You are on page 1of 11

Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 31(1), 34–44


C 2016 The Division for Learning Disabilities of the Council for Exceptional Children

DOI: 10.1111/ldrp.12093

Evidence-Based Practices: Applications of Concrete Representational Abstract


Framework across Math Concepts for Students with Mathematics Disabilities
Jugnu Agrawal
George Mason University
Lisa L. Morin
Old Dominion University

Students with mathematics disabilities (MD) experience difficulties with both conceptual
and procedural knowledge of different math concepts across grade levels. Research shows
that concrete representational abstract framework of instruction helps to bridge this gap for
students with MD. In this article, we provide an overview of this strategy embedded within
the explicit instruction framework. We highlight effective practices for each component of the
framework across different mathematical strands. Implications for practice are also discussed
and a detailed case study is provided.

Mathematics disability (MD) is a relatively nascent field of knowledge (Bottge, 2001). Additionally, the achievement gap
research (Watson & Gable, 2013). Estimates for the preva- for math between typically developing students and students
lence of MD vary widely, from 3 to 9 percent of the entire with disabilities continues to increase because students with
school-age population (Fuchs et al., 2010; Swanson, 2012). disabilities progress at a much slower rate as compared to
Although researchers have agreed on many commonalities their typically developing peers (Bottge, 2001; Cawley &
of the characteristics of MD, they have also found that MD Miller, 1989).
is highly complex with many factors. These factors include Conceptual knowledge is developing a deeper under-
age, grade level, math content, and tasks, along with read- standing of the mathematical concepts by linking new
ing disability comorbidity. Some researchers have warned phenomenon to previously existing phenomenon and under-
that MD should not be considered a “homogenous disorder” standing the relationships and patterns among these different
(Chong & Siegel, 2008, p. 314) because students deemed to pieces of information (Miller & Hudson, 2007). For example,
have MD may have very different cognitive and skill deficits. the student understands that multiplication and division have
This particularly impacts students with MD in math word an inverse relationship. Therefore, he/she uses this knowl-
problem solving, a multifaceted task that requires simulta- edge to check the answer to a multiplication problem by
neously decoding information presented linguistically and dividing the product with one of the multipliers. Concep-
applying math concepts, creating representations, and carry- tual knowledge also develops when students connect a newly
ing out procedural mathematical operations (Zheng, Flynn, learned math concept to a previously learned and stored
& Swanson, 2013). While researchers differ in their use of the concept. For example, the student understands place value
terms MD and learning disability (LD), for the purpose of this of whole numbers but when he/she learns decimals, he/she
article, mathematics skills deficits will be referred to as MD. connects the new math concept with the previously learned
The recommendations of the National Council of Teachers math concept of place value (Hattikudur, 2011; Kridler, 2012;
of Mathematics (NCTM, 2000) specify that students should Miller & Hudson, 2007; Mulcahy & Krezmien, 2009).
have an opportunity to develop understanding of mathemati- Miller and Hudson (2007) define “procedural knowledge
cal concepts and procedures by engaging in meaningful math as the ability to solve a mathematical task” (p. 50). It is
instruction. Common Core’s Standards for Mathematical also defined as the ability to follow step-by-step proce-
Practice (National Governors Association Center for Best dures to solve a math problem (Bottge, 2001; Carnine, 1997;
Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers, 2010) Goldman, Hasselbring, & The Cognition and Technology
highlight process standards that give priority to math rea- Group at Vanderbilt, 1997). Procedural knowledge can be
soning and making connections. The findings of a review of used for solving problems ranging from simple addition and
math interventions for low-achieving students indicate that subtraction to complex word problems. The development of
instruction for students with disabilities focuses on teaching procedural knowledge has been researched extensively for
computational skills and procedures rather than conceptual students with MD (Hattikudur, 2011; Kridler, 2012; Miller
& Hudson, 2007; Montague, 1992; Mulcahy & Krezmien,
2009).
Requests for reprints should be sent to Lisa L. Morin, Old Dominion Both conceptual and procedural knowledge are essen-
University. Electronic inquiries should be sent to lmorin@odu.edu tial for improving math achievement of students with MD
AGRAWAL AND MORIN: APPLICATIONS OF CRA 35

(Schneider, Rittle-Johnson, & Star, 2011). An ongoing de- (2010) also suggest the use of manipulatives like counters,
bate persists regarding which of these two types of knowledge algebra tiles, and geoboards to support students’ conceptual
develops first and which one is more important. Researchers understanding during the concrete phase.
have come to realize that it is neither procedural nor con- The second component of the CRA framework involves
ceptual knowledge alone; it is an integrated understanding of the use of visual representations, bridging the gap between
both conceptual and procedural knowledge that leads to math the use of manipulatives, and the unsupported use of ab-
proficiency (Rittle-Johnson, Siegler, & Alibali, 2001). The stract mathematical notations. These can take the form of
concrete representational abstract (CRA) framework helps pictures of the manipulatives, such as pictorial representa-
to develop a clear link between conceptual and procedural tions of Base-10 blocks (Mancl et al, 2012; Miller & Kaffar,
knowledge. 2011) or virtual manipulatives (Bouck & Flanagan, 2010;
Vukovic and Siegel (2010) highlighted the importance of Satsangi & Bouck, 2014; Suh & Moyer, 2008). Flores (2009,
teaching math in a manner that fosters mathematical thinking, 2010) used tally marks in the representational phase of her in-
further pointing out that a mathematical education that fo- tervention, while Bryant, Bryant, Gersten, Scammacca, and
cuses on skills and fluency instead of conceptual understand- Chavez (2008) used 100s charts and number lines. Other vi-
ing does not facilitate mathematic literacy. In their literature sual representations can take the form of a graphic organizer.
review, Zheng et al. (2013) found that successful research had Graphic organizers, in themselves a research-supported inter-
incorporated the use of explicit and strategic instruction (SI) vention, depict the relationship between facts and concepts,
to effectively support students with MD. Meyer, Salimpoor, helping a student to organize, and subsequently solve, math
Wu, Geary, and Menon (2010) recommended that problem problems (Strickland & Maccini, 2010, 2013). Research sup-
solving skills in students with MD were best supported by vi- ports the use of representational over pictorial graphics (van
sual representations. The growing body of empirical research Garderen & Montague, 2003).
supporting CRA validates its effectiveness as an evidence- Finally, the main objective of the CRA framework is for
based practice to teach numerous math concepts to students the students to demonstrate the ability to comprehend and
with MD through explicit and SI that incorporates visual access numbers and symbols without concrete or represen-
representations. tational scaffolds. Strickland and Maccini (2013) asserted
that generalizing the previously mastered conceptual under-
standing from the concrete and representational phases and
CRA applying that to the abstract phase is the most challenging
aspect for students with MD. Since concrete and representa-
The CRA framework is a process in which the teacher tional components of the CRA framework focus on concep-
guides the student through a mathematical concept and its tual knowledge, it contributes to fluency and automaticity of
corresponding computational process through the use of ma- procedural knowledge in the abstract phase. Often students
nipulatives and visual representations that illustrate the con- are supported in the transition between the representational
cept along with numbers. The concrete and representational and abstract phases through the application of a cognitive
phases are presented alongside the abstract notations. These strategy (e.g., mnemonic or cue) that prompts students to re-
serve as scaffolds with the ultimate goal of developing the member the steps in the given mathematical process (Mancl
student’s ability to comprehend and compute at the abstract et al., 2012). In addition, the abstract phase also focuses
level, which entails training students to decode and use tradi- on fluency in completion of the task (Flores, 2010). Miller,
tional mathematical notations. Recent research highlighting Stringfellow, Kaffar, Ferreira, and Mancl (2011) suggested
the CRA framework emphasizes the need to teach each phase that transition to the abstract phase is facilitated when stu-
to mastery (Flores, Hinton, Strozier, & Terry, 2014; Mancl, dents are exposed to mathematic notation alongside presen-
Miller, & Kennedy, 2012; Strickland & Maccini, 2013). tations of math problems in concrete and representational
The first component of CRA instruction is the use of formats.
hands-on activities with manipulatives. The use of well--
planned instruction using physical manipulatives allows
students to become active participants in knowledge EXPLICIT INSTRUCTION
construction (Stein & Bovalino, 2001). When students use
manipulatives to explore concepts, they are more engaged Effective delivery of CRA dictates the use of explicit instruc-
and motivated. They can participate in mathematical tion. The National Mathematics Advisory Panel (NMAP)
discourse, share their thinking, and reflect on their learning. asserted that explicit systematic instruction has been found
This leads to increased achievement and deeper understand- to specifically support students with learning disabilities in
ing of the concepts studied (NCTM, 2008). Furthermore, computation, math word problem solving, and generalizabil-
manipulatives are effective across grade levels and skill ity of math problems (NMAP, 2008), facilitating active en-
levels (Gersten, Jordan, & Flojo, 2005). For example, many gagement with math concepts (Strickland & Maccini, 2013).
researchers (Flores, 2009, 2010; Mancl et al., 2012; Miller & Explicit systematic instruction requires application of an ad-
Kaffar, 2011) made use of Base-10 blocks as manipulatives vance organizer, teacher modeling, guided practice, problem
to teach place value and regrouping concepts. Base-10 solving practice, independent practice, and feedback within
blocks, which represent one-to-one correspondence and can each component of the CRA framework as it is taught (refer
be used across many mathematical problem types, exemplify to Table 1). Each of the three components of the frame-
math tasks in a most literal way. Strickland and Maccini work must be taught explicitly and should include clear
36 LEARNING DISABILITIES RESEARCH

TABLE 1
Components of the Explicit Instruction Framework

Components Description
Advance organizer The teacher links the new lesson with the previously taught lesson by reviewing the objective of the previous
day. The teacher also identifies the objective for that day’s lesson and gives a rationale for learning the skill.
Teacher demonstration The teacher demonstrates the targeted skill while describing aloud the steps.
Guided practice The teacher gives prompts and cues to solve the few problems together using questions and answers.
Independent practice Student independently solves a few problems related to the targeted skill.
Problem-solving practice There are at least two word problems. The student and the teacher solve the first problem together and the
student solves the second one independently.
Feedback The student’s understanding is checked by monitoring his/her work. At this stage the instructional decision is
made to either continue the lesson or go back to step one of the lesson. This is contingent upon the
student’s performance on independent problems.

connections in order to transition between the stages of the fractions (Butler, Miller, Crehan, Babbit, & Pierce, 2003;
framework (Witzel, Riccomini, & Schneider, 2008). Strick- Jordan, Miller, & Mercer, 1999; Witzel & Riccomini, 2009),
land and Maccini (2010) emphasized that explicit instruction algebra (Strickland & Maccini, 2013; Witzel, 2005; Witzel
is an evidence-based strategy backed by decades of research & Riccomini, 2009), and other math concepts to students
for teaching math concepts to students with LD. with MD (U.S. Department of Education, Office of Special
Education Programs, 2000).

SI
Place Value
CRA and SI have been used simultaneously to support math
instruction (Hinton, Strozier, & Flores, 2014). Overlapping The concept of place value is the foundation upon which
with the explicit instruction model in components such as the command and mastery of other mathematical opera-
teacher modeling and feedback, SI differs in that it pro- tions lie. Through drill and practice, students may memo-
vides step-by-step instructions, often through the use of a rize the titles of place value columns—ones, tens, hundreds,
mnemonic or similar cues (e.g., cue cards), in decision mak- thousands—but use of manipulatives and representational
ing and solving math problems. As previously noted, this strategies equipped students in various studies to better grasp
strategic, procedural support is frequently provided when the number relationships involved in place value tasks and
students are transitioning to the abstract phase of the CRA better employ computational strategies in a variety of math
framework. Flores (2009) incorporated a mnemonic strategy tasks and operations (Bryant et al., 2008; Doabler, & Fien,
to support students in their transition to the abstract phase; 2013; Hiebert & Wearne, 1992). Individual counters, linking
this mnemonic, DRAW, supported students procedurally in clips, and unifix cubes can be used and grouped in sets of
the abstract phase to (1) discover the sign, (2) read the prob- tens and hundreds to demonstrate place value concepts in its
lem, (3) answer and check, and (4) write the answer. Miller most concrete form. The possibilities are endless. In addition,
and Kaffar (2011), followed by Mancl et al. (2012), incorpo- while a Base-10 block representing the “tens place” (i.e., a
rated a mnemonic, RENAME, into lessons involving addition rod) is a single component, the ten divisions that represents
and subtraction with regrouping at the abstract phase, aiding the “ones place” (i.e., units) are typically clearly demarcated
students to remember the steps, (1) read the problem, (2) on the rod. The same is true of a flat that is representative of
examine the ones column, (3) note ones in the ones column, the “hundreds place”: Students can clearly see the ones and
(4) address the tens column, (5) mark tens in the tens column, tens of which the block is constructed. Students may start out
(6) examine and note hundreds, and check. The former re- counting individual units on the tens and hundreds blocks,
searchers also provided students with cue cards and a poster eventually progressing to more efficiently subitizing, count-
of strategy steps (Miller & Kaffar, 2011). ing by tens and hundreds. As students gain mastery of place
value concepts, they will become less dependent on concrete
materials. Teachers can provide number charts and demon-
PROBLEM TYPES strate the use of tally marks and place-value cards as repre-
sentational forms that bridge the gap between the concrete
The CRA has been used effectively in research to support stu- and purely abstract forms of place value. Number charts, ar-
dents with MD across ages, grade levels, and math domains ranged in rows of ten, provide a visual representation of place
(Strickland & Maccini, 2010). It has been used successfully value concepts. Tally marks provide a strategy for students to
to teach place value (Bryant et al., 2008; Peterson, Mercer, construct their own visual representations that will be useful
& O’Shea, 1988), addition and subtraction with regrouping in performing a variety of math tasks (Flores, 2009; Fuson
(Flores, 2010; Flores, Hinton, & Strozier, 2014; Mancl et al., et al., 1997). Place-value cards (Cotter, 2000) demonstrate
2012), multiplication (Flores, Hinton, & Schweck, 2014; combining numbers of various place values (e.g., three cards
Flores, Hinton, & Strozier, 2014; Morin & Miller, 1998), that represent 300, 60, and 7, can be combined to create the
AGRAWAL AND MORIN: APPLICATIONS OF CRA 37

number 367). Ultimately, students must use the structure of the abstract phase. Base-10 blocks, paper plates with blocks
written numbers to determine information involving place or cubes, area models, counters, beads, and other such ma-
value that will lead to accurate computation. nipulatives can be used in the concrete phase to teach mul-
tiplication or division to students with MD. Circular or rect-
angular representations with tally marks or circles/squares
Addition and Subtraction can be used during the representational phase of instruction.
Division is opposite of multiplication, so manipulatives and
To introduce the concept of addition and subtraction, a representations similar to multiplication can be used to teach
teacher may want to start out with a group of simple coun- the inverse concept.
ters that can be used to “add to” or “put together,” or “take
from” or “compare the difference.” Several recent studies
have demonstrated success for higher-level multidigit addi- Fractions
tion (Miller & Kaffar, 2011) and subtraction involving re-
grouping (Flores, 2009, 2010; Flores, Hinton, & Strozier, Sammons (2010) identified fractions as the “hot spot” for
2014; Mancl et al., 2012) by combining the use of Base-10 students in upper elementary and middle school. “Hot spots”
blocks with explicit instruction. Researchers reported strong are curricular concepts with which students struggle con-
effect sizes across all grade levels for elementary school stu- stantly (Sammons, 2010, p. 142). Students have difficulty
dents with LD. with fractions because they fail to connect form and under-
When Base-10 blocks are placed in columns representing standing. Form is the syntax (e.g., symbols, numerals, and
ones, tens, and hundreds, then students can more easily un- algorithms) while understanding is the ability to relate math-
derstand that two numbers added together that make more ematical ideas to real-world situations (Hiebert, 1985). The
than nine units must be physically moved to the next column. new rules related to fractions conflict with the ideas about
Conversely, if a larger number cannot be subtracted from a whole numbers. Students seem to lack conceptual and pro-
smaller number, students can physically move the appropriate cedural knowledge of fraction concepts. Misquitta (2011)
block from the neighboring column. Students then transition noted that direct and explicit instructional strategies are ef-
to pictorial representations of Base-10 blocks. While picto- fective for developing conceptual and procedural knowledge
rial representations of Base-10 blocks cannot be physically of fractions for students who are at-risk for MD. Jordan et al.
manipulated, students can be taught to draw and adjust them (1999) noted that fourth grade students instructed with the
as they solve the addition and subtraction problems. Stu- concrete to semiconcrete to abstract strategy outperformed
dents can also be taught to use bar models, or strip diagrams, the students instructed with the textbook curriculum on iden-
representations that exemplify quantities and their relation- tification, comparison, equivalence, subtraction, and addition
ships, to support addition and subtraction (Englard, 2010; of fractions.
Ginsburg, Leinwand, Anstrom, & Pollock, 2005; for an ex- According to the recommendations of NMAP (2008), con-
ample, see Figure 1). When students have met the mastery ceptual and procedural knowledge of fractions is directly
criteria of the concept, and the teacher has demonstrated the linked with mastery of fractions. Instruction focused on con-
steps in addition or subtraction using terms such as “trad- ceptual knowledge is likely to improve the problem-solving
ing” and “regrouping,” students fully understand the process skills of students. The Rational Number Project (RNP) con-
conceptually. ducted research on students’ mastery of fraction concepts
(Cramer, Behr, Post, & Lesh, 2009). RNP’s belief is that stu-
dents should learn fractions using manipulatives to develop
Multiplication and Division concepts. They also emphasize order and equivalence as the
foundation for developing procedural and conceptual knowl-
If students learn multiplication facts through rote instruc- edge of fraction concepts.
tion only, little to no emphasis is placed on developing the Equivalence of fractions is the key for teaching addition
conceptual knowledge of multiplication. In that case, if they and subtraction of fractions with unlike denominators (But-
forget certain math facts, they have no way of calculating ler et al., 2003). Chips, double-sided counters, and shapes
or finding the correct answer. On the contrary, students who (e.g., circles, rectangles, and squares) can be used to teach
understand the concept are more likely to use other strategies equivalent fractions in the concrete phase. Butler et al. (2003)
like repeated addition to calculate the correct answer. Sev- used fraction circles, beans, and folded construction paper to
eral researchers have studied the impact of using CRA as an teach equivalent fractions for the concrete component. It is
intervention strategy to teach conceptual knowledge of mul- important to explicitly teach that fractions can be part of the
tiplication. Morin and Miller (1998) used paper plates with same whole (as in shape) or part of a group (as in counters).
wooden blocks during the concrete phase and boxes/circles Students can use whole circles and colored parts to repre-
containing dots/tallies in the representational phase to teach sent the unlike fractions. Then they change the parts of the
multiplication (see Figure 2) and related word problems. Flo- circle to make equivalent fractions. Once students change
res, Hinton, and Schweck (2014) and Flores, Hinton, and the fractions into equivalent fractions and show that with
Strozier (2014) used place value mats with Base-10 blocks manipulatives, they can add or subtract the numerators and
for the concrete phase, long and short tallies and squares for show the answer with a circle and the corresponding colored
the representational phase, and a mnemonic tool to remem- parts or with chips (see Figure 3). In the concrete phase,
ber the steps for multiplication problems with regrouping for the metacognitive steps of solving the problem (i.e., teacher
38 LEARNING DISABILITIES RESEARCH

Concrete Level Instruction for Subtraction Representational Level Instruction for


with Regrouping Subtraction with Regrouping
Step 1: For solving 274 — 148, use Base-10 Step 1: For solving 274 — 148 at the
Blocks to show the minuend and subtrahend. representational level, students can be trained to
draw figures that imitate Base-10 blocks.

Step 2: If there is not enough in a place value column to subtract from, a block (or tally mark) is
borrowed from the next column.

Step 3: The borrowed block (or tally mark) is exchanged for the respective number of blocks
representing the place value in which it has been placed. Using one-to-one correspondence, the
now matching units of Base-10 Blocks are compared from the minuend and the subtrahend, with
the remaining number of unmatched blocks forming the difference, or solution.

FIGURE 1 Examples of CRA applications for subtraction with regrouping.

labeling) and using manipulatives to help students concep- introduced for teaching the same concept. The students draw
tually understand the problem helps to provide meaning to shapes to show fractions or use schematic diagrams to make
the abstract concepts. Once the students have successfully equivalent fractions and then add or subtract the numerators.
mastered the fraction concept with manipulatives, visual rep- Finally, for the abstract phase, the students use fractions with-
resentations through pictures and schematic diagrams can be out manipulatives or representations to solve the problems.
AGRAWAL AND MORIN: APPLICATIONS OF CRA 39

Concrete Level Instruction for Representational Level Instruction for


Multiplication Multiplication
Step 1: For solving 14 × 4, use 4 paper plates Step 1: For solving 8 × 13, draw 8 groups
to show the groups

______ ______ ______ ______

______ ______ ______ ______

Step 2: Place 14 objects in each group Step 2: Draw 13 objects in each group

||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| |||||||||||||

||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| |||||||||||||

Step 3: Count the total number of objects in Step 3: Count the total number of objects in the 8
the 4 groups groups

||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| |||||||||||||

||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| ||||||||||||| |||||||||||||

=
14+14+14+14= 56 13 +13+13+13+13+13+13+13= 104

FIGURE 2 Examples of CRA applications for multiplication.

Algebra them concretely with objects such as string, and/or different-


sized sticks combined with mathematical symbols. Next, the
Algebra requires higher level mathematical problem solving students drew pictorial representations to show the steps of
skills. Due to the abstract nature of the algebraic equations, the problem, and eventually solved the problem abstractly
these present a problem for students with MD. Algebraic using symbols and numbers.
expressions can range from solving one variable problem It is important to represent the coefficients and the
to problems with multiple variables on both sides of the variables using different objects. Scheuermann, Deschler,
equation. In a review of interventions for algebra instruction, and Schumaker (2009) used three-dimensional objects like
Watt, Watkins, and Abbitt (2014) found CRA sequence of buttons and different colored unifix cubes to represent
instruction to be an effective strategy for teaching algebra the equations concretely, and two-dimensional drawings
to students with LD. In order to solve algebraic problems such as tallies, dots, and pictures for the representational
with multiple variables on both sides of the equal sign, the phase. Algebra tiles and their drawings can also be used
students have to be able to reduce expressions, solve single- for the concrete and representational phases (Maccini &
variable equations, transform equations on one side of the Hughes, 2000). Strickland and Maccini (2010) recom-
equal sign, and across the equal sign (Witzel, Mercer, & mended using diagrams and charts for the representational
Miller, 2003). Witzel et al. (2003) taught the students to phase to demonstrate the relationships within an algebraic
first manipulate the single variable problems by representing equation.
40 LEARNING DISABILITIES RESEARCH

Concrete Level Instruction for Fractions Word Problem Solving


Step 1: 1/9 + 2/3 Show the fractions using double Step 1: 1/2 + 2/6
colored chips. Show the fractions using circles. Word problem solving is embedded within the explicit in-
struction framework for CRA. The majority of the studies
of CRA and SIM incorporated word problem solving as part
+ = + = of the instructional framework. In the studies conducted for
teaching addition and subtraction (Miller & Kaffar, 2011),
Step 2: Do we have equal parts or denominators?
Step 2: Do we have equal parts or denominators?
We don’t have equalparts/denominators.
We don’t have equal parts/denominators. multiplication (Morin & Miller, 1998), fractions (Butler et al.,
How can we make equal parts/denominator?
How can we make equal parts/denominator?
We can make equal parts/denominator by 2003), and integers (Maccini & Ruhl, 2000) using the CRA
We can make equal parts/denominator by
changing 3 parts into 9 parts.
changing 2 parts into 6 parts. framework, students demonstrated substantial gains in their
word problem solving skills in addition to solving regular
problems.
+ =
Step 3: Change the numerator to match the
+
Step 3: Change the numerator to match the
= In essence, word problem solving should not be treated as
a separate entity but rather should be incorporated through-
denominator. denominator.
out the instruction. CRA aids students in understanding the
2/3 6/9 1/2 3/6 relevance of math concepts and procedures (Witzel et al.,
2008). However, as the authors pointed out, students with LD
generally experience difficulty applying concepts and skills

Step 4: Do we have equal parts?


+
Step 4: Do we have equal parts?
= across settings. They maintained that math word problems
included in all phases of the CRA framework can increase
1/9 and 6/9 3/6 and 2/6 the likelihood that concepts and skills will be generalized
by incorporating relevance and language experiences into in-
struction at each stage. For students struggling with language
involved in word problems, the CRA framework can also pro-
+ = + = vide support in making the words behind the math problem
Step 5: We add the numerators. The Step 5: We add the numerators. The
more meaningful through the use of concrete and represen-
parts/denominator stays the same.
1/9 + 6/9 = 7/9
parts/denominator stays the same.
3/6 + 2/6 = 5/6
tational components. For this reason, Witzel et al. (2008)
admonished practitioners against “allowing word problems
to be the dreaded completion of a math lesson” (p. 275).

+ = + = CRA IN ACTION

William is a friendly third grade student with an easy smile.


His reading is slightly above grade level, and he is particu-
larly interested in science and social studies activities. But
Representational Level Instruction for Fractions
when it is time for math to begin, his smile slowly disap-
pears. He suddenly feels the need to carve his initials into
Step 1: 1/2 + 2/8
Show the fractions using circles. the wood of his pencil with a pair of scissors he finds in
+ = his desk. William was diagnosed with a math learning dis-
ability in second grade. William is able to follow procedural
Step 2: Do we have equal parts or denominators? steps and solve math problems, but he is unable to explain
We don’t have equalparts/denominators.
How can we make equal parts/denominator? the reasoning behind the problem solving. A special edu-
We can make equal parts/denominator by
changing 2 parts into 8 parts.
+ = cation teacher or her paraprofessional assistant comes into
the classroom during the inclusive math class. William of-
ten works at a kidney-shaped table in a small-group setting
Step 3: Change the numerator to match the during math. The special education and general education
denominator.
teachers typically alternate in the responsibility of teaching
1/2 4/8
+ = the small group.

Step 4: Do we have equal parts?


4/8 and 2/8 Teaching Place Value
+ = Concrete Conceptualization
Step 5: We add the numerators. The
parts/denominator stays the same. William’s teachers have been focusing on incorporating the
4/8 + 2/8 = 6/8
CRA within the explicit instruction framework in order to
most effectively teach math concepts. Initially, as the teacher
introduces the topic of place value, she provides an overview
FIGURE 3 Addition of fractions. of the concept that will be taught in the form of an advance
AGRAWAL AND MORIN: APPLICATIONS OF CRA 41

organizer. Next, the teacher models the instruction. Initially, seems ready to work independently, accurately representing
she demonstrates one-to-one correspondence using individ- the number 251 on hundreds charts.
ual Base-10 blocks that represent “ones.” She actually counts
out 114 individual Base-10 blocks to represent the abstract
number 114. She notes that there are 114 individual blocks. Transition to Abstract Phase
After pointing out how time consuming it is to count out
114 blocks, she thinks aloud about the possibility of using Immediately following William’s demonstration of mastery
the Base-10 blocks that represent tens instead of the individ- in determining place value with the support of concrete and
ual ones blocks. She models counting up by tens to 110 and representational formats, the teacher introduces the concept
then adds four ones blocks to the group for a total of 114. in just the abstract form—with numbers and symbols only.
She points out how much easier this is rather than counting After providing an overview of the lesson, William’s teacher
individual blocks. Finally, the teacher presents the idea of models comparing two numbers, 157 and 136, thinking out
using the Base-10 block that represents 100. She points out loud about what each digit means and explaining the mental
the ones and tens units that are demarcated on the hundreds representations she has of each of these numbers, referencing
block. Now she lines up her number to accurately represent the use of Base-10 blocks in the previous lessons. She then
114: One hundred, one ten, and four ones. She notes that the models taking the greater than/less than sign, already paired
different Base-10 blocks represent different place values. She and practiced with the place value concept during the con-
reviews the entire concept by demonstrating another number crete and representational phases, and placing it appropriately
in its abstract form, 156. She discusses why 156 is greater between the two numbers so that the wider end points toward
than 114, and introduces the greater than/less than symbols. 157. She and William work on comparing several numbers
William is then guided through representing a three-digit together, and finally, William compares two numbers on his
number using individual Base-10 blocks, then blocks that own with immediate feedback. Conceptually, he now under-
represent tens and ones, and finally blocks representing hun- stands what a three-digit number means and why one number
dreds, tens, and ones. He is then guided through determining is larger than another number, and how to determine which
which of two numbers is larger and how to place the greater one is larger.
than/less than (>, <) sign between the two numbers. Once
he gains mastery of the concept, the teacher provides in-
dependent practice, providing specific feedback only upon Teaching Subtraction with Regrouping
completion of the practice.
After William demonstrates mastery of place value, his teach-
ers introduce addition and subtraction of numbers using the
Representational Conceptualization same Base-10 blocks and the same steps. They first intro-
duce addition and subtraction without having to regroup from
William’s teacher introduces representational materials that the next place value, and then, finally, they introduce addi-
demonstrate place value alongside the abstract numbers and tion and subtraction involving regrouping. After providing
symbols immediately after he shows mastery of place value an overview of a lesson on subtraction with regrouping, the
through the pairing of concrete and abstract formats in order teacher demonstrates the concept by laying out the appro-
to better help William understand the connection between priate Base-10 blocks to represent each number. She places
the concrete and representational phases of CRA. William’s Base-10 blocks to represent the minuend, 274, over top of
teachers choose to introduce the representational aspect of Base-10 blocks that represent the subtrahend, 148. She also
place value by first showing him pictures of Base-10 blocks. writes out the same subtraction problem in its abstract form,
After providing an overview and expectations for the lesson, carefully demonstrating how to line up the integers from the
the teacher introduces a picture of various outlines of Base- minuend over the subtrahend. She thinks aloud about the
10 blocks that represent hundreds, tens, and ones blocks. problem of not being able to subtract 8 from 4 in the ones
The teacher models coloring in the appropriate blocks to column. She then slides one of the seven 10s blocks in the
represent numbers. She models and thinks out loud as she minuend to the ones column; now in the one’s column she has
compares colored pictures of Base-10 blocks and places the a ten block and four ones. She then exchanges the tens block
greater than/less than sign between the numbers. The teacher for ten individual ones blocks. Now she can subtract each col-
and William color in the appropriate representations for two umn by using one-to-one correspondence, showing that the
other three-digit numbers together. They discuss which ab- answer, or difference, is the remaining blocks from the minu-
stract symbol (greater than or less than) should be placed be- end left after the blocks from the minuend and subtrahend are
tween the two numbers. Finally, William colors in the Base-10 matched up. She writes down the difference in the abstract
representations independently and determines which num- form of the problem. Once all the steps that comprise explicit
ber is greater with immediate feedback from his teacher. The instruction are followed for the concrete form of subtraction
teacher also shows William how similar a hundreds chart is to with regrouping, the teacher immediately transitions to com-
pictures of Base-10 blocks. They discuss how they could ap- bining the abstract concepts with the representational form.
ply their coloring technique to hundreds charts. The teacher Using the explicit instruction framework, William is taught to
demonstrates, coloring in a full hundreds chart, three com- use pictorial representations of Base-10 blocks. He is taught
plete lines, and four individual squares to represent the num- to cross out a Base-10 block in one column and to draw a de-
ber 134. She and William color some together, until William piction of the block in the next column. Ultimately, William
42 LEARNING DISABILITIES RESEARCH

TABLE 2
Websites for Math Resources

Description Website
Math vocabulary cards with r http://www.ncesd.org/Page/983
visuals
What works clearinghouse r http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/Publications_Reviews.aspx?f=All%20Publication%20and%20Product%20Types,3;
practice guides for math #pubsearch
r http://www.do2learn.com/sitemap/index.htm
Math tools r http://www.mathlanding.org/collections/pd_collection/strategies-teaching-studentsstruggling-mathematics
r https://www.teachervision.com/graphicorganizers/printable/6293.html
r http://nlvm.usu.edu/
Virtual manipulatives r http://www.glencoe.com/sites/common_assets/mathematics/ebook_assets/vmf/VMFInterface.html
Word problems with bar r http://www.mathplayground.com/thinkingblocks.html
models
r http://www.coolmath4kids.com/
Math games r http://www.funbrain.com/
r http://pbskids.org/games/math.html
Center on instruction r http://www.centeroninstruction.org/mathematics-instruction-for-students-with-learningdisabilities-or-difficulty-
learning-mathematicsa-guide-for-teachers

transitions to using just the abstract numbers and symbols Bryant, D. P., Bryant, B. R., Gersten, R., Scammacca, N., & Chavez, M.
that represent a subtraction problem that requires regrouping M. (2008). Mathematics intervention for first-and second-grade stu-
dents with mathematics difficulties: The effects of Tier 2 intervention
as soon as he demonstrates mastery of the concept in the
delivered as booster lessons. Remedial and Special Education, 29(1),
concrete and representational forms. He can now conceptu- 20–32. doi: 10.1177/0741932507309712
alize the process and is able to accurately complete the task. Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Crehan, K., Babbit, B., & Pierce, T. (2003).
The teachers discuss the next step of helping William to un- Fraction instruction for students with mathematics disabilities: Com-
derstand how to regroup across two columns using the same paring two teaching frameworks. Learning Disabilities Research and
Practice, 18, 99–111. doi:10.1111/1540-5826.00066
process of introducing the concept through concrete and then Carnine, D. (1997). Instructional design in mathematics for students with
representational means, combined with the abstract, before learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 130–141.
introducing the abstract by itself. doi:10.1177/002221949703000201
Cawley, J. F., & Miller, J. H. (1989). Cross-sectional compar-
isons of the mathematical performance of children with learn-
CONCLUSION ing disabilities: Are we on the right track toward comprehen-
sive programming? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 250–259.
doi:10.1177/002221948902200409
In conclusion, the CRA framework has shown promise for Chong, S. L., & Siegel, L. S. (2008). Stability of computa-
instructing students in different mathematical concepts. Stu- tional deficits in math learning disability from second through
dents with MD typically struggle with conceptual knowledge fifth grades. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 300–317.
and have difficulty connecting it with procedural knowledge doi:10.1080/87565640801982387
Cotter, J. A. (2000). Using language and visualization to teach place
(Watson & Gable, 2013). Mastery of conceptual and pro- value. Teaching Children Mathematics, 7(2), 108–114. Retrieved from
cedural knowledge is integral to math achievement of stu- http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ668812
dents with MD. CRA framework is an empirically supported Cramer, K., Behr, M., Post T., & Lesh, R. (2009). Rational Number Project:
instructional method that helps to connect conceptual and Initial fraction ideas. Originally published in 1997 as Rational Number
Project: Fraction lessons for the middle grades - Level 1. Dubuque, IA:
procedural knowledge in teaching various math concepts to Kendall/Hunt Publishing Co.
students with MD. To this end, a list of website resources for Doabler, C. T., & Fien, H. (2013). Explicit mathematics instruction
supporting CRA is provided in Table 2. The CRA instruc- what teachers can do for teaching students with mathematics dif-
tion should be bundled or layered with explicit instruction ficulties. Intervention in School and Clinic, 48(5), 276–285. doi:
framework. Progress monitoring at each phase dictates the 10.1177/1053451212473151
Englard, L. (2010). Raise the bar. Teaching Children Mathematics, 17(3),
transition to the next phase of the CRA framework. 156–163. Retrieved from http://commons.wvc.edu/agardner/math171/
downloads/Problem%20solving%20Singapore%20method.pdf
Flores, M. M. (2009). Teaching subtraction with regrouping to students
REFERENCES experiencing difficulty in mathematics. Preventing School Failure, 53,
145–152. doi: 10.3200/PSFL.53.3.145-152
Bottge, B. A. (2001). Reconceptualizing mathematics problem solving for Flores, M. M. (2010). Using the concrete-representational-abstract
low-achieving students. Remedial and Special Education, 22, 102–112. framework to teach subtraction with regrouping to students at
doi:10.1177/074193250102200204 risk for failure. Remedial and Special Education, 31, 195–207.
Bouck, E. C., & Flanagan, S. M. (2010). Virtual manipulatives: What they doi:10.1177/0741932508327467
are and how teachers can use them. Intervention in School and Clinic, Flores, M. M., Hinton, V. M., & Schweck, K. B. (2014). Teaching multi-
45(3), 186–191. doi: 0.1177/1053451209349530 plication with regrouping to students with learning disabilities.
AGRAWAL AND MORIN: APPLICATIONS OF CRA 43

Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 29(4), 171–183. Miller, S. P., & Hudson, P. J. (2007). Using evidence-based practices to
doi:10.1111/ldrp.12043 build mathematics competence related to conceptual, procedural, and
Flores, M. M., Hinton, V., & Strozier, S. D. (2014). Teaching subtraction declarative knowledge. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice,
and multiplication with regrouping using the concrete-representational- 22, 47–57. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00230.x
abstract sequence and strategic instruction model. Learning Disabilities Miller, S. P., & Kaffar, B. J. (2011). Developing addition and regrouping
Research & Practice, 29(2), 75–88. doi:10.1111/ldrp.12032 competence among second grade students with mathematics difficul-
Flores, M. M., Hinton, V. M., Strozier, S. D., & Terry, S. L. (2014). Us- ties. Investigations in Mathematics Learning, 4(1), 25–51. Retrieved
ing the concrete-representational-abstract sequence and the strategic from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ950987
instruction model to teach computation to students with autism spec- Miller, S. P., Stringfellow, J. L., Kaffar, B. J., Ferreira, D., & Mancl, D.
trum disorders and developmental disabilities. Education and Training B. (2011). Developing computation competence among students who
in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 49(4), 547–554. struggle with mathematics. Teaching Exceptional Children, 44(2), 38–
Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., 46. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ945988
Fuchs, D., et al. (2010). The effects of strategic counting instruction, Misquitta, R. (2011). A review of literature: Fraction instruction for strug-
with and without deliberate practice, on number combination skill gling learners in mathematics. Learning Disabilities Research and
among students with mathematics difficulties. Learning and Individual Practice, 26, 109–119. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2011.00330.x
Differences, 20(2), 89–100. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.09.003 Montague, M. (1992). The effects of cognitive and metacognitive strategy
Fuson, K. C., Wearne, D., Hiebert, J. C., Murray, H. G., Human, P. G., Olivier, instruction on the mathematical problem solving of middle school stu-
A. I., et al. (1997). Children’s conceptual structures for multidigit num- dents with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25,
bers and methods of multidigit addition and subtraction. Journal for 230–244. doi:10.1177/002221949202500404
Research in Mathematics Education, 28(2), 130–162. Retrieved from Morin, V. A., & Miller, S. P. (1998). Teaching multiplication to mid-
http://www.jstor.org/stable/749759 dle school students with mental retardation. Education and Treat-
Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early iden- ment of Children, 21, 22–36. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=
tification and interventions for students with mathematics dif- EJ567371
ficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 293–304. Mulcahy, C. A., & Krezmien, M. P. (2009). Effects of a contextualized
doi:10.1177/00222194050380040301 instructional package on the mathematics performance of secondary
Ginsburg, A., Leinwand, S., Anstrom, T., & Pollock, E. (2005). What the students with EBD. Behavioral Disorders, 34, 136–150. Retrieved from
United States can learn from Singapore’s world-class mathematics sys- http://hdl.handle.net/1903/7262
tem (and what Singapore can learn from the United States): An ex- National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). (2000). Principles
ploratory study. Washington, DC: American Institutes for Research. and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
Goldman, S. R., Hasselbring, T. S., & The Cognition and Technology Group National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). (2008). Professional
at Vanderbilt. (1997). Achieving meaningful mathematics literacy for standards for teaching mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council
30, 198–208. doi:10.1177/002221949703000207 of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common core state stan-
Hattikudur, S. D. (2011). Comparing concepts and procedures in math dards for mathematics. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from
learning (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations http://www.corestandards.org
and Theses database (UMI No. 3486791). National Mathematics Advisory Panel (NMAP). (2008). Foundations for
Hiebert, J. (1985). Children’s knowledge of common and decimal frac- success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel.
tions. Education and Urban Society, 17, 427–437. Retrieved from Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ322992 Peterson, S. K., Mercer, C. D., & O’Shea, L. (1988). Teaching learning
Hiebert, J., & Wearne, D. (1992). Links between teaching and learn- disabled students place value using the concrete to abstract sequence.
ing place value with understanding in first grade. Journal for Re- Learning Disabilities Research, 4(1), 52–56.
search in Mathematics Education, 23(2), 98–122. Retrieved from Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., & Alibali, M. W. (2001). Developing
http://www.jstor.org/stable/749496 conceptual understanding and procedural skill in mathematics: An
Hinton, V., Strozier, S. D., & Flores, M. M. (2014). Building mathematical iterative process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 346–362.
fluency for students with disabilities or students at-risk for mathematics doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.346
failure. International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science Sammons, L. (2010). Guided math. Huntington Beach, CA: Shell Education.
and Technology, 2(4). Retrieved from http://www.ulakbim.gov.tr Satsangi, R., & Bouck, E. C. (2014). Using virtual manipulative instruction
Jordan, L., Miller, D., & Mercer, C. D. (1999). The effects of con- to teach the concepts of area and perimeter to secondary students
crete to semiconcrete to abstract instruction in the acquisition with disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 38 (3), 174–186. doi:
and retention of fraction concepts and skills. Learning Disabili- 10.1177/0731948714550101
ties: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9(3), 115–122. Retrieved from Scheuermann, A. M., Deschler, D. D., & Schumaker, J. B. (2009). The
http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ594987 effects of the explicit inquiry routine on the performance of students
Kridler, P. G. (2012). Procedural and conceptual knowledge: A balanced ap- with learning disabilities on one-variable equation. Learning Disability
proach? (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations Quarterly, 32 (2), 103–120. doi: 10.2307/27740360
and Theses database (UMI No. 3521915). Schneider, M., Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. R. (2011). Relations among
Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (2000). Effects of a problem-solving strategy conceptual knowledge, procedural knowledge, and procedural flex-
on the introductory algebra performance of secondary students with ibility in two samples differing in prior knowledge. Developmental
learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 15 Psychology, 47(6), 1525. Retrieved from http://www.uni-trier.de/
(1), 10–21. doi: 10.1207/SLDRP1501_2 fileadmin/fb1/prof/PSY/PAE/Team/Schneider/SchneiderEtAl2011.
Maccini, P., & Ruhl, K. L. (2000). Effects of a graduated instructional pdf
sequence on the algebraic subtraction of integers by secondary students Stein, M. K., & Bovalino, J. W. (2001). Manipulatives: One piece of the puz-
with learning disabilities. Education & Treatment of Children (ETC), zle. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 6, 356–359. Retrieved
23(4), 465–489. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ630949 from http://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ668835
Mancl, D. B., Miller, S. P., & Kennedy, M. (2012). Using the concrete- Strickland, T. K., & Maccini, P. (2010). Strategies for teaching algebra
representational-abstract framework with integrated strategy instruc- to students with learning disabilities: Making research to practice
tion to teach subtraction with regrouping to students with learning dis- connections. Intervention in School and Clinic, 46(1), 38–45. doi:
abilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 27, 152–166. 10.1177/1053451210369519
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2012.00363.x Strickland, T. K., & Maccini, P. (2013). The effects of the concrete–
Meyer, M. L., Salimpoor, V. N., Wu, S. S., Geary, D. C., & Menon, V. (2010). representational–abstract integration strategy on the ability of stu-
Differential contribution of specific working memory components to dents with learning disabilities to multiply linear expressions within
mathematics achievement in 2nd and 3rd graders. Learning and Indi- area problems. Remedial and Special Education, 34(3), 142–153.
vidual Differences, 20(2), 101–109. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.08.004 doi:10.1177/0741932512441712
44 LEARNING DISABILITIES RESEARCH

Suh, J. M., & Moyer, P. S. (2008). Scaffolding special needs students’ learn- Watt, S. J., Watkins, J. R., & Abbitt, J. (2014). Teaching algebra to students
ing of fraction equivalence using virtual manipulatives. Proceedings of with learning disabilities: Where have we come and where should
the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, we go? Journal of Learning Disabilities, December 29, 2014, doi:
4, 297–304. 10.1177/0022219414564220
Swanson, H. L. (2012). Cognitive profile of adolescents with math- Witzel, B. S. (2005). Using CRA to teach algebra to students with
ematics disabilities: Are the profiles different from those with math difficulties in inclusive settings. Learning Disabilities—
reading disabilities? Child Neuropsychology, 18(2), 125–143. A Contemporary Journal, 3(2), 49–60. Retrieved from http://
doi:10.1080/09297049.2011.589377 www.catea.gatech.edu/scitrain/kb/FullText_Articles/Witzel_Using.
U.S. Department of Education, Office of Special Education Programs [U.S. pdf
DOE-OSEP]. (2000). Concrete-representational-abstract instruc- Witzel, B. S., Mercer, C. D., & Miller, M. D. (2003). Teaching algebra
tional approach. The Access Center (No. H326K020003). Retrieved to students with learning disabilities: An investigation of an explicit
from http://165.139.150.129/intervention/ConcreteRepresentational instruction model. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18,
AbstractInstructionalApproach.pdf 121–131. doi:10.1111/1540-5826.00068
van Garderen, D., & Montague, M. (2003). Visual-spatial representa-
Witzel, B. S., & Riccomini, P. J. (2009). Computation of fractions: Math
tion, mathematical problem solving, and students of varying abili-
intervention for elementary and middle grades students. Upper Saddle
ties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 18(4), 246–254. doi:
River, NJ: Merrill/Pearson.
10.1111/1540-5826.00079
Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2010). Academic and cognitive charac- Witzel, B. S., Riccomini, P. J., & Schneider, E. (2008). Implement-
teristics of persistent mathematics difficulty from first through fourth ing CRA with secondary students with learning disabilities in
grade. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 25(1), 25–38. doi: mathematics. Intervention in School and Clinic, 43, 270–276.
10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00298.x doi:10.1177/1053451208314734
Watson, S. M., & Gable, R. A. (2013). Unraveling the complex na- Zheng, X., Flynn, L. J., & Swanson, H. L. (2013). Experimental intervention
ture of mathematics learning disability: Implications for research studies on word problem solving and mathematics disabilities: A se-
and practice. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(3), 178–187. doi: lective analysis of the literature. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(2),
10.1177/0731948712461489 97–111. doi: 10.1177/0731948712444277

About the Authors

Jugnu Agrawal, Ph.D., is an adjunct professor in the College of Education and Human Development at George Mason University
in Fairfax, VA. Her research interests include mathematics interventions, word-problem solving and reading comprehension
interventions for students with learning disabilities and autism across K-12 settings.
Lisa L. Morin, Ph.D., is an adjunct special education professor at Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA. Dr. Morin’s research
interests includes mathematics instruction for students with learning disabilities. Dr. Morin has delivered many presentations
on international, national, and state levels on the topics of schematic-based instruction (SBI), working memory, math word
problem solving, and evidenced-based math strategies.

You might also like