Montmayeur, J.-P., & Buck, L. B. adult dyslexics. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, C. J. (1991). Synchronous bursts of action poten-
(2000). A family of candidate taste receptors in 12, 753–762. (14) tials in ganglion cells of the developing mamma-
human and mouse. Nature, 404, 601–604. (7) McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Big-brained people are lian retina. Science, 252, 939–943. (5)
Mattingley, J. B., Husain, M., Rorden, C., Kennard, smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship be- Melloni, L., Molina, C., Pena, M., Torres, D., Singer,
C., & Driver, J. (1998). Motor role of human in- tween in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intel- W., & Rodriguez, E. (2007). Synchronization of
ferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect ligence, 33, 337–346. (4) neural activity across cortical areas correlates
patients. Nature, 392, 179–182. (14) McEwen, B. S. (2000). The neurobiology of stress: with conscious perception. Journal of Neurosci-
Mattocks, C., Hines, M., Ness, A., Leary, S., From serendipity to clinical relevance. Brain Re- ence, 27, 2858–2865. (14)
Griffiths, A., Tilling, K., et al. (2010). Associa- search, 886, 172–189. (10, 12) Melone, M., Vitellaro-Zuccarello, L., Vallejo-
tions between sex-typed behaviour at age 3½ and McFarland, B. R., & Klein, D. N. (2009). Emotional Illarramendi, A., Pérez-Samartin, A., Matute, C.,
levels and patterns of physical activity at age 12: reactivity in depression: Diminished responsive- Cozzi, A., et al. (2001). The expression of gluta-
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and ness to anticipated reward but not to anticipated mate transporter GLT-1 in the rat cerebral cortex
Children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 95, punishment or to nonreward or avoidance. De- is down-regulated by the antipsychotic drug clo-
509–512. (11) pression and Anxiety, 26, 117–122. (15) zapine. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 380–386. (15)
Matuszewich, L., Lorrain, D. S., & Hull, E. M. McGowan, P. O., Sasaki, A., D’Alessio, A. C., Dy- Meltzer, H. Y., Matsubara, S., & Lee, J.-C. (1989).
(2000). Dopamine release in the medial preoptic mov, S., Labonté, B., Szyf, M., et al. (2009). Epi- Classification of typical and atypical antipsychot-
area of female rats in response to hormonal ma- genetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor ic drugs on the basis of dopamine D-1, D-2 and
References 523

serotonin2 pKi values. Journal of Pharmacology Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Mervis, C. B., & Berman, Minto, C. L., Liao, L.-M., Woodhouse, C. R. J.,
and Experimental Therapeutics, 251, 238–246. K. F. (2006). Neural mechanisms in Williams Ransley, P. G., & Creighton, S. M. (2003). The
(15) syndrome: A unique window to genetic influ- effect of clitoral surgery in individuals who
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation ences on cognition and behaviour. Nature Re- have intersex conditions with ambiguous geni-
of facial and manual gestures by human neo- views Neuroscience, 7, 380–393. (14) talia: A cross-sectional study. Lancet, 361,
nates. Science, 198, 75–78. (8) Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Miletich, R. S., Kohn, P. D., 1252–1257. (11)
Melzack, R., & Wall, P. D. (1965). Pain mecha- Esposito, G., Carson, R. E., Quarantelli, M., et al. Mishra, J., Zinni, M., Bavelier, D., & Hillyard, S. A.
nisms: A new theory. Science, 150, 971–979. (7) (2002). Reduced prefrontal activity predicts ex- (2011). Neural basis of superior performance of
Mendieta-Zéron, H., López, M., & Diéguez, C. aggerated striatal dopaminergic function in action videogame players in an attention-
(2008). Gastrointestinal peptides controlling schizophrenia. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 267–271. demanding task. Journal of Neuroscience, 31,
body weight homeostasis. General and Compara- (15) 992–998. (14)
tive Endocrinology, 155, 481–495. (10) Mezzanotte, W. S., Tangel, D. J., & White, D. P. Misrahi, M., Meduri, G., Pissard, S., Bouvattier, C.,
Merabet, L. B., Hamilton, R., Schlaug, G., Swisher, (1992). Waking genioglossal electromyogram Beau, I., Loosfelt, H., et al. (1997). Comparison
J. D., Kiriakopoulos, E. T., Pitskel, N. B., et al. in sleep apnea patients versus normal controls of immunocytochemical and molecular features
(2008). Rapid and reversible recruitment of early (a neuromuscular compensatory mechanism). with the phenotype in a case of incomplete male
visual cortex for touch. PLoS ONE, 3, e3046. (5) Journal of Clinical Investigation, 89, 1571–1579. pseudohermaphroditism associated with a muta-
Mergen, M., Mergen, H., Ozata, M., Oner, R., & (9) tion of the luteinizing hormone receptor. Journal
Oner, C. (2001). A novel melanocortin 4 recep- Michel, F., & Anderson, M. (2009). Using the anti- of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 82,
tor (MC4R) gene mutation associated with mor- saccade task to investigate the relationship be- 2159–2165. (11)
bid obesity. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & tween the development of inhibition and the de- Mistlberger, R. E., & Skene, D. J. (2004). Social in-
Metabolism, 86, 3448–3451. (10) velopment of intelligence. Developmental Science, fluences on mammalian circadian rhythms: Ani-
Merikangas, K. R., Akiskal, H. S., Angst, J., Green- 12, 272–288. (5) mal and human studies. Biological Rhythms, 79,
berg, P. E., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Petukhova, M., Mihalcescu, I., Hsing, W., & Leibler, S. (2004). Re- 533–556. (9)
et al. (2007). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence silient circadian oscillator revealed in individual Mitchell, D. E. (1980). The influence of early visual
of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Co- cyanobacteria. Nature, 430, 81–85. (9) experience on visual perception. In C. S. Harris
morbidity Survey Replication. Archives of Gener- Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., Lane, A., Corneille, O., (Ed.), Visual coding and adaptability (pp. 1–50).
al Psychiatry, 64, 543–552. (15) de Timary, P., & Luminet, O. (2010). Oxytocin Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (6)
Merton, P. A. (1972). How we control the contrac- makes people trusting, not gullible. Psychological Miyazawa, A., Fujiyoshi, Y., & Unwin, N. (2003).
tion of our muscles. Scientific American, 226(5), Science, 21, 1072–1074. (11) Structure and gating mechanism of the acetyl-
30–37. (8) Miles, F. A., & Evarts, E. V. (1979). Concepts of mo- choline receptor pore. Nature, 423, 949–955. (3)
Merzenich, M. M., Nelson, R. J., Stryker, M. P., tor organization. Annual Review of Psychology, Mochizuki, T., Crocker, A., McCormack, S., Yanagi-
Cynader, M. S., Schoppman, A., & Zook, J. M. 30, 327–362. (8) sawa, M., Sakurai, T., & Scammell, T. E. (2004).
(1984). Somatosensory cortical map changes Milich, R., & Pelham, W. E. (1986). Effects of sugar Behavioral state instability in orexin knock-out
following digit amputation in adult monkeys. ingestion on the classroom and playgroup behavior mice. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6291–6300. (9)
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 224, 591– of attention deficit disordered boys. Journal of Con- Modell, S., Ising, M., Holsboer, F., & Lauer, C. J.
605. (5) sulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 714–718. (10) (2005). The Munich vulnerability study on affec-
Meshi, D., Drew, M. R., Saxe, M., Ansorge, M. S., Miller, C. A., Gavin, C. F., White, J. A., Parrish, tive disorders: Premorbid polysomnographic
David, D., Santarelli, L., et al. (2006). Hippo- R. R, Honasoge, A., Yancey, C. R., et al. profile of affected high-risk probands. Biological
campal neurogenesis is not required for behav- (2010). Cortical DNA methylation maintains Psychiatry, 58, 694–699. (15)
ioral effects of environmental enrichment. Nature remote memory. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 664– Moeller, F. G., Dougherty, D. M., Swann, A. C., Col-
Neuroscience, 9, 729–731. (5) 666. (13) lins, D., Davis, C. M., & Cherek, D. R. (1996).
Mesulam, M.-M. (1995). Cholinergic pathways and Miller, E. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cogni- Tryptophan depletion and aggressive responding
the ascending reticular activating system of the tive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 59– in healthy males. Psychopharmacology, 126, 97–
human brain. Annals of the New York Academy of 65. (4) 103. (12)
Sciences, 757, 169–179. (4, 9) Miller, G. (2007a). Animal extremists get personal. Mohr, C., Landis, T., Bracha, H. S., & Brugger, P.
Mevorach, C., Hodsoll, J., Allen, H., Shalev, L., & Science, 318, 1856–1585. (1) (2003). Opposite turning behavior in right-
Humphreys, G. (2010). Ignoring the elephant in Miller, G. (2007b). The mystery of the missing handers and non-right-handers suggests a link
the room: A neural circuit to downregulate sa- smile. Science, 316, 826–827. (12) between handedness and cerebral dopamine
lience. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 6072–6079. (14) Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., & Jordan, B. D. (2007). asymmetries. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117,
Meyer, K., Kaplan, J. T., Essex, R., Webber, C., Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap 1448–1452. (14)
Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (2010). Predicting dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus? Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Ahola, S. (2006).
visual stimuli on the basis of activity in auditory Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 375–381. Becoming a face expert. Psychological Science, 17,
cortices. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 667–671. (7) (11) 930–934. (6)
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., Dolezal, C., Baker, S. W., Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2010). Scent of a woman: Money, J. (1967). Sexual problems of the chronically
& New, M. I. (2008). Sexual orientation in wom- Men’s testosterone responses to olfactory ovulation ill. In C. W. Wahl (Ed.), Sexual problems: Diagno-
en with classical or non-classical congenital adre- cues. Psychological Science, 21, 276–283. (7) sis and treatment in medical practice (pp. 266–
nal hyperplasia as a function of degree of prena- Milner, B. (1959). The memory defect in bilateral 287). New York: Free Press. (8)
tal androgen excess. Archives of Sexual Behavior, hippocampal lesions. Psychiatric Research Reports, Money, J., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1972). Man & wom-
37, 85–99. (11) 11, 43–58. (13) an, boy & girl. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Univer-
Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2010). From maps to mech- Minichiello, L. (2009). TrkB signaling pathways in sity Press. (11)
anisms through neuroimaging of schizophrenia. LTP and learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Money, J., & Lewis, V. (1966). IQ, genetics and ac-
Nature, 468, 194–202. (15) 10, 850–860. (13) celerated growth: Adrenogenital syndrome. Bul-
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Kohn, P., Mervis, C. B., Kip- Minokoshi, Y., Alquier, T., Furukawa, N., Kim, letin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital, 118, 365–373.
penhan, J. S., Olsen, R. K., Morris, C. A., et al. Y.-B., Lee, A., Xue, B., et al. (2004). AMP-kinase (11)
(2004). Neural basis of genetically determined regulates food intake by responding to hormonal Money, J., & Schwartz, M. (1978). Biosocial deter-
visuospatial construction deficit in Williams syn- and nutrient signals in the hypothalamus. Na- minants of gender identity differentiation and
drome. Neuron, 43, 623–631. (14) ture, 428, 569–574. (10) development. In J. B. Hutchison (Ed.), Biological
524 References

determinants of sexual behaviour (pp. 765–784). cade programming. Journal of Vision, 10(3), ar- Aergic neurons of the primate brain. Nature,
Chichester, England: Wiley. (11) ticle 16. (5) 381, 245–248. (15)
Monfils, M.-H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., & Moreno, C., Laje, G., Blanco, C., Jiang, H., Schmidt, Mueller, K. L. O., Marion, S. D., Paul, L. K., &
LeDoux, J. E. (2009). Extinction-reconsolidation A. B., & Olfson, M. (2007). National trends in Brown, W. S. (2009). Bimanual motor coordina-
boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar tion in agenesis of the corpus callosum. Behav-
memories. Science, 324, 951–955. (12) disorder in youth. Archives of General Psychiatry, ioral Neuroscience, 123, 1000–1011. (14)
Mongillo, G., Barak, O., & Tsodyks, M. (2008). 64, 1032–1039. (15) Muller, M. N., Thompson, M. E., & Wrangham,
Synaptic theory of working memory. Science, Morfini, G. A., You, Y-M., Pollema, S. L., Kamins- R. W. (2006). Male chimpanzees prefer mating
319, 1543–1546. (13) ka, A., Liu, K., Yoshioka, K., et al. (2009). Patho- with old females. Current Biology, 16, 2234–
Monk, C. S., Klein, R. G., Telzer, E. H., Schroth, genic huntingtin inhibits fast axonal transport by 2238. (11)
E. A., Mannuzza, S., Moulton, J. L., III, et al. activating JNK3 and phosphorylating kinesin. Mulligan, S. J., & MacVicar, B. A. (2004). Calcium
(2008). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens acti- Nature Neuroscience, 12, 864–871. (8) transients in astrocyte end feet cause cerebrovas-
vation to emotional facial expressions in children Morgan, L. K., MacEvoy, S. P., Aguirre, G. K., & cular constrictions. Nature, 431, 195–199. (2)
and adolescents at risk for major depression. Epstein, R. A. (2011). Distances between real- Munafo, M. R., Durrant, C., Lewis, G., & Flint, J.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 90–98. (15) world locations are represented in the human (2009). Gene x environment interactions at the
Monk, T. H., & Aplin, L. C. (1980). Spring and au- hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1238– serotonin transporter locus. Biological Psychiatry,
tumn daylight time changes: Studies of adjust- 1245. (13) 65, 211–219. (15)
ment in sleep timings, mood, and efficiency. Ergo- Mori, K., Mataga, N., & Imamura, K. (1992). Dif- Munk, M. H. J., Roelfsema, P. R., König, P., Engel,
nomics, 23, 167–178. (9) ferential specificities of single mitral cells in rab- A. K., & Singer, W. (1996). Role of reticular acti-
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life bit olfactory bulb for a homologous series of fatty vation in the modulation of intracortical syn-
stress, the “kindling” hypothesis, and the recur- acid odor molecules. Journal of Neurophysiology, chronization. Science, 272, 271–274. (9)
rence of depression. Psychological Review, 112, 67, 786–789. (7) Munoz, D. P., & Everling, S. (2004). Look away:
417–445. (15) Moritz, C. T., Perlmutter, S. I., & Fetz, E. E. (2008). The anti-saccade task and the voluntary control
Monteleone, P., Serritella, C., Scognamiglio, P., & Direct control of paralysed muscles by cortical of eye movement. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5,
Maj, M. (2010). Enhanced ghrelin secretion in neurons. Nature, 456, 639–643. (8) 218–228. (5, 8)
the cephalic phase of food ingestion in women Morley, J. E., Levine, A. S., Grace, M., & Kneip, J. Münzberg, H., & Myers, M. G., Jr. (2005). Molecu-
with bulimia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology, (1985). Peptide YY (PYY), a potent orexigenic lar and anatomical determinants of central leptin
35, 284–288. (10) agent. Brain Research, 341, 200–203. (10) resistance. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 566–570. (10)
Montgomery, K. J., Seeherman, K. R., & Haxby, J. V. Morris, J. A., Jordan, C. L., & Breedlove, S. M. Murali, M. S. (2001). Epidemiological study of
(2009). The well-tempered social brain. Psycho- (2004). Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate prevalence of mental disorders in India. Indian
logical Science, 20, 1211–1213. (8) nervous system. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1034– Journal of Community Medicine, 26, 198. (15)
Montgomery, S. A., Baldwin, D. S., Blier, P., Fine- 1039. (11) Murphy, D. L., Fox, M. A., Timpano, K. R., Moya,
berg, N. A., Kasper, S., Lader, M., et al. (2007). Morris, J. S., deBonis, M., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). P. R., Ren-Patterson, R., Andrews, A. M., et al.
Which antidepressants have demonstrated su- Human amygdala responses to fearful eyes. Neu- (2008). How the serotonin story is being rewrit-
perior efficacy? A review of the evidence. Inter- roImage, 17, 214–222. (12) ten by new gene-based discoveries principally re-
national Clinical Psychopharmacology, 22, 323– Morris, M., Lack, L., & Dawson, D. (1990). Sleep- lated to SLC6A4, the serotonin transporter gene,
329. (15) onset insomniacs have delayed temperature which functions to influence all cellular serotonin
Monti-Bloch, L., Jennings-White, C., & Berliner, rhythms. Sleep, 13, 1–14. (9) systems. Neuropharmacology, 55, 932–960. (3)
D. L. (1998). The human vomeronasal system: A Morrison, A. R., Sanford, L. D., Ball, W. A., Mann, Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D.
review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci- G. L., & Ross, R. J. (1995). Stimulus-elicited be- (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of emotions:
ences, 855, 373–389. (7) havior in rapid eye movement sleep without ato- A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behav-
Monti-Bloch, L., Jennings-White, C., Dolberg, nia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 972–979. (9) ioral Neuroscience, 3, 207–233. (12)
D. S., & Berliner, D. L. (1994). The human vom- Morton, A. J., Wood, N. I., Hastings, M. H., Hurel- Murphy, M. R., Checkley, S. A., Seckl, J. R., &
eronasal system. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, bink, C., Barker, R. A., & Maywood, E. S. (2005). Lightman, S. L. (1990). Naloxone inhibits oxy-
673–686. (7) Disintegration of the sleep–wake cycle and circa- tocin release at orgasm in man. Journal of Clini-
Moody, T. D., Chang, G. Y., Vanek, Z. F., & Knowl- dian timing in Huntington’s disease. Journal of cal Endocrinology & Metabolism, 71, 1056–
ton, B. J. (2010). Concurrent discrimination Neuroscience, 25, 157–163. (9) 1058. (11)
learning in Parkinson’s disease. Behavioral Neuro- Morton, G. J., Cummings, D. E., Baskin, D. G., Murray, G., Nicholas, C. L., Kleiman, J., Dwyer, R.,
science, 124, 1–8. (13) Barsh, G. S., & Schwartz, M. W. (2006). Central Carrington, M. J., Allen, N. B., et al. (2009). Na-
Moore, L. B., Goodwin, B., Jones, S. A., Wisely, nervous system control of food intake and body ture’s clocks and human mood: The circadian
G. B., Serabjit-Singh, C. J., Willson, T. M., et al. weight. Nature, 443, 289–295. (10) system modulates reward motivation. Emotion, 9,
(2000). St. John’s wort induces hepatic drug me- Moruzzi, G., & Magoun, H. W. (1949). Brain stem 705–716. (9)
tabolism through activation of the pregnane X reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Murrell, J., Farlow, M., Ghetti, B., & Benson, M. D.
receptor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiol- (1991). A mutation in the amyloid precursor
Sciences, USA, 97, 7500–7502. (15) ogy, 1, 455–473. (9) protein associated with hereditary Alzheimer’s
Moore, T., Rodman, H. R., Repp, A. B., & Gross, Moss, C. F., & Simmons, A. M. (1986). Frequency disease. Science, 254, 97–99. (13)
C. G. (1995). Localization of visual stimuli after selectivity of hearing in the green treefrog, Hyla Musacchia, G., Sams, M., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N.
striate cortex damage in monkeys: Parallels with cinerea. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 159, (2007). Musicians have enhanced subcortical au-
human blindsight. Proceedings of the National 257–266. (7) ditory and audiovisual processing of speech and
Academy of Sciences, USA, 92, 8215–8218. (6) Moss, S. J., & Smart, T. G. (2001). Constructing music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
Moore-Ede, M. C., Czeisler, C. A., & Richardson, inhibitory synapses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, ences, USA, 104, 15894–15898. (5)
G. S. (1983). Circadian timekeeping in health 2, 240–250. (3) Mutch, D. M., & Clément, K. (2006). Unraveling
and disease. New England Journal of Medicine, Mrosovsky, N. (1990). Rheostasis: The physiology of the genetics of human obesity. PLoS Genetics, 2,
309, 469–476. (9) change. New York: Oxford University Press. (10) e188. (10)
Morand, S. M., Grosbras, M.-H., Caldara, R., & Mrzljak, L., Bergson, C., Pappy, M., Huff, R., Lev- Myers, J. J., & Sperry, R. W. (1985). Interhemi-
Harvey, M. (2010). Looking away from faces: enson, R., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996). Lo- spheric communication after section of the fore-
Influence of high-level visual processes on sac- calization of dopamine D4 receptors in GAB- brain commissures. Cortex, 21, 249–260. (14)
References 525

Nadal, A., Díaz, M., & Valverde, M. A. (2001). The Nelson, D. O., & Prosser, C. L. (1981). Intracellular among U.S. military personnel. American Journal
estrogen trinity: Membrane, cytosolic, and nu­ recordings from thermosensitive preoptic neu- of Psychiatry, 165, 99–106. (15)
clear effects. News in Physiological Sciences, 16, rons. Science, 213, 787–789. (10) Niessing, J., & Friedrich, R. W. (2010). Olfactory
251–255. (11) Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mech- pattern classification by discrete neuronal net-
Nadarajah, B., & Parnavelas, J. G. (2002). Modes of anisms of aggression. Nature Reviews Neurosci- work states. Nature, 465, 47–52. (7)
neuronal migration in the developing cerebral ence, 8, 536–546. (12) Nieuwenhuys, R., Voogd, J., & vanHuijzen, C.
cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 423– Nemeroff, C. B., Heim, C. M., Thase, M. E., Klein, (1988). The human central nervous system (3rd
432. (5) D. N., Rush, J., Shatzberg, A. F., et al. (2003). Rev. ed.). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. (4, 10, 12, 14)
Nader, K., & Hardt, O. (2009). A single standard Differential responses to psychotherapy versus Nilsson, G. E. (1999, December). The cost of a
for memory: The case for reconsolidation. Nature pharmacotherapy in patients with chronic forms brain. Natural History, 108, 66–73. (4)
Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 224–241. (13) of major depression and childhood trauma. Pro- Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the
Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Rob- ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, brain: From phenomenology to neurophysiology.
erts, T., & Merzenich, M. M. (1999). Cortical 100, 14293–14296. (15) Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 88–100. (9)
auditory signal processing in poor readers. Pro- Netter, F. H. (1983). CIBA collection of medical il- Nishimaru, H., Restrepo, C. E., Ryge, J., Yanaga-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, lustrations: Vol. 1. Nervous system. New York: wa, Y., & Kiehn, O. (2005). Mammalian motor
96, 6483–6488. (14) CIBA. (11) neurons corelease glutamate and acetylcholine
Nakashima, Y., Kuwamura, T., & Yogo, Y. (1995). Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality at central synapses. Proceedings of the National
Why be a both-ways sex changer? Ethology, 101, variation in humans and other animals. American Academy of Sciences, USA, 102, 5245–5249.
301–307. (11) Psychologist, 61, 622–631. (12) (3)
Nakata, H., Yoshie, M., Miura, A., & Kudo, K. Neumeister, A., Hu, X.-Z., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Nishimura, Y., Onoe, H., Morichika, Y., Perfiliev, S.,
(2010). Characteristics of the athletes’ brain: Evi- Schwarz, M., Nugent, A. C., Bonne, O., et al. Tsukada, H., & Isa, T. (2007). Time-dependent
dence from neurophysiology and neuroimaging. (2006). Differential effects of x-HTTLPR geno- central compensatory mechanisms of finger dex-
Brain Research Reviews, 62, 197–211. (6) types on the behavioral and neural responses to terity after spinal cord injury. Science, 318, 1150–
Narr, K. L., Woods, R. P., Thompson, P. M., Szesz- tryptophan depletion in patients with major de- 1155. (5)
ko, P., Robinson, D., Dimtcheva, T., et al. (2007). pression and controls. Archives of General Psychi- Nitabach, M. N., & Taghert, P. H. (2008). Organi-
Relationships between IQ and regional cortical atry, 63, 978–986. (15) zation of the Drosophila circadian control circuit.
gray matter thickness in healthy adults. Cerebral Neumeister, A., Nugent, A. C., Waldeck, T., Geraci, Current Biology, 18, R84–R93. (9)
Cortex, 17, 2163–2171. (4) M., Schwarz, M., Bonne, O., et al. (2004). Neural Noaghiul, S., & Hibbeln, J. R. (2003). Cross-
Narrow, W. E., Rae, D. S., Robins, L. N., & Regier, and behavioral responses to tryptophan deple- national comparisons of seafood consumption
D. A. (2002). Revised prevalence estimates of tion in unmedicated patients with remitted ma- and rates of bipolar disorders. American Journal
mental disorders in the United States. Archives of jor depressive disorder and controls. Archives of of Psychiatry, 160, 2222–2227. (15)
General Psychiatry, 59, 115–123. (15) General Psychiatry, 61, 765–773. (15) Noonan, M. A., Bulin, S. E., Fuller, D. C., & Eisch,
Narumoto, J., Okada, T., Sadato, N., Fukui, K., & Neville, H. J., Bavelier, D., Corina, D., Rauschecker, A. J. (2010). Reduction of adult hippocampal
Yonekura, Y. (2001). Attention to emotion mod- J., Karni, A., Lalwani, A., et al. (1998). Cerebral neurogenesis confers vulnerability in an animal
ulates fMRI activity in human right superior organization for language in deaf and hearing model of cocaine addiction. Journal of Neurosci-
temporal sulcus. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, subjects: Biological constraints and effects of ex- ence, 30, 304–315. (3)
225–231. (12, 14) perience. Proceedings of the National Academy of Nordenström, A., Frisén, L., Falhammar, H., Fil-
Nassi, J. J., & Callaway, E. M. (2006). Multiple cir- Sciences, USA, 95, 922–929. (14) ipsson, H., Holmdahl, G., Janson, P. O., et al.
cuits relaying primate parallel visual pathways to Nevin, R. (2007). Understanding international crime (2010). Sexual function and surgical outcome
the middle temporal cortex. Journal of Neurosci- trends: The legacy of preschool lead exposure. En- in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia
ence, 26, 12789–12798. (6) vironmental Research, 104, 315–336. (12) due to CYP21A2 deficiency: Clinical perspec-
Nassi, J. J., & Callaway, E. M. (2009). Parallel Newcomer, J. W., Selke, G., Melson, A. K., Her- tive and the patients’ perception. Journal of
processing strategies of the primate visual sys- shey, T., Craft, S., Richards, K., et al. (1999). Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 95,
tem. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 360– Decreased memory performance in healthy hu- 3633–3640. (11)
372. (6) mans induced by stress-level cortisol treat- Nordenström, A., Servin, A., Bohlin, G., Larsson,
Nathans, J., Davenport, C. M., Maumenee, I. H., ment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 527– A., & Wedell, A. (2002). Sex-typed toy play be-
Lewis, R. A., Hejtmancik, J. F., Litt, M., et al. 533. (13) havior correlates with the degree of prenatal an-
(1989). Molecular genetics of human blue cone Nicholas, M. K., & Arnason, B. G. W. (1992). Im- drogen exposure assessed by CYP21 genotype in
monochromacy. Science, 245, 831–838. (6) munologic responses in central nervous system girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Journal
Naumer, M. J., & van den Bosch, J. J. F. (2009). transplantation. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 4, of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 87,
Touching sounds: Thalamocortical plasticity and 273–283. (8) 5119–5124. (11)
the neural basis of multisensory integration. Jour- Nicholls, M. E. R., Orr, C. A., Okubo, M., & Loftus, Norman, R. A., Tataranni, P. A., Pratley, R., Thomp-
nal of Neurophysiology, 102, 7–8. (7) A. (2006). Satisfaction guaranteed: The effect of son, D. B., Hanson, R. L., Prochazka, M., et al.
Nebes, R. D. (1974). Hemispheric specialization in spatial biases on responses to Likert scales. Psy- (1998). Autosomal genomic scan for loci linked
commissurotomized man. Psychological Bulletin, chological Science, 17, 1027–1028. (14) to obesity and energy metabolism in Pima Indi-
81, 1–14. (14) Nicklas, W. J., Saporito, M., Basma, A., Geller, ans. American Journal of Human Genetics, 62,
Nef, P. (1998). How we smell: The molecular and H. M., & Heikkila, R. E. (1992). Mitochondrial 659–668. (10)
cellular bases of olfaction. News in Physiological mechanisms of neurotoxicity. Annals of the New North, R. A. (1989). Neurotransmitters and their
Sciences, 13, 1–5. (7) York Academy of Sciences, 648, 28–36. (8) receptors: From the clone to the clinic. Seminars
Neitz, M., Kraft, T. W., & Neitz, J. (1998). Expres- Nicolelis, M. A. L., Ghazanfar, A. A., Stambaugh, in the Neurosciences, 1, 81–90. (3)
sion of L cone pigment gene subtypes in females. C. R., Oliveira, L. M. O., Laubach, M., Chapin, J. North, R. A. (1992). Cellular actions of opiates and
Vision Research, 38, 3221–3225. (6) K., et al. (1998). Simultaneous encoding of tac- cocaine. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-
Nelson, C. A., Wewerka, S., Thomas, K. M., Tribby- tile information by three primate cortical areas. ences, 654, 1–6. (3)
Walbridge, S., deRegnier, R., & Georgieff, M. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 621–630. (4) Noseda, R., Kainz, V., Jakubowki, M., Gooley, J. J.,
(2000). Neurocognitive sequelae of infants of Niebuhr, D. W., Millikan, A. M., Cowan, D. N., Saper, C. B., Digre, K., et al. (2010). A neural
diabetic mothers. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114, Yolken, R., Li, Y., & Weber, N. S. (2008). Se- mechanism for exacerbation of headache by light.
950–956. (5) lected infectious agents and risk of schizophrenia Nature Neuroscience, 13, 239–245. (9)
526 References

Nosenko, N. D., & Reznikov, A. G. (2001). Prenatal Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, Oxley, D. R., Smith, K. B., Alford, J. R., Hibbing,
stress and sexual differentiation of monoaminergic 419–428. (3) M. V., Miller, J. L., Scalora, M., et al. (2008). Po-
brain systems. Neurophysiology, 33, 197–206. (11) O’Leary, A. (1990). Stress, emotion, and human im- litical attitudes vary with physiological traits. Sci-
Nottebohm, F. (2002). Why are some neurons re- mune function. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 363– ence, 321, 1667–1670. (12)
placed in adult brain? Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 382. (12) Padmos, R. C., Hillegers, M. H. J., Knijff, E. M.,
624–628. (5) Oler, J. A., Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Rogers, J., Dyer, Vonk, R., Bouvy, A., Staal, F. J. T., et al. (2008). A
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of T. D., Davidson, R. J., et al. (2010). Amygdalar discriminating messenger RNA signature for bi-
indirect reciprocity. Science, 437, 1291–1298. (1) and hippocampal substrates of anxious tempera- polar disorder formed by an aberrant expression
Nugent, F. S., Penick, S. C., & Kauer, J. A. (2007). ment differ in their heritability. Nature, 466, of inflammatory genes in monocytes. Archives of
Opioids block long-term potentiation of inhibi- 864–868. (12) General Psychiatry, 65, 395–407. (15)
tory synapses. Nature, 446, 1086–1090. (13) Oliet, S. H. R., Baimouknametova, D. V., Piet, R., Pakaprot, N., Kim, S., & Thompson, R. F. (2009).
Numan, M., & Woodside, B. (2010). Maternity: & Bains, J. S. (2007). Retrograde regulation of The role of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus in
Neural mechanisms, motivational processes, and GABA transmission by the tonic release of oxy- short and long term memory for trace eyeblink
physiological adaptations. Behavioral Neurosci- tocin and endocannabinoids governs postsyn- conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 54–
ence, 124, 715–741. (11, 12) aptic firing. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 1325– 61. (13)
Nunes, S. O. V., Itano, E. N., Amarante, M. K., 1333. (3) Palinkas, L. A. (2003). The psychology of isolated
Reiche, E. M. V., Miranda, H. C., de Oliveira, Olney, J. W., & Farber, N. B. (1995). Glutamate re- and confined environments. American Psycholo-
C. E. C., et al. (2008). REN from Borna disease ceptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Archives gist, 58, 353–363. (9)
virus in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffec- of General Psychiatry, 52, 998–1007. (15) Pallier, P. N., Maywood, E. S., Zheng, Z., Chesham,
tive patients, and in their biological relatives. Olson, D. J., Kamil, A. C., Balda, R. P., & Nims, P. J. J. E., Inyushkin, A. N., Dyball, R., et al. (2007).
Journal of Clinical Laboratory Analysis, 22, 314– (1995). Performance of four seed-caching corvid Pharmacological imposition of sleep slows cogni-
320. (15) species in operant tests of nonspatial and spatial tive decline and reverses dysregulation of circa-
Nwulia, E. A., Miao, K., Zandi, P. P., Mackinnon, memory. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, dian gene expression in a transgenic mouse
D. F., DePaulo, J. R., Jr., & McInnis, M. G. 173–181. (13) model of Huntington’s disease. Journal of Neuro-
(2007). Genome-wide scan of bipolar II disor- Olson, E. J., Boeve, B. F., & Silber, M. H. (2000). science, 27, 7869–7878. (8)
der. Bipolar Disorders, 9, 580–588. (15) Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: Palop, J. J., Chin, J., & Mucke, L. (2006). A network
Ó Scalaidhe, S. P., Wilson, F. A. W., & Goldman- Demographic, clinical and laboratory findings in dysfunction perspective on neurodegenerative
Rakic, P. S. (1997). Areal segregation of face- 93 cases. Brain, 123, 331–339. (9) diseases. Nature, 443, 768–773. (13)
processing neurons in prefrontal cortex. Science, Olton, D. S., & Papas, B. C. (1979). Spatial memory Palva, S., Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., Näätänen, R., &
278, 1135–1138. (6) and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia, 17, Palva, J. M. (2005). Early neural correlates of
O’Dowd, B. F., Lefkowitz, R. J., & Caron, M. G. 669–682. (13) conscious somatosensory perception. Journal of
(1989). Structure of the adrenergic and related Olton, D. S., Walker, J. A., & Gage, F. H. (1978). Neuroscience, 25, 5248–5258. (7)
receptors. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 12, Hippocampal connections and spatial discrimi- Pandey, G. N., Pandey, S. C., Dwivedi, Y., Sharma,
67–83. (3) nation. Brain Research, 139, 295–308. (13) R. P., Janicak, P. G., & Davis, J. M. (1995). Plate-
O’Hara, M. W. (2009). Postpartum depression: Ono, M., Igarashi, T., Ohno, E., & Sasaki, M. let serotonin-2A receptors: A potential biological
What we know. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, (1995). Unusual thermal defence by a honeybee marker for suicidal behavior. American Journal of
1258–1269. (15) against mass attack by hornets. Nature, 377, Psychiatry, 152, 850–855. (12)
Ohira, K., Furuta, T., Hioki, H., Nakamura, K. C., 334–336. (10) Pandey, S. C., Zhang, H., Ugale, R., Prakash, A., Xu,
Kuramoto, E., Tanaka, Y., et al. (2010). Ornstein, R. (1997). The right mind. New York: T., & Misra, K. (2008). Effector intermediate-
Ischemia-induced neurogenesis of neocortical Harcourt Brace. (14) early gene Arc in the amygdala plays a critical role
layer 1 progenitor cells. Nature Neuroscience, 13, Ostrovsky, Y., Meyers, E., Ganesh, S., Mathur, U., & in alcoholism. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 2589–
173–179. (5) Sinha, P. (2009). Visual parsing after recovery 2600. (3)
Oka, Y., Omura, M., Kataoka, H., & Touhara, K. from blindness. Psychological Science, 20, 1484– Panov, A. V., Gutekunst, C.-A., Leavitt, B. R.,
(2004). Olfactory receptor antagonism between 1491. (6) Hayden, M. R, Burke, J. R., Strittmatter, W. J.,
odorants. The EMBO Journal, 23, 120–126. (7) Otmakhov, N., Tao-Cheng, J.-H., Carpenter, S., As- et al. (2002). Early mitochondrial calcium de-
O’Kane, G., Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). rican, B., Dosemici, A., & Reese, T. S. (2004). fects in Huntington’s disease are a direct effect
Evidence for semantic learning in profound am- Persistent accumulation of calcium/calmodulin- of polyglutamines. Nature Neuroscience, 5,
nesia: An investigation with patient H. M. Hip- dependent protein kinase II in dendritic spines 731–736. (8)
pocampus, 14, 417–425. (13) after induction of NMDA receptor-dependent Pardal, R., & López-Barneo, J. (2002). Low glucose-
O’Keefe, J., & Burgess, N. (1996). Geometric deter- chemical long-term potentiation. Journal of Neu- sensing cells in the carotid body. Nature Neurosci-
minants of the place fields of hippocampal neu- roscience, 25, 9324–9331. (13) ence, 5, 197–198. (10)
rons. Nature, 381, 425–434. (13) Ouchi, Y., Yoshikawa, E., Okada, H., Futatsubashi, Paré, M., Smith, A. M., & Rice, F. L. (2002). Distri-
Oláh, S., Füle, M., Komlósi, G., Varga, S., Báldi, R., M., Sekine, Y., Iyo, M., et al. (1999). Alterations bution and terminal arborizations of cutaneous
Barzó, P., et al. (2009). Regulation of microcir- in binding site density of dopamine transporter mechanoreceptors in the glabrous finger pads of
cuits by unitary GABA-mediated volume trans- in the striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and amyg- the monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
mission. Nature, 461,1278–1281. (3) dala in early Parkinson’s disease: Compartment 445, 347–359. (7)
Olanow, C. W., Goetz, C. G., Kordower, J. H., analysis for ß-CFT binding with positron emis- Paredes, R. G., & Vazquez, B. (1999). What do fe-
Stoessl, A. J., Sossi, V., Brin, M. F., et al. (2003). sion tomography. Annals of Neurology, 45, 601– male rats like about sex? Paced mating. Behav-
A double-blind controlled trial of bilateral fetal 610. (8) ioural Brain Research, 105, 117–127. (11)
nigral transplantation in Parkinson’s disease. An- Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, Parent, M. B., Habib, M. K., & Baker, G. B. (1999).
nals of Neurology, 54, 403–414. (8) M. H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2006). De- Task-dependent effects of the antidepressant/
Olds, J. (1958). Satiation effects in self-stimulation tecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, antipanic drug phenelzine on memory. Psycho-
of the brain. Journal of Comparative and Physio- 313, 1402. (14) pharmacology, 142, 280–288. (9)
logical Psychology, 51, 675–678. (3) Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Sten- Park, I.-H., Zhao, R., West, J. A., Yabuuchi, A.,
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement ton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., et al. (2010). Huo, H., Ince, T. A., et al. (2008). Reprogram-
produced by electrical stimulation of the septal Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465, ming of human somatic cells to pluripotency
area and other regions of the rat brain. Journal of 775–778. (5) with defined factors. Nature, 451, 141–146. (8)
References 527

Park, I. S., Lee, K. J., Han, J. W., Lee, N. J., Lee, Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, Pepperberg, I. M. (1994). Numerical competence in
W. T., Park, K. A., et al. (2009). Experience-de- England: Oxford University Press. (13) an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Jour-
pendent plasticity of cerebellar vermis in basket- Pearson, H. (2006). Freaks of nature? Nature, 444, nal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 36–44. (14)
ball players. Cerebellum, 8, 334–339. (8) 1000–1001. (8) Pepperberg, I. M. (2004). “Insightful” string-pulling
Park, S., Holzman, P. S., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Peet, M. (2004). Nutrition and schizophrenia: Be- in grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is affected by
(1995). Spatial working memory deficits in the yond omega-3 fatty acids. Prostaglandins, Leukot- vocal competence. Animal Cognition, 7, 263–
relatives of schizophrenic patients. Archives of rienes and Essential Fatty Acids, 70, 417–422. (15) 266. (14)
General Psychiatry, 52, 821–828. (15) Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri- Perälä, J., Suvisaari, J., Saarni, S. I., Kuoppasalmi,
Parker, G. H. (1922). Smell, taste, and allied senses in Desstro, M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G., & Or- K., Isometsä, E., Pirkola, S., et al. (2007). Life-
the vertebrates. Philadelphia: Lippincott. (7) ban, G. A. (2009). The representation of tool use time prevalence of psychotic and bipolar I disor-
Parr-Brownlie, L. C., & Hyland, B. I. (2005). Brady- in humans and monkeys: Common and uniquely ders in a general population. Archives of General
kinesia induced by dopamine D2 receptor block- human features. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, Psychiatry, 64, 19–28. (15)
ade is associated with reduced motor cortex ac- 11523–11539. (1) Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2005). The neural basis
tivity in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., Collette, F., of first and second language processing. Current
5700–5709. (8) Perrin, F., Reggers, J., et al. (2004). Are spatial Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 202–206. (14)
Parton, L. E., Ye, C. P., Coppari, R., Enriori, P. J., memories strengthened in the human hippo- Perera, T. D., Coplan, J. D., Lisanby, S. H., Lipira,
Choi, B., Zhang, C.-Y., et al. (2007). Glucose campus during slow wave sleep? Neuron, 44, C. M., Arif, M., Carpio, C., et al. (2007). Antide-
sensing by POMC neurons regulates glucose ho- 535–545. (9) pressant-induced neurogenesis in the hippocam-
meostasis and is impaired in obesity. Nature, 449, Peleg, G., Katzir, G., Peleg, O., Kamara, M., Brod- pus of adult nonhuman primates. Journal of Neu-
228–232. (10) sky, L., Hel-or, H., et al. (2006). Hereditary fam- roscience, 27, 4894–4901. (15)
Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., Shannon, ily signature of facial expressions. Proceedings of Peretz, I., Cummings, S., & Dube, M. P. (2007). The
R. W., Nicholson, E., Coleman, M., et al. (2005). the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, genetics of congenital amusia (tone deafness): A
Plasticity of face processing in infancy. Proceed- 15921–15926. (1) family-aggregation study. American Journal of
ings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, Pelli, D. G., & Tillman, K. A. (2008). The uncrowd- Human Genetics, 81, 582–588. (7)
102, 5297–5300. (6) ed window of object recognition. Nature Neuro- Perlow, M. J., Freed, W. J., Hoffer, B. J., Seiger, A.,
Pascual, A., Hidalgo-Figueroa, M., Piruat, J. I., Pin- science, 11, 1129–1135. (6) Olson, L., & Wyatt, R. J. (1979). Brain grafts re-
tado, C. O., Gómez-Díaz, R., & López-Barneo, J. Pellis, S. M., O’Brien, D. P., Pellis, V. C., Teitelbaum, duce motor abnormalities produced by destruc-
(2008). Absolute requirement of GDNF for P., Wolgin, D. L., & Kennedy, S. (1988). Escala- tion of nigrostriatal dopamine system. Science,
adult catecholaminergic neuron survival. Nature tion of feline predation along a gradient from 204, 643–647. (8)
Neuroscience, 11, 755–761. (5) avoidance through “play” to killing. Behavioral Pernía-Andrade, A. J., Kato, A., Witschi, R., Nyilas,
Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hind- Neuroscience, 102, 760–777. (12) R., Katona, I., Freund, T. F., et al. (2009). Spinal
marsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prena- Pellymounter, M. A., Cullen, M. J., Baker, M. B., endocannabinoids and CB1 receptors mediate C-
tal hormones and postnatal socialization by par- Hecht, R., Winters, D., Boone, T., et al. (1995). fiber-induced heterosynaptic pain sensitization.
ents as determinants of male-typical toy play in Effects of the obese gene product on body Science, 325, 760–764. (7)
girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Child weight regulation in ob/ob mice. Science, 269, Perrone, J. A., & Thiele, A. (2001). Speed skills:
Development, 76, 264–278. (11) 540–543. (10) Measuring the visual speed analyzing properties
Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. Penagos, H., Melcher, J. R., & Oxenham, A. J. of primate MT neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 4,
New York: Oxford University Press. (14) (2004). A neural representation of pitch salience 526–532. (6)
Patil, S. T., Zhang, L., Martenyi, F., Lowe, S. L., in nonprimary human auditory cortex revealed Pert, C. B. (1997). Molecules of emotion. New York:
Jackson, K. A., Andreev, B. V., et al. (2007). Acti- with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Touchstone. (7)
vation of mGlu 2/3 receptors as a new approach Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6810–6815. (7) Pert, C. B., & Snyder, S. H. (1973). The opiate re-
to treat schizophrenia: A randomized phase 2 Penfield, W. (1955). The permanent record of the ceptor: Demonstration in nervous tissue. Science,
clinical trial. Nature Medicine, 1102–1107. (15) stream of consciousness. Acta Psychologica, 11, 179, 1011–1014. (3, 7)
Patston, L L. M., Corballis, M. C., Hogg, S. L., & 47–69. (13) Pesold, C., & Treit, D. (1995). The central and ba-
Tippet, L. J. (2006). The neglect of musicians: Penfield, W., & Milner, B. (1958). Memory deficit solateral amygdala differentially mediate the anx-
Line bisection reveals an opposite bias. Psycho- produced by bilateral lesions in the hippocampal iolytic effect of benzodiazepines. Brain Research,
logical Science, 17, 1029–1031. (14) zone. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 79, 671, 213–221. (12)
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). 475–497. (13) Peters, F., Nicolson, N. A., Berkhof, J., Delespaul, P.,
Where do you know what you know? The rep- Penfield, W., & Perot, P. (1963). The brain’s record & deVries, M. (2003). Effects of daily events on
resentation of semantic knowledge in the hu- of auditory and visual experience. Brain, 86, mood states in major depressive disorder. Journal
man brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 595–696. (13) of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 203–211. (15)
976–987. (13) Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral Peters, R. H., Sensenig, L. D., & Reich, M. J. (1973).
Paul, L. K., Brown, W. S., Adolphs, R., Tyszka, cortex of man. New York: Macmillan. (4, 8) Fixed-ratio performance following ventromedial
J. M., Richards, L. J., Mukherjee, P., et al. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Ko- hypothalamic lesions in rats. Physiological Psy-
(2007). Agenesis of the corpus callosum: Ge- bayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., et al. chology, 1, 136–138. (10)
netic, developmental and functional aspects of (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Peters, R. M., Hackeman, E., & Goldreich, D.
connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, Nature, 399, 741–742. (11) (2009). Diminutive digits discern delicate de-
287–299. (14) Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations tails: Fingertip size and the sex difference in tac-
Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., by an African grey parrot. Zeitschrift für Tierpsy- tile spatial acuity. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1996). Is chologie, 55, 139–160. (14) 15756–15761. (7)
developmental dyslexia a disconnection syn- Pepperberg, I. M. (1993). Cognition and communi- Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-
drome? Brain, 119, 143–157. (14) cation in an African grey parrot (Psittacus eritha- term retention of individual verbal items. Journal
Paus, T., Marrett, S., Worsley, K. J., & Evans, A. C. cus): Studies on a nonhuman, nonprimate, non- of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198. (13)
(1995). Extraretinal modulation of cerebral mammalian subject. In H. L. Roitblat, L. M. Petitto, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E. J.,
blood flow in the human visual cortex: Implica- Herman, & P. E. Nachtigall (Eds.), Language and Dostie, D., & Evans, A. C. (2000). Speech-like ce-
tions for saccadic suppression. Journal of Neuro- communication: Comparative perspectives (pp. rebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing
physiology, 74, 2179–2183. (6) 221–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (14) signed languages: Implications for the neural basis
528 References

of human language. Proceedings of the National Plomin, R., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. Potegal, M. (1994). Aggressive arousal: The amyg-
Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, 13961–13966. (14) (1990). Individual differences in television view- dala connection. In M. Potegal & J. F. Knutson
Petrovic, P., Kalso, E., Petersson, K. M., & Ingvar, ing in early childhood: Nature as well as nurture. (Eds.), The dynamics of aggression (pp. 73–111).
M. (2002). Placebo and opioid analgesia—Imag- Psychological Science, 1, 371–377. (1) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (12)
ing a shared neuronal network. Science, 295, Ploner, M., Gross, J., Timmerman, L., & Schnitzler, Potegal, M., Ferris, C., Hebert, M., Meyerhoff, J. M.,
1737–1740. (7) A. (2006). Pain processing is faster than tactile & Skaredoff, L. (1996). Attack priming in female
Pfaff, D. W. (2007). The neuropsychology of fair play. processing in the human brain. Journal of Neuro- Syrian golden hamsters is associated with a c-fos
New York: Dana Press. (12) science, 26, 10879–10882. (7) coupled process within the corticomedial amyg-
Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., & Liberzon, I. Plutchik, R. (1982). A psychoevolutionary theory dala. Neuroscience, 75, 869–880. (12)
(2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A of emotions. Social Science Information, 21, 529– Potegal, M., Hebert, M., DeCoster, M., & Meyer-
meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in 553. (12) hoff, J. L. (1996). Brief, high-frequency stimula-
PET and fMRI. NeuroImage, 16, 331–348. (12) Poduslo, S. E., Huang, R., & Spiro, A. III (2009). A tion of the corticomedial amygdala induces a de-
Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1985). Positron genome screen of successful aging without cogni- layed and prolonged increase of aggressiveness in
emission tomography: Human brain function tive decline identifies LRP1B by haplotype anal- male Syrian golden hamsters. Behavioral Neuro-
and biochemistry. Science, 228, 799–809. (4) ysis. American Journal of Medical Genetics, B, science, 110, 401–412. (12)
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, 153B, 114–119. (1) Potegal, M., Robison, S., Anderson, F., Jordan, C., &
M., Andrew, C., Calder, A. J., et al. (1997). A spe- Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Shapiro, E. (2007). Sequence and priming in 15
cific neural substrate for perceiving facial expres- Asarnow, R. F., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2005). month-olds’ reactions to brief arm restraint: Evi-
sions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–498. (12) The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. dence for a hierarchy of anger responses. Aggres-
Pich, E. M., Pagliusi, S. R., Tessari, M., Talabot- Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5356–5364. (8) sive Behavior, 33, 508–518. (12)
Ayer, D., van Huijsduijnen, R. H., & Chiam- Poling, A., Schlinger, H., & Blakely, E. (1988). Fail- Poulsen, P., Esteller, M., Vaag, A., & Fraga, M. F.
ulera, C. (1997). Common neural substrates for ure of the partial inverse benzodiazepine agonist (2007). The epigenetic basis of twin discordance
the addictive properties of nicotine and cocaine. Ro15-4513 to block the lethal effects of ethanol in age-related diseases. Pediatric Research, 61(5),
Science, 275, 83–86. (3) in rats. Pharmacology, 31, 945–947. (12) Part 2, 38R–42R. (1)
Pierri, J. N., Volk, C. L. E., Auh, S., Sampson, A., & Polk, T. A., Drake, R. M., Jonides, J. J., Smith, M. R., Powell, L. H., Calvin, J. E., III, & Calvin, J. E., Jr.
Lewis, D. A. (2001). Decreased somal size of & Smith, E. E. (2008). Attention enhances the (2007). Effective obesity treatments. American
deep layer 3 pyramidal neurons in the prefrontal neural processing of relevant features and sup- Psychologist, 62, 234–246. (10)
cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Archives of presses the processing of irrelevant features in Premack, A. J., & Premack, D. (1972). Teaching lan-
General Psychiatry, 58, 466–473. (15) humans: A functional magnetic resonance imag- guage to an ape. Scientific American, 227(4), 92–
Pietropaolo, S., Feldon, J., Alleva, E., Cirulli, F., & ing study of the Stroop task. Journal of Neurosci- 99. (14)
Yee, B. K. (2006). The role of voluntary exercise ence, 28, 13786–13792. (14) Preti, G., Cutler, W. B., Garcia, C. R., Huggins,
in enriched rearing: A behavioral analysis. Behav- Poncer, J. C., & Malinow, R. (2001). Postsynaptic G. R., & Lawley, H. J. (1986). Human axillary
ioral Neuroscience, 120, 787–803. (5) conversion of silent synapses during LTP affects secretions influence women’s menstrual cycles:
Pillon, B., Ertle, S., Deweer, B., Sarazin, M., Agid, synaptic gain and transmission dynamics. Nature The role of donor extract of females. Hormones
Y., & Dubois, B. (1996). Memory for spatial lo- Neuroscience, 4, 989–996. (13) and Behavior, 20, 474–482. (7)
cation is affected in Parkinson’s disease. Neuro- Pons, T. P., Garraghty, P. E., Ommaya, A. K., Kaas, Price, M. P., Lewin, G. R., McIlwrath, S. L., Cheng, C.,
psychologia, 34, 77–85. (8) J. H., Taub, E., & Mishkin, M. (1991). Massive cor- Xie, J., Heppenstall, P. A., et al. (2000). The mam-
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: tical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in malian sodium channel BNC1 is required for nor-
HarperCollins. (14) adult macaques. Science, 252, 1857–1860. (5) mal touch sensation. Nature, 407, 1007–1011. (7)
Pinto, L., & Götz, M. (2007). Radial glial cell het- Pontieri, F. E., Tanda, G., Orzi, F., & DiChiara, G. Pritchard, T. C., Hamilton, R. B., Morse, J. R., &
erogeneity: The source of diverse progeny in the (1996). Effects of nicotine on the nucleus accum- Norgren, R. (1986). Projections of thalamic gus-
CNS. Progress in Neurobiology, 83, 2–23. (2) bens and similarity to those of addictive drugs. tatory and lingual areas in the monkey, Macaca
Pistoia, F., Conson, M., Trojano, L., Grossi, D., Po- Nature, 382, 255–257. (3) fascicularis. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
nari, M., Colonnese, C., et al. (2010). Impaired Poo, M.-m. (2001). Neurotrophins as synaptic modu- 244, 213–228. (7)
conscious recognition of negative facial expres- lators. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 24–32. (5) Pritchard, T. C., Macaluso, D. A., & Eslinger, P. J.
sions in patients with locked-in syndrome. Jour- Pope, H. G., Jr., Gruber, A. J., Hudson, J. I., Huestis, (1999). Taste perception in patients with insular
nal of Neuroscience, 30, 7838–7844. (12) M. A., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2001). Neuropsy- cortex lesions. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113,
Pizzagalli, D. A., Holmes, A. J., Dillon, D. G., chological performance in long-term cannabis us- 663–671. (7, 14)
Goetz, E. L., Birk, J. L., Bogdan, R., et al. (2009). ers. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 909–915. (3) Prober, D. A., Rihel, J., Onah, A. A., Sung, R.-J., &
Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens re- Poremba, A., Saunders, R. C., Crane, A. M., Cook, Schier, A. F. (2006). Hypocretin/orexin overex-
sponse to rewards in unmedicated individuals M., Sokoloff, L., & Mishkin, M. (2003). Func- pression induces an insomnia-like phenotype in
with major depressive disorder. American Journal tional mapping of the primate auditory system. zebrafish. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 13400–
of Psychiatry, 166, 702–710. (3) Science, 299, 568–572. (7) 13410. (9)
Pizzagalli, D. A., Nitschke, J. B., Oakes, T. R., Hen- Porter, J., Craven, B., Khan, R. M., Chang, S.-J., Provine, R. R. (1979). “Wing-flapping” develops in
drick, A. M., Horras, K. A., Larson, C. L., et al. Kang, I., Judkewicz, B., et al. (2007). Mecha- wingless chicks. Behavioral and Neural Biology,
(2002). Brain electrical tomography in depres- nisms of scent-tracking in humans. Nature Neu- 27, 233–237. (8)
sion: The importance of symptom severity, anxi- roscience, 10, 27–29. (7) Provine, R. R. (1981). Wing-flapping develops in
ety, and melancholic features. Biological Psychia- Posner, S. F., Baker, L., Heath, A., & Martin, N. G. chickens made flightless by feather mutations.
try, 52, 73–85. (15) (1996). Social contact, social attitudes, and twin Developmental Psychobiology, 14, 48 B 1–486. (8)
Pizzorusso, T., Medini, P., Berardi, N., Chierzi, S., similarity. Behavior Genetics, 26, 123–133. (1) Provine, R. R. (1984). Wing-flapping during devel-
Fawcett, J. W., & Maffei, L. (2002). Reactivation Post, R. M. (1992). Transduction of psychological opment and evolution. American Scientist, 72,
of ocular dominance plasticity in the adult visual stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective 448–455. (8)
cortex. Science, 298, 1248–1251. (6) disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, Provine, R. R. (1986). Yawning as a stereotyped ac-
Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and 999–1010. (15) tion pattern and releasing stimulus. Ethology, 72,
late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedur- Post, R. M., & Silberstein, S. D. (1994). Shared 109–122. (8)
al memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, mechanisms in affective illness, epilepsy, and mi- Prutkin, J., Duffy, V. B., Etter, L., Fast, K., Gardner, E.,
534–547. (9) graine. Neurology, 44(Suppl 7), S37–S47. (15) Lucchina, L. A., et al. (2000). Genetic variation and
References 529

inferences about perceived taste intensity in mice brain volume loss over time in cannabis-using Ramón y Cajal, S. see Cajal, S. R.
and men. Physiology & Behavior, 69, 161–173. (7) first-episode schizophrenia patients. American Ranson, S. W., & Clark, S. L. (1959). The anatomy
Ptak, R., & Schnider, A. (2010). The dorsal atten- Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 490–496. (15) of the nervous system: Its development and function
tion network mediates orienting toward behav- Raj, A., Rifkin, S. A., Andersen, E., & van Oude- (10th ed.). Philadelphia: Saunders. (4)
iorally relevant stimuli in spatial neglect. Journal naarden, A. (2010). Variability in gene expres- Rapoport, S. I., & Bosetti, F. (2002). Do lithium
of Neuroscience, 30, 12557–12565. (14) sion underlies incomplete penetrance. Nature, and anticonvulsants target the brain arachidonic
Puca, A. A., Daly, M. J., Brewster, S. J., Matise, T. C., 463, 913–918. (1) acid cascade in bipolar patients? Archives of Gen-
Barrett, J., Shea-Drinkwater, M., et al. (2001). A Rajkowska, G., Selemon, L. D., & Goldman-Rakic, eral Psychiatry, 59, 592–596. (15)
genome-wide scan for linkage to human excep- P. S. (1998). Neuronal and glial somal size in the Rapoport, S. I., & Robinson, P. J. (1986). Tight-
tional longevity identifies a locus on chromo- prefrontal cortex. Archives of General Psychiatry, junctional modification as the basis of osmotic
some 4. Proceedings of the National Academy of 55, 215–224. (15) opening of the blood–brain barrier. Annals of the
Sciences (U.S.A.), 98, 10505–10508. (1) Rakic, P. (1998). Cortical development and evolu- New York Academy of Sciences, 481, 250–267. (2)
Purcell, D. W., Blanchard, R., & Zucker, K. J. (2000). tion. In M. S. Gazzaniga & J. S. Altman (Eds.), Rasch, B., Pommer, J., Diekelmann, & Born, J.
Birth order in a contemporary sample of gay men. Brain and mind: Evolutionary perspectives (pp. (2009). Pharmacological REM sleep suppres-
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 29, 349–356. (11) 34–40). Strasbourg, France: Human Frontier sion paradoxically improves rather than im-
Purves, D., & Hadley, R. D. (1985). Changes in the Science Program. (5) pairs skill memory. Nature Neuroscience, 12,
dendritic branching of adult mammalian neu- Rakic, P. (2008). Interview. Nature Reviews Neurosci- 396–397. (9)
rones revealed by repeated imaging in situ. Na- ence, 9, 893–894. (5) Raschle, N. M., Chang, M., & Gaab, N. (2011).
ture, 315, 404–406. (5) Rakic, P., & Lidow, M. S. (1995). Distribution and Structural brain alterations associated with dys-
Purves, D., & Lotto, R. B. (2003). Why we see what density of monoamine receptors in the primate lexia predate reading onset. NeuroImage, 57,
we do: An empirical theory of vision. Sunderland, visual cortex devoid of retinal input from early 742–749. (14)
MA: Sinauer Associates. (6) embryonic stages. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, Rattenborg, N. C., Amlaner, C. J., & Lima, S. L.
Purves, D., Shimpi, A., & Lotto, R. B. (1999). An 2561–2574. (6) (2000). Behavioral, neurophysiological and evo-
empirical explanation of the Cornsweet effect. Ralph, M. A. L., Cipolotti, L., Manes, F., & Patter- lutionary perspectives on unihemispheric sleep.
Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 8542–8551. (6) son, K. (2010). Taking both sides: Do unilateral Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 817–
Putnam, S. K., Du, J., Sato, S., & Hull, E. M. (2001). anterior temporal lobe lesions disrupt semantic 842. (9)
Testosterone restoration of copulatory behavior memory? Brain, 133, 3243–3255. (13) Rattenborg, N. C., Mandt, B. H., Obermeyer, W.
correlates with medial preoptic dopamine release Ralph, M. R., Foster, R. G., Davis, F. C., & Menaker, H., Winsauer, P. J., Huber, R., Wikelski, M., et al.
in castrated male rats. Hormones and Behavior, M. (1990). Transplanted suprachiasmatic nucle- (2004). Migratory sleeplessness in the white-
39, 216–224. (11) us determines circadian period. Science, 247, crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys gam-
Queen, T. L., & Hess, T. M. (2010). Age differences 975–978. (9) belii). PLoS Biology, 2, 924–936. (9)
in the effects of conscious and unconscious Ralph, M. R., & Menaker, M. (1988). A mutation of Raum, W. J., McGivern, R. F., Peterson, M. A.,
thought in decision making. Psychology and Ag- the circadian system in golden hamsters. Science, Shryne, J. H., & Gorski, R. A. (1990). Prenatal
ing, 25, 251–261. (5) 241, 1225–1227. (9) inhibition of hypothalamic sex steroid uptake by
Quraishi, S., & Frangou, S. (2002). Neuropsychol- Ramachandran, V. S. (2003, May). Hearing colors, cocaine: Effects on neurobehavioral sexual differ-
ogy of bipolar disorder: A review. Journal of Affec- tasting shapes. Scientific American, 288(5), 52– entiation in male rats. Developmental Brain Re-
tive Disorders, 72, 209–226. (15) 59. (7) search, 53, 230–236. (11)
Rada, P. V., & Hoebel, B. G. (2000). Supraadditive Ramachandran, V. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phan- Rauskolb, S., Zagrebelsky, M., Dreznjak, A., De-
effect of ∆-fenfluramine plus phentermine on toms in the brain. New York: Morrow. (5) ogracias, R., Matsumoto, T., Wiese, S., et al.
extracellular acetylcholine in the nucleus accum- Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1998). The (2010). Global deprivation of brain-derived neu-
bens: Possible mechanism for inhibition of exces- perception of phantom limbs: The D. O. Hebb rotrophic factor in the CNS reveals an area-
sive feeding and drug abuse. Pharmacology Bio- lecture. Brain, 121, 1603–1630. (5) specific requirement for dendritic growth. Jour-
chemistry and Behavior, 65, 369–373. (10) Ramirez, J. J. (2001). The role of axonal sprouting in nal of Neuroscience, 30, 1739–1749. (5)
Radoeva, P. D., Prasad, S., Brainard, D. H., & Agu- functional reorganization after CNS injury: Les- Rayner, K., & Johnson, R. L. (2005). Letter-by-letter
irre, G. K. (2008). Neural activity within area V1 sons from the hippocampal formation. Restorative acquired dyslexia is due to the serial encoding of
reflects unconscious visual performance in a case Neurology and Neuroscience, 19, 237–262. (5) letters. Psychological Science, 16, 530–534. (14)
of blindsight. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Ramirez, J. J., Bulsara, K. R., Moore, S. C., Ruch, K., Redmond, D. E., Jr., Bjugstad, K. B., Teng, Y. D.,
20, 1927–1939. (6) & Abrams, W. (1999). Progressive unilateral Ourednik, V., Ourednik, J., Wakeman, D. R., et
Ragsdale, D. S., McPhee, J. C., Scheuer, T., & damage of the entorhinal cortex enhances synap- al. (2007). Behavioral improvement in a primate
Catterall, W. A. (1994). Molecular determi- tic efficacy of the crossed entorhinal afferent to Parkinson’s model is associated with multiple ho-
nants of state-dependent block of Na1 chan- dentate granule cells. Journal of Neuroscience, 19: meostatic effects of human neural stem cells. Pro-
nels by local anesthetics. Science, 265, 1724– RC42, 1–6. (5) ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA,
1728. (2) Ramirez, J. J., Campbell, D., Poulton, W., Barton, 104, 12175–12180. (8)
Rahman, Q., Kumari, V., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). C., Swails, J., Geghman, K., et al. (2007). Bilat- Redondo, R. L., & Morris, R. G. M. (2011). Making
Sexual orientation-related differences in prepulse eral entorhinal cortex lesions impair acquisition memories last: The synaptic tagging and capture
inhibition of the human startle response. Behav- of delayed spatial alternation in rats. Neurobiolo- hypothesis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12,
ioral Neuroscience, 117, 1096–1102. (11) gy of Learning and Memory, 87, 264–268. (5) 17–30. (13)
Rahman, Q., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). Born gay? Ramirez, J. J., McQuilkin, M., Carrigan, T., Mac- Reeves, A. G., & Plum, F. (1969). Hyperphagia,
The psychobiology of human sexual orientation. Donald, K., & Kelley, M. S. (1996). Progressive rage, and dementia accompanying a ventromedial
Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1337– entorhinal cortex lesions accelerate hippocampal hypothalamic neoplasm. Archives of Neurology,
1382. (11) sprouting and spare spatial memory in rats. Pro- 20, 616–624. (10)
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, Refinetti, R. (2000). Circadian physiology. Boca Ra-
B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997). Pain affect encoded 93, 15512–15517. (5) ton, FL: CRC Press. (9)
in human anterior cingulate but not somatosen- Ramirez-Amaya, V., Marrone, D. F., Gage, F. H., Refinetti, R., & Carlisle, H. J. (1986). Complemen-
sory cortex. Science, 277, 968–971. (7) Worley, P. F., & Barnes, C. A. (2006). Integration tary nature of heat production and heat intake
Rais, M., Cahn, W., Van Haren, N., Schnack, H., of new neurons into functional neural networks. during behavioral thermoregulation in the rat.
Caspers, E., Hulshoff, H., et al. (2008). Excessive Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12237–12241. (5) Behavioral and Neural Biology, 46, 64–70. (10)
530 References

Refinetti, R., & Menaker, M. (1992). The circadian Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality Riemann, D., König, A., Hohagen, F., Kiemen, A.,
rhythm of body temperature. Physiology & Be- in adolescent decision making. Psychological Sci- Voderholzer, U., Backhaus, J., et al. (1999). How
havior, 51, 613–637. (9) ence in the Public Interest, 7, 1–44. (5) to preserve the antidepressive effect of sleep depri-
Regan, T. (1986). The rights of humans and other Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and vation: A comparison of sleep phase advance and
animals. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 128(Sup- environmental influences on antisocial behavior: sleep phase delay. European Archives of Psychiatry
pl. 554), 33–40. (1) A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, 231–237. (15)
Reichelt, K. L., Seim, A. R., & Reichelt, W. H. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529. (12) Rigori, D., Brass, M., & Sartori, G. (2010). Post-
(1996). Could schizophrenia be reasonably ex- Rhees, R. W., Shryne, J. E., & Gorski, R. A. (1990). action determinants of the reported time of con-
plained by Dohan’s hypothesis on genetic inter- Onset of the hormone-sensitive perinatal period scious intentions. Frontiers in Human Neurosci-
action with a dietary peptide overload? Progress for sexual differentiation of the sexually dimor- ence, 4, article 38. (8)
in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychi- phic nucleus of the preoptic area in female rats. Rimmele, U., Hediger, K., Heinrichs, M., & Klaver,
atry, 20, 1083–1114. (15) Journal of Neurobiology, 21, 781–786. (11) P. (2009). Oxytocin makes a face in memory fa-
Reichling, D. B., Kwiat, G. C., & Basbaum, A. I. Rhodes, J. S., van Praag, H., Jeffrey, S., Girard, I., miliar. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 38–42. (11)
(1988). Anatomy, physiology, and pharmacology Mitchell, G. S., Garland, T., Jr., et al. (2003). Ex- Ringel, B. L., & Szuba, M. P. (2001). Potential
of the periaqueductal gray contribution to anti- ercise increases hippocampal neurogenesis to mechanisms of the sleep therapies for depression.
nociceptive controls. In H. L. Fields & J.-M. Bes- high levels but does not improve spatial learning Depression and Anxiety, 14, 29–36. (15)
son (Eds.), Progress in brain research (Vol. 77, pp. in mice bred for increased voluntary wheel run- Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial
31–46). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (7) ning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1006–1016. expression: A review of the neurological and psy-
Reick, M., Garcia, J. A., Dudley, C., & McKnight, (5) chological mechanisms for producing facial ex-
S. L. (2001). NPAS2: An analog of clock opera- Ricciardi, E., Bonino, D., Sani, L., Vecchi, T., Guaz- pressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 52–77. (8)
tive in the mammalian forebrain. Science, 293, zelli, M., Haxby, J. V., et al. (2009). Do we really Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K.-Y., Eaves.
506–509. (9) need vision? How blind people “see” the actions L., Hoh, J., et al. (2009). Interaction between the
Reid, C. A., Dixon, D. B., Takahashi, M., Bliss, of others. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 9719– serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stress-
T. V. P., & Fine, A. (2004). Optical quantal analy- 9724. (8) ful life events, and risk of depression. Journal of
sis indicates that long-term potentiation at single Rice, G., Anderson, C., Risch, N., & Ebers, G. the American Medical Association, 301, 2462–
hippocampal mossy fiber synapses is expressed (1999). Male homosexuality: Absence of linkage 2471. (15)
through increased release probability, recruit- to microsatellite markers at Xq28. Science, 284, Rittenhouse, C. D., Shouval, H. Z., Paradiso, M. A.,
ment of new release sites, and activation of silent 665–667. (11) & Bear, M. F. (1999). Monocular deprivation in-
synapses. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 3618– Richard, C., Honoré, J., Bernati, T., & Rousseaux, duces homosynaptic long-term depression in vi-
3626. (13) M. (2004). Straight-ahead pointing correlates sual cortex. Nature, 397, 347–350. (6)
Reiner, W. G., & Gearhart, J. P. (2004). Discor- with long-line bisection in neglect patients. Cor- Ritz, B., Ascherio, A., Checkoway, H., Marder,
dant sexual identity in some genetic males with tex, 40, 75–83. (14) K. S., Nelson, L. M., Rocca, W. A., et al. (2007).
cloacal exstrophy assigned to female sex at Richter, C. P. (1922). A behavioristic study of the Pooled analysis of tobacco use and risk of Par-
birth. New England Journal of Medicine, 350, activity of the rat. Comparative Psychology Mono- kinson’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 64, 990–
333–341. (11) graphs, 1, 1–55. (9) 997. (8)
Reisner, A. D. (2003). The electroconvulsive thera- Richter, C. P. (1936). Increased salt appetite in adre- Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The func-
py controversy: Evidence and ethics. Neuropsy- nalectomized rats. American Journal of Physiolo- tional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit:
chology Review, 13, 199–219. (15) gy, 115, 155–161. (10) Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature
Reiss, A. L., Eckert, M. A., Rose, F. E., Karchemskiy, Richter, C. P. (1950). Taste and solubility of toxic Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264–274. (8)
A., Kesler, S., Chang, M., et al. (2004). An ex- compounds in poisoning of rats and humans. Ro, T., Shelton, D., Lee, O.L., & Chang, E. (2004).
periment of nature: Brain anatomy parallels cog- Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psycholo- Extrageniculate mediation of unconscious vision
nition and behavior in Williams syndrome. Jour- gy, 43, 358–374. (7) in transcranial magnetic stimulation-induced
nal of Neuroscience, 24, 5009–5015. (14) Richter, C. P. (1967). Psychopathology of periodic blindsight. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Rennaker, R. L., Chen, C.-F. F., Ruyle, A. M., Sloan, behavior in animals and man. In J. Zubin & H. F. Sciences, USA, 101, 9933–9935. (4)
A. M., & Wilson, D. A. (2007). Spatial and tem- Hunt (Eds.), Comparative psychopathology (pp. Roane, B. M., & Taylor, D. J. (2008). Adolescent in-
poral distribution of odorant-evoked activity in 205–227). New York: Grune & Stratton. (9) somnia as a risk factor for early adult depression
the piriform cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, Richter, C. P. (1975). Deep hypothermia and its ef- and substance abuse. Sleep, 31, 1351–1356. (15)
1534–1542. (7) fect on the 24-hour clock of rats and hamsters. Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1995). Arousal sys-
Rensch, B. (1977). Panpsychic identism and its Johns Hopkins Medical Journal, 136, 1–10. (9) tems and attention. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),
meaning for a universal evolutionary picture. Sci- Richter, C. P., & Langworthy, O. R. (1933). The The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 703–720). Cam-
entia, 112, 337–349. (1) quill mechanism of the porcupine. Journal für bridge, MA: MIT Press. (9)
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). Psychologie und Neurologie, 45, 143–153. (4) Roberson, E. D., & Mucke, E. (2006). 100 years and
To see or not to see: The need for attention to Riddick, N. V., Czoty, P. W., Gage, H. D., Kaplan, counting: Prospects for defeating Alzheimer’s
perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, J. R., Nader, S. H., Icenhower, M., et al. (2009). disease. Science, 314, 781–784. (13)
8, 368–373. (14) Behavioral and neurobiological characteristics Roberts, L. E., Eggermont, J. J., Caspary, D. M.,
Reuter-Lorenz, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1981). Differ- influencing social hierarchy formation in female Shore, S. E., Melcher, J. R., & Kaltenbach, J. A.
ential contributions of the two cerebral hemi- cynomolgus monkeys. Neuroscience, 158, 1257– (2010). Ringing ears: The neuroscience of tin-
spheres to the perception of happy and sad faces. 1265. (12) nitus. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14972–
Neuropsychologia, 19, 609–613. (12) Ridding, M. C., & Rothwell, J. C. (2007). Is there a 14979. (7)
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Miller, A. C. (1998). The future for therapeutic use of transcranial mag- Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Carter, V., & Petrie,
cognitive neuroscience of human laterality: Les- netic stimulation? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences
sons from the bisected brain. Current Directions 8, 559–567. (15) in humans and the use of oral contraceptives.
in Psychological Science, 7, 15–20. (14) Riddle, W. J. R., & Scott, A. I. F. (1995). Relapse Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 2715–
Revusky, S. (2009). Chemical aversion treatment of after successful electroconvulsive therapy: The 2722. (7)
alcoholism. In S. Reilly & T. R. Schachtman use and impact of continuation antidepressant Robertson, I. H. (2005, Winter). The deceptive
(Eds.), Conditioned taste aversion (pp. 445–472). drug treatment. Human Psychopharmacology, 10, world of subjective awareness. Cerebrum, 7(1),
New York: Oxford University Press. (3) 201–205. (15) 74–83. (4)
References 531

Robertson, L., Treisman, A., Friedman-Hill, S., & Rommel, S. A., Pabst, D. A., & McLellan, W. A. in musician’s dystonia by restoring sensorimotor
Grabowecky, M. (1997). The interaction of spa- (1998). Reproductive thermoregulation in marine organization. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 14627–
tial and object pathways: Evidence from Balint’s mammals. American Scientist, 86, 440–448. (10) 14636. (5)
syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuropsychology, Roorda, A., & Williams, D. R. (1999). The arrange- Rosenzweig, M. R., & Bennett, E. L. (1996). Psy-
9, 295–317. (4) ment of the three cone classes in the living hu- chobiology of plasticity: Effects of training and
Robillard, T. A. J., & Gersdorff, M. C. H. (1986). man eye. Nature, 397, 520–522. (6) experience on brain and behavior. Behavioural
Prevention of pre- and perinatal acquired hear- Ropelle, E. R., Flores, M. B., Cintra, D. E., Rocha, Brain Research, 78, 57–65. (5)
ing defects: Part I. Study of causes. Journal of Au- G. Z., Pauli, J. R., Morar, J., et al. (2010). IL-6 Ross, D., Choi, J., & Purves, D. (2007). Musical
ditory Research, 26, 207–237. (7) and IL-10 anti-inflammatory activity links exer- intervals in speech. Proceedings of the National
Robins Wahlin, T.-B., Lundin, A., & Dear, K. cise to hypothalamic insulin and leptin sensitivi- Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 9852–9857.
(2007). Early cognitive deficits in Swedish gene ty through IKKb and ER stress inhibition. PLoS (14)
carriers of Huntington’s disease. Neuropsycholo- Biology, 8, e1000465. (10) Ross, E. D., Homan, R. W., & Buck, R. (1994). Dif-
gy, 21, 31–44. (8) Roppel, R. M. (1978). Cancer and mental illness. ferential hemispheric lateralization of primary
Rochira, V., Balestrieri, A., Madeo, B., Baraldi, E., Science, 201, 398. (15) and social emotions. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsy-
Faustini-Fustini, M., Granata, A. R. M., et al. Rosano, C., Venkatraman, V. K., Guralnik, J., chology, and Behavioral Neurology, 7, 1–19. (12)
(2001). Congenital estrogen deficiency: In search Newman, A. B., Glynn, N. W., Launer, L., et al. Rossi, A. F., Bichot, N. P., Desimone, R., & Unger-
of the estrogen role in human male reproduction. (2010). Psychomotor speed and functional leider, L. G. (2007). Top-down attentional defi-
Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 178, 107– brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical cits in macaques with lesions of lateral prefrontal
115. (11) activity treatment. Journals of Gerontology Series cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 11306–
Rodriguez, I., Greer, C. A., Mok, M. Y., & Mom- A-Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 65, 11314. (14)
baerts, P. A. (2000). A putative pheromone re- 639–647. (5) Rossi, A. F., Pessoa, L., Desimone, R., & Unger-
ceptor gene expressed in human olfactory muco- Rose, J. E., Brugge, J. F., Anderson, D. J., & Hind, J. leider, L. G. (2009). The prefrontal cortex and
sa. Nature Genetics, 26, 18–19. (7) E. (1967). Phase-locked response to low- the executive control of attention. Experimental
Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Pramstaller, P. P., frequency tones in single auditory nerve fibers of Brain Research, 192, 489–497. (14)
Ricken, J., Havel, M., Ricken, J., et al. (2004). A the squirrel monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, Rossi, D. J., Oshima, T., & Attwell, D. (2000).
marker for the end of adolescence. Current Biolo- 30, 769–793. (7) Glutamate release in severe brain ischaemia is
gy, 14, R1038–R1039. (9) Roselli, C. E., Larkin, K., Resko, J. A., Stellflug, J. N., mainly by reversed uptake. Nature, 403, 316–
Roenneberg, T., Kumar, C. J., & Merrow, M. (2007). & Stormshak, F. (2004). The volume of a sexu- 321. (5)
The human circadian clock entrains to sun time. ally dimorphic nucleus in the ovine medial pre- Rossi, E. A., & Roorda, A. (2010). The relation-
Current Biology, 17, R44–R45. (9) optic area/anterior hypothalamus varies with ship between visual resolution and cone spacing
Roffwarg, H. P., Muzio, J. N., & Dement, W. C. sexual partner preference. Endocrinology, 145, in the human fovea. Nature Neuroscience, 13,
(1966). Ontogenetic development of human 478–483. (11) 156–157. (6)
sleep-dream cycle. Science, 152, 604–609. (9) Roselli, C. E., Stadelman, H., Reeve, R., Bishop, Rossi, S., Miniussi, C., Pasqualetti, P., Babiloni, C.,
Roitman, M. F., Wheeler, R. A., Wightman, R. M., C. V., & Stormshak, F. (2007). The ovine sexually Rossini, P. M., & Cappa, S. F. (2004). Age-related
& Carelli, R. M. (2008). Real-time chemical re- dimorphic nucleus of the medial preoptic area is functional changes of prefrontal cortex in long-
sponses in the nucleus accumbens differentiate organized prenatally by testosterone. Endocrinol- term memory: A repetitive transcranial magnetic
rewarding and aversive stimuli. Nature Neurosci- ogy, 148, 4450–4457. (11) stimulation study. Journal of Neuroscience, 24,
ence, 11, 1376–1377. (3) Rosen, A. C., Prull, M. W., O-Hara, R., Race, E. A., 7939–7944. (13)
Rokers, B., Cormack, L. K., & Huk, A. C. (2009). Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., et al. (2002). Vari- Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., & Pribram, K. H.
Disparity- and velocity-based signals for three- able effects of aging on frontal lobe contributions (1954). Influence of amygdalectomy on social
dimensional motion perception in human MT1. to memory. NeuroReport, 13, 2425–2428. (13) behavior in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and
Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1050–1055. (6) Rosen, H. J., Perry, R. J., Murphy, J., Kramer, Physiological Psychology, 47, 173–178. (12)
Rolls, A., Shechter, R., & Schwartz, M. (2009). The J. H., Mychack, P., Schuff, N., et al. (2002). Roth, B. L., Willins, D. L., Kristiansen, K., &
bright side of the glial scar in CNS repair. Nature Emotion comprehension in the temporal vari- Kroeze, W. K. (1999). Activation is hallucino-
Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 235–241. (5) ant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 125, genic and antagonism is therapeutic: Role of
Rolls, E. T. (1995). Central taste anatomy and neu- 2286–2295. (12) 5-HT2A receptors in atypical antipsychotic drug
rophysiology. In R. L. Doty (Ed.), Handbook of Rosenbaum, R. S., Köhler, S., Schacter, D. L., actions. Neuroscientist, 5, 254–262. (15)
olfaction and gustation (pp. 549–573). New York: Moscovitch, M., Westmacott, R., Black, S. E., et Rottenberg, J., Kasch, K. L., Gross, J. J., & Gotlib,
Dekker. (7) al. (2005). The case of K. C.: Contributions of a I. H. (2002). Sadness and amusement reactivity
Rolls, E. T. (1996) The representation of space in memory-impaired person to memory theory. differentially predict concurrent and prospective
the primate hippocampus, and its relation to Neuropsychologia, 43, 989–1021. (13) functioning in major depressive disorder. Emo-
memory. In K. Ishikawa, J. L. McGaugh, & H. Rosenblatt, J. S. (1967). Nonhormonal basis of ma- tion, 2, 135–146. (15)
Sakata (Eds.), Brain processes and memory (pp. ternal behavior in the rat. Science, 156, 1512– Routtenberg, A., Cantallops, I., Zaffuto, S., Serra-
203–227). Amsterdam: Elsevier. (13) 1514. (11) no, P., & Namgung, U. (2000). Enhanced learn-
Rome, L. C., Loughna, P. T., & Goldspink, G. Rosenblatt, J. S. (1970). Views on the onset and main- ing after genetic overexpression of a brain growth
(1984). Muscle fiber activity in carp as a function tenance of maternal behavior in the rat. In L. R. protein. Proceedings of the National Academy of
of swimming speed and muscle temperature. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, & J. S. Rosen- Sciences, USA, 97, 7657–7662. (13)
American Journal of Psychiatry, 247, R272– blatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior Rouw, R., & Scholte, H. S. (2007). Increased struc-
R279. (8) (pp. 489–515). San Francisco: Freeman. (11) tural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia.
Romer, A. S. (1962). The vertebrate body. Philadel- Rosenblatt, J. S., Olufowobi, A., & Siegel, H. I. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 792–797. (7)
phia: Saunders. (5) (1998). Effects of pregnancy hormones on ma- Rovainen, C. M. (1976). Regeneration of Müller
Romero, E., Cha, G.-H., Verstreken, P., Ly, C. V., ternal responsiveness, responsiveness to estrogen and Mauthner axons after spinal transection in
Hughes, R. E., Bellen, H. J., et al. (2007). Sup- stimulation of maternal behavior, and the lordo- larval lampreys. Journal of Comparative Neurolo-
pression of neurodegeneration and increased sis response to estrogen stimulation. Hormones gy, 168, 545–554. (5)
neurotransmission caused by expanded full- and Behavior, 33, 104–114. (11) Rowland, D. L., & Burnett, A. L. (2000). Pharmaco-
length huntingtin accumulating in the cytoplasm. Rosenkranz, K., Butler, K., Williamson, A., & therapy in the treatment of male sexual dysfunc-
Neuron, 57, 27–40. (8) Rothwell, J. C. (2009). Regaining motor control tion. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 226–243. (11)
532 References

Roy, A., DeJong, J., & Linnoila, M. (1989). Cerebro- psychopharmacology. Brain Research Reviews, Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., Cash, S. S., Schomer, D., &
spinal fluid monoamine metabolites and suicidal 37, 59–67. (11) Halgren, E. (2009). Sequential processing of lexi-
behavior in depressed patients. Archives of Gen- Rusak, B., & Zucker, I. (1979). Neural regulation of cal, grammatical, and phonological information
eral Psychiatry, 46, 609–612. (12) circadian rhythms. Physiological Reviews, 59, within Broca’s area. Science, 326, 445–449. (14)
Royer, S., & Paré, D. (2003). Conservation of total 449–526. (9) Sakurai, T. (2007). The neural circuit of orexin
synaptic weight through balanced synaptic de- Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Wisniewski, S. R., (hypocretin): Maintaining sleep and wakefulness.
pression and potentiation. Nature, 422, 518– Stewart, J. W., Nierenberg, A. A., Thase, M. E., Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 171–181. (9)
522. (13) et al. (2006). Bupropion-SR, sertraline, or Salmelin, R., Hari, R., Lounasmaa, O. V., & Sams,
Rozin, P., Dow, S., Moscovitch, M., & Rajaram, S. venlaxine-XR after failure of SSRIs for depres- M. (1994). Dynamics of brain activation during
(1998). What causes humans to begin and end a sion. New England Journal of Medicine, 354, picture naming. Nature, 368, 463–465. (4)
meal? A role for memory for what has been eaten, 1231–1242. (15) Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental
as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in Russell, A. J., Munro, J. C., Jones, P. B., Hemsley, aging. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68–
amnesic patients. Psychological Science, 9, 392– D. R., & Murray, R. M. (1997). Schizophrenia 87. (5)
396. (13) and the myth of intellectual decline. American Salvatore, P., Ghidini, S., Zita, G., De Panfilis, C.,
Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. (1971). Specific hungers Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 635–639. (15) Lambertino, S., Maggini, C., et al. (2008). Circa-
and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations Russell, M. J., Switz, G. M., & Thompson, K. dian activity rhythm abnormalities in ill and re-
of learning. Psychological Review, 78, 459–486. (1980). Olfactory influences on the human men- covered bipolar I disorder patients. Bipolar Disor-
(10, 13) strual cycle. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Be- ders, 10, 256–265. (15)
Rozin, P., & Pelchat, M. L. (1988). Memories of havior, 13, 737–738. (7) Sánchez, I., Mahlke, C., & Yuan, J. (2003). Pivotal
mammaries: Adaptations to weaning from milk. Rüttiger, L., Braun, D. I., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Pe- role of oligomerization in expanded polygluta-
Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychol- tersen, D., Schönle, P., & Sharpe, L. T. (1999). mine neurodegenerative disorders. Nature, 421,
ogy, 13, 1–29. (10) Selective color constancy deficits after circum- 373–379. (8)
Rozin, P., & Schull, J. (1988). The adaptive-evolu- scribed unilateral brain lesions. Journal of Neuro- Sander, K., & Scheich, H. (2001). Auditory percep-
tionary point of view in experimental psychol- science, 19, 3094–3106. (6) tion of laughing and crying activates human
ogy. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Saad, W. A., Luiz, A. C., Camargo, L. A. A., Renzi, amygdala regardless of attentional state. Cogni-
Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.), Stevens’ hand- A., & Manani, J. V. (1996). The lateral preoptic tive Brain Research, 12, 181–198. (12)
book of experimental psychology (2nd ed.): Vol. area plays a dual role in the regulation of thirst in Sanders, A. R., Duan, J., Levinson, D. F., Shi, J., He,
1. Perception and motivation (pp. 503–546). the rat. Brain Research Bulletin, 39, 171–176. (10) D., Hou, C., et al. (2008). No significant associa-
New York: Wiley (13) Sabel, B. A. (1997). Unrecognized potential of surviv- tion of 14 candidate genes with schizophrenia in
Rubens, A. B., & Benson, D. F. (1971). Associative ing neurons: Within-systems plasticity, recovery of a large European ancestry sample: Implications
visual agnosia. Archives of Neurology, 24, 305– function, and the hypothesis of minimal residual for psychiatric genetics. American Journal of Psy-
316. (6) structure. The Neuroscientist, 3, 366–370. (5) chiatry, 165, 497–506. (15)
Rubin, B. D., & Katz, L. C. (2001). Spatial coding of Sabo, K. T., & Kirtley, D. D. (1982). Objects and ac- Sanders, S. K., & Shekhar, A. (1995). Anxiolytic ef-
enantiomers in the rat olfactory bulb. Nature tivities in the dreams of the blind. International fects of chlordiazepoxide blocked by injection of
Neuroscience, 4, 355–356. (7) Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 5, 241–242. (4) GABAA and benzodiazepine receptor antago-
Rubinow, M. J., Arseneau, L. M., Beverly, J. L., & Sacco, T., & Sacchetti, B. (2010). Role of secondary nists in the region of the anterior basolateral
Juraska, J. M. (2004). Effect of the estrous cycle sensory cortices in emotional memory storage amygdala of rats. Biological Psychiatry, 37, 473–
on water maze acquisition depends on the tem- and retrieval in rats. Science, 329, 649–656. (13) 476. (12)
perature of the water. Behavioral Neuroscience, Sack, R. L., & Lewy, A. J. (2001). Circadian rhythm Sanes, J. N., Donoghue, J. P., Thangaraj, V., Edel-
118, 863–868. (10) sleep disorders: Lessons from the blind. Sleep man, R. R., & Warach, S. (1995). Shared neural
Rubinstein, G. (1997). Schizophrenia, rheumatoid Medicine Reviews, 5, 189–206. (9) substrates controlling hand movements in hu-
arthritis and natural resistance genes. Schizophre- Sackeim, H. A., Putz, E., Vingiano, W., Coleman, man motor cortex. Science, 268, 1775–1777. (8)
nia Research, 25, 177–181. (15) E., & McElhiney, M. (1988). Lateralization in Sanes, J. R. (1993). Topographic maps and molecu-
Rudolph, U., Crestani, F., Benke, D., Brünig, I., Ben- the processing of emotionally laden information: lar gradients. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3,
son, J. A., Fritschy, J.-M., et al. (1999). Benzodi- I. Normal functioning. Neuropsychiatry, Neuro- 67–74. (5)
azepine actions mediated by specific g-aminobu- psychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 97–110. Sanger, T. D., Pascual-Leone, A., Tarsy, D., &
tyric acidA receptor subtypes. Nature, 401, (14) Schlaug, G. (2001). Nonlinear sensory cortex re-
796–800. (12) Sacks, O. (2010, August 30). Face-blind. The New sponse to simultaneous tactile stimuli in writer’s
Rudoy, J. D., Voss, J. L., Westerberg, C. E., & Paller, Yorker, 86(31), 36–43. (6) cramp. Movement Disorders, 17, 105–111. (5)
K. A. (2009). Strengthening individual memo- Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Deiber, Sanger, T. D., Tarsy, D., & Pascual-Leone, A.
ries by reactivating them during sleep. Science, M.-P., Ibañez, V., & Hallett, M. (1998). Neural (2001). Abnormalities of spatial and temporal
326, 1079. (9) networks for Braille reading by the blind. Brain, sensory discrimination in writer’s cramp. Move-
Rujescu, D., Giegling, I., Bondy, B., Gietl, A., Zill, P., 121, 1213–1229. (5) ment Disorders, 16, 94–99. (5)
& Möller, H.-J. (2002). Association of anger- Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibañez, Sapolsky, R. M. (1992). Stress, the aging brain, and
related traits with SNPs in the TPH gene. Mo- V., Deiber, M.-P., Dold, G., et al. (1996). Activa- the mechanisms of neuron death. Cambridge, MA:
lecular Psychiatry, 7, 1023–1029. (12) tion of the primary visual cortex by Braille read- MIT Press. (12)
Rumbaugh, D. M. (Ed.). (1977). Language learning ing in blind subjects. Nature, 380, 526–528. (5) Sapolsky, R. M. (1998). Why zebras don’t get ulcers.
by a chimpanzee: The Lana Project. New York: Sadri-Vakili, G., Kumaresan, V., Schmidt, H. D., New York: Freeman. (12)
Academic Press. (14) Famous, K. R., Chawla, P., Vassoler, F. M., et al. Satinoff, E. (1964). Behavioral thermoregulation in
Rumbaugh, D. M. (1990). Comparative psychology (2010). Cocaine-induced chromatin remodeling response to local cooling of the rat brain. Ameri-
and the great apes: Their competency in learning, increases brain-derived neurotrophic factor tran- can Journal of Physiology, 206, 1389–1394. (10)
language, and numbers. Psychological Record, 40, scription in the rat medial prefrontal cortex, Satinoff, E. (1991). Developmental aspects of be-
15–39. (14) which alters the reinforcing efficacy of cocaine. havioral and reflexive thermoregulation. In H. N.
Rupprecht, R., di Michele, F., Hermann, B., Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 11735–11744. (1) Shanir, G. A. Barr, & M. A. Hofer (Eds.), Devel-
Ströhle, A., Lancel, M., Romeo, E., et al. Saha, S., Chant, D., & McGrath, J. (2007). A system- opmental psychobiology: New methods and chang-
(2001). Neuroactive steroids: Molecular mech- atic review of mortality in schizophrenia. Archives ing concepts (pp. 169–188). New York: Oxford
anisms of action and implications for neuro- of General Psychiatry, 64, 1123–1131. (15) University Press. (10)
References 533

Satinoff, E., McEwen, G. N., Jr., & Williams, B. A. ous parasomnias and other disorders of disrupt- Schmidt, L. A. (1999). Frontal brain electrical activ-
(1976). Behavioral fever in newborn rabbits. Sci- ed nocturnal sleep in 170 adults. American Jour- ity in shyness and sociability. Psychological Sci-
ence, 193, 1139–1140. (10) nal of Medicine, 100, 333–337. (9) ence, 10, 316–320. (12)
Satinoff, E., & Rutstein, J. (1970). Behavioral ther- Schenk, T. (2006). An allocentric rather than per- Schmidt-Hieber, C, Jonas, P., & Bischofberger, J.
moregulation in rats with anterior hypothalamic ceptual deficit in patient D. F. Nature Neurosci- (2004). Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly
lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological ence, 9, 1369–1370. (6) generated granule cells of the adult hippocam-
Psychology, 71, 77–82. (10) Schenk, T., Mai, N., Ditterich, J., & Zihl, J. (2000). pus. Nature, 429, 184–187. (5)
Sato, M., & Stryker, M. P. (2008). Distinctive fea- Can a motion-blind patient reach for moving ob- Schmitt, K. C., & Reith, M. E. A. (2010). Regu-
tures of adult ocular dominance plasticity. Jour- jects? European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, lation of the dopamine transporter. Annals of
nal of Neuroscience, 28, 10278–10286. (6) 3351–3360. (6) the New York Academy of Sciences, 1187, 316–
Satz, P., & Green, M. F. (1999). Atypical handed- Scherer, S. S. (1986). Reinnervation of the extraoc- 340. (3)
ness in schizophrenia: Some methodological and ular muscles in goldfish is nonselective. Journal of Schneider, B. A., Trehub, S. E., Morrongiello, B. A.,
theoretical issues. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 63– Neuroscience, 6, 764–773. (5) & Thorpe, L. A. (1986). Auditory sensitivity in
78. (15) Scherrer, G., Imamachi, N., Cao, Y.-Q., Contet, C., preschool children. Journal of the Acoustical Soci-
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Language acquisi- Mennicken, F., O’Donnell, D., et al. (2009). Dis- ety of America, 79, 447–452. (7)
tion in a nonhuman species: Implications for the sociation of the opioid receptor mechanisms that Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, G., Specht, H. J.,
innateness debate. Developmental Psychobiology, control mechanical and heat pain. Cell, 137, Gutschalk, A., & Rupp, A. (2002). Morphology
23, 599–620. (14) 1148–1159. (7) of Heschl’s gyrus reflects enhanced activation in
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Schienle, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., Blecker, C., Ott, the auditory cortex of musicians. Nature Neuro-
Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, U., Kirsch, P., et al. (2002). The insula is not spe- science, 5, 688–694. (5)
D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape cifically involved in disgust processing: An fMRI Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation
and child. Monographs of the Society for Research study. NeuroReport, 13, 2023–2026. (12) and the adaptation of thought to ongoing real-
in Child Development, 58(Serial no. 233). (14) Schiff, N. D., Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., Victor, J. D., ity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 662–671.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, Baker, K., Gerber, M., et al. (2007). Behavioural (13)
K. E., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1992). Symbols: improvements with thalamic stimulation after Schomacher, M., Müller, H. D., Sommer, C.,
Their communicative use, communication, and severe traumatic brain injury. Nature, 448, 600– Schwab, S., & Schäbitz, W.-R. (2008). Endocan-
combination by bonobos (Pan paniscus). In L. P. 603. (5) nabinoids mediate neuroprotection after tran-
Lipsitt & C. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in Schiffman, S. S. (1983). Taste and smell in disease. sient focal cerebral ischemia. Brain Research,
infancy research (Vol. 7, pp. 221–278). Norwood, New England Journal of Medicine, 308, 1275– 1240, 213–220. (5)
NJ: Ablex. (14) 1279, 1337–1343. (7) Schou, M. (1997). Forty years of lithium treatment.
Savic, I., Berglund, H., Gulyas, B., & Roland, P. Schiffman, S. S., & Erickson, R. P. (1971). A psy- Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 9–13. (15)
(2001). Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like chophysical model for gustatory quality. Physiol- Schroeder, J. A., & Flannery-Schroeder, E. (2005).
compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalam- ogy & Behavior, 7, 617–633. (7) Use of herb Gymnema sylvestre to illustrate the
ic activations in humans. Neuron, 31, 661–668. (7) Schiffman, S. S., & Erickson, R. P. (1980). The issue principles of gustatory sensation: An undergrad-
Savic, I., & Lindström, P. (2008). PET and MRI of primary tastes versus a taste continuum. Neuro- uate neuroscience laboratory exercise. Journal of
show differences in cerebral asymmetry and science and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4, 109–117. (7) Undergraduate Neuroscience Education, 3, A59–
functional connectivity between homo- and het- Schiffman, S. S., Lockhead, E., & Maes, F. W. A62. (7)
erosexual subjects. Proceedings of the National (1983). Amiloride reduces the taste intensity of Schuckit, M. A., & Smith, T. L. (1996). An 8-year
Academy of Sciences, USA, 105, 9403–9408. (11) Na1 and Li1 salts and sweeteners. Proceedings of follow-up of 450 sons of alcoholic and control
Sawaguchi, T., & Iba, M. (2001). Prefrontal cortical the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 80, subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 202–
representation of visuospatial working memory 6136–6140. (7) 210. (3)
in monkeys examined by local inactivation with Schiffman, S. S., McElroy, A. E., & Erickson, R. P. Schuckit, M. C., & Smith, T. L. (1997). Assessing
muscimol. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 2041– (1980). The range of taste quality of sodium the risk for alcoholism among sons of alcoholics.
2053. (13) salts. Physiology & Behavior, 24, 217–224. (7) Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 141–145. (3)
Sawamoto, N., Honda, M., Hanakawa, T., Fuku- Schiller, D., Monfils, M.-H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, Schulkin, J. (1991). Sodium hunger: The search for a
yama, H., & Shibasaki, H. (2002). Cognitive D. C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2010). Pre- salty taste. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni-
slowing in Parkinson’s disease: A behavioral eval- venting the return of fear in humans using recon- versity Press. (10)
uation independent of motor slowing. Journal of solidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463, 49– Schurger, A., Pereira, F., Treisman, A., & Cohen,
Neuroscience, 22, 5198–5203. (8) 53. (12) J. D. (2010). Reproducibility distinguishes con-
Schacter, D. L. (1983). Amnesia observed: Re- Schlack, A., Krekelberg, B., & Albright, T. D. scious from nonconscious neural representa-
membering and forgetting in a natural environ- (2007). Recent history of stimulus speeds affects tions. Science, 327, 97–99. (14)
ment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 236– the speed tuning of neurons in area MT. Journal Schwab, M. E. (1998). Regenerative nerve fiber
242. (13) of Neuroscience, 27, 11009–11018. (6) growth in the adult central nervous system. News
Schaller, M., Miller, G. E., Gervais, W. M., Yager, S., Schlinger, H. D., Jr. (1996). How the human got its in Physiological Sciences, 13, 294–298. (5)
& Chen, E. (2010). Mere visual perception of spots. Skeptic, 4, 68–76. (1) Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan,
other people’s disease symptoms facilitates a Schmid, A., Koch, M., & Schnitzler, H.-U. (1995). J., & Rauch, S. L. (2003). Inhibited and uninhib-
more aggressive immune response. Psychological Conditioned pleasure attenuates the startle re- ited infants “grown up”: Adult amygdalar re-
Science, 21, 649–652. (12) sponse in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and sponse to novelty. Science, 300, 1952–1953. (12)
Scheer, F. A. J. L., Wright, K. P., Jr., Kronauer, R. E., Memory, 64, 1–3. (12) Schwartz, G. J. (2000). The role of gastrointestinal
& Czeisler, C. A. (2007). Plasticity of the intrin- Schmid, M. C., Mrowka, S. W., Turchi, J., Saunders, vagal afferents in the control of food intake: Cur-
sic period of the human circadian timing system. R. C., Wilke, M., Peters, A. J., et al. (2010). rent prospects. Nutrition, 16, 866–873. (10)
PLoS ONE, 8, e721. (9) Blindsight depends on the lateral geniculate nu- Schwartz, L., & Tulipan, L. (1933). An outbreak
Scheibel, A. B. (1983). Dendritic changes. In B. cleus. Nature, 466, 373–377. (6) of dermatitis among workers in a rubber manu-
Reisberg (Ed.), Alzheimer’s Disease (pp. 69–73). Schmidt, L., Palminteri, S., Lafargue, G., & Pessi- facturing plant. Public Health Reports, 48, 809–
New York: Free Press. (13) glione, M. (2010). Splitting motivation: Unilat- 814. (3)
Schenck, C. H., & Mahowald, M. W. (1996). Long- eral effects of subliminal incentives. Psychological Schwartz, M. F. (1995). Re-examining the role of
term, nightly benzodiazepine treatment of injuri- Science, 21, 977–983. (14) executive functions in routine action production.
534 References

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, Selzer, M. E. (1978). Mechanisms of functional re- Sheehan, T. P., Cirrito, J., Numan, M. J., & Numan,
321–335. (8) covery and regeneration after spinal cord transec- M. (2000). Using c-Fos immunocytochemistry to
Schwartz, W. J., & Gainer, H. (1977). Suprachias- tion in larval sea lamprey. Journal of Physiology, identify forebrain regions that may inhibit ma-
matic nucleus: Use of 14C-labeled deoxyglucose 277, 395–408. (5) ternal behavior in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience,
uptake as a functional marker. Science, 197, Semendeferi, K., Lu, A., Schenker, N., & Dama- 114, 337–352. (11)
1089–1091. (9) sio, H. (2002). Humans and great apes share a Shema, R., Sacktor, T. C., & Dudai, Y. (2007).
Schweinhardt, P., Seminowicz, D. A., Jaeger, E., large frontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 5, Rapid erasure of long-term memory associations
Duncan, G. H., & Bushnell, M. C. (2009). The 272–276. (4) in the cortex by an inhibitor of PKMz. Science,
anatomy of the mesolimbic reward system: A Sen, S., Duman, R., & Sanacora, G. (2008). Serum 317, 951–953. (13)
link between personality and the placebo analge- brain-derived neurotrophic factor, depression, Shen, H., Gong, Q. H., Aoki, C., Yuan, M., Ruder-
sic response. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 4882– and antidepressant medications: Meta-analyses man, Y., Dattilo, M., et al. (2007). Reversal of
4887. (7) and implications. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 527– neurosteroid effects at a(b(d GABAA receptors
Scott, S. H. (2004). Optimal feedback control and 532. (15) triggers anxiety at puberty. Nature Neuroscience,
the neural basis of volitional motor control. Na- Sereno, A. B., & Holzman, P. S. (1993). Express sac- 10, 469–477. (12)
ture Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 532–544. (8) cades and smooth pursuit eye movement func- Sher, L., Carballo, J. J., Grunebaum, M. F., Burke,
Scott, W. K., Nance, M. A., Watts, R. L., Hubble, J. tion in schizophrenic, affective disorder, and nor- A. K., Zalsman, G., Huang, Y.-Y., et al. (2006). A
P., Koller, W. C., Lyons, K., et al. (2001). Com- mal subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, prospective study of the association of cerebrospi-
plete genomic screen in Parkinson disease. Jour- 303–316. (15) nal fluid monoamine metabolite levels with le-
nal of the American Medical Association, 286, Sergent, C., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness thality of suicide attempts in patients with bipolar
2239–2244. (8) a gradual phenomenon? Psychological Science, 15, disorder. Bipolar Disorder, 8, 543–550. (12)
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent 720–728. (14) Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The integrative action of
memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Jour- Serino, A., Pizzoferrato, F., & Làdavas, E. (2008). the nervous system (2nd ed.). New York: Scrib-
nal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, Viewing a face (especially one’s own face) being ner’s. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press,
11–21. (13) touched enhances tactile perception on the face. 1947. (3)
Seal, R. P., Wang, X., Guan, Y., Raja, S., Woodbury, Psychological Science, 19, 434–438. (7) Sherrington, C. S. (1941). Man on his nature. New
C. J., Basbaum, A. I., et al. (2009). Injury-induced Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., York: Macmillan. (3)
mechanical hypersensitivity requires C-low Greischar, L. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right Shettleworth, S. J. (2009). The evolution of com-
threshold mechanoreceptors. Nature, 462, 651– dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and be- parative cognition: Is the snark still a boojum?
655. (7) havioral inhibition. Psychological Science, 20, Behavioural Processes, 80, 210–217. (1)
Seeley, R. J., Kaplan, J. M., & Grill, H. J. (1995). 1500–1506. (12) Shi, L., Fatemi, S. H., Sidwell, R. W., & Patterson,
Effect of occluding the pylorus on intraoral in- Shah, B., Shine, R., Hudson, S., & Kearney, M. P. H. (2003). Maternal influenza infection causes
take: A test of the gastric hypothesis of meal (2003). Sociality in lizards: Why do thick-tailed marked behavioral and pharmacological changes
termination. Physiology & Behavior, 58, 245– geckos (Nephrurus milii) aggregate? Behaviour, in the offspring. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 297–
249. (10) 140, 1039–1052. (10) 302. (15)
Seeman, P., Lee, T., Chau-Wong, M., & Wong, K. Shah, N. M., Pisapia, D. J., Maniatis, S., Men- Shih, R. A., Belmonte, P. L., & Zandi, P. P. (2004).
(1976). Antipsychotic drug doses and neuro- delsohn, M. M., Nemes, A., & Axel, R. (2004). A review of the evidence from family, twin and
leptic/dopamine receptors. Nature, 261, 717– Visualizing sexual dimorphism in the brain. Neu- adoption studies for a genetic contribution to
719. (15) ron, 43, 313–319. (11) adult psychiatric disorders. International Review
Segal, N. L. (2000). Virtual twins: New findings on Shakelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Bennett, K. of Psychiatry, 16, 260–283. (15)
within-family environmental influences on intel- (2002). Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differences Shima, K., Isoda, M., Mushiake, H., & Tanji, J.
ligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, in responses to a partner’s infidelity. Cognition (2007). Categorization of behavioural sequences
442–448. (1) and Emotion, 16, 299–307. (11) in the prefrontal cortex. Nature, 445, 315–318. (8)
Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psycho- Shalev, A. Y., Peri, T., Brandes, D., Freedman, S., Orr, Shimojo, S., Kamitani, Y., & Nishida, S. (2001). Af-
logical stress and the human immune system: A S. P., & Pitman, R. K. (2000). Auditory startle re- terimage of perceptually filled-in surface. Science,
meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psy- sponse in trauma survivors with posttraumatic 293, 1677–1680. (6)
chological Bulletin, 130, 610–630. (12) stress disorder: A prospective study. American Shimura, H., Schlossmacher, M. G., Hattori, N.,
Sehgal, A., Ousley, A., Yang, Z., Chen, Y., & Schot- Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 255–261. (12) Frosch, M. P., Trockenbacher, A., Schneider, R.,
land, P. (1999). What makes the circadian clock Shapiro, C. M., Bortz, R., Mitchell, D., Bartel, P., & et al. (2001). Ubiquitination of a new form of
tick: Genes that keep time? Recent Progress in Jooste, P. (1981). Slow-wave sleep: A recovery pe- a-synuclein by parkin from human brain: Impli-
Hormone Research, 54, 61–85. (9) riod after exercise. Science, 214, 1253–1254. (9) cations for Parkinson’s disease. Science, 293,
Sejnowski, T. J., & Destexhe, A. (2000). Why do we Sharbaugh, S. M. (2001). Seasonal acclimatization 263–269. (8)
sleep? Brain Research, 886, 208–223. (9) to extreme climatic conditions by black-capped Shine, R., Phillips, B., Waye, H., LeMaster, M., &
Selemon, L. D., Rajkowska, G., & Goldman-Rakic, chickadees (Poecile atricapilla) in interior Alaska Mason, R. T. (2001). Benefits of female mimicry
P. S. (1995). Abnormally high neuronal density (64° N). Physiological and Biochemical Zoology, in snakes. Nature, 414, 267. (10)
in the schizophrenic cortex. Archives of General 74, 568–575. (10) Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H.,
Psychiatry, 52, 805–818. (15) Sharma, J., Angelucci, A., & Sur, M. (2000). Induc- & Damasio, A. R. (2005). Investment behavior
Selkoe, D. J. (2000). Toward a comprehensive theo- tion of visual orientation modules in auditory and the negative side of emotion. Psychological
ry for Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of the New cortex. Nature, 404, 841–847. (5) Science, 16, 435–439. (12)
York Academy of Sciences, 924, 17–25. (13) Shatz, C. J. (1992, September). The developing Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Hopkins, R. O., Sage, J.,
Sellitto, M., Ciaramelli, E., & de Pellegrino, G. brain. Scientific American, 267(9), 60–67. (5) & Gluck, M. A. (2009). Distinct hippocampal
(2010). Myopic discounting of future rewards Shatz, C. J. (1996). Emergence of order in visual- and basal ganglia contributions to probabilistic
after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. system development. Proceedings of the National learning and reversal. Journal of Cognitive Neuro-
Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 16429–16436. (13) Academy of Sciences, USA, 93, 602–608. (6) science, 21, 1821–1833. (13)
Selye, H. (1979). Stress, cancer, and the mind. In J. Shaughnessy, M. F. (2009). An interview with Shohamy, D., Myers, C. E., Kalanithi, J., & Gluck,
Taché, H. Selye, & S. B. Day (Eds.), Cancer, Christian Ambler: Traumatic brain injury in M. A. (2008). Basal ganglia and dopamine contri-
stress, and death (pp. 11–27). New York: Plenum sports. North American Journal of Psychology, 11, butions to probabilistic category learning. Neurosci-
Press. (12) 297–308. (5) ence and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 219–236. (13)
References 535

Shoulson, I. (1990). Huntington’s disease: Cogni- Sincich, L. C., Park, K. F., Wohlgemuth, M. J., & brain (pp. 97–125). New York: Oxford Univer-
tive and psychiatric features. Neuropsychiatry, Horton, J. C. (2004). Bypassing V1: A direct ge- sity Press. (10)
Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 3, niculate input to area MT. Nature Neuroscience, Smith, K. (2007). Looking for hidden signs of con-
15–22. (8) 7, 1123–1128. (6) sciousness. Nature, 446, 355. (14)
Shrager, Y., Levy, D. A., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, Sinclair, J. R., Jacobs, A. L., & Nirenberg, S. (2004). Smith, K., Thompson, G. F., & Koster, H. D.
L. R. (2008). Working memory and the organi- Selective ablations of a class of amacrine cells al- (1969). Sweat in schizophrenic patients: Identifi-
zation of brain systems. Journal of Neuroscience, ters spatial processing in the retina. Journal of cation of the odorous substance. Science, 166,
28, 4818–4822. (13) Neuroscience, 24, 1459–1467. (6) 398–399. (15)
Shubin, N., Tabin, C., & Carroll, S. (2009). Deep Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Smith, L. T. (1975). The interanimal transfer phe-
homology and the origins of evolutionary nov­ Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for nomenon: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 81,
elty. Nature, 457, 818–823. (1) pain involves the affective but not sensory com- 1078–1095. (13)
Shutts, D. (1982). Lobotomy: Resort to the knife. ponents of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162. (7) Smith, M. A., Brandt, J., & Shadmehr, R. (2000).
New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. (4) Singh, S., & Mallic, B. N. (1996). Mild electrical Motor disorder in Huntington’s disease begins as
Siderowf, A., & Stern, M. (2003). Update on Par- stimulation of pontine tegmentum around locus a dysfunction in error feedback control. Nature,
kinson disease. Annals of Internal Medicine, 138, coeruleus reduces rapid eye movement sleep in 403, 544–549. (8)
651–658. (4, 8) rats. Neuroscience Research, 24, 227–235. (9) Smith, P. J., Blumenthal, J. A., Hoffman, B. M.,
Siebert, M., Markowitsch, H. J., & Bartel, P. (2003). Singleton, A. B., Farrer, M., Johnson, J., Singleton, Cooper, H., Strauman, T. A., Welsh-Bohmer, K.,
Amygdala, affect and cognition: Evidence from A., Hague, S., Kachergus, J., et al. (2003). et al. (2010). Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive
10 patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Brain, a-synuclein locus triplication causes Parkinson’s performance: A meta-analytic review of random-
126, 2627–2637. (12) disease. Science, 302, 841. (8) ized controlled trials. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72,
Siebner, H. R., Limmer, C., Peinemann, A., Drzez- Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., & Sunderland, 239–252. (5)
ga, A., Bloom, B. R., Schwaiger, M., et al. T. (1991). Multiple representations contribute to Smulders, T. V., Shiflett, M. W., Sperling, A. J., &
(2002). Long-term consequences of switching body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case DeVoogd, T. J. (2000). Seasonal changes in neu-
handedness: A positron emission tomography of autopagnosia. Brain, 114, 629–642. (7) ron numbers in the hippocampal formation of a
study on handwriting in “converted” left-hand- Sirotin, Y. B., Hillman, E. M. C., Bordier, C., & Das, food-hoarding bird: The black-capped chickadee.
ers. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2816–2825. A. (2009). Spatiotemporal precision and hemo- Journal of Neurobiology, 44, 414–422. (5)
(14) dynamic mechanism of optical point spreads in Snitz, B. E., O’Meara, E. S., Carlson, M. C., Arnold,
Siegel, J. M. (1995). Phylogeny and the function of alert primates. Proceedings of the National Acade- A. M., Ives, D. G., Rapp, S. R., et al. (2009). Ginkgo
REM sleep. Behavioural Brain Research, 69, 29– my of Sciences, 106, 18390–18395. (4) biloba for preventing cognitive decline in older
34. (9) Sjöström, M., Friden, J., & Ekblom, B. (1987). En- adults: A randomized trial. Journal of the American
Siegel, J. M. (2009). Sleep viewed as a state of adap- durance, what is it? Muscle morphology after an Medical Association, 302, 2663–2670. (13)
tive inactivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, extremely long distance run. Acta Physiologica Snowling, M. J. (1980). The development of grapheme-
747–753. (9) Scandinavica, 130, 513–520. (8) phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic
Siegel, S. (1983). Classical conditioning, drug toler- Skitzki, J. J., Chen, Q., Wang, W. C., & Evans, S. S. readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
ance, and drug dependence. Research Advances in (2007). Primary immune surveillance: Some like 29, 294–305. (14)
Alcohol and Drug Problems, 9, 279–314. (3) it hot. Journal of Molecular Medicine, 85, 1361– Snyder, L. H., Grieve, K. L., Brotchie, P., & Ander-
Siegel, S. (1987). Alcohol and opiate dependence: 1367. (10) sen, R. A. (1998). Separate body- and world-
Reevaluation of the Victorian perspective. Re- Sloan, D. M., Strauss, M. E., & Wisner, K. L. referenced representations of visual space in pari-
search Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems, 9, (2001). Diminished response to pleasant stimuli etal cortex. Nature, 394, 887–891. (8)
279–314. (3) by depressed women. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Solms, M. (1997). The neuropsychology of dreams.
Silber, B. Y., & Schmitt, J. A. J. (2010). Effects of chology, 110, 488–493. (15) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. (9)
tryptophan loading on human cognition, mood, Slob, A. K., Bax, C. M., Hop, W. C. J., Rowland, Solms, M. (2000). Dreaming and REM sleep are
and sleep. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Re- D. L., & van der Werff ten Bosch, J. J. (1996). controlled by different brain mechanisms. Behav-
views, 34, 387–407. (10) Sexual arousability and the menstrual cycle. Psy- ioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 843–850. (9)
Silbersweig, D. A., Stern, E., Frith, C., Cahill, C., choineuroendocrinology, 21, 545–558. (11) Solomon, S. G., & Lennie, P. (2007). The machinery
Holmes, A., Grootoonk, S., et al. (1995). A func- Sluming, V., Brooks, J., Howard, M., Downes, J. J., of colour vision. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8,
tional neuroanatomy of hallucinations in schizo- & Roberts, N. (2007). Broca’s area supports 276–286. (6)
phrenia. Nature, 378, 176–179. (15) enhanced visuospatial cognition in orchestral Somjen, G. G. (1988). Nervenkitt: Notes on the his-
Silk, J. B., Brosnan, S. F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povi- musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3799– tory of the concept of neuroglia. Glia, 1, 2–9. (2)
nelli, D. J., Richardson, A. S., et al. (2005). Chim- 3806. (14) Song, H., Stevens, C. F., & Gage, F. H. (2002). Neu-
panzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelat- Smith, C. N., & Squire, L. R. (2009). Medial tem- ral stem cells from adult hippocampus develop
ed group members. Nature, 437, 1357–1359. (1) poral lobe activity during retrieval of semantic essential properties of functional CNS neurons.
Silva, A. J., Zhou, Y., Rogerson, T., Shobe, J., & Ba- memory is related to the age of the memory. Jour- Nature Neuroscience, 5, 438–445. (5)
laji, J. (2009). Molecular and cellular approaches nal of Neuroscience, 29, 930–938. (13) Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J., & Haynes,
to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science, Smith, D. E., Rapp, P. R., McKay, H. M., Roberts, J.-D. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free
326, 391–395. (13) J. A., & Tuszynski, M. H. (2004). Memory im- decisions in the human brain. Nature Neurosci-
Silver, R. A. (2010). Neuronal arithmetic. Nature pairment in aged primates is associated with fo- ence, 11, 543–545. (8)
Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 474–489. (3) cal death of cortical neurons and atrophy of sub- Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Jerni-
Simner, J., Harrold, J., Creed, H., Monro, L., & cortical neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, gan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (1999). In vivo evidence
Foulkes, L. (2009). Early detection of markers 4373–4381. (13) for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal
for synaesthesia in childhood populations. Brain, Smith, G. P. (1998). Pregastric and gastric satiety. In and striatal regions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 309–
132, 57–64. (7) G. P. Smith (Ed.), Satiation: From gut to brain 315. (5)
Simner, J., & Ward, J. (2006). The taste of words on (pp. 10–39). New York: Oxford University Spalding, K. L., Bhardwaj, R. D., Buchholz, B. A.,
the tip of the tongue. Nature, 444, 438. (7) Press. (10) Druid, H., & Frisén, J. (2005). Retrospective birth
Simon, E. (2000). Interface properties of circum- Smith, G. P., & Gibbs, J. (1998). The satiating ef- dating of cells in humans. Cell, 122, 133–143. (5)
ventricular organs in salt and fluid balance. News fects of cholecystokinin and bombesin-like pep- Spangler, R., Wittkowski, K. M., Goddard, N. L.,
in Physiological Sciences, 15, 61–67. (10) tides. In G. P. Smith (Ed.), Satiation: From gut to Avena, N. M., Hoebel, B. G., & Leibowitz, S. F.
536 References

(2004). Opiate-like effects of sugar on gene ex- Steeves, J., Dricot, L., Goltz, H. C., Sorger, B., Pe- imagery. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1565–
pression in reward areas of the rat brain. Molecu- ters, J., Milner, A. D., et al. (2009). Abnormal face 1572. (6)
lar Brain Research, 124, 134–142. (10) identity coding in the middle fusiform gyrus of Stone, V. E., Nisenson, L., Eliassen, J. C., & Gazza-
Speer, N. K., Reynolds, J. R., Swallow, K. M., & two brain-damaged prosopagnosic patients. Neu- niga, M. S. (1996). Left hemisphere representa-
Zacks, J. M. (2009). Reading stories activates ropsychologia, 47, 2584–2592. (6) tions of emotional facial expressions. Neuropsy-
neural representations of visual and motor expe- Stefansson, H., Rujescu, D., Cichon, S., Pietiläinen, chologia, 34, 23–29. (14)
riences. Psychological Science, 20, 989–999. (8) O. P. H., Ingason, A., Steinberg, S., et al. (2008). Storey, K. B., & Storey, J. M. (1999, May/June).
Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic ap- Large recurrent microdeletions associated with Lifestyles of the cold and frozen. The Sciences,
titude for mathematics and science? American schizophrenia. Nature, 455, 232–236. (1) 39(3), 33–37. (10)
Psychologist, 60, 950–958. (4) Stein, M. B., Hanna, C., Koverola, C., Torchia, M., Stout, A. K., Raphael, H. M., Kanterewicz, B. I.,
Spencer, R. M. C., Zelaznik, H. N., Diedrichsen, J., & McClarty, B. (1997). Structural brain changes Klann, E., & Reynolds, I. J. (1998). Glutamate-
& Ivry, R. B. (2003). Disrupted timing of discon- in PTSD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci- induced neuron death requires mitochondrial
tinuous but not continuous movements by cere- ences, 821, 76–82. (12) calcium uptake. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 366–
bellar lesions. Science, 300, 1437–1439. (8) Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., 373. (5)
Sperry, R. W. (1943). Visuomotor coordination in Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differ- Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). In-
the newt (Triturus viridescens) after regeneration ences in future orientation and delay discounting. hibiting and facilitating conditions of the human
of the optic nerve. Journal of Comparative Neurol- Child Development, 80, 28–44. (5) smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback
ogy, 79, 33–55. (5) Steiner, T., Ringleb, P., & Hacke, W. (2001). Treat- hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Sperry, R. W. (1975). In search of psyche. In F. G. ment options for large hemispheric stroke. Neu- chology, 54, 768–777. (12)
Worden, J. P. Swazey, & G. Adelman (Eds.), The rology, 57(Suppl. 2), S61–S68. (5) Stricker, E. M. (1969). Osmoregulation and volume
neurosciences: Paths of discovery (pp. 425–434). Stella, N., Schweitzer, P., & Piomelli, D. (1997). A regulation in rats: Inhibition of hypovolemic
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (5) second endogenous cannabinoid that modulates thirst by water. American Journal of Physiology,
Spezio, M. L., Huang, P.-Y. S., Castelli, F., & long-term potentiation. Nature, 382, 677–678. (3) 217, 98–105. (10)
Adolphs, R. (2007). Amygdala damage impairs Stephens, T. W., Basinski, M., Bristow, P. K., Bue- Stricker, E. M., & Hoffmann, M. L. (2007). Presys-
eye contact during conversations with real peo- Valleskey, J. M., Burgett, S. G., Craft, L., et al. temic signals in the control of thirst, salt appetite,
ple. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 3994–3997. (12) (1995). The role of neuropeptide Y in the anti- and vasopressin secretion. Physiology & Behavior,
Spiegel, T. A. (1973). Caloric regulation of food in- obesity action of the obese gene product. Nature, 91, 404–412. (10)
take in man. Journal of Comparative and Physio- 377, 530–532. (10) Stricker, E. M., Swerdloff, A. F., & Zigmond, M. J.
logical Psychology, 84, 24–37. (10) Sterpenich, V., D’Argembeau, A., Desseiles, M., (1978). Intrahypothalamic injections of kainic
Spindler, K. A., Sullivan, E. V., Menon, V., Lim, Balteau, E., Albouy, G., Vandewalle, G., et al. acid produce feeding and drinking deficits in rats.
K. O., & Pfefferbaum, A. (1997). Deficits in mul- (2006). The locus coeruleus is involved in the Brain Research, 158, 470–473. (10)
tiple systems of working memory in schizophre- successful retrieval of emotional memories in Strickland, T. L., Miller, B. L., Kowell, A., & Stein,
nia. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 1–10. (15) humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 7416– R. (1998). Neurobiology of cocaine-induced or-
Spreux-Varoquaux, O., Alvarez, J.-C., Berlin, I., Ba- 7423. (9) ganic brain impairment: Contributions from
tista, G., Despierre, P.-G., Gilton, A., et al. Stevens, C. F. (2001). An evolutionary scaling law functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychology Re-
(2001). Differential abnormalities in plasma for the primate visual system and its basis in cor- view, 8, 1–9. (3)
5-HIAA and platelet serotonin concentrations tical function. Nature, 411, 193–195. (6) Striemer, C. L., Chapman, C. S., & Goodale, M. A.
in violent suicide attempters. Life Sciences, 69, Stevens, M., & Cuthill, I. C. (2007). Hidden messag- (2009). “Real-time” obstacle avoidance in the ab-
647–657. (12) es: Are ultraviolet signals a special channel in avian sence of primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the
Spurzheim, J. G. (1908). Phrenology (Rev. ed.) Phil- communication? Bioscience, 57, 501–507. (6) National Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.), 106,
adelphia: Lippincott. (4) Stevens, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1997). Word 15996–16001. (6)
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocam- learning in a special population: Do individuals Stroebele, N., de Castro, J. M., Stuht, J., Catenacci,
pus: A synthesis from findings with rats, mon- with Williams syndrome obey lexical con- V., Wyatt, H. R., & Hill, J. O. (2008). A small-
keys, and humans. Psychological Review, 99, 195– straints? Journal of Child Language, 24, 737– changes approach reduces energy intake in free-
231. (13) 765. (14) living humans. Journal of the American College of
Squires, T. M. (2004). Optimizing the vertebrate ves- Stewart, J. W., Quitkin, F. M., McGrath, P. J., Am- Nutrition, 28, 63–68. (10)
tibular semicircular canal: Could we balance any sterdam, J., Fava, M., Fawcett, J., et al. (1998). Strotmann, J., Levai, O., Fleischer, J., Schwarzen-
better? Physical Review Letters, 93, 198106. (7) Use of pattern analysis to predict differential re- bacher, K., & Breer, H. (2004). Olfactory recep-
St George-Hyslop, P. H. (2000). Genetic factors in lapse of remitted patients with major depression tor proteins in axonal processes of chemosensory
the genesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Annals of the during 1 year of treatment with fluoxetine or pla- neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 224, 7754–
New York Academy of Sciences, 924, 1–7. (13) cebo. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 334– 7761. (7)
St. Jacques, P. L., Dolcos, F., & Cabeza, R. (2009). 343. (15) Stryker, M. P., & Sherk, H. (1975). Modification of
Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the Stickgold, R., Malia, A., Maguire, D., Roddenberry, cortical orientation selectivity in the cat by re-
amygdala for subsequent memory of negative D., & O’Connor, M. (2000). Replaying the game: stricted visual experience: A reexamination. Sci-
pictures. Psychological Science, 20, 74–84. (12) Hypnagogic images in normals and amnesics. ence, 190, 904–906. (6)
Stalnaker, T. A., Roesch, M. R., Franz, T. M., Calu, Science, 290, 350–353. (13) Stryker, M. P., Sherk, H., Leventhal, A. G., &
D. J., Singh, T., & Schoenbaum, G. (2007). Stocco, A., Lebiere, C., & Anderson, J. R. (2010). Hirsch, H. V. B. (1978). Physiological conse-
Cocaine-induced decision-making deficits are Conditional routing of information to the cortex: quences for the cat’s visual cortex of effectively
mediated by miscoding in basolateral amygdala. A model of the basal ganglia’s role in cognitive restricting early visual experience with oriented
Nature Neuroscience, 10, 949–951. (3) coordination. Psychological Review, 117, 541– contours. Journal of Neurophysiology, 41, 896–
Stanford, L. R. (1987). Conduction velocity varia- 574. (4) 909. (6)
tions minimize conduction time differences Stockman, A., & Sharpe, L. T. (1998). Human cone Studer, L., Tabar, V., & McKay, R. D. G. (1998).
among retinal ganglion cell axons. Science, 238, spectral sensitivities: A progress report. Vision Transplantation of expanded mesencephalic pre-
358–360. (2) Research, 38, 3193–3206. (6) cursors leads to recovery in Parkinsonian rats.
Starr, C., & Taggart, R. (1989). Biology: The unity Stokes, M., Thompson, R., Cusack, R., & Duncan, Nature Neuroscience, 1, 290–295. (8)
and diversity of life. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/ J. (2009). Top-down activation of shape-specific Stunkard, A. J., Sorensen, T. I. A., Hanis, C., Teas-
Cole. (3, 4, 7, 8, 11) population codes in visual cortex during mental dale, T. W., Chakraborty, R., Schull, W. J., et al.
References 537

(1986). An adoption study of human obesity. panic disorder—an 11 year follow-up. Journal of for addictive properties of benzodiazepines. Na-
New England Journal of Medicine, 314, 193–198. Anxiety Disorders, 17, 223–232. (12) ture, 463, 769–774. (12)
(10) Syken, J., GrandPre, T., Kanold, P. O., & Shatz, C. Tanaka, J., Hayashi, Y., Nomura, S., Miyakubo, H.,
Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Neuropsycho- J. (2006). PirB restricts ocular-dominance plas- Okumura, T., & Sakamaki, K. (2001). Angioten-
logical studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological ticity in visual cortex. Science, 313, 1795–1800. sinergic and noradrenergic mechanisms in the
Bulletin, 95, 3–28. (4) (6) hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus participate
Sullivan, E. V., Deshmukh, A., Desmond, J. E., Szymusiak, R. (1995). Magnocellular nuclei of the in the drinking response induced by activation of
Mathalon, D. H., Rosenbloom, M. J., Lim, K. O., basal forebrain: Substrates of sleep and arousal the subfornical organ in rats. Behavioural Brain
et al. (2000). Contribution of alcohol abuse to regulation. Sleep, 18, 478–500. (9) Research, 118, 117–122. (10)
cerebellar volume deficits in men with schizo- Tabrizi, S. J., Cleeter, M. W. J., Xuereb, J., Taanman, Tanaka, J., Hori, K., & Nomura, M. (2001). Dipso-
phrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 894– J.-W., Cooper, J. M., & Schapira, A. H. V. (1999). genic response induced by angiotensinergic path-
902. (15) Biochemical abnormalities and excitotoxicity in ways from the lateral hypothalamic area to the
Sun, H.-S., Jackson, M. F., Martin, L. J., Jansen, K., Huntington’s disease brain. Annals of Neurology, subfornical organ in rats. Behavioural Brain Re-
Teves, L., Cui, H., et al. (2009). Suppression of 45, 25–32. (8) search, 118, 111–116. (10)
hippocampal TRPM7 protein prevents delayed Taddese, A., Nah, S. Y., & McCleskey, E. W. (1995). Tanaka, K., Sugita, Y., Moriya, M., & Saito, H.-A.
neuronal death in brain ischemia. Nature Neuro- Selective opioid inhibition of small nociceptive (1993). Analysis of object motion in the ventral
science, 12, 1300–1307. (5) neurons. Science, 270, 1366–1369. (7) part of the medial superior temporal area of the
Sun, Y.-G., & Chen, Z.-F. (2007). A gastrin-releasing Tagawa, Y., Kanold, P. O., Majdan, M., & Shatz, macaque visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiolo-
peptide receptor mediates the itch sensation in the C. J. (2005). Multiple periods of functional ocu- gy, 68, 128–142. (6)
spinal cord. Nature, 448, 700–703. (7) lar dominance plasticity in mouse visual cortex. Tanaka, M., Nakahara, T., Muranaga, T., Kojima, S.,
Sun, Y.-G., Zhao, Z.-Q., Meng, X.-L., Yin, J., Liu, Nature Neuroscience, 8, 380–388. (6) Yasuhara, D., Ueno, H., et al. (2006). Ghrelin con-
X.-Y., & Chen, Z. F. (2009). Cellular basis of itch Tager-Flusberg, H., Boshart, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. centrations and cardiac vagal tone are decreased
sensation. Science, 325, 1531–1534. (7) (1998). Reading the windows to the soul: Evi- after pharmacologic and cognitive-behavioral
Sunaert, S., Van Hecke, P., Marchal, G., & Orban, dence of domain-specific sparing in Williams treatment in patients with bulimia nervosa. Hor-
G. A. (1999). Motion-responsive regions of the syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, mones and Behavior, 50, 261–265. (10)
human brain. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 631–639. (14) Tanaka, Y., Kamo, T., Yoshida, M., & Yamadori, A.
355–370. (6) Taillard, J., Philip, P., Coste, O., Sagaspe, P., & Bio- (1991). “So-called” cortical deafness. Brain, 114,
Sur, M., & Leamey, C. A. (2001). Development and ulac, B. (2003). The circadian and homeostatic 2385–2401. (7)
plasticity of cortical areas and networks. Nature modulation of sleep pressure during wakefulness Tandon, S., Kambi, N., Lazar, L., Mohammed, H.,
Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 251–262. (6) differs between morning and evening chrono- & Jain, N. (2009). Large-scale expansion of the
Sutton, L. C., Lea, E., Will, M. J., Schwartz, B. A., types. Journal of Sleep Research, 12, 275–282. (9) face representation in somatosensory areas of
Hartley, C. E., Poole, J. C., et al. (1997). Inescap- Takahashi, K., Lin, J.-S., & Sakai, K. (2006). Neu- the lateral sulcus after spinal cord injuries in
able shock-induced potentiation of morphine ronal activity of histaminergic tuberomammil­ monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12009–
analgesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 1105– lary neurons during wake–sleep states in the 12019. (5)
1113. (7) mouse. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 10292– Tanji, J., & Shima, K. (1994). Role for supplemen-
Sutton, R. L., Hovda, D. A., & Feeney, D. M. 10298. (9) tary motor area cells in planning several move-
(1989). Amphetamine accelerates recovery of lo- Takahashi, T., Svoboda, K., & Malinow, R. (2003). ments ahead. Nature, 371, 413–416. (8)
comotor function following bilateral frontal cor- Experience strengthening transmission by driv- Tanner, C. M., Ottman, R., Goldman, S. M., Ellen-
tex ablation in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103, ing AMPA receptors into synapses. Science, 299, berg, J., Chan, P., Mayeux, R., et al. (1999). Par-
837–841. (5) 1585–1588. (13) kinson disease in twins: An etiologic study. Jour-
Suvisaari, J. M., Haukka, J. K., Tanskanen, A. J., & Takano, T., Tian, G.-F., Peng, W., Lou, N., Libionka, nal of the American Medical Association, 281,
Lönnqvist, J. K. (1999). Decline in the incidence W., Han, X., et al. (2006). Astrocyte-mediated 341–346. (8)
of schizophrenia in Finnish cohorts born from control of cerebral blood flow. Nature Neurosci- Taravosh-Lahn, K., Bastida, C., & Delville, Y.
1954 to 1965. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, ence, 9, 260–267. (2) (2006). Differential responsiveness to fluoxetine
733–740. (15) Takashima, A., Nieuwenhuis, I. L. C., Jensen, O., during puberty. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120,
Suzdak, P. D., Glowa, J. R., Crawley, J. N., Schwartz, Talamini, L. M., Rijpkema, M., & Fernández, G. 1084–1092. (12)
R. D., Skolnick, P., & Paul, S. M. (1986). A selec- (2009). Shift from hippocampal to neocortical Tarr, M. J., & Gauthier, I. (2000). FFA: A flexible
tive imidazobenzodiazepine antagonist of etha- centered retrieval network with consolidation. fusiform area for subordinate-level visual pro-
nol in the rat. Science, 234, 1243–1247. (12) Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 10087–10093. (13) cessing automatized by experience. Nature Neu-
Swaab, D. F., & Hofman, M. A. (1990). An enlarged Takatsuru, Y., Fukumoto, D., Yoshitomo, M., roscience, 3, 764–769. (6)
suprachiasmatic nucleus in homosexual men. Nemoto, T., Tsukada, H., & Nabekura, J. (2009). Tashiro, A., Makino, H., & Gage, F. H. (2007).
Brain Research, 537, 141–148. (11) Neuronal circuit remodeling in the contralateral Experience-specific functional modification of
Swan, S. H., Liu, F., Hines, M., Kruse, R. L., Wang, cortical hemisphere during functional recovery the dentate gyrus through adult neurogenesis: A
C., Redmon, J. B., et al. (2010). Prenatal phthal- from cerebral infarction. Journal of Neuroscience, critical period during an immature stage. Journal
ate exposure and reduced masculine play in boys. 29, 10081–10086. (5) of Neuroscience, 27, 3252–3259. (5)
International Journal of Andrology, 33, 259-269. Takehara-Nishiuchi, K., & McNaughton, B. L. Tattersall, G. J., Andrade, D. V., & Abe, A. S. (2009).
(11) (2008). Spontaneous changes of neocortical code Heat exchange from the toucan bill reveals a con-
Swithers, S. E., & Davidson, T. L. (2008). A role for for associative memory during consolidation. Sci- trollable vascular thermal radiator. Science, 325,
sweet taste: Calorie predictive relations in energy ence, 322, 960–963. (13) 468–470. (10)
regulation by rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Taub, E., & Berman, A. J. (1968). Movement and
161–173. (10) Geminiani, G., Weiskrantz, L., et al. (2009). Un- learning in the absence of sensory feedback. In
Swoboda, E., Conca, A., König, P., Waanders, R., & seen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast S. J. Freedman (Ed.), The neuropsychology of spa-
Hansen, M. (2001). Maintenance electroconvul- emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National tially oriented behavior (pp. 173–192). Home-
sive therapy in affective and schizoaffective disor- Academy of Sciences (U.S.A.), 106, 17661– wood, IL: Dorsey. (5)
ders. Neuropsychobiology, 43, 23–28. (15) 17666. (6) Taylor, J. P., Hardy, J., & Fischbeck, K H. (2002).
Swoboda, H., Amering, M., Windhaber, J., & Tan, K. R., Brown, M., Laboèbe, G., Yvon, C., Cre- Toxic proteins in neurodegenerative disease. Sci-
Katschnig, H. (2003). The long-term course of ton, C., Fritschy, J.-M., et al. (2010). Neural bases ence, 296, 1991–1995. (13)
538 References

Taylor, M. A. (1969). Sex ratios of newborns: As- (2000). Reduced number of hypocretin neurons tence in Africa and Europe. Nature Genetics, 39,
sociated with prepartum and postpartum schizo- in human narcolepsy. Neuron, 27, 469–474. (9) 31–40. (10)
phrenia. Science, 164, 723–721. (15) Thapar, A., Fowler, T., Rice, F., Scourfield, J., van Tiwari, S. K., & Wang, J. L. (2006). The epidemiol-
Taziaux, M., Keller, M., Bakker, J., & Balthazart, J. den Bree, M., Thomas, H., et al. (2003). Mater- ogy of mental and substance use-related disor-
(2007). Sexual behavior activity tracks rapid nal smoking during pregnancy and attention ders among White, Chinese, and other Asian
changes in brain estrogen concentrations. Journal deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in off- populations in Canada. Canadian Journal of Psy-
of Neuroscience, 27, 6563–6572. (11) spring. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, chiatry, 51, 904–912. (15)
Teff, K. L., Elliott, S. S., Tschöp, M., Kieffer, T. J., 1985–1989. (5) Tobin, V. A., Hashimoto, H., Wacker, D. W., Takay-
Rader, D., Heiman, M., et al. (2004). Dietary Thase, M. E., Greenhouse, J. B., Frank, E., Reynolds, anagi, Y., Langnaese, K., Caquíneau, C., et al.
fructose reduces circulating insulin and leptin, C. F., III, Pilkonis, P. A., Hurley, K., et al. (1997). (2010). An intrinsic vasopressin system in the
attenuates postprandial suppression of ghrelin, Treatment of major depression with psychothera- olfactory bulb is involved in social recognition.
and increases triglycerides in women. Journal of py or psychotherapy-psychopharmacology combi- Nature, 464, 413–417. (11)
Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 89, 2963– nations. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 1009– Toh, K. L., Jones, C. R., He, Y., Eide, E. J., Hinz,
2972. (10) 1015. (15) W. A., Virshup, D. M., et al. (2001). An hPer2
Teicher, M. H., Glod, C. A., Magnus, E., Harper, D., Thase, M. E., Trivedi, M. H., & Rush, A. J. (1995). phosphorylation site mutation in familial ad-
Benson, G., Krueger, K., et al. (1997). Circadian MAOIs in the contemporary treatment of de- vanced sleep phase syndrome. Science, 291,
rest–activity disturbances in seasonal affective pression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 12, 185– 1040–1043. (9)
disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 124– 219. (15) Toien, Ø., Blake, J., Edgar, D. M., Grahn, D. A.,
130. (15) Theusch, E., Basu, A., & Gitschier, J. (2009). Heller, H. C., & Barnes, B. M. (2011). Hiberna-
Teitelbaum, P. (1961). Disturbances in feeding and Genome-wide study of families with absolute tion in black bears: Independence of metabolic
drinking behavior after hypothalamic lesions. In pitch reveals linkage to 8q24.21 and locus het- suppression from body temperature. Science,
M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposia on Motiva- erogeneity. American Journal of Human Genet- 331, 906–909. (9)
tion 1961 (pp. 39–69). Lincoln: University of ics, 85, 112–119. (7) Tokizawa, K., Yasuhara, S., Nakamura, M., Uchida,
Nebraska Press. (10) Thiele, A., Henning, P., Kubischik, K.-P., & Hoff- Y., Crawshaw, L. I., & Nagashma, K. (2010).
Teitelbaum, P., & Epstein, A. N. (1962). The lateral man, K.-P. (2002). Neural mechanisms of sacca- Mild hypohydration induced by exercise in the
hypothalamic syndrome. Psychological Review, dic suppression. Science, 295, 2460–2462. (6) heat attenuates autonomic thermoregulatory re-
69, 74–90. (10) Thier, P., Dicke, P. W., Haas, R., & Barash, S. sponses to the heat, but not thermal pleasantness
Teitelbaum, P., Pellis, V. C., & Pellis, S. M. (1991). (2000). Encoding of movement time by popula- in humans. Physiology & Behavior, 100, 340–
Can allied reflexes promote the integration of a tions of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature, 405, 345. (10)
robot’s behavior? In J. A. Meyer & S. W. Wilson 72–76. (8) Tomchik, S. M., Berg, S., Kim, J. W., Chaudhari, N.,
(Eds.), From animals to animats: Simulation of Thomas, C., Avidan, G., Humphreys, K., Jung, & Roper, S. D. (2007). Breadth of tuning and
animal behavior (pp. 97–104). Cambridge, MA: K., Gao, F., & Behrmann, M. (2009). Reduced taste coding in mammalian taste buds. Journal of
MIT Press/Bradford Books. (8) structural connectivity in ventral visual cortex in Neuroscience, 27, 10840–10848. (7)
Temel, Y., Boothman, L. J., Blokland, A., Magill, congenital prosopagnosia. Nature Neuroscience, Tominaga, M., Caterina, M. J., Malmberg, A. B.,
P. J., Steinbusch, H. W. M., Visser-Vandewalle, 12, 29–31. (6) Rosen, T. A., Gilbert, H., Skinner, K., et al.
V., et al. (2007). Inhibition of 5-HT neuron ac- Thompson, B. L., Levitt, P., & Stanwood, G. D. (1998). The cloned capsaicin receptor integrates
tivity and induction of depressive-like behavior (2009). Prenatal exposure to drugs: Effects on multiple pain-producing stimuli. Neuron, 21,
by high-frequency stimulation of the subthalam- brain development and implications for policy 531–543. (7)
ic nucleus. Proceedings of the National Academy of and education. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, Tomo, I., de Monvel, J. B., & Fridberger, A. (2007).
Sciences, USA, 104, 17087–17092. (8) 303–312. (5) Sound-evoked radial strain in the hearing organ.
Terayama, H., Nishino, Y., Kishi, M., Ikuta, K., Thompson, R. F. (1986). The neurobiology of learn- Biophysical Journal, 93, 3279–3284. (7)
Itoh, M., & Iwahashi, K. (2003). Detection of ing and memory. Science, 233, 941–947. (13) Tong, Q., Ye, C.-P., Jones, J. E., Elmquist, J. K., &
anti-Borna virus (BDV) antibodies from pa- Ticku, M. K., & Kulkarni, S. K. (1988). Molecular Lowell, B. B. (2008). Synaptic release of GABA
tients with schizophrenia and mood disorders in interactions of ethanol with GABAergic system by AgRP neurons is required for normal regula-
Japan. Psychiatry Research, 120, 201–206. (15) and potential of Ro15-4513 as an ethanol an- tion of energy balance. Nature Neuroscience, 11,
Terman, M., Terman, J. S., & Ross, D. C. (1998). A tagonist. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behav- 998–1000. (10)
controlled trial of timed bright light and negative ior, 30, 501–510. (12) Toni, N., Buchs, P.-A., Nikonenko, I., Bron, C. R., &
air ionization for treatment of winter depression. Timms, B. G., Howdeshell, K. L., Barton, L., Muller, D. (1999). LTP promotes formation of
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 875–882. (15) Richter, C. A., & vom Saal, F. S. (2005). Estro- multiple spine synapses between a single axon ter-
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, genic chemicals in plastic and oral contracep- minal and a dendrite. Nature, 402, 421–425. (13)
T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Sci- tives disrupt development of the fetal mouse Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., Xian, H.,
ence, 206, 891–902. (14) prostate and urethra. Proceedings of the Nation- Chantarujikapong, S., Seidman, L. J., et al.
Tetrud, J. W., Langston, J. W., Garbe, P. L., & Rut- al Academy of Sciences, USA, 102, 7014–7019. (2003). A twin study of the neuropsychological
tenber, A. J. (1989). Mild Parkinsonism in per- (11) consequences of stimulant abuse. Archives of
sons exposed to 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6- Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. Oxford, General Psychiatry, 60, 303–310. (3)
tetrahydropyridine (MPTP). Neurology, 39, England: Oxford University Press. (1) Torrey, E. F. (1986). Geographic variations in
1483–1487. (8) Tinbergen, N. (1973). The search for animal roots schizophrenia. In C. Shagass, R. C. Josiassen,
Teychenne, M., Ball, K., & Salmon, J. (2010). Sed- of human behavior. In N. Tinbergen (Ed.), The W. H. Bridger, K. J. Weiss, D. Stoff, & G. M.
entary behavior and depression among adults: A animal in its world (Vol. 2, pp. 161–174). Cam- Simpson (Eds.), Biological psychiatry 1985 (pp.
review. International Journal of Behavioral Medi- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (1) 1080–1082). New York: Elsevier. (15)
cine, 17, 246–254. (15) Tingate, T. R., Lugg, D. J., Muller, H. K., Stowe, Torrey, E. F., & Miller, J. (2001). The invisible plague:
Thalemann, R., Wölfling, K., & Grüsser, S. M. R. P., & Pierson, D. L. (1997). Antarctic isola- The rise of mental illness from 1750 to the present.
(2007). Specific cue reactivity on computer tion: Immune and viral studies. Immunology and New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. (15)
game-related cues in excessive gamers. Behavioral Cell Biology, 75, 275–283. (12) Torrey, E. F., Miller, J., Rawlings, R., & Yolken, R. H.
Neuroscience, 121, 614–618. (3) Tishkoff, S. A., Reed, F. A., Ranciaro, A., Voight, (1997). Seasonality of births in schizophrenia
Thanickal, T. C., Moore, R. Y., Nienhuis, R., Ra- B. F., Babbitt, C. C., Silverman, J. S., et al. (2006). and bipolar disorder: A review of the literature.
manathan, L., Gulyani, S., Aldrich, M., et al. Convergent adaptation of human lactase persis- Schizophrenia Research, 28, 1–38. (15)
References 539

Torrey, E. F., Rawlings, R., & Yolken, R. H. (2000). Tsai, G., Passani, L. A., Slusher, B. S., Carter, R., Uhr, M., Tontsch, A., Namendorf, C., Ripke, S., Lu-
The antecedents of psychosis: A case-control Baer, L., Kleinman, J. E., et al. (1995). Abnormal cae, S., Ising, M., et al. (2008). Polymorphisms in
study of selected risk factors. Schizophrenia Re- excitatory neurotransmitter metabolism in the drug transporter gene ABCBx predict anti-
search, 46, 17–23. (15) schizophrenic brains. Archives of General Psychia- depressant treatment response in depression.
Torrey, E. F., & Yolken, R. H. (2005). Toxoplasma try, 52, 829–836. (15) Neuron, 57, 203–209. (15)
gondii as a possible cause of schizophrenia. Bio- Tsai, G. E., Ragan, P., Chang, R., Chen, S., Linnoila, Unkelbach, C., Guastella, A. J., & Forgas, J. P.
logical Psychiatry, 57, 128S. (15) M. I., & Coyle, J. T. (1998). Increased glutama- (2008). Oxytocin selectively facilitates recogni-
Toufexis, D. (2007). Region- and sex-specific mod- tergic neurotransmission and oxidative stress af- tion of positive sex and relationship words. Psy-
ulation of anxiety behaviours in the rat. Journal of ter alcohol withdrawal. American Journal of Psy- chological Science, 19, 1092–1094. (11)
Neuroendocrinology, 19, 461–473. (12) chiatry, 155, 726–732. (3) Unterberg, A. W., Stover, J., Kress, B., & Kiening,
Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., Covington, J., West- Tsankova, N., Renthal, W., Kumar, A., & Nestler, E. J. K. L. (2004). Edema and brain trauma. Neurosci-
erfield, M., Harris, N. S., Lyden, P., et al. (1999). (2007). Epigenetic regulation in psychiatric disor- ence, 129, 1021–1029. (5)
Spatial attention deficits in patients with ac- ders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 355–367. (1) Urry, H. L., Nitschke, J. B., Dolski, I., Jackson,
quired or developmental cerebellar abnormality. Ts’o, D. Y., & Roe, A. W. (1995). Functional com- D. C., Dalton, K. M., Mueller, C. J., et al.
Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5632–5643. (8) partments in visual cortex: Segregation and in- (2004). Making a life worth living: Neural cor-
Tran, P. B., & Miller, R. J. (2003). Chemokine recep- teraction. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive relates of well-being. Psychological Science, 15,
tors: Signposts to brain development and disease. neurosciences (pp. 325–337). Cambridge, MA: 367–372. (12)
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 444–455. (5) MIT Press. (6) U.S.–Venezuela Collaborative Research Project.
Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. (1993). The covert learn- Tsujita, T., Niikawa, N., Yamashita, H., Imamura, (2004). Venezuelan kindreds reveal that genetic
ing of affective valence does not require struc- A., Hamada, A., Nakane, Y., et al. (1998). Ge- and environmental factors modulate Hunting-
tures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Jour- nomic discordance between monozygotic twins ton’s disease age of onset. Proceedings of the Na-
nal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 79–88. (13) discordant for schizophrenia. American Journal tional Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 3498–
Travers, S. P., Pfaffmann, C., & Norgren, R. (1986). of Psychiatry, 155, 422–424. (15) 3503. (8)
Convergence of lingual and palatal gustatory Tu, Y., Kroener, S., Abernathy, K., Lapish, C., Sea- Uwano, T., Nishijo, H., Ono, T., & Tamura, R.
neural activity in the nucleus of the solitary tract. mans, J., Chandler, L. J., et al. (2007). Ethanol in- (1995). Neuronal responsiveness to various sen-
Brain Research, 365, 305–320. (7) hibits persistent activity in prefrontal cortical neu- sory stimuli, and associative learning in the rat
Trefilov, A., Berard, J., Krawczak, M., & rons. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 4765–4775. (3) amygdala. Neuroscience, 68, 339–361. (12)
Schmidtke, J. (2000). Natal dispersal in rhesus Tucker, D. M., Luu, P., & Pribram, K. H. (1995). Vaishnavi, S., Calhoun, J., & Chatterjee, A. (2001).
macaques is related to serotonin transporter Social and emotional self-regulation. Annals of Binding personal and peripersonal space: Evi-
gene promoter variation. Behavior Genetics, 30, the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 213– dence from tactile extinction. Journal of Cognitive
295–301. (12) 239. (8) Neuroscience, 13, 181–189. (14)
Treisman, A. (1999). Feature binding, attention and Tups, A. (2009). Physiological models of leptin re- Valente, E. M., Abou-Sleiman, P. M., Caputo, V.,
object perception. In G. W. Humphreys, J. Dun- sistance. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 21, 961– Muquit, M. M. K., Harvey, K., Gispert, S., et al.
can, & A. Treisman (Eds.), Attention, space, and 971. (10) (2004). Hereditary early-onset Parkinson’s dis-
action (pp. 91–111). Oxford, England: Oxford Turk, D. J., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Funnell, ease caused by mutations in PINKx. Science, 304,
University Press. (4) M. G., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Macrae, C. N. (2002). 1158–1160. (8)
Trevena, J. A., & Miller, J. (2002). Cortical move- Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain pa- Valli, I., Stone, J., Mechelli, A., Bhattacharyya, S.,
ment preparation before and after a conscious tient. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 841–842. (14) Raffin, M., Allen, P., et al. (2011). Altered medial
decision to move. Consciousness and Cognition, Turner, R. S., & Anderson, M. E. (2005). Context- temporal activation related to local glutamate lev-
11, 162–190. (8) dependent modulation of movement-related dis- els in subjects with prodromal signs of psychosis.
Trim, R. S., Schuckit, M. A., & Smith, T. L. (2009). charge in the primate globus pallidus. Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 69, 97–99. (15)
The relationships of the level of response to alco- Neuroscience, 25, 2965–2976. (8) Vallines, I., & Greenlee, M. W. (2006). Saccadic
hol and additional characteristics to alcohol use Twenge, J. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age, suppression of retinotopically localized blood
disorders across adulthood: A discrete-time sur- gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth co- oxygen level-dependent responses in human pri-
vival analysis. Alcoholism—Clinical and Experi- hort differences on the children’s depression in- mary visual area V1. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,
mental Research, 33, 1562–1570. (3) ventory: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal 5965–5969. (6)
Trimble, M. R., & Thompson, P. J. (1986). Neuro- Psychology, 111, 578–588. (15) Valzelli, L. (1973). The “isolation syndrome” in
psychological and behavioral sequelae of sponta- Tye, K. M., Tye, L. D., Cone, J. J., Hekkelman, E. F., mice. Psychopharmacologia, 31, 305–320. (12)
neous seizures. Annals of the New York Academy Janak, P. H., & Bonci, A. (2010). Methylpheni- Valzelli, L. (1980). An approach to neuroanatomical
of Sciences, 462, 284–292. (15) date facilitates learning-induced amygdala plas- and neurochemical psychophysiology. Torino, Italy:
Trinh, K., Andrews, L., Krause, J., Hanak, T., Lee, ticity. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 475–481. (13) C. G. Edizioni Medico Scientifiche. (10, 15)
D., Gelb, M., et al. (2010). Decaffeinated coffee Uchida, N., Takahashi, Y. K., Tanifuji, M., & Mori, Valzelli, L., & Bernasconi, S. (1979). Aggressiveness
and nicotine-free tobacco provide neuroprotec- K., (2000). Odor maps in the mammalian olfac- by isolation and brain serotonin turnover chang-
tion in Drosophila models of Parkinson’s disease tory bulb: Domain organization and odorant es in different strains of mice. Neuropsychobiology,
through an NRF2-dependent mechanism. Jour- structural features. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 5, 129–135. (12)
nal of Neuroscience, 30, 5525–5532. (8) 1035–1043. (7) van Anders, S. M., Brotto, L., Farrell, J., & Yule, M.
Trivedi, M. H., Fava, M., Wisniewski, S. R., Thase, Udry, J. R., & Chantala, K. (2006). Masculinity– (2009). Associations among physiological and
M. E., Quitlein, F., Warden, D., et al. (2006). femininity predicts sexual orientation in men but subjective sexual response, sexual desire, and
Medication augmentation after failure of SSRIs not in women. Journal of Biosocial Science, 38, salivary steroid hormones in healthy premeno-
for depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 797–809. (11) pausal women. Journal of Sex Medicine, 6, 739–
354, 1243–1252. (15) Udry, J. R., & Morris, N. M. (1968). Distribution of 751. (11)
Trivers, R. L. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park, coitus in the menstrual cycle. Nature, 220, 593– van Anders, S. M., & Goldey, K. L. (2010). Testoster-
CA: Benjamin/Cummings. (1) 596. (11) one and parnering are linked via relationship sta-
Trudel, E., & Bourque, C. W. (2010). Central clock Uekita, T., & Okaichi, H. (2005). NMDA antago- tus for women and “relationship orientation” for
excites vasopressin neurons by waking osmosen- nist MK-801 does not interfere with the use of men. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 820–826. (11)
sory afferents during late sleep. Nature Neurosci- spatial representation in a familiar environment. van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., Schmidt, N., &
ence, 13, 467–474. (10) Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 548–556. (13) Watson N. V. (2007). Associations between tes-
540 References

tosterone secretin and sexual activity in women. van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., & Gage, F. H. Vessal, M., & Darian-Smith, C. (2010). Adult neu-
Hormones and Behavior, 51, 477–482. (11) (1999). Running increases cell proliferation and rogenesis occurs in primate sensorimotor cortex
van Anders, S. M., Hamilton, L. D., & Watson, neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. following cervical dorsal rhizotomy. Journal of
N. V. (2007). Multiple partners are associated Nature Neuroscience, 2, 266–270. (5) Neuroscience, 30, 8613–8623. (5)
with higher testosterone in North American van Praag, H., Schinder, A. F., Christie, B. R., Toni, Vetencourt, J. F. M., Sale, A., Viegi, A., Baroncelli,
men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 51, N., Palmer, T. D., & Gage, F. H. (2002). Func- L., DePasquale, R., O’Leary, O. F., et al. (2008).
454–459. (11) tional neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plastici-
van Anders, S. M., & Watson, N. V. (2006). Rela- Nature, 415, 1030–1034. (5) ty in the adult visual cortex. Science, 320, 385–
tionship status and testosterone in North Ameri- Van Wanrooij, M. M., & Van Opstal, A. J. (2004). 388. (15)
can heterosexual and non-heterosexual men and Contribution of head shadow and pinna cues to Virkkunen, M., DeJong, J., Bartko, J., Goodwin,
women: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data. chronic monaural sound localization. Journal of F. K., & Linnoila, M. (1989). Relationship of
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 715–723. (11) Neuroscience, 24, 4163–4171. (7) psychobiological variables to recidivism in violent
Van Cantfort, T. E., Gardner, B. T., & Gardner, Van Wanrooij, M. M., & Van Opstal, A. J. (2005). offenders and impulsive fire setters. Archives of
R. A. (1989). Developmental trends in replies to Relearning sound localization with a new ear. General Psychiatry, 46, 600–603. (12)
Wh- questions by children and chimpanzees. In Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5413–5424. (7) Virkkunen, M., Eggert, M., Rawlings, R., & Lin-
R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner, & T. E. Van Van Zoeren, J. G., & Stricker, E. M. (1977). Effects noila, M. (1996). A prospective follow-up study
Cantfort (Eds.), Teaching sign language to chim- of preoptic, lateral hypothalamic, or dopamine- of alcoholic violent offenders and fire setters. Ar-
panzees (pp. 198–239). Albany: State University depleting lesions on behavioral thermoregulation chives of General Psychiatry, 53, 523–529. (12)
of New York Press. (14) in rats exposed to the cold. Journal of Comparative Virkkunen, M., Nuutila, A., Goodwin, F. K., & Lin-
van den Heuvel, M. P., Mandl, R. C. W., Stam, C. J., and Physiological Psychology, 91, 989–999. (10) noila, M. (1987). Cerebrospinal fluid mono-
Kahn, R. S., & Pol, H. E. H. (2010). Aberrant Vanduffel, W., Fize, D., Mandeville, J. B., Nelissen, amine metabolite levels in male arsonists. Ar-
frontal and temporal complex network structure K., Van Hecke, P., Rosen, B. R., et al. (2001). Vi- chives of General Psychiatry, 44, 241–247. (12)
in schizophrenia: A graph theoretical analysis. sual motion processing investigated using con- Virkkunen, M., Rawlings, R., Tokola, R., Poland,
Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 15915–15926. (15) trast agent-enhanced fMRI in awake behaving R. E., Guidotti, A., Nemeroff, C., et al. (1994).
van den Pol, A. N. (1999). Hypothalamic hypo- monkeys. Neuron, 32, 565–577. (6) CSF biochemistries, glucose metabolism, and di-
cretin (orexin): Robust innervation of the spi- Vargas-Irwin, C. E., Shakhnarovich, G., Yadollah- urnal activity rhythms in alcoholic, violent of-
nal cord. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 3171– pour, P., Mislow, J. M. K., Black, M. J., & Dono- fenders, fire setters, and healthy volunteers. Ar-
3182. (10) ghue, J. P. (2010). Decoding complete reach and chives of General Psychiatry, 51, 20–27. (3)
Van der Borght, K., Havekes, R., Bos, T., Eggen, grasp actions from local primary motor cortex Visser, E. K., Beersma, G. M., & Daan, S. (1999).
B. J. L., & Van der Zee, E. A. (2007). Exercise populations. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 9659– Melatonin suppression by light in humans is
improves memory acquisition and retrieval in the 9669. (8) maximal when the nasal part of the retina is illu-
Y-maze task: Relationship with hippocampal Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Copp, A., & minated. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 14, 116–
neurogenesis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 324– Mishkin, M. (2005). FOXPx and the neuroanat- 121. (9)
334. (5) omy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Viswanathan, A., & Freeman, R. D. (2007). Neuro-
Van der Does, A. J. W. (2001). The effects of tryp- Neuroscience, 6, 131–138. (14) metabolic coupling in cerebral cortex reflects syn-
tophan depletion on mood and psychiatric Vasey, P. L., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2010). An adap- aptic more than spiking activity. Nature Neurosci-
symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 64, tive cognitive dissociation between willingness to ence, 10, 1308–1312. (4)
107–119. (12) help kin and nonkin in Samoan Fa’afafine. Psycho- Vliegen, J., Van Grootel, T. J., & Van Opstal, A. J.
van der Vegt, B. J., Lieuwes, N., van de Wall, E. H. E. logical Science, 21, 292–297. (11) (2004). Dynamic sound localization during rap-
M., Kato, K., Moya-Albiol, L., Martínez- Vawter, M. P., Evans, S., Choudary, P., Tomita, H., id eye–head gaze shifts. Journal of Neuroscience,
Sanchis, W., et al. (2003). Activation of seroto- Meador-Woodruff, J., Molnar, M., et al. (2004). 24, 9291–9302. (7)
nergic neurotransmission during the perfor- Gender-specific gene expression in post-mortem Vocci, F. J., Acri, J., & Elkashef, A. (2005). Medica-
mance of aggressive behavior in rats. Behavioral human brain. Localization to sex chromosomes. tion development for addictive disorders: The
Neuroscience, 117, 667–674. (12) Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 373–384. (11) state of the science. American Journal of Psychia-
Van Essen, D. C., & DeYoe, E. A. (1995). Concur- Velanova, K., Wheeler, M. E., & Luna, B. (2009). try, 162, 1432–1440. (3)
rent processing in the primate visual cortex. In The maturation of task set-related activation Volavka, J., Czobor, P., Goodwin, D. W., Gabrielli,
M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosci- supports late developmental improvements in W. F., Penick, E. C., Mednick, S. A., et al. (1996).
ences (pp. 383–400). Cambridge, MA: MIT inhibitory control. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, The electroencephalogram after alcohol adminis-
Press. (6) 12558–12567. (8) tration in high-risk men and the development of
van Honk, J., Harmon-Jones, E., Morgan, B. E., & Velliste, M., Perel, S., Spalding, M. C., Whitford, alcohol use disorders 10 years later. Archives of
Schutter, D. J. L. G. (2010). Socially explosive A. S., & Schwartz, A. B. (2008). Cortical control General Psychiatry, 53, 258–263. (3)
minds: The triple imbalance hypothesis of reactive of a prosthetic arm for self-feeding. Nature, 453, Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G.-J., Telang, F.,
aggression. Journal of Personality, 78, 69–94. (12) 1098–1101. (8) Logan, J., Jayne, M., et al. (2010). Cognitive con-
van Honk, J., & Schutter, D. J. L. G. (2007). Testos- Venna, V. R., Deplanque, D., Allet, C., Belarbi, K., trol of drug craving inhibits brain reward re-
terone reduces conscious detection of signals Hamdane, M., & Bordet, R. (2008). PUFA in- gions in cocaine abusers. NeuroImage, 49, 2536–
serving social correction. Psychological Science, 18, duce antidepressant-like effects in parallel to 2543. (3)
663–667. (12) structural and molecular changes in the hippo- Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Telang, F., Fowler, J. S.,
van Leeuwen, M., Peper, J. S., van den Berg, S. M., campus. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 199– Logan, J., Childress, A.-R., et al. (2006). Cocaine
Brouwer, R. M., Pol, H. E. H., Kahn, R. S., et al. 211. (15) cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: Mecha-
(2009). A genetic analysis of brain volumes and Verhage, M., Maia, A. S., Plomp, J. J., Brussard, nism of craving in cocaine addiction. Journal of
IQ in children. Intelligence, 37, 181–191. (4) A. B., Heeroma, J. H., Vermeer, H., et al. (2000). Neuroscience, 26, 6583–6588. (3)
van Meer, M. P. A., van der Marel, K., Wang, K., Synaptic assembly of the brain in the absence of von Békésy, G. (1956). Current status of theories of
Otte, W. M., el Bouzati, S., Roeling, T. A. P., et al. neurotransmitter secretion. Science, 287, 864– hearing. Science, 123, 779–783. (7)
(2010). Recovery of sensorimotor function after 869. (5) von der Ohe, C. G., Darian-Smith, C., Garner, C. C., &
experimental stroke correlates with restoration Verrey, F., & Beron, J. (1996). Activation and supply Heller, H. C. (2006). Ubiquitous and temperature-
of resting-state interhemispheric functional ac- of channels and pumps by aldosterone. News in dependent neural plasticity in hibernators. Journal of
tivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3964–3972. (5) Physiological Sciences, 11, 126–133. (10) Neuroscience, 26, 10590–10598. (9)
References 541

von Gall, C., Garabette, M. L., Kell, C. A., Frenzel, structures during language production in normal tial of male rats prenatally exposed to the influ-
S., Dehghani, F., Schumm-Draeger, P. M., et al. man. Brain and Language, 25, 224–233. (14) ence of alcohol, stress, or both factors. Behavioral
(2002). Rhythmic gene expression in pituitary Wallman, J., & Pettigrew, J. D. (1985). Conjugate Neuroscience, 108, 1188–1195. (11)
depends on heterologous sensitization by the and disjunctive saccades in two avian species Ward, I. L., & Ward, O. B. (1985). Sexual behavior
neurohormone melatonin. Nature Neuroscience, with contrasting oculomotor strategies. Journal of differentiation: Effects of prenatal manipulations
5, 234–238. (9) Neuroscience, 5, 1418–1428. (6) in rats. In N. Adler, D. Pfaff, & R. W. Goy (Eds.),
von Melchner, L., Pallas, S. L., & Sur, M. (2000). Walsh, T., McClellan, J. M., McCarthy, S. E., Add- Handbook of behavioral neurobiology (Vol. 7, pp.
Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections ington, A. M., Pierce, S. B., & Cooper, G. M. 77–98). New York: Plenum Press. (11)
directed to the visual pathway. Nature, 404, 871– (2008). Rare structural variants disrupt multiple Ward, O. B., Monaghan, E. P., & Ward, I. L. (1986).
876. (5) genes in neurodevelopmental pathways in Naltrexone blocks the effects of prenatal stress
Vrba, E. S. (1998). Multiphasic growth models and schizophrenia. Science, 320, 539–543. (15) on sexual behavior differentiation in male rats.
the evolution of prolonged growth exemplified by Walsh, V., & Cowey, A. (2000). Transcranial mag- Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 25,
human brain evolution. Journal of Theoretical Bi- netic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience. Na- 573–576. (11)
ology, 190, 227–239. (5) ture Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 73–79. (4) Ward, O. B., Ward, I. L., Denning, J. H., French,
Vuga, M., Fox, N. A., Cohn, J. F., George, C. J., Lev- Walters, E. T., (2009). Chronic pain, memory, and J. A., & Hendricks, S. E. (2002). Postparturi-
enstein, R. M., & Kovacs, M. (2006). Long-term injury: Evolutionary clues from snail and rat no- tional testosterone surge in male offspring of rats
stability of frontal electroencephalographic ciceptors. International Journal of Comparative stressed and/or fed ethanol during late pregnan-
asymmetry in adults with a history of depression Psychology, 22, 127–140. (7) cy. Hormones and Behavior, 41, 229–235. (11)
and controls. International Journal of Psychophysi- Wan, C. Y., Wood, A. G., Reutens, D. C., & Wilson, Ward, R., Danziger, S., Owen, V., & Rafal, R.
ology, 59, 107–115. (15) S. J. (2010). Early but not late-blindness leads to (2002). Deficits in spatial coding and feature
Vuillermot, S., Weber, L., Feldon, J., & Meyer, U. enhanced auditory perception. Neuropsychologia, binding following damage to spatiotopic maps in
(2010). A longitudinal examination of the neu- 48, 344–348. (5) the human pulvinar. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 99–
rodevelopmental impact of prenatal immune ac- Wang, A., Liang, Y., Fridell, R. A., Probst, F. J., Wil- 100. (4)
tivation in mice reveals primary defects in dopa- cox, E. R., Touchman, J. W., et al. (1998). Asso- Warren, R. M. (1999). Auditory perception. Cam-
minergic development relevant to schizophrenia. ciations of unconventional myosin MYO15 mu- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. (7)
Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1270–1287. (15) tations with human nonsyndromic deafness Wässle, H. (2004). Parallel processing in the mamma-
Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Cognitive science: Staring DFNB3. Science, 280, 1447–1451. (7) lian retina. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 747–757. (6)
fear in the face. Nature, 433, 22–23. (12) Wang, A. C. J., Hara, Y., Janssen, W. G. M., Rapp, Watanabe, D., Savion-Lemieux, T., & Penhune,
Vyazovskiy, V. V., Cirelli, C., Pfister-Genskow, M., P. R., & Morrison, J. H. (2010). Synaptic estro- V. B. (2007). The effect of early musical training
Faraguna, U., & Tononi, G. (2008). Molecular gen receptor-a levels in prefrontal cortex in fe- on adult motor performance: Evidence for a sen-
and electrophysiological evidence for net synap- male rhesus monkeys and their correlation with sitive period in motor learning. Experimental
tic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. cognitive performance. Journal of Neuroscience, Brain Research, 176, 332–340. (5)
Nature Neuroscience, 11, 200–208. (9) 30, 12770–12776. (11) Watanabe, M., & Munoz, D. P. (2010). Presetting
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokolik, Wang, D. O., Kim, S. M., Zhao, Y., Hwang, H., basal ganglia for volitional actions. Journal of
A., Casey, K. L., Davidson, R. J., et al. (2004). Miura, S. K., Sossin, W. S., et al. (2009). Syn- Neuroscience, 30, 10144–10157. (8)
Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the antici- apse- and stimulus-specific local translation dur- Wattendorf, E., Welge-Lüssen, A., Fiedler, K., Bile-
pation and experience of pain. Science, 303, ing long-term neuronal plasticity. Science, 324, cen, D., Wolfensberger, M., Fuhr, P., et al. (2009).
1162–1167. (7) 1536–1540. (13) Olfactory impairment predicts brain atrophy in
Wager, T. D., Scott, D. J., & Zubieta, J.-K. (2007). Wang, Q., Schoenlein, R. W., Peteanu, L. A., Ma- Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
Placebo effects on human m-opioid activity dur- thies, R. A., & Shank, C. V. (1994). Vibrationally 15410–15413. (8)
ing pain. Proceedings of the National Academy of coherent photochemistry in the femtosecond pri- Waxman, S. G., & Ritchie, J. M. (1985). Organiza-
Sciences, USA, 104, 11056–11061. (7) mary event of vision. Science, 266, 422–424. (6) tion of ion channels in the myelinated nerve fiber.
Wagner, E. L., & Gleeson, T. T. (1997). Postexercise Wang, T., Okano, Y., Eisensmith, R., Huang, S. Z., Science, 228, 1502–1507. (2)
thermoregulatory behavior and recovery from Zeng, Y. T., Wilson, H. Y. L., et al. (1989). Mo- Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A.,
exercise in desert iguanas. Physiology & Behavior, lecular genetics of phenylketonuria in Orientals: D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al.
61, 175–180. (10) Linkage disequilibrium between a termination (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal be-
Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & mutation and haplotype 4 of the phenylalanine havior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847–854. (1)
Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight. Nature, hydroxylase gene. American Journal of Human Webb, W. B. (1974). Sleep as an adaptive response.
427, 352–355. (9) Genetics, 45, 675–680. (1) Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 1023–1027. (9)
Waisbren, S. R., Brown, M. J., de Sonneville, L. M. J., Wang, X., Lu, T., Snider, R. K., & Liang, L. (2005). Weber-Fox, C. M., & Neville, H. J. (1996). Matura-
& Levy, H. L. (1994). Review of neuropsycho- Sustained firing in auditory cortex evoked by tional constraints on functional specializations
logical functioning in treated phenylketonuria: preferred stimuli. Nature, 435, 341–346. (7) for language processing: ERP and behavioral evi-
An information-processing approach. Acta Paedi- Warach, S. (1995). Mapping brain pathophysiology dence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive
atrica, 83(Suppl. 407), 98–103. (1) and higher cortical function with magnetic reso- Neuroscience, 8, 231–256. (14)
Waldherr, M., & Neumann, I. D. (2007). Centrally nance imaging. The Neuroscientist, 1, 221–235. (4) Wegener, D., Freiwald, W. A., & Kreiter, A. K.
released oxytocin mediates mating-induced Ward, I. L., Bennett, A. L., Ward, O. B., Hendricks, (2004). The influence of sustained selective atten-
anxiolysis in male rats. Proceedings of the Na- S. E., & French, J. A. (1999). Androgen threshold tion on stimulus selectivity in macaque visual area
tional Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 16681– to activate copulation differs in male rats prena- MT. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 6106–6114. (14)
16684. (11) tally exposed to alcohol, stress, or both factors. Wehr, T. A., Turner, E. H., Shimada, J. M., Lowe,
Waldvogel, J. A. (1990). The bird’s eye view. Ameri- Hormones and Behavior, 36, 129–140. (11) C. H., Barker, C., & Leinbenluft, E. (1998).
can Scientist, 78, 342–353. (6) Ward, I. L., Romeo, R. D., Denning, J. H., & Treatment of a rapidly cycling bipolar patient by
Wallen, K. (2005). Hormonal influences on sexually Ward, O. B. (1999). Fetal alcohol exposure using extended bed rest and darkness to stabilize
differentiated behavior in nonhuman primates. blocks full masculinization of the dorsolateral the timing and duration of sleep. Biological Psy-
Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 26, 7–26. (11) nucleus in rat spinal cord. Physiology & Behav- chiatry, 43, 822–828. (15)
Wallesch, C.-W., Henriksen, L., Kornhuber, H. H., ior, 66, 571–575. (11) Wei, W., Nguyen, L. N., Kessels, H. W., Hagiwara,
& Paulson, O. B. (1985). Observations on regional Ward, I. L., Ward, B., Winn, R. J., & Bielawski, D. H., Sisodia, S., & Malinow, R. (2010). Amyloid
cerebral blood flow in cortical and subcortical (1994). Male and female sexual behavior poten- beta from axons and dendrites reduces local
542 References

spine number and plasticity. Nature Neuroscience, for predicting significant life history outcomes in Williams, G., Cai, X. J., Elliott, J. C., & Harrold,
13, 190–196. (13) female specific-pathogen-free (SPF) rhesus ma- J. A. (2004). Anabolic neuropeptides. Physiology
Weidensaul, S. (1999). Living on the wind. New caques (Macaca mulatta) maintained in captive & Behavior, 81, 211–222. (10)
York: North Point Press. (10) breeding groups. Journal of Medical Primatology, Williams, M. T., Davis, H. N., McCrea, A. E., Long,
Weinberger, D. R. (1996). On the plausibility of “the 32, 95–104. (12) S. J., & Hennessy, M. B. (1999). Changes in the
neurodevelopmental hypothesis” of schizophre- Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., hormonal concentrations of pregnant rats and
nia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 14, 1S–11S. (15) Kim, H., Polis, S., et al. (2004). Human amyg- their fetuses following multiple exposures to a
Weinberger, D. R. (1999). Cell biology of the hip- dala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. stressor during the third trimester. Neurotoxicol-
pocampal formation in schizophrenia. Biological Science, 306, 2061. (12) ogy and Teratology, 21, 403–414. (11)
Psychiatry, 45, 395–402. (15) Wheeler, M. E., & Treisman, A. (2002). Binding in Williams, R. W., & Herrup, K. (1988). The control
Weindl, A. (1973). Neuroendocrine aspects of cir- short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimen- of neuron number. Annual Review of Neurosci-
cumventricular organs. In W. F. Ganong & L. tal Psychology: General, 131, 48–64. (4) ence, 11, 423–453. (2, 8)
Martini (Eds.), Frontiers in neuroendocrinology White, L. E., Coppola, D. M., & Fitzpatrick, D. Willingham, D. B., Koroshetz, W. J., & Peterson,
1973 (pp. 3–32). New York: Oxford University (2001). The contribution of sensory experience E. W. (1996). Motor skills have diverse neural bas-
Press. (10) to the maturation of orientation selectivity in es: Spared and impaired skill acquisition in Hun-
Weinshenker, D., & Schroeder, J. P. (2007). There ferret visual cortex. Nature, 411, 1049–1052. tington’s disease. Neuropsychology, 10, 315–321. (8)
and back again: A tale of norepinephrine and (6) Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A. D., & Kapur, N. (1995).
drug addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, Whitman, B. W., & Packer, R. J. (1993). The photic Dense amnesia in a professional musician follow-
1433–1451. (3) sneeze reflex: Literature review and discussion. ing herpes simplex virus encephalitis. Journal of
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., Neurology, 43, 868–871. (8) Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17,
& Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the Whyte, J., Katz, D., Long, D., DiPasquale, M. C., 668–681. (13)
hemianopic field following a restricted occipital Polansky, M., Kalmar, K., et al. (2005). Predic- Wilson, J. D., George, F. W., & Griffin, J. E. (1981).
ablation. Brain, 97, 709–728. (6) tors of outcome in prolonged posttraumatic dis- The hormonal control of sexual development.
Weiss, A. P., Ellis, C. B., Roffman, J. L., Stufflebeam, orders of consciousness and assessment of medi- Science, 211, 1278–1284. (11)
S., Hamalainen, M. S., Duff, M., et al. (2009). cation effects: A multicenter study. Archives of Wilson, R. I., & Nicoll, R. A. (2002). Endocannabi-
Aberrant frontoparietal function during recogni- Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, 453– noid signaling in the brain. Science, 296, 678–
tion memory in schizophrenia: A multimodal 462. (5) 682. (3)
neuroimaging investigation. Journal of Neurosci- Wichmann, T., Vitek, J. L., & DeLong, M. R. Wilson, S. M., Dronkers, N. F., Ogar, J. M., Jang, J.,
ence, 29, 11347–11359. (15) (1995). Parkinson’s disease and the basal ganglia: Growdon, M. E., Agosta, F., et al. (2010). Neural
Weiss, P. (1924). Die funktion transplantierter am- Lessons from the laboratory and from neurosur- correlates of syntactic processing in the nonflu-
phibienextremitäten. Aufstellung einer resonan- gery. The Neuroscientist, 1, 236–244. (8) ent variant of primary progressive aphasia. Jour-
ztheorie der motorischen nerventätigkeit auf Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gal- nal of Neuroscience, 30, 16845–16854. (14)
grund abstimmter endorgane [The function of lese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us dis- Winer, G. A., Cottrell, J. F., Gregg, V., Fournier, J. S., &
transplanted amphibian limbs. Presentation of a gusted in my insula: The common neural basis of Bica. L. A. (2002). Fundamentally misunderstand-
resonance theory of motor nerve action upon seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664. ing visual perception: Adults’ belief in visual emis-
tuned end organs]. Archiv für Mikroskopische (8, 12) sions. American Psychologist, 57, 417–424. (2)
Anatomie und Entwicklungsmechanik, 102, 635– Widom, C. S., & Brzustowicz, L. M. (2006). MAOI Winfree, A. T. (1983). Impact of a circadian clock
672. (5) and the “cycle of violence”: Childhood abuse and on the timing of human sleep. American Journal
Weiss, P. H., & Fink, G. R. (2009). Grapheme- neglect, MAOA genotype, and risk of violent and of Physiology, 245, R497–R504. (9)
colour synaesthetes show increased grey matter antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 60, Winocur, G., & Hasher, L. (1999). Aging and time-
volumes of parietal and fusiform cortex. Brain, 684–689. (12) of-day effects on cognition in rats. Behavioral
132, 65–70. (7) Wiesel, T. N. (1982). Postnatal development of the Neuroscience, 113, 991–997. (9)
Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., visual cortex and the influence of environment. Winocur, G., & Hasher, L. (2004). Age and time-
& Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of Nature, 299, 583–591. (6) of-day effects on learning and memory in a non-
momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neurosci- Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1963). Single-cell matching-to-sample test. Neurobiology of Aging,
ence, 9, 971–978. (4) responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of 25, 1107–1115. (9)
Welchman, A. E., Stanley, J., Schomers, M. R., Miall, vision in one eye. Journal of Neurophysiology, 26, Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M., & Sekeres, M.
C., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2010). The quick and the 1003–1017. (6) (2007). Memory consolidation or transforma-
dead: When reaction beats intention. Proceedings Wild, H. M., Butler, S. R., Carden, D., & Kulikow­ tion: Context manipulation and hippocampal
of the Royal Society, B, 277, 1667–1674. (8) ski, J. J. (1985). Primate cortical area V4 impor- representations of memory. Nature Neuroscience,
Weller, L., Weller, A., Koresh-Kamin, H., & Ben- tant for colour constancy but not wavelength 10, 555–557. (13)
Shoshan, R. (1999). Menstrual synchrony in a discrimination. Nature, 313, 133–135. (6) Wirdefeldt, K., Gatz, M., Pawitan, Y., & Pedersen,
sample of working women. Psychoneuroendocri- Wilensky, A. E., Schafe, G. E., Kristensen, M. P., & N. L. (2005). Risk and protective factors for Par-
nology, 24, 449–459. (7) LeDoux, J. E. (2006). Rethinking the fear circuit. kinson’s disease: A study in Swedish twins. An-
Weller, L., Weller, A., & Roizman, S. (1999). Hu- Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12387–12396. (12) nals of Neurology, 57, 27–33. (8)
man menstrual synchrony in families and among Willems, R. M., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. Wirz-Justice, A., Werth, E., Renz, C., Müller, S., &
close friends: Examining the importance of mu- (2010). Body-specific representations of action Kräuchi, K. (2002). No evidence for a phase de-
tual exposure. Journal of Comparative Psychology, verbs: Neural evidence from right- and left- lay in human circadian rhythms after a single
113, 261–268. (7) handers. Psychological Science, 21, 67–74. (14) morning melatonin administration. Journal of Pi-
Wessinger, C. M., VanMeter, J., Tian, B., Van Lare, Willerman, L., Schultz, R., Rutledge, J. N., & Bigler, neal Research, 32, 1–5. (9)
J., Pekar, J., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2001). Hierar- E. D. (1991). In vivo brain size and intelligence. Wise, R. A. (1996). Addictive drugs and brain stim-
chical organization of the human auditory cortex Intelligence, 15, 223–228. (4) ulation reward. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
revealed by functional magnetic resonance imag- Williams, C. L. (1986). A reevaluation of the con- 19, 319–340. (3)
ing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 1–7. (7) cept of separable periods of organizational and Wissman, A. M., & Brenowitz, E. A. (2009). The
Westergaard, G. C., Cleveland, A., Trenkle, M. K., activational actions of estrogens in development role of neurotrophins in the seasonal-like growth
Lussier, I. D., & Higley, J. D. (2003). CSF of brain and behavior. Annals of the New York of the avian song control system. Journal of Neu-
5-HIAA concentration as an early screening tool Academy of Sciences, 474, 282–292. (11) roscience, 29, 6461–6471. (5)
References 543

Witelson, S. F., Beresh, H., & Kigar, D. L. (2006). sonality measures. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 468– ulating food intake and energy balance. Journal of
Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem 476. (12, 15) Neuroscience, 29, 179–190. (10)
brains: Sex, lateralization and age factors. Brain, Wright, I. C., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Woodruff, P. W. R., Yavich, L., Forsberg, M. M., Karayiorgou, M., Go-
129, 386–398. (4) David, A. S., Murray, R. M., & Bullmore, E. T. gos, J. A., & Männistö, P. T. (2007). Site-specific
Witelson, S. F., Kigar, D. L., & Harvey, T. (1999). (2000). Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes role of catechol-o-methyltransferase in dopa-
The exceptional brain of Albert Einstein. Lancet, in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, mine overflow within prefrontal cortex and dor-
353, 2149–2153. (4) 157, 16–25. (15) sal striatum. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10196–
Witelson, S. F., & Pallie, W. (1973). Left hemi- Wu, S. S., & Frucht, S. J. (2005). Treatment of Par- 10202. (3)
sphere specialization for language in the new- kinson’s disease: What’s on the horizon? CNS Yee, B. K., Balic, E., Singer, P., Schwerdel, C.,
born: Neuroanatomical evidence of asymmetry. Drugs, 19, 723–743. (8) Grampp, T., Gabernet, L., et al. (2006). Disrup-
Brain, 96, 641–646. (14) Wulfeck, B., & Bates, E. (1991). Differential sensi- tion of glycine transporter 1 restricted to fore-
Wolf, S. (1995). Dogmas that have hindered under- tivity to errors of agreement and word order in brain neurons is associated with a precognitive
standing. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, and antipsychotic phenotypic profile. Journal of
Science, 30, 3–4. (12) 3, 258–272. (14) Neuroscience, 26, 3169–3181. (15)
Wolff, P. H. (1993). Impaired temporal resolution Wulff, K., Gatti, S., Wettstein, J. G., & Foster, R. G. Yeh, M. T., Coccaro, E. F., & Jacobson, K. C. (2010).
in developmental dyslexia. Annals of the New (2010). Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in Multivariate behavior genetic analyses of aggres-
York Academy of Sciences, 682, 87–103. (14) psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Na- sive behavior subtypes. Behavior Genetics, 40,
Wolkin, A., Rusinek, H., Vaid, G., Arena, L., Lafar- ture Neuroscience, 11, 589–599. (9) 603–617. (12)
gue, T., Sanfilipo, M., et al. (1998). Structural Wurtman, J. J. (1985). Neurotransmitter control of Yehuda, R. (1997). Sensitization of the hypothalamic-
magnetic resonance image averaging in schizo- carbohydrate consumption. Annals of the New pituitary-adrenal axis in posttraumatic stress disor-
phrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, York Academy of Sciences, 443, 145–151. (3) der. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
1064–1073. (15) Wyart, C., Webster, W. W., Chen, J. H., Wilson, 821, 57–75. (12)
Wolpert, L. (1991). The triumph of the embryo. Ox- S. R., McClary, A., Khan, R. M., et al. (2007). Yehuda, R. (2002). Post-traumatic stress disorder.
ford, England: Oxford University Press. (5) Smelling a single component of male sweat alters New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 108–
Womelsdorf, T., Schoffelen, J.-M., Oostenveld, R., levels of cortisol in women. Journal of Neurosci- 114. (12)
Singer, W., Desimone, R., Engel, A. K., et al. ence, 27, 1261–1265. (7) Yeomans, J. S., & Frankland, P. W. (1996). The
(2007). Modulation of neuronal interactions Wynne, C. D. L. (2004). The perils of anthropo- acoustic startle reflex: Neurons and connections.
through neuronal synchronization. Science, 316, morphism. Nature, 428, 606. (1) Brain Research Reviews, 21, 301–314. (12)
1609–1612. (14) Wynne, L. C., Tienari, P., Nieminen, P., Sorri, A., Yeshurun, Y., Dudai, Y., & Sobel, N. (2008). Work-
Wong, P. C. M., Skoe, E., Russo, N. M., Dees, T., Lahti, I., Moring, J., et al. (2006). Genotype- ing memory across nostrils. Behavioral Neurosci-
& Kraus, N. (2007). Musical experience environment interaction in the schizophrenia ence, 122, 1031–1037. (14)
shapes human brainstem encoding of linguis- spectrum: Genetic liability and global family rat- Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of
tic pitch perception. Nature Neuroscience, 10, ings in the Finnish adoption study. Family Pro- the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Re-
420–422. (5) cess, 45, 419–434. (15) views Neuroscience, 7, 464–476. (8)
Wong, P. C. M., Warrier, C. M., Penhune, V. B., Roy, Xu, H.-T., Pan, F., Yang, G., & Gan, W.-B. (2007). Yiu, G., & He, Z. (2006). Glial inhibition of CNS
A. K., Sadehh, A., Parrish, T. B., et al. (2007). Choice of cranial window type for in vivo imag- axon regeneration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
Volume of left Heschl’s gyrus and linguistic pitch ing affects dendritic spine turnover in the cortex. 7, 617–627. (5)
learning. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 828–836. (4) Nature Neuroscience, 10, 549–551. (5) Yolken, R. H., Bachmann, S., Rouslanova, I., Lille-
Wong-Riley, M. T. T. (1989). Cytochrome oxi- Xu, T., Yu, X., Perlik, A. J., Tobin, W. F., Zweig, J. hoj, E., Ford, G., Torrey, E. F., et al. (2001). Anti-
dase: An endogenous metabolic marker for A., Tennant, K., et al. (2009). Rapid formation bodies to Toxoplasma gondii in individuals with
neuronal activity. Trends in Neurosciences, 12, and selective stabilization of synapses for en- first-episode schizophrenia. Clinical Infectious
94–101. (2) during motor memories. Nature, 462, 915– Diseases, 32, 842–844. (15)
Woodberry, K. A., Giuliano, A. J., & Seidman, L. J. 919. (13) Yoo, S.-S., Hu, P. T., Gujar, N., Jolesz, F. A., &
(2008). Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: A meta- Xu, Y., Padiath, Q. S., Shapiro, R. E., Jones, C. R., Walker, M. P. (2007). A deficit in the ability to
analytic review. American Journal of Psychiatry, Wu, S. C., Saigoh, N., et al. (2005). Functional form new human memories without sleep. Na-
165, 579–587. (15) consequences of a CKxd mutation causing famil- ture Neuroscience, 10, 385–392. (9)
Wooding, S., Kim, U., Bamshad, M J., Larsen, J., ial advanced sleep phase syndrome. Nature, 434, Yoon, K. L., Hong, S. W., Joormann, J., & Kang, P.
Jorde, L. B., & Drayna, D. (2004). Natural selec- 640–644. (9) (2009). Perception of facial expressions of emo-
tion and molecular evolution in PTC, a bitter- Yamaguchi, S., Isejima, H., Matsuo, T., Okura, R., tion during binocular rivalry. Emotion, 9, 172–
taste receptor gene. American Journal of Human Yagita, K., Kobayashi, M., et al. (2003). Synchro- 182. (14)
Genetics, 74, 637–646. (1) nization of cellular clocks in the suprachiasmatic Yoshida, J., & Mori, K. (2007). Odorant category
Woodson, J. C., & Balleine, B. W. (2002). An as- nucleus. Science, 302, 1408–1412. (9) profile selectivity of olfactory cortex neurons.
sessment of factors contributing to instrumental Yamamoto, S., & Kitazawa, S. (2001). Reversal of Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9105–9114. (7)
performance for sexual reward in the rat. Quar- subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Yoshida, K., Li, X., Cano, G., Lazarus, M., & Saper,
terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55B, Nature Neuroscience, 4, 759–765. (2) C. B. (2009). Parallel preoptic pathways for ther-
75–88. (11) Yamamoto, T. (1984). Taste responses of cortical neu- moregulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 29,
Woodworth, R. S. (1934). Psychology (3rd ed.). rons. Progress in Neurobiology, 23, 273–315. (7) 11954–11964. (10)
New York: Holt. (2) Yanagisawa, K., Bartoshuk, L. M., Catalanotto, Young, A. B. (1995). Huntington’s disease: Lessons
Woolf, N. J. (1996). Global and serial neurons form F. A., Karrer, T. A., & Kveton, J. F. (1998). Anes- from and for molecular neuroscience. The Neuro-
a hierarchically arranged interface proposed to thesia of the chorda tympani nerve and taste phan- scientist, 1, 51–58. (8)
underlie memory and cognition. Neuroscience, toms. Physiology & Behavior, 63, 329–335. (7) Young, S. N., & Leyton, M. (2002). The role of sero-
74, 625–651. (9) Yang, G., Pan, F., & Gan, W.-B. (2009). Stably tonin in human mood and social interaction: In-
Wray, N. R., James, M. R., Gordon, S. D., Dumenil, maintained dendritic spines are associated with sight from altered tryptophan levels. Pharmacol­
T., Ryan, L., Coventry, W. L., et al. (2009). Ac- lifelong memories. Nature, 462, 920–924. (5) ogy, Biochemistry and Behavior, 71, 857–865. (12)
curate, large-scale genotyping of 5HTTLPR and Yang, L., Scott, K. A., Hyun, J., Tamashiro, K. L., Yousem, D. M., Maldjian, J. A., Siddiqi, F., Hummel,
flanking single nucleotide polymorphisms in an Tray, N., Moran, T. H., et al. (2009). Role of dor- T., Alsop, D. C., Geckle, R. J., et al. (1999). Gen-
association study of depression, anxiety, and per- somedial hypothalamic neuropeptide Y in mod- der effects on odor-stimulated functional mag-
544 References

netic resonance imaging. Brain Research, 818, Zhang, X., & Firestein, S. (2002). The olfactory re- Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading ac-
480–487. (7) ceptor gene superfamily of the mouse. Nature quisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled
Yu, T. W., & Bargmann, C. I. (2001). Dynamic reg- Neuroscience, 5, 124–133. (7) reading across languages: A psycholinguistic
ulation of axon guidance. Nature Neuroscience Zhang, X., Smith, D. L., Meriin, A. B., Engemann, grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131,
Supplement, 4, 1169–1176. (5) S., Russel, D. E., Roark, M., et al. (2005). A po- 3–29. (14)
Zadra, A., & Pilon, M. (2008). Polysomnographic tent small molecule inhibits polyglutamine ag- Zihl, J., von Cramon, D., & Mai, N. (1983). Selec-
diagnosis of sleepwalking: Effects of sleep depri- gregation in Huntington’s disease neurons and tive disturbance of movement vision after bilat-
vation. Annals of Neurology, 63, 513–519. (9) suppresses neurodegeneration in vivo. Proceed- eral brain damage. Brain, 106, 313–340. (6)
Zakharenko, S. S., Zablow, L., & Siegelbaum, S. A. ings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, Zipser, B. D., Johanson, C. E., Gonzalez, L., Berzin,
(2001). Visualization of changes in presynaptic 102, 892–897. (8) T. M., Tavares, R., Hulette, C. M., et al. (2006).
function during long-term synaptic plasticity. Zhang, Y., Proenca, R., Maffei, M., Barone, M., Leo- Microvascular injury and blood–brain barrier
Nature Neuroscience, 4, 711–717. (13) pold, L., & Friedman, J. M. (1994). Positional leakage in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of
Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., & Belin, P. (2004). Sen- cloning of the mouse obese gene and its human Aging, 28, 977–986. (2)
sitivity to auditory object features in human tem- homologue. Nature, 372, 425–432. (10) Zola, S. M., Squire, L. R., Teng, E., Stefanacci, L.,
poral neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, Zhao, Y., Terry, D., Shi, L., Weinstein, H., Buffalo, E. A., & Clark, R. E. (2000). Impaired
3637–3642. (7) Blanchard, S. C., & Javitch, J. A. (2010). Single- recognition memory in monkeys after damage
Zeevalk, G. D., Manzino, L., Hoppe, J., & Sonsalla, molecule dynamics of gating in a neurotransmit- limited to the hippocampal region. Journal of
P. (1997). In vivo vulnerability of dopamine neu- ter transporter homologue. Nature, 465, 188– Neuroscience, 20, 451–463. (13)
rons to inhibition of energy metabolism. Europe- 193. (3) Zorrilla, E. P., Luborsky, L., McKay, J. R., Rosenthal,
an Journal of Pharmacology, 320, 111–119. (8) Zhao, Z., & Davis, M. (2004). Fear-potentiated R., Houldin, A., Tax, A., et al. (2001). The rela-
Zeitzer, J. M., Buckmaster, C. L., Parker, K. J., startle in rats is mediated by neurons in the deep tionship of depression and stressors to immuno-
Hauck, C. M., Lyons, D. M., & Mignot, E. layers of the superior colliculus/deep mesence- logical assays: A meta-analytic review. Brain, Be-
(2003). Circadian and homeostatic regulation of phalic nucleus of the rostral midbrain through havior, and Immunity, 15, 199–226. (12)
hypocretin in a primate model: Implications for the glutamate non-NMDA receptors. Journal of Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Neglect
the consolidation of wakefulness. Journal of Neu- Neuroscience, 24, 10326–10334. (12) disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417,
roscience, 23, 3555–3560. (9) Zheng, B., Larkin, D. W., Albrecht, U., Sun, Z. S., 138. (14)
Zeki, S. (1980). The representation of colours in the Sage, M., Eichele, G., et al. (1999). The mPer2 Zucker, K. J., Bradley, S. J., Oliver, G., Blake, J.,
cerebral cortex. Nature, 284, 412–418. (6) gene encodes a functional component of the mam- Fleming, S., & Hood, J. (1996). Psychosexual de-
Zeki, S. (1983). Colour coding in the cerebral cor- malian circadian clock. Nature, 400, 169–173. (9) velopment of women with congenital adrenal
tex: The responses of wavelength-selective and Zheng, H., Patterson, L. M., & Berthoud, H.-R. hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 30, 300–
colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to (2007). Orexin signaling in the ventral tegmental 318. (11)
changes in wavelength composition. Neurosci- area is required for high-fat appetite induced by Zuckerman, L, Rehavi, M., Nachman, R., & Weiner,
ence, 9, 767–781. (6) opioid stimulation of the nucleus accumbens. I. (2003). Immune activation during pregnancy in
Zeki, S., McKeefry, D. J., Bartels, A., & Frackowiak, Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 11075–11082. (10) rats leads to a post-pubertal emergence of dis-
R. S. J. (1998). Has a new color area been discov- Zhou, F., Zhu, X., Castellani, R. J., Stimmelmayr, rupted latent inhibition, dopaminergic hyper-
ered? Nature Neuroscience, 1, 335. (6) R., Perry, G., Smith, M. A., et al. (2001). Hiber- function, and altered limbic morphology in the
Zeki, S., & Shipp, S. (1988). The functional logic of nation, a model of neuroprotection. American offspring: A novel neurodevelopmental model of
cortical connections. Nature, 335, 311–317. (6) Journal of Pathology, 158, 2145–2151. (9) schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28,
Zhang, T. Y., Hellstron, I. C., Bagot, R. C., Wen, Zhu, Y., Fenik, P., Zhen, G., Mazza, E., Kelz, M., 1778–1789. (15)
X. L., Dioro, J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Mater- Aston-Jones, G., et al. (2007). Selective loss of Zurif, E. B. (1980). Language mechanisms: A neu-
nal care and DNA methylation of a glutamic acid catecholaminergic wake-active neurons in a mu- ropsychological perspective. American Scientist,
decarboxylase 1 promoter in rat hippocampus. rine sleep apnea model. Journal of Neuroscience, 68, 305–311. (14)
Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 13130–13137. (1) 27, 10060–10071. (9)
Name Index
Abate, P., 77 Al-Rashid, R. A., 318 Asteggiano, G., 211 Barton, D. A., 464
Abbar, M., 370 Alsop, D., 197 Aston-Jones, G., 273 Barton, L., 331
Abbott, N. J., 34 Altar, C. A., 467 Athos, E. A., 197 Barton, R. A., 100, 101
AbdelMalik, P., 476 Alter, M. D., 13 Attwell, D., 33, 142 Bartoshuk, L., 211
Abe, A. S., 298 Alto, L. T., 144 Atzei, A., 147 Bartoshuk, L. M., 216, 217, 218,
Abell, C. W., 77 Amanzio, M., 211 Audinat, E., 33 219, 220
Abi-Dargham, A., 480 Amaral, D. G., 372, 373 Auh, S., 478 Bartsch, T., 401
Aboitiz, F., 441 Amateau, S. K., 331 Avena, N. M., 322 Barua, L. A., 400
Abrahamsen, B., 206 Ambady, N., 373 Avissar, M., 197 Basbaum, A. I., 64, 209, 212
Abrahamsson, N., 437 American Psychiatric Association, Axel, R., 126, 222, 223 Basil, J. A., 401, 402
Abramov, E., 405 461, 472 Axmacher, N., 447 Baskin, D. G., 314
Abrams, W., 144 American Psychological Association Aziz, N., 382 Basma, A., 255
Abutalebi, J., 437 (APA), 138 Azrin, N. H., 80 Bassett, A. S., 476
Acri, J., 81 Amering, M., 376 Babich, F. R., 410 Basso, A., 430
Acuna-Goycolea, C., 316 Ames, M. A., 348, 349 Babiloni, C., 208 Basterzi, A. D., 465
Adamec, R. E., 366 Amiry-Moghaddam, M., 34 Babinet, C., 132 Bastida, C., 369
Adams, D. B., 336, 337 Amlaner, C. J., 281 Babkoff, H., 266 Basu, A., 197
Adams, R. B., Jr., 373 Amsterdam, J. D., 462 Babor, T. F., 77 Bates, E., 438, 439
Adams, W., 477 Anagnostaras, S. G., 290 Backlund, E.-O., 257 Battersby, S., 311
Addis, D. R., 398 Andersen, E., 13 Bäckman, J., 289 Baudry, M., 372
Ader, R., 382 Andersen, J. L., 234 Baddeley, A. D., 390, 395 Bauer, E. P., 372
Adler, E. K., 218 Andersen, R. A., 242 Baer, J. S., 77 Baulac, S., 424
Adler, N. T., 333 Andersen, T. S., 106 Baer, L., 77 Baum, A., 382
Admon, R., 374 Anderson, C., 347 Bagemihl, B., 347 Bauman, M. D., 373
Adolphs, R., 358, 363, 374, 375, Anderson, D. J., 197 Baghdoyna, H. A., 282 Bautista, D. M., 204
376, 427 Anderson, F., 367 Bailey, C. H., 412 Bavelier, D., 159, 451
Adrien, J., 282 Anderson, J. R., 96 Bailey, J. M., 347, 348, 349 Bax, C. M., 336
Agarwal, N., 211 Anderson, M., 138 Bailey, K. G. D., 439 Bayley, P. J., 396, 403
Agerbo, E., 476 Anderson, M. E., 251 Baimoukhametova, D. V., 75 Baylis, G. C., 183
Aglioti, S., 147, 219, 422, 452 Anderson, N. D., 139 Bains, J. S., 75 Baylor, D. A., 129
Agrawal, A. F., 327 Andersson, K.-E., 334 Baird, A. A., 373 Bean, B. P., 42
Agster, K. L., 400 Andrade, D. V., 298 Baker, B. N., 198 Bear, M. F., 177
Aguglia, E., 464 Andreasen, N. C., 114, 472, 473, Baker, G. B., 292 Beaton, E. A., 373
Aguirre, G. K., 173, 400 478 Baker, L., 13 Beauchamp, M. S., 226
Agyei, Y., 347, 348 Andres, K. H., 204 Baker, S. W., 346 Bechara, A., 364, 374
Aharon, I., 72 Andresen, J. M., 258 Bakker, J., 331, 333 Beck, M., 343
Ahlskog, J. E., 319 Andrew, D., 206 Balaji, J., 414 Beck, S., 137
Ahmed, E. I., 330, 331 Andrews, T. J., 159 Balda, R. P., 401, 402 Becker, C., 376
Ahn, S. J., 312 Angelucci, A., 132 Ball, W. A., 281, 282 Becker, H. C., 377
Ahola, S., 184 Angulo, M. C., 33 Ballard, P. A., 255 Becks, L., 327
Aimone, J. B., 126 Ankney, C. D., 118 Balleine, B. W., 331 Beebe, D. W., 283
Airaksinen, M. S., 130 Antanitus, D. S., 33 Ballon, J. S., 476 Beehner, J. C., 368, 383
Airan, R. D., 465 Anton, E. S., 125 Balthasar, N., 316 Beeli, G., 225
Akbarian, S., 481 Antoniadis, E. A., 372 Balthazart, J., 331, 333 Beeman, M. J., 425, 427
Alanko, K., 347 Antoun, N., 361 Banich, M. T., 427 Beer, T. M., 334
Albanese, M.-C., 208 Apfelbach, R., 376 Banks, W. P., 245 Beersma, G. M., 271
Alberts, J. R., 339 Aplin, L. C., 269 Barak, O., 396 Behrens, M., 218
Albouy, G., 404 Apostolakis, E. M., 333 Barash, S., 249 Belforte, J. E., 481
Albrecht, D. G., 176 Applebaum, S., 224 Barbe, R. P., 466 Belin, P., 199
Albright, T. D., 28, 186 Araneda, R. C., 222 Barber, J. R., 193 Bella, S. D., 197
Aldrich, M. S., 284 Arango, V., 370 Barbour, D. L., 199 Bellini, F., 466
Aleman, A., 371, 474 Araripe, L. O., 330 Bargary, G., 226 Bellugi, U., 436
Alerstam, T., 289 Archer, J., 368 Bargmann, C. I., 128 Belmonte, P. L., 461
Alexander, G. M., 332 Archer, S. N., 273 Barinaga, M., 142 Belova, M. A., 372
Alexander, J. T., 316 Arendt, J., 270 Barnea, G., 224 Benedetti, F., 211
Alford, J. R., 374 Arnason, B. G., 257 Barnes, B. M., 289 Benes, F. M., 125, 478
al-Haythem, I., 153 Arnold, A. P., 330 Barnes, C. A., 126 Bennet, A. L., 349
Alkire, M. T., 118 Arnold, S. E., 478 Barnett, K. J., 225, 226 Bennett, E. L., 133
Allen, H., 450 Arnsten, A. F. T., 380 Baron-Cohen, S., 436 Bennett, K., 342
Allen, H. L., 11 Arseneau, L. M., 299 Barr, H. M., 77 Benoit, S. C., 317
Allen, J. S., 119, 198 Arvidson, K., 216 Barratt-Fornell, A., 219 Benschop, R. J., 380
Allen, L. S., 349 Asarnow, J. R., 466 Barrett, L. F., 373 Ben-Shoshan, R., 225
Alleva, E., 130, 133 Åsberg, M., 370 Barsh, G. S., 314 Benson, D. F., 184
Allison, T., 185, 289 Ascherio, A., 256 Bartel, P., 288, 375 Benson, M. D., 405
Almazen, M., 436 Aserinsky, E., 278 Bartels, A., 186 Berard, J., 369
Almeida, O. P., 462 Ashley, R., 135 Bartke, A., 331 Berardi, D., 466
Almli, C. R., 318 Ashmore, L. J., 273 Bartko, J., 370 Berdoy, M., 372
Alnwick, K. A., 337 Assal, G., 428 Bartolotti, B., 466 Berenbaum, S. A., 344

545
546 Name Index

Beresh, H., 118 Blossom-Stach, C., 440 Brewer, W. J., 474 Burt, A. D., 336, 337
Berg, S., 218 Bobrow, D., 348 Breysse, N., 257 Burton, H., 134
Bergé, D., 376 Bochukova, E. G., 321 Bridge, H., 173 Burton, L. A., 427
Berger, R. J., 288 Bocklandt, S., 348 Bridge, J. A., 466 Burton, R. F., 300
Berger-Sweeney, J., 125 Bode, L., 462 Bridgeman, B., 173 Burwell, R. D., 271
Berglund, H., 225 Boder, E., 442 Bridges, R. S., 338 Busche, M. A., 405
Bergman, T. J., 383 Boehm, S., 69 Brisbare-Roch, C., 280 Buschman, T. J., 450
Bergmann, O., 127 Boettiger, C. A., 77 Broadbent, N. J., 401 Bushnell, M. C., 209, 211
Berkhof, J., 461 Boeve, B. F., 285 Broca, P., 109, 421, 438 Buss, D. M., 341, 342
Berlin, H. A., 363 Bogaert, A. F., 347, 348 Brody, A. L., 466 Bussiere, J. R., 334
Berliner, D. L., 224 Bogdanov, S., 147 Broman, S. H., 436 Butelman, H. R., 77
Berlucchi, G., 147, 219, 422, 425 Bohlin, G., 344 Bron, C. R., 414, 415 Butler, K., 137
Berman, A. J., 148 Boivin, D. B., 270 Bronson, R. T., 338 Butler, M. P., 273
Berman, K. F., 436, 478 Bonath, B., 106 Brook, C. G. D., 344 Butler, S. R., 186
Bernal, D., 300 Bondar, I. V., 184 Brooks, D. C., 282 Buxbaum, L. M., 452
Bernasconi, S., 369 Bonhoeffer, T., 414 Brooks, J., 441 Byl, N. N., 137
Bernati, T., 451 Bonifati, V., 254 Brotchie, P., 242 Byne, W., 345, 350
Bernstein, J. J., 144 Bonneh, Y. S., 447 Brotto, L., 334 Byrne, J. H., 411
Berntson, G. G., 374 Bookheimer, S. Y., 396 Brouwer, G. J., 186 Byrne, M., 476
Beron, J., 307 Boot, W. R., 133, 451 Brown, A. S., 477 Cabeza, R., 139, 383, 406
Berridge, C. W., 279 Booth, F. W., 234 Brown, C. E., 144 Caccappolo-van Vliet, E., 442
Berridge, K. C., 72 Booth, W., 327 Brown, G. L., 370 Cacioppo, J. T., 374
Berryhill, M. E., 406 Bordier, C., 112 Brown, G. M., 283 Cadenhead, K. S., 476
Berson, D. M., 271 Borgland, S. L., 316 Brown, J., 77 Cadoret, R. J., 367
Berthoud, H.-R., 316 Borgman, J., 5 Brown, J. R., 338 Cahill, H., 165
Bertram, L., 405 Born, J., 290, 291, 292 Brown, M. J., 14 Cahill, L., 329, 330, 395, 422
Beuming, T., 73 Borodinsky, L. N., 60 Brown, R. W., 145 Cahn, W., 478
Bever, T. G., 432 Borsutzky, S., 404 Brown, T. L., 110 Cai, D. J., 290
Beverly, J. L., 299 Bortz, R., 288 Brown, T. P., 255 Cai, X., 316
Bevilacqua, L., 367 Bos, P. A., 337 Brown, W. S., 430 Caine, S. B., 73
Bhardwaj, R. D., 126 Bos, T., 133 Browning, M. D., 414 Cajochen, C., 274
Bhatia, K. P., 481 Boshart, J., 436 Bruce, K., 343 Caldara, R., 138, 185
Bialystok, E., 437 Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 13 Bruck, M., 184 Calder, A. J., 361
Biben, M., 366 Boucsein, K., 375 Brugge, J. F., 197 Caldwell, J. A., 283
Bica, L. A., 153 Boudin, H., 62 Brugger, P., 430 Calhoun, J., 452
Bichot, N. P., 450 Bouffard, M., 199 Brummelte, S., 462 Calignano, A., 75
Bielawski, D., 349 Bourque, C. W., 305, 306 Brunet, A., 378 Callaway, E. M., 157, 172, 182, 186
Biernaskie, J., 149 Boutrel, B., 282 Bruno, R. M., 54 Calvin, J. E., Jr., 321
Bierut, L. J., 461 Bower, C., 475 Brunswick, N., 442 Calvin, J. E., III, 321
Bigler, E. D., 118 Bowles, C. A., 382 Brus, R., 145 Camargo, L. A. A., 306
Bilgüvar, K., 131 Bowles, S., 17, 18 Bruss, J., 119, 198 Camerer, C. F., 363
Bilkey, D. K., 401 Bowmaker, J. K., 161, 165 Bryan, A., 77, 79 Cameron, H. A., 383
Billington, C. J., 316 Boycott, B. B., 169 Bryant, R. A., 383 Cameron, N. M., 131
Bimler, D., 165 Boyd, J. D., 144 Bryden, M. P., 430 Caminiti, R., 429
Binart, N., 338 Boyeson, M. G., 144 Bryk, K., 344 Camp, N. J., 461
Binder, G. K., 34 Bracha, H. S., 430 Brzustowicz, L. M., 367 Campbell, S. S., 289
Binget, U., 211 Bradley, M. M., 72 Bubash, S., 410 Camperio-Ciani, A., 347, 348
Bingman, V. P., 289 Bradwejn, J., 376 Buchanan, A., 178 Campfield, L. A., 314
Bini, L., 467 Brainard, D. H., 173 Büchel, C., 211, 375 Canal, C. E., 395
Bioulac, B., 267 Brakke, K. E., 433 Buchen, L., 110 Canepari, M., 234
Birbaumer, N., 111 Branch, B. J., 331 Buchholz, B. A., 126 Cannon, M., 476
Bird, A., 9 Branco, T., 55 Buchs, P.-A., 414, 415 Cannon, W. B., 299, 356
Birmaher, B., 466 Brand, M., 404 Buck, L., 222, 223, 224 Cano, G., 303
Birring, S. S., 368 Brandfonbrener, A. G., 137 Buck, L. B., 218 Cantallops, I., 416
Bischofberger, J., 62, 126 Brandon, J. G., 133 Buck, R., 361 Cantalupo, C., 429
Bishop, C. V., 351 Brandt, F., 358 Buckholtz, J. W., 69 Cao, M., 285
Bitran, D., 331 Brandt, J., 258 Bucy, P. C., 104 Cao, Y. Q., 208
Bizzi, E., 282 Brandt, T., 203 Buell, S. J., 125 Capela, J. P., 76
Bjork, J. M., 77 Brann, J. H., 333 Buka, S. L., 477 Capiluppi, C., 347
Björklund, A., 257 Brans, R. G. H., 139 Bulbena, A., 376 Caplan, A. H., 240
Björnsdotter, M., 206 Brass, M., 245 Bulin, S. E., 79 Capleton, A. C., 255
Blackless, M., 344 Brasted, P. J., 257 Bulsara, K. R., 144 Caraci, F., 464
Blackwell, A., 438 Braun, A. R., 242, 282 Bülthoff, H. H., 237 Caravolas, M., 442
Blake, R., 178, 225, 448 Braun, C., 111 Bundgaard, M., 34 Carden, D., 186
Blakely, E., 377 Bravo, E., 447 Bundy, H., 476 Cardno, A. G., 474
Blakeslee, S., 147 Breedlove, S. M., 130, 329, 330, 351 Burdge, G. C., 12 Cardoso, F. L., 347
Blanchard, R., 348 Breer, H., 224 Burgess, N., 400 Carelli, R. M., 72
Blanke, O., 106 Breidenbach, W. C., 147 Burk, C., 370 Carey, D. P., 183, 242
Blankenburg, F., 207 Breiter, H. C., 72 Burke, D., 349 Carlisle, H. J., 300
Blasko, D. G., 450 Bremmer, F., 188 Burman, D. D., 137 Carlson, J. R., 193
Bleuler, E., 472 Brenowitz, E. A., 126 Burn, P., 314 Carlsson, A., 59
Bliss, T. V. P., 412, 416 Brent, D. A., 466 Burnett, A. L., 333 Carlsson, T., 257
Bliss-Moreau, E., 373 Brent, H. P., 184 Burns, M. M., 163 Carlström, E., 347, 348
Bloch, G. J., 331 Breslin, P. A., 224 Burr, D. C., 188 Carlyon, R. P., 452
Bloch, M., 462 Bressler, K., 207 Burrell, B., 117 Carmichael, S. T., 144
Name Index 547

Caron, M. G., 64 Chiarello, C., 425, 427 Conner, W. E., 193 Dale, A., 72
Caroni, P., 378 Chin, J., 404 Connine, C. M., 450 Dale, A. M., 186
Carpenter, G. A., 266 Chin, S., 478 Connor, P. D., 77 Dale, N., 411
Carr, T. S., 368 Chitins, X., 118 Connors, B. W., 271 Dale, P. S., 13
Carreiras, M., 135 Chiueh, C. C., 255, 256 Conrad, C. D., 383 Dalterio, S., 331
Carrigan, T., 144 Chivers, M. L., 347 Considine, R. V., 315 Dalton, P., 224
Carroll, S., 4 Cho, K., 270 Constante, M., 482 Damasio, A., 374, 375, 399
Carruth, L. L., 330 Choi, D.-S., 77 Conway, G. S., 344 Damasio, A. R., 363, 364
Carter, C. S., 334 Choi, J., 441 Cook, B. M., 351 Damasio, H., 104, 119, 198, 364,
Carter, V., 221 Choi, J. S., 372 Cook, C. C. H., 80 374, 375, 427
Casasanto, D., 440 Chomsky, N., 437 Cooperman, A., 447 Damsma, G., 72
Cash, S. S., 277, 439 Chow, E. W. C., 476 Copeland, D., 366, 423 Dan, Y., 281
Caspi, A., 367, 461 Christensen, P., 337 Copp, A., 435 Daniel, D. G., 478
Caspy, T., 266 Chu, H.-P., 333 Coppola, D. M., 174, 177 Dantzer, R., 382
Cassia, V. M., 184, 185 Chuang, H., 212 Corballis, M. C., 219, 422, 426, 452 Danziger, S., 107
Cassone, V. M., 271 Churchland, P. S., 3 Corbett, D., 149 Darby, D. G., 288
Castellucci, V., 411, 412 Ciaramelli, E., 406 Corcoran, A. J., 193 Darian-Smith, C., 126, 289
Castner, S. A., 396 Cicchetti, D. V., 289 Corkin, S., 149, 397, 398, 399, 404 Darley, J. M., 363
Castrén, E., 465 Cicchetti, P., 372 Corley, R., 13 Dartnall, H. J. A., 161
Catalano, S. M., 129 Cicone, N., 438 Cormack, L. K., 186 Darwin, C., 15, 341
Catalanotto, F. A., 219 Cipolotti, L., 358, 406 Corna, F., 347 Das, A., 112
Catania, K. C., 221 Cirelli, C., 291 Corradi, N., 442 Daskalakis, Z. J., 110
Catchpole, C. K., 6 Cirrito, J., 339 Cosmelli, D., 448, 449 Davalos, D., 33
Catmur, C., 243 Cirulli, F., 133 Coss, R. G., 133 Davidson, R. J., 358, 361, 373, 383,
Catterall, W. A., 41, 45 Clahsen, H., 436 Costa, E., 377 463
Cavanagh, P., 107, 184, 186 Clark, B. A., 55 Costa, M., 111 Davidson, T. L., 321
Caviness, V. S., 185 Clark, D. W., 100, 101 Costa, R. M., 254 Davies, G., 476
Ceci, S. J., 184 Clark, J. H., 333 Costa, V. D., 72 Davies, M., 368
Censits, D. M., 478 Clark, J. J., 450 Coste, O., 267 Davies, P., 2, 405
Cepeda-Benito, A., 79 Clark, L., 371 Costello, P., 449 Davies, P. L., 301
Cerletti, U., 467 Clark, R. E., 393, 394, 401 Cote, K. A., 291 Davis, E. C., 331
Cervantes, M. C., 369 Clark, S., 428 Cottrell, J. F., 153 Davis, F. C., 271
Cespuglio, R., 282 Clark, S. L., 101 Coultrap, S. J., 414 Davis, G. W., 475
Chabris, C. F., 119 Clark, W. S., 6 Coupland, C., 474 Davis, H. N., 349
Chafee, M. V., 396 Clarkson, A. N., 144 Courchesne, E., 93 Davis, J. I., 358
Chalmers, D. J., 3, 446 Claus, E. D., 406 Cousins, A. J., 337 Davis, J. M., 482
Chalmers, D. L., 3 Clayton, D. A., 414 Coventry, M., 343 Davis, K. D., 147, 207
Chalupa, L. M., 429 Clayton, E. C., 279 Cowansage, K. K., 378 Davis, K. W., 79
Chan, A. W.-Y., 184 Cleary, L. J., 411 Cowart, B. J., 226 Davis, M., 371, 372
Chan, C. S., 257 Cleary, M., 178 Cowell, P. E., 478 Davis, M. B., 77
Chang, E., 110 Clegg, R. A., 149 Cowey, A., 110, 173, 187 Dawkins, R., 16, 18
Chang, E. F., 198 Cleghorn, J. M., 283 Cox, J. J., 209 Dawson, D., 267
Chang, G. Y., 403 Clelland, C. D., 126 Crago, M. B., 435 Dawson, T. M., 61, 256
Chang, G.-Q., 320 Clément, K., 321 Craig, A. D., 206 Dawson, V. L., 61, 256
Chang, K., 316 Cleveland, A., 369 Craig, A. M., 62 Dax, G., 438
Chang, M., 441 Clifford, J., 13 Crair, M. C., 177 Dax, M., 438
Changeux, J.-P., 74 Cline, H., 414 Cravchik, A., 71 Day, S., 225
Chant, D., 475, 476 Cloninger, C. R., 77 Crawshaw, L. I., 300 Deacon, T. W., 103, 117, 437
Chantala, K., 347 Clutton-Brock, T., 17 Creed, H., 226 Deadwyler, S. A., 280
Chao, M., 130 Coan, J. A., 383 Creighton, S. M., 346 Dean, C. E., 483
Chapman, C. S., 173 Cobos, P., 357 Cremers, C. W. R. J., 200 Dean, K. A., 476
Charpak, S., 33 Coburn, C., 313 Crick, F., 291 Dear, K., 258
Chatterjee, A., 452 Coccaro, E. F., 367, 370 Crick, F. C., 446 DeArmond, S. J., 306
Chaudhari, N., 217, 218 Coderre, T. J., 212 Critchley, H. D., 318, 358, 361 de Brabander, J. M., 401
Chaudhri, N., 77 Cohen, D., 251 Crockett, M. J., 371 de Bruin, J. R. C., 401
Chau-Wong, M., 479, 480 Cohen, J. D., 363, 447 Crossin, K. L., 125 de Castro, J. M., 313, 314
Checkley, S. A., 337 Cohen, L. G., 134 Crossley, N. A., 482 de Costa, B. R., 75
Chee, M. J. S., 318 Cohen, M. J., 409 Crow, T. J., 477, 478 de Gelder, B., 185
Chee, M. W. L., 288 Cohen, N. J., 398 Crystal, J. D., 400 deBonis, M., 375
Cheer, J. F., 75 Cohen, S., 383 Cummings, D. E., 312, 314, 321 de Jong, W. W., 271
Chen, C., 212 Cohen-Kittenis, P. T., 346 Cummings, S., 197 de la Rocha, J., 199
Chen, C.-F. F., 224 Cohen-Tannoudji, M., 132 Cunningham, W. A., 373 de los Santos, H. C., 218
Chen, E., 382 Colantuoni, C., 322 Cur, R. C., 478 De Luca, M., 442
Chen, J.-Y., 217 Colapinto, J., 346 Curio, G., 292 de Maat, S., 466
Chen, L. M., 207 Cole-Harding, S., 349 Cusack, R., 172, 452 de Monvel, J. B., 197
Chen, N., 482 Coleman, E., 427 Cuthill, I. C., 159 de Pellegrino, G., 406
Chen, Q., 303 Coleman, P. D., 125 Cutler, W. B., 225 de Peralta Menendez, R. G., 173
Chen, S., 273 Collie, A., 288 Cuzon, V. C., 131 de Sonneville, L. M. J., 14
Chen, S. A., 79 Collier, T., 318 Czech, G., 462 De Waal, F., 25
Chen, Y., 273 Collingridge, G. L., 412 Czeisler, C. A., 266, 269, 270, 278 De Wall, C. N., 212
Chen, Z.-Y., 371 Colmers, W. F., 318 Czosnyka, M., 141 DeCoster, M., 366
Cheney, D. L., 383 Colom, R., 118 Daan, S., 271 DeCoursey, P. J., 267
Cheour-Luhtanen, M., 428 Conca, A., 467 Dabbs, J. M., Jr., 368 Dees, T., 135
Chernenko, G., 149 Cone, R. D., 316 Dacey, D., 318 DeFelipe, C., 208
Chiang, M.-C., 119 Conn, M., 23 Dail, W. G., 144 DeFries, J. C., 13
548 Name Index

Dehaene, S., 102, 114, 447, 449 Dodds, C. M., 184 Eggen, B. J. L., 133 Fane, B. A., 344
deHan, R. S., 126 Doesburg, S. M., 447 Eggert, M., 370 Fang, H., 224
Deichmann, R., 207 Dolan, R., 358 Ehrhardt, A. A., 330, 344 Fantz, R. L., 184
DeJong, J., 370 Dolan, R. J., 375 Ehrlich, P. R., 298 Faraguna, U., 291
Del Cerro, M. C. R., 339 Dolberg, D. S., 224 Eichenbaum, H., 396, 397, 401, 402 Farah, M. J., 185, 416
Del Cul, A., 447 Dolcos, F., 383 Eichenbaum, H. B., 400 Farber, N. B., 481
Delahanty, D. L., 384 Dolezal, C., 346 Eidelberg, E., 141 Farivar, R., 183
Delaney, K. R., 144 Domhoff, G. W., 173, 292 Eimer, M., 452 Farley, F., 138
Delespaul, P., 461 Dominguez, J. M., 333 Eippert, F., 211 Farlow, M., 405
Deliagina, T. G., 237 Donahoe, P. K., 343 Eisch, A. J., 79 Farmer, M. E., 442
DeLong, M. R., 254 Donley, J. M., 300 Eisenberger, N. I., 212 Farol, P., 125
Delville, Y., 369 Donoghue, J. P., 240 Eisenegger, C., 368 Farrell, J., 334
Dement, W., 288 Doolittle, N., 224 Eisenstein, E. M., 409 Fatemi, S. H., 476, 477
Dement, W. C., 278, 279, 291 Doricchi, F., 452 Ek, M., 303 Featherstone, R. E., 338
Demers, P., 430 Doty, R. L., 224 Ekblom, B., 234 Feeney, D. M., 144
Denburg, N. L., 363 Douglas, R. H., 271 Ekman, P., 361, 427 Fehér, O., 290
Denk, W., 111 Dow, S., 396 Elbert, T., 135, 137 Fehr, E., 337, 368
Dennett, D. C., 3, 446 Dowling, J. E., 169 Eldridge, L. L., 396 Feig, S. L., 93, 168, 279
Denning, J. H., 349 Downar, J., 207 Elias, C. F., 314 Feinstein, J. S., 374, 375
deQuervain, D. J.-F., 395 Downes, J. J., 441 Eliassen, J. C., 427 Feldman, H. M., 138
Derégnaucourt, S., 290 Downing, P. E., 184 Elkashef, A., 81 Feldman, R., 337
DeRoshia, C. W., 269 Doyle, P., 179 Ellacott, K. L. J., 316 Feldon, J., 133, 372, 477
Descartes, R., 127, 154, 445 Doyon, J., 290 Elliott, J. C., 316 Fell, J., 447
DeSimone, J. A., 218 Drago, F., 464 Elliott, T. R., 59 Felleman, D. J., 182
Desimone, R., 450 Drain, M., 185 Ellis, A. B., 119 Feller, G., 302
DeSimone, S. K., 218 Drake, R. M., 450 Ellis, L., 348, 349 Fendrich, R., 173
Desmurget, M., 242 Dreger, A., 346 Ellis-Behnke, R. G., 144 Feng, G., 139
Destexhe, A., 288 Dreger, A. D., 345 Ells, L. J., 310, 311 Feng, J., 12, 119
Detre, J. A., 112 Drewnowski, A., 219 Elmquist, J. K., 316 Fentress, J. C., 238
deTribolet, N., 428 Driver, J., 207, 452 Emery, N. J., 373 Ferrari, F., 333
Deutsch, D., 197 Druid, H., 126 Emmorey, K., 198 Ferraro, V., 439
Deutsch, J. A., 312 Drzyzga, L. R., 465 Enard, W., 437 Ferreira, F., 439
DeValois, R. L., 176 Du, J., 333 Eng, M. Y., 78 Ferrero, P., 377
Devane, W. A., 75 Du, Y. M., 464 Engel, A. K., 279, 447 Ferris, C., 366
DeVoogd, T. J., 126 Duan, X., 475 Engel, S. A., 396 Ferszt, R., 462
Devor, E. J., 77 Dube, M. P., 197 Engert, F., 414 Fetz, E. E., 240
Devor, M., 212 Dubernard, J.-M., 147 Enoch, M.-A., 367 Fiber, J. M., 333
Devos, R., 314 Ducci, F., 77 Epping-Jordan, M. P., 74 Fibiger, H. C., 72
deVries, M., 461 Duck, S. C., 344 Epstein, A. N., 317 Field, E. F., 331
Dewey, M. M., 306 Ducommun, C. Y., 198 Epstein, R. A., 400 Fields, H. L., 64
DeYoe, E. A., 182 Dudai, Y., 416, 428 Erickson, C. J., 333 Figner, B., 406
Dhingra, R., 321 Dudchenko, P. A., 400 Erickson, K. I., 139 Filosa, J. A., 33
Di Lorenzo, P. M., 42, 217, 218 Dudley, C., 273 Erickson, R. P., 193, 215, 217, 218 Fils-Aime, M.-L., 77
Di Page, E., 442 Duelli, R., 35 Ernst, M. B., 315 Finch, C. E., 17
Diamond, L. M., 347 Duerden, E. G., 208 Eschenko, O., 291 Fine, A., 416
Diamond, M., 346 Duff, M. C., 398 Eskelinen, M. H., 404 Fine, I., 179
Dias-Ferreira, E., 383 Duffy, J. F., 270 Eslinger, P. J., 219, 422 Finger, S., 438
Díaz, M., 329 Duffy, V. B., 220 Esser, S. K., 281 Fink, G. R., 225, 242, 428
DiCarlo, J. J., 183 Dulac, C., 224 Esslinger, C., 475 Finsterbusch, J., 211
Dichgans, J., 248 Duman, R., 465 Esteller, M., 12 Firestein, S., 222
DiChiara, G., 74 Dunbar, G. L., 260 Etcoff, N. L., 361, 427 Fisch, L., 447
Dick, D. M., 77 Duncan, G. H., 208, 209, 211 Etgen, A. M., 333 Fischbacher, U., 337
Dick, F., 439 Duncan, J., 172 Eulig, C., 225 Fischbeck, K. H., 406
Dicke, P. W., 249 Duncan, S. L., 373 Euston, D. R., 290 Fisher, L., 370
Dickens, W. T., 333 Dunn, F. A., 271 Evans, A. C., 188 Fisher, R. S., 318
Dickerson, S. S., 382 Dunn, S., 173 Evans, D. A., 405 Fisher, S. E., 435
Dickinson, A., 79 Dunnett, S. B., 257 Evans, S. S., 303 Fiske, A., 461
Dickman, D. K., 475 Durrant, C., 461 Evarts, E. V., 232, 235 Fiss, H., 173
Diedrichsen, J., 248, 424 Dutton, G. N., 178 Evenson, K. R., 142 Fitch, R. H., 441
Diéguez, C., 315 Duvarci, S., 372 Everhart, A. W., 146 Fite, K. V., 401, 402
Diekelmann, S., 292 Dyal, J. A., 410 Everitt, B. J., 79, 280 Fitts, D. A., 307
Diener, H. C., 249 Dysarz, F. A., III, 75 Everling, S., 138, 252 Fitzgerald, P. B., 110
Dierks, T., 102 Earnest, D. J., 271 Ewald, H., 476 Fitzpatrick, D., 177
Dijk, D.-J., 269, 273 Eastman, C. I., 269, 470 Fabiani, M., 133, 451 Fjell, A. M., 139
Dikkes, P., 338 Eaton, W. W., 476 Facoetti, A., 442 Fjerdingstad, E. J., 410
Dilks, D. D., 145 Eaves, L. J., 13 Fadda, F., 61 Flannery-Schroeder, E., 218
Diller, L., 161 Ebers, G., 347 Fadiga, L., 243 Flatz, G., 310
DiMarzo, V., 75 Ecker, A. S., 175 Fadool, D. A., 224 Fleet, W. S., 149
Dimond, S. J., 426 Eckhorn, R., 447 Fagen, Z. M., 74 Fleischer, J., 224
Dinstein, I., 243 Edelman, G. M., 129 Fahey, S., 429 Fleming, A. S., 338
Disterhoft, J. F., 393 Edelman, R. R., 240 Fahle, M., 442 Fletcher, P. C., 478
Ditterich, J., 187 Edinger, J. D., 285 Fahrenkrug, J., 271 Fletcher, R., 165
Diwadkar, V. A., 479 Edinger, K. L., 334 Falleti, M. G., 288 Flicker, L., 462
Dixon, D. B., 416 Edlinger, M., 482 Fan, G., 12 Flint, J., 461
Dobkin, D. S., 298 Edman, G., 370 Fan, W., 316 Flöel, A., 430
Name Index 549

Flor, H., 147 Frith, C., 114 Ge, S., 126 Goldman, D., 71, 77, 367
Florence, S. L., 146 Frith, C. D., 243 Gearhart, J. P., 345 Goldman, L. S., 474
Flores, L. C., 393 Frith, U., 442 Gebhard, P. H., 351 Goldman, P. S., 479
Floyd, T. F., 112 Fritsch, G., 240 Geerling, J. C., 307 Goldman-Rakic, P. S., 105, 185,
Flynn, J. M., 442 Frohman, L. A., 320 Geier, C. F., 138 396, 478
Flynn, J. R., 333 Frucht, S. J., 257 Geiger, B. M., 320 Goldreich, D., 204
Foerde, K., 403 Frye, C. A., 334 Geiger, G., 442, 443 Goldschmidt, R. B., 400
Foerster, S., 218 Fu, L.-Y., 316 Gelderd, J. B., 144 Goldspink, G., 234
Fogassi, L., 243 Fu, Q., 461 Geller, H. M., 255 Goldstein, A., 211
Fogel, S. M., 291 Fu, W., 216 Gentile, R. L., 217 Goldstein, J. M., 329
Fogg, L. F., 470 Fuchs, T., 289 Georg, B., 271 Goldstein, M., 62
Foldi, N. S., 438 Fujimoto, M., 377 George, F. W., 331 Golestani, N., 114, 135
Földy, C., 75 Fujiwara, E., 404 Gerardin, D. C. C., 331 Golgi, C., 29
Folsom, T. D., 476 Fujiyoshi, Y., 63 Gersdorff, M. C. H., 200 Golombok, S., 332
Foltz, E. I., 209 Fukui, K., 361, 427 Gervais, W. M., 382 Gonzalez Andino, S. L., 173
Foo, H., 211 Fukuyama, H., 254 Gerwig, M., 393 González-Maeso, J., 481
Foraker, R. E., 142 Fulker, D., 13 Geschwind, N., 428, 429, 441 Gonzalez-Zulueta, M., 61
Forgas, J. P., 337 Fuller, D. C., 79 Ghashghaei, H. T., 125 Goodale, M. A., 173, 183, 242
Forger, N. G., 130, 329 Fuller, R. K., 80 Ghetti, G., 405 Goodrich-Hunsaker, N. J., 401
Forgie, M. L., 331 Fullwood, S. D., 146 Ghilardi, M. F., 290 Goodwin, F. K., 370
Formisano, E., 185 Furman, A. C., 197 Ghosh, A., 143 Gooley, J. J., 271
Foroni, F., 243 Fusar-Poli, P., 361, 373 Gianaros, P. J., 361 Gooren, L. J. G., 334
Forsberg, M. M., 67 Fusco, M. M., 306 Gibbs, F. P., 271 Gopnik, M., 435
Forster, B., 426 Fuster, J. M., 104, 141 Gibbs, J., 312 Gordon, H. L., 373
Forti, B., 464 Gaab, N., 441 Gibson, N., 367 Gore, J. C., 185
Fortin, N. J., 400 Gable, P. A., 315 Giese, M. A., 184 Gori, S., 442
Foster, R. G., 271, 288 Gable, S. L., 382 Giguere, J. F., 197 Gorman, M. R., 290
Fotopoulou, A., 404 Gabrieli, J. D. E., 404, 441 Gil, M., 333 Goro, F., 38
Foulkes, L., 226 Gadian, D. G., 435 Gilbert, P. E., 400 Gorski, R. A., 331, 332, 349
Fournier, J. C., 466 Gado, M. H., 366 Gilbert, S., 114 Gosling, L. M., 221
Fournier, J. S., 153 Gaetani, S., 312 Gilbert, S. J., 105 Goss, R. G., 32
Fouse, S., 12 Gage, F. H., 126, 133, 400 Gilbertson, M. W., 384 Goswami, U., 441
Fox, A. N., 193 Gage, P., 363 Gillespie, D. C., 177 Gotlib, I. H., 461
Fox, C., 333 Gago, J., 376 Gillette, M. U., 274 Gottesman, I. I., 474, 475
Fox, N. A., 361 Gais, S., 290, 292 Gilman, M. U., 77 Gottfried, J. A., 224
Fox, P. T., 426 Galaburda, A. M., 441 Gilmore, J. H., 118 Götz, J., 405
Frackowiak, R. S. J., 186 Gale, S., 331 Gilmour, D., 125 Götz, M., 33
Frady, R. L., 368 Galea, L. A. M., 462 Giltay, E. J., 334 Gøtzsche, P. C., 211
Fraga, M. F., 12 Galin, D., 429 Ginsberg, M. D., 142 Gougoux, F., 134
Franc, B., 282 Gall, F., 114 Giraux, P., 147 Gowing, G., 142
Francia, N., 130 Gallant, J. L., 113 Gitschier, J., 197 Gozal, D., 283
Frangou, S., 118 Gallese, V., 243 Giuffrida, A., 75 Grabowecky, M., 107
Frank, L. M., 291 Gamer, M., 375 Giuliani, D., 333 Grabowski, T. J., 119
Frank, M. G., 361, 427 Gan, W.-B., 133, 139 Giuliano, A. J., 473 Grace, M., 316
Frank, M. J., 257, 406 Ganesh, S., 179 Giustetto, M., 412 Grafman, J., 144, 207, 436
Frank, R. A., 218 Gangestad, S. W., 337, 341, 342 Gläscher, J., 374 Grafton, S. T., 393
Frankland, P. W., 126, 371, 376 Ganguly, K., 416 Glaser, R., 381 Graham-Kevan, N., 368
Franz, E. A., 424, 429 Gao, B., 34 Glatt, S. J., 80 Granados-Fuentes, D., 271
Frascino, J. C., 403 Gao, J.-H., 248 Gleason, C. A., 244 GrandPre, T., 177
Frassinetti, F., 452 Gao, X., 254 Gleeson, T. T., 301 Gratton, G., 133, 451
Frayling, T. M., 321 Gao, X.-B., 291 Glenberg, A. M., 358 Gray, C. M., 447
Freed, C. R., 257 Garai, J., 333 Glendenning, K. K., 198 Gray, J., 361
Freedman, M. S., 271 Garbe, P. L., 255 Glick, I. D., 482 Gray, M. A., 361
Freeman, J. H., 393 Garcia, C., 144 Glickman, M. E., 119 Graziadei, P. P. C., 126
Freeman, R. D., 112 Garcia, C. R., 225 Globus, A., 133 Graziano, M. S. A., 102, 104, 240
Freeman, W., 105 Garcia, J. A., 273 Gloria, R., 79 Green, A. E., 198
Freiwald, W. A., 450 Garcia, R., 372 Gluck, M. A., 403 Green, J. J., 447
French, J. A., 349 Garcia-Falgueras, A., 351 Gluckman, P. D., 12 Green, K. N., 405
Frese, M., 270 Garcia-Fernández, J.-M., 271 Glykys, J., 377 Green, M. F., 478
Freund, G. G., 382 Gardner, B. T., 432 Goard, M., 281 Green, S. J., 218
Frey, K., 358 Gardner, C. O., 461 Goate, A., 405 Greenberg, M. E., 338
Frey, S. H., 147 Gardner, H., 438, 439 Godfrey, K. M., 12 Greene, G. C., 289
Friberg, U., 216 Gardner, R. A., 432 Godley, M., 80 Greene, J. D., 363
Fridberger, A., 197 Garner, C. C., 289 Goebel, R., 185 Greenlee, M. W., 188
Friden, J., 234 Garver-Apgar, C. E., 337, 342 Gogolla, N., 378 Greenough, W. T., 133
Fried, I., 447 Gaser, C., 135, 329 Gogos, J. A., 67, 126 Greer, C. A., 224
Friedman, E. S., 466 Gasparini, M., 452 Golarai, G., 436 Gregg, V., 153
Friedman, M., 320 Gatchel, R. J., 382 Gold, A. R., 336, 337 Greischar, L. L., 361
Friedman, R. M., 207 Gatti, S., 288 Gold, P. E., 395 Grieve, K. L., 242
Friedman-Hill, S., 107 Gatz, M., 256, 461 Gold, R. M., 319 Griffin, D. E., 34
Friedrich, R. W., 222 Gauthier, I., 186 Goldberg, R., 194 Griffin, D. R., 193
Friehs, G. M., 240 Gautier, T., 346 Goldberg, T. E., 478 Griffin, J. E., 331
Frisén, J., 126 Gavrilets, S., 348 Golden, S. M., 74 Griffiths, T. D., 199
Frisén, L., 344, 346 Gaysinskaya, V., 320 Goldey, K. L., 334 Grill, H. J., 312
Friston, K. J., 243 Gazzaniga, M. S., 173, 425, 426, 427 Goldin-Meadow, S., 437 Grillon, C., 371
550 Name Index

Gritton, H. J., 269 Hamann, S. B., 404 Haynes, J.-D., 113, 114, 224, 245 Hind, J. E., 197
Groc, L., 380 Hamer, D. H., 347, 348 Hazeltine, E., 424 Hindersson, P., 271
Grosbras, M.-H., 138 Hamilton, L. D., 334 He, S., 449 Hines, M., 332, 333, 344
Gross, C. G., 102, 153, 173 Hamilton, N. B., 33 He, S. M., 464 Hippisley-Cox, J., 474
Gross, J., 208 Hamilton, R. B., 219 He, W., 218 Hirsch, H. V. B., 178
Gross, J. J., 461 Hamilton, W. D., 18 He, Z., 144 Hirschman, A., 141
Gross, R. A., 376 Hammer, M., 411 Head, K., 118 Hirstein, W., 147
Grossberg, S., 266 Hammer, N. J., 316 Heath, A. C., 13 Hitch, G. J., 395
Grosshans, D. R., 414 Hampton, R. E., 280 Hebb, D. O., 394, 410 Hitchcock, J. M., 372
Grossman, S. P., 318 Han, C. J., 75 Hebert, M., 366 Hitzig, E., 240
Growdon, J. H., 404 Han, J.-H., 414 Heck, G. L., 218 Hiyama, T. Y., 306
Gruber, A. J., 75 Hanada, R., 303 Hediger, K., 337 Hnasko, T. S., 75
Grueschow, M., 224 Hanakawa, T., 254 Heeger, D. J., 186, 243, 448 Hobson, J. A., 292, 468
Grueter, M., 185 Hanaway, J., 366 Hegdé, J., 183 Hochberg, L. R., 240
Grundke-Iqbal, I., 405 Haney, A., 289 Heidbreder, C. A., 372 Hodsoll, J., 450
Grutzendler, J., 139 Hänggi, J., 225 Heien, M. L. A. V., 75 Hoebel, B. G., 317, 318, 319, 322
Guariglia, P., 452 Hankey, G. J., 462 Heikkila, R. E., 255 Hoeft, F., 441
Guastella, A. J., 337 Hanlon, F. M., 473 Heilman, K. M., 149 Hoese, E. K., 269
Gubernick, D. J., 339 Hanna, C., 383 Heims, H. C., 358 Hoffer, A., 473
Guérit, J.-M., 276 Hannibal, J., 271 Heinrichs, M., 337, 368 Hoffman, E., 292
Guerrero, L., 346 Hansen, M., 467 Heinze, H.-J., 245 Hoffman, F., 292
Guidotti, A., 377 Hanson, K. L., 76 Heisler, L. K., 317 Hoffman, K.-P., 186, 188
Guilleminault, C., 285 Hanson, M. A., 12 Helenius, P., 442 Hoffman, M. L., 306
Guillemot, F., 110 Hansson, E., 34 Heller, H. C., 289 Hoffman, P. L., 77, 377
Guillery, R. W., 93, 168, 279 Haqq, C. M., 343 Heller, W., 427 Hofman, M. A., 349
Guisez, Y., 314 Hara, J., 285 Helmholtz, H. von, 160 Hogg, S. L., 452
Guiso, L., 119 Hara, Y., 334 Hemmeter-Spernal, J., 468 Hohmann, C. F., 125
Gujar, N., 290 Harada, N., 331 Hemsley, D. R., 478 Hökfelt, T., 62
Güler, A. D., 271 Hardt, O., 395 Hen, R., 282 Holcombe, A. O., 107
Gulyas, B., 225 Hardy, J., 406 Henderson, J. M., 450 Hollingworth, A., 450
Gulyás, B., 279 Hare, E. H., 477 Henderson, S. A., 219 Hollister, J. M., 476
Gunn, S. R., 285 Hargreaves, G. A., 376 Hendricks, S. E., 349 Hollon, A. D., 466
Gunn, W. S., 285 Hargreaves, R., 98 Hendriks, W., 271 Hollon, S. D., 466
Guo, S.-W., 219, 220 Hari, R., 111, 112 Hendry, S. H. C., 172 Holmans, P., 461
Gupta, A. S., 291 Harkness, K. L., 461 Hengst, J., 398 Holmes, C. J., 130
Gur, R. E., 478 Harlaar, N., 13 Hennessy, M. B., 349 Holsboer, F., 468
Gusella, J. F., 259 Harley, B., 437 Hennig, J., 370 Holstege, G., 333
Gustafsson, B., 415 Harmon-Jones, E., 315, 367, 368 Hennig, R., 234 Holy, T. E., 224
Gutowski, K. A., 358 Harms, M. P., 478 Henning, P., 186 Holzman, P. S., 474, 478
Gutschalk, A., 199 Harper, L. V., 12, 260 Henny, P., 281 Homan, R. W., 361
Gvilia, I., 293 Harraid, J. H., 79 Henriksen, L., 438 Homewood, J., 422
Gwinner, E., 266 Harris, C. H., 342 Henriques, J., 361 Hommer, D. W., 77
Haas, H., 279 Harris, C. R., 206 Henson, R. N. A., 105 Honda, M.
Haas, R., 249 Harris, K. M., 31 Henthorn, T., 197 Honda, S.-I., 331
Haber, J., 141 Harrison, E. M., 290 Herdener, M., 135 Honea, R., 477, 478
Habib, M. K., 292 Harrison, G. H., 309 Heresco-Levy, U., 481 Hong, S. W., 447
Hacke, W., 142 Harrison, N. A., 361 Herkenham, M., 75 Honoré, J., 451
Hackeman, E., 204 Harrold, J., 226 Hermans, E. J., 368 Hoogenraad, C. C., 380
Hadjikhani, N., 186 Harrold, J. A., 316 Hernandez, L., 319 Hoover, J. E., 249
Hadley, R. D., 133 Hart, B. L., 317 Herrero, S., 193 Hop, W. C. J., 336
Hagenbuch, B., 34 Hartl, D. L., 330 Herring, E., 162 Hopkins, R. O., 396, 397, 401, 403
Haggard, P., 244 Hartline, H. K., 170 Herron, J., 429 Hopkins, W. D., 429
Haggarty, J. M., 470 Harvey, A. G., 383 Herrup, K., 28, 248 Hoppe, J., 256
Hagiwara, H., 405 Harvey, M., 138 Herry, C., 378 Hoptman, M. J., 427
Hagoort, P., 440 Harvey, P. H., 100, 101 Hertzog, C., 133 Hori, K., 307
Haider, H., 290 Harwich, C., 270 Herz, R. S., 221, 342, 422 Horne, J. A., 283, 288
Haidt, J., 363 Hasher, L., 267 Herzog, E. D., 271 Horridge, G. A., 409, 410
Haier, R. J., 118 Hassabis, D., 399 Hess, B. J. M., 203 Horst, W. D., 464
Haimov, I., 270, 274 Hassan, B., 347 Hess, T. M., 139 Horton, J. C., 188
Hains, B. C., 146 Hassani, O. K., 280, 281 Hesse, M. D., 242 Horvath, S., 348
Hakuta, K., 437 Hassett, J. M., 332 Hettema, J. M., 376 Horvath, T. L., 315
Halaas, J. L., 314 Hasson, U., 243 Heyes, C., 243 Hou, Y., 279, 280
Halaris, A. E., 318 Hatcher, J. M., 255 Hibbeln, J. R., 470 Houk, C. P., 346
Halaschek-Wiener, J., 17 Haueisen, J., 244 Hibbing, M. V., 374 Hovda, D. A., 144
Haley, J., 472 Haukka, J. K., 474 Higgs, S., 396 Howard, J. D., 224
Halgren, E., 439 Häusser, M., 55 Highnote, S. M., 165 Howard, M., 441
Hall, J., 475 Havas, D. A., 358 Higley, J. D., 369 Howdeshell, K. L., 331
Hall, M., 450 Havekes, R., 133 Hikosaka, O., 242 Howell, S., 369
Hallem, E. A., 193 Havlicek, J., 221 Hill, D. L., 318 Howes, O. D., 479
Haller, S., 349 Hawkins, R. D., 412 Hill, S., 281 Howland, J. G., 412
Halligan, P. W., 428, 452 Haworth, C. M. A., 13 Hill, S. Y., 78 Howlett, A. C., 75
Hallmayer, J., 285 Haxby, J. V., 243 Hillis, A. E., 452 Hrdy, S. B., 342
Halpern, S. D., 159 Hayashi-Takagi, A., 475 Hillman, E. M. C., 112 Hróbjartsson, A., 211
Halverson, H. E., 393 Haydon, P. G., 33 Hillyard, S. A., 451 Hsieh, P.-J., 176
Hamaguchi, T., 406 Hayes, J. E., 220 Himmelbach, M., 183 Hsing, W., 288
Name Index 551

Hsu, K.-s., 126 Irwin, M. R., 382 Jooste, P., 288 Keane, J., 361
Hu, N., 347 Isham, E. A., 245 Jordan, B. D., 336 Kearney, M., 298
Hu, P., 290 Ishii, T. M., 45 Jordan, C., 367 Kee, N., 126
Hu, P. T., 290 Ising, M., 468 Jordan, C. L., 329 Keefe, R. S. E., 474
Hu, S., 347 Isoda, M., 242 Jordan, H. A., 312 Keele, S. W., 249
Hu, W., 441 Itan, Y., 310 Josephs, O., 199 Keller, M., 333
Hua, J. Y., 129 Ito, M., 248, 249 Josselyn, S. A., 376 Kelley, A. E., 373
Huang, A. L., 218 Ittner, L. M., 405 Jouvet, M., 277, 279, 280, 282 Kelley, K. W., 382
Huang, B. S., 144 Ivry, R. B., 248, 249, 424 Joyner, A. L., 110 Kelley, M. S., 144
Huang, L., 450 Ivy, G. O., 338, 429 Judge, J., 442 Kelley, W. M., 198
Huang, R., 17 Iwata, J., 372 Judica, A., 442 Kelly, B. D., 474
Huang, Y.-J., 64 Iwema, C. L., 224 Jueptner, M., 251 Kelly, P. A., 338
Huang, Y.-Y., 412 Iyengar, S., 466 Julius, D., 204 Kelly, T. L., 266
Hubbard, E. M., 102 Iyer, A., 242 Jung, R. E., 118 Kemeny, M. E., 382
Hubel, D., 173, 174, 175, 177, 178, Jablensky, A. V., 475, 476 Juraska, J. M., 299 Kempermann, G., 133
182 Jacobs, B., 30, 125 Jutras, M. J., 271 Kendell, R. E., 477
Hubel, D. H., 163 Jacobs, G. D., 463 Juvonen, H., 474 Kendler, K. S., 14, 376, 461, 474
Huber, M. T., 468 Jacobs, G. H., 165 Kaas, J. H., 102, 146, 207 Kennard, C., 163, 452
Huber, R., 290 Jacobson, A. L., 410 Kacelnik, O., 201 Kennaway, D. J., 269
Hudson, J. I., 75, 322, 461 Jacobson, K. C., 367 Kagan, I., 242 Kennedy, D. P., 374, 375
Hudson, S., 298 Jaeger, E., 211 Kagan, J., 373 Kennedy, L. M., 218
Hudspeth, A. J., 196 Jain, N., 146 Kahn, R. S., 474, 478 Kennedy, S. H., 320
Huestis, M. A., 75 Jakobson, L. S., 183, 242 Kahneman, D., 72 Kennerley, S. W., 219, 424
Huey, E. D., 144 James, W., 356, 357, 364 Kaiser, A., 349 Kenny, P. J., 79, 320
Hugdahl, K., 424 Jameson, D., 162 Kalanithi, J., 403 Kensinger, E. A., 398
Huggins, G. R., 225 Jameson, K. A., 165 Kalat, J. W., 306, 390 Kerchener, G. A., 412
Hughes, J. C., 80 Jamrozik, K., 462 Kalat, R., 80 Kerns, J. G., 473
Hughes, J. R., 334 Janak, P. H., 77 Kales, A., 283 Kerr, J. N. D., 111
Huk, A. C., 186 Jäncke, L., 225 Kales, J. D., 283 Keshavan, M. S., 479
Hull, E. M., 72, 331, 333 Janowsky, J. S., 334 Kalin, N. H., 373 Kesner, R. P., 400
Hull, R., 437 Janssen, W. G. M., 334 Kalogeris, T. J., 312 Kessels, H. W., 405
Hulsebosch, C. E., 146 Järlaker, B., 234 Kalso, E., 211 Kety, S. S., 475
Hulshoff, H. E., 478 Jarrard, L. E., 400 Kambi, N., 146 Keverne, E. B., 224
Humphreys, G., 450 Javitt, D. C., 481 Kamil, A. C., 401, 402 Khan, U., 73
Humphreys, G. W., 396 Jbabdi, S., 173 Kamitani, Y., 163, 450 Khan, Y., 125
Hunt, G. E., 376 Jechura, T. J., 289 Kamo, T., 198 Khashau, A. S., 476
Hunt, S. P., 209, 212 Jemigan, T. L., 436 Kanady, J. C., 290 Khateb, A., 173
Huntington’s Disease Collaborative Jenner, A. R., 441 Kandel, E. R., 28, 411, 412 Kidd, J. R., 220
Research Group, 258 Jennings-White, C., 224 Kang, M. J., 417 Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., 381
Hurovitz, C. S., 173 Jerison, H. J., 117 Kang, P., 447 Kiehn, O., 62
Hurst, J. A., 435 Jernigan, T. L., 130 Kanold, P. O., 177 Kiening, K. L., 142
Hurvich, L. M., 162 Jessell, T. M., 28 Kanterewicz, B. I., 142 Kigar, D. L., 118
Husain, M., 452. Ji, D., 290 Kanwisher, N., 176, 184, 185, 186 Kilgour, A. R., 185
Husted, J., 476 Jia, Z., 301 Kaplan, J. M., 312 Killackey, H. P., 146, 429
Hutcheson, D. M., 79 Jiang, J., 130 Kaplan, J. R., 370 Killeen, P. R., 383
Hutchison, J., 333 Jiang, Y., 449 Kapur, N., 390 Killeffer, F. A., 318
Hutchison, K. E., 77, 79 Jin, X., 254 Kapur, S., 482 Kilner, J. M., 243
Hutson, K. A., 198 Johansson, C., 470 Karama, S., 118 Kim, C.-Y., 225
Hwang, J.-A., 376 Johansson, O., 62 Karatayev, O., 320 Kim, J. W., 218
Hyde, J., 118 Johnson, D. H., 327 Karayiorgou, M., 67 Kim, M. J., 383
Hyde, J. S., 119 Johnson, E. L., 358 Kargo, W. J., 251 Kim, S., 393
Hyde, K. L., 136, 197 Johnson, I. R., 373 Karkanias, G. B., 333 Kim, U., 219
Hyland, B. I., 254 Johnson, J. K., 77 Karlsson, M., 291 Kim, Y.-K., 376
Hyltenstam, K., 437 Johnson, L. C., 288 Karmiloff-Smith, A., 436 Kindt, M., 378
Hynd, G. W., 441 Johnson, M. R., 75 Karnath, H. O., 183 King, A. J., 201
Iacono, W. G., 77 Johnson, M. T. V., 254 Karrer, T., 211 King, B. M., 319, 320
Iba, M., 396 Johnson, P. M., 320, 376 Karrer, T. A., 219 King, V. R., 474
Ibrahim, H. M., 481 Johnson, R. L., 442 Kas, M. J. H., 316 Kingstone, A., 426, 427
Igarashi, T., 298 Johnson, R. W., 382 Kasai, K., 478 Kini, A. D., 222
Iggo, A., 204 Johnsrude, I., 199 Kasch, K. L., 461 Kinnett, S., 452
Ikeda, H., 212 Johnstone, J., 429 Kasimatis, M., 358 Kinomura, S., 279
Ikonomidou, C., 131 Jolesz, F. A., 290 Kasper, S., 464 Kinsbourne, M., 428
Imamura, K., 222 Jonas, P., 62, 126 Kasthuri, N., 139 Kinsey, A. C., 351
Imayoshi, I., 126 Jones, A. R., 34 Kataoka, H., 222 Kiriakakis, V., 481, 482
Imel, Z. E., 466 Jones, B. E., 280, 281 Katkin, E. S., 362, 463 Kirik, D., 257
Imperato-McGinley, J., 346 Jones, C. R., 273 Katschnig, H., 376 Kirkland, J., 165
Ingram, C. J. E., 310 Jones, E. G., 146 Katsuyuki, S., 114 Kirkpatrick, P. J., 141
Ingvar, M., 211 Jones, H. S., 283 Katz, J., 212 Kirsch, I., 465, 466
Innocenti, G. M., 423, 429 Jones, J. E., 316 Katz, L. C., 222 Kirtley, D. D., 102
Inouye, S. T., 271 Jones, M. R., 319 Kauer, J. A., 413 Kischka, U., 363
International Schizophrenia Jones, M. V., 75 Kavanau, J. L., 289 Kiss, L., 416
Consortium, 12, 476 Jones, P. B., 476, 478, 482 Kawamura, H., 271 Kitamura, O., 79
Inzlicht, M., 221, 342 Jones, W., 436 Kawasaki, H., 360 Kitazawa, S., 37
Iqbal, K., 405 Jonides, J. J., 450 Kay, K. N., 113 Kivipelto, M., 404
Irle, E., 375 Joormann, J., 447 Kazmi, H., 437 Klann, E., 142, 378
552 Name Index

Klaver, P., 337 Kripke, D. F., 470 Larsen, R. J., 358 Levine, J. A., 321
Kleck, R. E., 373 Krishnan, V., 383 Larsson, A., 344 Levine, J. D., 64
Kleen, J. K., 383 Krishnan-Sarin, S., 80 Larsson, J., 279 Levitin, D. J., 436
Klein, D. F., 358 Kristensen, M. P., 372 Lashley, K., 391, 392 Levitt, P., 131
Klein, D. N., 461 Kristiansen, K., 482 Lassonde, M., 430 Levitzki, A., 64
Klein, R. M., 442 Kriz, J., 142 Latty, E., 347 Levy, D. A., 397
Kleitman, N., 278, 288 Kroeze, W. K., 482 Lau, H. C., 244 Levy, H. L., 14
Klitgaard, H., 234 Kronauer, R. E., 266, 270 Lauer, C. J., 468 Levy, J., 427
Kluger, M. J., 303 Krout, K., 206 Laurent, J.-P., 282 Levy, L. S., 255
Klüvery, H., 104 Krueger, J. M., 281 Lautenbacher, S., 468 Leweke, F. M., 477
Knabl, J., 212 Krugers, H. J., 380 Lavenex, P., 373 Lewis, D. A., 478
Knauer, R. S., 278 Krupa, D. J., 393, 394 Lavidor, M., 423 Lewis, E. R., 158
Knaut, H., 125 Krushel, L. A., 125 Lavie, P., 274 Lewis, G., 461
Kneip, J., 316 Krutsch, A. J., 424 Lavond, D. G., 393, 394 Lewis, T. L., 177, 178
Knoll, J., 254 Krystal, J. H., 80 Lawden, M., 163 Lewy, A. J., 269, 470
Knösche, T. R., 244 Kuba, H., 45 Lawley, H. J., 225 Leyton, M., 370
Knowlton, B. J., 251, 254, 255, 396, Kubischik, K.-P., 186 Lawrence, A. D., 360 Li, N., 183
403 Kubischik, M., 188 Lazar, L., 146 Li, P., 144
Knox, P. C., 442 Kubista, H., 69 Lazarus, M., 303 Li, R., 159
Knudsen, S. M., 271 Kuczewski, N., 414 Lè, A. D., 77 Li, X., 303
Knyazev, G. G., 361 Kudo, K., 159 Le Grand, R., 184 Li, X. T., 464
Ko, C.-H., 72 Kujala, T., 442 Leamey, C. A., 159 Liang, F.-Q., 271
Kobayakawa, K., 221 Kujawa, S. G., 200 Leber, A. B., 451 Liang, L., 199
Kobett, P., 312 Kulikowski, J. J., 186 Lebiere, C., 96 Liao, L.-M., 346
Koch, C., 446, 447 Kullmann, D. M., 57 LeBihan, D., 114 Liberles, S. D., 224
Koch, M., 371 Kumakura, Y., 479 Lederman, S. J., 185 Liberman, M. C., 200
Kodituwakku, P. W., 131 Kumar, A., 12 LeDoux, J. E., 372, 378 Liberzon, I., 360
Koenigs, M., 363, 384 Kumar, C. J., 269 Lee, B. B., 161 Libet, B., 244, 245
Koepp, M. J., 72 Kumaran, D., 399, 400 Lee, G. P., 364 Lichenstein, P., 347, 348
Kohler, E., 243 Kumari, V., 347 Lee, H.-J., 376 Lichtenberger, L., 436
Kohn, M., 17 Kumpik, D. P., 201 Lee, I., 393 Lichtman, J. W., 139
Komhuber, H. H., 438 Kundermann, B., 468 Lee, J.-C., 482 Lidow, M. S., 177
Komisaruk, B. R., 333 Kurtz, D. B., 224 Lee, K. M., 135 Lieberman, J., 478
Komorowski, R. W., 402 Kuschinsky, W., 35 Lee, M. G., 280, 281 Lieberman, M. D., 212
Komura, Y., 94 Kusel, J., 61 Lee, O. L., 110 Liebman, M., 321
Konarski, J. Z., 320 Kusunoki, M., 186 Lee, P. A., 346 Lie-Nemeth, T., 137
Kondo, I., 209 Kuwamura, T., 343 Lee, R., 370 Lightman, S. L., 337
König, P., 279, 447, 467 Kveton, J. F., 219 Lee, S.-H., 448 Lillycrop, K. A., 12
Konopka, G., 11, 437 Kwiat, G. C., 209 Lee, T., 479, 480 Lim, K. O., 478
Koob, G. F., 74, 79 Kwon, E., 139 Lee, T. M., 269 Lim, M. M., 338, 339
Korenberg, J. R., 435 Kwon, J. S., 478 Lefkowitz, R. J., 64 Lima, S. L., 281
Koresh-Kamin, H., 225 Kwon, J.-T., 372 Legrand, L. N., 77 Lin, C.-S., 102
Korman, M., 290 Laburn, H. P., 131, 477 Lehky, S. R., 107 Lin, D. Y., 222
Kornhuber, H. H., 248 LaChance, H., 79 Lehrer, J., 416 Lin, J.-S., 279, 280
Koroshetz, W. J., 251 Lack, L., 267 Lehrman, D., 333 Lin, L., 285
Korpi, E. R., 377 Lacroix, L., 372 Leibler, S., 288 Lindberg, N. O., 313
Kosfeld, M., 337 Làdavas, E., 452 Leibniz, G., 2 Lindberg, S. M., 119
Kosslyn, S. M., 172, 183 Laeng, B., 185, 225 Leibowitz, S. F., 316, 317, 318, 320 Lindemann, B., 218
Koster, H. D., 474 Lafargue, G., 428 Lein, E. S., 177 Lindenberger, U., 133
Kostrzewa, J. P., 145 LaFerla, F. M., 405 Leinders-Zufall, T., 224 Lindholm, P., 257
Kostrzewa, R. M., 145 LaForge, K. S., 77 Leinonen, S., 442 Lindner, A., 242
Kotowicz, Z., 363 Lahti, T. A., 269 LeMaster, M., 298 Lindsay, P. H., 200
Kourtzi, Z., 186 Lai, C., 125 Lemos, B., 330 Lindström, P., 349
Koverola, C., 383 Lai, C. S. L., 435, 437 Leng, G., 64 Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., 207
Kowell, A., 74 Lai, Z., 436 Lenggenhager, B., 106 Linn, M. C., 119
Kozlov, A. S., 33 Laing, P., 476 Lenhart, R. E., 463 Linnoila, M., 370
Kraemer, D. J. M., 198 Lake, R. I. E., 13 Lennie, P., 161 Liou, Y.-C., 301
Kraft, T. W., 165 Lalancette-Hébert, M., 142 Lenz, F. A., 137 Lisman, J., 414
Krajbich, I., 363 Lamarck, J., 15 Leon, L. R., 303 Lisman, J. E., 63
Krakauer, A. H., 18 Lambie, J. A., 450 Leopold, D. A., 184 Litt, A., 73
Kramer, A. F., 133, 451 Lamsa, K. P., 57 Leppämäki, S., 468 Litt, M. D., 77
Kranzler, H. R., 77 Land, E., 163 Leriche, L., 257 Liu, A. K., 186
Kräuchi, K., 274 Landin, A. M., 217 Lescaudron, L., 260 Liu, F., 11, 144
Kraus, N., 135 Landis, D. M. D., 29 Leshchinskiy, S., 42, 217 Liu, L., 269, 470
Krause, E. G., 307 Landis, T., 173, 430 Lettvin, J. Y., 442, 443 Liu, L. Y., 382
Krawczak, M., 369 Landt, O., 370 Levai, O., 224 Liu, P., 401
Kreek, M. J., 77 Lang, P. J., 72 Levander, S., 370 Liu, X., 273
Kreiman, G., 447 Lange, W., 462 LeVay, S., 349, 350 Liu, Z.-W., 291
Kreiter, A. K., 450 Langford, G., 283 Levenson, R. W., 78 Livingston, K. E., 366
Kreitzer, A. C., 75, 257 Langston, J. W., 255 Leventhal, A. G., 178 Livingstone, M. S., 163, 182
Krekelberg, B., 186, 188 Långström, N., 347, 348 LeVere, T. E., 148, 277 Ljungberg, M. C., 257
Kress, B., 142 Langworthy, O. R., 91 Levi-Montalcini, R., 129, 130 Locantore, J. K., 139
Krieg, J.-C., 468 Lara, A. H., 219 Levin, E. D., 74 Lock, E., 437
Kriegeskorte, N., 185 LaRana, G., 75 Levine, A., 337 Lockhead, E., 218
Kringelbach, M. L., 360 Larkin, K., 351 Levine, A. S., 316 Lockwood, A. H., 200
Name Index 553

Loe, I. M., 138 MacVicar, B. A., 33 Martin, L. L., 358 McHugh, P. R., 312
Loewenstein, W. R., 204 Maes, F. W., 218 Martin, N. G., 13 McIntosh, A. R., 139
Loewi, O., 59 Maes, H. H. M., 13 Martin, P. R., 161 McIntyre, M., 334
Loewy, A. D., 307 Maffei, A., 177 Martin, R. C., 440 McIntyre, R. S., 320
Loftus, A., 451 Magee, J. J., 361, 427 Martin, S. D., 466 McKay, H. M., 396
Logan, C. G., 393 Magnuson, V. L., 347 Martindale, C., 160 McKay, K. M., 466
Löken, L., 206 Magoun, H. W., 279 Martinez, V., 269 McKay, R. D. G., 257, 383
Löken, L. S., 206 Maguire, D., 399 Martinez-Vargas, M. C., 333 McKeefry, D. J., 186
Lomber, S. G., 198 Maguire, E. A., 399, 400 Martinowich, K., 465 McKeever, W. F., 424, 430
Lømo, T., 234, 412 Mahlke, C., 260 Maruff, P., 288 McKemy, D. D., 204
Long, M. A., 271 Mahowald, M. W., 285 Marvin, E., 197 McKenzie, A., 137
Long, S. J., 349 Mai, N., 187, 188 Masland, R. H., 159 McKinley, M. J., 303
Lönnqvist, J., 468 Maier, S. F., 381, 382 Mason, M. F., 113 McKinnon, W., 382
Lönnqvist, J. K., 474 Maischein, H.-M., 125 Mason, P., 211 McKnight, S. L., 273
Lonsdorf, T. B., 371 Maj, M., 322 Mason, R. T., 298 McLellan, W. A., 302
López, M., 315 Majdan, M., 177 Mason, W. A., 373 McMenamin, B. W., 361
López-Barneo, J., 313 Makino, H., 126 Massa, F., 75 McMurray, J., 428
Lord, G. M., 315 Makous, W., 159 Massimini, M., 281, 290 McNaughton, B. L., 290, 401
Lorincz, A., 42 Malaspina, D., 476 Masterton, R. B., 198 McNeil, R., 159
Lorrain, D. S., 72, 333 Malenka, R. C., 177, 257 Mataga, N., 222 McNeill, D., 437
Lorusso, M. L., 442 Malhotra, P., 452 Mathalon, D. H., 478 McPhee, J. C., 41
Lott, I. T., 405 Malhotra, S., 198 Mathias, C. J., 358 McQuilkin, M., 144
Lotto, R. B., 163, 164, 165 Malia, A., 399 Mathies, R. A., 159 McStephen, M., 288
LoTurco, J., 441 Malinow, R., 405, 414 Mathur, U., 179 Meaden, P. M., 470
Lotze, M., 147 Mallic, B. N., 282 Matrisciano, F., 465 Mechelli, A., 437
Loughna, P. T., 234 Mallis, M. M., 269 Matsubara, S., 482 Meddis, R., 283
Loui, P., 197 Malmberg, A. B., 212 Matsumoto, Y., 288 Mednick, S. A., 290, 476
Lounasmaa, O. V., 111 Malterer, M. B., 466 Matsunami, H., 218 Mednick, S. C., 290
Lowell, B. B., 316 Mameli, M., 79 Matthews, G. A., 344 Meduna, L., 467
Lowenstein, G., 364 Manani, J. V., 306 Mattingley, J. B., 452 Meek, P. S., 78
Lozsádi, D. A., 93, 279 Mancuso, K., 165 Mattocks, C., 332 Meier, P. J., 34
Lu, A., 104 Mandl, R. C. W., 478 Matuszewich, L., 72, 333 Meiselman, H. L., 217
Lu, B., 465 Mandler, A., 183 Maurer, D., 177, 178, 184 Meister, M., 129, 224
Lu, T., 199 Maner, J. K., 225 Maurice, D., 292 Melcher, J. R., 199
Lucarelli, M. J., 358 Manes, F., 406 Maxwell, J. S., 361 Melloni, L., 447
Lucas, B. K., 338 Manfredi, M., 254 May, P. R. A., 141 Melone, M., 482
Lucas, R. J., 271 Mangan, M. A., 285 Mayberry, R. L., 437 Meltzer, H. Y., 482
Luciana, M., 76 Mangiapane, M. L., 307 Mayer, A. D., 338 Meltzoff, A. N., 243
Luczak, S. E., 80 Mangun, G. R., 425 Maze, I., 79 Melvin, L. S., 75
Luders, E., 119, 329, 429 Manji, H., 465 Mazziotta, J., 110 Melzack, R., 211, 212
Ludwig, H., 462 Mann, G. L., 281, 282 Mazziotta, J. C., 111 Menaker, M., 271
Ludwig, M., 64 Mann, J. J., 370 McAllen, R. M., 303 Menchetti, M., 466
Lugg, D. J., 383 Mann, T., 321 McArthur, A. J., 274 Mendel, G., 9
Luiz, A. C., 306 Männistö, P. T., 67 McBurney, D. H., 217 Mendelsohn, M., 126
Luna, B., 138, 252 Manns, J. R., 402 McCall, C., 422 Mendieta-Zéron, H., 315
Lund, J. S., 157 Mantyh, P. W., 209, 212 McCarley, R. W., 292 Menon, D. K., 141
Lund, R. D., 157 Manuck, S. B., 370 McCarthy, G., 185 Menon, V., 478
Lundin, A., 258 Manzino, L., 256 McCarthy, M. M., 331 Merabet, L. B., 134
Lussier, I. D., 369 Maquet, P., 242, 282, 290 McClarty, B., 383 Mergen, H., 320
Lutgendorf, S., 358 Maraganore, D. M., 254 McCleskey, E. W., 211 Mergen, M., 320
Lüthi, A., 378 Marcar, V. L., 187 McClintock, M. K., 225 Merrow, M., 269
Lutz, A., 451 Marcel, A. J., 450 McCloskey, M., 145 Merton, P. A., 235
Luu, P., 242 March, S. M., 77 McConnell, J., 410 Mervis, C. B., 436
Lyman, C. P., 289 Marchal, G., 186 McConnell, S. K., 132 Merzenich, M. M., 102, 146, 198
Lynall, M.-E., 478 Marchetti, C., 199 McCoy, A. N., 174 Meshi, D., 126
Lynn, A. B., 75 Marcinowska, A., 465 McCrea, A. E., 349 Messing, R. B., 370
Lytle, L. D., 370 Maricich, S. M., 204 McCrory, E., 442 Mesulam, M.-M., 93, 281
Lyytinen, H., 442 Mariño, G., 203 McDaniel, M. A., 118 Metzinger, T., 106
Macaluso, D. A., 219, 422 Marion, S. D., 430 McDonald, J. J., 447 Mevorach, C., 450
MacCabe, J. H., 476 Mark, G. P., 322 McElhiney, M., 427 Meyer, J. R., 137
Macdonald, D. W., 372 Markou, A., 74, 79 McEwen, B., 380 Meyer, K., 198
MacDonald, K., 144 Markowitsch, H. J., 375, 404 McEwen, B. S., 299 Meyer, U., 477
MacDonald, M. E., 259 Markowitz, J. D., 466 McEwen, G. N., Jr., 303 Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., 346
Macdonald, R. L., 376 Marois, R., 225 McFarland, B. R., 461 Meyerhof, W., 218
MacEvoy, S. P., 400 Marrett, S., 188 McGaugh, J. L., 395 Meyerhoff, J. L., 366
Macey, P. M., 283 Marris, E., 23 McGeary, J., 77 Meyerhoff, J. M., 366
MacFarlane, J. G., 283 Marrone, D. F., 126 McGehee, D. S., 74 Meyer-Lindenberg, A., 435, 436,
MacFarquhar, L., 17 Marsden, C. D., 481 McGinty, D., 293 477, 480
Machado, C. J., 373 Marshall, J., 173 McGivern, R. F., 331 Meyers, E., 179
MacIsaac, S. E., 144 Marshall, J. C., 452 McGlone, F., 206 Meyers, R., 80
Mackay, C. E., 477, 478 Marshall, J. F., 148 McGowan, P. O., 12 Mezzanotte, W. S., 284
MacLeod, D. I. A., 179 Martens, M. A., 436 McGrath, J., 476 Miall, C., 237
MacLusky, N. J., 332 Martin, C. E., 351 McGregor, I. S., 376 Michael, C. R., 193
Macphail, E. M., 117 Martin, E. R., 254 McGue, M., 13, 77 Michel, F., 138
Macrae, C. N., 198, 337 Martin, G., 159 McGuire, S., 13 Mickel, S. F., 404
554 Name Index

Mierson, S., 218 Moody, A. D., 178 Murray, R. M., 476, 478 Niebuhr, D. W., 477
Mihalcescu, I., 288 Moody, T. D., 403 Murrell, J., 405 Nielsen, D. A., 77
Mikolajczak, M., 337 Moore, F. R., 289 Mursch, K., 375 Niessing, J., 222
Mikulincer, M., 266 Moore, L. B., 464 Mushiake, H., 242 Nieuwenhuys, R., 96, 97, 302, 366,
Mikulis, D. J., 207 Moore, M. K., 243 Mutch, D. M., 321 425
Miles, F. A., 235 Moore, S., 327 Muzio, J. N., 291 Nikonenko, I., 414, 415
Milich, R., 310 Moore, S. C., 144 Myers, C. E., 403 Nilsson, G. E., 117
Miller, A. C., 426 Moore, T., 104, 173, 240 Myers, J. J., 426 Nimmo-Smith, I., 360
Miller, B. L., 74 Moore-Ede, M. C., 269, 278 Myers, M. G., Jr., 316, 318 Nims, P. J., 401, 402
Miller, C. A., 414 Morales, J. M., 333 Mylander, C., 437 Nir, Y., 292
Miller, E., 105 Moran, T. H., 312 Näätäen, R., 207 Nisenson, L., 427
Miller, E. K., 450 Morand, S. M., 138 Nachman, R., 477 Nishida, S., 163
Miller, G., 23, 336, 358 Moreno, A., 343 Nadal, A., 329 Nishimaru, H., 62
Miller, G. E., 382 Morfini, G. A., 259 Nadarajah, B., 125 Nishimura, Y, 143
Miller, G. W., 255 Morgan, B. E., 368 Nader, K., 395 Nishita, J. K., 331
Miller, J., 244, 474, 476 Morgan, C. A., III, 371 Nader, R., 291 Nitabach, M. N., 273
Miller, J. L., 374 Morgan, L. K., 400 Naef, M., 368 Nitsch, C., 349
Miller, R. D., 342 Morgan, V., 475 Naftolin, F., 332 Nitz, D. A., 251
Miller, R. J., 125 Mori, K., 222, 224 Nagarajan, S., 442 Noaghiul, S., 470
Miller, S. L., 225 Moritz, C. T., 240 Nagarajan, S. S., 137 Noda, M., 306
Mills, R., 331 Moriya, M., 186 Nagashma, K., 300 Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 461
Milne, A. B., 337 Morland, A. B., 163 Nah, S. Y., 211 Nomura, M., 307
Milner, A. D., 183, 242 Morley, J. E., 316 Nakamura, M., 300 Noonan, M. A., 79
Milner, B., 396, 397 Morlock, G. W., 148 Nakashima, Y., 343 Nordenström, A., 344, 346
Milner, P., 71 Moroney, D. N., 42, 217 Nakata, H., 159 Norgren, R., 219
Minard, A., 288 Morris, D. L., 142 Nakell, L., 429 Norman, D. A., 200
Ming, G.-l., 126 Morris, J. A., 329 Namgung, U., 416 Norman, R. A., 321
Minichiello, L., 414 Morris, J. S., 375 Narr, K. L., 119, 329 North, R. A., 63, 75
Minokoshi, Y., 315 Morris, M., 267, 283 Narrow, W. E., 461, 474 Noseda, R., 271
Minto, C. L., 346 Morris, N. M., 336 Narumoto, J., 361, 427 Nosenko, N. D., 349
Miozzo, M., 442 Morris, R. G. M., 415 Naselaris, T., 113 Nottebohm, F., 126
Mirksy, A. F., 373 Morrison, A. R., 25, 281, 282 Nassi, J. J., 157, 172, 182, 186 Nowak, M. A., 18
Mishkin, M., 435 Morrison, I., 206 Nataraj, K., 177 Nowak, P., 145
Mishra, J., 451 Morrison, J. H., 334 Nathans, J., 165 Nugent, F. S., 413
Misrahi, M., 343 Morrison, S. E., 372 National Stroke Association, 141 Numan, M., 338, 339, 356
Mistlberger, R. E., 269 Morrone, M. C., 188 Naumer, M. J., 226 Numan, M. J., 339
Mitchell, D., 288 Morrongiello, B. A., 194 Neal, A., 243 Nunes, S. O. V., 462
Mitchell, D. E., 178 Morse, J. R., 219 Neale, M. C., 347, 348, 376 Nusser, Z., 42
Mitchell, K. J., 226 Mortensen, P. B., 476 Nebes, R. D., 424 Nüsslein-Volhard, C., 125
Mitchison, G., 291 Morton, A. J., 285 Nef, P., 224 Nuutila, A., 370
Mitchum, R., 74 Morton, G. J., 314 Neiss, M. B., 334 Nystrom, L. E., 363
Mitra, P. P., 100, 101, 290 Moruzzi, G., 279 Neitz, J., 165 Ó Scalaidhe, S. P., 185
Miura, A., 159 Moscovitch, M., 396, 402 Neitz, M., 165 Obata, K., 131
Miyazawa, A., 63 Moskowitz, H. R., 217 Nelson, C. A., 131 O’Brien, R. C., 289
Mize, S. J. S., 218 Moss, C. F., 193 Nelson, D. O., 303 Obuchowicz, E., 465
Mochizuki, T., 285 Moss, S. J., 63 Nelson, R. J., 102, 371 Ochsner, K. N., 358
Modell, S., 468 Moustafa, A. A., 257 Nelson, S. B., 177 O’Connor, J. C., 382
Mody, I., 144 Moutoussis, K., 186 Nemeroff, C. B., 466 O’Connor, M., 399
Moeller, F. G., 370 Mrosovsky, N., 299 Nemes, A., 126 Oddo, S., 405
Mohammed, H., 146 Mrzljak, L., 482 Nestero, P. J., 406 O’Dowd, B. F., 64
Mohr, C., 430 Mucke, E., 405 Nestler, E. J., 12 Oelmann, H., 225
Mok, M. Y., 224 Mucke, L., 404 Netter, F. H., 328 O’Hara, M. W., 463
Molaison, H., 396 Mueller, K. L. O., 430 Netter, P., 370 O’Hearn, K., 138
Molina, J. C., 77 Mukand, J. A., 240 Nettle, D., 369 Ohira, K., 126
Molko, N., 114 Mulcare, C. A., 310 Neu, A., 75 Öhman, A., 362
Mölle, M., 291 Muldoon, M. F., 370 Neufer, P. D., 234 Ohmori, H., 45
Mombaerts, P. A., 224 Müller, H. D., 143 Neuhausser, W. M., 204 Ohno, E., 298
Monaco, A. P., 435 Muller, H. K., 383 Neumann, I. D., 337 Oka, Y., 222
Monaghan, E. P., 349 Müller, Johannes, 154 Neumeister, A., 465 Okada, T., 361, 427
Mondloch, C. J., 184 Muller, M. N., 342 Neville, H. J., 437, 438 Okado, H., 306
Money, J., 330, 344, 346 Müller, S., 274 Nevin, R., 367 Okaichi, H., 400, 415
Monfils, M.-H., 378 Mulligan, S. J., 33 Nevo, E., 271 O’Kane, G., 398
Mongillo, G., 396 Munafo, M. R., 461 New, M. I., 346 O’Keefe, J., 400
Monk, C. S., 461 Munk, M. H. J., 279 Newcomer, J. W., 481 Okubo, M., 451
Monk, T. H., 269 Munk-Jørgensen, P., 477 Newell, F. N., 226 Oláh, S., 65
Monro, L., 226 Munoz, D. P., 138, 252 Newman, T. K., 367 Olanow, C. W., 257
Monroe, S. M., 461 Muñoz, M., 271 Ngandu, T., 404 Olausson, H., 206
Montaguti, M. B., 466 Munro, J. C., 478 Nguyen, L. N., 405 Olazabal, D. E., 339
Monte, F., 119 Münzberg, H., 316 Niaura, R., 79 Olds, J., 71
Monteleone, P., 322 Murali, M. S., 461 Nicholas, M. K., 257 O’Leary, A., 381
Montgomery, K. J., 243 Murphy, D. L., 67 Nicholls, M. E. R., 451 Oler, J. A., 373
Montgomery, S. A., 465 Murphy, F. C., 360, 361 Nicklas, W. J., 255 Oliet, S. H. R., 75
Monti-Bloch, L., 224 Murphy, M. R., 337 Nicolelis, M. A. L., 102, 251 Olney, J. W., 481
Montmayeur, J.-P., 218 Murphy, T. H., 144 Nicoll, R. A., 75, 412 Olp, J. J., 342
Montrose, D. M., 479 Murray, G., 267 Nicolson, N. A., 461 Olson, D. J., 401
Name Index 555

Olson, E. J., 285 Parker, J., 23 Peru, A., 452 Poremba, A., 198
Olson, I. R., 406 Parks, C., 349 Pesold, C., 376 Porrino, L., 280
Olton, D. S., 400 Parnavelas, J. G., 125 Pessiglione, M., 428 Porter, J., 220, 222
Olufowobi, A., 338 Parr-Brownlie, L. C., 254 Pessoa, L., 450 Posel, B., 18
O’Malley, S. S., 80 Parsons, T. D., 197 Peteanu, L. A., 159 Posner, M. I., 13, 28
Omura, M., 222 Parton, L. E., 316 Peters, F., 461 Posner, S. F., 13
Onah, A. A., 280 Partonen, T., 468 Peters, R. H., 319 Post, R. M., 461
Oner, C., 320 Pascalis, O., 185 Peters, R. M., 204 Potegal, M., 366, 367
Oner, R., 320 Pascual, A., 130 Peterson, C. K., 367 Pothos, E., 322
Ono, K., 406 Pascual-Leone, A., 137 Peterson, E. W., 251 Poulsen, P., 12
Ono, M., 298 Pashler, H., 450 Peterson, M. A., 331 Powell, L. H., 321, 322
Onoda, N., 216 Passingham, D., 477, 478 Peterson, M. J., 395 Prasad, S., 173
Ootsuka, Y., 303 Passingham, R. E., 114, 244 Peterson, R. E., 346 Pratt, J., 119
Orban, G. A., 186 Pasterski, V. L., 344 Petersson, K. M., 211 Premack, A. J., 432
O’Regan, J. K., 450 Patel, A. D., 441 Petitto, L. A., 432, 438, 440 Premack, D., 432
Orlovsky, G. N., 237 Patil, S. T., 481 Petrie, M., 221 Prenger, R. J., 113
Ormandy, C. J., 338 Paton, J. J., 372 Petrovic, P., 211 Prescott, C. A., 461
Ornstein, R., 428 Patston, L. L. M., 452 Pettigrew, J. D., 157 Preskorn, S. H., 464
Orr, C. A., 451 Pattatucci, A. M. L., 347 Pfaff, D. W., 363 Preti, G., 225
Orzi, F., 74 Patterson, K., 406 Pfaffmann, C., 219 Pribram, K. H., 242, 373
Osborne, J., 126 Patterson, L. M., 316 Pfaus, J. G., 72 Price, C., 442
Oshima, T., 142 Patterson, P. H., 477 Pfefferbaum, A., 478 Price, C. J., 135, 318
Oshinsky, M. L., 273 Patterson, R. D., 199 Pfister-Genskow, M., 291 Price, D. D., 209
Ostrovsky, Y., 179 Paul, L. K., 430 Phan, K. L., 359, 360 Price, M. P., 204
Oswald, I., 283 Paul, S. M., 377 Phebus, L., 370 Priftis, K., 452
Otmakhov, N., 414 Paulesu, E., 442 Phelps, M. E., 111 Pritchard, T. C., 219, 422
Ottersen, O. P., 34 Paulson, O. B., 438 Philip, P., 267 Prober, D. A., 280
Ouchi, Y., 254 Paus, T., 110, 188 Phillips, A. G., 72 Prosser, C. L., 303
Ousley, A., 273 Pavani, F., 452 Phillips, B., 298 Provine, R. R., 237
Overduin, J., 312 Pavlov, I., 390 Phillips, M. L., 360 Prutkin, J., 220
Owen, A. M., 133, 446 Pavlova, G. A., 237 Phillips, N. H., 288 Ptak, R., 452
Owen, V., 107 Pawitan, Y., 256 Phillips, P. E. M., 75 Puca, A. A., 17
Oxenham, A. J., 199 Pearl, D. K., 244 Phuong, L., 406 Puce, A., 185
Oxley, D. R., 374 Pearson, A. J. D., 283 Piazza, M., 102 Purcell, D. W., 348
Oyama, O. N., 78 Pearson, H., 234 Picasso, L., 406 Purves, D., 133, 159, 163, 164, 165,
Ozata, M., 320 Peciña, S., 73 Pich, E. M., 74 174, 441
Ozdoba, J. M., 42, 217 Peckham, W., 349 Pickard, J. D., 141 Purves, H. R., 174
Pabst, D. A., 302 Pedersen, N. L., 256 Pierri, J. N., 478 Putnam, S. K., 333
Pace-Schott, E. F., 292 Peelen, M. V., 184 Pierson, D. L., 383 Putz, E., 427
Packer, R. J., 237 Peet, M., 474 Piet, R., 75 Pytte, C., 290
Padmanabhan, A., 138 Peeters, R., 4 Pietropaolo, S., 133 Queen, T. L., 139
Pakaprot, N., 393 Pegna, A. J., 173 Pillard, R. C., 347, 348 Quinn, N. P., 481
Palinkas, L. A., 288 Peigneux, P., 290 Pillon, B., 254 Rada, P. V., 322
Pallas, S. L., 132 Peineau, S., 412 Pilon, M., 285 Radoeva, P. D., 173
Paller, K. A., 290 Pelchat, M. L., 310, 311 Pinel, P., 102 Rae, D. S., 461
Pallie, W., 429 Peleg, G., 9 Pinker, S., 437, 439 Rafal, R., 107
Pallier, C., 114 Pelham, W. E., 310 Pinsker, H., 411 Raghavachari, S., 63
Pallier, P. N., 260 Pellis, S. M., 254, 331, 366 Pinto, L., 33 Ragland, J. D., 478
Palmeri, T. J., 225 Pellis, V. C., 254 Piomelli, D., 75 Ragsdale, D. S., 41
Palminteri, S., 428 Pellymounter, M. A., 315 Pistoia, F., 358 Raguse, J.-D., 218
Palmiter, R. D., 75 Pembrey, M. E., 435 Pittman, B., 80 Rahman, Q., 347, 348, 349
Palop, J. J., 404 Penagos, H., 199 Pizzagalli, D. A., 72, 463 Raimonde, A. J., 384
Palva, J. M., 207 Penfield, W., 103, 242, 396, 409 Pizzorusso, T., 177 Rainville, P., 208, 209
Palva, S., 207 Penhune, V. B., 136 Plailly, J., 224 Rais, M., 478
Pan, F., 133 Penick, S. C., 413 Plihal, W., 292 Raj, A., 13
Pandey, G. N., 370 Pennell, K. D., 255 Pliskin, N. H., 478 Rajaram, S., 396
Pandey, S. C., 77 Pennington, B. F., 119 Plomin, R., 13 Rajarethinam, R., 479
Panov, A. V., 259 Penton-Voak, I. S., 337 Ploner, M., 208 Rajkowska, G., 478
Pantev, C., 135 Pepperberg, I. M., 434 Plum, F., 318 Rakic, P., 124, 126, 177
Panula, P., 279 Perälä, J., 474 Plutchik, R., 356 Ralph, M. A. L., 406
Papafrangos, E. D., 289 Perani, D., 437 Podd, M. H., 478 Ralph, M. R., 271
Papas, B. C., 400 Pereira, F., 447 Poduslo, S. E., 17 Ramachandran, V. S., 147, 226
Paradiso, M. A., 177 Pereira, O. C. M., 331 Pol, H. E. H., 478 Ramchandani, V. A., 77
Pardal, R., 313 Perel, S., 240 Polat, U., 159 Ramirez, J. J., 144
Paré, D., 372, 412 Perera, T. D., 467 Poldrack, R. A., 251, 403 Ramírez, Y., 159
Paré, M., 204 Peretz, I., 197 Poling, A., 377 Ramirez-Amaya, V., 126
Paredes, R. G., 334 Pérez, N., 357 Polk, T. A., 450 Ramón y Cajal, S., 28–29, 52
Parent, M. B., 292 Perlmutter, S. I., 240 Pomeroy, W. B., 351 Ramsey, N. F., 368
Parisi, T., 137 Perlow, M. J., 257 Pommer, J., 292 Ramus, F., 441
Park, I., 248 Pernía-Andrade, A. J., 212 Poncer, J. C., 414 Randall, P. K., 319
Park, I.-H., 257 Perot, P., 409 Pons, T. P., 146 Ransley, P. G., 346
Park, K. F., 188 Perrone, J. A., 186 Pontieri, F. E., 74 Ranson, S. W., 101
Park, S., 478 Perry, S. H., 163 Poo, M., 416 Rantamäki, T., 465
Parker, C., 474 Pert, C., 74, 209 Poo, M.-M., 130 Raphael, H. M., 142
Parker, G. H., 215 Pert, C. B., 74 Pope, H. G., Jr., 75 Rapoport, S. I., 34
556 Name Index

Rapp, P. R., 334, 396 Riddle, W. J. R., 467 Rosen, A. C., 396 Sadri-Vakili, G., 12
Rasch, B., 292 Riegel, S., 462 Rosen, G. D., 441 Sagaspe, P., 267
Raschle, N. M., 441 Rieger, G., 347 Rosen, H. J., 361 Sage, J., 403
Rasmussen, T., 103, 242 Riemann, D., 468 Rosen, T. J., 149 Sage, J. R., 290
Ratcliff, M., 271 Rifkin, S. A., 13 Rosenau, B. J., 145 Sagi, D., 447
Rattenborg, N. C., 289, 291 Rigori, D., 245 Rosenblatt, J. S., 338, 339 Saha, S., 475
Rauch, S. L., 373 Rihel, J., 280 Rosenkranz, K., 137 Sahin, N. T., 439
Raum, W. J., 331 Rimmele, U., 337 Rosenzweig, M. R., 133 Sahuque, L. L., 77
Rauskolb, S., 130 Ringach, D., 23 Ross, D., 441 Saito, H.-A., 186
Rawlings, R., 370, 477 Ringel, B. L., 468 Ross, D. C., 470 Sakai, K., 279, 280
Rayner, K., 442 Ringleb, P., 142 Ross, D. J., 142 Sakal, R. R., 307
Redish, A. D., 291 Rinn, W. E., 240 Ross, E. D., 361 Sakmann, B., 54
Redmond, D. E., Jr., 257 Riolo, J. V., 72 Ross, J., 188 Sakurai, T., 280
Redondo, R. L., 415 Ripley, R. E., 472 Ross, R. J., 281, 282 Saleh, M., 240
Reed, D. R., 219, 220 Risch, N., 347, 461 Rossi, A. F., 450 Salmelin, R., 111, 442
Rees, G., 114, 207 Ritchie, J. M., 45 Rossi, S., 396 Salthouse, T. A., 133
Reeve, R., 351 Rittenhouse, C. D., 177 Rossignol, J., 260 Saltin, B., 234
Reeves, A. G., 318 Ritz, B., 256 Rosvold, H. E., 373 Salvatore, P., 470
Refinetti, R., 269, 271, 300 Rizzolatti, G., 243 Roth, B. L., 482 Salzman, C. D., 372
Regan, T., 23 Ro, T., 110, 226 Roth, H. P., 80 Samanta, J., 257
Regehr, W. G., 75 Roane, B. M., 468 Rothwell, J. C., 137, 467 Sampson, A., 478
Regier, D. A., 461 Robbins, T. W., 79, 257, 280, 371 Rotshtein, P., 396 Sampson, P. D., 77
Rehavi, M., 477 Roberson, E. D., 405 Rottenberg, J., 461 Sams, M., 106, 111
Reich, M. J., 319 Roberts, J. A., 396 Rousseaux, M., 451 Sanacora, G., 465
Reichling, D. B., 209 Roberts, K. C., 113 Routtenberg, A., 318, 416 Sánchez, I., 260
Reick, M., 273 Roberts, L. E., 200 Rouw, R., 225 Sánchez, M., 357
Reid, C. A., 416 Roberts, M. P., Jr., 366 Rovainen, C. M., 144 Sander, K., 361
Reid, R. C., 172 Roberts, N., 441 Rowland, D. L., 333, 336 Sanders, A. R., 475
Reiner, W. G., 345 Roberts, S. C., 221 Roy, A., 370 Sanders, M. D., 173
Reis, D. J., 372 Robertson, I. H., 106, 452 Royer, S., 412 Sanders, R. J., 432
Reisberg, B., 405 Robertson, L., 107 Rozin, P., 306, 310, 311, 390, 396 Sanders, S. K., 376
Reisert, I., 330 Robillard, T. A. J., 200 Rubens, A. B., 184 Sandkühler, J., 62
Reisner, A. D., 467 Robins, L. N., 461 Rubin, B. D., 222 Sandstrom, M. I., 260
Reiss, A. L., 435 Robinson, J. K., 75 Rubin, N., 243 Sanes, J. N., 240
Reith, M. E. A., 73 Robinson, P. J., 34 Rubinow, M. J., 299 Sanes, J. R., 128, 139
Ren, X., 339 Robinson, T. E., 72 Rubinstein, G., 474 Sanford, L. D., 281, 282
Rennaker, R. L., 224 Robin Wahlin, T.-B., 258 Ruch, K., 144 Sanger, T. D., 137
Rensch, B., 3 Robison, S., 367 Ruddock, K. H., 163 Santi, R. M., 377
Rensink, R. A., 450 Rockstroh, B., 135 Rudolph, U., 377 Sanyal, S., 271
Renthal, W., 12 Rodenberry, D., 399 Rudoy, J. D., 290 Saper, C. B., 303
Renz, C., 274 Rodman, H. R., 173 Ruff, C. C., 207 Sapienza, P., 119
Renzi, A., 306 Rodriguez, I., 224 Ruffino, M., 442 Sapolsky, R. M., 380, 383
Repp, A. B., 173 Roe, A. W., 182, 207 Ruhf, A., 307 Saporito, M., 255
Resko, J. A., 351 Roe, D., 438 Rujescu, D., 370 Sara, S. J., 291
Restrepo, C. E., 62 Roelfsema, P. R., 279 Rumbaugh, D. M., 432, 433 Sarter, M., 269
Reutens, D. C., 134, 436 Roenneberg, T., 267, 268, 269 Rumsby, P. C., 255 Sartori, G., 245
Reuter, M., 370 Roffwarg, H. P., 291 Rupp, A., 199 Sasaki, M., 298
Reuter-Lorenz, P., 361 Rogers, R. D., 244 Rupprecht, R., 335 Satinoff, E., 299, 303
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., 426 Rogers, T. T., 406 Rusak, B., 269 Sato, M., 177
Revusky, S., 80 Rogerson, T., 414 Rusconi, M. L., 430 Sato, S., 333
Reyes, A. D., 199 Roitman, M. F., 72 Rush, A. J., 464, 465 Satz, P., 478
Reyes, P., 447 Roizman, S., 225 Rushton, L., 255 Saunders, J. C., 197
Reyna, V. F., 138 Rojas, L. M., 159 Russell, A. J., 478 Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., 433
Reynolds, C. P., 382 Rokers, B., 186 Russell, J. L., 429 Savic, I., 225, 349
Reynolds, I. J., 142 Roland, P., 225 Russell, M. J., 225 Savion-Lemieux, T., 136
Reynolds, J. R., 243 Roland, P. E., 279 Russo, N. M., 135 Sawaguchi, T., 396
Reynoso, J. T., 79 Rolls, A., 144 Rüter, J., 183 Sawamoto, N., 254
Reznikov, A. G., 349 Rolls, E. T., 219, 318, 363, 400 Rütiger, L., 161 Scalora, M., 374
Rhees, R. W., 331 Rome, L. C., 234 Rutledge, J. N., 118 Schäbitz, W.-R., 143
Rhodes, J. S., 133 Romeo, R. D., 349 Rutstein, J., 303 Schacher, S., 411
Riad, J. K., 368 Romer, A. S., 127 Ruttenber, A. J., 255 Schacter, D., 404
Ricciardi, E., 243 Romero, E., 259 Rüttiger, L., 186 Schacter, D. L., 398
Rice, F. L., 204 Rommel, S. A., 302, 303 Ruyle, A. M., 224 Schaefer, H. S., 383
Rice, G., 347 Rönnback, L., 34 Ryge, J., 62 Schaeffer, M. A., 382
Rice, W. R., 348 Roorda, A., 161 Saad, W. A., 306 Schafe, G. E., 372
Richard, C., 451 Roozendaal, B., 395 Saarma, M., 130 Schal, C., 327
Richardson, G. S., 269 Ropelle, E. R., 315 Sabatinelli, D., 72 Schaller, M., 382
Richardson, U., 442 Roper, S. D., 217, 218 Sabo, K. T., 102 Schalling, D., 370
Richfield, E. K., 75 Roppel, R. M., 474 Sacchetti, B., 416 Scharf, M. B., 283
Richter, C., 266, 270 Rorden, C., 452. Sacco, T., 416 Scheer, F. A. J. L., 266
Richter, C. A., 331 Rosamond, W. D., 142 Sack, R. L., 269 Scheibel, A. B., 125, 405
Richter, C. P., 91, 193, 306 Rosano, C., 134 Sackeim, H. A., 427 Scheich, H., 361
Rickard, T. C., 290 Rose, J. E., 74, 197 Sacks, O., 185 Schenck, C. H., 285
Riddick, N. V., 369 Rose, R. J., 77 Sacktor, T. C., 416 Schenk, T., 183, 187
Ridding, M. C., 467 Roselli, C. E., 351 Sadato, N., 134, 361, 427 Schenker, N., 104, 198
Name Index 557

Scherer, S. S., 144 Segal, N. L., 13 Shizgal, P., 72 Smith, E. E., 450
Scherg, M., 199 Segerstrom, S. C., 382 Shobe, J., 414 Smith, F. J., 314
Scherrer, G., 211 Seghier, M. L., 185 Shohamy, D., 403 Smith, G. P., 312
Scheuer, T., 41 Sehgal, A., 273 Shore, A. B., 219 Smith, J. C., 147
Schienle, A., 361 Seidman, L. J., 473 Shoulson, I., 258 Smith, K., 446, 474
Schier, A. F., 280 Seitz, K. S., 424 Shouval, H. Z., 177 Smith, K. B., 374
Schiff, M., 199 Sejnowski, T. J., 288 Shrager, Y., 397 Smith, L. T., 410
Schiff, N. D., 143 Sekeres, M., 402 Shryne, J. E., 331 Smith, M. A., 258
Schiffman, S. S., 193, 217, 218 Selemon, L. D., 478 Shubin, N., 4 Smith, M. J., 424
Schiller, D., 378 Selkoe, D. J., 405 Shuman, T., 290 Smith, M. R., 450
Schlack, A., 186 Sellitto, M., 406 Shusta, E. V., 34 Smith, P. J., 134
Schlaug, G., 135, 137, 197 Selten, J. P., 474 Shutts, D., 105 Smith, R. L., 320
Schlinger, H., 377 Selye, H., 380, 384 Siderowf, A., 257 Smith, S. J., 129
Schlinger, H. D., 18 Selzer, M. E., 144 Sidwell, R. W., 477 Smith, T. L., 78
Schloerscheidt, A. M., 337 Semendeferi, K., 104 Siebert, E. R., 332 Smolen, A., 77, 79
Schmeichel, B. E., 279 Semin, G. R., 243 Siebert, M., 375 Smulders, T. V., 126
Schmid, A., 371 Seminowicz, D. A., 211 Siebner, H. R., 430 Snider, R. K., 199
Schmid, M. C., 173 Semjen, A., 424 Siegel, H. I., 338 Snitz, B. E., 416
Schmidt, L., 428 Semrud-Clikeman, M., 441 Siegel, J. M., 280, 288, 289, 291 Snowling, M. J., 442
Schmidt, L. A., 361 Sen, S., 465 Siegel, S., 79, 80 Snozzi, R., 368
Schmidt, N., 334 Senghas, A., 358 Siegelbaum, S. A., 416 Snyder, D., 463
Schmidt-Hieber, C., 126 Sensenig, L. D., 319 Sigmund, K., 18 Snyder, L. H., 242
Schmidtke, J., 369 Serences, J. T., 145 Sigmundson, H. K., 346 Snyder, S., 74, 209
Schmitt, J. A. J., 311 Sereno, A. B., 474 Silber, B. Y., 311 Snyder, S. G., 282
Schmitt, K. C., 73 Sergent, C., 449 Silber, M. H., 285 Snyder, S. H., 74
Schmitz, S., 349 Serrano, P., 416 Silberstein, S. D., 461 Sobel, N., 428
Schneider, B. A., 194 Serritella, C., 322 Silk, J. B., 17 Soczynska, J. K., 320
Schneider, F., 147 Serruya, M. D., 240 Silva, A. J., 414 Soeter, M., 378
Schneider, P., 135 Servin, A., 344 Silver, R. A., 57 Soininen, H., 404
Schnider, A., 404, 452 Settle, R. G., 224 Simard, A., 142 Solms, M., 279, 292, 404
Schnitzler, A., 208 Sevcik, R. A., 433 Simion, F., 184, 185 Solomon, S. G., 161
Schnitzler, H.-U., 371 Seyfarth, R. M., 383 Simmons, A. M., 193 Soltesz, I., 75
Schoenlein, R. W., 159 Shackman, A. J., 361 Simner, J., 226 Somjen, G. G., 33
Scholte, H. S., 225 Shadmehr, R., 258 Simon, E., 306 Sommer, C., 143
Schomacher, M., 143 Shadwick, R. E., 300 Simons, D. J., 133, 451 Sommerville, R. B., 363
Schomer, D., 439 Shah, B., 298 Simons, J. S., 105 Song, H., 126
Schomers, M. R., 237 Shah, N. M., 331, 349 Simpson, J. A., 337, 341 Sonsalla, P., 256
Schotland, P., 273 Shakelford, T. K., 342 Simpson, J. B., 307 Soon, C. S., 245
Schou, M., 469 Shalev, A. Y., 383 Sincich, L. C., 188 Sorger, B., 185
Schroeder, J. A., 218 Shalev, L., 450 Sing, H. C., 266 Sotak, B. N., 75
Schroeder, J. P., 73 Shank, C. V., 159 Singer, T., 209 Southwick, S. M., 371
Schuckit, M. A., 78 Shapiro, C. M., 288 Singer, W., 279, 447 Sowell, E. R., 130
Schulkin, J., 306 Shapiro, E., 367 Singh, S., 282 Spalding, K. L., 126
Schull, J., 390 Sharbaugh, S. M., 309 Singleton, A. B., 254 Spalding, M. C., 240
Schulman, H., 414 Sharma, J., 132 Singleton, J., 437 Spangler, R., 322
Schultz, R., 118 Sharpe, L. T., 165 Sinha, P., 179 Spear, N. E., 77
Schurger, A., 447 Shatz, C. J., 129, 132, 177 Sinigaglia, C., 243 Speer, N. K., 243
Schutter, D. J. L. G., 368, 369 Shaughnessy, M. F., 141 Sinkkonen, S. T., 377 Spelke, E. S., 119
Schwab, M. E., 144 Shea, S. D., 222 Sirigu, A., 147, 207 Spence, C., 452
Schwab, S., 143 Shechter, R., 144 Sirotin, Y. B., 112 Spence, I., 119
Schwartz, A. B., 240 Sheehan, T. P., 339 Sisodia, S., 405 Spencer, R. M. C., 248
Schwartz, C. E., 373 Shekhar, A., 376 Sisson, R. W., 80 Sperling, A. J., 126
Schwartz, G. J., 312 Shelton, D., 110 Sitomer, M. T., 383 Sperry, L., 376
Schwartz, J. H., 411 Shelton, S. E., 373 Sjöström, M., 234 Sperry, R. W., 127, 424, 426
Schwartz, L., 80 Shema, R., 416 Skaredoff, L., 366 Spiegel, T. A., 312
Schwartz, M., 144, 346 Shen, H., 376 Skene, D. J., 269 Spindler, K. A., 478
Schwartz, M. F., 242 Sher, L., 370 Skitzki, J. J., 303 Spinelli, D., 442
Schwartz, M. W., 314 Sherk, H., 178 Skoe, E., 135 Spinelli, S., 372
Schwarzenbacher, K., 224 Sherman, G. F., 441 Slachevsky, A., 447 Spiro, A., III, 17
Schweinhardt, P., 211 Sherman, S. J., 257 Slater, P. J. B., 6 Spoonster, E., 384
Schweitzer, P., 75 Sherrington, C., 29, 52, 53, 238 Sloan, A. M., 224 Spotts, J. L., 282
Schwob, J. E., 224 Shettleworth, S. J., 21 Sloan, D. M., 461 Spreux-Varoquaux, O., 370
Scognamigli, P., 322 Shi, J., 80 Slob, A. K., 336 Spurzheim, J. G., 115
Scott, A. I. F., 467 Shi, L., 477 Slobodskaya, H. R., 361 Squire, L. R., 396, 397, 400, 401,
Scott, D. J., 211 Shibasaki, H., 254 Sluming, V., 441 403, 404, 416
Scott, J., 5 Shiflett, M. W., 126 Smallman, H. S., 179 Squires, T. M., 203
Scott, S. H., 242 Shih, R. A., 461 Smania, N., 147, 452 St George, M., 436
Scott, S. K., 135 Shima, K., 242 Smart, T. G., 63 St George-Hyslop, P. H., 405
Scott, W. K., 254 Shimojo, S., 163 Smielewski, P., 141 St. Jacques, P. L., 383
Scoville, W. B., 396 Shimpi, A., 163 Smith, A. M., 204 Stadelman, H., 351
Seal, R. P., 212 Shimura, H., 254 Smith, C. L., 333 Staehler, F., 218
Seckl, J. R., 337 Shin, L. M., 373 Smith, C. N., 416 Staggs, D., 173
Seeherman, K. R., 243 Shine, R., 298 Smith, C. T., 291 Stahl, B. H., 476
Seeley, R. J., 312 Shipp, S., 182 Smith, D. E., 396 Stalnaker, T. A., 74
Seeman, P., 479, 480 Shiv, B., 73, 364 Smith, D. L., 260 Stam, C. J., 478
558 Name Index

Stanford, L. R., 37 Swaab, D. F., 349, 351 Terman, J. S., 470 Treisman, A., 107, 447, 450
Stanley, J., 237 Swallow, D. M., 310 Terman, M., 470 Treit, D., 376
Stanwood, G. D., 131 Swallow, K. M., 243 Terrace, H. S., 432 Trenkle, M. K., 369
Starbuck, E. M., 307 Swan, S. H., 331, 333 Terry, R. D., 405 Trevena, J. A., 244
Stark-Adamec, C., 366 Swart, M., 371 Terwilliger, E. F., 339 Trim, R. S., 78
Starr, C., 65, 66, 206, 233, 329 Sweeney, J. A., 479 Terwilliger, R., 138 Trimble, M. R., 467
Steer, C. D., 367 Swithers, S. E., 321 Teslovich, T., 138 Trinh, K., 256
Steeves, J., 185 Switz, G. M., 225 Tetrud, J. W., 255 Trivedi, M. H., 464, 465
Stefansson, H., 12, 475 Swoboda, E., 467 Thalemann, R., 79 Trivers, R. L., 18
Stein, D. G., 141 Swoboda, H., 376 Thangaraj, V., 240 Troughton, E., 367
Stein, M. B., 383 Syken, J., 177 Thanickal, T. C., 285 Trudel, E., 305
Stein, R., 74 Syme, D. A., 300 Thapar, A., 131 Tsai, G., 77, 481
Steinberg, L., 138 Szabó, G., 377 Thase, M. E., 464, 466 Tsankova, N., 12, 13
Steiner, T., 142 Szuba, M. P., 468 Theusch, E., 197 Tseng, A., 271
Steinhoff, B. J., 375 Szymusiak, R., 280, 281, 293 Thiele, A., 186 Tsien, R. W., 63
Stella, N., 75 Tabakoff, B., 77, 377 Thiele, C. M., 242 Ts’o, D. Y., 182
Stellflug, J. N., 351 Tabar, V., 257 Thier, P., 249 Tsodyks, M., 396
Stellick, R. L., 279 Tabibnia, G., 351 Thomas, C., 185 Tsujita, T., 474
Stephan, K. E., 242 Tabin, C., 4 Thomas, M. G., 310 Tu, Y., 77
Stephens, T. W., 316 Tabrizi, S. J., 258 Thomas, O., 173 Tucker, D. M., 242
Stepper, S., 358 Taddese, A., 211 Thompson, B. L., 131 Tulipan, L., 80
Stern, G., 257 Tadi, T., 106 Thompson, G. F., 474 Tuomilehto, J., 404
Stern, M., 257 Tagawa, Y., 177 Thompson, J. K., 393, 394 Tups, A., 315
Stern, W. E., 318 Tager-Flusberg, H., 436 Thompson, K., 225 Turati, C., 184, 185
Stern, Y., 442 Taggart, R., 65, 66, 206, 233, 329 Thompson, M. E., 342 Turk, D. J., 426
Sterpenich, V., 279 Taghert, P. H., 273 Thompson, P. J., 467 Turnbull, O., 404
Stevens, C. F., 126, 159 Taillard, J., 267 Thompson, P. M., 130, 429 Turner, A., 293
Stevens, J. K., 31 Takahashi, K., 279 Thompson, R., 172 Turner, R. S., 251
Stevens, T., 436 Takahashi, M., 416 Thompson, R. F., 372, 393, 394 Turrigiano, G. G., 177
Stevenson, R. J., 422 Takahashi, T., 414 Thompson, W. L., 172 Turtle, M., 125
Steward, O., 400 Takahashi, Y. K., 222 Thorell, L. G., 176 Tuszynski, M. H., 396
Stewart, H., 178 Takano, T., 33 Thornhill, R., 342 Twenge, J. M., 461
Stewart, J. W., 465 Takao, M., 271 Thorpe, L. A., 194 Tybur, J. M., 336
Stewart, M. A., 367 Takashima, A., 416 Tierney, M. C., 144 Tye, K. M., 417
Stickgold, R., 292, 399 Takatsuru, Y., 143 Tiippana, K., 106 Tyszka, J. M., 374
Stocchi, F., 254 Takehara-Nishiuchi, K., 401 Timmerman, L., 208 Uchida, N., 222
Stocco, A., 96, 97 Tamietto, M., 173 Timms, B. G., 331 Uchida, Y., 300
Stockman, A., 165 Tan, K. R., 377 Tinbergen, N., 1, 4, 24 Udry, J. R., 336, 347
Stoerig, P., 173 Tanaka, J., 307 Tingate, T. R., 383 Uekita, T., 415
Stokes, M., 172 Tanaka, J. N., 185 Tippet, L. J., 452 Uhr, M., 466
Stone, V. E., 427 Tanaka, K., 186 Tishkoff, S. A., 310 Umiltà, C., 452
Storey, J. M., 301 Tanaka, M., 303, 322 Tiwari, S. K., 461 Underwood, M. D., 370
Storey, K. B., 301 Tanaka, Y., 198 Tobin, V. A., 338 Ungerleider, L. G., 450
Stormshak, F., 351 Tanda, G., 74 Tobler, I., 289 Unkelbach, C., 337
Stout, A. K., 142 Tandon, S., 146 Tocilj, A., 301 Unterberg, A. W., 142
Stover, J., 142 Tangel, D. J., 284 Toga, A., 110 Unwin, N., 63
Stowe, R. P., 383 Tanifuji, M., 222 Toga, A. W., 130, 329, 429 Uppenkamp, S., 199
Strack, F., 358 Tanji, J., 242 Toh, K. L., 273 Urry, H. L., 361
Strauss, M. E., 461 Tanner, C. M., 255 Toien, Ø., 289 U. S. Food and Drug
Streissguth, A. P., 77 Tanskanen, A. J., 474 Tokizawa, K., 300 Administration, 464
Strick, P. L., 249 Tanzi, R. E., 405 Tomaiuolo, F., 452 U. S. Supreme Court, 138
Stricker, E. M., 306, 307, 313, 320 Taravosh-Lahn, K., 369 Tomchik, S. M., 218 U. S.–Venezuela Collaborative
Strickland, T. L., 74 Tarr, M. J., 186 Tominaga, M., 212 Research Project, 258, 259
Striemer, C. L., 173 Tarsy, D., 137 Tomo, I., 197 Vaag, A., 12
Stroebele, N., 321 Tashiro, A., 126 Tonegawa, S., 212 Vacca, L., 254
Strotmann, J., 224 Tassinari, G., 219, 422 Tong, F., 450 Vaccarino, A. L., 212
Stryker, M. P., 177, 178 Tatsuno, M., 290 Tong, Q., 316 Vaccarino, F. J., 376
Studer, L., 257 Tattersall, G. J., 298 Toni, N., 414, 415 Vaid, J., 437
Stunkard, A. J., 320 Taub, E., 135, 148 Tononi, G., 281, 290, 291, 292 Vaishnavi, S., 452
Stylos-Allan, M., 290 Taube, J. S., 400 Toomey, R., 74 Valente, E. M., 254
Sugari, T., 216 Taylor, A. N., 331 Tootell, R. B. H., 186 Vallbo, A., 206
Sugita, Y., 186 Taylor, C. S. R., 104, 240 Torchia, M., 383 Valli, I., 481
Sullivan, E. V., 149, 478 Taylor, D. J., 468 Torrey, E. F., 474, 476, 477, 478 Vallines, I., 188
Sun, Y.-G., 142, 212 Taylor, J. P., 406 Toscano, J. E., 373 Valverde, M. A., 329
Sunaert, S., 186 Taylor, M. A., 474 Toufexis, D., 372 Valzelli, L., 319, 369, 482
Sunderland, T., 207 Taylor, S. F., 360 Touhara, K., 222 van Anders, S. M., 334
Sung, R.-J., 280 Taziaux, M., 333 Touretzsky, D. S., 291 Van Bavel, J. J., 373
Sur, M., 102, 132, 159 Tchernichovski, O., 290 Townsend, J., 249 Van Cantfort, T. E., 432
Sutton, B. C., 269 Teff, K. L., 321 Trainor, B. C., 371 van den Bosch, J. J. F., 226
Sutton, L. C., 211 Teicher, M. H., 470 Tran, P. B., 125 van den Heuvel, M. P., 478
Sutton, R. L., 144 Teitelbaum, P., 254, 317 Tranel, D., 358, 363, 374, 375, 398, van den Pol, A. N., 316
Suvisaari, J. M., 474 Teixeira, C. M., 126 427 Van der Borght, K., 133
Suzdak, P. D., 377 Temel, Y., 257 Travers, S. P., 219 Van der Does, A. J. W., 370
Svartdal, F., 225 Terayama, H., 462 Trefilov, A., 369 van der Meer, M. A. A., 291
Svoboda, K., 414 Terburg, D., 337 Trehub, S. E., 194 van der Vegt, B. J., 371
Name Index 559

van der Wertff ten Bosch, J. J., 336 Wager, T. D., 211, 360 Weinberger, D. R., 476, 478, 479 Wilson, M. A., 290
Van der Zee, E. A., 133 Wagner, A. D., 112 Weiner, I., 477 Wilson, R. I., 75
Van Dorp, C. F., 269 Wagner, E. L., 301 Weinshenker, D., 73 Wilson, R. S., 133
Van Essen, D. C., 182, 183 Wagner, U., 290, 292 Weiskopf, N., 243 Wilson, S. J., 134, 436
Van Eys, P. L., 424 Waisbren, S. R., 14 Weiskrantz, L., 173 Wilson, S. M., 438
Van Grootel, T. J., 200 Waldherr, M., 337 Weiss, A. P., 478 Windhaber, J., 376
Van Hecke, P., 186 Waldie, K. E., 424 Weiss, P., 126, 127 Winer, G. A., 153
van Honk, J., 337, 368, 369 Waldvogel, J. A., 157 Weiss, P. H., 225 Winfree, A. T., 270
van Leeuwen, M., 119 Walker, J. A., 400 Weisse, C. S., 382 Winkels, W. A. M., 401
van Lieshout, D. P., 168 Walker, M. P., 290 Weissman, D. H., 113 Winkler, C., 257
van Meer, M. P. A., 143 Wall, P. D., 211 Weitzman, E. D., 278 Winn, R. J., 349
Van Opstal, A. J., 195, 200, 201 Wall, T. L., 80 Welchman, A. E., 237 Winocur, G., 267, 402
van Oudenaarden, A., 13 Wallen, K., 332 Welham, J., 476 Winslow, J. T., 372
van Praag, H., 126, 133 Wallesch, C.-W., 438 Weller, A., 225, 337 Wirdefeldt, K., 255, 256
van Rijn, P. M., 200 Wallis, J. D., 219 Weller, L., 225 Wirz-Justice, A., 274
van Rijn, S., 371 Wallman, J., 157 Weng, Y. C., 142 Wise, R. P., 157
Van Wanrooij, M. M., 195, 201 Walsh, T., 476 Weniger, G., 375 Wisner, K. L., 461
Van Zoeren, J. G., 303 Walsh, V., 110, 243, 423 Wenkstern, D., 72 Wissman, A. M., 126
Vandertie, D., 343 Walters, E. T., 212 Werblin, F. S., 158 Witelson, S. F., 118, 429
VanderLaan, D. P., 348 Wampold, B. E., 466 Werth, E., 274 Wohlgemuth, M. J., 188
Vanduffel, W., 186 Wan, C. Y., 134 Wessberg, J., 206 Woldorff, M. G., 113
Vanek, Z. F., 403 Wang, A., 200 Wessinger, C. M., 173, 199 Wolf, S., 356
vanHuijzen, C., 96, 302, 366, 425 Wang, A. C. J., 334 Westerberg, C. E., 290 Wolff, P. H., 442
Vann, S. D., 399 Wang, A. H., 126 Westergaard, G. C., 369 Wolkin, A., 477
Vargas-Irwin, C. E., 240 Wang, D. O., 415 Wettstein, J. G., 288 Wolpert, L., 131
Vargha-Khadem, F., 435 Wang, J. L., 461 Whalen, P. J., 375, 376, 383 Womelsdorf, T., 447
Vargo, E. L., 327 Wang, P. O., 436 Wheeler, M. E., 107, 252 Wong, A. T., 398
Vasey, P. L., 348 Wang, Q., 159 Wheeler, R. A., 72 Wong, K., 479, 480
Vasquez, B., 334 Wang, S. S.-H., 100, 101 Wheye, D., 298 Wong, P. C. M., 135
Vawter, M. P., 330 Wang, T., 14 Whishaw, I. Q., 331 Wong, R. O. L., 129
Velanova, K., 138, 252 Wang, W., 437 White, A. J. R., 161 Wong-Riley, M. T. T., 35
Velliste, M., 240 Wang, W. C., 303 White, D. P., 284 Wood, A. G., 134
Venna, V. R., 474 Wang, X., 199 White, L. E., 177 Woodberry, K. A., 473
Verhage, M., 130 Wang, Y. T., 412 White, L. E., Jr., 209 Woodbury, M. A., 218
Verleger, R., 290 Wang, Z., 339 Whitford, A. S., 240 Woodhouse, C. R. J., 346
Verrey, F., 307 Wapner, W., 438 Whitman, B. W., 237 Woods, R., 332
Versace, F., 72 Warach, S., 114, 240 Whitten, P. L., 383 Woodside, B., 338, 356
Vervliet, B., 378 Ward, B., 349 Whyte, J., 144 Woodson, J. C., 331
Vessal, M., 126 Ward, I. L., 331, 349 Wichmann, T., 254 Woodworth, G., 367
Vetencourt, J. F. M., 465 Ward, J., 226 Wicker, B., 243, 360 Woodworth, R. S., 46
Vezina, P., 74 Ward, L. M., 447 Widom, C. S., 367 Woolf, N. J., 97, 279
Victor, J. D., 217 Ward, O. B., 331, 349 Wienbruch, C., 135 Woolsey, T. A., 366
Vighetti, S., 211 Ward, R., 107 Wiens, S., 362 Worley, P. F., 126
Viken, R. J., 77 Warren, R. M., 197 Wiesel, T., 173, 174, 175, 177, 178 Worsley, K. J., 188
Vila, J., 357 Warrington, E. K., 173 Wightman, R. M., 72, 75 Wrangham, R. W., 342
Vilain, E., 348 Wassef, M., 132 Wigström, H., 415 Wray, N. R., 371, 461
Vingiano, W., 427 Wasserman, L. M., 165 Wilcox, T., 332 Wright, C. I., 373
Vinogradova, Y., 474 Wassermann, E. M., 144 Wild, H. M., 186 Wright, E. W., 244
Virkkunen, M., 77, 370 Wassum, K. M., 75 Wilensky, A. E., 372 Wright, I. C., 477
Visscher, K. M., 113 Watanabe, D., 136 Wiles, J., 126 Wright, K. P., Jr., 266
Visser, E. K., 271 Watanabe, E., 306 Wiley, E., 437 Wu, F. C. W., 368
Viswanathan, A., 112 Watanabe, M., 252 Wilkin, G. P., 257 Wu, S. S., 257
Vitek, J. L., 254 Watkins, K. E., 435 Wilkins, K., 320 Wulfeck, B., 439
Vliegen, J., 200 Watkins, L. R., 381, 382 Willems, R. M., 440 Wulff, K., 288
Vocci, F. J., 81 Watkins, S. S., 74 Willerman, L., 118, 349 Wurtman, J. J., 62
Voke, J., 165 Watrous, S., 147 Williams, B. A., 303 Wyart, C., 225
Volavka, J., 78 Watson, N. V., 334 Williams, C. C., 119 Wynne, C. D. L., 1
Volk, C. L. E., 478 Watson, S. J., 333 Williams, C. L., 279, 330 Wynne, L. C., 475
Volkow, N. D., 79 Wattendorf, E., 254 Williams, D. R., 161 Xu, H. S., 197
vom Saal, F. S., 331 Watts, C., 257 Williams, G., 316 Xu, H.-T., 133
von Békésy, G., 197 Waxman, S. G., 45 Williams, G. A., 165 Xu, T., 414
von Cramon, D., 188 Waye, H., 298 Williams, K. D., 212 Xu, Y., 273
von der Ohe, C. G., 289 Weaver, I. C. G., 12 Williams, M. T., 349 Yager, S., 382
von Gall, C., 273 Webb, W. B., 288 Williams, R. W., 28, 248 Yamada, M., 406
von Melchner, L., 132 Weber, L., 477 Williams, S. C. R., 118 Yamadori, A., 198
Voogd, J., 96, 97, 302, 366, 425 Weber-Fox, C. M., 437 Williamson, A., 137 Yamaguchi, S., 271
Voss, J. L., 290 Webster, F. A., 193 Williamson, A. C., 396 Yamamoto, S., 37
Vouimba, R.-M., 372 Webster, J. P., 372 Willingham, D. B., 251, 258 Yamamoto, T., 219
Vrba, E. S., 124 Weddle, M. G., 376 Willins, D. L., 482 Yanagawa, Y., 62, 131
Vuga, M., 463 Wedell, A., 344 Wilson, B. A., 390 Yanagisawa, K., 219
Vuillermot, S., 477 Wegener, D., 450 Wilson, D. A., 224 Yang, A. K., 464
Vuilleumier, P., 375 Wehr, T. A., 470 Wilson, F. A. W., 185 Yang, C.-H., 126
Vul, E., 176 Wei, W., 405 Wilson, G. D., 347, 348, 349, 361 Yang, G., 133, 316
Vyazovskiy, V. V., 291 Weidensaul, S., 300 Wilson, J. D., 331 Yang, J.-C., 376
Waanders, R., 467 Weiller, C., 251 Wilson, K. D., 185 Yang, Z., 273
560 Name Index

Yantis, S., 145 Yoon, H.-K., 376 Zablow, L., 416 Zheng, H., 316
Yasuhara, S., 300 Yoon, K. L., 447 Zacks, J. M., 243 Zhou, F., 289
Yasui, E., 138 Yoshida, J., 224 Zadra, A., 285 Zhou, S. F., 464
Yates, W. R., 367 Yoshida, K., 303 Zaffuto, S., 416 Zhou, Y., 414
Yavich, L., 67 Yoshida, M., 198 Zagoory-Sharon, O., 337 Zhu, Y., 273, 284
Ye, C.-P., 316 Yoshie, M., 159 Zak, P. J., 337 Ziegler, J. C., 441
Ye, H., 338 Yoshimura, H., 216 Zakharenko, S. S., 416 Zihl, J., 187, 188
Yeap, B. B., 462 Young, A. B., 258 Zandi, P. P., 461 Zimmerman, J. C., 278
Yee, B. K., 133, 481 Young, A. W., 361 Zatorre, R. J., 199 Zingales, L., 119
Yeh, H. H., 131 Young, L. J., 339 Zeevalk, G. D., 256 Zinni, M., 451
Yeh, M. T., 367 Young, M. A., 470 Zeevi, Y. Y., 158 Zipser, B. D., 35
Yeh, P. W. L., 131 Young, R. C., 312 Zegarra-Moran, O., 442, 443 Zoccolotti, P., 442
Yehuda, R., 383, 384 Young, S. N., 370 Zeineh, M. M., 396 Zola, S. M., 400
Yellachich, L.-A., 475 Young, T., 159 Zeitzer, J. M., 280 Zorrilla, E. P., 382
Yeo, R. A., 118 Young, W. G., 312 Zeki, S., 163, 182, 186 Zorzi, M., 442, 452
Yeomans, J. S., 371, 376 Youngentob, S. L., 224 Zelaznik, H. N., 248 Zubieta, J.-K., 211
Yeshurun, Y., 428 Youngstedt, S. D., 269 Zhang, T. Y., 12 Zubrick, S. R., 475
Yin, H. H., 251, 254, 255 Yousem, D. M., 224 Zhang, W., 119 Zucker, I., 269
Yiu, G., 144 Yovel, G., 185, 186 Zhang, X., 222, 260 Zucker, K. J., 346, 348
Yogo, Y., 343 Yu, T. W., 128 Zhang, Y., 314 Zuckerman, L., 477
Yolken, R. H., 477 Yuan, J., 260 Zhao, Y., 73 Zurif, E. B., 438, 439
Yonekura, Y., 361, 427 Yule, M., 334 Zhao, Z., 372 Zusho, H., 224
Yoo, S.-S., 290 Yurgelun-Todd, D., 75 Zheng, B., 273 Zwiebel, L. J., 193
Subject Index/Glossary
Note: Italicized page numbers refer to figures, illus- Adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) chemical Amino acids acids containing an amine group
trations, and tables. released from the anterior pituitary gland, (NH2), 60, 61, 61
which enhances metabolic activity and ele- brain and, 34, 35
Abducens nerve, 92 vates blood levels of sugar, 66, 67, 68, 343, DNA/RNA and, 10
Ablation removal of a brain area, generally with a 380 PKU diet and, 14
surgical knife, 109 Aerobic requiring the use of oxygen during move- Amisulpride, 482
Absolute pitch, 197 ments, 234 Amnesia memory loss, 396
Absolute refractory period a time when the mem- Afferent axon axon that brings information into a Alzheimer’s disease and, 404, 405
brane is unable to produce an action poten- structure, 32, 32 basal ganglia and, 403, 404
tial, 43 Affinities, 71 Korsakoff ’s syndrome and, 404
Accessory nerves, 92 Age results and patients with, 406
Acetaldehyde, 80 circadian rhythms and, 267, 267, 268, 269 types of, 396–398, 399
Acetyl groups, 12, 13 sleep patterns and, 291, 291 AMPA receptor a glutamate receptor that can
Acetylcholine a chemical similar to an amino acid, Agonist a drug that mimics or increases the effects respond to a-amino-3-hydrozy-5-methyl-4-
except that it includes an N(CH3)3 group of a neurotransmitter, 71 isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA), 413–416,
instead of an NH2, group 60, 60 Agouti-related peptide (AgRP) inhibitory trans- 414, 415
Alzheimer’s disease and, 405 mitter that blocks the satiety actions of the Amphetamine a drug that blocks reuptake of
excitatory, 63 paraventricular nucleus, 316 dopamine and other neurotransmitters, 73,
motor neurons and, 62 AgRP. See Agouti-related peptide (AgRP) 74, 76
as a neurotransmitter, 61 AIDS, 336, 349, 350 Amplitude the intensity of a sound wave, 194
postganglionic axons and, 91 Alarm (stress stage), 380 AMPT (radioactively labeled drug), 480
receptor, 63, 63 Alcohol, 77 Amputated limb, 146–147
skeletal muscles and, 232 anxiety and, 377, 378 Amusia, 197
wakefulness and sleep, 281, 282 brain damage recovery behavior and, 149 Amygdala
Across-fiber principle idea that each receptor brain development and, 131 anxiety and, 371
responds to a wide range of stimuli and con- demasculinization of early development, 331 damage to, 372, 374–375, 376
tributes to the perception of every stimulus disulfiram (Antabuse) and, 463 emotions and, 359, 360, 361
in its system, 215 fetal alcohol syndrome and, 131, 131 fear and, 374–375
ACTH. See Adrenocorticotropic hormone genetics and, 77 learned fears and, 372, 372
(ACTH) heritability and, 14 location of, 366
Action potentials messages sent by axons, 40 medications to combat, 80, 81 PTSD and, 384
all-or-none law and, 41 risk factors of, 77, 78, 78 responses and, 363, 373, 373
auditory neurons and, 196, 197 Ro15-4513 and, 377 visual stimuli and, 373
local neurons and, 45, 46 sexual orientation and prenatal, 349 Amyloid-b a protein that accumulates to higher
molecular basis of, 41, 42 thiamine deficiency and, 35, 404 than normal levels in the brains of people
movements of ions during, 41, 42 water regulation and, 305 with Alzheimer’s disease, 405, 406
myelin sheath and, 44, 45 Alcohol dependence (alcoholism) the habitual Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, 246
neurotransmitter and, 60 use of alcohol despite medical or social Anaerobic proceeding without using oxygen at the
presynaptic terminal and, 60 harm, 77 time of a reaction, 234
propagation of, 43, 44 Alcoholics Anonymous, 80 Anandamide chemical that binds to cannabinoid
refractory period and, 42, 43 Aldosterone adrenal hormone that causes the receptors, 69, 75
SCN and, 271 body to retain salt, 67, 307 Anatomical directions, 87
temporal pattern and, 217 Allied reflexes, 237 Androgen insensitivity. See Testicular feminization
touch receptors and, 204 All-or-none law principle that the amplitude and Androgens testes-produced hormones that are
Activating effect temporary effect of a hormone, velocity of an action potential are indepen- more abundant in males, 67, 328
which occurs at any time in life while the dent of the stimulus that initiated it, 42 steroid hormones and, 329
hormone is present, 330 Allostasis the adaptive way in which the body Angel dust, 481
Activation-synthesis hypothesis idea that a dream changes its set points depending on the situa- Anger, 360, 361, 362, 370
represents the brain’s effort to make sense of tion, 299 Angiotensin II hormone that constricts the blood
sparse and distorted information, 292 Alpha waves a steady series of brain waves at a fre- vessels, compensating for the drop in blood
Active transport a protein-mediated process that quency of 8 to 12 per second that are charac- pressure; triggers thirst, 307
expends energy to enable a molecule to cross teristic of relaxation, 276, 277 Animal Defense League, 23
a membrane, 34, 35 Alpha-fetoprotein protein that binds with estra- Anomia difficulty recalling the names of objects,
Acute transient global amnesia, 401 diol in the bloodstream of immature mam- 440
Adaptation decreased response to a stimulus as a mals, 332 Antabuse (disulfram) drug that antagonizes the
result of recent exposure to it, 217 Altruistic behavior an action that benefits some- effects of acetaldehyde dehydrogenase by
Addictions one other than the actor, 17 binding to its copper ion, 80
behavior and, 78–80, 81 Alzheimer’s disease condition characterized by Antagonist a drug that blocks a neurotransmitter, 71
benzodiazepines and, 377 memory loss, confusion, depression, restless- Antagonistic muscles opposing sets of muscles
brain reorganization and, 79 ness, hallucinations, delusions, sleeplessness, that are required to move a leg or arm back
explanation for, 79 and loss of appetite, 404 and forth, 232, 233
opiate drugs and, 74 brain atrophy and, 405 Anterior, 88
Ritalin and, 74 brain damage and, 141 Anterior cingulate cortex, 407
Adenosine, 61 Ginkgo biloba and, 416 Anterior commissure bundle of axons that connects
ADHD (attention deficit disorder), 74, 131, 138 neuronal degeneration and, 405 the two hemispheres of the cerebral cortex,
Adrenal cortex, 67 nucleus basalis and, 97 430
Adrenal medulla, 67 Amacrine cells, 155, 168 corpus callosum and, 95, 100
Adrenaline, 59 American Sign Language, 432 hemispheres and, 425
561
562 Subject Index/Glossary

Anterior pituitary portion of the pituitary gland, pitch perception and, 196–197 Behavioral plasticity, 412
composed of glandular tissue, 66, 67 sound localization and, 200, 201 Behaviors
Anterograde amnesia inability to form memories sound waves and, 194 addictions and, 74
for events that happened after brain damage, Auditory system, 424 addictive, 322
396 Autism, 243 adolescence, 138
Antibodies Y-shaped proteins that attach to par- Autoimmune disease, 381 alcohol and effects on, 77
ticular kinds of antigens, 381 Autonomic nervous system part of the PNS that animal research and, 21, 22
Antidepressant drugs controls the heart, intestines, and other organs, antisocial, 367
effectiveness of, 465–466 87 appearance and, 6
mechanisms of, 464, 464, 465 branches of, 356 attack and escape, 366–378
types of, 463–464 common sense view of emotions and, 356 biological explanations of, 4–5, 6
Antidiuretic hormone (ADH) hormone that sympathetic nervous system and, 89, 90, 357 body temperature and, 298, 299
enables the kidneys to reabsorb water from Autosomal genes all chromosomes other than X brain anatomy and, 114–116, 116
urine; also known as vasopressin, 305 and Y, 11 brain damage and, 110
Antigens surface proteins on a cell that identify the Axon thin fiber of constant diameter; the neuron’s and brain relationships, 116
cell as your own, 381 information sender, 30, 31 brain structures and effects on, 281
Antipsychotic (neuroleptic) drugs that tend to action potentials and, 40–45, 46 categories of, 4
relieve schizophrenia and similar conditions, activity of a neuron and, 38 drug effects and, 64
479, 480 all-or-none law and, 41 environment and, 13, 14
Antisaccade task a voluntary eye movement away astrocytes and, 33, 33 epigenetics and, 12
from the normal direction, 138, 251 chemical gradient and, 128 evolution and, 14–18
Anxiety development of, 125, 130 fear and anxiety, 371–375, 376
alcohol and, 377 incorrect regeneration of, 145, 145 fear response and, 89
amygdala response and, 373, 374 myelin sheath and, 44, 45 food selection and, 310, 311
antiplacebos and, 211 nerve connection experiments and, 127, 128 genes and, 14, 77
disorders, 376–378 pathfinding by, 126–128, 127, 128 heritability and, 13
medications and, 376, 377 threshold and synapses, 57, 57 impulsiveness, 138
relearning and, 378 white matter and, 88, 89 ionotropic synapses and, 64
serotonin transporters and, 67 Axon hillock a swelling where the axon exits the mechanisms of temperature regulation, 300
startle reflex and, 371 cell body (soma), 43, 45 metabotropic synapses and, 64
Aphasia language impairment, 438 mirror neurons and, 243
Aplysia, 411, 411 B cells, 381, 382 old age, 139
Apoptosis a programmed mechanism of cell Babinski reflex the extension of the big toe and oxytocin and, 337
death, 130 fanning of the others, by an infant, when the parental, 338–339
fetal alcohol syndrome and, 131 sole of the foot is stroked, 236, 236 pheromones and, 224, 225
Parkinson’s disease and, 257 Backward masking a brief visual stimulus after prenatal vulnerabilities and child, 131
Arachidonic acid, 469 another brief visual stimulus that leads to receptors and, 64
Archives of General Psychiatry, 7 failure to remember the first, 447 recording brain activity and, 116
Arcuate nucleus hypothalamic area with sets of Ballistic movement motion that proceeds as a sin- schizophrenia diagnosis and, 472
neurons for hunger and satiety, 315, 316 gle organized unit that cannot be redirected self-generated rhythms and, 266
Aripiprazole, 482 once it begins, 237, 248 sequences of, 237, 238
Aromatic compound, 331 BAS. See Behavioral Activation System (BAS) serotonin and, 370
Aromatize, 331 Basal forebrain area anterior and dorsal to the sex differences and childhood, 332, 332, 333
Artificial selection a process of selecting plants/ hypothalamus; includes cell clusters that pro- smell and, 221
animals for desired traits, 15 mote wakefulness and sleep, 96, 97, 280, 281 stimulant drugs and, 73, 74
Aspartame (NutraSweet), 217, 370 Basal ganglia a group of subcortical forebrain stimulation and, 110
Aspartate, 61 structures lateral to the thalamus, 96, 97, 249 unconscious activity and, 449
Associativity property that a weak input paired Huntington’s disease, 96 voluntary/involuntary movements and, 236–
with a stronger input enhances its later location of, 251 238
response, 412, 413 motor habits and, 251 yawning and, 5
Astigmatism a decreased responsiveness to one movement and, 94 See also Emotional behaviors; Sexual behaviors
kind of line or another, caused by an asym- Parkinson’s disease, 96, 254 Bell-Magendie law the concept that the entering
metric curvature of the eyes, 178, 178 tardive dyskinesia and, 482 dorsal roots carry sensory information and
Astrocytes star-shaped glia that synchronize the Basal metabolism energy used to maintain a con- the exiting ventral roots carry motor informa-
activity of the axons, 33, 33, 34 stant body temperature while at rest, 300. See tion, 89
ATP, 61 also Body temperature Benperidol, 480
Attack behaviors BDNF (brain-derived neurotrophic factor) a Benzodiazepines anti-anxiety drugs, 376, 377
amygdala and, 366, 367 neurotrophin similar to nerve growth factor, Biceps, 232, 233
hormones and, 367–368, 369 130, 414, 465 Big Bang, 2, 445
human, 370 Bed nucleus of the stria terminalis a set of neu- Binding problem question of how various brain
nonhuman, 369 rons that connect to the amygdala, 372, 372 areas produce a perception of a single object,
serotonin synapses and, 369 Behavioral Activation System (BAS) left brain 105
violence and, 367 hemispheric activity marked by low to mod- Binocular input stimulation from both eyes, 177
Attention, 449, 450–453 erate autonomic arousal and a tendency to Binocular rivalry, 447, 448, 448
Attention deficit disorder (ADHD), 74 approach, which could characterize either Biological clock
Atypical antidepressants miscellaneous group of happiness or anger, 361 jet lag and, 270
drugs with antidepressant effects but only Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) right brain mechanisms of, 271–274
mild side effects, 464 hemispheric activity, which increases atten- melatonin and, 274
Audition tion and arousal, inhibits action, and stimu- setting the, 268–269
auditory cortex and, 198–199 lates emotions such as fear and disgust, 361 shift work and, 270
ear structure and, 194–195, 195, 196 Behavioral medicine field that includes the effects Biological explanations, 6
experience and development of, 198 of diet, smoking, exercise, stressful experi- Biological psychology the study of the physiologi-
hearing loss and, 200 ences, and other behaviors on health, 380 cal, evolutionary, and developmental mecha-
phase and loudness differences in, 201, 201 Behavioral Neuroscience, 7 nisms of behavior and experience, 3
Subject Index/Glossary 563

animal research and, 21–23 divisions of the vertebrate, 91 structures of arousal and attention, 279–280,
career opportunities and, 6, 7 dreams and the, 292–293 281, 281
explanation of, 3 emotion and areas of the, 359–361 tactile stimulation and, 333
Bipolar cell type of neuron in the retina that endorphins and, 211 taste coding in the, 219
receives input directly from the receptors, and eye connections, 177 transporters and, 67
155, 156, 158 face recognition and the, 184–185, 186 tryptophan and, 62
excitation and inhibition of, 170, 170 fat reserves and, 314, 315 ventricles and, 98
fovea and, 157 fish and, 311 ventromedial hypothalamus and, 318, 319
retina and, 32, 33 forebrain and, 94–97 vestibular system and, 203
Bipolar disorder (manic-depressive disorder) a function and REM sleep, 282 views of the, 3, 4, 94, 95
condition that alternates between depression gender differences and, 329, 330, 330 visual coding and, 154–165
and mania, 469, 469 genetic males/females and, 346 visual information processes and the, 169
Bipolar I disorder a condition including full- glucose and, 313 wake–sleep rhythms and the, 267
blown episodes of mania, 469 glutamate and, 63 weight of, 124
Bipolar II disorder a condition with milder manic gray and white matter of, 118 Brain and movement
phases, characterized mostly by agitation or hemispheres of the, 422, 423 basal ganglia, 249–250, 251
anxiety, 469 hemispheric contributions of the, 361 cerebellum, 247–249
Birds hemispheric specializations and, 428 cerebral cortex, 240–247
body temperature and, 298, 301, 302 hindbrain and, 91–92, 93 inhibition, 251–252
eating strategies and, 309 of homosexual/heterosexual males and females, motor learning, 251
foveas and, 157 349 Brain damage
learning and, 390 hormones and, 65, 66 adult neuron generation and, 126
motor programs and, 237, 238 hunger and mechanisms of the, 315–319, 320 afferent nerves and, 148
retinal receptors and, 157 Huntington’s disease and, 258 Babinski reflex and, 236, 237
sleep and, 288, 289 hypothalamus and pituitary gland locations in, 66 Broca’s area and, 109
spatial memory and, 401, 402 infants and face recognition by the, 184, 184, causes of, 141
Birdsong, 6, 126 185, 185 decision making and, 363, 364
Birth-control pills, 335, 336, 337 insectivores and, 100, 101 dreams and, 293
BIS. See Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) ionotropic synapses and, 64 dyslexia and, 442
Blind spot area at the back of the retina where the lateral hypothalamus and, 317, 318 effects of, 116
optic nerve exits; it is devoid of receptors, lead and, 367 emotions and, 358, 361
155, 156 learning methods and the, 390 failure of attention and, 451
Blindsight the ability to respond in limited ways to learning to read and, 135 fetal alcohol syndrome and, 131
visual information without perceiving it con- maternal behavior and, 338 hippocampus and, 396–402
sciously, 173 mechanisms and body temperature, 302, 302, 303 Huntington’s disease and, 258
Blood–brain barrier mechanism that excludes mechanisms of sleeping and waking, 280 insomnia and, 283
most chemicals from the brain, 34, 35 memory and areas of, 406 language and, 438–440
glucose and, 35 metabotropic receptors and, 64 motion vision and, 187
osmotic pressure and third ventricle of, 306 midbrain and, 93 neglect and, 451–453
trytophan and, 61, 62 molecules and, 34 new neurons and, 126
Body temperature motion perception and the, 186–187 phantom limb and, 147
advantages of high, 301, 302 music training and, 135, 136 plasticity after, 141–149
brain mechanisms and, 302, 302, 303 neurons and, 28, 62 prefrontal cortex and, 242
methods of controlling, 300, 301 neuropeptides and, 64 progressive, 478
See also Circadian rhythms nitric oxide and, 60, 61 rotational forces and whiplash, 141
Bonobos, 432, 433 non-task-related activity and, 113 schizophrenia and, 473
Borna disease, 462, 462 olfactory receptors and, 222, 223, 224 SCN and, 271
BOTOX (botulinum toxin), 358 opiate drugs and, 74, 75 short-term recovery and, 141–142, 143
Brain osmotic pressure and, 306, 306 somatosensory cortex and, 146–147
abnormalities and schizophrenia, 477–478 pain receptors and the, 242 split-brain people and, 424–426
adaptations by the, 134 pain stimuli and pathways to the, 209 view of brains and, 142
alcohol and effects on the, 77 people’s intentions and, 113, 114 Brain damage recovery
amino acids and, 61, 62 and physical activity, 134 axon regrowth and sprouting as, 144, 145, 145
androgens and, 329 prenatal hormones and, 333 brain stimulation and, 143, 144
animal research and, 21, 22 purpose of, 232 denervation supersensitivity and, 145, 146
anti-anxiety drugs and, 376 readiness potential and, 244, 244, 245, 245 learned behavioral adjustments and, 148
arachidonic acid and, 469 reinforcement components of, 72, 73 short-term recovery and, 141–142, 143
arcuate nucleus and, 316, 317 relationship of mind and, 445, 446 stimulant drugs and, 144
area V4 and, 186 reorganization, 136–137, 136–137 therapy and, 149
astrocytes and, 33 reorganized sensory representations and, 146– Brain death condition with no sign of brain activ-
attention and, 449, 450 147 ity and no response to any stimulus, 276
and behavior relationship, 116 research methods and the, 109–119 Brain development
binding problem and the, 106, 107 sagittal section and, 93 adaptation and, 134
binocular rivalry and activity of, 447–448, 449 satiety messages and the, 312 area maturation during, 130
biological clock and, 271 SCN and, 272 behavioral development and, 138–139
biological motion and the, 184 self-stimulation of the, 71, 72 cortex differentiation and, 132, 132
of birds and mammals, 126 size and intelligence, 117–119 experience and, 124, 132–137
blood–brain barrier and, 34–35 sleep and activity inhibition of, 281, 282 glucose and, 131
cannabinoid receptors of the, 75 somatosensory cortex and, 146–147 later life neuron development, 125, 126
cells of the, 3 spatial summation and, 54 maternal influence and, 131
cerebral cortex and, 100–106 speech comprehension and, 429 myelination and, 125
color perception and, 165 spinal cord and the, 88 neuron development and, 124, 125
comparison of mammalian species, 100 stages of sleep and the, 276, 277 neuron survival and, 129
development of the, 124–139 stimulation effects, 110, 116 schizophrenia and, 475
564 Subject Index/Glossary

Brain development (continued) several species and, 22 Cingulate gyrus, 93, 95, 360, 363
special experiences and, 134–137 ventral view of the brain and, 4 Circadian rhythms
stages of, 125, 125 Cerebral cortex layers of cells on the outer surface age differences in, 268, 269
Toxoplasma gondii and, 477 of the cerebral hemisphere of the forebrain, biochemistry of the, 273, 273, 274
vulnerabilities during, 131 100 biologically driven, 267
Brain Research, 7 conscious decisions and movements, 244–245 body temperature and, 267, 270, 271, 281, 284,
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor. See BDNF dorsal and ventral view of, 95 289
Brainstem the medulla, pons, midbrain, and cen- forebrain and, 91, 93, 94 depression and, 468, 468
tral structure of the forebrain, 91, 91 frontal lobe and, 104 light and, 271
limbic system and, 95 lack of control and, 240 melatonin and, 274
Broca’s aphasia (nonfluent aphasia) brain damage learning and memory of the, 392 phase-delay/phase-advance and, 270, 283
that causes impaired language, 438, 439, 440 long-term memory and, 416 sleep disorders and, 273, 274, 283–285, 284
Broca’s area portion of the brain that is associated mirror neurons and, 243 suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) and, 271
with language production, 109, 438, 438 movement and, 240–243 Classical conditioning type of conditioning
Bulimia nervosa a condition in which people alter- neurons and, 32, 46, 126 produced by the pairing of two simuli, one
nate between binges of overeating and peri- occipital lobe and, 102 of which evokes an automatic response, 390,
ods of strict dieting, 322 olfactory bulb and, 222, 224 391
Buprenorphine, 81 organization of the, 100–101, 102 Hebbian synapse and, 410
Bupropion, 464 parietal lobe and, 102 Clinico-anatomical hypothesis idea that considers
Butyrophenones a chemical family that includes primate groups and the, 101, 101 dreams as a type of thinking that occurs
antipsychotic drugs (haloperidol) that relieve proportion of the brain and, 104, 104 under unusual conditions, 292
the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, 479, sensory and motor information in, 103 Clitoris-reduction surgery, 345
480 spinal cord and, 246 Cloacal exstrophy, 345
synesthesia and, 226 Closed head injury a result of a sharp blow to the
CaMKII protein, 414 temporal lobe and, 102, 104 head that does not puncture the brain, 141
Cannabinoid receptors, 75 thalamus and, 94, 96 Clozapine, 480, 482
Cannabinoids chemicals related to D9-THC, 75, tongue and, 219, 219 CNS. See Central nervous system (CNS)
76, 142, 211 ventral view of the brain and, 4 Cocaine a drug that blocks reuptake of dopamine,
Capsaicin a chemical, found in hot peppers, that Cerebral palsy, 246 73, 74, 76
produces a painful burning sensation by Cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) a clear fluid similar to brain reorganization and, 79
releasing substance P; high dosages damage blood plasma produced by choroid plexus in prenatal exposure and, 131
pain receptors, 208, 211 the brain ventricles, 98 Cochlea structure in the inner ear containing audi-
Carbachol, 282 development of the brain and, 124 tory receptors, 196, 196, 197, 197
Carbamazepine, 469 Cervix, 333 Cognitive functions
Carbon 14 dating, 126 Chemical senses conscious/unconscious processes, 445–453
Cardiac muscles muscles of the heart that have chemical coding and, 215 language and, 432–443
properties intermediate between those of olfaction and, 221–223, 224 lateralization and, 422–430
smooth and skeletal muscles, 232, 233 pheromones and, 224, 225 Cognitive symptoms, 473
CAT scan. See Computerized axial tomography synesthesia and, 225–226 Cold-blooded, 234
(CT or CAT scan) taste and, 216–220 Collateral sprouts newly formed branches of an
Cataplexy, 284 Chemokines, 125 axon, 144, 145
Catecholamines compounds that contain a cate- Chewing gum, 311, 311 Color constancy the ability to recognize colors
chol and an amine group, 60, 61 Children despite changes in lighting, 163, 164, 186
Caudate nucleus large subcortical structure, part CAH girls as, 344 Color perception
of the basal ganglia, 250 heritability and adopted, 13 across-fiber pattern code and, 215
CCK. See Cholecystokinin (CCK) intersexes and, 343, 344 synesthesia and, 226
Celexa (citalopram), 464 language learning and, 437 Color vision
Cell body (soma) structure containing the nucleus, schizophrenia and, 474, 475, 475 cones, 158
ribosomes, and mitochondria, 30, 31 Chimpanzees deficiency, 165, 166
activity of a neuron and, 38 altruism and, 17 opponent-process theory and, 161–162, 163
neuropeptides and, 60 humans and, 14 retinex theory and, 163–164, 165
Central canal a fluid-filled channel in the center of mutated genes and, 11 trichromatic theory and, 160, 161
the spinal cord, 98 See also Research animals wavelengths and, 160, 160, 161, 161
Central nervous system (CNS) the brain and the Chloride ions Color vision deficiency inability to perceive color
spinal cord, 86, 86 alcohol and, 377 differences, 166
cannabinoids and, 211 polarization and, 40 Colorblindness. See Color vision deficiency
input to the, 206, 207 Chlorpromazine (Thorazine) antipsychotic drug Column collection of cells having similar proper-
neurons and, 130 that relieves the positive symptoms of schizo- ties, arranged perpendicular to the laminae,
pain path and, 206 phrenia for most, though not all, patients, 88, 101, 102
spinal nerves of the, 206 463, 479, 480 Coma an extended period of unconsciousness with
Central pattern generators neural mechanisms in Cholecystokinin (CCK) hormone released a low level of brain activity, 276
the spinal cord that generate rhythmic pat- from the duodenum that constricts the Combination pill, 336
terns of motor output, 237 sphincter muscle between the stomach Complex cell type of visual cortex cell located in
Central sulcus one of the deepest grooves in the and duodenum, limiting the meal size, 312, areas V1 and V2 that responds to a pattern
surface of the cerebral cortex, 102 316 of light in a particular orientation anywhere
Cerebellar cortex the surface of the cerebellum, anxiety and, 376 within its large receptive field, 174, 174
248, 248, 249 Chorda tympani, 219 Computerized axial tomography (CT or CAT
cellular organization of, 249, 250 Choroid plexus, 98 scan) method of visualizing a living brain by
mammalian brain and, 100 Chromosomes strands of genes, 9 injecting a dye into the blood and placing a
memory and, 392, 393 gender differences and, 330 person’s head into a CT scanner; x-rays are
of a mouse, 29, 62 Huntington’s disease and, 258, 259 passed through the head and recorded by
movements and damage to, 248 sex differences on the X and Y, 330, 345 detectors on the opposite side, 114, 115
neurons and, 28 sex-linked and sex-limited genes on, 11 COMT (catechol-o-methyltransferase) enzyme
role of, 248, 249 Cingulate cortex, 209, 211 that breaks down excess dopamine into
Subject Index/Glossary 565

inactive chemicals that cannot stimulate the CPAP. See Continuous Positive Airway Pressure eating disorders and, 322
dopamine receptors, 67 (CPAP) mask ECT and, 467, 468
alcoholism and, 77 CR. See Conditioned response (CR) genetics and, 461
Concentration gradient difference in distribution Cranial nerves nerves that control sensations from happiness and, 460
of ions across the neuron’s membrane, 39 the head, muscle movements in the head, and hemispheric dominance and, 463
sodium channels, 41 much of the parasympathetic output to the iproniazid and, 463
Concordance similarity between individuals with organs, 91 obesity and, 320
regard to a trait, 474, 475 chorda tympani and, 219 PER protein and, 273, 274
Conditioned response (CR) response evoked by a name, number, and functions of, 92 psychotherapy and, 466
conditioned stimulus after it has been paired Craniosacral system. See Parasympathetic nervous SAD and, 470
with an unconditioned stimulus, 390, 391 system sleep disorders and, 283–285
Conditioned stimulus (CS) stimulus that evokes a CREB (cyclic adenosine monophosphate respon- sleep patterns and, 468
particular response only after it has been sive element-binding protein), 414 testosterone deprivation and, 334
paired with an unconditioned stimulus, 390, Cross-adaptation reduced response to one taste tryptophan and, 465
391 after exposure to another, 217 Dermatome area of the body connected to a par-
Conductive deafness (middle-ear deafness) hear- CS. See Conditioned stimulus (CS) ticular spinal nerve, 206, 207
ing loss that occurs if the bones of the middle Cutaneous rabbit illusion, 207 Diabetes
ear fail to transmit sound waves properly to Cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cyclic AMP), 64 insulin and, 313, 313
the cochlea, 200 Cyclotron, 111 testosterone and, 334
Cones type of retinal receptor that contributes to Cytokines small proteins that combat infections weight and, 314
color perception, 158 and communicate with the brain to elicit Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
wavelength-sensitivity and, 160 appropriate behaviors, 303, 381 Disorders, 459, 472
Confabulation a distinctive symptom of Diaschisis decreased activity of surviving brain
Korsakoff ’s syndrome in which patients fill in Deafferent to remove or disable the sensory nerves neurons after damage to other neurons,
memory gaps with guesses, 404 from a body part, 148 143
Congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) the overde- Decisions, 244, 245, 407 Diencephalon, 94
velopment of the adrenal glands from birth, Declarative memory deliberate recall of informa- Diet
343 tion that one recognizes as a memory, 399, amino acids and, 61
Conscious capable of reporting the presence of a 400, 403, 403 carbohydrates and, 62
stimulus, 446 Delayed matching-to-sample task task in which PKU and, 14
Conscious decisions, 244, 245 an animal sees a sample object and then after Dieting, 321, 322
Consciousness a delay must choose an objet that matches Differential diagnosis one that rules out
attention and, 450–453 the sample, 400 other conditions with similar symptoms,
brain activity and, 446–449, 450 Delayed nonmatching-to-sample task task in 473
experiments and, 447, 448, 448 which an animal sees an object and then after Differentiate to develop the axon and dendrites
explanation of, 3 a delay must choose an object that does not that give a neuron its distinctive properties,
hard problem and, 3, 446 match the sample, 400, 400 125
threshold phenomenon and, 449 Delayed-response task assignment in which an Digestion
timing of, 449, 450 animal must respond on the basis of a signal dairy products and, 310
unconscious processing vs., 446, 447 that it remembers but that is no longer pres- digestive system and, 309, 310, 310
Consolidate (consolidation) to strengthen a mem- ent, 105, 395 food selection and, 310, 311
ory and make it more long-lasting, 395 D9-tetrahydrocannabinol (D9-THC) chemical con- small intestines and, 310, 310
Contextual memory, 401, 402 tained in marijuana leaves, 75 Digestive system
Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP) Delusions unjustifiable beliefs, 472 ghrelin and, 316
mask, 284 Dementia praecox, 472 lateral hypothalamus and, 318
Contralateral, 88 Dendrites branching fibers from a neuron that Dihydrotestosterone, 346
Control of movement, 232–235, 236 receive information from other neurons DISC1 (disrupted in schizophrenia 1) gene that con-
Cooperativity tendency for nearly simultaneous brain development and, 132–133, 134 trols production of dendritic spines and the
stimulation by two or more axons to development of, 125, 130, 133 generation of new neurons in the hippocam-
produce long-term potentiation much gray matter and, 88, 89 pus, 475
more effectively than stimulation by just inhibitory synapses and, 57 Diseases. See specific names
one, 412 neuropeptide and, 64 Disgust, 360, 361, 362
Coronal plane, 87, 88 Dendritic spines short outgrowths that increase Dissociative identity disorder, 472
Corpus callosum bundle of axons that connects the surface area available for synapses, 30, 31 Distal, 88
the two hemispheres of the cerebral cortex, experiences and, 133 Disulfiram (Antabuse), 80, 463
100, 219, 422, 422 Denervation supersensitivity (receptor super­ Diuresis, 305
absence of, 430 sensitivity) increased sensitivity to neu- Dizygotic twins fraternal (non-identical) twins
anterior commissure and, 95 rotransmitters by a postsynaptic neron after derived from two eggs, 13, 475, 475
effects of damage to, 425 loss of input, 145 Dominant gene gene that shows a strong effect in
lateralization and, 423 Dentatorubro-pallidoluysian dystrophy, 259 either the homozygous or heterozygous con-
maturation of the, 429 Deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) double-stranded dition, 10
Corpus luteum, 335, 336 molecule that is part of the chromosomes, 9, Dopamine
Cortical blindness, 102 10, 10 antipsychotic drugs and, 479
Cortical cells, 174, 176, 178 Depakene (valproate), 469 drugs and, 71–72, 73
Corticospinal tracts paths from the cerebral cortex Depakote (valproate), 469 D2 receptors and, 480
to the spinal cord, 246 Depolarize (depolarization) to reduce polarization impulsive behavior and, 69
Corticosterone, 67 toward zero across a membrane, 40, 41 neurons and, 64
Cortisol hormone secreted by the adrenal cortex AMPA/NMDA receptors and, 413, 414 as a neurotransmitter, 61
that elevates blood sugar and enhances marijuana and, 75 nicotine and, 74
metabolism, 67, 380 neurotransmitters and, 62 pain sensitivity and, 211
ACTH and, 343, 344 summation and, 55 Parkinson’s disease and, 254
memory storage and, 395 Depression placebos and, 211
stress and, 270, 380 alternative therapies and, 468, 469 release and endorphins, 74, 75
violence and, 368 biological influences and, 462, 463 synapse, 71, 72
566 Subject Index/Glossary

Dopamine (continued) ECT. See Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) Entorhinal cortex, 144
synthesis of, 61 Ecstasy (MDMA), 76, 76 Environment
type 4 receptor, 77 Edema accumulation of fluid, 142 biological clock and light–dark, 267
Dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia idea that EEG. See Electroencephalograph (EEG) contributory evidence of, 13, 14
schizophrenia results from excess activity at Efferent axon neuron that carries information experience and enriched, 133, 134
dopamine synapses in certain brain areas, away from a structure, 32, 32 gene expression and, 12
479 Efficacy a drug’s tendency to activate the receptor, genetics and, 367
Dopamine transporter a protein that is reab- 71 interventions by, 14
sorbed at the presynaptic terminal, 67, 73 Electrical gradient difference in electrical lead in the, 367
Dorsal located toward the back, 87, 88, 89, 95 charges between the inside and outside of obesity and prenatal, 320
Dorsal raphe and pons, 281 the cell, 37 Parkinson’s disease and, 255
Dorsal root ganglia clusters of sensory neurons leakage of potassium ions, 38, 39 violence and, 367
outside the spinal cord, 88, 89 potassium channels and, 41 Enzymes biological catalysts that regulate chemical
Dorsal stream visual path in the parietal cortex Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) a treatment for reactions in the body, 10
that helps the motor system locate objects; depression by electrically inducing a seizure, Epididymis, 328
the “where” path, 183, 186, 187 467 Epigenetics a field that deals with changes in gene
Double-jointed, 376 Electroencephalograph (EEG) a device that expression without modification of the DNA
Down syndrome, 405 records electrical activity of the brain sequence, 12
Dreams and dreaming through electrodes attached to the scalp, experiences and, 13
biological perspectives on, 292–293 111 Epilepsy a condition characterized by repeated epi-
brain and, 281 sleep and, 276, 277, 278 sodes of excessive synchronized neutral activ-
prefrontal cortex and, 242 Electron micrograph, 62 ity, 424
REM sleep and, 278, 279 11-cis-retinal, 159 bromides and, 463
Dronabinol, 75 Emotion(s) cocaine and, 74
Droperidol, 480 autonomic nervous system and, 356 corpus callosum damage and, 424
Drugs behavior and, 367 emotions and, 360
abused, 71–76 brain damage and, 363, 364 hippocampal damage and, 396
addiction and, 74 components of, 356 insomnia and, 283
affinity and efficacy of, 71 frowning and, 358 Epinephrine
antipsychotic, 479–481 functions of, 362–362, 363 catecholamines and, 61
autonomic nervous system and, 91 gut feeling and, 362 list of hormone-releasing glands and, 67
cancer and, 64 hemispheric contributions and, 361 memory storage and, 395
commonality of abused, 71–72, 73 hormones and, 367, 368 as a neurotransmitter, 61
insomnia and, 283 limbic system and, 359, 360 Episodic memory memories of single personal
MAO and, 62 memory storage and, 395 events, 398, 400, 404
mechanisms, 71 moral decisions and, 362, 363 Equilibrium, 92
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory, 212 particular, 360 Equipotentiality concept that all parts of the cortex
Parkinson’s disease and, 257 physiological arousal and, 357, 358 contribute equally to complex behaviors; any
prenatal exposure and, 131 right hemisphere and, 427 part of the cortex can substitute for any other,
psychiatric use of, 59 smiles and, 358 392
receptors and, 64 stress and health, 380 Escape behavior
REM sleep and, 292 Emotional behaviors anxiety disorders and, 376–378
reuptake and, 73 attack and escape as, 366–378 fear and anxiety as, 371–375, 376
stimulant, 73, 74 emotions and, 355–365 Estradiol a hormone in the estrogen family, 329
synapses and, 69 stress and health as, 380–384 female development and, 331
See also Substance abuse Encephalitis, 390 maternal behavior and, 338
Dualism belief that mind and body are different Endocrine glands hormone-producing glands, 65, menstrual cycle and, 335, 335, 336
kinds of substance that exist independently, 65 Estrogens family of hormones that are more abun-
445 Endogenous circadian rhythm self-generated dant in females, 329
Duodenum part of the small intestine adjoining rhythm that lasts about a day, 266 list of hormone-releasing glands and, 67
the stomach; first digestive site that absorbs See also Circadian rhythms Etch-A-Sketch, 429
nutrients, 312 Endogenous circannual rhythm self-generated Evoked potentials (evoked responses) electrical
Dyslexia a specific impairment of reading in some- rhythm that lasts about a year, 266 recordings on the scalp from brain activity in
one with adequate vision, adequate motiva- Endogenous cycles, 266–267, 268 response to a stimulus, 111
tion, and adequate overall cognitive skills, Endopiriform cortex, 216 Evolution a change over generations in the
441–443 Endoplasmic reticulum network of thin tubes that frequencies of various genes in a popula-
Dysthymia, 466 transport newly synthesized proteins to other tion, 14
locations, 28 language development and, 437
Eating disorders obesity and, 315 misunderstandings of, 15, 16
bulimia nervosa, 322 Endorphins transmitters that attach to the same mutation and, 11
genetics and, 320 receptors as morphine, 209, 210, 211 sexual orientation hypotheses and, 347, 348
obesity and, 320 as neuropeptides, 74, 75 sleep and, 288
schizophrenia and, 473 as neurotransmitters, 61 species and, 14, 15
weight gain and, 320, 321 Endothelial cells Evolutionary explanation understanding in terms
weight loss and, 321, 322 blood–brain barrier and, 34, 35 of the evolutionary history of a structure or
Eating regulations End-stopped (hypercomplex) cell type of visual behavior, 4
glucagon and, 313 cortex cell that resembles complex cells; mating behavior and, 341–342, 343
glucose and, 312 responds best to stimuli of a precisely limited type of biological explanation and, 6
insulin and, 313, 313 type, anywhere in a large receptive field, with Evolutionary psychology field that deals with how
leptin and, 314, 315 a strong inhibitory field at one end of its field, behaviors evolved, 17
melanocortin receptors and, 316 175, 175 Excitatory cells, 281
oral factors and, 311, 312 Energy conservation, 288–289, 309, 315, 321 Excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) graded
seasonal eating and, 314 Engram physical representation of what has been depolarization, 53
stomach and intestines with, 312 learned, 391, 394 Exhaustion (stress stage), 380
Subject Index/Glossary 567

Exocytosis a release of neurotransmitter from Flash suppression, 447 globus pallidus and, 250
the presynaptic neuron into the synaptic Flexor muscle that flexes the limb, 232, 233 inhibitory ionotropic synapses and, 63, 64
cleft that separates one neuron from inhibitory synapses and, 55, 56 as a neurotransmitter, 61
another, 62 Fluphenazine, 480 panic disorder and, 376
Experience fMRI. See Functional magnetic resonance imaging sleep and, 281, 281
brain development and, 132–137 (fMRI) stroke induced laboratory animals and, 144
Explicit memory deliberate recall of information Focal hand dystonia (musician’s cramp) a disorder GABAA receptor complex structure that includes
that one recognizes as a memory, 399 where one or more fingers is in constant con- a site that binds GABA as well as sites that
Extensor muscle that straightens the limb, 232, traction or where moving one finger indepen- modify the sensitivity of the GABA site,
233 dently of others is difficult, 137 376, 377
Extinction Focus point in the brain where an epileptic seizure Gamma waves, 447
response and, 79 begins, 424 Ganglion, 88, 88
Extracellular fluid, 305 Follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) chemical Ganglion cells type of neuron in the retina that
Eyes released from the anterior pituitary; pro- receives input from the bipolar cells, 155,
axon connections and, 127, 128 motes the growth of a follicle in the ovary, 67, 156
biceps and movement of, 232, 233 68, 335, 335 categories of, 172, 172
bipolar cell and, 32 Food fovea and, 157
brain connections and, 155–159, 177, 423, 423, olfaction and, 221 Gap junction a direct contact of one neuron with
424 serotonin synthesis and, 370 another, enabling electrical transmission, 69
cannabinoids and glaucoma, 75 taste and smell of, 216 Gases one of the categories of neurotransmitters,
cerebellar damage and, 248 Footbridge dilemma, 363 including nitric oxide and possibly others, 60,
consciousness and, 448 Forebrain most anterior part of the brain; consists 61
cranial nerves and movement of, 92 of two cerebral hemispheres, 93 Gastric bypass surgery, 322
deprived visual experience in, 177 basal forebrain, 96, 97 Gastrin-releasing peptide, 213
dyslexia and, 441, 442 basal ganglia, 96, 97 Gastrulation, 131
fear and, 375, 376 hypothalamus and, 94, 96 Gate theory idea that stimulation of certain
genes and, 10 pituitary gland, 96 axons can close the “gates” for pain messages,
learning conditioning and, 393 thalamus and, 94 211
oxygen and, 292 Fornix, 360 Gender identity sex with which a person identifies,
14
retinal ganglion cells and, 273 C, 126 343
saccades and, 188 Fovea a tiny area of the retina specialized for acute, assignment and rearing with, 345, 346
schizophrenia and, 474 detailed vision, 155, 157, 158 CAH girls and, 344, 345
visual cortex and, 102 FOXP2 gene, 11, 437 intersexes and, 343, 344
See also Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep; Free nerve ending, 205 sexual appearance and, 346, 347
Vision Frequency the number of cycles per second, mea- testicular feminization and, 345
sured in Hz, 194 Gene-knockout approach, 110
Facial expressions Frequency theory concept that the basilar mem- General adaptation syndrome a generalized
amygdala response and, 373, 373 brane vibrates in synchrony with a sound, response to stress, 380, 381
depression and, 460 causing auditory nerve axons to produce Genes units of heredity that maintain their struc-
genetics and, 9 action potentials at the same frequency, 196 tural identity from one generation to another,
Facial nerves, 92 Frontal cortex 9
Facilitating interneuron, 412 infant reflexes and, 236 acetaldehyde metabolism and, 80
Fast-twitch fibers muscle fibers that produce fast Frontal lobe section of cerebral cortex that extends alcoholism and, 77
contractions but fatigue rapidly, 234 from the central sulcus to the anterior limit Alzheimer’s disease and, 405
Fat cells, 67, 311, 313, 314 of the brain, 103, 104, 360 antisocial behavior and, 367, 367
Fear Frown, 358 brain chemicals and, 14
amygdala and, 373–375 FSH. See Follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH) C-A-G repetitions and, 259, 259
brain area and, 360 FTO gene, 320, 321 circadian rhythm and, 273
escape behavior and, 362 Functional explanation understanding why a homosexual orientation and, 347, 348
eyes and, 375, 376 structure or behavior evolved as it did, 5, 6 Huntington’s disease and, 258–259, 259
reactivity, 374 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) a that inactivate pain, 209
relearning and, 378 modified version of MRI that measures ener- lactose and, 310
startle reflex and, 371, 374 gies based on hemoglobin instead of water; language and, 435
sympathetic nervous system and, 89 determines the brain areas receiving the language and mutation of, 437
Feature detectors neurons whose responses indi- greatest supply of blood and using the most obesity and, 314, 315, 320
cate the presence of a particular feature, 176 oxygen, 112, 112, 113 Parkinson’s disease and, 254, 255
Fenfluramine, 321 amygdala response and, 373 PER and TIM, 273, 273, 274
Fen–phen, 321, 322 brain activity and, 446 schizophrenia and, 475, 476
Fetal alcohol syndrome a condition resulting from memory and, 398, 399 seasonal affective disorder and, 470
prenatal exposure to alcohol and marked by posterior parietal cortex and, 242 Genetic drift, 5
hyperactivity, impulsiveness, decreased alert- shape perception and, 184 Genetics
ness, varying degrees of mental retardation, Fungiform papillae, 220 alcoholism and, 77
motor problems, heart defects, and facial Fusiform gyrus brain area of the inferior temporal bipolar disorder and, 469
abnormalities, 131, 131 cortex that recognizes faces, 184–185, 184, depression and, 461, 461
Fever, 303 186 fast-twitch/slow-twitch fibers and, 234
Finger-to-nose test, 248 fungiform papillae and, 220
Fissure, 88 G protein a protein coupled to guanosine triphos- gene expression and, 11–12, 13
Fitness the spreading of genes; number of copies of phate (GTP), an energy-storing molecule, 64 impact of heredity and environment on, 13
one’s genes that endure in later generations, olfactory receptor and, 222, 223 Mendelian, 9–12, 13
16 second messenger and, 64 mutation and, 11, 12
5a-reductase 2, 346 taste and, 218 nerve deafness and, 200
5-HT2 receptors, 482 GABA (gamma-amino-butyric acid) sexual orientation and, 347
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) serotonin’s alcohol and, 131 synesthesia and, 225, 226
main metabolite, 369, 370, 370 endorphin synapses and, 74, 75 tone-deaf and, 197
568 Subject Index/Glossary

Ghrelin chemical released by the stomach during a Hair cells the auditory receptors that lie along the despite changes in the environment’s temper-
period of food deprivation; also released as a basilar membrane in the cochlea, 196, 196 ature, 300
neurotransmitter in the brain, where it stim- Hair erection Homozygous having two identical genes for a
ulates eating, 315, 316 evolutionary explanation of, 5 given characteristic, 10
Ginkgo biloba, 416 goose bumps and, 89, 300 outcomes of mating and, 11
Glia type of cell in the nervous system that, in con- Hair-follicle receptors, 205 Horizontal cell type of cell that receives input
trast to neurons, does not conduct impulses Hallucinations false sensory experiences, 472 from receptors and delivers inhibitory input
over long distances, 33 Hallucinogenic drugs drugs that distort percep- to bipolar cells, 168, 169
axon growth and, 144 tion, 76, 76 Horizontal plane, 87, 88, 94
explanation of, 3 Haloperidol, 331, 480, 482 Hormone chemical that is secreted by cells in one
glutamate and glutamine with, 33 Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, 465, 466 part of the body and conveyed by the blood
shapes of cells, 33 Handedness, 430, 478 to influence other cells, 65
Globus pallidus large subcortical structure; part of Happiness, 360, 375, 376 activation of, 11
the basal ganglia, 250 Hard problem philosophical question as to why aggressive behavior and, 367, 368
Glossopharyngeal nerves, 92 and how brain activity becomes conscious, depression and, 462, 463
Glucagon pancreatic hormone that stimulates the 3, 446 functions of, 67
liver to convert stored glycogen to glucose, 67, Hearing intersexes and, 343, 344
313 brain hemispheres and, 422 rearing experiences and prenatal, 333
Glucose a simple sugar, 35 contralateral input and, 219 releasing glands list, 67
brain and, 34, 35 cranial nerves and, 92 sodium-specific hunger and, 307
brain development and, 131 dyslexia and, 442 vasopressin and angiotensin II as, 307
digestive system and, 312, 313 Heschl’s gyrus and, 114 Hospital dilemma, 363
muscles and, 234 impairment, 200 HPA axis the hypothalamus, pituitary gland, and
Glutamate inferior colliculus and, 93 adrenal cortex, 380
alcohol and, 131 ionotropic synapses and, 64 Hunger
AMPA/NMDA receptors and, 413, 414, 414 primary sensory cortex and, 103 brain mechanisms and, 315–319, 320
edema and, 142 schizophrenia and, 473 different species and, 309, 309
Huntington’s disease and, 258 See also Audition; Sensory system digestion and food selection for, 309–310, 311
ionotropic effect and, 64 Hebbian synapse a synapse that increases in effec- eating disorders and, 320–322
ionotropic synapses and, 63 tiveness because of simultaneous activity in the eating regulation and, 311–314, 315
LTP and, 413 presynaptic and postsynaptic neurons, 410 Huntingtin protein produced by the gene whose
motor neurons and, 62 Hemiplegia, 246 mutation leads to Huntington’s disease, 259
neuron transmitters and, 33 Hemispheres (brain), 424–428 Huntington’s disease a severe neurological disor-
as a neurotransmitter, 61 Hemorrhage type of stroke resulting from a rup- der characterized by jerky arm movements
pain and, 208 tured artery, 141, 142 and facial twitches and later by tremors,
schizophrenia and, 481 Heredity writhing movements, and psychological
taste receptors and, 217 alcoholism and, 77 symptoms, 258, 258
Glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia theory behavior and, 14 basal ganglia and, 96
proposal that schizophrenia relates in part to contributory evidence of, 13 C-A-G repeats and age, 259, 259
deficient activity at glutamate synapses, espe- PKU and, 14 globus pallidus and, 250
cially in the prefrontal cortex, 481 violence and, 367 heredity and, 258
Glutamine Heritability estimate of the degree to which varia- narcolepsy and, 285
amino acids and, 10 tion in a characteristic depends on genetic schizophrenia and, 473
glia and, 33 variations in a given population, 13 Hyperpolarization increased polarization across a
Glycine Hermaphrodite individual who appears to be a membrane, 40
antipsychotic drugs and, 481, 481 mixture of male and female anatomies, 343 Hypnagogic hallucinations, 284
as inhibitory transmitter, 63 Heroin, 74, 76, 77, 78, 79 Hypocretin. See Orexin (hypocretin)
as a neurotransmitter, 61 Heschl’s gyrus, 114 Hypoglossal nerves, 92
Golgi tendon organs receptors that respond to Heterozygous having two unlike genes for a given Hypomania, 469
increases in muscle tension; inhibit further trait, 10, 11 Hypothalamus small area near the base of the
contractions, 235 Hibernation, 288, 289, 313, 315 brain, ventral to the thalamus, 94
Gonadotropins, 66 Hindbrain the posterior part of the brain, 91, arousal and sleep by, 281
Goose bumps, 91 91–92, 93 body temperature and, 302, 303
evolutionary explanation of, 4 Hippocampal commissures, 425 brain and, 93
Graded potential a membrane potential that varies Hippocampus a large structure located toward the eating and, 315–319, 320
in magnitude in proportion to the intensity posterior of the forebrain, between the thala- cannabinoid receptors and, 75
of the stimulus, 45, 46 mus and the cerebral cortex, 95, 97 forebrain and, 94, 96
partial depolarization and, 53 cortisol and, 270 hormone-releasing glands and, 67, 68
Gradual learning, 403 depression and, 465 lesions and, 110, 320, 320
Grasp reflex a reflexive grasp of an object placed effects of damage to, 396–399 panic disorder and, 376
firmly in the hand, 236, 236 entorhinal cortex and, 144 pituitary hormones and, 68
Gray matter areas of the nervous system that are episodic memory and, 398, 400 vasopressin and, 338, 339
densely packed with cell bodies and den- limbic system and, 360 Hypovolemic thirst thirst provoked by low blood
drites, 88, 89 location of, 397 volume, 307, 307
music training and learning to read, 135, 135 new brain neurons and, 126
schizophrenia and, 477, 477, 478 old age and, 139 IBZM (radioactively labeled drug), 480
synesthesia and, 225 PTSD and, 383 Identity position view that mental processes and
Group selection sleep and, 290, 291 certain kinds of brain processes are the same
altruistic behavior and, 17, 18 stress and, 383 thing, described in different terms, 445, 446
Growth hormone (GH), 66, 67, 68 theoretical function of the, 400–402 Immune system structures that protect the body
Gyrus, 88 Histamines, 212, 279, 464 against viruses, bacteria, and other intruders,
Histones, 12 381
Habit learning, 403 Homeostasis tendency to maintain a variable, such bacteria infection and, 382
Habituation a decrease in response to a stimulus as temperature, within a fixed range, 299 leukocytes and, 381
that is presented repeatedly and accompanied Homeothermic using physiological mechanisms to myelin sheath and, 45
by no change in other stimuli, 411, 412 maintain a nearly constant body temperature physical exercise and, 315
Subject Index/Glossary 569

psychoneuroimmunology and, 382 Ionotropic effects synaptic effects that depend on Lateral corticospinal tract a set of axons from the
stress and, 382, 383 the rapid opening of some kind of gate in the primary motor cortex, surrounding areas, and
Immunoglobulins, 125 membrane, 63, 64 midbrain area that is primarily responsible
Implicit learning, 403 Ions for controlling the peripheral muscles, 246,
Implicit memory an influence of experience on gap junction and, 69 247
behavior, even if the influence is not recog- membrane of a neuron and, 38, 38–39, 40 Lateral geniculate nucleus thalamic nucleus that
nized, 399 Iowa Gambling Task, 364 receives incoming visual information, 168,
Impotence the inability to have an erection, 334 Iproniazid, 463 170
INAH-3. See Interstitial nucleus of the anterior Ipsilateral, 88 motion detection and, 188
hypothalamus Ischemia type of stroke resulting from a blood clot Lateral hypothalamus area of the hypothalamus
Indoleamines, 61 or other obstruction in an artery, 141, 142 that controls insulin secretion, alters taste
Infants responsiveness, and facilitates feeding in
mirror neurons and, 243, 243 James-Lange theory proposal that an event first other ways, 317
pair bonding and, 337 provokes the autonomic arousal and skeletal hypothalamic lesions and, 320
primary motor cortex and, 246 responses and that the feeling aspect of emo- pathways from the, 318
reflexes, 236, 237 tion is the perception of those responses, 356, recovery after damage to, 317, 317
REM sleep and, 291 357, 358 satiety and, 316
Infections, 303 Jealousy, 342 Lateral inhibition the reduction of activity in one
Inferior, 88 Jet lag a disruption of circadian rhythms due to neurons by activity in neighboring neurons,
Inferior colliculus swelling on each side of the crossing time zones, 270, 270 170, 171
tectum; important for auditory processing, Joint laxity, 376 Lateral interpositus nucleus (LIP) a nucleus of
93, 93 Journal of Neuroscience, 7 the cerebellum that is essential for learning,
Inferior temporal cortex portion of the cortex 393
where neurons are highly sensitive to com- K-complex a sharp brain wave associated with Lateral preoptic area part of the hypothalamus
plex aspects of the shape of visual stimuli temporary inhibition of neuronal firing, that controls drinking, 306
within very large receptive fields, 182, 183 276–277, 277 hypothalamic lesions and, 320
Influenza, 477 Kenyon cell, 32 Lateralization divisions of labor between the two
Inhibitory cells, 281 Ketamine, 481 brain hemispheres, 422, 423
Inhibitory postsynaptic potential (IPSP) tempo- Ketones, 35 corpus callosum and, 424–427, 428
rary hyperpolarization of a membrane, 55 Kidneys development of, 428, 429
Inner-ear deafness. See Nerve deafness (inner-ear aldosterone and, 307 hemispheric connections and, 423, 424
deafness) hormone-releasing glands and, 67 schizophrenia and, 478
Innervates, 206, 232 renin enzyme and, 307 Laughing gas, 60
Insomnia inadequate sleep, 283, 284 Kin selection selection for a gene that benefits the Law of the conservation of matter and energy, 445
Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, 22 individual’s relatives, 18 Law of specific nerve energies statement that
Instrumental conditioning (operant conditioning) Klüver-Bucy syndrome a behavioral disorder whatever excites a particular nerve always
a type of condition in which reinforcement or caused by temporal lobe damage, 104, 373 sends the same kind of information to the
punishment changes the future probabilities Knee-jerk reflex, 235 brain, 154
of a given behavior, 390, 391 Koniocellular neurons small ganglion cells that L-dopa chemical precursor to dopamine, 256
Insula, 219, 219 occur throughout the retina, 172, 172 Learning
Insular cortex (insula), 360, 361 Korsakoff ’s syndrome (Wernicke-Korsakoff syn- amnesic patients and, 398
Insulin pancreatic hormone that enables glucose to drome) brain damage caused by prolonged basal ganglia and, 403, 404
enter the cells, 313 thiamine deficiency, 404 brain and, 135
carbohydrates and, 62 alcoholism and, 35 cerebral cortex and, 392
hunger and, 313 Krause end bulbs, 205 consolidation and, 416
lateral hypothalamus and, 317 curiosity and, 417
list of hormone-releasing glands and, 67 LAAM (levomethadyl acetate), 81 excitation and, 392
schizophrenia and, 467 Labeled-line principle concept that each receptor excitatory cells and, 281
ventromedial hypothalamus damage and, 319, responds to a limited range of stimuli and has extinction procedures and, 378
320 a direct line to the brain, 215 ghrelin and, 316
Intelligence Lactase intestinal enzyme that metabolizes lactose, Hebbian synapse and, 410
brain size and, 117–119 310, 311 locus coeruleus and, 279
comparison of species and, 117 Lactose sugar in milk, 310 long-term potentiation and, 412–415, 416
comparisons among humans, 118 Lamarckian evolution theory of evolution music and, 135
impaired language and, 435–436 through the inheritance of acquired charac- nervous system and, 409, 410
IQ test and, 407 teristics, 15 sleep and, 283, 290, 291
language and, 436 Lamina(e) layer of cell bodies that are parallel to stimulant drugs and, 416
men vs. women and, 118, 119 the surface of the cerebral cortex and sepa- Stroop effect and, 450
MRI and, 118 rated from each other by layers of fibers, Leptin peptide produced by the body’s fat cells, 67,
schizophrenia and, 473 100 314, 315, 338
Internal regulation composition of, 101 Lesion damage to a structure, 109
hunger and, 309–322 nervous system and, 88 Leukocytes white blood cells, 303, 381
temperature regulation and, 298–303 Language Levomethadyl acetate (LAAM), 81
thirst and, 305–307 absolute pitch and tonal, 197 LH. See Luteinizing hormone (LH)
Interneuron neuron whose axons and dendrites are brain damage and, 438–440 Librium (chlordiazepoxide), 376
all confined within a given structure, 32 dyslexia and, 441–443, 442, 443 Lifeboat dilemma, 363
inhibitory synapses and, 55, 56 Heschl’s gyrus and, 114 Ligand-gated channels channel that opens when a
Intersexes people whose sexual development is human evolution and, 434–437 neurotransmitter attaches, 63
intermediate or ambiguous, 343–346 left hemisphere and, 422 Light, 268–269, 270, 271, 470
Interstitial nucleus of the anterior hypothalamus music and, 441 Limbic system interlinked structures that form a
(INAH-3), 349–351, 350 nonhuman precursors of, 432–433, 434 border around the brainstem, 94, 95, 282,
Intestines, 312 production difficulty and, 438, 439 359
Intracellular fluid, 305 Language acquisition device a built-in mechanism LIP. See Lateral interpositus nucleus (LIP)
Intrinsic neuron neuron whose axons and den- for acquiring language, 437 Lithium element whose salts are often used as
drites are all confined within a given struc- Large-scale integration problem, 105 therapy for bipolar disorder, 469
ture, 32 Lateral, 88 Liver, 67
570 Subject Index/Glossary

Local anesthetics drugs that attach to the sodium Mechanical senses sexual arousal and, 333
channels of the membrane, stopping action itch and, 212–213 testosterone and, 334
potentials, 41 pain and, 207–211, 212 Meninges membranes that surround the brain and
Local neurons neurons without an axon, 45, 46 somatosensation and, 203–206, 207 spinal cord, 98
Locus coeruleus a small structure in the pons that vestibular sensation and, 203 Menstrual cycle a periodic variation in hormones and
emits bursts of impulses in response to Medial, 88 fertility over the course of about 28 days, 335
meaningful events, especially those that pro- Medial corticospinal tract set of axons from many Mental retardation
duce emotional arousal, 279, 281 parts of the cerebral cortex, midbrain, and immunoglobulins and, 125
Long-term depression (LTD) a prolonged medulla; responsible for control of bilateral Prader-Willi syndrome and, 321
decrease in response at a synapse where the muscles of the neck, shoulders, and trunk, syndromal obesity and, 321
axons have been less active than certain other 246, 247 thyroid function and, 131
axons afferent to that neuron, 412 Medial geniculate nucleus, 393 Mentalism view that only the mind really exists
Long-term memory memory of events that Medial preoptic area (MPOA). See MPOA (medial and that the physical world could not exist
occurred further back in time, 394 preoptic area) unless some mind were aware of it, 445
impaired, 397, 398 Medial temporal lobe, 396 Merkel disks, 204, 205
Long-term potentiation (LTP) phenomenon that Medium-wavelength cone, 160, 162 Mesolimbocortical system a set of neurons that
when one or more axons connected to a den- Medulla hindbrain structure located just above the project from the midbrain tegmentum to the
drite bombard it with a rapid series of stim- spinal cord; could be regarded as an enlarged limbic system, 481, 482
uli, some of the synapses become more extension of the spinal cord, 91 Mesostriatal system, 481
responsive to new input of the same type for opiates and, 75, 92 Metabotropic effect a sequence of metabolic reac-
minutes, days, or weeks, 412–416 pain stimuli and, 209 tions that produce slow and long-lasting
Long-wavelength cone, 160, 162 part of the hindbrain, 91, 91 effects at a synapse, 63, 64, 64
Loudness perception of the intensity of a sound, pyramids and, 246 Methadone drug similar to heroin and morphine
194 MEG. See Magnetoencephalograph (MEG) that is taken orally, 80, 74
LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide), 76, 76, 480, 481 Meissner’s corpuscles, 204, 205 Methamphetamine, 73
LTD. See Long-term depression (LTD) Melanocortin type of chemical that promotes sati- Methyl groups, 13
LTP. See Long-term potentiation (LTP) ety, 315, 316 Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) or
Luteinizing hormone (LH) hormone released obesity and, 320 “ectasy,” 76, 76
from the anterior pituitary that causes the Melanopsin, 273 Methylphenidate (Ritalin) stimulant drug pre-
follicle to release an ovum, 66, 67, 68, 335, Melatonin hormone that influences both circadian scribed for ADHD that increases the stimu-
335 and circannual rhythms, 67, 273–274 lation of dopamine synpases by blocking the
Luvox (fluvoxamine), 464 Membrane structure that separates the inside of reuptake of dopamine by the presynaptic
the cell from the outside environment, 29, neuron, 74, 76, 416
Machado-Joseph disease, 259 29, 30 Microdeletions, 12, 476
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) method of Memory Microduplications, 12, 15, 476
imaging a living brain by using a magnetic basal ganglia and, 403, 404 Microelectrode
field and a radio frequency field to make benzodiazepines and, 377 cerebral cortex and, 173, 174
atoms with odd atomic weights all rotate in blood–brain barrier and, 34 neurons and, 53
the same direction and then removing those brain damage retrieval and, 148, 149 resting potential and, 37, 38
fields and measuring the energy that the cerebral cortex and, 392 Microglia cells that remove waste material and
atoms release , 114, 116, 135 consolidation and, 395 other microorganisms from the nervous sys-
Magnetoencephalograph (MEG) a device that consumption of fish and, 311 tem, 33, 33
measures the faint magnetic fields generated ECT and, 467 Midbrain middle part of the brain, 93, 93
by brain activity, 111, 112, 135 hippocampus and, 97, 396–402 wakefulness and, 279
Magnocellular neurons large cell bodies with large Huntington’s disease and, 258, 260 Middle temporal cortex (MT or V5) area of the
receptive fields that are distributed evenly improving, 416–417 brain that detects moving objects, 186, 187
throughout the retina, 172, 172, 186 Korsakoff ’s syndrome and, 35 Middle-ear deafness. See Conductive deafness
Major depression a condition in which people feel learning and, 391, 392 (middle-ear deafness)
sad and helpless every day for weeks at a locus coeruleus and, 279 Midget ganglion cells ganglion cells in the fovea of
time, 460 long-term/short-term, 394 humans and other primates, 157
Major histocompatibility complex, 342 marijuana and, 75 Migraine headache
Mamillary bodies, 360 new neurons and, 126 light and, 273
Mammals, 144, 100, 100 old age and, 139 meninges and, 98
Mania a condition characterized by restless activity, Parkinson’s disease and, 254 Migration movement of brain neurons or glia, 125
excitement, laughter, self-confidence, ram- schizophrenia and, 478 MIH. See Müllerian inhibiting hormone (MIH)
bling speech, and loss of inhibitions, 469 sex hormones and, 334 Mind–brain problem (or mind–body problem)
MAO (monoamine oxidase) enzyme that converts sleep and, 290, 291 question about the relationship between
catecholamines and serotonin into synapti- spatial neglect and, 452 mental experience and brain activity, 3, 445
cally inactive forms, 62 stress and, 380, 383, 395 Minimally conscious state condition of decreased
MAOI. See Monoamine oxidase inhibitors taste experience, 219 brain activity with occasional, brief periods of
(MAOIs) types of, 394–395, 396 purposeful actions and limited speech com-
Marijuana working, 395, 396, 473 prehension, 276
as abused drug, 75, 76 Men Miraculin, 217
demasculinization of early development and, alcoholism and, 77, 78 Mirror neurons cells that are active during a move-
331 androgens and, 329 ment and while watching someone else per-
stroke treatment and, 142, 143 dopamine and castrated, 333 form the same movement, 243
Masking use of one stimulus to block perception of hormones and sex of, 328, 328 Mitochondrion structure that performs metabolic
another, 447 mating behavior and, 341, 342 activities, 29, 29, 30
Mass action concept that the cortex works as a penile erection and, 333 Mixed agonist-antagonist, 71
whole and the more cortex, the better, 392 puberty and, 346 Möbius syndrome, 358, 359
Materialism view that everything that exists is REM behavior disorder and, 285 Molarity, 305
material or physical, 445 REM sleep and, 278 Molindone, 480
MDMA (methylenedioxymethamphetamine or schizophrenia and, 474 Monism belief that the universe consists of only
“ectasy,”) 76, 76 sex orientation and, 347 one kind of substance, 445
Subject Index/Glossary 571

Monoamine oxidase (MAOA), 367 Parkinson’s disease and, 254 Neural Darwinism principle of competition
Monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) drugs proprioceptor and control of, 235, 235, 236 among axons, 129
that block the enzyme monoamine oxidase Music, 441 Neuroanatomy the anatomy of the nervous sys-
(MAO), a presynaptic terminal enzyme that Mutation a heritable change in a DNA molecule, tem, 85
metabolizes catecholamines and serotonin 11, 15 Neurodevelopmental hypothesis proposal that
into inactive forms, 464 Myasthenia gravis, 381 schizophrenia begins with abnormalities in
Monoamines chemicals formed by a change in cer- Myelin an insulating material composed of fats and the prenatal or neonatal development of the
tain amino acids, 60, 61 proteins, 44, 125 nervous system, based on either genetics or
Monosodium glutamate (MSG), 217 Myelin sheath insulating material that covers ver- other influences, 476–478, 479
Monozygotic twins twins derived from one egg, tebrate axon, 30, 31 Neurogliaform cell a kind of neuron that releases
13, 474, 475, 477 glia cells and, 33 huge amounts of GABA all at once, produc-
Mood disorders immune system and, 45 ing widespread inhibition, 65
antidepressant drugs and, 463–466 Myelinated axons axons covered with myelin Neurology, 7
bipolar disorder as, 469, 470 sheaths, 44, 45 Neuromodulators. See Neuropeptides
major depressive disorder and, 460–462, 463 Myelination process by which glia produce Neuromuscular junction a synapse between a
psychotic features and, 473 the insulating fatty sheaths that accelerate motor neuron axon and a muscle fiber, 232
seasonal affective disorder as, 470 transmission in many vertebrate axons, Neurons cells that receive information and trans-
Moperone, 480 125 mit it to other cells, 28
Morphine, 76 brain and, 62, 124–125, 126
as opiate drug, 74 Naloxone (Revia), 80 cannabinoids and, 75
pain reliever and, 208 Narcolepsy a condition characterized by frequent development of nervous system and, 130
postsurgical pain and, 211 periods of sleepiness during the day, 284 diverse shapes of, 32
use of, 212 Narcotics Anonymous, 80 explanation of, 3
Morris water maze a procedure used to test for Natural killer cells, 381, 382 glucose and, 35
spatial memory in nonhumans, 401, 401 Natural selection, 17 magnification of, 4
Motion blindness an impaired ability to perceive Nature Neuroscience, 7 neurotransmitters and, 62, 130
movement, 187 Nausea recycling system of, 33
Motor neuron neuron that receives excitation from autonomic nervous system and, 356 resting potential of, 37–39, 40
other neurons and conducts impulses to a marijuana and, 75 sex hormones and, 330
muscle, 30, 31 serotonin 3 receptor and, 335 sodium and potassium ions in, 38, 39
inhibitory synapses and, 56 Neck sensations, 92 stroke and, 142
spinal cord and, 62, 88 Necrosis, 130 structure of, 29–32
synapses and, 52 Negative color afterimage result of staring at a synthesis of transmitters and, 61, 61, 62
Motor program a fixed sequence of movements, colored object for a prolonged length of time Neuropeptide Y (NPY) peptide that blocks the
237 and then looking at a white surface, the image satiety actions of the paraventricular nucleus,
Movement is seen as a negative image, with a replace- 61, 316
brain mechanisms of, 240–252 ment of red with green, green with red, yel- Neuropeptides chains of amino acids, 60
cerebellum and, 248–249 low and blue with each other, and black and distinctive features of, 64, 65, 65
control of, 231–238 white with each other, 161, 162, 163 endorphins as, 74, 75
feedback and, 237 Negative feedback homeostatic processes that resynthesized, 68
matter and muscle, 445 reduce discrepancies from the set point, 299 Neuroscience, 3, 29, 55
motor learning and, 251 Negative symptoms absence of behaviors ordinar- Neurotransmitters chemicals released by neurons
muscles and, 232–235, 236 ily seen in normal people (e.g., lack of emo- that affect other neurons, 60
planning, 242, 243 tional expression) 473 chemical steps in synthesis of, 61
Purkinje cells and, 249 Neglect (attention), 451, 452 drugs and, 71
units of, 236–238 Nerve, 88 as first messenger system, 64
voluntary/involuntary, 236, 237 Nerve deafness (inner-ear deafness) hearing loss neurotrophins and, 130
Movement disorders that results from damage to the cochlea, the release and diffusion of, 62
Huntington’s disease, 258–259 hair cells, or the auditory nerve, 200 storage of, 62
Parkinson’s disease, 254–257 Nerve growth factor (NGF) a protein that pro- types of, 60, 61
tardive dyskinesia, 481, 482, 482 motes the survival and growth of axons in the Neurotrophin a chemical that promotes the
MPOA (medial preoptic area), 333, 338, 339 sympathetic nervous system and certain survival and activity of neurons, 130, 257,
MPP1 a chemical that accumulates in, and then axons in the brain, 130 465
destroys, neurons that release dopamine, 255, Nervous system NGF. See Nerve growth factor (NGF)
256 anatomical directions in the, 87, 88 Nicotine a stimulant drug that stimulates certain
MPTP a chemical that the body converts to animal cell structures and, 28–29, 30 acetylcholine receptors, 74
MPP1, 255, 256 cell loss and development of, 130, 130 effects of, 76
MRI. See Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) cerebral cortex and, 100–107 genes and, 77
mRNA concentrations, 273 chemical events at a synapse and, 60–69 withdrawal, 79
MSG. See Monosodium glutamate (MSG) chemical transmission and the, 59, 59 Night terrors experiences of intense anxiety
MST. See Middle temporal cortex control of movement and, 236–238 from which a person awakens screaming in
MT. See Middle temporal cortex early development of, 124 terror; more severe than a nightmare,
mTOR (mammalian Target Of Rapamycin), 144 glia and, 33, 34 285
Müllerian ducts, 328, 328 glutamate and, 63 Nitric oxide (NO) a gas released by many small
Müllerian inhibiting hormone (MIH), 328 inhibitory synapses and, 55, 56 local neurons, 61
Multiple personality disorder, 472 nerve cells and, 28–35 neurotransmitters and, 60, 62
Multiplier effect, 333 nerve impulses and, 37–45, 46 penile erection and, 333
Muscle spindle a receptor parallel to the muscle neurons and, 30–32, 33 postsynaptic neurons and, 69
that responds to a stretch, 235 structure of the vertebrate, 85–98 retrograde transmitter and, 416
Muscles terminology of the, 86–87, 88 NMDA glutamate receptor, 481, 481
categories of, 232 types of research methods and the, 109–119 NMDA receptor a glutamate receptor that can
contraction of, 232, 235 viruses and, 34 respond to the drug N-methyl-D-aspartate
fast and slow, 234 visual wavelength cones and, 160 (NMDA), 413–416, 414, 415
fish and use of, 234 wiring diagram of the, 57, 57 Nocebos (antiplacebos), 211
572 Subject Index/Glossary

Nodes of Ranvier interruptions in the myelin nerve, 92 Ovum, 335


sheath of vertebrate axons, 30 receptors, 125, 222 Oxygen
action potential and, 44, 45, 45 Olfactory cells neurons responsible for smell, debt, 234
Non-REM (NREM) sleep stages of sleep other located on the olfactory epithelium in the eyes and, 292
than REM, 278, 285, 291, 291 rear of the nasal air passages, 222, 224 Oxytocin hormone released by posterior pituitary;
Nontasters, 219, 219 Oligodendrocytes glia cells that build myelin important for sexual and parental behaviors,
Norepinephrine sheaths, 33, 33 66, 67, 68
amphetamine and, 73 Ondansetron, 64 behavioral change and, 337
catecholamines and, 61 Ontogenetic explanation understanding in
hormone, 67 terms of how a structure or behavior devel- Pacinian corpuscle receptor that responds to a
metabotropic synapses and, 64 ops, 4, 6 sudden displacement of the skin or high-fre-
as a neurotransmitter, 61 Opiate drugs drugs derived from the opium poppy, quency vibrations on the skin, 204, 205
pathways and the brain, 319 74 Pain
postganglionic synapses and, 91 dull pain relief and, 211 cannabinoids and, 75
REM sleep and, 282 effects of, 76 itch and, 213
Nose. See Olfaction; Smell itch and, 213 messages and the brain, 209, 209
Novacain, 213 medications to combat abuse of, 80, 81 metabotropic synapses and, 64
NPY. See Neuropeptide Y (NPY) medulla and, 75 relieving, 209–210, 211
NTS. See Nucleus of the tractus solitarius (NTS) Opioid mechanisms systems that respond to opi- sensitization of, 211, 212
Nuclei of the cerebellum clusters of cell bodies in ate drugs and similar chemicals, 209 social, 212
the interior of the cerebellum, 249 Opponent-process theory idea that we perceive stimuli and paths of, 208, 209
Nucleosome, 12 color in terms of opposites, 162 Pancreas, 67
Nucleus structure that contains the chromosomes, Opsins, 159 Panic attack period marked by extreme sympa-
29 Optic chiasm area where axons from each eye cross thetic nervous system arousal, 358
animal cells and, 29 to the opposite side of the brain, 424 Panic disorder condition marked by frequent
cranial nerves and, 92 axons and connections to, 423, 423 periods of anxiety and rapid breathing,
nervous system and, 88 brain and, 66 increased heart rate, sweating, and trem-
neuron and, 31 optic nerves and, 168, 303 bling, 376
Nucleus accumbens brain area that is rich in dopa- suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) and, 271 Papillae, 216
mine and is central to the brain’s reinforce- visual system and, 170 Paradoxical sleep sleep that is deep in some ways
ment system, 72 Optic nerve ganglion cell axons that exit through and light in others, 278
alcohol and, 77 the back of the eye and continue to the brain, See also Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep
marijuana and, 75 155 Parallel fibers, 249, 250
nicotine and, 74 brain and, 95 Paralysis, 246
reinforcements and response of, 79 cranial nerves and, 92 Paraplegia, 246
sagittal section and, 93 ganglion cell axons and, 155, 168 Parasympathetic nervous system system of nerves
Nucleus basalis a forebrain structure that lies on retina and, 169 that facilitate vegetative, nonemergency
the ventral surface; receives input from the retinohypothalamic path and, 271 responses by the body’s organs, 89, 90, 158,
hypothalamus and basal ganglia; sends sensory axons and, 127 356, 357
axons to areas in the cerebral cortex, 96, 97 visual system and, 170 Parathyroid, 67
Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease, 97 Optogenetics, 110 Parathyroid hormone, 67
Nucleus of the tractus solitarius (NTS) structure Orbitofrontal cortex, 406, 407 Paraventricular nucleus (PVN) part of the hypo-
in the medulla that receives input from taste Orexin (hypocretin) neurotransmitter that thalamus in which activity tends to limit meal
receptors, 219, 219, 318 increases wakefulness and arousal, 279 size and damage leads to excessively large
NutraSweet, 14 anxiety and, 376 meals, 306, 317
Nutritional abnormalities, 473, 476 narcolepsy and, 285 hypothalamic lesions and, 320
panic disorder and, 376 satiety and, 316
Obesity. See Eating disorders satiety and, 316 Parental behavior, 338–339
Occipital lobe posterior section of the cerebral cor- wakefulness and, 280 intersexes and, 344–346
tex, 102 Organizing effects long-lasting effects of a hor- Parietal cortex
brain and, 93 mone that are present during a sensitive attention and, 450
cerebral cortex and, 103 period early in development, 330 stimulus and activity in, 447
touch stimuli and, 133 sexual orientation and, 348 Parietal lobe section of the cerebral cortex between
visual pathways and the, 182 Orgasm the occipital lobe and the central sulcus, 102
Oculomotor nerve, 92 behavioral change and, 337 cerebral cortex and, 103
Odors. See Olfaction CAH and, 345, 346 visual pathways and the, 182
Office of Animal Care and Use, 23 drugs and, 333 Parkinson’s disease malady caused by damage to a
Olanzapine, 482 Orlistat (Xenical), 322 dopamine pathway, resulting in slow move-
Old age Osmotic pressure tendency of water to flow across ments, difficulty initiating movements, rigid-
behavior, 139 a semipermeable membrane from the area of ity of the muscles, and tremors, 254
brain damage recovery behavior and, 149 low solute concentration to the area of high basal ganglia and, 96, 403
genetics and, 17 solute concentration, 305, 306 brain damage and, 141
Oleoylethanolamide (OEA), 312 Osmotic thirst thirst triggered by certain neurons causes of, 254, 255, 255
Olfaction the sense of smell, which is the response that detect the loss of their own water, 305, insomnia and, 283
to chemicals that contact the membranes 306 L-dopa treatment of, 256, 257
inside the nose, 221, 222 Otoliths, 203, 204 lifestyles and, 255, 256
coding implications and, 222 Oval window a membrane of the inner ear, 195, 195 nucleus basalis and, 97
mate preference and, 342 Ovary (ovaries) other treatments for, 257
Parkinson’s disease and, 254 hormone-releasing glands and, 67 REM behavior disorder and, 285
receptors and, 222 pituitary gland and, 336, 336 Parvocellular neurons small cell bodies with small
schizophrenia and, 474 SRY gene and, 328, 328 receptive fields in or near the fovea, 172, 172
Olfactory OVLT (organum vasculosum laminae termina- Paxil (paroxetine), 464
bulb and brain, 103, 126, 222–223, 224 lis) brain area that detects osmotic pressure PCP. See Phencyclidine
bulb and limbic system, 360 and salt content of the blood, 306, 307 Penis, 328, 328, 333, 345
Subject Index/Glossary 573

Peptide hormones hormones composed of short lateral hypothalamus and, 318 Prefrontal cortex anterior portion of the frontal
chains of amino acids, 61, 65 ovary and, 336, 336 lobe, which responds mostly to the sensory
Perfect pitch, 197 PKU. See Phenylketonuria (PKU) stimuli that signal the need for a movement,
Periaqueductal gray area area of the brainstem Place theory concept that pitch perception 104, 138
that is rich in enkephalin synapses, 209, 210 depends on which part of the inner ear attention/distraction in, 450
Periodic limb movement disorder a sleep disorder has cells with the greatest activity level, autonomic responses and, 358
characterized by repeated involuntary move- 196 delayed response task and, 395, 396
ment of the legs and sometimes the arms, Placebo a drug or other procedure with no phar- depression and, 463
285 macological effects, 211 dreams and, 242, 292, 293
Periovulatory period time around the middle of Planum temporale section of the temporal cortex D2 receptors and, 480
the menstrual cycle of maximum fertility and that is larger in the left hemisphere, 429, 429 emotions and, 363
high estrogen levels, 336 POA/AH. See Preoptic area/anterior hypothala- glutamate synapses and, 481
Peripheral nervous system (PNS) nerves outside mus (POA/AH) memory and, 396
the brain and spinal cord, 86, 86 Poikilothermic maintaining the body at the same movement inhibitions and, 251, 252
Personality temperature as the environment, 300, 301 schizophrenia and abnormalities of, 478, 479
hemispheric activity and, 361 Polarization. See Electrical gradient stimulus and activity in, 447
PET. See Position-emission tomography (PET) Poliomyelitis, 246 Prefrontal lobotomy surgical disconnection of the
PGO waves a distinctive pattern of high-amplitude Polysomnograph a combination of EEG and eye- prefrontal cortex from the rest of the brain,
electrical potentials that occur first in the movement records, 276, 277 105, 105
pons, then in the lateral geniculate, and then Pons hindbrain structure that lies anterior and Preganglionic axons, 90, 90
in the occipital cortex, 282, 283 ventral to the medulla, 92, 93, 210, 282, Premotor cortex area of the frontal cortex, active
Phantom limb a continuing sensation of an ampu- 293 during the planning of a movement, 241,
tated body part, 147 Pontomesencephalon part of the reticular forma- 242
Phase difference, 201 tion that contributes to cortical arousal, 279, Prenatal environment
Phencyclidine (PCP) (angel dust) drug that inhib- 281 alcohol and, 77
its the NMDA glutamate receptors, 481 Position-emission tomography (PET) method of schizophrenia and, 476
Phenothiazines a chemical family that includes mapping activity in a living brain by record- Preoptic area/anterior hypothalamus (POA/
antipsychotic drugs (chlorpromazine) that ing the emission of radioactivity from AH) brain area important for temperature
relieve the positive symptoms of schizophre- injected chemicals, 111, 112 regulation, thirst, and sexual behavior, 303
nia, 479, 480 REM sleep and, 282 hypothalamic lesions and, 320
Phentermine, 321, 322 Positive symptoms presence of behaviors not seen Prepulse inhibition, 347
Phenylalanine, 61, 62, 370 in normal people, 472 Presynaptic neuron neuron that delivers transmis-
Phenylketonuria (PKU) a genetic inability to Postcentral gyrus area just posterior to the central sion to another neuron, 53, 415, 416
metabolize the amino acid phenylalanine, 14 gyrus; primary receptor site for touch and Presynaptic terminal (end bulb or bouton) point
amino acids and, 61 other body sensations, 102, 103 where an axon releases chemicals, 30, 31
Phenythiocarbamide (PTC), 219 hearing and hand movement, 135, 135, 136 astrocytes and, 33, 34
Pheromones chemicals released by an animal that Posterior, 88 calcium and, 62
affect the behavior of other members of the Posterior parietal cortex area with a mixture of Primary auditory cortex (area A1) area in the
same species, 224 visual, somatosensory, and movement func- superior temporal cortex in which cells
maternal behavior and, 338, 339 tions, particularly in monitoring the position of respond best to tones of a particular fre-
Phi phenomenon tendency to see something as the body relative to objects in the world, 242 quency, 198, 198, 199, 199
moving back and forth between positions Posterior pituitary portion of the pituitary gland, Primary motor cortex area of the prefrontal cortex
when in fact it is alternately blinking on and which releases hormones synthesized by the just anterior to the central sulcus; a primary
off in those positions, 449 hypothalamus, 66, 302 point of origin for axons conveying messages
Phospholipids, 37 neural signals and, 68 to the spinal cord, 104, 137, 240
Photopigments chemicals contained in rods and Postganglionic fibers, 90, 90, 91 areas of control, 240, 241
cones that release energy when struck by Postpartum depression depression after giving movement and, 103
light, 159 birth, 462, 463 outcome and, 242
Phrenology a process of relating skull anatomy to Postsynaptic neuron neuron that receives trans- stimulation and, 242, 242
behavior, 114, 115 mission from another neuron, 53 Primary somatosentory cortex, 102, 103
Phthalates, 331, 333 activating receptors and, 63–65, 66 Primary visual cortex (area V1) area of the cortex
Physical exercise, 315 negative feedback and, 69 responsible for the first stage of visual pro-
Physiological explanation understanding in terms NGF and, 130 cessing, 172
of the activity of the brain and other organs, retrograde transmitters and, 60 blindsight and, 173
4, 6 synaptic activation and, 54 columnar organization of the, 175, 175, 176
Pimozide, 480 Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) a condi- dreams and, 172
Pineal gland an endocrine gland located just poste- tion resulting from a severe traumatic experi- eyes and, 102
rior to the thalamus that releases the hor- ence, leading to a long-lasting state of fre- feature detectors and, 176
mone melatonin, 273 quent distressing recollections (flashbacks) impaired infant vision and, 179
hormone-releasing, 67 and nightmares about the traumatic event, pathways in the, 182, 182, 183
mind and brain interaction point at, 445 avoidance of reminders of it, and exaggerated types of cells and, 173–174, 174, 175, 175
SCN and, 272 arousal in response to noises and other stim- ventral/dorsal stream and, 182
Pinna the outer ear structure of flesh and cartilage uli, 383 Primates order of mammals that includes mon-
that sticks out from each side of the head, propanolol and, 378 keys, apes, and humans, 100
194 startle reflex and, 371 brain size of, 101
Pitch the related aspect of perception, 194, 196– Potassium channels, 41 human brains vs. chimpanzees, 124
197 Potassium ions midget ganglion cells and, 157
Pituitary gland an endocrine gland attached to the concentration gradient and, 39, 41, 41 new neurons and, 126
base of the hypothalamus, 65, 66, 96 electrical gradient and, 38, 41, 41 Procedural memory the development of motor
body temperature and, 302 Prader-Willi syndrome, 321 skills and habits; a special kind of implicit
brain and, 93 Precentral gyrus posterior portion of the frontal memory, 399
forebrain and, 94 lobe just anterior to the central sulcus; spe- Prochlorperazine, 480
function of, 68 cialized for fine movement control, 104 Productivity ability of language to produce new
hormone-releasing glands and, 67 music training and learning to read, 135, 135 signals to represent new ideas, 432
574 Subject Index/Glossary

Progesterone steroid hormone that prepares the Readiness potential recordable activity in the obesity and, 314, 314
uterus for the implantation of a fertilized motor cortex prior to voluntary movement, orexin and, 280
ovum and promotes the maintenance of preg- 244 organizing effects of sex hormones and, 331,
nancy, 329 Receptive field the area in visual space that excites 332
list of hormone-releasing glands and, 67 or inhibits any neuron, 171, 171 Parkinson’s disease and, 257
menstrual cycle and, 335, 335 simple and complex, 173–174, 175 pattern codes and, 217
Prolactin, 66, 67, 68, 338 Recessive gene one that shows effects only in the PGO waves and, 282
Proliferation production of new cells, 124, 131 homozygous condition, 10 reasons for, 21, 22
Promazine, 480 Reciprocal altruism helping others who may be reorganized sensory representations and, 146
Propagation of the action potential transmission helpful in return, 18 SCN and, 271, 272
of an action potential down an axon, 43, 44 Reconsolidation restrengthening of a memory by a serotonin and aggressive behavior of, 369
Propanolol, 378 similar later experience, 378, 395 sex hormones and rodents, 333, 334
Proprioceptor a receptor that detects the position Red nucleus a midbrain area that is primarily sexual orientation and, 351
or movement of a part of the body, 235, 236 responsible for controlling the arm muscles, sleep and, 277, 278
Prosopagnosia the inability to recognize faces due 246 sleep apnea and, 284
to damage of several brain areas, 185 engram and, 394 startle reflex and, 371, 372
Prostaglandins, 303, 381 learning and, 393 stimulant drugs and, 144
Prostate, 328 Reflex arc a circuit from sensory neuron to muscle stress and, 383
Protein hormones hormones composed of long response, 52, 52 stroke treatments and, 142
chains of amino acids, 65 Reflexes automatic muscular responses to stimuli, testosterone and, 331, 332
Proteins 52, 236 toy choices and, 332
amino acids and, 61 infants and, 236, 237 use of, 463
DNA and histones, 12 knee-jerk, 235 visual cortex and, 173, 174
11-cis-retinal and, 159 speed of conduction and, 53, 53 Research methods
genes and, 14 Refractory period time when the cell resists the brain anatomy correlated with behavior and,
high temperature and, 302 production of further action potentials, 43 114–116
PER and TIM, 273, 273, 274 Reinforcer any event that increases the future brain damage effects and, 109, 110
ribosomes and, 30 probability of the preceding response, 390, brain stimulation effects and, 110, 111
RNA bases and, 10, 10 391 recording brain activity and, 111–113, 114
Proximal, 88 Relative refractory period time after the absolute Resistance (stress stage), 380
Prozac, 464 refractory period that requires a stronger Resting potential
Psychoneuroimmunology study of the ways in stimulus to initiate an action potential, 43 neurons and measuring, 37, 38
which experiences, especially stressful ones, Releasing hormone hormone released by the reason for, 40
alter the immune system and how the hypothalamus that flows through the blood Reticular formation a structure that extends from
immune system influences the central ner- to the anterior pituitary, 66 the medulla into the forebrain; controls
vous system, 382 REM. See Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep motor areas of the spinal cord and selectively
Psychotherapy, 466 REM behavior disorder a condition in which peo- increases arousal and attention in various
PTSD. See Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) ple move around vigorously during REM forebrain areas, 92, 279
Puberty sleep, 285, 291, 292 Retina the rear surface of the eye, which is lined
leptin and, 315 Renin, 67, 307 with visual receptors, 155
Pudendal nerve, 333 Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, 467 distribution of cones in, 161
Punishment an event that suppresses the fre- Reproductive behaviors hemispheres and, 423
quency of the preceding response, 390, 391 sex and hormones with, 327–339 lateral inhibition and, 168–170, 171
Pupil an opening in the center of the iris where sexual behavior variations and, 341–350, organization of the vertebrate, 169
light enters, 155 351 periphery of the, 157
Pure autonomic failure condition when output Research animals primate ganglion cells and, 172, 172
from the autonomic nervous system to the alcohol and, 377 rods and cones of the, 158–159, 159
body fails, 357 amygdala damage and, 373 route within, 155, 155–156, 157
Purines a category of chemicals including adenos- anti-anxiety drugs and, 376 Retinal disparity the discrepancy between what
ine and several of its derivatives, 60, 61 antidepressant drugs and, 292 the left and right eyes see, 178
Purkinje cell flat cells in sequential planes, in the axon growth and, 144 Retinal ganglion cells, 273
cerebellar cortex, parallel to one another, 32, basal ganglia and, 250, 251 Retinex theory concept that the cortex compares
33, 249, 250 brain damage and, 109, 110, 479, 479 information from various parts of the retina
Putamen large subcortical structure, part of the brain mechanisms of movement and, 240 to determine the brightness and color for
basal ganglia, 250 circadian rhythms and, 271, 273, 274 each area, 163, 164, 165
PVN. See Paraventricular nucleus (PVN) color vision deficiency, 165 Retinohypothalamic path, 271
Pyramidal cell, 32 cytokines and, 477 Retinotopic organization, 170, 170
Pyramidal tract, 246 deafferented limbs and, 148 Retrograde amnesia loss of memory for events
declarative memory and, 400 that occurred before brain damage, 396
Quadriplegia, 246 delayed response task and, 396 Retrograde transmitter a transmitter released by a
diet and exercise of, 321 postsynaptic cell that travels back to the pre-
Radial glia cells that guide the migration of neu- engram of memory and, 392, 393 synaptic cell to modify it, 416
rons and the growth of axons and dendrites enhanced memory and, 416 Reuptake reabsorption of a neurotransmitter by
during embryological development, 33, 33 ethical debate and, 22–23 the presynaptic terminal, 67
Radial maze an apparatus used to test spatial food deprivation and, 322 drugs and, 71, 73, 74
memory in nonhumans, 400, 401 four tastes and, 218 Reversed polarity, 41, 41
Raphe nucleus, 303 gene and olfactory bulb, 224 Revia (naloxone), 80
Raphe system brain areas that send axons to much glutamate receptors and, 481 Rheumatoid arthritis, 381
of the forebrain, modifying the brain’s readi- Huntington’s disease and, 260 Rhythms, 266, 268
ness to respond to stimuli, 93 increased appetite and, 319 Ribonucleic acid (RNA) a single-strand chemical,
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep sleep stage language and, 432–433, 434 10, 10
with rapid eye movements, high brain activ- learning and, 390, 409, 410 Ribosomes sites for cell synthesization of new pro-
ity, and relaxation of the large muscles, 278, middle temporal cortex and, 187 tein molecules, 28
291, 291, 292 MPP1 and, 256 Risperidone, 482
Subject Index/Glossary 575

Ritalin, 74, 76, 416 Sensitive period time early in development when Sex-linked gene gene on either the X or the
Rods type of retinal receptor that detects bright- experiences have a particularly strong and Y chromosome, 11
ness of light, 158 enduring influence, 177 Sexsomnia, 285
Ro15-4513, 377 language learning and, 437 Sexual behavior
Rooting reflex when an infant’s cheek is touched, organizing effects of sex hormones and, 330 dopamine and, 333
the infant turns toward the stimulated cheek sexual orientation and, 348 evolutionary interpretations of, 341–342, 343
and begins to suck, 236, 236 Sensitization an increase in response to mild stim- gender-differentiated, 344–346
Ruffini endings, 204, 205 uli as a result of exposure to more intense gender identity and, 343, 344
stimuli, 411 hormonal changes and, 333
Saccades voluntary eye movements, 188 Sensory neuron neuron that is highly sensitive mate preferences and, 342
cerebellar damage and, 248 to a specific type of stimulation, 30, 31, 32, sexual orientation and, 347-350, 351
Saccule, 203, 204 52 Sexual orientation
SAD. See Seasonal affective disorder (SAD) Sensory receptors, 205 behavioral and anatomical differences in, 347
Sadness, 360 Sensory systems brain anatomy and, 349–351
Sagittal plane, 87, 88, 93 audition and, 194–201 genetics and, 347, 348
Saltatory conduction the jumping of action poten- chemical senses and, 215–226 Sexual selection tendency for a gene to spread in
tials from node to node, 45, 45 mechanical senses and, 203–213 the population if it makes individuals more
Satiety, 312, 315 Sentinel behavior watching for danger and warn- appealing to the opposite sex, 341
Schizophrenia a psychotic disorder characterized ing others, 18 Sexually dimorphic nucleus area in the anterior
by a deteriorating ability to function in every- Septal nuclei, 360 hypothalamus that is larger in males than in
day life and by some combination of halluci- Septum, 71 females and contributes to control of male
nations, delusions, thought disorder, move- Serotonin sexual behavior, 331
ment disorder, and inappropriate emotional amphetamine and, 73 Sexually transmitted diseases (STD), 336
expressions, 472 brain and, 64 SFO. See Subfornical organ (SFO)
brain abnormalities and, 477–478 “ectasy” and, 76 Sham lesion, 110
brain development and, 475 effects of, 370, 371 Sham-feeding procedure in which everything that
chlorpromazine and, 463 LSD and, 76 an animal swallows leaks out a tube con-
demographic data and, 474 as a neurotransmitter, 61 nected to the esophagus or stomach, 312
diagnosis of, 472–473, 474 REM sleep and, 282 Shivering, 300, 301, 303, 309
genetics and, 474–475, 476 sexual behavior and, 333 Short-term memory memory of events that have
hallucinations and, 458 social isolation and, 369 just occurred, 394, 395
neurodevelopmental hypothesis and, 476–478, stress and effect of, 461 Short-wavelength cones, 160, 162
479 synthesis of, 61 Sibutramine (Meridia), 322
patient and brain activity, 482 turnover, 369 Sign language, 432, 437
symptoms of, 472, 473 violence and, 368 Sildenafil (Viagra), 333
treatments of, 479–482 Serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors Simple cell type of visual cortex cell that has a
Schwann cells glia cells that build myelin sheaths, (SNRIs) drugs that block the reuptake of receptive field with fixed excitatory and
33, 33 serotonin and norepinephrine, 464 inhibitory zones, 174, 174
Scientific American Mind, 7 Seroxat (paroxetine), 464 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP), 219
SCN. See Suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) Set point a value that the body works to maintain, Skeletal (striated) muscles muscles that control
Seasonal affective disorder (SAD) depression 299 movement of the body in relation to the envi-
that recurs during a particular season, such as Sex differences ronment, 232, 233
winter, 470 abilities and, 118, 119 Skin sensations
Season-of-birth effect tendency for people born in androgen and estrogens as, 329 cranial nerves and, 92
winter to have a slightly greater probability of brain activity and, 111 reorganized sensory representations and, 146
developing schizophrenia than people born at brain size and intelligence in, 118–119 somatosensory receptors and, 204
other times of the year, 476, 477 childhood behavior and, 332, 332, 333 Sleep
Second messenger a chemical that, when activated chromosomes and, 330 alcohol and, 283
by a neurotransmitter, initiates communica- circadian rhythms and, 269 bipolar disorder and, 470
tion to many areas within the neuron, 64 early sensitive period and, 331 brain activity and, 276
Secondary visual cortex (area V2) area of the human genitals and, 328 brain activity inhibition and, 281, 282
brain that processes information from the hypothalamus and, 331–332 brain mechanisms and, 279–281, 282
primary visual cortex and transmits it to mating behavior and, 341–342, 343 depression and, 468
additional areas, 182, 183 odors and, 224 deprivation, 288, 291
Second-generation antipsychotics drugs that alle- pheromones and reactions by, 224, 225 disorders of, 283–285
viate schizophrenia without producing move- taste sensitivity and, 220 energy conservation and, 288–289
ment problems, 482, 482 touch sensitivity and, 204 functions of, 288–291, 292
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) Sex and hormones histamines and, 279
drug that blocks the reuptake of serotonin in activating effects of, 333, 334 jet lag and, 270, 271
the presynaptic terminal, 464 organizing effects of, 330–332, 333 learning and, 290
Selectively permeability ability of some chemicals parental behavior and, 338–339 melatonin and, 274
to pass more freely than others through a prenatal development of, 328 memory and, 290, 291
membrane, 38 unisex structures and, 328, 328 orexin and, 280
Self-control, 406 Sex hormones paradoxical or REM, 277–278, 278, 279
Self-stimulation of the brain behavior that is rein- activating effects of, 333, 334 proteins and genes, 273, 273, 274
forced by electrical stimulation of a brain humans and, 334–337 rhythms and, 267
area, 71, 73, 79 hypothalamus and, 331, 332 shift work and, 270
Semantic dementia a loss of semantic memory, memory and, 334 sleepwalking and, 281, 285
406 organizing effects of, 330–332, 333 stages of, 276, 277, 278
Semicircular canals structures located in the ves- sex-related stimuli and, 336, 337 Sleep apnea impaired ability to breathe while
tibular organ, oriented in three planes and Sex-limited gene gene that exerts its effects pri- sleeping, 283, 284
lined with hair cells; sensitive to the direc- marily in one sex because of activation by Sleep paralysis, 284
tional tilt of the head, 203 androgens or estrogens, although members of Sleep spindle 12 to 14 Hz brain waves in bursts
Seminal vesicles, 328, 328 both sexes may have the gene, 11 that last at least half a second, 276, 277, 291
576 Subject Index/Glossary

Sleepwalking, 285 motor cortex and, 242 String theory, 2


Slow-twitch fibers muscle fibers that have less vig- motor neurons and, 62 Stroke a temporary loss of normal blood flow to a
orous contractions and no fatigue, 234 myelin and, 125 brain area, 141
Slow-wave sleep (SWS) stages 3 and 4 of sleep, nervous system and the, 86, 86, 88, 89, 89 amygdala damage and, 374
which are occupied by slow, large-amplitude periaqueductal gray area and, 210 axon sprouting and, 144, 145
brain waves, 277, 277 reflexes and, 52, 53 Broca’s area and, 438
Smell reflexive signals and, 235 cocaine and, 74
cranial nerves and, 92 sensory and motor nerves of the, 92 coma and, 276
metabotropic synapses and, 64 sensory nerves and, 148, 148 primary motor cortex and, 246, 247
olfactory bulb and, 103 touch and pain pathways of the, 208, 208 treatments for, 142, 143, 143
See also Olfaction viruses and, 34 Stroop effect the difficulty of saying the color of
Smile, 358 Spiroperidol, 480 ink of a word instead of reading the word
Smooth muscles those that control the digestive Splanchnic nerves nerves that convey information itself, 450
system and other organs, 232, 233 about the nutrient contents of the stomach to Subfornical organ (SFO) brain structure adjoining
SNRI. See Serotonin norepinephrine reuptake the brain, 312 the third ventricle of the brain, where its cells
inhibitors (SNRIs) Split-brain people people who have undergone monitor blood volume and relay information to
Social behavior, 337, 338, 339 surgery to the corpus callosum, 424–426, the preoptic area of the hypothalamus, 306
Sodium channels, 41, 45 425 Substance abuse
Sodium ions, 38, 39 Spontaneous firing rate a periodic production of alcohol and alcoholism as, 77, 78
Sodium–potassium pump mechanism that action potentials even without synaptic input, drugs and their effects, 76
actively transports sodium ions out of the cell 57 hallucinogenic drugs, 76
while drawing in two potassium ions, 38 SRY gene (sex-determining region on the Y chro- marijuana, 75
distribution of ions and, 41 mosome) gene that causes the primitive medications to combat, 80, 81
stroke and, 142 gonads to develop into testes, 328 nicotine, 74
Sodium-specific hunger an automatic strong crav- SSRI. See Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors opiates, 74, 75
ing for salty tastes, 307, 307 (SSRIs) schizophrenia and, 473, 478
Solutes, 305 St. John’s wort, 464 tolerance of, 78
Soma. See Cell body (soma) Startle reflex response that one makes after a sud- withdrawal of, 79
Somatic nervous system part of the PNS that den, unexpected loud noise or similar sudden Substance P a neurotransmitter released by pain
consists of the axons conveying messages stimulus, 371 axons in the spinal cord, 61, 208, 210
from the sense organs to the CNS and from Statoacoustic nerve, 92 Substance-induced psychotic disorder condition
the CNS to the muscles, 86, 86 Stem cells undifferentiated cells that divide and similar to schizophrenia, provoked by large,
Somatomedins, 67 produce daughter cells that develop more repeated doses of a drug, 480
Somatosensory cortex specialized properties, 124, 125, 257 Substantia nigra a midbrain structure that gives
attention and, 450 Stereoscopic depth perception, 178 rise to a pathway releasing dopamine, 93,
body location map and, 207 Stereotaxic instrument a device for the precise 254, 255
brain reorganization and, 136, 137 placement of electrodes in the brain, 109 Subthreshold stimulation, 40
insular cortex and, 206 Steroid hormones hormones that contain four car- Sugar, 310, 321
painful information and, 209 bon rings, 329, 329 Sulcus, 88
sensory representations and, 146-147, 146 Stimulant drugs drugs that increase excitement, Superior, 88
tongue stimulation and, 219, 219 alertness, and activity while elevating mood Superior colliculus swelling on either side of the
Somatosensory receptors, 203, 204, 205, 205 and decreasing fatigue, 73, 74 tectum; important to visual processing, 93,
Somatosensory system sensory network that brain damage recovery and, 144 93, 168, 170
monitors the surface of the body and its memory and, 396 Superior longitudinal fasciculus, 452
movements, 203 narcolepsy and, 285 Superior temporal cortex, 198
central nervous system input and, 206, 207 See also Substance abuse Supertasters people with heightened sensitivity to
pain and, 203, 209 Stomach all tastes and mouth sensations, 219, 219
receptors, 204, 205, 205 lateral hypothalamus and, 318 Supplementary motor cortex area of the frontal
Somatotropin (growth hormone), 66, 67, 68 satiety messages and the, 312 cortex; active during preparation of a rapid
Sound shadow, 200, 200 Stomach distension, 312 sequence of movements, 241, 242
Sound waves, 194, 195, 201 Strabismus (or strabismic amblyopia or lazy eye) a Suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) part of the hypo-
Spatial memory, 400, 401 condition in which the eyes do not point in thalamus; provides the main control of the
Spatial neglect a tendency to ignore the left side of the same direction, 178, 178 circadian rhythms for sleep and body temper-
the body or the left side of objects, 451 Stress the nonspecific response of the body to any ature, 271, 272
Spatial summation combination of effects of activ- demand made upon it; also defined as events light and resetting of, 272, 273
ity from two or more synapses onto a single that are interpreted as threatening, 380 Supraoptic nucleus part of the hypothalamus that
neuron, 53, 54 brain damage recovery behavior and, 149 controls the release rate of vasopressin, 306
Specificity property that highly active synapses concepts of, 380 Sweating, 300, 303, 305
become strengthened and less active synapses control of, 383 Sympathetic nervous system a network of nerves
do not, 412 cortisol and, 270 that prepare the organs for vigorous activity, 89
Speech depression and, 461, 461 autonomic nervous system and, 89, 90
left hemisphere and, 430 HPA axis and, 380, 381 bipolar cells and, 158
Spinal and bulbar muscular dystrophy, 259 immune system and, 382, 383 fight-or-flight system and, 366
Spinal cord part of the CNS; it communicates memory and, 395 parasympathetic nervous system and, 357
with all the sense organs and muscles except offspring behavior and, 131 Sympathetic pain, 209
those of the head, 88 pregnancy and effects of, 476 Synapse a specialized gap as a point of communi-
brain and, 93 prenatal environment and, 349 cation between two neurons, 51
central canal and, 98 pure autonomic failure and, 357, 358 addiction and, 78–80, 81
cerebral cortex and, 246 sleepwalking and, 285 anatomy of, 62
development of, 124, 124 stages of, 380 behavioral building blocks and, 69
digestive secretions and, 318 substance abuse and, 79 chemical events sequence of, 60, 60–69
disorders of the, 246 Stretch reflex a reflexive contraction of a muscle in chemical transmission discovery at, 59
glycine and, 63 response to a stretch of that muscle, 235, 235 drugs and, 69, 71–76, 76
itch sensation and, 213 Striate cortex, 102, 172 electrical, 69, 69
Subject Index/Glossary 577

evidence for delay of, 53, 53 globus pallidus and, 250 Tryptophan
inhibitory, 55, 56 information routes to the cerebral cortex, 96 amino acids and, 10
for pain and its inhibition, 210 intrinsic neuron and, 32 brain and, 61, 61, 62
properties of, 52–55, 56 melanopsin and, 273 depression and, 465
sleep and, 291 Parkinson’s disease and, 254 melatonin and, 311
spatial summation and, 53, 54 tongue and, 219, 219 serotonin and, 370
spinal cord and, 53 Thermophiles, 302 turkey and, 310
wiring diagrams for, 57, 57 Thiamine a B1 vitamin necessary to use glucose, 35 Tryptophan hydroxylase, 370
Synaptic cleft, 60, 73 Thioridazine, 480 TSH-releasing hormone, 66, 67, 68
Synaptic receptors, 30 Thiothixene, 480 Turnover release and resynthesis of a neurotrans-
Synaptogenesis formation of synapses, 125 Thirst mitter, 369
Syndromal obesity, 321 angiotensin II and, 307 Twins (monozygotic/dizygotic), 474, 475, 477
Synesthesia the experience some people have in hypovolemic, 307 2-AG (sn-2 arachidonylglycerol) chemical that
which stimulation of one sense evokes a per- osmotic, 305–306 binds to cannabinoid receptors, 75
ception of that sense and another one also, 225 sodium-specific hunger and, 307 Tympanic membrane the eardrum, 195
Syphilis, 336 types of, 305–306 Type I (Type A) alcoholism alcohol abuse with
water-regulating mechanisms and, 305 gradual onset; only a weak genetic predispo-
T cells, 381, 382 Threshold of excitation minimum amount of sition, 77
Tabes dorsalis, 246 membrane depolarization necessary to trigger Type II (Type B) alcoholism alcohol abuse with
Tardive dyskinesia a movement disorder charac- an action potential, 40, 53 rapid onset and a strong genetic basis, 77
terized by tremors and other involuntary Throat movement, 92
movements, 481, 482, 482 Thymosin, 67 UCR. See Unconditioned response (UCR)
Taste Thymus, 67 UCS. See Unconditioned stimulus (UCS)
bitter receptors and, 218 Thyroid, 66, 67, 68
buds, 216, 217 Thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH), 66, 67 Umami, 217, 218
chemicals and taste buds, 217 Thyroxine, 67 Unconditioned response (UCR) response auto-
coding and the brain, 219, 219 Tickle, 206 matically evoked by an unconditioned stimu-
cranial nerves and, 92 Time of arrival, 201 lus, 390, 391
differences in, 219–220 Tinnitus frequent or constant ringing in the ears, 200 Unconditioned stimulus (UCS) stimulus that
food and alterations of, 217 Tissue plasminogen activator (tPA) drug that automatically evokes an unconditioned
metabotropic synapses and, 64 breaks up blood clots, 142 response, 390, 391
organs of, 216 Tofranil (imipramine), 463 Unipolar disorder mood disorder with only one
phantoms, 219 Tolerance decrease of effects, 78, 79 extreme (or pole), generally depression, 469
receptors, 216–220 Tone-deafness, 197 Universe, 2, 3
receptors and salt intake, 307 Tongue, 219 Urbach-Wiethe disease, 374, 375
tongue and, 216, 216 Tongue muscles, 92 Urethra, 328
Tau protein part of the intracellular support struc- Tonotopic, 199 Uterus, 328, 335
ture of axons, 405, 406 TOPDV (Topography dorsoventral), 128 Utricle, 203, 204
Tectum roof of the midbrain, 93 Touch
nerve connection experiment and, 127, 128 contralateral input and, 219 Vagina, 328, 333, 345, 346
Tegmentum intermediate level of the midbrain, 93 paths and motor control, 246, 247 Vagus nerve tenth cranial nerve, which has branches
Telencephalon, 94 Touch receptors to and from stomach and several other organs,
Temperature regulation action potential and, 204 59, 92, 303, 312
body and, 300–303 cranial nerves and, 206, 206 Valium (diazepam), 376
homeostasis and allostasis, 299 vestibular organ and, 203 Valproate (Depakene, Depakote), 469
Temporal cortex Toxoplasma gondii, 372, 477 Vas deferens, 328, 328
cortex differentiation and, 132 Trace conditioning, 393 Vasopressin (antidiuretic hormone) hormone
emotions and, 361 Tract, 88 released by posterior pituitary; raises blood
encephalitis and, 390 Transcranial magnetic stimulation the application pressure and enables kidneys to conserve
evoked responses and, 112 of an intense magnetic field to a portion of water, 66
Heschl’s gyrus and, 114 the scalp, temporarily inactivating neurons hypothalamus and, 68
old age and, 139 below the magnet, 110, 110 list of hormone-releasing glands and, 67
Temporal lobe the lateral portion of each hemi- Transmitter-gated channel ion channel that opens social behavior and, 338, 339
sphere, near the temples, 102, 103, 104 temporarily when a neurotransmitter binds to water and, 305
Temporal summation a cumulative effect of it, 63 See also Antidiuretic hormone (ADH)
repeated stimuli within a brief time, 53, 54 Transporter special membrane protein where reup- Vegetative state condition in which someone has
Testes (testis) sperm-producing organs, 328,67 take occurs in the neurotransmitter, 60, 67 decreased brain activity and alternates
Testicular feminization condition in which indi- Trazodone, 480 between wakefulness and sleep but shows
viduals with an XY chromosome pattern Triceps muscles, 233 only limited responsiveness, such as increased
have the genital appearance of a female, 345 Trichromatic theory (Young-Helmholtz theory) heart rate in response to a painful stimulus,
Testosterone an androgen chemical, 329 theory that color is perceived through the rel- 276
aggressive behavior and, 367, 368, 368 ative rates of response by three kinds of Ventral toward the stomach, 87, 88, 89, 95
children’s toy play and, 333, 344 cones, each one maximally sensitive to a dif- Ventral stream visual paths in the temporal cortex
depression and, 462 ferent set of wavelengths, 160 that are specialized for identifying and recog-
emotions and, 369 Tricyclic antidepressant drug that blocks the reup- nizing objects; the “what” path, 183
gender patterns and, 330 take of catecholamines and serotonin by pre- Ventricles four fluid-filled cavities within the brain,
masculinized anatomy and, 344 synaptic terminals, 463 98, 98
prenatal development and, 330–331, 332 Trifluperazine, 480 Ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) region of
puberty and, 346 Trifluperidol, 480 the hypothalamus in which damage leads to
sexual arousal and levels of, 333, 334 Trigeminal nerve, 92 faster stomach emptying and increased secre-
trust and, 337 Triiodothyronine, 67 tion of insulin, 318
Thalamus a pair of structures in the center of the Triple imbalance hypothesis, 368 effects of damage to, 318, 319, 319
forebrain, 94 Trochlear nerve, 92 hypothalamic lesions and, 320
brain hemispheres and, 425 Trolley dilemma, 363 Ventromedial prefrontal cortex, 407
578 Subject Index/Glossary

Vesicles tiny nearly spherical packets filled with face recognition and the, 184–185, 186 Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, 404
neurotransmitter molecules, 62 visual deprivation and, 179 Wernicke’s aphasia (fluent aphasia) condition
drugs and, 71 visual sensitivity and, 159 characterized by poor language comprehen-
Vestibular nucleus cluster of neurons in the brain- See also Primary visual cortex sion and impaired ability to remember the
stem, primarily responsible for motor Visual field area of the world that an individual name of objects, 439, 440, 440
responses to vestibular sensation, 246 can see at any time, 155, 161, 423 Wernicke’s area portion of the brain located near
Vestibular sensation, 203, 204, 204 Vital reflexes, 92 the auditory cortex, associated with language
Viagra, 333 VNO. See Vomeronasal organ (VNO) comprehension, 438, 439
Vision Volley principle tenet that the auditory nerve as a White blood cells, 303
astigmatism and, 178, 179, 179 whole produces volleys of impulses for White matter area of the nervous system consist-
binocular, 177, 177 sounds even though no individual axon ing mostly of myelinated axons, 88, 89, 477,
cataracts and, 179 approaches that frequency, 197 477
color perception and, 186 Voltage-gated channel membrane channel whose Williams syndrome condition in which the person
color vision, 160–165 permeability to sodium (or some other ion) has relatively good language abilities in spite
contralateral input and, 219 depends on the voltage difference across the of their impairments in other regards, 435,
cortical cells and, 174, 178 membrane, 41, 62 436
cranial nerves and, 92 Vomeronasal organ (VNO) a set of receptors, Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, 478
deprived experience of, 177 located near, but separate from, the olfactory Withdrawal effects of drug cessation, 79
depth perception and, 178 receptors, 224 Wolffian ducts precursors to male internal struc-
eye–brain connections and, 155–159, 177 tures, 328, 328
face recognition and, 184–185, 186 Wada procedure, 361 Women
foveal and peripheral, 159 Wakefulness and sleep depression and, 461
hemispheres and, 423, 424 brain and stages of, 275–285 estradiol and, 334, 335
ionotropic synapses and, 64 rhythms of, 266–274 estrogens and, 329
law of specific nerve energies and, 154 Waking and sleeping rhythms hormones and sex of, 328, 328
major system connections of, 170 biological clock and, 268, 269–270 leptin and pregnant, 315
motion blindness and, 187, 188 endogenous cycles and, 266–267, 268 maternal behavior and, 338–339
multiple sclerosis and, 45 Water mating behavior and, 342
occipital lobe and, 102 evaporation of, 300 oxytocin and, 337
overview of mammalian system of, 168 molecule, 3 prenatal influences of, 348, 349
perception principles and, 154 osmotic thirst and, 305, 306, 307 REM sleep and, 278
primary sensory cortex and, 103 regulating mechanisms of, 305 schizophrenia and, 474
primary visual cortex and, 172–175, 176 Waterfall illusion, 176 schizophrenia and pregnancy of, 476, 477
receptive fields and, 171, 171, 172 Websites sexual activities and, 336, 337, 337
retina processing and, 168–170, 171 Bio Motion Lab, 184 sexual arousal and, 333, 337
shape perception and, 183–185, 186 color constancy, 163 sexual orientation and, 347
stimulation and, 178 Comparative Mammalian Brain Collections, testosterone and, 368
strabismus (lazy eye), 178 117 Woodpeckers, 141
superior colliculus and, 93 Dana Foundation, 7 Working memory, 395–397, 473
visual cortex development and, 176–178 Intersex Society of North America (ISNA), 344
visual and reasoning process of, 165 MetaNeuron program, 41 Xanax (alprazolam), 376
visual receptors and, 158–159 National Stroke Association, 141
See also Eyes Office of Animal Care and Use, 23 Yawning, 5, 238
Visual agnosia an inability to recognize objects Weight gain Young-Helmholtz theory. See Trichromatic theory
despite otherwise satisfactory vision, 183 diabetes and, 314
Visual cortex testosterone and, 334 Zeitgeber stimulus that resets the circadian
brain damage and, 148 See also Eating disorders rhythm, 269
cell loss and, 130 Wellbutrin, 464 Zoloft (sertraline), 464

You might also like