You are on page 1of 12

P1: GDP

Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 31, No. 1, February 2002, pp. 51–62 (°
C 2002)

Sexual Partner Preference in Female Japanese Macaques

Paul L. Vasey, Ph.D.1

Received March 29, 2001; revision received August 28, 2001; accepted September 1, 2001

Whether animals ever exhibit a preference for same-sex sexual partners is a subject of debate. Japanese
macaques represent excellent models for examining issues related to sexual preference in animals be-
cause females, in certain populations, routinely engage in both heterosexual and homosexual behavior
over the course of their life spans. Multiple lines of evidence indicate that female homosexual behavior
in Japanese macaques is a sexual behavior, not a sociosexual one. Additional evidence indicates that
female Japanese macaques do not engage in homosexual behavior simply because acceptable male
mates are unavailable or unmotivated to copulate. Patterns of sexual partner choice by female Japanese
macaques that are the focus of intersexual competition indicate that females of this species choose
same-sex sexual partners even when they are simultaneously presented with a motivated, opposite-
sex alternative. Thus, in some populations of Japanese macaques, females prefer certain same-sex
sexual partners relative to certain male mates, and vice versa. Taken together, this evidence suggests
that female Japanese macaques are best characterized as bisexual in orientation, not preferentially
homosexual or preferentially heterosexual.

KEY WORDS: homosexual behavior; sexual preference; Japanese macaques.

Most researchers would agree that same-sex interac- opposite-sex alternatives? Or, are animals with bisexual
tions involving genital contact are widespread among an- potential always preferentially heterosexual? Before one
imals and can occur independent of any hormonal or neu- can address the larger question of whether animals ever
rological manipulations (Adkins-Regan, 1988; Bagemihl, exhibit a preference for same-sex sexual partners, these
1999; Dixson, 1998; Nadler, 1990; Vasey, 1995; Wallen more fundamental issues must be addressed.
& Parsons, 1997). More contentious, however, is the issue In this paper, I review my previous research on female
of whether these interactions ever reflect an actual pref- homosexual behavior in Japanese macaques (Macaca fus-
erence for same-sex sexual partners (cf. Bagemihl, 1999; cata) that bears directly on the questions outlined above.
Dixson, 1998; Wallen & Parsons, 1997). I then address how female sexual preference in Japanese
This debate is actually fueled by a number of more macaques might best be conceptualized on a species-wide
fundamental disagreements about the nature of animal level.
sexual behavior. First, are same-sex “sexual” interactions
in animals really sexual? Or, are they merely sociosexual METHODS
interactions that are devoid of sexual motivation and en-
acted to mediate some sort of adaptive social goal? Second, Subjects
are same-sex sexual interactions in animals manifested
in the presence of opposite-sex alternatives? Or, do they Study subjects were housed at the Université de
simply occur because opposite-sex partners are difficult Montréal’s Laboratory of Behavioral Primatology (est.
to access? Third, do individuals actually choose same- 1992) near St-Hyancinthe, Québec. The group consisted
sex sexual partners when simultaneously presented with of three unrelated matrilines named A, B, and C in ac-
cordance with their overall dominance relationships (A >
1 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, B > C). The founding members of the study group origi-
4401 University Drive, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4; e-mail: nated from the Arashiyama East (Japan) troop of Japanese
paul.vasey@uleth.ca. macaques that was translocated to Texas in 1972 (Fedigan,

51
0004-0002/02/0200-0051/0 °
C 2002 Plenum Publishing Corporation
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

52 Vasey

1991). The monkeys lived in five indoor rooms (25 × Two types of baseline data were collected following
15.5 × 3.6 m3 ) and two outdoor enclosures (17 × 16 × the 1993–94 mating season. First, between 7 March and
4 m3 ). The rooms and enclosures were furnished with 15 April 1994, baseline data collection focused on record-
swinging and climbing devices and loose enrichment ob- ing all occurrences of dyadic aggression and interventions
jects (e.g., branches, pails, coconuts). Animals were fed by members of the study group (N = 45 hr). Second, be-
daily with a mixture of grains, monkey chow, fruit, and tween 21 March and 4 May 1995, baseline data collec-
vegetables, which was distributed in deep wood-chip litter tion focused on recording all occurrences of grooming
covering the indoor floors. Water was available at will. between nonkin females (N = 36 hr). Observations for
During the 1993–94 mating season, the study group both the pre- and postconsortship baseline periods took
consisted of 37 individuals (18 sexually active females, place between 9:00 and 17:00.
ages 3.5–23 years; 5 sexually active males, ages 4.5– Unless otherwise stated, the data discussed in this
9.5 years; 14 sexually immature individuals). During the paper are derived from the 1993–94 mating season.
1994–95 mating season, the study group consisted of
38 individuals (1 sexually inactive female, age 24 years; Data Collection During the 1994–95 Mating Season
16 sexually active females, ages 3.5–24 years; 5 sexually
active males, ages 4–10.5 years; 16 sexually immature During the 1994–95 mating season, subjects were ob-
individuals). served during a baseline and experimental period. Base-
line and experimental observations were conducted on
General Data Collection identical days across separate 4-week periods. Thus, data
were collected during December 1994 (baseline period)
All observations were recorded on paper in 30-min and January 1995 (experimental period) on identical days
time blocks. Group members were individually recog- (2, 5–24; N = 21 days/4-week period). Three hours of
nized. During observation periods, the monkeys occupied observation took place every day between 9:00 and 13:00
the two largest indoor rooms of the laboratory and the two during which all occurrences of consortship formation and
outdoor enclosures. Observations took place from a raised sexual solicitations were recorded. A total of 126 hr of be-
mezzanine that looked out onto all the rooms and enclo- havioral sampling data were collected.
sures occupied by the monkeys. As such, all members of In the afternoon of January 1, 1995, an experimen-
the study group were readily observable regardless of the tal subgroup was created by removing four of the group’s
room or enclosure they occupied. five sexually active males. To control for the confound-
ing effects of male dominance acquisition over females,
Data Collection During the 1993–94 Mating Season the study group’s alpha male was chosen as the sole male
to remain in the experimental subgroup. Removal of the
Observations were made daily from dawn to dusk other four males was accomplished in a few minutes by
(approximately 7:00–16:30) during the mating season be- channeling the monkeys through a network of rooms and
tween 20 September 1993 and 11 March 1994, excluding sliding doors. The resulting experimental and all-male
a 4-day period from 25 to 29 December. All occurrences subgroups were physically and visually separated from
of sexual activity (i.e., mounting, consortship formation, each other.
sexual solicitations) were recorded during this period. Fo-
cal data and scan sample data (Altmann, 1974) were col- Definitions
lected daily for female Japanese macaques engaged in ho-
mosexual consortships, between 17 November 1993 and Homosexual consortships occurred when two fe-
13 February 1994. Focal and scan sample data were col- males engaged in series mounting (three or more mounts
lected for both consort partners simultaneously because within a 10-min period). These consortships were deemed
they were almost always interacting together. When two to have terminated if the two female partners were sepa-
homosexual consortships occurred simultaneously, I ob- rated by a distance of more than 1 m and exhibited no
served the one for which I had the least data. I collected mounting for 10 min. Homosexual behavior is used to
129 hr of focal data. For every minute of focal data, I collectively refer to homosexual consortships, mounting,
recorded one scan sample of both consort partners. and sexual solicitations.
Before the 1993–94 mating season, (23 March– Double foot-clasp mounts involved the mounter gras-
30 April, 1993, 45 hr; 29 July–3 August, 1993, 10 hr; ping with her feet just above the mountee’s ankles and with
N = 55 hr) baseline data collection focused on recording her hands on the mountee’s back. As such, the mounter’s
all occurrences of dyadic aggression and interventions. groin region was in contact with the mountee’s perineum.
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques 53

Sitting mounts occurred when the mounter sat on the and observational evidence indicates that female Japanese
mountee’s back jockey-style, while grasping the moun- macaques treat each other as nonkin, in terms of sexual
tee’s upper back with her hands and the mountee’s lower and nonsexual affiliation, when their level of relatedness
back with her feet. In this position, the mounter’s perineum is less than r = .25 (Chapais, Gauthier, Prud’homme, &
was in contact with the mountee’s lower back. Standing Vasey, 1997). This evidence suggests that female Japanese
mounts involved the mounter standing bipedally with her macaques categorize individual as nonkin when they share
feet on the ground and her knees slightly bent, while gras- a level of relatedness of r < .25 (e.g., aunt–nieces).
ping the mountee’s lower back with her hands. As such, the
mounter’s groin region was in contact with the mountee’s Homosexual Behavior in Female Japanese Macaques
perineum. Reclining mounts occurred when the mounter
lay ventrally on the mountee’s back, using her feet to grasp Japanese macaques represent excellent models for
the mountee’s legs just above the ankles and her hands examining issues related to sexual preference in animals
to grasp fur on the mountee’s upper back. Each of these because females, in certain populations, routinely engage
four different types of mounts could be performed with or in both heterosexual and homosexual behavior over the
without pelvic thrusts by the mounter for a total of eight course of their life spans. Female homosexual activity
different mount types. occurs in both captive (Chapais et al., 1997; Chapais &
Hindquarter presentations were performed by po- Mignault, 1991; Lunardini, 1989; Rendall & Taylor, 1991;
tential mountees who stood quadrupedally with their arms Vasey, 1996, 1998; Vasey, Chapais, & Gauthier, 1998;
and legs flexed and their perineum oriented towards the po- Vasey & Gauthier, 2000) and free-ranging (Eaton, 1978;
tential mounter. Back presentations were performed by po- Enomoto, 1974; Fedigan & Gouzoules, 1978; Gouzoules
tential mountees who sat with their forearms slightly bent & Goy, 1983; Takahata, 1982; Wolfe, 1984) populations
and their backs inclined and oriented towards the potential of this species. As such, these interactions cannot be dis-
mounter. Hands-on-hindquarters solicitations were per- missed as abnormal products of captivity.
formed by potential mounters who grasped or placed both Female homosexual behavior in Japanese macaques
hands on the hindquarters of the potential mountees. occurs during temporary, but exclusive, associations with
Dominance relationships were determined by the di- same-sex sexual partners. These relationships, termed ho-
rection of submissive behavior(s), that is, displacements, mosexual consortships, can be brief, lasting less than
fear grimaces, rapid fleeing, piercing screams, and de- 1 hr, or they can continue for over a week (Wolfe, 1984).
fensive hunching. Submission had to be unidirectional During homosexual consortships, females solicit
between two individuals for the recipient to be consid- each other to mount, and to be mounted, using vocaliza-
ered dominant. Dyads for which submission was bidi- tions and a variety of postural and facial gestures. Sexual
rectional were considered to have ambiguous dominance solicitations include pushing, hitting, grabbing, slapping
relationships. the ground, head bobbing, screaming, lip quivering, body
Interventions referred to the intrusion of an individual spasms, and intense gazing. A female can specifically re-
(supporter) into an ongoing conflict between two oppo- quest to be mounted by presenting her inclined back or
nents, during which the intruding animal supported one of her hindquarters to a potential mounter. Conversely, a fe-
the opponents (recipient of support) against the other (tar- male can specifically request to mount another female by
get). Supporters and recipients of support were referred to placing her hands on the other’s hindquarters (Vasey et al.,
as allies and were said to have formed alliances. Oppor- 1998).
tunities for intervention included all incidences of dyadic Single mounts between females are rarely observed.
aggression, except displacements, during which one indi- Instead, multiple or series mounting between consort part-
vidual could intervene to support another against a third. ners is the norm. During these interactions, females mount
Conservative interventions occurred when the targets of each other in a bidirectional manner and routinely employ
aggression ranked below the supporter and the recipient of up to eight different types of mounting postures (Vasey
support. Bridging interventions occurred when the targets et al., 1998; see definitions). During mounts, mounters of-
of aggression ranked below the supporter, but above the ten thrust against mountees, who in turn, commonly reach
recipient of support. Opportunities for rank changes oc- back to grasp the mounter and gaze intently into her eyes.
curred when an individual participated in an intervention Mounting between consort partners and the accom-
against a dominant target, or one with whom they shared panying sexual solicitations can continue uninterrupted
an ambiguous dominance relationship. for well over an hour. Thus, sexual behavior is clearly
Females from different matrilines were considered the defining feature of homosexual consortships. It would
to be nonkin, as were aunt–niece dyads. Experimental be erroneous, however, to characterize these relationships
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

54 Vasey

solely in terms of sex. Between bouts of sexual activity, thereby reducing the threat of incipient aggression as-
partners exhibit highly synchronized activity: huddling sociated with social tension (Wickler, 1967). According
and sleeping together, as well as grooming, following, to this “rank demonstration hypothesis,” mounting is an
and defending each other for prolonged periods of time expression of dominance, whereas allowing oneself to
(Gouzoules & Goy, 1983; Vasey, 1996; Wolfe, 1984). be mounted expresses subordinate status relative to the
Partners will also forage side-by-side during homosex- mounter (Wickler, 1967). Consequently, a test of this hypo-
ual consortships, although this form of cofeeding is not a thesis should demonstrate that the roles individuals
prominent feature of these relationships. adopt during same-sex mounting interactions are differ-
The frequency of female homosexual behavior varies entiated on the basis of their rank. In addition, mounting
both within and between Japanese macaque populations. interactions among same-sex individuals should be par-
In some populations, the behavior appears to be non- ticularly common at the outset of aggressive interactions,
existent. In others, every sexually mature female engages at which time reiterating an acceptance of the dominance
in homosexual behavior, and they do so frequently. For status quo should function to reduce the threat of escalated
example, during the 1993–94 mating season, every sex- fighting.
ually mature female in my study group engaged in ho- Several lines of evidence indicate that female
mosexual behaviour. Moreover, the majority of sexual Japanese macaques do not use homosexual behavior to
consortships formed (N = 95) during this period were communicate about their dominance relationships in the
homosexual (55%), not heterosexual (45%). During these manner predicted by the rank demonstration hypothesis
consortships, females (N = 14) solicited their partners for (for further details, see Vasey et al., 1998). During ho-
sex, on average, 28 times per hour of observation, and mosexual consortships, dominant and subordinate part-
they mounted each other, on average, 31 times per hour ners typically take turns mounting and being mounted,
of observation (N = 129 observation hours; Vasey, 1996; in a bidirectional manner. Of the 3,945 mounts observed,
Vasey et al., 1998). Some researchers have argued that this 56.9% were performed by dominant consort partners, and
intergroup variation can be explained in terms of “proto- 43.1% were performed by subordinate consort partners.
cultural” differences in sexual “traditions” among pop- This difference was not significant (Wilcoxon matched-
ulations (e.g., Fedigan, 1982; Itani & Nishimura, 1973; pairs signed-ranks test, N = 21, Z 0 = 0.2, P = .85).
Stephenson, 1973). Even when the catch-all category of “mounts” was sub-
divided into eight different mount types that were then
analyzed separately, it was found that neither partner was
Is Homosexual Behavior a Sociosexual Adaptation? significantly more or less responsible for mounting or be-
ing mounted (Table I).
Same-sex interactions in animals involving genital In keeping with the spirit of the rank demonstra-
contact have traditionally been interpreted as “sociosex- tion hypothesis, one could also predict that solicitations
ual” behaviors (Dixson, 1998; Wickler, 1967). Within to mount, and to be mounted, should be performed dif-
ethological circles, sociosexual behaviors are defined as ferentially by dominant and subordinate consort partners.
behaviors that are sexual in terms of their outward form, Dominant consort partners should perform hands-on-
but enacted to facilitate some type of adaptive social goal. hindquarters solicitations more often because they func-
Despite over 40 years of intensive research on Japanese tion as requests to mount and prompt the receivers to
macaques, there is not a single study demonstrating that prepare to be mounted. However, dominant consort part-
female homosexual behavior in this species is a sociosex- ners did not perform hands-on-hindquarters solicitations
ual adaptation. On the contrary, several studies indicate significantly more often than their subordinate counter-
that female Japanese macaques do not use homosexual be- parts. Of the 720 hands-on-hindquarters solicitations ob-
havior to facilitate sociosexual goals (Gouzoules & Goy, served, 35.8% were performed by dominant consort part-
1983; Vasey, 1995, 1996, 1998; Vasey et al., 1998). ners, whereas 64.2% were performed by the subordinate
partners (Wilcoxon test, N = 20, Z 0 = 1.6, P = .1).
Similarly, one would predict that subordinate con-
Dominance Demonstration and the Regulation sort partners should perform hindquarter presentations and
of Social Tension back presentations more often than their dominant coun-
terparts, because these signals function as requests to be
Same-sex mounting interactions have long been con- mounted. Subordinate consort partners did not, however,
ceptualized as ritualized gestures that the participants use perform hindquarter or back presentations significantly
to communicate about their dominance relationships, more often than their dominant partners. Of the 1,225
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques 55

Table I. Comparison of Dominant and Subordinate Consort Partners’ Behaviour During Mounting Interactions
Using Wilcoxon Matched-Pairs Signed-Ranks Test

Total Dominant Subordinate N Z 0 /T P

Double-foot clasp mount, thrusting 1578 1112 466 20 1.80 .07


Double-foot clasp mount, no thrusting 1043 749 294 20 1.70 .09
Sitting mount, thrusting 749 159 590 12 53 .15
Sitting mount, no thrusting 297 105 192 14 55.5 .88
Standing mount, thrusting 40 24 16 10 39 .28
Standing mount, no thrusting 84 49 35 12 44 .73
Reclining mount, thrusting 30 11 19 6 11.5 .92
Reclining mount, no thrusting 124 35 89 9 28 .57

Note. Prime (0 ) indicates Z value corrected for ties. All other values are T values.

hindquarter presentations observed, 38% were performed Alliance Formation


by dominant consort partners, whereas 62% were per-
formed by the subordinate partners (Wilcoxon test, N = A number of primate species appear to use homosex-
20, Z 0 = 1.6, P = .1). Of the 463 back presentations ob- ual behavior as a mechanism for alliance formation (e.g.,
served, 46% were performed by dominant consort part- Fairbanks, McGuire, & Kerber, 1977; Idani, 1991; Parish,
ners, and 54% were performed by subordinate consort 1994; Smuts & Watanabe, 1990). Among female Japanese
partners (Wilcoxon test, N = 17, Z 0 = 0.2, P = .81). macaques, homosexual consortships and alliance forma-
In accordance with the rank demonstration hypoth- tion are causally related (for further details, see Vasey,
esis, one would further predict that subordinate consort 1996). Dominant and subordinate consort partners inter-
partners would facilitate mounts by their dominant part- vene for each other significantly more often during ho-
ners more often than the reverse, by clasping the domi- mosexual consortships than either before (Wilcoxon test
nant mounter during mounts-in-progress. However, nei- for dominant partners, N = 15, Z 0 = 3.4, P < .01; sub-
ther partner was significantly more or less responsible ordinate partners, N = 16, Z 0 = 3.5, P < .001) or after
than the other for clasping during mounts-in-progress. Of (Wilcoxon test for dominant partners, N = 16, Z 0 = 3.4,
the 1,248 incidences of clasping observed during mounts- P < .01; subordinate partners, N = 16, Z 0 = 3.5, P <
in-progress, 62.6% were performed by dominant consort .001) these relationships. During homosexual consort-
partners and 37.4% by subordinate partners (Wilcoxon ships, females (N = 9) intervened significantly more for
test, N = 20, Z 0 = 1.8, P = .08). their sexual partners (χ = 1.33 interventions/hr) than
Additional evidence suggests that female Japanese for other sexually mature, nonkin females (χ = 0.09
macaques do not use homosexual behavior to control in- interventions/hr; Wilcoxon test: dominant partners, N =
cipient aggression as predicted by the rank demonstration 16, Z 0 = 3.57, P < .001; subordinate partners, N = 17,
hypothesis (for further details, see Vasey et al., 1998). In Z 0 = 4, P < .001). As such, the tendency for consorting
Japanese macaques, a close temporal relationship does females to intervene more for each other did not reflect
not exist between homosexual mounting and escalating a generalized motivation to intervene for any sexually
social tension associated with incipient aggression. Only mature, nonkin females. Despite this causal relationship,
0.004% of all mounts (N = 2,244) and 0.002% of hands- patterns of partner choice and affiliation between consort
on-hindquarters solicitations (N = 462) by dominant partners suggest that females were not engaging in ho-
partners occurred within 1 min of an aggressive interac- mosexual consortships for the express purpose of alliance
tion between consort partners. Similarly, only 0.02% of formation (for further details, see Vasey, 1996, 2000b).
back presentations (N = 250) and 0.005% of hindquarter If the primary reason for engaging in homosexual
presentations (N = 760) by subordinate partners occurred consortships is to mediate alliance formation, then two
within 1 min of an aggressive interaction between consort key behavioral patterns should be expected to occur. First,
partners. higher ranking females should be preferred as consort part-
Taken together, these multiple lines of evidence sug- ners because they represent more powerful allies and, as
gest that female Japanese macaques do not use homo- such, are able to challenge targets that they dominate but
sexual behavior to communicate about their dominance that their subordinate consort partners do not. By provid-
relationships or control social tension associated with in- ing support against targets that rank above their subor-
cipient aggression. dinate partners, but below themselves, dominant consort
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

56 Vasey

partners can help their partners increase in dominance 1,328 instantaneous scan samples during which groom-
temporarily, at little risk to themselves (for data in sup- ing was recorded, dominant females groomed their sub-
port of this point, see Vasey, 1996, and below). Females ordinate consort partners 86.2% of the time, and subor-
should, thus, prefer to consort with dominant partners, dinate partners groomed 13.8% of the time (Wilcoxon
and competition for such partners should ensue. The most test, N = 18, Z 0 = 3.7, P = .001). Thus, dominant con-
successful competitors should also be the most dominant sort partners were actually more responsible for maintain-
ones. Given the constraints against the formation of con- ing homosexual consortships than were their subordinate
sortships with close kin (Chapais et al., 1997; Chapais & partners.
Mignault, 1991; Wolfe, 1984), this should have resulted In sum, although homosexual consortships and al-
in a concentration of consortships between members of liance formation are causally related, patterns of partner
the two highest ranking matrilines in the study group (i.e., choice and affiliation between consort partners suggest
A–B consortships vs. A–C or B–C consortships). Keep- that females were not engaging in these relationships for
ing in mind that consortships between females were pos- the express purpose of alliance formation. As such, associ-
sible only if two individuals were receptive on the same ations with useful allies during homosexual consortships,
days, a possible 42 A–B consortships could have poten- let alone powerful ones, would be variable at best. This
tially been formed during the 1993–94 mating season, of suggests that homosexual behavior was not designed by
which 42.9% actually occurred. Of a possible 12 A–C con- natural selection as a sociosexual adaptation for alliance
sortships, 41.7% actually occurred. Of a possible 13 B–C formation.
consortships, 38.5% actually occurred. Thus, nonkin in-
dividuals of higher rank (A–B) did not form consortships Acquisition of Alloparental Care
together significantly more often than other types of con-
sorthips (i.e., A–C, B–C; χ 2 = 0.066, df = 2; P > .95). Research on various bird species, particularly gulls
There was also no significant difference in the number of and terns, suggests that females will form pair-bonds with
days over which each of the three different types of con- other females to obtain alloparental care for their depen-
sortships took place (χ for A–B dyads = 5 days; χ, A–C dant offspring (for reviews, see Bagemihl, 1999; Diamond,
dyads = 4.4 days; χ , B–C dyads = 5.8 days; Kruskal- 1989). Several lines of evidence suggest that female
Wallis test: N = 28, H 0 = 0.1, df = 2, P = .93). Japanese macaques do not engage in homosexual con-
Second, in light of the dominance-related benefits sortships to obtain alloparental care for their immature
they can derive from homosexual consortships, subordi- offspring (for further details, see Vasey, 1998).
nate partners should be more responsible for showing the Grooming and alliance support are the most overt ex-
affiliative behaviors that maintain these associations. This pressions of nonsexual affiliation in Japanese macaques,
prediction was not supported, however. During homosex- but these activities did not characterize interactions be-
ual consortships, the overwhelming majority of affilia- tween females and their consort partners’ immature off-
tion flowed in a remarkably bidirectional manner, with no spring. For example, consorting females (N = 17) were
statistically significant differences between dominant and within arm’s reach of their same-sex partners’ immature
subordinate consort partners (Chapais & Mignault, 1991; offspring during 9.7% of the scan samples in which they
Vasey, 1996; Vasey et al., 1998). For example, 49% of were observed (N = 6,190 scan samples). Despite this
approaches (N = 2386) were performed by dominant proximity, they virtually never groomed these immature
members of the consortships and 51% by their subor- individuals (0.0005% of scan samples).
dinate partners (Wilcoxon test, N = 21, Z 0 = 0.82, P = During homosexual consortships, females (N = 16)
.41). Similarly, 45.2% of sexual solicitations (N = 3649) had various opportunities to intervene in support of their
were performed by dominant consort partners and 54.8% same-sex consort partners’ immature offspring when the
by their subordinate partners (Wilcoxon test, N = 21, Z 0 = latter were involved in conflicts (N = 134 opportunities
0.3, P = .77). In addition, dominant females intervened for support, χ = 8.4). None of the females supported their
in support of their subordinate consort partners during partners’ immature offspring in this manner. It is, however,
29.2% of the opportunities to do so (N = 411), whereas striking that during 10.5% of these conflicts, females in-
subordinate females intervened for their dominant consort tervened against their partners’ immature offspring (χ =
partners during 8.7% of available opportunities (N = 601; 0.88). This difference was significant (Wilcoxon test, N =
Wilcoxon test, N = 16, Z 0 = 1.4, P = .16). The sole dif- 6, T = 21, P < .05). These interventions occurred dur-
ference in affiliative behavior involved grooming, which ing 42.9% of all homosexual consortships for which one
the dominant consort partners performed significantly or both partners had immature offspring (N = 14), and
more often than their subordinate counterparts. Out of 37.5% of homosexually consorting females (N = 16)
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques 57

intervened against their partners’ immature offspring ignore or even threaten males who solicit them for sex
when given the opportunity. (Vasey, 1998; Wolfe, 1984).
In addition, females aggressed their partners’ imma- Taken together, the available evidence indicates that
ture offspring significantly more often during homosex- female Japanese macaques do not engage in homosexual
ual consortships (N = 73 aggressive interactions) than behavior to attract male mates.
either before (N = 22 aggressive interactions; Wilcoxon
test, N = 10, T = 47, P < .05) or after (N = 15 aggres-
Is Homosexual Behavior in Female
sive interactions; Wilcoxon test, N = 13, T = 88.5, P =
Japanese Macaques Sexual?
.001). These aggressive interactions occurred during
85.7% of all homosexual consortships for which one or
An implicit theme in much of the literature is the
both partners had immature offspring (N = 14), and
notion that same-sex interactions in animals involving
70.6% of homosexually consorting females aggressed
genital contact are devoid of any sexual motivation, de-
their partners’ immature offspring at least once (N = 17,
spite the fact that they appear to be sexual in terms of
χ = 4).
their superficial form. Consequently, homosexual behav-
In sum, there was no evidence that female Japanese
ior, especially female homosexual behavior, is argued by
macaques provided alloparental care to the immature off-
some researchers to be a phenomenon that is virtually
spring of their same-sex consort partners, and this was not
unique among humans, with almost no counterparts in
for lack of opportunities to do so. In fact, the relationship
nonhuman animals (e.g., Dixson, 1998; for discussion,
between females and their same-sex consort partners’ im-
see Bagemihl, 1999; Nadler, 1990; Vasey, 1995, 2000a;
mature offspring could best be described as agonistic, not
Wallen & Parsons, 1997).
alloparental, in nature. This points to the conclusion that
Various types of evidence indicate that female ho-
female homosexual behavior in Japanese macaques has
mosexual behavior in Japanese macaques is sexual. First,
not been designed by natural selection as a sociosexual
homosexual interactions follow many of the same patterns
adaptation for the acquisition of alloparental care from
as heterosexual copulation (e.g., Fedigan & Gouzoules,
same-sex sexual partners.
1978; Gouzoules & Goy, 1983; Wolfe, 1984). Although
heterosexual copulation should not be the yardstick by
Acquisition of Male Mates
which all other types of sexual behaviors are measured,
heterosexual copulation is considered, a priori, by many
Parker and Pearson (1976) proposed that female ho-
researchers, to be sexual (e.g., Dixson, 1998; for a discus-
mosexual behavior functions to increase the reproductive
sion, see Bagemihl, 1999; Vasey, 2000a). Consequently,
success of the mounting female. By mimicking the copu-
demonstrating that homosexual behavior parallels hetero-
latory pattern of rival males, Parker and Pearson argue that
sexual behavior in many aspects of its expression repre-
the mounting female can attract male sexual partners and
sents powerful evidence for some researchers that the for-
increase her chances of insemination. If the primary rea-
mer is, indeed, a sexual behavior. With this caveat in mind,
son for engaging in homosexual consortships is to attract
it is interesting to note that like heterosexual behavior,
male mates, three key behavioral patterns should be ex-
pected to occur. First, female homosexual behavior should (1) female homosexual behavior is never observed
only occur in the presence of males. Second, female ho- outside of the species’ fall–winter mating season;
mosexual behavior should only occur when the mounting (2) females that engage in homosexual behavior ex-
female is fertile. Third, mounting females should cease hibit a reddening of the face and perineum that
homosexual mounting after receiving sexual solicitations is indicative of increased sexual receptivity;
from a male. (3) female homosexual behavior occurs within the
Several lines of evidence indicate that female context of temporary, but exclusive, sexual pair-
Japanese macaques do not use homosexual behavior to at- bonds called consortships;
tract male mates. First, consorting females often attempt (4) female homosexual behavior is temporally linked
to spatially and visually separate themselves from other to the bidirectional exchange of sexual solicita-
group members (Gouzoules & Goy, 1983). Second, fe- tions between partners (Vasey, 1996; Vasey et al.,
male homosexual behavior in female Japanese macaques 1998). These sexual solicitations appear to be
frequently occurs following conception, when the issue identical to those exchanged in heterosexual
of insemination is moot (Fedigan & Gouzoules, 1978; contexts;
Gouzoules & Goy, 1983; Vasey, 1998; Wolfe, 1984). Third, (5) females employ certain mounting postures in a
females engaged in homosexual consortships tend to homosexual context (e.g., double-foot clasp
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

58 Vasey

mounts with thrusting), which appear to be in- N = 67 opportunities for support) for their sub-
distinguishable from those that males employ in ordinate partners as conservative interventions
a heterosexual context (Vasey et al., 1998); and (31.7% of N = 205 opportunities for support;
(6) female mountees do not try to push or shake off Wilcoxon test, N = 14, Z 0 = 0.6, P = .55).
the mounter; instead, they typically brace their (4) normal dominance relationships are temporarily
bodies to support the mounters. Often, the moun- destabilized during homosexual consortships be-
tees will facilitate mounts-in-progress by reach- cause, subordinate consort partners receive sup-
ing back and clasping the mounters, while gazing port from their dominant partners against targets
into the mounters’ eyes. The same pattern occurs that normally rank above them, or with whom
during many heterosexual mounts. they share ambiguous dominance relationships
(for further detail, see Vasey, 1996). Over half
When considering the issue of sexual motivation, it
of all subordinate consort partners (57.1%; N =
is also important to note that patterns of affiliation dur-
13) rose in rank during the consortship period.
ing homosexual consortships do not mimic generalized
They temporarily increased in dominance dur-
patterns of social affiliation that occur outside of these as-
ing 40% of the opportunities to do so, and fell
sociations. Rather, patterns of affiliation between females
in dominance during 1.5% of the opportunities
during homosexual consortships represent radical depar-
to do so (N = 65). This difference is significant
tures from normal patterns of affiliation that are widely
(Wilcoxon test, N = 13, Z 0 = 2.3, P < .05).
recognized as social. This suggests that homosexual con-
(5) dominant consort partners groom their subordi-
sortships are not social relationships, but rather, are sexual
nate counterparts significantly more often than
ones. For example,
the reverse (for more details, see Vasey, 1996;
(1) female Japanese macaques exercise incest avoi- see above). Outside of homosexual consortships,
dance with close female kin (r ≤ .25; for more the opposite pattern holds true: female Japanese
details, see Chapais et al., 1997). Consequently, macaques overwhelmingly direct grooming up
mounting, sexual solicitations, and consortships the hierarchy, not down (Chapais, Gauthier, &
are never observed between mothers and daugh- Prud’homme, 1995; Koyama, 1991).
ters, sisters, or grandmothers and granddaughters (6) during homosexual consortships, females are al-
(Chapais et al., 1997; Chapais & Mignault, 1991; most exclusively in proximity with their nonkin
Wolfe, 1984). In contrast, these close categories consort partners (Gouzoules & Goy, 1983; Vasey,
of female kin commonly engage in other types 1996, 1998; Wolfe, 1984). Outside the consort-
of affiliative behavior with each other, such as ship period, however, female Japanese macaques
grooming, huddling, and interventions (Chapais, spend most of their time in proximity with close
1992; Koyama, 1991; Kurland, 1977). kin (Kurland, 1977; Singh, D’Souza, & Singh,
(2) female consort partners intervene in conflicts to 1992).
support each other at consistently high rates dur-
ing homosexual consortships. Outside the con- Finally, clitoral contact and stimulation occur during
sortship period, these same females rarely sup- many homosexual mounts. During sitting mounts, for ex-
port each other, if at all (for more details, see ample, mounters rub their clitorises repeatedly and vigor-
Vasey, 1996; see above). ously against the back of mountees. During some double-
(3) during homosexual consortships, a dramatic in- foot clasp and standing mounts, mounters thrust against
crease occurs in the number of bridging interven- the mountees’ clitorises. In addition, mounters and moun-
tions performed by dominant consort partners for tees rub their clitorises with their tails during many same-
their subordinate counterparts (for further detail, sex mounts (Vasey, 1996; Vasey et al., 1998; Wolfe, 1984).
see Vasey, 1996). This pattern is highly unusual Taken together, these various lines of evidence sug-
because outside of homosexual consortships, gest that female homosexual behavior in Japanese
nonkin female Japanese macaques almost never macaques is a sexual behavior, not a social one, or even
perform bridging interventions for each other; a sociosexual one. Moreover, the overwhelmingly bidi-
instead, they perform conservative interventions rectional flow of sexual solicitations, mounts, clasps, ap-
(Chapais, 1992; Chapais, Girard, & Primi, 1991). proaches, and interventions within consortships (Chapais
Yet, when opportunities for support are con- & Mignault, 1991; Vasey, 1996; Vasey et al., 1998)
sidered, dominant consort partners are as likely suggests that these relationships are based on a mutual
to perform bridging interventions (55.2% of sexual attraction between the partners.
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques 59

Are Female Japanese Macaques That Engage the experimental period of the study (N = 21 days), the
in Homosexual Behavior Unable sole male who was present solicited females for sex, but
to Obtain Male Mates? they simply ignored him and opted to engage in homosex-
ual activity instead.
It is commonly thought that homosexual behavior We reasoned that females might be rejecting solicita-
in animals is more frequently manifested when individu- tions by the sole available male in favor of same-sex sexual
als have difficulty accessing opposite-sex mates and are, partners because they did not favor him as a mate under any
thus, obliged to accept same-sex sexual partners (e.g., circumstances. If this were the case, one might conclude
Nadler, 1990; Wallen & Parsons, 1997). Several factors that for all intents and purposes such females really had
might compromise an individual’s ability to access an no heterosexual options during the study’s experimental
opposite-sex mate. Most obviously, opposite-sex mates period. However, the data did not support this conclusion.
might simply be scarce or, in some cases, entirely ab- The majority of females that rejected a sexual solicitation
sent. Alternatively, opposite-sex mates might be present from the sole available male in favor of a same-sex sex-
but disinterested in sexual activity. ual partner also formed heterosexual consortships with the
My colleague and I conducted a behavioral experi- same male at some other point during the study (83.3%;
ment to test whether female Japanese macaques engage N = 6). This indicates that females who engaged in ho-
in homosexual behavior because they are unable to access mosexual behavior in the presence of the sole available
male mates (for more details, see Vasey & Gauthier, 2000). male also recognized him, not only as a potential mate,
The experiment involved two phases (see Methods). We but also as an acceptable one.
began with a period of baseline observation on the in- Taken together, this research suggests that, in most
tact study group that had a female-biased operational sex instances, female Japanese macaques do not engage in
ratio (i.e., number of sexually active males to sexually ac- homosexual behavior simply because they are unable to
tive females) typical for this species (5 males:16 females). obtain sexually motivated and acceptable male mates.
Following this, we removed four of the group’s sexually
active males to create an experimental subgroup with an
operational sex ratio that was heavily skewed towards fe- Are Female Japanese Macaques
males (1 male:11 females). Thus, the experimental sub- Preferentially Heterosexual?
group contained all of the adult females and all of the
immature individuals present during the baseline period, Do individuals actually choose same-sex sexual part-
but only one of the sexually active males. We found that ners when simultaneously presented with opposite-sex al-
females engaged in significantly more homosexual con- ternatives? Or, are animals with bisexual potential always
sortships in the context of this female-skewed operational preferentially heterosexual? I examined these issues by an-
sex ratio, as compared to the baseline period (Wilcoxon alyzing patterns of sexual partner choice by anovulatory
test, N = 6, T = 21, P < .05). Thus, homosexual activ- female Japanese macaques that were the focus of inter-
ity increased among Japanese macaques as the number of sexual competition (for more detail, see Vasey, 1998). In-
adult males in the population decreased. Increases in fe- tersexual competition for female sexual partners occurred
male homosexual activity during the experimental period when a sexually motivated male and female (competitors)
of the study could not be attributed to some change in the simultaneously sought access to the same female sexual
reproductive status of the subjects between the baseline partner (focus of competition). Competitors were consid-
and experimental periods, because none of the subjects ered to be sexually motivated, vis-à-vis the focus of com-
conceived during the period of data collection. Three of petition, if they solicited her for sex while competing in-
the females conceived prior to the study period, and the tersexually for her or if they solicited her for sex at some
others did not conceive at all during the 1994–95 mating point during the same day.
season. Intersexual competition for female sexual partners
On the face of it, it would appear that females were was initiated when a sexually motivated male intruded on
engaging in more homosexual behavior simply because a homosexual consortship, targeting one of the females
they lacked male mates. However, this was not an ade- as the focus of competition and the other as his com-
quate explanation to account for the increased level of petitor (N = 120 intrusions). Intrusions took the form of
female homosexual activity observed at this time. During approaches and sexual solicitations directed at the focus
the experimental period of the study, the sole male was of competition, as well as displacements and aggression
frequently available and motivated to engage in hetero- directed at the female competitor. They functioned to in-
sexual activity. For example, on 71% of the days during crease the males’ access to an anovulatory female sexual
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

60 Vasey

partner (focus of competition), while decreasing or pre- Proceptive females did not perform counterchallen-
venting her consort partner from doing the same. ges simply because they were in a heightened state of
Intrusions on homosexual consortships did not reflect arousal and thus, more assertive. Rather, proceptive fe-
a generalized motivation among males to disrupt any type males (N = 9) retaliated, in kind, against males who ag-
of affiliation between nonkin females. Males (N = 4) in- gressed or displaced them significantly more often when
truded on homosexual consortships for female sexual part- they were engaged in homosexual consortships (χ =
ners 4.3 times more often (0.6 intrusions/hr, χ = 0.15) 17.4%), as compared to when they were not (χ = 0.01%;
than they interfered with nonkin female grooming dyads one-tailed Wilcoxon test, N = 5, T = 15, P = .03). Fur-
observed outside the mating season (0.14 grooming inter- thermore, females were never seen to retaliate against
ferences/hr, χ = 0.04). The tendency to disrupt females males who disrupted their grooming interactions with
engaged in sexual affilation, but not those engaged in so- nonkin females outside the mating season. The females’
cial affiliation, strongly suggests that intrusions are cor- tendency to challenge males who disrupted their sexual,
rectly identified as sexual, not social, competition. but not their social, affiliation strongly suggests that coun-
Following 62% of intrusions, intersexual competition terchallenges are correctly identified as sexual competi-
for female sexual partners terminated. In such cases, the tion and are not part of a more ubiquitous pattern of social
consorting females would (1) ignore the male, who would competition.
eventually leave, (2) move away from the male altogether, Following episodes of intersexual competition, fe-
(3) separate and then rejoin to continue their consortships, males that were the foci of competition could choose be-
or (4) terminate their consortships. Intersexual compe- tween two sexually motivated competitors, one male and
tition for female sexual partners could, however, esca- the other female, who were simultaneously available. Con-
late with the female competitor posing a counterchallenge sequently, these interactions represented an excellent “nat-
against the intruding male. During counterchallenges, the ural experiment” for testing whether females preferred
female competitor attempted to maintain exclusive access certain female sexual partners relative to certain male
to her consort partner, the focus of competition, while pre- alternatives when simultaneously given a choice. In the
venting the male competitor from doing the same. Coun- overwhelming majority of cases (N = 120), the foci of
terchallenges took the form of approaches and sexual so- competition chose to continue mounting with their female
licitations directed at the focus of competition, as well sexual partners significantly more often than they chose
as displacements and aggression directed at the intrud- to begin mounting with the male competitor (Wilcoxon
ing male competitor. During some counterchallenges, test, N = 14, T = 105, P = .002) or to cease mounting
female competitors would also interpose themselves be- altogether (Wilcoxon test, N = 13, T = 91, P = .002;
tween the male competitor and the focus of competition. Fig. 1).
Overall, 38% of intrusions by male competitors pro- Taken together, this research suggests that in those
voked counterchallenges by female competitors (N = 45 populations where female Japanese macaques engage in
counterchallenges). both homosexual and heterosexual behavior, females are

Fig. 1. Choice of competitors by foci of competition following intersexual competition


interactions.
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques 61

not preferentially heterosexual. Females will choose sexual partners with particular characteristics, be they
same-sex sexual partners, even when they are simulta- male or female. Taken together, this evidence suggests
neously presented with sexually motivated, opposite-sex that, in some populations, female Japanese macaques are
alternatives. best characterized as bisexual in orientation, not preferen-
tially homosexual or preferentially heterosexual.
Sexual Preference in Female Japanese Macaques
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
In light of the evidence presented above, I now return
to the question with which I opened this paper: Do animals I thank Michael Bailey, Ray Blanchard, Bernard
ever exhibit a preference for same-sex sexual partners? If Chapais, Lee Ellis, Carole Gauthier, Stefani Kovacovsky,
female Japanese macaques are the animals under consid- Richard Pawsey, Sergio Pellis, Jim Pfaus, Anne Russon,
eration, the answer to the question appears to be a qualified Ken Zucker, and two anonymous reviewers. Various stages
“yes.” Although female Japanese macaques demonstrate of this research were funded by the Universite de Montreal,
a preference for particular same-sex sexual partners, it the Sigma Xi Society, the Fonds pour la formation des
would be incorrect to refer to female Japanese macaques chercheurs et l’aide de recherche (FCAR) Quebec, the
as “homosexual” or to suggest that they are “preferen- University of Lethbridge, and the Natural Science and En-
tially homosexual.” This is because, in those populations gineering Research Council (NSERC) of Canada.
where female Japanese macaques engage in homosex-
ual behavior, they almost invariably do so in combina-
tion with heterosexual behavior over the course of their REFERENCES
life spans. Exclusive preference for same-sex sexual part-
ners over the entire life span has been documented in Adkins-Regan, E. (1988). Sex hormones and sexual orientation in ani-
mals. Psychobiology, 16, 347–355.
Japanese macaques, but such cases are exceptionally rare Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods.
(McDonald-Pavelka, 1993, p. 108). It is possible that in Behaviour, 49, 267–277.
certain populations some females manifest exclusive ho- Bagemihl, B. (1999). Biological exuberance: Animal homosexuality and
natural diversity. New York: St. Martin’s Press.
mosexual behavior throughout an entire mating season. Chapais, B. (1992). The role of alliances in the social inheritance of rank
As a rule, however, females who engage in homosexual among female primates. In A. Harcourt & F. M. B. de Waal (Eds.),
behavior also engage in heterosexual behavior during the Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals (pp. 29–60).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
same mating season, and, indeed, they sometimes even Chapais, B., Girard, M., & Primi, G. (1991). Non-kin alliances, and the
alternate between these activities on the same day. stability of matrilineal dominance relations in Japanese macaques.
How then might female sexual preference in Japanese Animal Behaviour, 41, 481–491.
Chapais, B., Gauthier, C., & Prud’homme, J. (1995). Dominance com-
macaques best be characterized? In some populations, petition through affiliation and support in Japanese macaques: An
individual females, when faced with a pool of potential experimental study. International Journal of Primatology, 16, 521–
sexual partners, prefer certain same-sex sexual partners 536.
Chapais, B., Gauthier, C., Prud’homme, J., & Vasey, P. L. (1997). Re-
relative to the available opposite-sex mates. It is impor- latedness threshold for nepotism in Japanese macaques. Animal
tant to stress, however, that the reverse is equally true: Behaviour, 53, 533–548.
the same females will prefer certain male mates rela- Chapais, B., & Mignault, C. (1991). Homosexual incest avoidance
among females in captive Japanese macaques. American Journal
tive to the available female sexual partners. As such, fe- of Primatology, 23, 171–183.
males do not invariably prefer males over females, nor vice Diamond, J. M. (1989). Goslings of gay geese. Nature, 340, 101.
versa. Dixson, A. F. (1998). Primate sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
The likelihood that a female sexual partner will be Eaton, G. G. (1978). Longitudinal studies of sexual behavior in the Ore-
chosen over a male increases whenever preferred female gon troop of Japanese macaques. In T. E. McGill, D. A. Dewsbury,
sexual partners are abundant in the population. Conse- & B. D. Sachs (Eds.), Sex and behavior (pp. 35–59). New York:
Plenum.
quently, it is not a lack of males per se that promotes the Enomoto, T. (1974). The sexual behavior of Japanese monkeys. Journal
expression of female homosexual activity in the context of Human Evolution, 3, 351–372.
of female-skewed operational sex ratios; rather, it is the Fairbanks, L. A., McGuire, M. T., & Kerber, W. (1977). Sex and aggres-
sion during rhesus monkey group formation. Aggressive Behavior,
relative paucity of preferred male mates coupled with a 3, 241–249.
relative abundance of preferred female sexual partners. Fedigan, L. M. (1982). Primate paradigms: Sex roles and social bonds.
The precise nature of individual sexual preferences Montréal: Eden.
Fedigan, L. M. (1991). History of the Arashiyama West Japanese
will vary greatly from one female to the next and will de- macaques in Texas. In L. M. Fedigan & P. J. Asquith (Eds.), The
pend largely on idiosyncratic predilections for particular monkeys of Arashiyama: Thirty-five years of research in Japan
P1: GDP
Archives of Sexual Behavior pp350-aseb-365185 December 28, 2001 8:29 Style file version July 26, 1999

62 Vasey

and the West (pp. 54–73). Albany: State University of New York Singh, M., D’Souza, L., & Singh, M. R. (1992). Hierarchy, kinship
Press. and social interaction among Japanese monkeys, Macaca fuscata.
Fedigan, L. M., & Gouzoules, H. (1978). The consort relationship in a Journal of Bioscience, 17, 15–27.
troop of Japanese monkeys. In D. J. Chivers & J. Herbert (Eds.), Smuts, B. B., & Watanabe, J. M. (1990). Social relationships and ritual-
Recent advances in primatology: Vol. 1. Behavior (pp. 493–495). ized greetings in adult male baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis).
New York: Academic Press. International Journal of Primatology, 11, 147–172.
Gouzoules, H., & Goy, R. W. (1983). Physiological and social influences Stephenson, G. R. (1973). Testing for group specific communication
on mounting behavior of troop-living female monkeys (Macaca patterns in Japanese macaques. In E. W. Menzel, Jr. (Ed.), Sym-
fuscata). American Journal of Primatology, 5, 39–49. posia of the Fourth International Congress of Primatology: Vol. 1.
Idani, G. (1991). Social relationships between immigrant and resident Precultural behaviour in primates (pp. 51–75). Basel: S. Karger.
bonobo (Pan paniscus) females at Wamba. Folia Primatologica, 57, Takahata, Y. (1982). The sociosexual behavior of Japanese macaques.
83–95. Z. Tierpsychologie, 59, 89–108.
Itani, J., & Nishimura, A. (1973). The study of infrahuman culture in Vasey, P. L. (1995). Homosexual behavior in primates: A review of ev-
Japan. In E. W. Menzel, Jr. (Ed.), Symposia of the Fourth Interna- idence and theory. International Journal of Primatology, 16, 173–
tional Congress of Primatology: Vol. 1. Precultural behaviour in 204.
primates (pp. 26–50). Basel: S. Karger. Vasey, P. L. (1996). Interventions and alliance formation between
Koyama, N. (1991). Grooming relationships in the Arashiyama group female Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata, during homosexual
of Japanese monkeys. In L. M. Fedigan & P. J. Asquith (Eds.), consortships. Animal Behaviour, 52, 539–551.
The monkeys of Arashiyama: Thirty-five years of research in Japan Vasey, P. L. (1998). Female choice and inter-sexual competition for fe-
and the West (pp. 211–226). Albany: State University of New York male sexual partners in Japanese macaques. Behaviour, 135, 579–
Press. 597.
Kurland, J. A. (1977). Kin selection in the Japanese monkey. Basel: Vasey, P. L. (2000a). Animal studies. In B. Zimmerman (Ed.), Ency-
S. Karger. clopedia of lesbian histories and cultures (pp. 55–56). New York:
Lunardini, A. (1989). Social organization in a confined group of Japanese Garland Press.
macaques (Macaca fuscata): An application of correspondence Vasey, P. L. (2000b). Comment on the evolution of human homosexual
analysis. Primates, 30, 175–185. behavior. Current Anthropology, 41, 402–403.
McDonald-Pavelka, M. S. (1993). Monkeys of the mesquite: The social Vasey, P. L., Chapais, B., & Gauthier, C. (1998). Mounting interactions
life of the south Texas snow monkey. Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt. between female Japanese macaques: Testing the influence of dom-
Nadler, R. D. (1990). Homosexual behavior in nonhuman primates. inance and aggression. Ethology, 104, 387–398.
In D. P. McWhirter, S. A. Sanders, & J. M. Reinisch (Eds.), Vasey, P. L., & Gauthier, C. (2000). Skewed sex ratios and female ho-
Homosexuality/heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual orientation mosexual activity in Japanese macaques: An experimental analysis.
(pp. 138–170). New York: Oxford University Press. Primates, 41, 17–25.
Parish, A. R. (1994). Sex and food control in the “uncommon” chim- Wallen, K., & Parsons, W. A. (1997). Sexual behavior in same-sexed
panzee: How bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of nonhuman primates: Is it relevant to understanding homosexuality?
male dominance. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 157–179. Annual Review of Sex Research, 7, 195–223.
Parker, G. A., & Pearson, R. G. (1976). A possible origin and adap- Wickler, W. (1967). Socio-sexual signals and their intra-specific im-
tive significance of the mounting behavior shown by some fe- itation among primates. In D. Morris (Ed.), Primate ethology
male mammals in oestrous. Journal of Natural History, 10, 241– (pp. 69–79). Chicago: Aldine.
245. Wolfe, L. D. (1984). Japanese macaque female sexual behavior: A com-
Rendall, D., & Taylor, L. L. (1991). Female sexual behavior in the ab- parison of Arashiyama East and West. In M. F. Small (Ed.), Female
sence of male–male competition in Japanese macaques. Zoo Biol- primates: Studies by women primatologists (pp. 141–157). New
ogy, 10, 319–328. York: Alan R. Liss.

You might also like