You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51591449

Neural Substrates of Decision Making as Measured With the Iowa Gambling


Task in Men With Alexithymia

Article in Psychosomatic Medicine · August 2011


DOI: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318223c7f8 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

34 428

3 authors, including:

Michiko Kano Shin Fukudo


Tohoku University Tohoku University
139 PUBLICATIONS 2,869 CITATIONS 450 PUBLICATIONS 11,592 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Michiko Kano on 20 December 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Neural Substrates of Decision Making as Measured With the Iowa Gambling
Task in Men With Alexithymia
MICHIKO KANO, MD, PHD, MASATOSHI ITO, MD, PHD, AND SHIN FUKUDO, MD, PHD
Objective: Individuals with alexithymia have a reduced ability to use their feelings to guide their behavior appropriately in social
situations. To reveal the capacity to use emotional signals in alexithymia under conditions of uncertainty, this study investigates neural
substrates and performance on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), which was developed to assess decision making based on emotion-
guided evaluation. Methods: The participants were 10 men with alexithymia and 13 without. Alexithymia was assessed by the 20-item
Toronto Alexithymia Scale. Regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) was measured by [15O]-H2O positron emission tomography during
four trials of the IGT and two visuomotor control tasks. Results: The participants with alexithymia failed to learn an advantageous
decision-making strategy, with performance differing significantly from the nonalexithymic group in the fourth IGT trial (p = .029).
Comparing performance between the IGT and the control tasks, both groups showed brain activation in the dorsolateral frontal
area, inferior frontal lobe, preYsupplementary motor area, inferior parietal lobe, fusiform gyrus, and cerebellum. Men with alexithymia
showed lower rCBF in the medial frontal area (Brodmann area [BA] 10) and higher rCBF in the caudate and occipital areas in the
first and second IGT trials, which are within a learning phase according to test performance data. All brain data were significant at
p e .001, uncorrected. Conclusions: BA10 activity may be associated with using internal signals accompanying affective evaluation of
the stimuli, which is crucial for successful decision making. Reduced BA10 activity in participants with alexithymia suggests that they
may not use an emotion-based biasing signal to lead to advantageous decision making. Key words: emotional regulation, alexithymia,
somatic marker, medial frontal cortex, decision making, neuroimaging.

BA = Brodmann area; IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; rCBF = regional valenced sentences, words, faces, and scenes. Compared with
cerebral blood flow; STAI = Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety In- controls, individuals with alexithymia recalled fewer responses
ventory; TAS-20 = 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale; vmPFC = for emotional words but the same for neutral words (8). Brain
ventromedial prefrontal cortex. imaging studies using emotion-inducing images or the recall
of emotional events has shown that people with alexithymia
exhibit different activation of emotion-related brain regions
INTRODUCTION (9Y11) and impaired understanding of other people’s desires

A lexithymia is a personality trait derived from clinical ob-


servations of patients with psychosomatic illness (1) and
is considered a disorder of affect regulation (2). The salient
and intentions, in addition to reduced levels of empathy for
other people’s pain (12).
Each of the studies mentioned previously focused on one
features of alexithymia include difficulty in recognizing and stage of information processing in alexithymic individuals, that
verbalizing one’s feelings, difficulty in distinguishing feelings is, either basic emotional or cognitive-emotional level. In ad-
from bodily sensations of emotional arousal, and an externally dition to these difficulties in cognitive-emotional processing,
oriented cognitive style (3). Alexithymia has been reported to people with alexithymia have been thought to be unable to use
be a risk factor for a variety of medical and psychiatric disor- feelings to guide their behavior appropriately, which may lead
ders, such as somatization, chronic pain, functional gastroin- to difficulties in the social environment (2). In this research, we
testinal disorders, eating disorders, substance use disorders, investigate the ability to use emotional signals to guide their
anxiety, and depression (2,3). Moreover, alexithymia reduces behavior in social situations.
life satisfaction (4) and has also been prospectively associ- Bechara et al. (13) developed the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)
ated with all-cause mortality (5). The prevalence of alexithymia to assess decision making in patient populations. The task
in healthy populations is reported to be approximately 8% to emphasizes the contribution of emotional processing to deci-
10% (3); therefore, investigating the problems of affect regu- sion making. It requires the subject to learn the rewards and
lation in alexithymia may have important medical and social punishments associated with four card decks to maximize
implications. the earning of pretend money. High-risk decisions involve
Research has repeatedly shown impairment of affect regu- the chance of great reward but also a high risk of loss (pun-
lation in alexithymia. Lane et al. (6,7) reported that highly ishment). In contrast, low-risk decisions may lead to better
alexithymic individuals perform poorly on a perception task long-term payoff. The task requires the ability to distinguish
of recognizing basic emotions associated with emotionally between high- and low-risk financial decisions, and learning
based on the experience of losses over time. Patients with bi-
From the Behavioral Medicine (M.K., S.F.), Tohoku University Graduate lateral lesions in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)
School of Medicine, and Cyclotron and Radioisotope Center (M.I.), Tohoku
University, Sendai, Japan. have shown impairment on the task; they remained unable
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Shin Fukudo, MD, PhD, to develop affective reactions with appropriate affective judg-
Behavioral Medicine, Tohoku University Graduate School of Medicine, 2-1 ment (14Y16) and displayed poor decision making in everyday
Seiryo-cho, Aoba-ku, Sendai 980-8575, Japan. E-mail: sfukudo@med.
tohoku.ac.jp life. Correspondingly, they showed poor IGT performance,
This work was supported by Grants-in-Aid from the Ministry of Education, persistently selecting from the ‘‘risky’’ decks characterized
Science, Sports, and Culture, and the Ministry of Health and Welfare, Japan by large immediate rewards but larger long-term punishments
(M.K.).
Received for publication July 13, 2010; revision received April 20, 2011. (14Y16). The role of vmPFC in decision making has been un-
DOI: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318223c7f8 derstood further by imaging studies in healthy subjects who

588 Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011)


0033-3174/11/7307Y0588
Copyright * 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRAIN IMAGING OF GAMBLING TASK IN ALEXITHYMIA

show signal changes in the vmPFC while carrying out the IGT Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (24), and International Classification
trials (17Y19). of Diseases 10 (25) criteria. The subjects were given a description of the study
protocol, and their written informed consents were obtained. This study was
Such decision making as seen in the IGT presupposes the approved by the Ethics Committee of Tohoku University School of Medi-
theory of somatic markers, as developed by Damasio (20). This cine and was performed in accordance with the policies of the Declaration
theory argues that optimal decision making is not simply the of Helsinki.
result of rationally, cognitively calculated categorization of
gains and losses but is also based on good or bad affective Decision-Making Task
The decision-making task is a computerized gambling card game that
reactions and emotionally guided evaluation. The decision- tests the ability to choose between high gains with a risk for even higher
making deficits found after vmPFC damage are due to an in- losses and low gains with a risk for smaller losses. The 100-item task is com-
ability to use emotion-based biasing signals generated from the pleted four times, and each run-through takes at least 2.5 minutes to com-
body (or ‘‘somatic markers’’) when appraising different re- plete. The participants were instructed to accumulate as much (play) money
sponse options. The reasoning is influenced by crude biasing as possible by picking one card at a time from each of the four decks (A, B,
C, and D) until 100 cards were chosen. Once a card was selected, the mes-
signals arising from the neural machinery that underlies emo- sage ‘‘You get l5000’’ (or l10,000; equivalent to approximately US $50 or
tion. Following Damasio (20), emotion is the representation $100) appeared immediately on the screen and remained for 1.5 seconds until
and regulation of the complex array of homeostatic changes the subject was prompted to play again by the message, ‘‘Pick a card.’’ If a loss
that occur in different levels of the brain and body in given was attached to the choice, a message lasting 1.5 seconds was added to the
situations. When making decisions, a somatic marker arising screen (e.g., ‘‘You must pay l75,000’’; US $750). At the end of the 1.5 seconds,
the subject was prompted to make the next selection. The cumulative net
from the periphery or the central representation of the periphery amount of money earned (i.e., the payout) was updated on the screen after each
indicates our emotional reaction to a response option. For every card pick. The subjects continued to play until 100 cards were selected. The
response option contemplated, a somatic state is generated, scan started on average at 85 seconds after the task began, corresponding
including sensations from the viscera, internal milieu, and the to the completion of 34 cards or trials. During the 70-second scan, 28 trials were
skeletal and smooth muscles. These somatic markers serve as completed, and 38 cards were completed after the PET scan ended. The sub-
jects continued to play until 100 cards were selected.
an indicator of the value of what is represented and also as a Task decks differed along two dimensions: immediate gain and risk of
booster signal for continued working memory and attention. penalties. The accumulated penalties were larger than the accumulated gains
Particularly in situations of complexity and uncertainty, these in decks A and B (disadvantageous decks) and were smaller than the accu-
marker signals help to reduce the problem space to a tractable mulated gains in decks C and D (advantageous decks). The control task
size by marking response options with an ‘‘emotional’’ signal. was designed to replicate the decision-making task in all aspects except for
decision making and was similar in sensorimotor demands and in exposure
Only those options that are marked as promising are processed to gains and losses. The participants were instructed to pick cards from the
in a full, cognitive fashion. decks sequentially in the fixed order of A-B-C-D-A-B-C-D and so on.
In this experiment, we aimed to investigate the strategy and
neural substrates for decision making in alexithymia while Positron Emission Tomography
performing the IGT. Because the defining feature of alexi- PET scans of the distribution of [15O]-H2O were obtained using an SET-
2400W PET scanner (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) operated in the high-sensitivity
thymia is disordered emotional regulation and difficulty in un-
three-dimensional mode with average axial resolution of 4.5 mm at maximum
derstanding bodily sensations associated with emotions, it is strength and sensitivity for a 20-cm cylindrical phantom of 48.6 kcps/kBq
expected that individuals with alexithymia cannot use emotion- permilliliter (26). PET sessions included seven 1-minute scans at 10-minute
based biasing signals (i.e., somatic markers) in IGT trials and intervals, each after the intravenous injection of approximately 5 mCi (185 MBq)
that they exhibit brain activation different from controls dur- of [15O]-H2O to assess regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). Before the PET
study, the subjects performed a 1-minute training task. The seven scans con-
ing IGT performance, particularly in the areas associated with
sisted of four decision-making tasks, two control tasks, and one visual fixa-
IGT processing. tion task. The fixation task, used for quality control, was not part of the analysis.
The order of tasks was counterbalanced across subjects. The PET data were
MATERIALS AND METHODS obtained between April 2003 and September 2004.
Subjects
Twenty-three healthy male volunteers (10 with alexithymia and 13 with- Performance Data
out) participated in this study. All participants were right-handed and aged 18 Score on the IGT was defined as the number of choices from the advanta-
to 26 years. Individuals with 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) geous decks (C and D) minus the number of choices from the disadvanta-
scores greater than 61 (61Y73) were considered alexithymic, whereas those geous decks (A and B) for 100 trials. This net score (decks [C + D] j decks
with TAS-20 scores less than 51 (26Y46) were considered nonalexithymic, [A + B]) within each 25-choice time block enables the assessment of learn-
according to published cut-off criteria (2). The Japanese version of the TAS-20 ing on the task.
has previously been found to be psychometrically valid (21). In addition, in-
dividual trait score of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) Statistical Analysis
(22) was used as a confounding factor in positron emission tomographic Statistical parametric mapping software (SPM2, Wellcome Department
(PET) analysis to remove variance from the TAS-20 due to self-reported neg- of Cognitive Neurology, London, England, UK) was used for PET image
ative affect, which tends to correlate positively with the TAS-20 (23), to ensure realignment, normalization, and smoothing and to create statistical maps of
its emotional processing deficit aspect. Individual TAS-20 score significantly significant rCBF changes (27). All rCBF images were stereotaxically nor-
correlated with the trait score of STAI (r = 0.41, p G .05) by the Pearson malized into the standard space corresponding to the Montreal Neurological
correlation method. Each participant underwent a basic evaluation that in- Institute template. The normalized images were smoothed using a 12  12 
cluded a medical history review to exclude individuals with organic dis- 12-mm Gaussian filter. The contribution of each parameter of interest to
eases. None of the volunteers had a history of psychiatric or neurological changes in rCBF was estimated by SPM2 according to the general linear
disorders according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental model at voxel level. The global cerebral blood flow effects were controlled

Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011) 589

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
M. KANO et al.
with analysis of covariance by participant, and the mean global value was TABLE 1. Iowa Gambling Task Performance of Alexithymic and
set to 50 mL/min per deciliter. Estimates were made using linear combina- Nonalexithymic Groups, Indicated by Net Score (Advantaged Decks
tions of contrasts, and the individual trait score on the STAI was covaried for [C + D] j Disadvantaged Decks [A + B])
the potentially confounding effects of negative emotion. The resulting set of
voxel values constituted a parametric map for each contrast. Nonalexithymic Group Alexithymic Group
To determine the regional brain activity associated with IGT perfor-
mance, activity during the two control tasks was subtracted from that during M SD M SD
the four IGT tasks in each group (alexithymic and nonalexithymic). A correla-
tion analysis was also performed between brain activity during the four IGT 1st
IGT tasks and individual TAS-20 scores for all 23 subjects. In addition, to 1Y25 j3.71 7.83 j1.70 6.90
detect differences in cerebral regional activation during the gambling task (GT) 26Y50 0.50 5.13 j1.30 4.42
compared with the control task between groups, GT-control contrast was sub- 51Y75 j1.50 6.02 0.70 10.69
tracted between groups (28). The eigenvariate values at the brain region where
76Y100 j0.93 10.25 3.70 12.07
the activity differed between groups were demonstrated with the volume of
interest function in SPM2, and the value was compared between two groups IGT 2nd
with the Student’s t test, using SPSS 18.0 (IBM, New York, NY). Voxel ac- 1Y25 j1.00 9.15 0.20 16.71
tivation was considered statistically significant at a threshold of p G .001 26Y50 3.29 8.30 3.60 14.02
(uncorrected), with an extent threshold of 20 voxels. However, to reduce false- 51Y75 j1.29 8.87 0.60 18.13
positive findings, we considered only those clusters reaching significance at
76Y100 j0.21 8.68 j0.20 15.50
the p corrected G .05 cluster level (correction for multiple comparisons) in
the main subtraction analysis. Repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) was IGT 3rd
used to examine the effect of alexithymia on each of the four IGT tasks with 1Y25 2.71 8.11 j2.60 16.54
a two-level between-subjects variable (alexithymic and nonalexithymic) and 26Y50 5.50 8.31 j4.80 13.64
a within-subjects variable of the 25-choice time blocks on the IGT (four 51Y75 7.29 12.86 j6.00 13.96
levels), using SPSS 18.0.
76Y100 5.00 14.05 j5.00 15.17
RESULTS IGT 4tha
Performance Score of IGT 1Y25 10.14 13.21 j2.80 14.22
Each subject performed four IGT trials, and separate 26Y50 10.14 12.42 j8.80 16.29
PET scans were taken during each trial. The mean (standard 51Y75 8.71 16.41 j13.40 12.99
deviation) of the net score for each group (alexithymic and 76Y100 8.07 16.85 j7.40 17.61
nonalexithymic) across the four trials is presented in Table 1. a
p = .029, group by time-block interaction by repeated two-way analysis of
Repeated-measures ANOVA of net scores across the variance.
25-choice time blocks (four levels: 1Y25, 26Y50, 51Y75, and IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
76Y100) by group showed significant interaction effects
(F(3,63) = 3.19, p = .029, partial G2 = 0.13) in the fourth GT.
trials (first, second, third, and fourth) against two control
There were no significant interaction effects in the first
tasks. The brain areas where rCBF was higher in individuals in
(F(3,63) = 0.78, p = .51, partial G2 = 0.03), second (F(3,63) =
the nonalexithymic group than the alexithymic group were
0.06, p = .9, partial G2 = 0.002), or third (F(3,63) = 0.75, p = .53,
the inferior temporal gyrus and the left medial frontal gyrus.
partial G2 = 0.03) GTs.
In contrast, the brain areas where rCBF was higher in indi-
Brain Activation viduals with alexithymia than those without were the right
Group Activation in IGT Versus Control Conditions cuneus and the lingual gyrus. Subtraction data of the rCBF in
Subtraction of the rCBF associated with the visuomotor the control conditions from that for each IGT condition are
control conditions (two scans) from that associated with the shown in Table 4 and Figure 3. In the first and second IGT
IGT (first, second, third, and fourth) conditions is represented conditions, individuals without alexithymia showed higher
in Table 2 and Figure 1. The nonalexithymic group showed brain activation in the left medial frontal gyrus than those
activation of areas in the middle frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal with alexithymia. There was no brain area where the rCBF
cortex (OFC), inferior parietal gyrus, insula, anterior cingulate was higher in the nonalexithymic group than in the alexithy-
gyrus, and cerebellum. The alexithymic group showed activation mic group in the third IGT. In the fourth IGT condition, the
of areas in the middle and inferior frontal gyrus, inferior parietal precuneus and the precentral gyrus were the brain areas where
gyrus, caudate, precuneus, fusiform gyrus, and cerebellum. rCBF was higher in the nonalexithymic group. In contrast,
the brain areas where the alexithymic participants showed higher
Correlation Between Brain Activation in IGT and
rCBF were the occipital lobe (V3), caudate, lingual gyrus, cin-
Alexithymia Score
gulate gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and precentral gyrus in the
The TAS-20 score negatively correlated with rCBF in
first IGT condition; precuneus in the second IGT condition; and
the middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann area [BA] 10) with other
the fusiform gyrus in the third IGT condition. There was no brain
brain regions (Table 3 and Fig. 2).
area where rCBF was higher in the individuals with alexithymia
Comparison Between Groups that the nonalexithymic group in the fourth IGT condition.
Table 4 and Figure 2 show group comparisons between Figure 4 shows the subtraction of the average eigenvari-
participants with alexithymia and those without in the IGT ate values associated with the visuomotor control conditions

590 Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011)

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRAIN IMAGING OF GAMBLING TASK IN ALEXITHYMIA
TABLE 2. Brain Regions With Significant Increases in Cerebral Blood Flow During Decision-Making Tasks in Alexithymic and Nonalexithymic Groups

Talairach-Tournoux Coordinate

Group, Contrast, and Brain Region (Brodmann Area) T Score k (Cluster Extent) X Y Z

IGT 1Y4 9 control (nonalexithymic group)


Inferior parietal lobule (40) 5.7 1714 44 j64 46
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (46) 5.66 1952 42 36 24
Cerebellum pyramis 5.3 1346 j16 j84 j28
Cerebellum tuber (left) 4.6 392 j52 j66 j32
Orbitofrontal cortex (47)/insula (13) 4.82 526 28 22 j4
Middle frontal gyrus (6) 4.8 486 40 6 54
Inferior temporal gyrus (20) 4.42 108 64 j28 j18
Cerebellum tuber (right) 4.4 786 54 j62 j38
Fusiform gyrus (19) 4.37 426 j40 j68 j14
Insula (13) 3.87 59 j24 20 j2
Cingulate gyrus (32) 3.67 145 8 32 28
IGT 1Y4 9 control (alexithymic group)
Medial frontal gyrus (10) 6.24 1092 40 60 2
Cerebellum 5.9 3009 j36 j66 j36
Precuneus (7) 5.62 532 12 j68 34
Fusiform gyrus (19) 4.87 448 38 j72 j16
Superior parietal lobe (7) 4.81 489 34 j72 44
Caudate 4.3 195 20 18 8
Inferior parietal lobe (40) 4.25 226 52 j50 46
Orbitofrontal cortex (47) 3.92 58 34 18 j8
Middle occipital gyrus (18) 3.78 33 j12 j98 14
Fusiform gyrus (37) 3.76 52 j54 j50 j20
Superior frontal gyrus (8) 3.52 22 28 28 54

T score indicates value for contrast in which rCBF during the IGT trials was greater than rCBF during control tasks. Significance level was set at p G .05, corrected for
false discovery rate for multiple comparisons. Cluster extent was set at 20.
IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; rCBF = regional cerebral blood flow.

from those associated with the IGT at the medial frontal gy- ing, working memory, conflict monitoring, and visual atten-
rus (BA10) across the four IGT conditions between the two tion. Comparing all four IGT tasks to the control tasks, both
groups. In the first and second IGTs, the value was signif- the nonalexithymic and alexithymic groups showed activa-
icantly different between the two groups (p = .004 and tion in the dorsolateral frontal area (BA46 and BA10), OFC
p = .003, respectively). No significant difference was detected (BA47), preYsupplementary motor area (BA6 and BA8), infe-
in the third (p = .06) or fourth (p = .16) IGT. rior parietal lobe (BA40), fusiform gyrus (BA19 and BA37),
and cerebellum, predominantly right lateralized. These areas
DISCUSSION are consistent with previous neuroimaging studies of GTs
The neural substrate engaged in emotion-based decision (18,19,29Y31). The emotional attributes of decision making
making differed in subjects with alexithymia compared with in tasks that involve rewards or risk taking may favor neural
those without alexithymia. In addition, individuals with alex- processing in the right hemisphere (31). Neuropsychological
ithymia showed lower task performance. Activity in the medial and PET studies indicate that IGT processing depends on
frontal areaVwhere patients with lesions here show poor de- working memory functions associated with activity of the
cision makingVwas lower in the alexithymic group during the dorsolateral PFC (15,16,29,32,33). The inferior frontal lobe
first and second IGT trials. The IGT performance was signi- is the lateral part of the orbital area and was commonly acti-
ficantly worse in the alexithymic group than the nonalexithy- vated in previous GT studies (30Y32) involved in flexible rep-
mic group in the fourth IGT trial. resentation of reinforcement of learning from reward and
punishment (30,31). The preYsupplementary motor area is
GT Versus Control Task involved in planning motor responses under internal control
Decision-making tasks are embedded in the processes in complex tasks (34,35), and activation in BA8 and BA6
that precede them (evaluation of stimuli) and those that fol- is associated with uncertainty and decision conflict (35,36).
low (response to outcomes). So, the IGT engages many brain Visual attentionYrelated areas such as the occipital, temporal,
structures associated with emotional and motivational cod- and parietal cortices were often reported to be activated during

Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011) 591

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
M. KANO et al.

Figure 1. Brain images showing greater increase in cerebral blood flow during decision-making tasks in alexithymic and nonalexithymic individuals. Significance
level was set at p G .05, corrected. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; Fusiform = fusiform gyrus; INS = insular cortex; IPL =
inferior parietal lobule; MFG = middle frontal gyrus; OFC = orbitofrontal cortex; PCu = precuneus.

GTs, even if it is expected that mimicking visuomotor control


tasks may cancel the visual processing component of GTs. This
may be because of the fact that salient/arousing aspects of the TABLE 3. Brain Regions Where the Cerebral Blood Flow During
task will have a greater effect on visual attention (31). Although Decision-Making Tasks Negatively Correlated With
Individual TAS-20 Score
cerebellum function refers to movement control, the activities
may relate to error-based learning (37) and could be used to Talairach-Tournoux
improve performance during GTs (32). k Coordinate
In addition, men without alexithymia showed activation Group, Contrast, and Brain T (Cluster
Region (Brodmann Area) Score Extent) X Y Z
in the bilateral insula (BA13), cingulate gyrus (BA32), and
inferior temporal gyrus (BA20), which is the visual processing Cuneus (18) 4.88 930 j14 j70 16
area. The insula is thought to be one of the key regions for Posterior cingulate (30) 4.35 651 22 j56 10
successful IGT performance (38). The right anterior insula Fusiform gyrus (20) 4.15 93 j44 j12 j22
activation is related to explicit awareness of interoceptive sig- Middle temporal gyrus (37) 4.05 195 50 j62 6
nals (31) and may represent negative somatic states, includ- Middle frontal gyrus (10) 4 72 j8 62 0
ing those associated with risk taking and punishment (39). The Precuneus (7) 3.49 44 18 j72 36
left insula is reported to be activated mainly by positive and Superior temporal gyrus (38) 3.75 51 j26 18 j28
affiliative emotional feelings (40). The bilateral anterior insula Superior temporal gyrus (21) 3.66 47 38 2 j30
has been linked to response selection process accompanying Fusiform gyrus (20) 3.47 24 32 j38 j14
sensory perception (40). BA32 may relate to attention and Inferior occipital gyrus (18) 3.47 26 34 j82 j8
mediates response conflict in well-defined tasks where rules Cerebellum 3.4 15 2 j44 j2
are known (36). Meanwhile, individuals with alexithymia
demonstrated that caudate activationVwhich is also thought to T score indicates value for contrast in which rCBF during the IGT trials was
negatively correlated with TAS-20 scores. Significance level was set at p G .001,
be a part of a feedback loop critically involved in motor control, uncorrected. Cluster extent was set at 20.
cognition, and emotional processingVis often activated in GTs IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; rCBF = regional cerebral blood flow; TAS-20 =
(18,29,30,32). 20<item Toronto Alexithymia Scale.

592 Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011)

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRAIN IMAGING OF GAMBLING TASK IN ALEXITHYMIA

Figure 2. Medial prefrontal cortex (BA10) was activated during the IGT trails more in nonalexithymic patients (A) rCBF was higher in BA10 in group comparison
between nonalexithymics and alexithymics in the IGT trials against two control tasks. (B) rCBF in BA10 was negatively correlated with individual TAS-20 scores of
23 subjects.

Comparison Between Subjects With and


Without Alexithymia TABLE 4. Brain Regions With a Significant Difference in Increase in
Cerebral Blood Flow During Decision-Making Tasks Compared With
Performance Data Control Tasks Between Alexithymic and Nonalexithymic Groups
Participants without alexithymia learned a strategy to choose
a good deck and gradually increase their net score, reaching Talairach-Tournoux
k Coordinate
around 10 by the fourth IGT trial. In contrast, those with Group, Contrast, and Brain T (Cluster
alexithymia failed to learn such an advantageous decision- Region (Brodmann Area) Score Extent) X Y Z
making strategy. The net average scores of the two groups
were almost the same during the first and second IGTs, but Nonalexithymic 9 alexithymic groups (IGT 1Y4)
diverged slightly in the third IGT, and showed a significant Inferior temporal gyrus (20) 3.7 55 j60 j38 j14
difference in the fourth IGT. These performance data suggest Medial frontal gyrus (10) 3.44 23 j10 66 6
that the first and second IGTs were probably the learning Alexithymic 9 nonalexithymic
groups (IGT 1Y4)
phases to find a winning strategy and involve a stage of brain
Precuneus (7) 4.03 115 12 j66 32
activity that processes complexity and uncertainty. In contrast,
Lingual gyrus (left) (19) 3.81 28 j22 j64 j6
it is possible that the third IGT, and certainly the fourth IGT,
Lingual gyrus (right) (19) 3.79 49 30 j76 j2
followed a learning period and likely do not involve guessing
Caudate 3.45 23 20 16 6
or searching for the correct strategy, but more likely involve just
Nonalexithymic 9 alexithymic groups (IGT 1)
a visuomotor process. Performance deterioration in latter tri-
Medial frontal gyrus (10) 3.51 44 j12 60 j4
als has often been observed in patient populations such as those
Alexithymic 9 nonalexithymic groups (IGT 1)
with vmPFC damage or a variety of psychiatric conditions
Occipital lobe (7) 4.2 165 10 j66 30
(41). Recently, an interesting article has reported the person-
Caudate 4.05 46 18 18 6
ality characteristics among healthy subjects who persisted in
Occipital lobe (19) 3.73 69 30 j76 j2
selecting from the risky deck B as the number of big losses
Cingulate gyrus (32) 3.61 35 18 14 36
increased. They were characterized as more deliberate, less
Superior temporal gyrus (38) 3.59 31 48 22 j26
self-consciousness, and with lower fantasy and aesthetics rat-
Precentral gyrus (6) 3.53 43 j40 j14 36
ings, but they were implicitly impulsive (42). These charac-
Nonalexithymic 9 alexithymic groups (IGT 2)
teristics reportedly overlap with alexithymia in regard to lower
Medial frontal gyrus (10) 3.37 18 j8 58 6
fantasy rating and external-oriented thinking (3). In addition,
Alexithymic 9 nonalexithymic groups (IGT 2)
Haviland and Reies (43) stated that the emphasized commu-
Precuneus (7) 3.38 26 14 j66 32
nication through action is extremely characteristic of alex-
Alexithymic 9 nonalexithymic groups (IGT 3)
ithymia. This impulsivity or action orientation during emotional
Fusiform gyrus (19) 3.44 26 j24 j66 j4
arousal might be associated with the risky choice and poor per-
Nonalexithymic 9
formance of IGT in individuals with alexithymia.
alexithymic groups (IGT 4)
Precuneus (left) (7) 3.87 67 j16 j68 52
Comparison Between Alexithymia and Nonalexithymia Precentral gyrus (4) 3.86 74 28 j28 56
During IGTs Precuneus (right) (7) 3.75 47 28 j74 24
Compared with men without alexithymia, neural activity in
T score indicates value for contrast in which rCBF during the IGT trials was
the medial frontal cortex (BA10) and inferior temporal gyrus greater than rCBF during control tasks. Significance level was set at p G .001,
(BA20) was lower during the IGT in subjects with alexithymia. uncorrected. Cluster extent was set at 20.
In contrast, activities in the visual processing area (BA7 and IGT = Iowa Gambling Task; rCBF = regional cerebral blood flow.

Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011) 593

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
M. KANO et al.

Figure 3. A, Change in mean difference indicated by net score (advantaged decks [C + D] j disadvantaged decks [A + B]) during the first, second, third, and fourth
IGT trials. Errors bars indicate standard error of the mean. * Significant interaction effect (p = .029) with repeated-measures ANOVA of net scores across 25-choice
time blocks (four levels: 1Y25, 26Y50, 51Y75, and 76Y100) by group (alexithymic and nonalexithymic). B, Brain regions with a significant difference in increase in
cerebral blood flow during the first, second, third, and fourth IGT trials compared with control tasks between nonalexithymic and alexithymic patients. B-1 shows the
brain areas where the rCBF is higher in nonalexithymic than alexithymic patients, and B-2 shows those where the rCBF is higher in alexithymic than nonalexithymic
group. Significance level was set at p G .001, uncorrected. Cluster extent was set at 20. a = medial frontal gyrus; b = medial frontal gyrus; c = no activation area;
d = precuneus; e = caudate, cingulate gyrus, and occipital lobe; f = precuneus; g = fusiform gyrus; h = no activation area. IGT = Iowa Gambling Task.

BA19) and caudate were higher in subjects with alexithymia Recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) stud-
compared with those without. BA10 activity during the IGT ies have provided a functional role for BA10 in GTs. BA10 was
was also negatively correlated with individual TAS-20 score. found to be significantly correlated with task performance (net
The medial frontal cortex (BA10) is part of the key areas score) in risky versus safe decks decisions, suggesting that it is
needed for successful performance on the IGT (13). Damasio’s
classification of vmPFC includes the entirety of the medial
and aspects of the lateral OFC and overlaps with BAs 10, 11, 12
(medial aspects), lower 24, 25, and 32 (33). Observation of
patients with vmPFC lesions led to the original development
of the IGT (13). These patients fail to demonstrate appropriate
affective reactions and judgment, and display poor decision
making in everyday life. The role of the vmPFC in IGT pro-
cessing is to use emotion-based biasing signals generated from
the body (or somatic markers). The role of the medial frontal
cortex in the GT is thought to be the integration of reward-
related autonomic input. Studies of neurological patients sug-
gest that lesions in both the ventral and/or dorsal medial PFC Figure 4. Average eigenvariate values associated with visuomotor control con-
ditions from that associated with the IGT at the medial frontal gyrus (BA10) across
result in fairly specific neuropsychological impairments that the four IGT conditions between two groups. Error bars indicate standard errors.
would affect IGT performance (16,38). Significant difference from zero: * p G .005.

594 Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011)

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRAIN IMAGING OF GAMBLING TASK IN ALEXITHYMIA

crucial for successful task performance (18). Neural activity in performance in the learning phase, although the performance
the vmPFC during affective judgment of emotional pictures was results look the same. Particularly, the function of caudate is
shown to be positively related to IGT performance, whereas the to regulate or control impulsivity and disinhibition (48), and the
amount of neuronal activity in the vmPFC seems to determine activity of it during decision making has been suggested to be
our ability to make affective judgments, which in turn influences a maker of risk attitude (49). Individuals with alexithymia may
our decision-making performance (44). Furthermore, activation work on IGT impulsively rather than using emotional-based
in this region may be associated with the development of an- signal. In the third and fourth IGT trials, after the learning
ticipatory arousal during risky decisions (31). The somatic phase, there were no significant differences in the groups in
marker hypothesis suggests that the feedback of arousal, in the medial frontal areas, and differences were only observed
addition to generating feeling states, may bias social behavior in the visual processing areas. This may be because the trick
and decision making (20). for advantageous decision making in IGT is clear for indi-
Another account of the role of medial BA10 in general viduals without alexithymia, so they no longer need to use
cognition suggests that it might be specialized for the explicit medial frontal area function during the third and fourth IGT
processing of internal mental states or the introspective eval- trials. Tasks demanding neural activity in this period might
uation of one’s own thoughts and feelings (45). The anterior be less intense compared with what is required in the learn-
PFC (medial BA10) is also suggested to be involved in epi- ing phase for the nonalexithymic group, so there is no dif-
sodic memory retrieval (45), especially in task structure re- ference in the core brain areas related to IGT performance.
quiring the scheduled retrieval of multiple elements of a past However, differences were observed in the visual processing
episode (46). In decision making, the anterior PFC is specif- areas in the fourth IGT task. Each group had almost made their
ically engaged when subjects suspend the execution of an decision during the learning phase (A or B for the alexithy-
ongoing task associated with a priori largest future rewards to mic group and C or D for the nonalexithymic group), such that
explore a potentially more rewarding task (47). The function the behavioral load might be equivalent. Consequently, the
may be an evolutionary adaptation to overcome the limita- imaging results across groups did not differ substantially in the
tions of more posterior prefrontal processes by enabling the fourth trial.
emergence of more flexible cognitive control of decision
making (48).
In the context of IGT-related processing, BA10 activity Alexithymia Trait
may be associated with the use of internal signals accompa- GT impairment accompanying altered brain activities was
nying affective evaluation of the stimuli, which is crucial for observed in association with personality traits, indicating that
successful decision making. Compared with nonalexithymic alexithymia is an independent clinical condition.
subjects, the BA10 area in those with alexithymia did not The behavioral form of the IGT has been shown to be a
function during the IGT, especially in the first and second highly sensitive measure of impaired decision making in a va-
IGT trials, and even by the last IGT trial these subjects failed riety of neurological and psychiatric conditions characterized
to perform the task successfully. The difference of BA10 ac- by real world decision-making impairments, such as focal brain
tivity in the learning phase indicates that subjects with alex- damage, addiction, obsessive compulsive disorder, pathologi-
ithymia could not use brain function related to emotion-based cal gambling, psychosis, bipolar disorder, attention deficit
biasing signals, which are described as somatic markers by hyperactivity disorder, eating disorder, and chronic pain (50).
Damasio (20), in the learning phase of IGTs in which the sig- Although around 20% of healthy adults perform disadvanta-
nal should be most useful as an indicator leading to advanta- geously on the IGT, those that do so may in fact score high in
geous decision making in situations of uncertainty. In a more ‘‘cognitive disinhibition,’’ defined as having a personality style
general cognitive aspect, subjects with alexithymia may not that leads to ‘‘myopia for the future,’’ which would explain their
be able to suspend persistent selection from risky decks char- pattern of IGT performance (51).
acterized by large immediate rewards but larger long-term Concerning the overlapping characteristics between alex-
punishments because of the inability to access internal men- ithymia and patients with vmPFC who remain unable to de-
tal states (own thought or feeling). Neither might they be able velop affective reactions with appropriate affective judgment
to learn based on past episodes of loss. Consequently, they (6Y8,14Y16), the results of the present study may indicate that
may lose flexible cognitive control of decision making and end emotion plays a necessary role in achieving successful perfor-
up failing to choose the option of the largest expected future mance on the IGT even in nonclinical populations. In addition
rewards. to personality characteristics such as cognitive disinhibition
In contrast, men with alexithymia showed more brain ac- (48) or impulsiveness (42), which have been suggested to im-
tivity in the caudate, cingulate gyrus (BA32), precentral gyrus, pact on IGT performance, emotional dysregulation may un-
and occipital areas during the first IGT trial. These areas are derlie the impairment seen on the IGT. Emotion-based biasing
associated with motor control, attention, and visual processing signals may promote or inhibit impulsiveness during the IGT.
rather than emotion-based signal processing in the vmPFC From the point of view of cognitive-developmental model
or insula during the IGT. Therefore, it is suggested that indi- of emotional awareness (52), the problem of alexithymia has
viduals with alexithymia use different strategies during IGT been posited as deficient development in the cognitive mature

Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011) 595

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
M. KANO et al.

stage, which allows some rudimentary form of emotional REFERENCES


experiences like awareness of bodily sensations or action ten- 1. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristic mechanisms in
dency (52). We have demonstrated that subjects with alex- psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom 1973;21:133Y6.
ithymia were less aware of affective stimuli at the cognitive 2. Taylor GJ. Recent developments in alexithymia theory and research. Can J
Psychiatry 2000;45:134Y42.
level but highly aroused by affective stimuli at the physiolog- 3. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. Disorder of Affect Regulation: Alex-
ical level in our previous studies (10,53). The present study ithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. New York: Cambridge Uni-
provides further support for this phenomenon in alexithy- versity Press; 1997:29Y30.
4. Mattila AK, Poutanen O, Koivisto AM, Salokangas RK, Joukamaa M.
mia. Reduced BA10 activity, which is the neural substrate to Alexithymia and life satisfaction in primary healthcare patients. Psycho-
appreciate complex emotion and enable flexible cognitive re- somatics 2007;48:523Y9.
gulation, and greater activity in the caudate associated with 5. Kauhanen J, Kaplan GA, Cohen RD, Julkunen J, Salonen JT. Alexithymia
and risk of death in middle-aged men. J Psychosom Res 1996;41:541Y9.
impulsiveness or action orientation may indicate lack of the 6. Lane RD, Sechrest L, Reidel R, Weldon V, Kaszniak A, Schwartz GE.
function emotionally matured and the rudimentary form of Impaired verbal and nonverbal emotion recognition in alexithymia.
emotional response in individuals with alexithymia. With re- Psychosom Med 1996;58:203Y10.
7. Lane RD, Sechrest L, Riedel R, Shapiro DE, Kaszniak AW. Pervasive
gard to the relationship between emotions and physical dis- emotion recognition deficit common to alexithymia and the repressive
eases, these neural substrates in individuals with alexithymia coping style. Psychosom Med 2000;62:492Y501.
are inconsistent with the unifying framework by Lane (52). 8. Luminet O, Vermeulen N, Demaret C, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Alexithymia
and levels of processing: evidence for an overall deficit in remembering
emotion words. J Res Pers 2006;40:713Y33.
Limitations 9. Berthoz S, Artiges E, Van de Moortele PF, Poline JB, Rouquette S, Consoli
SM, Martinot JL. Effect of impaired recognition and expression of emo-
The current study recruited only men because of reported tions on frontocingulate cortices: an fMRI study of men with alexithymia.
sex differences in IGT performance and brain activation Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:961Y7.
(18,48). Further studies should examine whether neural activity 10. Kano M, Fukudo S, Gyoba J, Kamachi M, Tagawa M, Mochizuki H, Itoh M,
Hongo M, Yanai K. Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people
related to alexithymia is different in women. Although the with alexithymia: an (H2O)-O-15-PET study. Brain 2003;126:1474Y84.
results indicate significant differences in men with alexithy- 11. Mantani T, Okamoto Y, Shirao N, Okada G, Yamawaki S. Reduced acti-
mia during IGT performance, the number of subjects in this vation of posterior cingulate cortex during imagery in subjects with high
degrees of alexithymia: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
study was relatively small. This might limit the power to ob- Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:982Y90.
serve differences between groups and type II error remains a 12. Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, Matsuda
possibility. Given the unbalanced sample size of 13 in the H, Komaki G. Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alex-
ithymia. Cerebral Cortex 2007;17:2223Y34.
nonalexithymic group and 10 in the alexithymic group, it 13. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future
cannot be ruled out that the significant difference between the consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition
groups was caused by a number effect. Furthermore, because 1994;50:7Y15.
14. Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, London ED, Kiehl KA, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi
of the limited temporal and spatial resolution of PET meth- C, Matochik JA, Kurian V, Cadet JL, Kimes AS, Funderburk FR, Ernst M.
odology compared with fMRI, we were not able to clarify re- Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a
gional activation associated with temporally distinct elements decision-making task. Neuroimage 2003;19:1085Y94. Accessed March 1,
2003.
of IGT performance, such as responses during risky versus 15. Clark L, Manes F, Nagui A, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. The contributions
advantageous decisions. In future studies, fMRI studies with of lesion laterality and lesion volume to decision-making impairment fol-
large numbers of subjects are expected to reveal further de- lowing frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia 2003;41:1474Y83.
tails of alexithymic features during IGT performance. 16. Manes F, Sahakian B, Clark L, Rogers R, Antoun N, Aitken M, Robbins T.
Decision-making processes following damage to the prefrontal cortex.
Brain 2002;125:624Y39.
17. Fukui H, Murai T, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Hanakawa T. Functional ac-
CONCLUSIONS tivity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa Gambling
In this study, we examined performance and neural sub- Task. Neuroimage 2005;24:253Y9.
strates for an emotion-based decision-making task in alex- 18. Lawrence NS, Jollant F, O’Daly O, Zelaya F, Phillips ML. Distinct roles of
prefrontal cortical subregions in the Iowa Gambling Task. Cerebral Cortex
ithymia. Subjects with alexithymia showed lower BA10 activity 2009;19:1134Y43.
during the uncertain learning phase and failed to achieve suc- 19. Li XR, Lu ZL, D’Argembeau A, Ng M, Becharal A. The Iowa Gambling
cessful IGT performance compared with subjects without Task in fMRI images. Hum Brain Mapp 2010;31:410Y23.
20. Damásio AR. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain.
alexithymia. These results suggest that they might not use an New York: Putnam; 1994.
emotion-based biasing signal, which should be the most use- 21. Komaki G, Maeda M, Arimura T, Nakata A, Shinoda H, Ogata I, Shimura
ful indicator leading to advantageous decision making. This M, Kawamura N, Kubo C. The reliability and factorial validity of the
Japanese version of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. J Psychosom
study suggests that people with alexithymia may be unable to Res 2003;43:839Y46.
use feelings to guide their behavior appropriately in addition to 22. Speilberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. STAI Manual. Palo Alto, CA:
being unable to recognize emotions, which supports previous Consulting Psychologist Press; 1979.
23. Leising D, Grande T, Faber R. The Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20): a
findings. measure of general psychological distress. J Res Pers 2009;43:707Y10.
24. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders, 4th edition text revision. Arlington, VA: American Psy-
We would like to thank Hiroyuki Sasaki for programming the IGT, chiatric Publishing; 2000.
and M. Miyake, Y. Ishikawa, and S. Watanuki for technical assistance 25. International Classification of Diseases. Available at: http://apps.who.int/
with the PET imaging studies. classifications/apps/icd/icd10online/.

596 Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011)

Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRAIN IMAGING OF GAMBLING TASK IN ALEXITHYMIA
26. Fujiwara T, Watanuki S, Yamamoto S, Miyake M, Seo S, Itoh M, Ishii K, activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related
Orihara H, Fukuda H, Satoh T, Kitamura K, Tanaka K, Takahashi S. Per- to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage 2003;19:1439Y48.
formance evaluation of a large axial field-of-view PET scanner: SET- 40. Craig AD. How do you feelVnow? The anterior insula and human
2400W. Ann Nucl Med 1997;11:307Y13. awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;1:59Y70.
27. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Poline JB, Frith CD, Worsley KJ, Frackowiak RSJ. 41. Buelow MT, Suhr JA. Construct validity of the Iowa Gambling Task.
Statistical maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Neuropsychol Rev 2009;19:102Y14.
Brain Mapp 1994;2:189Y210. 42. Takano Y, Takahashi N, Tanaka D, Hironaka N. Big losses lead to irrational
28. Price CJ, Friston KJ. Cognitive conjunction: a new approach to brain ac- decision-making in gambling situations: relationship between deliberation
tivation experiments. Neuroimage 1997;5:261Y70. and impulsivity. PLoS One 2010;5:2.
29. Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Williams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian 43. Haviland MG, Reise SP. A California Q-set alexithymia prototype and
BJ, Robbins TW. Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, un- its relationship to ego-control and ego-resiliency. J Psychosom Res 1996;
likely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 41:597Y608.
1999;19:9029Y38. 44. Northoff G, Grimm S, Boeker H, Schmidt C, Bermpohl F, Heinzel A, Hell
30. Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Mackay C, Wilson JL, Jezzard P, Carter CS, Smith D, Boesiger P. Affective judgment and beneficial decision making: ven-
SM. Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal tromedial prefrontal activity correlates with performance in the Iowa
cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision- Gambling Task. Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27:572Y87.
making cognition. Biol Psychiatry 2004;55:594Y602. 45. Ramnani N, Owen AM. Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function
31. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5:184Y94.
supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:189Y95. 46. Koechlin E, Hyafil A. Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human
32. Ernst M, Grant SJ, London ED, Contoreggi CS, Kimes AS, Spurgeon L. decision-making. Science 2007;318:594Y8.
Decision making in adolescents with behavior disorders and adults with 47. Daw ND, O’Doherty JP, Dayan P, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Cortical substrates
substance abuse. Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:33Y40. for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature 2006;441:876Y9.
33. Bechara A, Martin EM. Impaired decision making related to working 48. Newcombe VF, Outtrim JG, Chatfield DA, Manktelow A, Hutchinson PJ,
memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychol- Coles JP, Williams GB, Sahakian BJ, Menon DK. Parcellating the neuro-
ogy 2004;18:152Y62. anatomical basis of impaired decision-making in traumatic brain injury.
34. Vorobiev V, Govoni P, Rizzolatti G, Matelli M, Luppino G. Parcellation of Brain 2011;134:759Y68.
human mesial area 6: cytoarchitectonic evidence for three separate areas. 49. Hinvest NS, Elliott R, McKie S, Anderson IM. Neural correlates of choice
Eur J Neurosci 1998;10:2199Y203. behavior related to impulsivity and venturesomeness [published online
35. Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY. Error monitoring using external feedback: ahead of print February 18, 2011]. Neuropsychologia 2011.
specific roles of the habenular complex, the reward system, and the cin- 50. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD. The somatic marker hypothesis: a
gulate motor area revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:239Y71.
J Neurosci 2003;23:4308Y14. 51. Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL. Sex-related differences
36. Volz KG, Schubotz RI, von Cramon DY. Variants of uncertainty in decision- in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cerebral Cortex
making and their neural correlates. Brain Res Bull 2005;67:403Y12. 2004;14:1226Y32.
37. Doya K. Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning 52. Lane RD. Neural substrates of implicit and explicit emotional processes:
and motor control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2000;10:732Y9. a unifying framework for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med
38. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW. Dissociation of working 2008;70:214Y31.
memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J 53. Kano M, Hamaguchi T, Itoh M, Yanai K, Fukudo S. Correlation between
Neurosci 1998;18:428Y37. alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain
39. Paulus MP, Rogalsky C, Simmons A, Feinstein JS, Stein MB. Increased 2007;132:252Y63.

Psychosomatic Medicine 73:588Y597 (2011) 597

View publication stats Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychosomatic Society. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like