You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2009.01204.

x
922
volume 53 part 11 pp 922–931 november 2009

Social information processing in boys with autistic


spectrum disorder and mild to borderline intellectual
disabilities jir_1204 922..931

P. Embregts1 & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen2


1 Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen and HAN University of Applied Sciences, Nijmegen,
The Netherlands
2 Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands

Abstract typically developing boys in their encoding of infor-


mation; they focus on negative and emotional infor-
Background Children with autistic spectrum disor-
mation in the social situation. They differ from boys
ders (ASD) and mild to borderline intellectual dis-
with mild to borderline ID in response generation,
ability (ID) have less adaptive behaviour and more
evaluation of inadequate solutions (aggressive
behaviour problems than children with mild to bor-
and submissive responses) and assertive response
derline ID. Social information processing appears to
decision.
be an important mechanism in the explanation of
Conclusions The present study extends our knowl-
the socially inadequate behaviour of children with
edge regarding social information processing of
mild to borderline ID; however, little is known
children with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to bor-
about the social information processing skills of
derline ID. This knowledge may be helpful in
children with ASD and mild to borderline ID.
designing and adapting programmes (e.g. social
Method In the present study, a total of 136 boys in
skills training, self-management training) for the
the age of 10–14 years participated; 26 with ASD
management of behaviour problems and develop-
(specifically Pervasive Developmental Disorder –
ment of adaptive behaviour of children with ASD
Not Otherwise Specified) and mild to borderline
and mild to borderline ID.
ID, 54 with mild to borderline ID without ASD and
56 typically developing boys. They completed the Keywords autism spectrum disorders, mild
Social Problem Solving Test to measure their social intellectual disabilities, social behaviour, social
information processing. information processing
Results The research results show boys with PDD-
NOS and mild to borderline ID to differ from

Correspondence: Dr Petri Embregts, Behavioural Science Institute,


Introduction
Radboud University Nijmegen, PO Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen,
Autistic spectrum disorders (ASD) co-occur with
The Netherlands (e-mail: p.embregts@pwo.ru.nl).
Authors’ note
any level of intelligence, but around 50% of indi-
We would like to thank the anonymous reviewer for his valuable viduals with ASD also have intellectual disabilities
remarks on a previous version of the manuscript. (IDs) (Chakrabarti & Fombonne 2005; Baird et al.

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
923
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

2006). Children with ASD and mild intellectual The aim of the present study is to examine social
disability (MID; limitations in intellectual function- information processes in lower cognitive functioning
ing (IQ between 55 and 85) and in social and adap- children with ASD (specifically Pervasive Develop-
tive behaviour) find it difficult to interact with mental Disorder – Not Otherwise Specified; PDD-
others, and have less adaptive behaviour and more NOS) and mild to borderline ID. Research on
behaviour problems than children with MID but social cognition of children with ASD focuses on
without ASD (Carpentieri & Morgan 1996; Hartley higher cognitive functioning children. In particular
et al. 2008). Concerning employment, education, children with Asperger’s Syndrome (AS) have
independent living and peer relations, the prognosis recently been examined on their emotional and
of those individuals is poor (Billstedt et al. 2005). social cognitive skills, and were found to differ in
Therefore, it is highly important to investigate the SIP from children without AS. In a recent study by
underlying processes of the problems with social Meyer et al. (2006), young people with AS were
interactions in these lower cognitive functioning shown to make more mistakes in the comprehen-
children with ASD. It has been argued that a lack of sion and analysis of social situations than children
social cognitive skills are a defining feature of a without this syndrome. When young people with AS
variety of atypical developmental disorders, includ- were asked to evaluate hypothetical problem situa-
ing ASD. Despite ongoing attempts to elucidate the tions, such as a peer entry situation presented on
complex relationship of IDs to ASD, there is not video, they had difficulties recognising the problem
much research known on social cognitive skills of within the conflict situations.
children with ASD and MID. Children with ASD also face difficulties with rec-
Various mechanisms, including emotional and ognition of emotions. Buitelaar et al. (1999) found
cognitive processes during social interaction, are children with ASD to have no problems with recog-
supposed to influence social behaviour. With respect nising simple emotions such as happiness, sadness
to inadequate social behaviour, the theory of social or anger when portrayed by photographed or drawn
information processing (SIP) which describes social faces. Other researchers have determined that a dif-
problem solving skills (Dodge 1986; Crick & ference exists when more complex emotions are
Dodge 1994) seems promising. Various behaviour involved such as shame, humour or flirtation. Adult
problems have been related to atypical social individuals with autism barely recognise these emo-
problem solving (Crick & Dodge 1994), and this tions, and such problems may be exacerbated when
model may be applicable to inadequate social one can speak of an accompanying ID as well
behaviour of clients with ASD and ID as well. (Baron-Cohen et al. 1997). Deficits in identifying
Within the SIP model, it is assumed that social facial expressions have also been reported in chil-
behaviour is preceded by the mental steps of encod- dren with PDD-NOS (Castelli 2005). Serra et al.
ing, interpretation, goal clarification, response gen- (2003) concluded that children with PDD-NOS
eration and response decision – with the latter were significantly slower in face recognition than
including response evaluation, judgments of self- age-matched typically developing (TD) children,
efficacy and response selection. That is, individuals while the recognition of abstract visuospatial pat-
encode and interpret the information in a given terns did not discriminate between the groups.
social situation and then set goals, search for pos- In addition to the social perception procedures of
sible responses, evaluate their response options and encoding and interpretation of social information,
select a response for enactment upon the basis of researchers have studied the generation of appropri-
the information that they have processed. It is pro- ate strategies for resolving social problems. The
posed that individual differences in the different ability of higher cognitive functioning children with
steps of SIP lead to different behavioural responses. ASD to cope with social problem situations differs
Considerable evidence exists to support the hypoth- from that of children without ASD. In a number of
esis that the behaviour problems of children without studies, it has been found that children with AS can
MID are related to their social problem solving generate just as many solutions to a problem as
skills (Crick & Dodge 1994; Matthys & Lochman children without this syndrome but that the nature
2005). of the solutions differs (Channon et al. 2001; Meyer

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
924
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

et al. 2006). Children with AS generate more to borderline ID and TD children. We expected
passive, and fewer assertive solutions than children both groups to differ in their SIP performance from
without the syndrome. TD children, but we also expected to find differ-
Despite these studies on higher functioning chil- ences between the two disability groups.
dren with ASD, knowledge on SIP in children with
ASD and ID is still lacking. From recent studies
with children with MIDs we have learned that their Method
SIP differs from that of children with average intel-
ligence and that SIP of children with MID is Participants
related to externalising and aggressive behaviour A total of 136 boys in the age of 10–14 years par-
(Leffert & Siperstein 1996; Van Nieuwenhuijzen ticipated in the present study; 26 with ASD (PDD-
et al. 2004, 2005, 2006). Children with MID tend NOS) and mild to borderline ID, 54 with mild to
to encode more negative information and also are borderline ID without ASD (PDD-NOS) and 56
less likely to generate assertive strategies than chil- boys with average intelligence (TD controls).
dren with average intellectual functioning (Van According to the most recent definition of ID
Nieuwenhuijzen et al. 2004). Children with behav- provided by the American Association on Mental
iour problems in addition to MID show primarily Retardation children with IDs are characterised by
difficulties with encoding of information and gener- ‘significant limitations both in intellectual function-
ating adequate solutions to social problem situa- ing and in adaptive behaviour as expressed in
tions but show no problems whatsoever with the conceptual, social and practical adaptive skills’
selection of the most adequate solution when pre- (Luckasson et al. 2002, p. 1). Both children with
sented with various alternatives (Van Nieuwen- ASD (PDD-NOS) and children with mild to bor-
huijzen et al. 2005, 2006, 2009). SIP thus appears derline ID were selected from schools for special
to be an important mechanism for explaining the education in the Netherlands. The criterion for such
socially inadequate behaviour of children with MID. school attendance is below-average intellectual
Social cognitive processes in children with ASD functioning (i.e. IQ range = 55–85) and limitations
have also been studied using another theoretical in social and adaptive behaviour. Boys with ASD
framework: Theory of Mind (ToM). ToM refers to (PDD-NOS) were selected if they had been diag-
the ability to attribute mental states, beliefs and nosed as having PDD-NOS by a psychologist or
thoughts to the self and to others, and is essential psychiatrist. The TD group was selected from
for understanding and interpreting social situations regular schools and had a higher mean IQ than
and interacting adequately. Three meta-analyses by both the other groups. The group of children with
Yirmiya et al. (1998) have shown that children with PDD-NOS and mild to borderline ID had a
ASD are characterised by deficits in ToM; both higher mean age than both the other groups (see
high and low functioning children with ASD differ Table 1).
from TD children and children with MID.
However, children with MID also have deficits in
Instruments
ToM abilities when compared with TD children,
albeit less severe than in children with ASD. One IQ was determined on the basis of results of indi-
plausible hypothesis, then, is that children with vidual intelligence testing obtained from school
ASD and MID differ in social cognitive processing records. In instances in which testing had not been
compared with both children with MID and TD performed within the preceding 12 months, we esti-
children. mated the child’s IQ using the Raven’s Standard
Thus, the question is whether SIP in children Progressive Matrices (Raven et al. 1983), which con-
with ASD and MID differs from children with mild sists of incomplete arrays of abstract geometrical
to borderline ID and TD children. Therefore, in the figures. Participants were asked to discover the rela-
present study the SIP skills of children with ASD, tions between the elements in the incomplete arrays
more specific PDD-NOS and mild to borderline ID and choose the right figure on the basis of this
will be compared with those of children with mild information to complete the array. The internal

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
925
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

Table 1 Age and IQ scores for children


ASD Mild/borderline ID TD with ASD (PDD-NOS), children with
mild to borderline ID, and typically
developing children
N = 26 N = 54 N = 56

M SD M SD M SD F P

Age 12.54a,b 1.27 11.19a 1.60 10.54b 1.68 14.24 0.000


IQ 83.33a 16.32 78.52b 14.98 97.25a,b 14.52 22.07 0.000

a, b: Mean scores with corresponding superscripts differ significantly.

consistency values are around 0.90, and the test– assessed the encoding of cues: ‘What happened in this
retest reliability varies from 0.90 for the short term scene?’ The mention of verbal, situational, emo-
to 0.80 for the long term. This instrument has mod- tional or negative cues was coded. Phrases literally
erate to high correlations with other situational tests repeated from the segment were coded as verbal
of intelligence (Raven et al. 1983). cues. Verbal descriptions of what happened in the
The Social Problem Solving Test (SPT, Sociale vignette, such as ‘they are playing with Lego’, were
Probleem oplossings Test) (Cuperus 1997; Matthys coded as situational cues. Remarks regarding the
et al. 1999) was designed to measure SIP. The origi- feelings of one of the children in the vignette (e.g.
nal SPT consists of 12 video vignettes with a struc- ‘he looks a bit sad’ or ‘he gets angry and starts
tured interview. For the present study, the original shouting’) were coded as the encoding of emotional
version of the SPT was revised for use with chil- cues. When cues were interpreted negatively (e.g.
dren with MID (SPT-MID; Van Nieuwenhuijzen ‘he said it in an unfriendly way’), they were coded
et al. 2001). Because a pilot study had shown the 12 as negative cues. The average number of cues
vignettes to demand too much from children with encoded and the average number of verbal, situ-
MID with regard to their attention and concentra- ational, emotional and negative cues encoded were
tion, the number of video vignettes was reduced to calculated by summing the relevant number of cues
five and the questions were simplified. The remain- across the five vignettes and dividing by five.
ing vignettes include both peer-entry and provoca- The second question assessed children’s interpre-
tion situations, which have shown to be part of one tation of the information presented: ‘[event] hap-
and the same factor ‘Being disadvantaged’ (Matthys pened, why did this happen?’ The participant was
et al. 2001). With the assistance of child actors, the asked to select one of three answers: (1) benign
social problem of being placed at a disadvantage is intent (‘it wasn’t his fault, he tried to help me’); (2)
displayed in each vignette. Each videotaped vignette unkind (but not hostile) intent (‘he is clumsy’); or
consisted of two parts. First, the social problem was (3) hostile intent (‘he did it on purpose’). A total
presented (e.g. the protagonist trying to build a hostile intent attribution score was obtained by
Lego plane but not succeeding followed by another counting the number of vignettes for which an
boy offering to help but breaking the plane). There- unkind or hostile answer was provided, with a
after, the protagonist enacted three solutions to the minimum of 0 (for none of the vignettes) and a
problem: a pro-social/assertive response, an maximum of 5 (for all of the vignettes).
antisocial/aggressive response and a passive/ The next question assessed response generation:
submissive response. ‘What would you do?’ After provision of an initial
Prior to the presentation of each video vignette, response, participants were asked to think of other
participants were asked to identify with the protago- ways of responding to the situation. The total
nist. Following the viewing of a vignette but prior to number of responses generated was then coded
the presentation of the three different solutions, a along with the quality of each response: pro-social/
number of questions were posed. The interviewers assertive, antisocial/aggressive or passive/submissive.
noted participants’ responses. The first question Spontaneous response scores were obtained by

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
926
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

counting the number of times each kind of response behaviour in class as reported by the teacher
was provided as the initial response, with a (r = 0.49) (Van Nieuwenhuijzen et al. 2005).
minimum of 0 (never) and a maximum of 5
(always). A percentage score was then calculated Data analysis
for each response category by summing the number
In order to determine whether the SIP skills of the
of responses representing a particular category
boys with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline
across the five vignettes and dividing this number
ID differed from those of the boys with mild to
by the total number of responses. A total response
borderline ID and from the TD boys, a multivariate
generation score was also obtained by summing
analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted
the number of generated responses across the
with group (ASD, MID, TD) as the independent
five vignettes and dividing this number by five.
variable, the SIP variables as the dependent vari-
A response variability score (i.e. indicator of a
ables and age as covariate because groups differed
child’s response repertoire) was obtained by
in age.
counting the number of qualitatively different
responses across the five vignettes and dividing by
five.
Results
Next, the different enacted solutions to the
problem were viewed. Each solution was followed After controlling for age, a multivariate main effect
by a number of questions to assess response evalua- of group was found, F38,230 = 4.17, P = 0.000. Next,
tion and self-efficacy. The first question concerned univariate ANOVAs were conducted for each SIP
evaluation of response outcomes either on the basis variable separately.
of moral values or on pragmatic considerations such Univariate main effects of group were found on
as likely outcome: ‘Was this a good way for the the encoding of emotional cues, F2,136 = 4.31,
child to respond?’ to be answered ‘yes’ or ‘no’. The P = 0.02 and a trend for negative cues, F2,136 = 2.80,
second question addressed participants’ confidence P = 0.06. As can be seen from Table 2, the group
in enacting the response (self-efficacy): ‘Would you ASD (PDD-NOS) encoded more emotional and
be able to behave in the same way?’ For each solu- more negative information than the group TD.
tion, a total evaluation score and a total self-efficacy Concerning response generation univariate main
score were calculated by summing the number of effects of group were found on the spontaneous
positive answers across the five vignettes, with a submissive response generation, F2,136 = 4.57,
minimum of 0 (never positive) and a maximum of 5 P = 0.01. Both groups ASD and TD generated
(always positive). After presentation of the three fewer spontaneous submissive responses than the
alternative solutions and assessment of the partici- group mild to borderline ID. Univariate main
pant’s reactions, the three videotaped solutions were effects of group were also found on the evaluation
again presented to assess response selection. Partici- of assertive, F2,136 = 16.29, P = 0.000, aggressive,
pants were asked: ‘Which of the three responses F2,136 = 10.28, P = 0.000, and submissive responses,
would you choose?’ The total number of assertive F2,136 = 8.13, P = 0.000. As can be seen in Table 2,
and aggressive solutions chosen was summed over compared with the group TD, both groups ASD
the five vignettes. (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline ID positively
To assess inter-rater reliability for coding of par- evaluated assertive responses less often, with the
ticipants’ answers to the open questions, both a group mild to borderline ID the least. In addition,
graduate student and the first author scored these the group ASD (PDD-NOS) positively evaluated
answers for 50 randomly chosen vignettes. The aggressive and submissive response less often than
Kappa values were found to be 0.79 for encoding both the groups mild to borderline ID and TD.
and 0.88 for response generation. In a previous Concerning self-efficacy, univariate main effects
study on SIP in children with MID the answers to of group were found on self-efficacy of enacting
the response generation question have correlated assertive F2,136 = 10.37, P = 0.000, and submissive
moderately with actual behaviour in real-life responses, F2,136 = 7.37, P = 0.001. The groups ASD
problem situations (r = 0.30) and highly with (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline ID were less

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
927
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

Table 2 SIP scores for children with ASD (PDD-NOS), with mild to borderline ID, and typically developing children

ASD Mild/borderline ID TD

N = 26 N = 54 N = 56

M SD M SD M SD F P

Encoding
Situational cues 2.71 1.23 2.77 1.15 2.92 0.97 2.58 n.s.
Verbal cues 0.84 0.50 0.68 0.48 0.59 0.44 1.56 n.s.
Emotional cues 0.30a 0.25 0.18 0.21 0.11a 0.16 4.31 0.02
Negative cues 0.55a 0.26 0.52 0.24 0.46a 0.21 2.80 0.06
Total cues 3.85 1.55 3.64 1.51 3.62 1.09 0.29 n.s.
Interpretation
Hostile intent attribution 2.73 1.00 2.74 0.94 2.57 0.95 0.41 n.s.
Response generation
Spontaneous assertive 2.85 1.57 2.43 1.18 2.80 1.33 1.52 n.s.
Spontaneous aggressive 0.88 1.03 0.69 0.93 0.89 1.07 0.69 n.s.
Spontaneous submissive 1.19a 1.17 1.87a,b 1.24 1.30b 1.01 4.57 0.01
Mean number of responses 2.18 1.15 1.99 0.32 1.90 0.36 0.96 n.s.
Evaluation
Assertive response 3.31a,b 1.12 2.67a,c 1.03 3.79b,c 1.00 16.29 0.000
Aggressive response 0.81a,b 0.85 1.96a,c 1.12 1.45b,c 1.31 10.28 0.000
Submissive response 1.92a 1.16 2.87a,b 1.26 2.09b 1.18 8.13 0.000
Self-efficacy for enactment
Assertive response 2.35a 1.35 2.22b 1.33 3.23a,b 1.19 10.37 0.000
Aggressive response 1.38 1.30 1.67 1.08 1.46 1.32 0.76 n.s.
Submissive response 1.27a 0.83 2.33a,b 1.37 1.63b 1.26 7.37 0.001
Selection
Assertive response 3.42a 1.21 2.54a,b 1.18 3.02b 1.24 4.40 0.01
Aggressive response 0.46 0.90 1.00 1.24 0.79 1.02 2.34 n.s.

a, b, c: Mean scores with corresponding superscripts differ significantly.

confident in enacting an assertive response than the differ from both boys with mild to borderline ID
group TD. In addition, both the groups ASD and TD, but in different ways. On the one hand,
(PDD-NOS) and TD were less confident in boys with ASD (PDD-NOS) differ from TD in
enacting submissive responses than the group mild their encoding of information and evaluation of
to borderline ID. Finally, a univariate effect was assertive responses; they focus on negative and
found on assertive response decision, F2,136 = 4.40, emotional information in the social situation and
P = 0.01. Both the groups ASD (PDD-NOS) and evaluate assertive responses less positively. On the
TD chose more often an assertive response than the other hand they differ from mild to borderline ID
group mild to borderline ID. in response generation, evaluation of inadequate
solutions (aggressive and submissive responses) and
assertive response decision. In other words, boys
Discussion
with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline ID
In the present study the SIP skills of boys with appear to demonstrate greater competence in some
PDD-NOS and mild to borderline ID are com- aspects of social problem-solving than boys with
pared with those of boys with mild to borderline ID mild to borderline ID. But even though boys with
and TD boys. The research results show boys with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline ID seem
ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to borderline ID to to know that aggressive and submissive responses

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
928
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

are not quite adequate to solve social problem situ- Another explanation is that boys with ASD and
ations, they do not believe assertive responses have mild to borderline ID may not actually be demon-
positive consequences and do not consider them- strating greater skill in social cognition than chil-
selves capable enough to enact those responses. dren with ID, but rather different patterns. There is
The results seem to be in line with other research evidence from previous research (Siperstein &
findings (Van Nieuwenhuijzen et al. 2009), that Leffert 1997) suggesting that ‘low-risk’ submissive
showed children with MID to choose more often an strategies may actually be adaptive for this popula-
assertive response out of three possible solutions, tion. In this study, the socially accepted children
but not to generate them spontaneously. Similar to with ID were more likely to generate submissive
children with MID, children with ASD (PDD- strategies than socially rejected children with ID.
NOS) and mild to borderline ID seem to know how Although these findings were obtained in public
to respond adequately in social problem situations, school settings, they may be true for special schools
but apparently do not behave accordingly; possibly as well. It may be that children with ID find peer
because they find difficulties in transferring their interactions and conflicts risky and that based on
knowledge to other social situations or because they their experiences, they have concluded that the best
are not confident enough in enacting adequate way to cope with these conflicts is by employing
responses. submissive strategies. Only if we had data regarding
At the same time, however, they focus on nega- the social behaviour and/or social status of these
tive information in a social situation more often children could we interpret these results more
than TD boys. A similar phenomenon appears to clearly. Nevertheless, this study uncovered differ-
occur for children who are aggressive, anxious or ences in SIP between the two disability groups, and
depressed: Such children tend to interpret situa- further research is needed to explore the connection
tions more often as threatening and hostile than between social cognition and social behaviour in
children who do not show these characteristics this population so that we can better understand
(Garber et al. 1991; Quiggle et al. 1992; Bell-Dolan their characteristic patterns of social cognition and
1995; Daleiden & Vasey 1997; Orobio de Castro the role that these patterns play in their overall
et al. 2002). Apparently such children view the social functioning.
outside world with suspicion. While it is not clear In addition we did not expect children with mild
where this predisposition comes from, it is possible to borderline ID and ASD to encode more emo-
that previous experiences in which others intention- tional information than TD children, because chil-
ally mistreated them have been saved as schemes in dren with ASD are generally characterised by poor
their memories. emotion processing. However, previous studies have
There are, however, also some unexpected find- shown children with PDD-NOS to have better
ings. Boys with mild to borderline ID and ASD abilities than children with ASD regarding faces
appear to demonstrate greater competence in some recognition and emotion processing, and even the
aspects of social problem-solving than boys with same as TD children (Serra et al. 1998).
mild to borderline ID alone; they generated less When interpreting the results of our study, its
submissive responses and chose more often an strengths and limitations should be taken into
assertive response. One possible explanation for the account. An important strength concerns the inclu-
higher assertive scores in the ASD (PDD-NOS) sion of children with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild
group is that children with PDD-NOS may have to borderline ID; the study is the first to apply the
received extra training in social problem-solving SIP theory to children with ASD (PDD-NOS). It is
skills. At almost all schools for special education important to gain knowledge on their social cogni-
children receive social skills training. However, tive skills, as more and more children are being
although we did not collect data on treatment, it is identified as having ASD (PDD-NOS).
very likely that part of these children with PDD- A potential limitation concerns the sample.
NOS received some kind of treatment outside of Because the study only included boys in the age
school because of their dual diagnosis, which may range of 10–14 years old, these results cannot be
have influenced their problem solving skills. generalised to all children with ASD (PDD-NOS)

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
929
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

and mild to borderline ID. In addition, the group of would be helpful in overcoming this gap (e.g.
children with ASD (PDD-NOS) and mild to bor- Sherer et al. 2001). Procedures of self-management,
derline ID was relatively small compared with the such as self-monitoring, self-recording, self-
other two groups in the study. Therefore, we suggest evaluation and self-administering consequences
that future research studies replicate these findings have shown to be effective with youth with MID
on a larger scale. and behaviour problems (Embregts 2000, 2002).
A second limitation concerns social abilities of Research has shown that self-monitoring can be
the respondents in this sample. The premise of the helpful in reducing external control with people
study is that both children with ASD (PDD-NOS) with ASD and by consequence, to facilitate gener-
and mild to borderline ID, and children with mild alisation across stimulus dimensions (e.g. Koegel &
to borderline ID might be socially impaired owing Koegel 1990). Therefore, we recommend a combi-
to atypical SIP. However, this study lacks data on nation of a social problem-solving training protocol
social ability or social impairment of the respon- with self-management in the natural setting (at
dents. All respondents were selected from schools home or the classroom) of the child to maximise
for special education in the Netherlands. Admission the effects of the training.
criteria for these schools imply that they have Additional research is needed with larger repre-
below-average IQ and limitations in adaptive behav- sentative samples including girls, not only to further
iour. Previous research has shown that children study SIP of children with ASD (PDD-NOS) and
from special education score lower on social skills mild to borderline ID, but also to examine the rela-
and social adaptive behaviour than TD children tions between social problem-solving skills and
from normal schools (Dekker et al. 2002; Van Nieu- behaviour problems, in order to develop effective
wenhuijzen et al. 2004). interventions. More research to improve the devel-
These findings have implications for treatment of opmental prognosis for such children is needed and
behaviour problems in children with ASD (PDD- more research to adapt the parenting and guidance
NOS) and mild to borderline ID and intervention of these children to their potential and possibilities
programmes. If children with ASD (PDD-NOS) is essential.
know how to respond adequately, but do not evalu-
ate those responses positively and have not enough
confidence to enact those responses, they should be References
trained in problem-solving skills. By practising these
Baird G., Simonoff E., Pickles A., Chandler S., Loucas T.,
skills in role plays they will experience positive out- Meldrum D. et al. (2006) Prevalence of disorders of the
comes and which may increase their self-esteem. Autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in
Recently, a cognitive behaviour training programme South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project
has been developed for children with MID, based (SNAP). Lancet 368, 210–15.
on the proven effective Coping Power Program Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. & Jolliffe T. (1997) Is
(Lochman & Wells 2003, 2004). The programme there a ‘language of the eyes’? Evidence from normal
adults and adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome.
entitled ‘Samen Stevig Staan’ [Standing Strong
Visual Cognition 4, 311–32.
Together] includes a social problem-solving training
Bell-Dolan D. J. (1995) Social cue interpretation of
protocol for children and a training of parenting anxious children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 24,
skills for their parents (Van ‘t Hof et al. 2009). The 1–10.
programme may be relevant for children with ASD Billstedt E., Gillberg C., & Gillberg C. (2005) Autism
(PDD-NOS) as well, after adaptation for children after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year
with ASD. follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diag-
Past literature that has looked at children’s per- nosed childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 35, 351–60.
formance of social cognitive processes has cited the
existence of a gap between children’s ability to Buitelaar J. K., Van der Wees M., Swaab-Barneveld H. &
Van der Gaag R. J. (1999) Theory of mind and
reason in hypothetical or practice situations and emotion-recognition functioning in autistic spectrum
their behaviour in response to social challenges in disorders and in psychiatric control and normal chil-
real life. The use of self-management procedures dren. Development and Psychopathology 11, 39–58.

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
930
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

Carpentieri S. & Morgan S. B. (1996) Adaptive and intel- Koegel R. L. & Koegel L. K. (1990) Extended reductions
lectual functioning in autistic and nonautistic retarted in stereotypic behavior of students with autism through
children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders a self-management treatment package. Journal of Applied
26, 611–20. Behavior Analysis 23, 119–27.
Castelli F. (2005) Understanding emotions from standard- Leffert J. S. & Siperstein G. N. (1996) Assessment of
ized facial expressions in autism and normal develop- social-cognitive processes in children with mental retar-
ment. Autism 9, 428–49. dation. American Journal on Mental Retardation 100,
Chakrabarti S. & Fombonne E. (2005) Pervasive develop- 441–55.
mental disorders in preschool children: confirmation of Lochman J. E. & Wells K. C. (2003) Effectiveness of the
high prevalence. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, coping power program and of classroom intervention
1133–41. with aggressive children: outcomes at a 1-year follow-up.
Channon S., Charman T., Heap J., Crawford S. & Rios P. Behavior Therapy 34, 493–515.
(2001) Real-life-type problem-solving in Asperger’s syn- Lochman J. E. & Wells K. C. (2004) The coping power
drome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31, program for preadolescent aggressive boys and their
451–69. parents: outcome effects at the 1-year follow-up. Journal
Crick N. R. & Dodge K. A. (1994) A review and reformu- of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 72, 571–8.
lation of social information processing mechanisms in Luckasson R., Borthwick-Duffy S., Buntinx W., Coulter
children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin 115, D., Craig P., Reeve A. et al. (2002) Mental Retardation:
74–101. Definition, Classification and Systems of Supports, 10th
Cuperus J. M. (1997). Sociale probleemoplossing bij kin- edn. American Association on Mental Retardation,
deren met gedragsstoornissen [Social problem solving Washington, DC.
in children with behaviour problems]. Dissertation, Matthys W. & Lochman J. E. (2005) Social problem
Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands. solving in aggressive children. In: Social Problem Solving
Daleiden E. L. & Vasey M. W. (1997) An information pro- and Offenders (eds M. McCurran & J. McGuire), pp.
cessing perspective on childhood anxiety. Clinical Psy- 51–66. Wiley, Chichester.
chology Review 17, 407–29. Matthys W., Cuperus J. M. & Van Engeland H. (1999)
Dekker M. C., Koot H. M., van der Ende J. & Verhulst Deficient social problem-solving in boys with ODD/CD,
F. C. (2002) Emotional and behavioral problems in with ADHD, and with both disorders. Journal of the
children and adolescents with and without intellectual American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 38,
disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 43, 311–21.
1087–98. Matthys W., Maassen G. H., Cuperus J. M. & Van
Dodge K. A. (1986) A social information processing Engeland H. (2001) The assessment of the situational
model of social competence in children. In: Minnesota specificity of children’s problem behavior in peer-peer
Symposium on Child Psychology:Vol. 18. Cognitive Perspec- context. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42,
tives on Children’s Social and Behavioral Development (ed. 413–20.
M. Perlmutter), pp. 77–125. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. Meyer J. A., Mundy P. C., Van Hecke A. V. & Durocher
Embregts P. J. C. M. (2000) Effectiveness of video feed- J. S. (2006) Social attribution processes and comorbid
back and self-management on inappropriate social psychiatric symptoms in children with Asperger syn-
behavior of youth with mild mental retardation. Research drome. Autism 10, 383–402.
in Developmental Disabilities 21, 409–23. Orobio de Castro B., Veerman J. W., Koops W., Bosch
Embregts P. J. C. M. (2002) Effect of resident and direct- J. D. & Monshouwer H. J. (2002) Hostile attribution of
care staff training on responding during social interac- intent and aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Child
tions. Research in Developmental Disabilities 23, 353–66. Development 73, 916–34.

Garber J., Quiggle N. L., Panak W. & Dodge K. A. (1991) Quiggle N. L., Garber J., Panak W. F. & Dodge K. A.
Aggression and depression in children: comorbidity, (1992) Social information processing in aggressive and
specificity and social cognitive processing. In: Internaliz- depressed children. Child Development 63, 1305–20.
ing and Externalizing Expressions of Dysfunction: Rochester Raven J. C., Court J. H. & Raven J. (1983) Manual for
Symposium on Developmental Psychology, Vol. 2 (eds D. Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Lewis,
Cicchetti & S. L. Toth), pp. 225–61. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, London.
NL. Serra M., Jackson A. E., van Geert P. L. & Minderaa
Hartley S. L., Sikora D. M. & McCoy R. (2008) Preva- R. B. (1998) Brief report: interpretation of facial expres-
lence and risk factors of maladaptive behaviour in sions, postures and gestures in children with a pervasive
young children with Autistic Disorder. Journal of Intellec- developmental disorder not otherwise specified. Journal
tual Disability Research 52, 819–29. of Autism and Developmental Disorders 28, 257–63.

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 11 november 2009
931
P. Embregts & M. van Nieuwenhuijzen • SIP and ASD

Serra M., Althaus M., de Sonneville L. M., Stant A. D., cessing, and behavior problems. European Journal of
Jackson A. E. & Minderaa R. B. (2003) Face recogni- Developmental Psychology 1, 215–29.
tion in children with a pervasive developmental disorder Van Nieuwenhuijzen M., Bijman E. R., Lamberix I. C. W.,
not otherwise specified. Journal of Autism and Develop- Wijnroks L., Orobio de Castro B., Vermeer A. et al.
mental Disorders 33, 303–17. (2005) Do children do what they say? Responses to
Sherer M., Pierce K. L., Paredes S., Kisacky K. L., Inger- hypothetical and real-life social problems in children
soll B. & Schreibman L. (2001) Enhancing conversation with mild intellectual disabilities and behaviour prob-
skills in children with autism via video technology. lems. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 49, 419–
Behavior Modification 25, 140–58. 33.
Siperstein G. N. & Leffert J. S. (1997) Comparison of Van Nieuwenhuijzen M., Orobio de Castro B., Valk I. van
socially accepted and rejected children with mental der Wijnroks L., Vermeer A. & Matthys W. (2006) Do
retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation 101, social information processing models explain aggressive
339–51. behaviour by children with mild intellectual disabilities
Van ‘t Hof C., Eikelenboom M., Matthys W., Van Nieu- in residential care? Journal of Intellectual Disability
wenhuijzen M. & Orobio de Castro B. (2009) Samen Research 50, 801–12.
Stevig Staan. Ouder-en kindtraining ter vermindering van Van Nieuwenhuijzen M., Orobio de Castro B., Wijnroks
gedragsproblemen bij jeugdigen met een licht verstandelijke L., Vermeer A. & Matthys W. (2009) Social problem
beperking [Standing Strong Together. Parent and Child solving and mild intellectual disabilities: relations with
Training to Reduce Behavior Problems in Youth with Mild externalizing behavior and therapeutic context. American
Intellectual Disabilities]. Utrecht University and VOBC- Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 114,
LVG, Utrecht. 42–51.
Van Nieuwenhuijzen M., Bijman E. R., Lamberix I. C. W., Yirmiya N., Erel N., Shaked M. & Solomonica-Levi D.
Wijnroks L. & Matthys W. (2001) Handleiding voor de (1998) Meta-anlayses comparing theory of mind abili-
SPT-MLK. [Manual for the SPT-MLK]. Unpublished ties of individuals with autism, individuals with mental
Manuscript, Universiteit Utrecht, Utrecht. retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psy-
Van Nieuwenhuijzen M., Orobio de Castro B., Wijnroks chological Bulletin 124, 283–307.
L., Vermeer A. & Matthys W. (2004) The relations
between intellectual disabilities, social information pro- Accepted 17 July 2009

© 2009 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2009 Blackwell Publishing Ltd

You might also like