You are on page 1of 5

Disorder Versus Disability:

The Challenge of ADHD in


the Context of a High IQ
Kevin M. Antshel, Ph.D., Kaitlin Hendricks, Stephen V. Faraone, Ph.D,
and Michael Gordon, Ph.D.

Most research on ADHD has focused are misinterpreted by teachers or care- “If this child were referred to you by
on individuals with average intel- givers as hyperactivity. his teacher for evaluation, what do you
ligence. Considerably less is known Similarly, Webb and Latimer (1993) think the underlying explanation for his
about ADHD in populations at either assert that inattention or inability to stay behavior would be?” Form B asked, “If
end of the IQ spectrum. Indeed, wheth- on task in children with a high IQ may this child were referred to you by his
er ADHD should even be considered in be the result of boredom rather than teacher for evaluation, do you think the
individuals with low IQ has generated an underlying impairment. They claim cause of his behavior could be attrib-
much debate (Antshel et al., 2006) and that off-task behavior in these children uted to Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
research (Fee, Matson & Benavidez, may be the result of their attempts at Disorder (ADHD) or due to his being
1994; Pearson & Aman, 1994; Pearson self-amusement due to poorly matched gifted and talented (G/T)?”
et al., 2000). Similar questions have curriculum or boredom associated with Results indicated that participants
been raised about how ADHD might waiting for their classmates to catch up. given form B were significantly more
be manifested differently in high IQ The majority of critics against the valid- likely to diagnose giftedness, other, or
populations. For example, some have ity of ADHD in the high IQ population both giftedness and ADHD than partic-
expressed concerns that individuals argue that large symptom overlap leads ipants given form A. Forty-six percent
with superior intellectual abilities are to misinterpretation about the cause of of participants given form B diagnosed
vulnerable to being overdiagnosed as the behavior by health care profession- the child with giftedness (14%) or both
having ADHD (Baum, Olenchak, & als, teachers, and parents. These critics (32%). None of the participants given
Owens, 1998; Gallagher & Harradine, argue that on the surface, the behavior form A suggested giftedness or both as
1997; Hartnett, Nelson, & Rinn, 2004; of individuals with a high IQ may look a diagnosis. The researchers concluded
Leroux & Levitt-Perlman, 2000; Mika, very much like ADHD and may lead to that there is a possibility that counselor
2006; Webb & Latimer, 1993). In this ar- a misdiagnosis. training programs do not clearly de-
ticle, we review current debates and at- Hartnett, Nelson, and Rinn (2004) ex- lineate the differences between ADHD
tendant (albeit scant) research related to amined the possibility of misdiagnosis and high IQ. The authors suggested
ADHD diagnosis and high IQ. of ADHD in high IQ individuals. Spe- that without the suggestion of gifted-
Much of the controversy regarding cifically, they investigated suggestion ness, participants were unaware of the
the interplay between superior cog- effects in the diagnosis of ADHD versus similarities in behavior between ADHD
nitive ability and ADHD focuses on high IQ in children. Participants were 44 and giftedness.
the potential for bright children to be graduate students enrolled in a school- Baum, Olechak, and Owen (1998)
misdiagnosed with having the disor- counseling program. Participants were more specifically addressed the mis-
der. Many who warn about the over- randomly assigned to one of two condi- diagnosis and over-identification of
diagnosis of ADHD in children with a tions. Two case vignettes were used in ADHD in children with a high IQ. They
high IQ cite Dabrowski’s (1966) theory this study, form A and form B. Both case first asserted that there has been an in-
of positive disintegration. Dabrowski’s vignettes had identical descriptions of a crease in the number of children with
theory asserts that children with a high 7-year-old boy. The passages were con- high IQs diagnosed with ADHD, and
IQ have “overexcitabilities” in five structed based on previous research ex- they outlined several alternative expla-
general areas: psychomotor, sensual, amining the similarity between ADHD nations as to why those children may
intellectual, imagination, and emotion- symptoms and high IQ (Webb & Latim- struggle with inattention and hyperac-
al. Psychomotor “overexcitabilities” er, 1993). Both forms required the par- tivity in a traditional education setting.
may be rapid speech, excessive move- ticipants to read the description and The authors opined that children with
ment, fidgeting, or impulsive actions. identify the cause of the child’s behav- a high IQ possess “psychomotor over-
Hartnett, Nelson, and Rinn (2004) ar- ior. However, the vignettes differed in excitabilities” (Piechowski, 1986) that
gue that these behaviors, which may the amount of diagnostic suggestion be- may be the result of either boredom or
be common in children with a high IQ, ing made. For example, form A asked, simply excitement about new ideas. Ac-

4 • The ADHD Report © 2011 The Guilford Press


cording to the authors, these “psycho- 2000; Webb & Latimer, 1993). They also larly suggests that children with ADHD
motor overexcitabilities” are the result provide some evidence of suggestion have an IQ about 9 points lower than
of an overly restrictive classroom setting bias in clinicians who may be working typically developing peers (Frazier, De-
that is stifling to the high IQ child’s cu- with these children (Hartnett, Nelson, maree, & Youngstrom, 2004). Likewise,
riosity. A second possible explanation is & Rinn, 2004). while there is an association (r = −0.3)
that children with a high IQ found their Mika (2006) addressed the issue of between ADHD symptoms and IQ in
classes too boring and routine. Research misdiagnosis of ADHD in children the general population (Goodman, Si-
has demonstrated that many high IQ with a high IQ by directly responding monoff, & Stevenson, 1995; Rapport,
students are being taught below their to the article by Hartnett and colleagues Scanlan, & Denney, 1999), this relation-
instructional level and that much of the (2004). Mika argued that the Hartnett ship is modest at best. Thus, not all (or
curriculum is redundant (Gallagher, study on suggestion bias was interpret- even a majority of) individuals with a
1990; Stanley, 1978). This may lead chil- ed incorrectly. Mika asserts that the case high IQ will demonstrate ADHD symp-
dren with a high IQ to act out due to vignette provided no evidence of high toms.
boredom or sensation seeking. IQ; however, after being given the op- From our perspective, much of the
Gardener’s theory of multiple in- tion of giftedness as a cause, significant- debate about high IQ and ADHD has
telligences is also cited as an explana- ly more counseling students selected it unfolded because it has focused on
tion for inattention and hyperactive as a possible diagnosis. Mika also con- the potential symptom overlap, not on
behavior in children with a high IQ tends that the case vignette described whether those symptoms cause impair-
(Baum, Olenchak, & Owen, 1998). Gar- many behaviors that would support ment. As we and others have demon-
dener’s theory states that intelligence ADHD as a possibility, such as hyperac- strated (Gordon et al., 2006), having the
is grouped into eight domains: verbal, tivity, poor impulse control, inattentive- symptoms of this disorder is not tanta-
logical-mathematical, spatial, kinesthet- ness, careless, and noncompliance. mount to having impairment because
ic, musical, naturalistic, interpersonal, Mika (2006) also directly addresses of those symptoms. Therefore, the IQ
and intrapersonal. Many high IQ stu- Dabrowski’s notion of “overexcitabili- factor is irrelevant unless it can be dem-
dents are often low achieving and inat- ties” and that the idea of overexcit- onstrated that having a high IQ some-
tentive in school. The authors attribute abilities has been misinterpreted. Mika how impacts the onset or staging of the
this low achievement and inattention argued that severe psychomotor over- functional impairment required by the
to a discrepancy between the students’ excitability and hyperactivity associat- diagnosis. Put another way, if a bright
strengths and the intelligences being ed with ADHD are descriptively identi- child with ADHD symptoms is 1) more
assessed in school. For example, the in- cal; there is no clear distinction between impaired than other bright children and
telligences being assessed in school are these two behaviors. Additionally, as a 2) as impaired as children with typi-
primarily verbal and logical-mathemat- single indicator, intense psychomotor cal IQs, controversies about ADHD in
ical, whereas the students’ strengths overexcitability has never been an indi- highly intelligent individuals become
may lie in the spatial and kinesthetic cation of high IQ. Mika contended that largely moot.
domains. This mismatch causes the Dabrowski implies that psychomotor To supplant conjecture and opin-
child to be inattentive and unsuccessful overexcitability must occur in conjunc- ion with data, the first author and col-
in a classroom setting. However, when tion with other excitabilties (e.g., imagi- leagues have addressed these questions
engaged in tasks in an area of strength, nation) in order to contribute to positive with a series of studies (Antshel et al.,
these children may perform exception- personality development. 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010). For example, in
ally with virtually no behavior prob- Despite these theoretical consider- one project, (Antshel et al., 2007), we
lems or inattention. Additionally, the ations, no solid empirical evidence has examined the validity of an ADHD di-
adults’ inability to deal with precocious been present to support any of these agnosis in the pediatric high IQ popu-
children may be a possible explanation contentions. The only data showing lation. We hypothesized that 1) high
for ADHD-like behaviors in those with that ADHD is not overdiagnosed are IQ children with ADHD would have
a high IQ. The authors argue that adults inferential. It comes from the notion greater levels of functional impair-
may be overwhelmed or intimidated that, were ADHD commonly over- ment compared with high IQ controls;
by an extremely intelligent child and diagnosed in individuals with high IQ, 2) high IQ children with ADHD would
will therefore not be able to control that studies would find higher IQs in the have similar impairments with aver-
child’s behavior. ADHD population compared with the age IQ children with ADHD; 3) high IQ
Overall, skeptics of the validity of an non-ADHD population. In fact, the op- children with ADHD would have more
ADHD diagnosis in the high IQ popu- posite is true. A meta-analysis compar- first degree relatives with ADHD than
lation focus on the potential symptom ing 1,021 ADHD adults with 928 non- high IQ children without ADHD; and,
overlap between high IQ and ADHD ADHD adults found lower IQs among 4) the rate of first-degree relatives with
(Baum et al., 1998; Gallagher & Harra- those with ADHD (Bridgett & Walker, ADHD would be comparable between
dine, 1997; Leroux & Levitt-Perlman, 2006). A pediatric meta-analysis simi- high IQ children with ADHD and chil-

© 2011 The Guilford Press The ADHD Report • 5


dren with average IQ and ADHD. Data arguments supporting the validity of Clinical Implications
from the Massachusetts General Hospi- ADHD in high IQ children. These findings have an array of impli-
tal (MGH) Longitudinal Family Stud- In subsequent studies on this topic, cations for diagnosis and, by extension,
ies of ADHD was used in this study. In we demonstrated the temporal stabil- the granting of legal accommodations
addition, controls were selected from ity of ADHD and associated functional based on the diagnosis of ADHD. As
pediatric practices in the greater Bos- impairments in the high IQ population; for clinical diagnosis, we cannot find a
ton area. High IQ was defined as an IQ seventy-eight percent of children in the justification for considering a high IQ
greater than 120. high IQ ADHD group maintained their as especially germane. Bright individu-
Participants included 92 children ADHD diagnosis after 4½ years (Antshel als can be just as impaired by ADHD
with high IQ who did not meet the cri- et al., 2008). Using data from the MGH symptoms as those with average IQs. Of
teria for ADHD and 49 children with family study of ADHD, we showed that course, it is also possible that a highly
high IQ who met the criteria for ADHD. adults with ADHD and a high IQ share intelligent individual can be energetic
Participants were given the Block De- a similar profile of functional impair- and excitable without necessarily quali-
sign and Vocabulary subtests from the ments in diverse domains such as fam- fying as having ADHD. The correlation
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children ily, occupational, and social domains. In between having the symptoms and be-
– Revised (WISC–R) or the Wechsler addition, adults with ADHD and a high ing impaired by them, whether in high
Intelligence Scale for Children – Third IQ had more comorbid mood and anxi- IQ populations or normal ones, is sim-
Edition (WISC–III) to estimate cognitive ety disorders than their high IQ control ply too modest to be of import.
ability. They were also given the Wide counterparts. Higher levels of familial
As for testing accommodations, the
Range Achievement Test – Revised ADHD were also found in the first-
IQ/ADHD link, or lack therefore, has
edition (WRAT–R) to assess academic degree relatives of high-IQ adults with
direct implications. To qualify for aca-
achievement and the Social Adjustment ADHD (28%) relative to high IQ con-
demic accommodations such as extend-
Inventory for Children and Adolescents trols (5%) (Antshel et al., 2009). Adults ed time on tests, a high school, college
(SAICA) to assess global functioning. with ADHD and a high IQ performed ,or graduate student must document
Parents completed the Child Behavior less well on multiple tests of executive a disabling condition. In the United
Checklist (CBCL) as a parent report functioning relative to high IQ control States, the Americans with Disabilities
measure of psychopathology. participants. Nonetheless, mean perfor- Act of 1990 (ADA), including changes
We examined group differences in mance of high IQ adults with ADHD made by the ADA Amendments Act of
academic achievement and global was average on executive functioning 2008 (P.L. 110-325), includes ADHD as
functioning. Results revealed support measures (Antshel et al., 2010). a disabling condition. Thus, extended
for all four hypotheses. Compared to Taken as a whole, these findings sug- time on examinations like the SAT rea-
controls, high IQ children with ADHD gest that high IQ is not a relevant diag- soning test (formerly the Scholastic Ap-
exhibited significantly more functional nostic consideration. Superior cognitive titude Test), the Law School Admission
impairments in social, academic, and ability seems not to protect a highly im- Test (LSAT) or Medical College Admis-
behavioral domains. High IQ children pulsive individual from the full range sion Test may be granted to individuals
with ADHD were also more likely to of impairments that commonly flow with ADHD. Most high schools, testing
agencies, and colleges/universities re-
repeat grades and require academic from being highly impulsive. It there-
quire psychological testing to support
support than controls. Also, relative fore appears that being smart may not
the validity of an ADHD diagnosis and
to high IQ controls, children with a meaningfully forestall or overcome the
the need for academic accommoda-
high IQ with ADHD had much higher symptoms of clinically significant im-
tions.
rates of mood, anxiety, and disruptive pairment in impulse control and atten-
behavior disorders. Additionally, chil- tion. High IQ children and adults with By definition, most individuals en-
dren with a high IQ with ADHD had a diagnosis of ADHD display more tering postsecondary education have
significantly more first-degree relatives functional and academic impairments, managed at least average academic
with ADHD (22.9% versus 5.6% for con- higher rates of comorbid psychiatric functioning (Gordon & Kaiser, 1998). In
trols). The rate of first-degree relatives disorders, and higher rates of familial other words, it is hard to argue that an
with ADHD was similar in both groups ADHD than high IQ controls (Antshel individual is significantly impaired if
(high IQ, average IQ) of children with et al., 2007, 2008, 2009, 2010). Further- he or she has performed well academi-
ADHD. We interpreted these data as more, worries that bright children can cally under relatively normal circum-
providing some support for the valid- easily be overdiagnosed as ADHD are stances and has no history of receiving
ity of ADHD in the high IQ population. unfounded as long as the clinical focus academic accommodations. In the face
of this reality, some try to justify a di-
The clear evidence of impairment in the is on impairment and not simply the ex-
agnosis and/or accommodations by ar-
ADHD-high IQ cohort, when compared tent to which an individual presents as
guing that students with high IQ, while
with controls, and the familiality of the energetic and active.
not impaired in the absolute sense (that
ADHD, we believed, were the strongest
is, compared to most individuals), are

6 • The ADHD Report © 2011 The Guilford Press


impaired relative to their intellectual term. The case of ADHD in the context for the trees? Gifted Child Quarterly, 42(2),
potential. As some have argued (Gor- of a high IQ appears to be an example 96-104.
don, Lewandowski, & Kaiser, 1999) of how it may be possible to have a Biederman, J. (2005). Attention-Deficit/Hy-
relativistic diagnoses are hard to justify, disorder that would warrant treatment peractivity Disorder: A Selective Overview.
especially given the extent to which IQ by a health professional, yet not have Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1215-1220.
accounts for a relatively small percent a legally defined disability. Further ef- Bridgett, D.J., & Walker, M.E. (2006). Intel-
of the variance in predicting outcome. forts, both research and clinical, should lectual functioning in adults with ADHD:
In fact, socioeconomic status comes in a attempt to demarcate the line between A meta-analytic examination of full scale
close second to IQ in terms of predicting disorder and disability. This type of IQ differences between adults with and
“potential.” Few would argue that indi- clarity may help guide practitioners without ADHD. Psychological Assessment,
18, 1-14.
viduals should be considered disabled who evaluate and treat individuals with
if they performed below what might be a high IQ and ADHD. Busch, B., Biederman, J., Cohen, L.G., Sayer,
expected from their social class. J.M., Monuteaux, M.C., Mich, E., Zallen, B.,
& Faraone, S.V. (2002). Correlates of ADHD
The authors are affiliated with the State among children in pediatric and psychiatric
Neither the original nor the amended
University of New York–Upstate Medical clinics. Psychiatric Services, 53, 1103-1111.
versions of the Americans with Dis-
University, Department of Psychiatry and
abilities Act allow for an individual to Dabrowski, K. (1966). The theory of posi-
Behavioral Sciences in Syracuse. Drs. Ant- tive disintegration. International Journal of
be qualified as disabled if the worst
shel and Gordon are also on the Advisory Psychiatry, 2(2), 229-243.
functioning still falls within a normal
Board of The ADHD Report. Dr. Antshel
range. The law was designed to reduce DuPaul, G., Schaughency, E., Weyandt, L.,
can be reached via email at antshelk@up- Tripp, G., Kiesner, J., Ota, K., et al. (2001).
discrimination against individuals who
state.edu. Self-report of ADHD symptoms in universi-
were substantially impaired in a ma-
ty students: Cross-gender and cross-nation-
jor life activity. While the amendment
al prevalence. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
to the ADA was designed to make the References
34(4), 370-379.
definition of disability somewhat more American Psychiatric Association. (1994).
Faraone, S. (2005). The scientific founda-
liberal, it is clear in maintaining the Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
tion for understanding attention-deficit/
“average person standard” as the met- disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
hyperactivity disorder as a valid psychiatric
ric for judging impairment. Therefore, Antshel, K., Faraone, S., Stallone, K., Nave, disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psy-
arguing that well-functioning individu- A., Kaufmann, F., Doyle, A., et al. (2007). Is chiatry, 14(1), 1-10.
als should receive accommodations attention deficit hyperactivity disorder a
Fee, V.E., Matson, J.L., & Benavidez, D.A.
because they might help them perform valid diagnosis in the presence of high IQ?
(1994). Attention deficit-hyperactivity
even better might well be viewed as Results from the MGH Longitudinal Family
disorder among mentally retarded children.
unconvincing by many testing organi- Studies of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15,
zations (Gordon, 2009). While these or- and Psychiatry, 48(7), 687-694.
67-79.
ganizations are quick to help those with Antshel, K., Faraone, S., Maglione, K.,
Frazier T.W., Demaree, H.A., Youngstrom,
real abnormalities, they are often slow Doyle, A., Fried, R., Seidman, L., et al.
E.A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual
to offer advantages to individuals who (2008). Temporal stability of ADHD in the
and neuropsychological test performance
high-IQ population: Results from the MGH
are already advantaged. in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
longitudinal family studies of ADHD.
Neuropsychology, 18, 543-555.
Another argument put forth by some Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(7), 817-825. Gallagher, J. (1990). The public and profes-
advocating for accommodations in high sional perception of the emotional status
functioning students is that ADHD Antshel, K.M., Faraone, S.V., Maglione, K.,
of children with a high IQ. Journal for the
takes on a different, less impairing form Doyle, A., Fried, R., Seidman, L., & Bieder-
Education of the Gifted, 13(3), 202-211.
man, J. (2010). Executive functioning in
among those who are bright. Our data Gallagher, J., & Harradine, C. (1997). Gifted
high-IQ adults with ADHD. Psychological
do not support this position. We have students in the classroom. Roeper Review, 19,
Medicine, 40, 1909-1918.
demonstrated that bright individuals 132-136.
Antshel, K.M., Faraone, S.V., Maglione, K.,
with ADHD show the same array of Goodman, R., Simonoff, E., & Stevenson,
Doyle, A., Fried, R., Seidman, L., & Bieder-
limitations as those with average IQs. J. (1995). The impact of child IQ, parent IQ
man, J. (2009). Is adult attention deficit hy-
Again, what might qualify a person peractivity disorder a valid diagnosis in the and sibling IQ on child behavioural devi-
with a high IQ for a diagnosis of ADHD presence of high IQ? Psychological Medicine, ance scores. Journal of Child Psychiatry and
and for legal accommodations should 39, 1325-1335. Psychology, 36, 409-425.
have little to do with IQ and much to do Antshel, K.M., Phillips, M.H., Gordon, M., Gordon, M. (2009). ADHD on trial: Court-
with the extent of impairment relative Barkley, R.A., & Faraone, S.V. (2006). Is room clashes over the meaning of “disability”.
to most people. ADHD a valid disorder in children with in- Westport, CT: Greenwood.
tellectual delays? Clinical Psychology Review, Gordon, M., Antshel, K., Faraone, S.,
As a field, we need to better under- 26, 555-572. Barkley, R., Lewandowski, L., Hudziak, J.J.,
stand the relationship between a psy- Biederman, J., & Cunningham, C. (2006).
Baum, S., Olenchak, F., & Owen, S. (1998).
chiatric disorder and a disability. Disor- Gifted students with attention deficits: Fact Symptoms versus impairment: The case for
der is a medical term, disability a legal and/or fiction? Or, can we see the forest

© 2011 The Guilford Press The ADHD Report • 7


respecting DSM-IV’s Criterion D. Journal of vey Replication (NCS-R). Archives of General Rapport, M., Scanlan, S., & Denney, C.
Attention Disorders, 9, 465-475. Psychiatry, 62(6): 617-627. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
Gordon, M., & Kaiser, S. (1998). Accom- Leroux, J., & Levitt-Perlman, M. (2000). The order and scholastic achievement: A model
modations in higher education under the gifted child with attention deficit disorder: of dual developmental pathways. Journal
Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA). An identification and intervention chal- of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(8),
DeWitt, NY: GSI. lenge. Roeper Review, 22(3), 171-176. 1169-1183.

Gordon, M., Lewandowski, L., & Kaiser, S. Lovett, B., & Lewandowski, L. (2006). Reis, S., & McCoach, D. (2002). Under-
(1999). The LD label for relatively well-func- Gifted students with learning disabilities: achievement in gifted and talented students
tioning students: A critical analysis. Journal Who are they? Journal of Learning Disabili- with special needs. Exceptionality, 10(2),
of Learning Disabilities, 32, 485-490. ties, 39(6), 515-527. 113-125.

Harrison, A., Edwards, M., & Parker, K. Mika, E. (2006). Research commentary Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S. (2005). Causal models
(2007). Identifying students faking ADHD: point-counterpoint: Diagnosis of giftedness of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
Preliminary findings and strategies for de- and ADHD. Roeper Review, 28, 237-242. From common simple deficits to multiple
tection. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, developmental pathways. Biologic Psychia-
Moon, S., Zentall, S., Grskovic, J., Hall,
22(5), 577-588. try, 57, 1231-1238.
A., & Stormont, M. (2001). Emotional and
Hartnett, D., Nelson, J., & Rinn, A. (2004). social characteristics of boys with AD/HD Stanley, J. (1978). Radical acceleration: Re-
Gifted or ADHD? The possibilities of misdi- and giftedness: A comparative case study. cent educational innovation at JHU. Gifted
agnosis. Roeper Review, 26(2), 73-76. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 24(3), Child Quarterly, 22(1), 62-67.

Hervey A.S., Epstein J.N., & Curry J.F. 207-247. Sullivan, K., Lange, R., & Dawes, S. (2007).
(2004). Neuropsychology of adults with Pearson, D.A., & Aman, M.G. (1994). Methods of detecting malingering and
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A Ratings of hyperactivity and developmen- estimated symptom exaggeration base rates
meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 18, tal indices: Should clinicians correct for in Australia. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychol-
485-503. developmental level? Journal of Autism & ogy, 4(4), 49-70.

Kaufman, A., Kaufman, J., Liu, X., & John- Developmental Disorders, 24, 395-411. Webb, J.T., & Latimer, D. (1993). ADHD
son, C. (2009). How do educational attain- Pearson, D.A., Lachar, D., Loveland, K.A., and children who are gifted. Exceptional
ment and gender relate to fluid intelligence, Santos, C.W., Faria, L.P., Azzam, P.N., Hent- Children, 60, 183-184.
crystallized intelligence, and academic ges, B.A., & Cleveland, L.A. (2000). Patterns Zentall, S., Moon, S., Hall, A., & Grskovic, J.
skills at ages 22–90 years? Archives of Clini- of behavioral adjustment and maladjust- (2001). Learning and motivational charac-
cal Neuropsychology, 24(2), 153-163. ment in mental retardation: Comparison of teristics of boys with AD/HD and/or gift-
Kessler R.C., Chiu W.T., Demler, O., & children with and without ADHD. American edness. Exceptional Children, 67(4), 499-519.
Walters E.E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, Journal of Mental Retardation, 105, 236-251. Disorder Versus Disability:
and comorbidity of twelve-month DSM-IV Piechowski, M. (1986). The concept of de- The Challenge of ADHD in the
disorders in the National Comorbidity Sur- velopmental potential. Roeper Review, 8(3), Context of a High IQ
190-197.

Cortico-Striatal-Thalamic-Cortical (CSTC)
Circuits, Tourette’s Disorder, and OCD
Florence Levy, M.D., FRANZCP
With the advent of increasingly spe- ity, reaching a peak at about 24 months sions (factor scores) could be generated
cific deep brain stimulation techniques of age. The content of these behaviors from data collected by the Tourette Syn-
(DBT) in adults, it may be important to is described as closely resembling ob- drome Association International Con-
understand the architecture of tic-relat- sessive compulsive (OC) symptom di- sortium for Genetics Affected Sibling
ed disorders of childhood (Levy, 2011). mensions, including arranging objects, Peer Study. They report that 50% of the
While it is too early to apply DBT tech- repeating actions, and concern with siblings with TS were found to have
niques in children, a small number of dirt or cleanliness, as well as hoarding comorbid tic-related OCD and greater
severe and resistant cases of Tourette’s behaviors. While direct evidence link- than 30% of mothers and 10 % of fathers
Disorder may ultimately benefit from ing these behaviors to OCD is lacking, also had a diagnosis of OCD. Based on
such specific treatments, once clear subclinical obsessions and compulsions complex segregation analyses, signifi-
safety has been established in adults. are encountered in children at a rate as cant evidence for genetic transmission
According to Leckman et al. (2009} high as 8% (Leckman et al., 2009). was obtained for all factors (Leckman et
typically developing children engage Leckman et al. (2003) selected all al., 2009).
in a significant amount of ritualistic, available affected TS pairs and their Neurosurgical procedures have, ac-
repetitive and compulsive-like activ- parents for whom OC symptom dimen- cording to Leckman (2002), reinforced

8 • The ADHD Report © 2011 The Guilford Press

You might also like