You are on page 1of 19

Test Bank for Basics of Research Methods for Criminal

Justice and Criminology 4th Edition by Maxfield ISBN


1305261100 9781305261105
Full download link at:
Test bank: https://testbankpack.com/p/test-bank-for-basics-of-research-methods-for-
criminal-justice-and-criminology-4th-edition-by-maxfield-isbn-1305261100-
9781305261105/
CHAPTER 5 TEST BANK
Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs

TRUE/FALSE

1. Experimentation is best suited for descriptive studies.

ANS: F REF: The Classical Experiment

2. The defining feature of an experiment lies in the control of the independent variable by
the experimenter.

ANS: T REF: The Classical Experiment

3. In a classical experiment, the independent variable must be a ratio level variable.

ANS: F REF: The Classical Experiment

4. In a classical experiment, subjects are measured on the independent variable before the
experiment begins and again after the dependent variable has been manipulated by the
researcher.

ANS: F REF: The Classical Experiment

5. Construct validity refers to generalizing from our experimental observations to causal


processes in the real world.

ANS: T REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

6. Experiments in criminal justice typically require only one experimental and one control
group for each study.

ANS: F REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

59
7. The threat of statistical regression is a concern any time the researcher begins with
subjects who exhibit extreme values on the dependent variable.

ANS: T REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

8. Construct validity is concerned with the ability to generalize from the results of the
experimental group to the control group.

ANS: F REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

60
9. Random assignment cannot be used in some criminal justice research for legal and ethical
reasons.

ANS: T REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

10. In case-oriented research, a great number of cases are examined in order to understand a
small number of variables.

ANS: T REF: Variable-Oriented Research, Case Studies and Scientific Realism

MULTIPLE CHOICE

1. Which statement is most accurate?


a. Experiments are only useful in the physical sciences such as chemistry and
physics.
b. Experiments involve observing phenomena but do not try to produce them.
c. Experiments can be used in scientific as well as nonscientific human inquiry.
d. Experiments in the classical form require at least three groups of subjects..

ANS: C REF: The Classical Experiment

2. Which statement is most accurate?


a. Experimentation are rarely appropriate for hypothesis testing.
b. Experiments are well suited to research involving poorly-defined concepts.
c. There is no need to generate a hypothesis for a classical experiment.
d. Experiments are appropriate for evaluation research.

ANS: D REF: The Classical Experiment

3. An experiment examines the effects of a(n) ____ variable.


a. dependent
b. control
c. independent
d. masking

ANS: C REF: The Classical Experiment

61
4. Which statement is true?
a. A variable can be an independent in one experiment and dependent in another.
b. The dependent variable is manipulated by the experimenter.
c. A classical experiment examines the effects of the dependent variable.
d. In a classical experiment, the independent variable is viewed as the outcome.

ANS: A REF: The Classical Experiment

5. Which statement is most accurate?


a. Neither the independent nor the dependent variable need be operationally defined.
b. Only the dependent variable needs to be operationally defined.
c. Only the independent variable needs to be operationally defined.
d. Both the independent and dependent variables must be operationally defined.

ANS: D REF: The Classical Experiment

6. In most cases, the methods used to select subjects must meet the scientific norm of ____.
a. generalizability
b. informed consent
c. equivalence
d. neutrality

ANS: A REF: The Classical Experiment

7. Which procedure is suitable for random assignment to groups?


a. Assigning subjects in the order they arrive, so that one group is filled before the
next is started
b. Letting subjects express a preference and then assigning them to a non-preferred
group
c. Flipping a coin assigning subjects to the control group and to the experimental
group.
d. Asking each subject to secretly decide which group they want to be in

ANS: B REF: The Classical Experiment

62
8. Which statement best fits randomization as it is used in experiments?
a. “all other things being equal”
b. “use your best guess”
c. “it is close enough for government work”
d. “you are never 100% sure, so don’t worry about it”

ANS: A REF: The Classical Experiment

9. Which statement is a potential threat to internal validity in an experimental design?


a. generalization
b. instrumentation
c. randomization
d. correlation

ANS: B REF: The Classical Experiment

10. Which situation presents a threat to internal validity?


a. subjects dropping out of an experiment
b. selecting a random sample
c. randomization of group assignments
d. very high reliability of outcome measures

ANS: A REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

11. Shortening the time between pretest and posttest or perhaps even offering cash payments
to participants in an experiment are techniques that may be used to ____.
a. limit diffusion
b. decrease experimental mortality
c. decrease the effects of instrumentation
d. control threats to testing

ANS: B REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

12. _______________ validity is the correspondence between the empirical test of a


hypothesis and the underlying causal process that the experiment is intended to represent.
a. Construct
b. Compensatory
c. Correlational
d. Criterion

63
ANS: A REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

13. Which statement is accurate?


a. Threats to external validity are decreased by carefully controlling conditions.
b. Threats to internal validity are decreased by carefully controlling conditions.
c. Threats to generalizability are inherent in research and cannot be changed.
d. Threats to internal and external validity covary, and if one is high, the other will
also be high.

ANS: B REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

14. When using a nonequivalent-groups design, the researcher will handle subject assignment
to groups by ____.
a. random assignment to experimental and control groups
b. allowing subjects to pick which group they want to be in
c. matching subjects in the experimental and comparison groups
d. using waves of cohorts

ANS: C REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental


Design

15. In Widom’s study of the effects of child abuse and its impact upon later criminal
offenses, she selected a sample of children with court records of being abuse victims and
gathered a comparison group that were matched on gender, race, age and socioeconomic
status. The technique used in this case was ____.
a. individual matching
b. aggregate matching
c. randomization
d. probabilistic aggregation

ANS: A REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental


Design

16. Graduates majoring in criminal justice and graduates majoring in psychology from the
State University were part of a study to assess employment success of those receiving
their degrees in May 2002. Six months after graduation (November 2002) a questionnaire
was sent out asking about their job seeking success. What type of design is this?
a. a double-blind experiment
b. a non-equivalent groups design
c. a time-series design

64
d. a cohort design

ANS: D REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental


Design

17. The history threat refers to the possibility that ____.


a. because of their personal experiences, some subjects will be uncooperative
b. most important research in the social sciences has already been done
c. events external to the experiment that will impact the results
d. the experiment will be judged to be unimportant

ANS: C REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

18. The maturation threat refers to _____.


a. older subjects being more likely to take research participation seriously
b. the impact of natural, developmental processes on experimental outcomes
c. the natural development of researcher expertise as more research is completed
d. younger subjects generally do better because they are easier to manipulate

ANS: B REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

19. Which action would be most likely to result in selection bias?


a. soliciting volunteers from a college class
b. randomly assigning experimental and control groups
c. matching experimental and comparison groups
d. running a double blind experiment

ANS: A REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

20. Experimental mortality is especially likely to be a problem when ____.


a. subjects are paid an excessive amount to participate
b. participation requires only one session, but it is an hour long
c. students must participate as a course requirement
d. participation in the experiment requires substantial effort by subjects

ANS: D REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

65
21. You have just completed the LSAT and scored 85. Your roommate, who is no smarter
than you, took the LSAT with you and scored 165. You were so upset with your score
that you decide to take the test again and you convince your roommate to take it with you
for moral support. You both receive scores on the second exam of 120. What accounts for
the differences in scores?
a. statistical regression
b. testing
c. instrumentation
d. experiment mortality

ANS: A REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

22. Neither the subjects nor the researcher can identify which group is the control group and
which is the experimental group. Which design is being used?
a. cross-sectional
b. double-blind
c. a blind experiment
d. of no value

ANS: B REF: The Classical Experiment

23. External validity in an experimental design refers to _____.


a. the accuracy of measurements
b. the lack of concern with issues internal to the study
c. the ability to apply findings to other populations
d. whether appropriate outcome measures have been used

ANS: C REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

24. Threats to internal validity in an experimental design can be minimized by _____.


a. proper selection and assignment of subjects to groups
b. large sample size
c. small sample size
d. nothing can rule out threats to internal validity

ANS: A REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

25. Which technique can be used to help reduce the threat to experimental mortality?

66
a. increasing the sample size
b. decreasing the sample size
c. shortening the time between the pretest and the posttest
d. selecting only subjects who are female

ANS: C REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

26. Random assignment to experimental and control groups should reduce the threats to
_____.
a. instrumentation
b. maturation
c. reliability
d. small sample

ANS: B REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

27. If randomization is not possible, the researcher would be well-advised to ____.


a. do nothing, but mention the issue in the research report
b. switch to a classical design
c. switch to a quasi-experimental design
d. use pseudorandomization

ANS: C REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

28. An interrupted time-series design can be very useful in ____ research.


a. applied
b. basic
c. primary
d. double-blind

ANS: A REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental


Design

29. A questionnaire is sent to 1,000 police departments in Florida and gathers information on
the number of sick days officers use after being involved in a shooting incident. This is
an example of ____ research.
a. variable-oriented
b. case-oriented
c. quasi-experimental
d. cohort

67
ANS: B REF: Variable-Oriented Research, Case Studies and Scientific Realism

30. Experimental mortality is also known as ____.


a. selection
b. regression
c. attrition
d. relevance

ANS: C REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental Design

COMPLETION

1. In a classical experiment, the researcher uses a(n) _______________ to offset the effects
of the experiment itself.

ANS: control group REF: The Classical Experiment

2. A(n) _______________ experiment guards against experimenters’ tendency to prejudge


results by eliminating the possibility of either the researcher or the subjects knowing
which group is the control and which group is the experimental.

ANS: double-blind, double blind REF: The Classical Experiment

3. The goal of _______________ is to ensure that the experimental and groups will be
statistically equivalent.

ANS: randomization REF: The Classical Experiment

4. _______________ relates to whether results from experiments conducted in one setting


would be obtained in other settings.

ANS: External validity REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

5. When random assignment to experimental and control groups is not possible, the
researcher may be forced to use a(n) _______________ design.

ANS: nonequivalent-group REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental Design

6. Instead of making one pre-test and one post-test measure, the _______________ makes a
longer series of observations before and after introducing an experimental treatment.

68
ANS: interrupted time-series design REF: Variations on the Classical Experimental Design

7. In _______________ research, a large number of variables that are associated with a


small number of cases or subjects are studied.

ANS: variable-oriented REF: Variable-Oriented Research, Case Studies, and


Scientific Realism

8. A(n) _______________ is a type of variable-oriented research.

ANS: case study REF: Variable-Oriented Research, Case Studies, and


Scientific Realism

9. Increasing experimental control will result in decreased _______________ validity.

ANS: internal REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

10. _______________ refers to the extent to which findings from an experiment can be
applied to the real world.

ANS: Generalizability REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

ESSAY

1. Explain why experimentation is not well suited for the purposes of description and
exploration.

REF: REF: The Classical Experiment

2. Explain the classical experiment in terms of the following:


Independent and dependent variables
Pre-testing and Post-testing
Experimental and Control groups

REF: The Classical Experiment

3. Explain the role of randomization in classical experiments.

REF: The Classical Experiment

4. Identify and discuss at least five (5) threats to internal validity in experimental designs.
Include in your answer ways of controlling for each of the threats you identify.

69
REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

5. Give two (2) specific examples of research where a quasi-experimental design would be
more appropriate than a classical experiment. Why would each of your examples not fit
the classical experiment model?

REF: Quasi-Experimental Designs

6. Describe an experimental design to test the causal hypothesis that D.A.R.E. reduces drug
use. Is your experimental design feasible? Why or why not?

REF: Quasi-Experimental Designs

7. Experiments are often conducted in public health research where a distinction is made
between an efficacy experiment and an effectiveness experiment. Efficacy experiments
focus on whether a new health program works under ideal conditions; effectiveness
experiments test the program under typical conditions that health professionals encounter
in their day-to-day work. Discuss how efficacy experiments and effectiveness
experiments reflect concerns about internal validity threats on the one hand and
generalizability on the other.

REF: The Classical Experiment

8. Crime hot spots are areas where crime reports, calls for police service, or other measures
of crime are especially common. Police in departments with a good analytic capability
routinely identify hot spots and launch special tactics to reduce crime in these areas.
What kinds of validity threats should researchers be especially attentive to in studying the
effects of police interventions on hot spots?

REF: Experiments and Causal Inference

70
Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
about donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation.”

• You provide a full refund of any money paid by a user who


notifies you in writing (or by e-mail) within 30 days of receipt that
s/he does not agree to the terms of the full Project Gutenberg™
License. You must require such a user to return or destroy all
copies of the works possessed in a physical medium and
discontinue all use of and all access to other copies of Project
Gutenberg™ works.

• You provide, in accordance with paragraph 1.F.3, a full refund of


any money paid for a work or a replacement copy, if a defect in
the electronic work is discovered and reported to you within 90
days of receipt of the work.

• You comply with all other terms of this agreement for free
distribution of Project Gutenberg™ works.

1.E.9. If you wish to charge a fee or distribute a Project


Gutenberg™ electronic work or group of works on different
terms than are set forth in this agreement, you must obtain
permission in writing from the Project Gutenberg Literary
Archive Foundation, the manager of the Project Gutenberg™
trademark. Contact the Foundation as set forth in Section 3
below.

1.F.

1.F.1. Project Gutenberg volunteers and employees expend


considerable effort to identify, do copyright research on,
transcribe and proofread works not protected by U.S. copyright
law in creating the Project Gutenberg™ collection. Despite
these efforts, Project Gutenberg™ electronic works, and the
medium on which they may be stored, may contain “Defects,”
such as, but not limited to, incomplete, inaccurate or corrupt
data, transcription errors, a copyright or other intellectual
property infringement, a defective or damaged disk or other
medium, a computer virus, or computer codes that damage or
cannot be read by your equipment.

1.F.2. LIMITED WARRANTY, DISCLAIMER OF DAMAGES -


Except for the “Right of Replacement or Refund” described in
paragraph 1.F.3, the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation, the owner of the Project Gutenberg™ trademark,
and any other party distributing a Project Gutenberg™ electronic
work under this agreement, disclaim all liability to you for
damages, costs and expenses, including legal fees. YOU
AGREE THAT YOU HAVE NO REMEDIES FOR NEGLIGENCE,
STRICT LIABILITY, BREACH OF WARRANTY OR BREACH
OF CONTRACT EXCEPT THOSE PROVIDED IN PARAGRAPH
1.F.3. YOU AGREE THAT THE FOUNDATION, THE
TRADEMARK OWNER, AND ANY DISTRIBUTOR UNDER
THIS AGREEMENT WILL NOT BE LIABLE TO YOU FOR
ACTUAL, DIRECT, INDIRECT, CONSEQUENTIAL, PUNITIVE
OR INCIDENTAL DAMAGES EVEN IF YOU GIVE NOTICE OF
THE POSSIBILITY OF SUCH DAMAGE.

1.F.3. LIMITED RIGHT OF REPLACEMENT OR REFUND - If


you discover a defect in this electronic work within 90 days of
receiving it, you can receive a refund of the money (if any) you
paid for it by sending a written explanation to the person you
received the work from. If you received the work on a physical
medium, you must return the medium with your written
explanation. The person or entity that provided you with the
defective work may elect to provide a replacement copy in lieu
of a refund. If you received the work electronically, the person or
entity providing it to you may choose to give you a second
opportunity to receive the work electronically in lieu of a refund.
If the second copy is also defective, you may demand a refund
in writing without further opportunities to fix the problem.

1.F.4. Except for the limited right of replacement or refund set


forth in paragraph 1.F.3, this work is provided to you ‘AS-IS’,
WITH NO OTHER WARRANTIES OF ANY KIND, EXPRESS
OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING BUT NOT LIMITED TO
WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR
ANY PURPOSE.

1.F.5. Some states do not allow disclaimers of certain implied


warranties or the exclusion or limitation of certain types of
damages. If any disclaimer or limitation set forth in this
agreement violates the law of the state applicable to this
agreement, the agreement shall be interpreted to make the
maximum disclaimer or limitation permitted by the applicable
state law. The invalidity or unenforceability of any provision of
this agreement shall not void the remaining provisions.

1.F.6. INDEMNITY - You agree to indemnify and hold the


Foundation, the trademark owner, any agent or employee of the
Foundation, anyone providing copies of Project Gutenberg™
electronic works in accordance with this agreement, and any
volunteers associated with the production, promotion and
distribution of Project Gutenberg™ electronic works, harmless
from all liability, costs and expenses, including legal fees, that
arise directly or indirectly from any of the following which you do
or cause to occur: (a) distribution of this or any Project
Gutenberg™ work, (b) alteration, modification, or additions or
deletions to any Project Gutenberg™ work, and (c) any Defect
you cause.

Section 2. Information about the Mission of


Project Gutenberg™
Project Gutenberg™ is synonymous with the free distribution of
electronic works in formats readable by the widest variety of
computers including obsolete, old, middle-aged and new
computers. It exists because of the efforts of hundreds of
volunteers and donations from people in all walks of life.

Volunteers and financial support to provide volunteers with the


assistance they need are critical to reaching Project
Gutenberg™’s goals and ensuring that the Project Gutenberg™
collection will remain freely available for generations to come. In
2001, the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation was
created to provide a secure and permanent future for Project
Gutenberg™ and future generations. To learn more about the
Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation and how your
efforts and donations can help, see Sections 3 and 4 and the
Foundation information page at www.gutenberg.org.

Section 3. Information about the Project


Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation
The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation is a non-
profit 501(c)(3) educational corporation organized under the
laws of the state of Mississippi and granted tax exempt status by
the Internal Revenue Service. The Foundation’s EIN or federal
tax identification number is 64-6221541. Contributions to the
Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation are tax
deductible to the full extent permitted by U.S. federal laws and
your state’s laws.

The Foundation’s business office is located at 809 North 1500


West, Salt Lake City, UT 84116, (801) 596-1887. Email contact
links and up to date contact information can be found at the
Foundation’s website and official page at
www.gutenberg.org/contact

Section 4. Information about Donations to


the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive
Foundation
Project Gutenberg™ depends upon and cannot survive without
widespread public support and donations to carry out its mission
of increasing the number of public domain and licensed works
that can be freely distributed in machine-readable form
accessible by the widest array of equipment including outdated
equipment. Many small donations ($1 to $5,000) are particularly
important to maintaining tax exempt status with the IRS.

The Foundation is committed to complying with the laws


regulating charities and charitable donations in all 50 states of
the United States. Compliance requirements are not uniform
and it takes a considerable effort, much paperwork and many
fees to meet and keep up with these requirements. We do not
solicit donations in locations where we have not received written
confirmation of compliance. To SEND DONATIONS or
determine the status of compliance for any particular state visit
www.gutenberg.org/donate.

While we cannot and do not solicit contributions from states


where we have not met the solicitation requirements, we know
of no prohibition against accepting unsolicited donations from
donors in such states who approach us with offers to donate.

International donations are gratefully accepted, but we cannot


make any statements concerning tax treatment of donations
received from outside the United States. U.S. laws alone swamp
our small staff.

Please check the Project Gutenberg web pages for current


donation methods and addresses. Donations are accepted in a
number of other ways including checks, online payments and
credit card donations. To donate, please visit:
www.gutenberg.org/donate.

Section 5. General Information About Project


Gutenberg™ electronic works
Professor Michael S. Hart was the originator of the Project
Gutenberg™ concept of a library of electronic works that could
be freely shared with anyone. For forty years, he produced and
distributed Project Gutenberg™ eBooks with only a loose
network of volunteer support.

Project Gutenberg™ eBooks are often created from several


printed editions, all of which are confirmed as not protected by
copyright in the U.S. unless a copyright notice is included. Thus,
we do not necessarily keep eBooks in compliance with any
particular paper edition.

Most people start at our website which has the main PG search
facility: www.gutenberg.org.

This website includes information about Project Gutenberg™,


including how to make donations to the Project Gutenberg
Literary Archive Foundation, how to help produce our new
eBooks, and how to subscribe to our email newsletter to hear
about new eBooks.

You might also like